Carlson 2013
Carlson 2013
Carlson 2013
Executive function (EF) refers to the set of neurocognitive skills involved in goal-directed
problem solving, including working memory, inhibitory control, and set shifting/flexibility.
EF depends importantly upon neural networks involving prefrontal cortex, and continues
to improve into early adulthood, although major advances in EF occur during the
preschool period. Individual differences in EF are increasingly recognized as a key
predictor of long-term cognitive and social developmental outcomes. Research suggests
that EF is influenced by both distal and proximal factors in development (e.g.,
socioeconomic status, culture, language, caregiving, gene–environment interactions, and
sleep). Importantly, EF can be trained, with corresponding changes to brain structure and
function. In this chapter, we review the structure of EF, including “hot EF” (EF in
motivationally significant contexts), age-related changes, atypical development,
measurement issues, theories of underlying mechanisms, outcomes associated with EF,
influences on EF development, and the recent emergence of training studies.
Keywords: executive function, cognitive control, prefrontal cortex, working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive
flexibility, interventions
Key Points
1. EF emerges early, develops rapidly during the preschool years, and continues to
develop into the third decade of life.
2. EF can be thought of as a unitary construct that becomes increasingly
differentiated with development and experience.
Page 1 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
genes, sleep, and motor development have all been shown to have an impact on EF
in childhood.
10. EF can be trained, with corresponding changes to brain structure and function.
Introduction
Executive function (EF) refers to an interrelated set of neurocognitive skills that are
critical for adaptive function. EF is most closely associated with neural networks
involving prefrontal cortex (PFC) and is exhibited when individuals engage in conscious,
goal-directed thought and action under novel or unfamiliar circumstances, where
previously established routines for responding are nonexistent or, as is often the case,
directly interfere with the desired response. EF in childhood has received increasing
attention, as reflected by a threefold increase in the number of publications on this topic
in the past decade (Fig. 25.1). One reason for this explosion of interest is that executive
dysfunction has been implicated in a number of childhood disorders (e.g., attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD], autism, and conduct disorder; see Casey,
Tottenham, & Fosella, 2002) and is associated with a variety of negative adjustment
outcomes (e.g., Blair & Razza, 2007; Hughes & Ensor, 2011; Ozonoff & Jensen, 1999;
Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). For example, poor EF in childhood predicts criminal
behavior, alcohol and drug abuse, lower socioeconomic status (SES), and other problems
in adulthood (Moffitt, Arseneault, Belsky, Dickson, Hancox, et al., 2011).
Page 2 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Page 3 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
In contrast to research with older children and adults, most research of this kind with
preschoolers finds a one-factor solution to be most parsimonious (Wiebe, Espy, &
Charack, 2008; Wiebe, Sheffield, Nelson et al., 2011; but see Hughes, 1998a; Miller,
Geisbrecht, Müller, McInerney, & Kerns, 2012). Wiebe and colleagues (2008) used a
battery of preschool EF measures, including three tasks considered a priori to demand
working memory and seven tasks that putatively require inhibitory control. Seven models
were tested, and for reasons of goodness of fit and parsimony, the unitary EF model was
preferred. Together, the evidence for a unitary EF factor in preschool and at least two
distinct components (WM and shifting) in later childhood appears to be consistent with a
general process of increasing functional specialization of neural systems that are initially
relatively undifferentiated but that become more specialized (or modularized) as part of a
developmental process of adaptation (e.g., Johnson & Munakata, 2005; see also Durston
et al., 2006). Evidence consistent with the increasing differentiation of EF from other
cognitive functions has also been found, and will be discussed later (Zelazo et al., in
press).
Although the factors emerging from a factor analysis are constrained by the initial choice
of tasks, as well as any shared method variance among tasks loading on a factor, this
approach represents a valuable starting point in generating hypotheses about EF that can
be tested experimentally. In this chapter, we adopt a “unity and diversity” approach, and
consider EF both as a class of cognitive processes (WM, inhibition, shifting) and as an
overarching coordination of these processes in achieving a certain goal (i.e., a functional
characterization of EF), although of course no class can be a member of itself (“Russell’s
paradox;” Whitehead & Russell, 1927).
Page 4 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Finally, a key region within the medial frontal cortex is the anterior cingulate cortex
(ACC). The ACC is part of a circuit that connects OFC and parts of the limbic system
involved in learning and effortful activities (Powell & Voeller, 2004). Thus, PFC is
connected to limbic areas of the brain in two ways, via the OFC and via the anterior
cingulate (Tucker, Luu, & Pribram, 1995). These ventromedial pathways are thought to
have relatively greater involvement than DL-PFC in social and emotional aspects of
cognition as well as behavioral regulation.
Page 5 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
The growth of PFC beyond infancy has been documented using a variety of measures, and
a number of consistent patterns have been noted. First, myelination within PFC (and
connecting PFC to other regions) increases monotonically over the course of childhood
and into adulthood, and likely contributes to increased efficiency, processing speed, and
integration of information (Giedd et al., 1996; Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997;
Klingberg, Vaidya, Gabrieli, Moseley, & Hedehus, 1999; Yakovlev & Lecours, 1967).
Second, in contrast to these age-related increases in white matter, gray matter (measured
as gray matter volume, synaptic density, or cortical thickness) in PFC shows a pattern of
early increases followed by gradual decreases that start in late childhood and continue
into adulthood (e.g., Gogtay et al., 2004; Huttenlocher, 1990; O’Donnell et al., 2005; Fig.
25.3). This inverted-U–shaped pattern of growth may reflect the overproduction and
subsequent pruning of synapses—a process that would allow the brain to be shaped by
experience (e.g., Casey, Giedd, & Thomas, 2000; Durston et al., 2001). In addition to these
structural changes, several researchers have noted a shift from more diffuse to more
focal activation of PFC during performance on measures of EF—more activation in areas
related to EF in adults, and less activation in areas unrelated to EF (e.g., Bunge, Dudovic,
Thomason, Vaidya, & Gabrieli, 2002; Durston et al., 2006; Luna et al., 2001). For example,
Durston and colleagues (2006) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to
examine neural activation in children and adults as they performed a Go/No-Go task, and
observed an age-related decrease in activation in brain regions unimportant for task
performance. The only increase occurred in an area of VL-PFC, and this increase was
associated with improved task performance. This focalization as a function of age and EF
suggests that the functional development of PFC may be associated with more specialized
and efficient processing.
Page 6 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Page 7 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
A variety of hot EF tasks are suitable for use with preschoolers, including the Less is
More Task (Carlson, Davis, & Leach, 2005), the Children’s Gambling Task (Kerr & Zelazo,
2004), the Delay of Gratification Task (Mischel & Ebbesen, 1970), and the Delay Choice
Paradigm (Beck, Schaefer, Pang, & Carlson, 2011; Prencipe & Zelazo, 2005; Thompson,
Baressi, & Moore, 1997). These measures generally show age-related declines in
impulsive responding, as well as individual differences that are correlated with
performance on relatively cool EF measures and, at least in the case of Delay of
Gratification, remain fairly stable over time (for a summary see Mischel, Ayduk, Berman,
Casey, Gotlib, et al., 2010).
Page 8 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Infancy and toddlerhood. Search for hidden objects is perhaps the earliest emerging
behavior that unambiguously requires EF: infants must keep the location of a desired
object in mind and use this information to govern their behavior. By 7 to 8 months, infants
can retrieve an attractive object when it is hidden in one of two locations and then briefly
obscured for 2 to 3 seconds before infants are allowed to reach for it (Diamond, 1985). By
around 9 to 12 months, they are able to remember the location for longer delays, but they
may perseverate by returning to a previous location after watching the object being
hidden at a new location (the A-not-B error; Marcovitch & Zelazo, 1999; Piaget, 1954).
Perseverative behavior of this nature is common in infants and toddlers (Chelune & Baer,
1986; Levin et al., 1991; Welsh et al., 1991) and can be seen in older children as well
(Sophian & Wellman, 1983; Spencer, Smith, & Thelen, 2001; Zelazo, Reznick, &
Spinazolla, 1998). However, the fact that longer delays are required to elicit the A-not-B
error with age suggests that infants have a gradually increasing capacity to shift to a new
Page 9 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
response set, and that this increasing capacity may be tied to working memory (Diamond,
1985; Diamond & Doar, 1989). When adapting to changing circumstances, toddlers also
begin to make use of feedback to guide their own behavior (Overman, Bachevalier,
Schuhmann, & Ryan, 1996) and to evaluate when a goal has been met. For example,
when 18- and 24-month-olds were asked to evaluate the accuracy of towers built by an
adult, they distinguished between correctly and incorrectly built versions, and by 28
months, toddlers correctly assessed their own performance as well (Bullock &
Lütkenhaus, 1988). As the authors noted, however, many toddlers persisted with the
action even after the task had been completed, which is reminiscent of Luria’s (1961)
observation of young children pressing a rubber bulb repeatedly even after the desired
effect (illumination of a light) had occurred.
Preschool period. From age 2 to 5 years of age, children make substantial gains in their
ability to represent problems, plan solutions, hold a plan in mind, execute problem-
solving actions, evaluate the outcomes, and correct errors (Zelazo et al., 1997). During
this period, children also become increasingly accurate and efficient at inhibiting
prepotent behaviors or response tendencies (Carlson & Moses, 2001; Diamond & Taylor,
1996; Espy, 1997). Evidence of improvement in problem representation and flexibility can
be seen, for example, in the ability to use symbolic representations of a problem space in
order to guide behavior in a novel situation (Carlson, Davis, & Leach, 2005; DeLoache,
1987). Children show increases in the complexity of the rules that they can represent and
use to control their behavior (Carlson, Faja, & Beck, in press; Zelazo & Frye, 1998).
These improvements can be observed using measures of rule use, which require holding
one or more rules in mind and flexibly switching between rules. Zelazo and Reznick
(1991; Zelazo et al., 1995), for example, investigated rule use in 2.5- to 3-year-olds using
a card sort in which children were presented with a pair of ad hoc rules (e.g., “If it’s
something (p. 713) found inside the house, then put it here. If it’s something found
outside the house, then put it there”) and then asked them to use these rules to separate
10 test cards. Whereas 3-year-olds performed well, 2.5-year-olds typically failed to use
the rules despite possessing knowledge about the cards: when they erred, it usually
involved putting a card into the box in which they had put a card on the previous trial
(i.e., perseveration errors). With Luria’s tapping test (Luria, 1966), Diamond and Taylor
(1996) demonstrated that children are able to hold two rules in mind for increasing
durations between 3 and 6 years. Similarly, on the Grass/Snow task, children are
instructed to point to a white card when the examiner says “grass” and to point to a green
card when she says “snow.” Performance on this task improves significantly from 3 to 5
years of age (Carlson & Moses, 2001; Passler, Isaac, & Hynd, 1985). Similar
improvements are observed in the conflict condition of the Day/Night Stroop-like task,
especially between age 3.5 and 4.5 years (Gerstadt et al., 1994), and on the switch
condition of the Shape School task (Espy, 1997). Thus, across tasks, it is now clear that
children develop the ability to represent and use a pair of rules during the preschool
period. While perseveration errors continue to be made, the ability to hold rules in mind
Page 10 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
for longer periods, to inhibit more automatically, and to switch more flexibly develops
typically between ages 3 and 6.
A crucial element of EF is the ability to recognize when actions do not have the intended
effect, necessitating further processing. Under these circumstances, children must be
able to detect and correct errors and make use of environmental feedback. Error
detection includes both objective errors and discrepancies between internal intentions or
beliefs and the achieved outcome (Ohlsson, 1996), and there is evidence for both by age
3. During preschool, children become more strategic in their approach to correcting
errors (DeLoache, Sugarman, & Brown, 1985). Nonetheless, 3-year-olds have been shown
to persist with a response pattern even when not reinforced—sometimes showing
remarkable resistance to extinction (e.g., Carlson et al., 2005; Happaney, Zelazo, & Stuss,
2004; Russell, Mauthner, Sharpe, & Tidswell, 1991), although improvements are seen
between 3 and 5 years of age (see also Jeffrey & Cohen, 1965; Stevenson & Weir, 1961).
Similarly, Gladstone (1969) found a more rapid extinction period in 4.5-year-olds than 3.5-
year-olds when a reward was no longer available in a transparent container.
As can be seen in tasks such as the Tower of Hanoi (Piaget, 1976), Tower of London
(Shallice, 1982), Dog-cat-mouse (Klahr, 1985), and Truck Loading (Carlson, Moses, &
Claxton, 2004), all of which involve moving objects to a goal configuration, preschool-age
children also become more proactive in planning their approaches to problems. For
example, 3-year-old children performing the Truck Loading task exhibited planning by
remembering to load all the items into the truck, but not yet demonstrating a means–end
strategy in which they had to load items in reverse order for the stops along the route
(Carlson et al., 2004). Verbally guided tasks, in which the child tells the examiner what to
do, may offer a “purer” measure of planning ability versus execution (Klahr & Robinson,
1981), and such measures have been used to examine the development of goal-focused
thinking on tasks such as the Tower of London (Byrd, Van Der Veen, McNamara, & Berg,
2004).
Two key aspects of performance for many EF tasks, inhibition and WM, improve markedly
during the preschool period, and the isolation of these factors has received considerable
attention in the research literature. Some authors have suggested that increases in WM
reduce the effort required to inhibit an incorrect response, thus leading to improved
performance (Engle, 2002; Roberts & Pennington, 1996). WM may be especially
important in planning and execution of problem solving (Zelazo et al., 1997). Senn, Espy,
and Kaufmann (2004) used path analysis to investigate the relation between WM,
inhibition, and problem solving in children ages 2 to 6 years. They found that both WM
and inhibition predicted problem solving, with inhibition being the strongest predictor in
younger children and WM being the strongest predictor for older children (see also Espy
& Bull, 2005). Thus, inhibition may be a more critical factor early in development,
whereas WM and the ability to represent more complex problems may emerge as critical
factors for EF during preschool and contribute to performance throughout development.
Page 11 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task (Zelazo, 2006) has been extensively used
over the past two decades to probe the development of cognitive flexibility in early
childhood, in part because it reveals rapid and dramatic changes in children’s EF and
neural functioning in early development, and in part because it lends itself to
experimental manipulation of task parameters and multiple levels of analysis (Buss &
Spencer, 2008; Zelazo, Müller, Frye, & Marcovitch, 2003). In this (p. 714) task, children
are asked to flexibly switch between dimensions such as “shape” and “color” when
sorting cards in the face of conflicting shape and color cues. Thus, this task requires
inhibitory control to prevent responding based on the irrelevant dimension, WM to
maintain representations of the relevant task rules, and task switching to update all of
these processes after the rule switch (Garon et al., 2008). Although 4-year-olds have little
trouble switching rules, 3-year-olds perseverate and continue using the first set of rules
after they are instructed to switch. This perseveration is robust and persists despite
regular reminders that the rules have changed. Indeed, it is commonly found that 3-year-
olds can correctly respond to explicit questions about the current rules but nonetheless
continue to perseverate on the basis of the previous rules, referred to as a knowledge–
action dissociation (Zelazo et al., 1996). Unlike some planning studies, however, 3-year-
old children tend to make the same error whether they do it themselves or evaluate the
performance of others (Jacques, Zelazo, Kirkham, & Semcesen, 1999; see also Moriguchi,
Lee, & Itakura, 2007; Moriguchi, Kanda, Ishiguro, & Itakura, 2010), and research by
Munakata and Yerys (2001) also shows that children err on knowledge questions that
make reference to both dimensions. Together, these findings are consistent with the
suggestion that young children do not represent a higher-order rule that allows them to
select the current rules instead of the previous rules.
Recent neuroimaging research has confirmed the role of lateral PFC in children’s
performance on the DCCS and related tasks (Crone, Donohue, Honomichl, Wendelken, &
Bunge, 2006; Moriguchi & Hiraki, 2009; Morton, Bosma, & Ansari, 2009). In addition,
event-related potential (ERP) analyses comparing preschool-age children who switched
flexibly (passed) to those who perseverated on postswitch trials (failed) found that the
amplitude of the N2 component was smaller (less negative) for children who passed the
DCCS than for children who failed, suggesting that the N2, often associated with conflict
monitoring, may serve as a neural marker of individual differences in executive function.
This finding is consistent with the downregulation of ACC-mediated conflict detection in
children who reflect upon their conflicting rule representations and apprehend the
hierarchical structure of the task. An ERP study of older children using a more difficult
version of the task also implicated the N2 component during conflict trials (Waxer &
Morton, 2011). Thus, recent data suggest that this simple task can provide insight to the
full spectrum of EF and associated neural changes in early childhood.
School-age years. By the time they enter school, children have gradually gained increased
control of their thought and behavior for problem solving across many domains, such as
the ability to control attention to relevant aspects of the environment and inhibit
attention to distraction (Ponitz, McClelland, Matthews, & Morrison, 2009; West &
Abravanel, 1972). Nevertheless, as indicated by the lifespan trajectory of EF, advances
Page 12 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
The contributions of memory and inhibitory control to EF have also been explored with
school-aged children, with several authors suggesting distinct roles for each (Diamond,
2002; Pennington, 1994, 1998; Welsh, 2002). In one study with groups of 6- and 8-year-
olds, memory and inhibitory load were manipulated independently, and results suggested
independence of these two factors, despite development of both during the age range
investigated (Beveridge, Jarrold, & Pettit, 2002). Similarly, confirmatory factor analysis
with a cross-sectional group of 6- to 13-year-olds revealed three factors—disinhibition,
speed/arousal, and WM/fluency—with different developmental courses of maturation
(Brocki & Bohlin, 2004). The processes that underlie EF are best thought of as functional
categories that have their own complex developmental trajectories. Inhibitory control can
be seen to develop in late infancy, as indexed by the A-not-B task (Marcovitch & Zelazo,
1999; Thelen, Schöner, Scheier, & Smith, 2001), but continues to develop in more
complex contexts that pit internalized rules against prepotent behaviors, such as
multistep multilocation search (Zelazo et al., 1998), the Simon task (Gerardi-Caulton,
2000), the flanker task (p. 715) (Rueda et al., 2004), Stroop-like tasks (Carlson, 2005),
and Go/No-Go tasks (Dowsett & Livesey, 2000), among many others. Similarly, working
memory increases in capacity between 1 and 5 years, as measured in search and change
detection tasks (Diamond, 1985; Hughes & Ensor, 2005; Isaacs & Vargha-Khadem, 1989;
Logie & Pearson, 1997; Simmering, Shutte, & Spencer, 2008), but capacity continues to
increase into childhood (e.g., Cowan, Elliott, Saults, Morey, Mattox, Hismajatullina, &
Conway, 2005; Simmering et al., 2008; Vicari, Bellucci, & Carlesimo, 2003). Finally, task
switching shows severe impairments around 2 years of age: children are able to reliably
engage in one-rule tasks, but perseveratively use only one rule if the task contains more
(Carlson, 2012; Zelazo & Reznick, 1991). Switching from one rule set to another is
possible in some tasks at 3 years of age (e.g., Brace, Morton, & Munakata, 2006; Carlson,
Mandell, & Williams, 2004; Diamond, Carlson, & Beck, 2005; Müller, Dick, Gela, Overton,
& Zelazo, 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003), but as shown with the standard version of the DCCS,
it is not until 4 years of age that children can reliably switch from one rule set to another
using bivalent, integrated, and conflicting stimuli (Müller et al., 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003).
More complex rules (a higher-order rule governing two lower-order rules) make the task
challenging through 6 years of age (Carlson, 2012; Zelazo, 2006). Switch costs (e.g.,
response time delays) are still seen into adolescence and adulthood (Diamond & Kirkham,
Page 13 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
2005; Morton et al., 2009) and show an inverted-U–shaped pattern across the lifespan
(Cepeda et al., 2001; Weintraub et al., in press; Zelazo et al., 2004).
A number of other childhood disorders also have associated executive impairments. For
instance, several investigations link executive impairments with reading difficulty,
although effect sizes vary depending on the task and criteria for defining reading
difficulties (Alloway, Gathercole, Kirkwood, & Elliott, 2009; Booth, Boyle, & Kelly, 2010).
There is also some evidence that specific language impairment, which has primarily been
linked with poor WM (e.g., Archibald & Gathercole, 2006; Briscoe & Rankin, 2009), may
also include impaired inhibitory control (Spalding, 2010). Tourette syndrome, a
neurodevelopmental disorder involving frontostriatal function (Singer, 2005), is
associated primarily with inhibitory impairments in the executive domain (Eddy, Rizzo, &
Cavanna, 2009). Finally, several other conditions that may affect brain development more
broadly, such as malnutrition or severe environmental deprivation, prematurity, epilepsy,
phenylketonuria, and prenatal exposure to alcohol, also have executive dysfunction
correlates (Anderson & Doyle, 2004; Diamond, Prevor, Callender, & Druin, 1997;
Page 14 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Lukowski et al., 2010; Luton, Burns, & DeFilippis, 2010; Rasmussen, 2005; Rose,
Feldman, & Jankowski, 2011; Stevens et al., 2008).
Finally, insults to the development of the frontal lobe prenatally or perinatally or during
childhood are also associated with executive dysfunction. Early focal damage to the
frontal lobe has been investigated, although it is more difficult to study in children than in
adults because of the relative rarity of cases and the protracted development of the
frontal lobes among typically developing children who serve as a comparison. An early
case study by Ackerly and Benton (1948) described a boy (J. P.) with congenital bilateral
prefrontal degeneration due to an abscess. J. P. lacked impulse control and abstraction
beyond what was present in the immediate environment, but his IQ, initiation of action,
and conversational language were within the normal limits. Associated difficulties
included lack of fearfulness, failure to alter his behavior as a result of punishment, and
lack of social understanding, social reciprocity, and empathy, reminiscent of the case of
Phineas Gage (Damasio et al., 1994). Subsequent pediatric case studies describe
executive, social-emotional, and moral deficits as a result of early damage to the
prefrontal region (Eslinger, Flaherty-Craig, & Benton, 2004). Of the cases reviewed, the
most severely affected children had early damage to one or both frontal poles and/or
orbitofrontal and inferior mesial prefrontal cortex.
Page 15 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Taken together, a number of clinical disorders are associated with executive impairments.
In many cases, this literature divides EF into component processes (shifting, inhibitory
control, WM, planning, fluency) and employs tasks that either broadly measure multiple
components of EF or use a single measure to represent EF within a broad battery of
measures for other domains. To date, few investigations have attempted to parse the high
rates of comorbidity between disorders that share executive dysfunction (e.g., ASD,
ADHD, and obsessive compulsive disorder; Leyfer et al., 2006). Nonetheless, explicit
comparisons provide the opportunity to better understand the unique contribution of
executive dysfunction to the disorder, and to understand overlap between disorders with
respect to potential endophenotypes, and provide a unique perspective by which to
understand the typical development of EF, its correlates, and its constraints.
Page 16 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Measurement Issues
A major challenge in research on EF has been methodological: the absence of measures
that are equally suitable across a range of ages. Carlson (2005) made a significant
contribution to the understanding of how EF tasks relate to one another and across
different ages by measuring the performance of 600 children ages 2 to 6 years across 24
different measures of EF. An age effect was found for 65% of the tasks administered, and
Carlson (2005) predicted that effects would have been detected with task modifications
for the remaining 35%. This broad investigation of EF during the preschool period
provides useful information for researchers regarding the difficulty level of many
common EF tasks.
Another major methodological advance to the study of EF is the creation of the NIH
Toolbox Cognition Battery (Zelazo & Bauer, in press), described earlier (see Fig. 25.5). In
an initial validation study, these measures were administered to 476 participants between
the ages of 3 and 85 years, and readministered to a subset of participants (approximately
30%) 1 to 3 weeks later. Results of the validation study revealed excellent developmental
sensitivity across the lifespan, excellent reliability, and good convergent validity. In
addition, correlations between EF and age were higher for younger children (3 to 6
years) than for older children (8 to 15 years), suggesting that EF develops more rapidly
during the preschool years than during the transition to adolescence. There was also
evidence of increasing differentiation (with age) of EF from other aspects of cognition,
consistent with the notion that neurocognitive development in general involves the
increasing functional specialization of neural systems that are initially relatively
undifferentiated but that become more specialized (or modularized) as a function of
experience (e.g., Johnson, 2005).
Page 17 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
children. This presents a particular challenge to those who seek to develop standard or
comparable methods for studying children with rapidly developing abilities or for making
comparisons between typical and atypical development at various ages.
Another methodological concern is that tasks with high internal validity may miss
important aspects of real-world EF (i.e., ecological validity). One approach to this issue is
to collect information about EF and self-regulation in various settings through parent
and/or teacher surveys, such as the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (Rothbart, Ahadi,
Hershey, & Fisher, 2001) and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, or
BRIEF (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000). Gioia and Isquith (2004) recommended
comparing carefully defined measures of specific process components against
ecologically valid assessments of real-world function (e.g., ADHD symptomatology).
Alternatively, tasks that seek to replicate aspects of real-world EF in the laboratory have
also been developed (e.g., the clean-up task; Kochanska & Aksan, 1995).
A particularly useful design for detecting sequences is to give children slightly different
versions of the same task that assess different components of EF or levels of difficulty
(see Wellman & Liu, 2004, for the successful use of this design for assessing the
acquisition sequence for ToM tasks). By examining the mean performance on each
version or the percent of children who pass each version at each age, one can infer the
order in which the aspects of EF are acquired. Also, a scalogram analysis could examine
how many children pass all of the hypothesized easier versions of the task before a
hypothesized more difficult task. That is, if the hypothesized ordering of component tasks
from easiest to hardest is A, B, C, D, then the outcome of interest is how many children
passed A, B, and C, but not D; A and B but not C and D; and A but not B, C, and D. Carlson
(2012) has applied this method to the DCCS by designing a series of seven levels of the
task that gradually increase in difficulty, thus making it developmentally sensitive for
children ages 2 to 6 years. Measures that are built around a core set of identified EF
skills (e.g., WM, inhibitory control, and set shifting), are appropriate across a wide age
range, and yet retain continuity in basic task features will be extremely valuable for
providing age norms and tracking intra- and interindividual changes in EF that occur as a
Page 18 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Finally, longitudinal studies are ideal to detect sequences, but few exist. Several
longitudinal studies have examined the Tower of Hanoi or Tower of London task, with a
focus on developmental sequences in strategy use (McNamara et al., 2007), as well as the
family, cognitive, school achievement, and social adjustment correlates (Friedman et al.,
2007; Jacobson & Pianta, 2007). On inhibitory tasks, one striking sequence identified is
that performance on a delay of gratification task at age 4 predicts, and thus may be a
developmental precursor for, performance on inhibitory tasks such as the Go/No-Go task
at age 18 (Eigsti et al., 2006) and after age 40 (Casey et al., 2010). In longitudinal studies
from ages 2 to 4 (Carlson, Mandell, & Williams, 2004; Hughes & Ensor, 2005, 2007), and
even from infancy to age 4 (Morasch & Bell, 2011), EF (aggregates of inhibition, WM, and
shifting tasks) showed stable individual differences, indicating the predictive validity of
early EF for later EF, as well as related skills such as ToM. Psychometric research on EF
in early childhood is just beginning and will benefit from further development of age-
appropriate tasks that tap into the core aspects of EF and can be administered in a
variety of settings across the preschool period, to provide age norms and to tell us more
about the early organization and differentiation of EF (Carlson, Faja, & Beck, in press).
Page 19 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
when provided with a scaffold such as (p. 719) congruent flankers around the test
stimulus, which purportedly increases the strength of the appropriate rule representation
(Jordan & Morton, 2012).
Research in developmental cognitive neuroscience has provided support for aspects of all
four of these approaches, and indeed, it now seems likely that EF involves the
orchestration of a variety of processes, including inhibitory control, WM, and reflection
Page 20 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
and rule complexity. It may also depend in part on conceptual changes (e.g., about how
our minds govern our actions), at least early in development. In general, most of these
theories depict EF development as involving an increasing ability to resolve conflict. They
differ in whether this conflict is between rules that eventually become hierarchically
organized (Zelazo et al., 2003), latent and active representations (e.g., habits vs.
attention/working memory, Munakata, 2001), or the current representation versus
prepotent mental sets or behaviors (Diamond, 2006). Most also emphasize the role of
changes in underlying neural networks. In particular, Posner and Rothbart (2007; see also
Garon et al., 2008) propose that the development of the central attentional system plays
the major role in the resolution of conflict by regulating other brain networks. Posited
developmental change is both qualitative (e.g., changing from simpler to more complex
rule systems, Zelazo; a new way of describing stimuli, Perner) and quantitative (e.g.,
strengthening active representations, Munakata; age-related increases in inhibitory
control and working memory, Diamond).
Individual Differences
Although there are general characteristics of PFC structure and development, such as its
hierarchical formation, that are likely to constrain observed age-related changes in early
childhood, resulting in a typical developmental course, there are also striking individual
differences in EF. Next, we turn to the correlates of individual differences in EF that can
be detected using developmentally sensitive measures. Examining the correlates of EF
helps to contextualize it as a critically important aspect of cognitive and social
development; furthermore, it also has the potential to inform theories of direct (p. 720)
Page 21 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
ToM and EF are linked in fundamental ways (Moses & Carlson, 2004). First, imaging
studies indicate that along with temporoparietal cortex, PFC is also implicated in thinking
about mental states (e.g., Amodio & Frith, 2006; Frith & Frith, 2003; Sabbagh & Taylor,
2000; Siegal & Varley, 2002), although recent evidence suggests this might be specific to
relatively affective ToM tasks (see Saxe & Baron-Cohen, 2011). Second, individuals with
autism have deficits in both ToM and EF (e.g., McEvoy, Rogers, & Pennington, 1993;
Pellicano, 2010; Russell, 1997; Zelazo, Jacques, Burack, & Frye, 2002), as do children
with traumatic brain injury (Dennis, Agostino, Roncadin, & Levin, 2009). Third, Moses
and Carlson (2004) posited that EF might influence ToM by placing constraints on both its
expression and its emergence. On the one hand, preschool children might have
rudimentary concepts of mind but have difficulty making effective use of these concepts
because—in the absence of sufficiently developed EF—they cannot flexibly direct their
attention to appropriate aspects of the situation (e.g., set aside what they think and put
themselves in someone else’s shoes). On the other hand, a certain level of EF
development may be critical for the very emergence of mental state concepts (Russell,
1996). Children would need to have some ability to disengage attention from salient
stimuli in order to entertain the existence of abstract mental representations. In contrast,
however, Perner and Lang (1999; Perner, Lang, & Kloo, 2002) explained the correlation
between EF and ToM differently; they proposed that ToM (or more specifically a concept
of one’s own intention) must be acquired before children can reflect on and subsequently
monitor and control their own behavior.
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
specificity of the relation to conflict EF tasks (Carlson et al., 2002, 2004). They concluded
that acquiring and expressing mental state concepts requires both holding in mind the
relevant perspectives (WM) and suppressing the irrelevant ones (inhibition)—putative DL-
PFC functions. Frye, Zelazo, and colleagues (e.g., Frye et al., 1995; Frye, Zelazo, &
Burack, 1998) emphasized the (p. 721) RL-PFC function of reflecting upon and selecting
among conflicting perspectives. Furthermore, longitudinal studies have shown that the
relation persists over time and that early individual differences in EF predict later-
developing ToM better than the reverse, suggesting that EF is an important influence on
the emergence of at least some aspects of mental state understanding (Carlson, Mandell,
& Williams, 2004; Hughes, 1998b; Hughes & Ensor, 2005, 2007). Recent research
suggesting much earlier understanding of false belief in infancy (see Baillargeon, Scott, &
He, 2010) would appear to obviate the need for EF as a prerequisite, but more
longitudinal studies like the one by Thoermer, Sodian, Vuori, Perst, and Kristen (2012)
showing continuity of implicit and explicit false belief understanding from infancy to
preschool will help inform this debate. Beyond early childhood, the relation between EF
and ToM remains close and is now a topic of considerable research (e.g., Apperly, Warren,
Andrews, Grant, & Todd, 2011; Best & Miller, 2010).
Socioemotional Outcomes
EF is also implicated in the development of compliance, emotion regulation, and social
competence. Effortful control, defined by Rothbart and Bates (1998) as the ability to
voluntarily inhibit a dominant response to activate a subdominant response, is a
dimension of temperament thought to be closely related to EF (e.g., Carlson & Moses,
2001; Kochanska et al., 1996; Rothbart, 2007). The nature of the relation remains unclear,
but it seems likely that effortful control may capture relatively stable individual
differences in EF and related aspects of personality, such as conscientiousness (e.g.,
Duckworth & Carlson, in press; see the chapter by Shiner & DeYoung in this handbook).
Kochanska and colleagues have conducted several studies examining the sociomoral
correlates of effortful control. Both compliance with social demands and moral
conscience (internalization of rules) were related to individual differences in effortful
control during the preschool years, both concurrently and over time (e.g., Kochanska,
Coy, & Murray, 2001; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000).
Several studies have also demonstrated a relation between high effortful control and low
negative emotionality using parent report of one or both constructs (e.g., Eisenberg,
Fabes, Shepard, et al., 1997; Kochanska, Coy, Tjebkes, & Husarek, 1998), but surprisingly
little research has examined the overlap between EF and control of emotion expression.
In one exception, Carlson and Wang (2007) found a significant correlation using
behavioral measures of both constructs in preschoolers. This finding is consistent with
the suggestion that aspects of EF are used in the service of controlling not only attention
and action but also emotional expression (e.g., LeDoux, 1996; Zelazo & Cunningham,
Page 23 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
2007). Interestingly, however, the study also uncovered a nonlinear relation between
these constructs, in which both low and high levels of effortful control (which may be
associated with anxiety) were deleterious for the regulation of emotion. In a recent
longitudinal study, effortful control measured in middle childhood predicted conduct
problems and depression in early adolescence, but this was mediated by individual
differences in frustration regulation (Zalewski, Lengua, Wilson, Trancik, & Bazinet,
2011). This research suggests that EF and temperament likely make interactive
contributions to socioemotional development.
Long-Term Outcomes
Although very few long-term longitudinal studies have been conducted, the stability and
predictive value of at least one aspect of EF, delay of gratification, is particularly
impressive. Mischel and colleagues found that waiting for a larger reward at age 4 years
positively predicted cognitive and social competence, effective coping skills, and
performance on the Scholastic Aptitude Test (p. 722) in adolescence, independent of
earlier intelligence scores (Mischel, Shoda, & Peake, 1988). Preschool delay of
gratification also predicted more efficient attentional control on a Go/No-Go test at age
18 years (Eigsti et al., 2006), as well as social understanding, goal-setting, and self-
regulatory abilities at age 30 (Ayduk, Mendoza-Denton, Mischel, Downey, Peake, &
Rodriguez, 2000). They also showed better performance on a social (happy faces) version
of Go/No-Go and differentiation of neural correlates of task performance in their mid-40s
(Casey et al., 2011). In contrast, deficiencies in self-control that persist into adulthood
have been associated with criminal behavior, poor physical health, and interpersonal
difficulties, in addition to being a central component of many psychiatric disorders
(Strayhorn, 2002). Indeed, as described earlier, Moffitt and colleagues (2011) found that
self-control between ages 3 and 11 years predicted (as a gradient) physical health,
substance dependence, SES, and the likelihood of a criminal conviction at age 32 years,
Page 24 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
even after controlling for social class of origin and IQ. Together, the evidence suggests
long-term stability of early individual differences in at least some aspects of EF that have
meaningful consequences for a host of real-life outcomes.
Page 25 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Socioeconomic Status
One of the most exciting advances in developmental science is the increasing integration
of neuroscience into research on the development of children’s behavior (see, e.g., Stiles,
2009; Zelazo, Chandler, & Crone, 2010). Many have argued that the protracted postnatal
development of PFC and other relevant regions leaves a substantial window of
opportunity for environmental (e.g., SES) influences on the development of PFC systems
(Kolb, Forgie, Gibb, Gorny, & Rowntree, 1998) and related cognitive, social, and
emotional functions (Hackman, Farah, & Meaney, 2010).
Although the longitudinal evidence described earlier has sometimes been taken to
suggest that individual differences in performance on measures of EF reflect stable
personality traits (e.g., Ego-resiliency—Funder, Block, & Block, 1983; Big Five
Conscientiousness—Moffitt et al., 2011), possibly reflecting genetic variation (Friedman,
Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley, & Hewitt, 2008), it might instead reflect stable
environmental influences. For example, it is now clear that low SES is associated with
deficits in EF, over and above general cognitive deficits (Blair & Razza, 2007; Buckner et
al., 2003; Diamond et al., 2007; Farah et al., 2006, 2008; Masten, 2007; Mezzacappa,
2004; Noble et al., 2005; Obradović, 2010). Noble and colleagues (2005) found that low-
SES children performed relatively poorly on measures of EF, and that parent education
level among low-SES African-American families was related to kindergartners’ EF and
language skills to a greater degree than other major aspects of neurocognitive
development. Masten and colleagues (e.g., Masten, Heistad, Cutuli, Herbers, Obradović,
Chan, Hinz, & Long, 2008; Obradović, 2010) have found that EF in particular appears to
be impaired in homeless and highly mobile children. Additional research suggests that
the relation between EF and social functioning in childhood is moderated by distal
environmental factors, such as neighborhood (e.g., Caspi, Taylor, Moffitt, & Plomin, 2000),
and by associated proximal risk factors such as maternal education, family chaos or
trauma, and child stress (e.g., Ardila, Rosselli, Matute, & Guajardo, 2005; Blair, Granger,
Willoughby et al., 2011; DePrince, Weinzierl, & Combs, 2009; Hughes & Ensor, 2005;
Lengua, 2006; Mezzacappa, 2004). The social policy implications of SES differences in EF
are potentially profound (e.g., Shonkoff, 2011).
Page 26 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Gender
Gender is also related to performance on some measures of EF in children and
adolescents. Differences often favor females (e.g., Bjorklund & Kipp, 1996; Carlson &
Moses, 2001; Isquith, Gioia, & Espy, 2004; Kalkut, Han, Lansing, Holdnack, & Delis, 2009;
Kochanska et al., 1996; Wiebe, Espy, & Charak, 2008), although girls perform more
poorly than boys on measures of reversal learning and on gambling tasks (Crone et al.,
2005; Overman, 2004; Overman et al., 1996). One possibility is that cool EF (abstract,
decontextualized, strong reliance on verbal ability) is better in girls, whereas hot EF
(flexible representation of the (p. 723) reinforcement value of stimuli) is better in boys.
This would be consistent with research suggesting that OFC function develops more
rapidly in males (e.g., Overman et al., 1996), and is under hormonal control (Clark &
Goldman-Rakic, 1989), whereas language is acquired more rapidly in girls than boys.
Indeed, when verbal ability was held constant in one study with several EF tasks, the sex
difference disappeared (Carlson et al., 2004). Interactions among demographic variables
and EF in predicting child outcomes suggest that complex nature–nurture connections
are at play (e.g., see the chapters by Deater-Deckhard, Lickliter, and Moore in this
handbook).
Language
Consistent with a Vygotskian perspective, in which behavior is controlled via language
and symbolic functioning more generally (Vygotsky, 1978), studies have consistently
shown that vocabulary size is related to and predicts EF development (e.g., Bell et al.,
2007; Carlson et al., 2004; Kray, Eber, & Lindenberger, 2004; Wolfe & Bell, 2004).
Following from the insights of Luria (1959, 1961), one reason for this general association
might be the beneficial effects of self-speech or labeling for EF task performance (for a
review, see Cragg & Nation, 2010). For example, Müller, Zelazo, Leone, and Hood (2004)
examined the effect of labeling on a rule-use task in which children were presented with
colored Smarties candies on large cards whose colors mismatched the colors of the
Smarties (e.g., a green Smartie on a yellow card). Children were then asked to turn
around and retrieve a small card that matched the color of the card on which the Smartie
was placed. Whereas 3-year-olds performed poorly by attending only to the color of the
Smartie rather than the color of the large card, 4- to 6-year-olds had no difficulty. When 3-
year-old children were specifically asked to label the color of the card before making a
selection, however, they performed near ceiling. Similarly, labeling the items facilitated
toddlers’ performance on a search task, with the greatest improvement when they had to
generate the label themselves (Miller & Marcovitch, 2011).
Page 27 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
A number of researchers have examined the effects of labeling on the DCCS, and there is
evidence that asking children to label the relevant dimension on test cards improves
performance (Kirkham et al., 2003; Towse, Redbond, Houston-Price, & Cook, 2000; but
see Müller, Zelazo, Lurye, & Liebermann, 2008). In addition, using the Flexible Item
Selection Task, Jacques and Zelazo (2005) reported that asking 4-year-olds to label their
basis for selection 1 (e.g., “Why do those two pictures go together?”) improved their
performance on selection 2 (“Show me two pictures that go together in a different way”).
This was true whether children provided the label themselves or whether the
experimenter generated it for them (see also Deák, 2000). These results suggest that
labeling does not simply change the relative salience of stimuli and redirect children’s
attention to the postswitch dimension, but instead may facilitate reflection on their initial
construal of the stimuli.
In addition to the effects of verbal labeling, a growing body of research has suggested
that bilingualism is associated with better EF. Bialystok (2001) posited that a key aspect
of EF, namely inhibitory control over attentional resources, develops more rapidly in
children with extensive experience shifting between languages. Several studies of
preschool children by Bialystok and colleagues, using a number of conflict EF tasks (e.g.,
the DCCS, Simon Task, Global-Local), have supported this hypothesis (e.g., Bialystok,
1999, 2010; Bialystok & Martin, 2004; Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, 2008). In each case,
heterogeneous samples of bilinguals were better than monolinguals at selectively
attending to a stimulus in the presence of distracting information. Extending these
findings, Carlson and Meltzoff (2008) found that kindergarten children who were native
speakers of both English and Spanish performed significantly better on a battery of EF
tasks than monolingual (English) speakers in traditional schools and those enrolled in
second-language immersion kindergarten. The bilingual advantage was significant for
conflict EF tasks, which specifically call for managing conflicting attentional demands,
whereas there was no advantage on delay EF tasks. Carlson and Choi (2009) reported
parallel results for Korean-English bilinguals ages 4 to 7 years. More recent
investigations have pushed the envelope in terms of the youngest age at which such
findings might emerge, and found evidence for a bilingual advantage on conflict EF tasks
at age 3 years (Bialystok, Barac, Blaye, & Poulin-Dubois, 2010), 24 months (Poulin-
Dubois, Blaye, Coutya, & Bialystok, 2011), and even 7 months, using a visual switch task
requiring inhibitory control (Kovacs & Mehler, 2009). Further research will be needed to
examine early versus late exposure to a second language, whether EF advantages accrue
as a function of proficiency in both languages, and the manner in which initial dual-
language input influences (p. 724) brain development (Vaid, 2002). Additionally, as will be
discussed below, it is important to take cultural background into account. In a noteworthy
exception to the bilingualism findings, Morton and Harper (2007) compared French-
English and English-only children who were well matched on ethnicity and SES and found
no significant differences in EF performance (but see Bialystok, 2009). Such findings
raise questions about the robustness of the bilingual advantage when these other
experience-dependent factors are held constant.
Page 28 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Several studies have demonstrated the power of psychological distancing for enhancing
delay of gratification (Mischel & Rodriguez, 1993; Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989).
For instance, children cued to focus on the “cool” features of rewards (e.g., “If you want
to, when you want to, you can think about how the marshmallows look like white puffy
clouds”) waited twice as long as children cued to dwell on their consummatory, “hot”
features (e.g., “If you want to, when you want to, you can think about how sweet and
chewy the marshmallows taste”) (Mischel & Baker, 1975). In fact, children faced with
actual treats but cued to pretend they were pictures by essentially “putting a frame
around them in your head” waited more than twice as long as when they were shown
actual rewards or pictures of the rewards but asked to imagine that they were real
(Moore, Mischel, & Zeiss, 1976). Similarly, preschoolers given the option to point to
symbols for different quantities of treats in a reverse contingency task (e.g., a mouse to
represent a small amount and an elephant to represent a large amount) were significantly
better at inhibiting an impulsive response and optimizing their long-term rewards than
children presented with the treats themselves (Carlson, et al., 2005). Apperly and Carroll
(2009) replicated this finding and extended it by showing that children were able to
transfer successful performance from the symbolic to the real case.
Even when children are not cued directly to transform the test stimuli, they are more
likely to delay gratification when indirectly primed by being told about a character who
chooses to delay (Kesek, Cunningham, Packer, & Zelazo, 2011), and the effects of indirect
priming are significantly greater than the effects of direct instruction (i.e., “Get the most
rewards that you can”). Young preschoolers are also better at optimizing delayed rewards
when asked what someone else should do than when asked what they will do. In a study
by Prencipe and Zelazo (2005), 3-year-olds typically chose an immediate reward for
Page 29 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
themselves and a delayed reward for the experimenter, suggesting they are capable of
adaptive decision making but have particular difficulty regulating approach behavior in
motivationally significant situations. Psychological distancing in this case may be
accomplished by the adoption of a third-person perspective on one’s own behavior
(Barresi & Moore, 1996).
Culture
At a more macro level of analysis, research has investigated cultural variation in EF and
EF development—variation that may reflect any number of influences, including not only
language and genes (see below) but also cultural values and child-rearing practices. To
date, this research has centered mainly on the differences between Asian and Western
samples. In a large study, Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses, and Lee (2006) found that
preschoolers in Beijing, China, significantly outperformed their American counterparts on
all eight tasks in an EF battery, displaying a 6-month developmental advantage on
average. Similarly, Oh and Lewis (2008) compared Korean and U.K. preschoolers on
several EF measures and reported that only the Korean children were at ceiling on most
tasks. Lan and colleagues (2011) extended these findings in showing that Beijing
preschool children were specifically advanced over U.S. children on inhibitory control
(Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders task) and selective attention (Woodcock-Johnson Pair
Cancelation task), but not on WM (a verbal span task). A slightly different pattern was
found in a comparison of Korean and American children, in which Koreans did
significantly better on selective attention and a forward memory span task, but worse
than the American children on Delay of Gratification (Carlson & Choi, 2009).
Interestingly, in this same study, Korean-American bilinguals, who also outperformed
Page 30 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Although cultural groups differ in many ways and it is difficult to determine the causes of
observed group differences, it is noteworthy that Asian cultures tend to place a relatively
strong emphasis on self-control during the primary school years. Observational studies
indicate intensive training on skills such as following directions and concentration in
Chinese classrooms (Lan, Ponitz, Miller et al., 2009). Similarly, Korean teachers enforce
discipline (Kwon, 2004), whereas in American classrooms, teachers tend to place high
value on freedom of self-expression (Chen, Hastings, Rubin et al., 1998). It remains
unclear why there might be worse performance among Asian children on delay of
gratification EF tasks, although we speculate that these measures are more heavily
influenced by political and economic resource histories (e.g., trust, scarcity) than are the
cool EF measures. Alongside these experiential differences in the shaping of EF, however,
genetic variation within and across cultural groups must be considered, a topic to which
we return below.
Page 31 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Caregiving
At a more proximal level, parent–child relations arguably constitute the most intense and
enduring relationships of early childhood, thereby representing the very core of the
young child’s environment, especially in the first few years of life. A recent body of
research is beginning to suggest that the quality of parent–child interactions is related to
children’s EF. Three specific dimensions of parental interactive behavior have been
proposed to promote the development of child EF (Carlson, 2003): sensitivity, autonomy-
support, and mind-mindedness. Sensitivity, which consists of appropriate and consistent
responses to children’s signals, provides children with information that supports
expectations of the world as orderly and predictable; this might enhance children’s
emerging confidence in their self-regulatory capacities. Scaffolding, or offering children
age-appropriate support during problem solving, is likely to yield more problem-solving
opportunities and successes, potentially affecting motivation and mastery, in addition to
learning. Finally, mind-mindedness, or parents’ tendency to comment appropriately on
their children’s mental states while interacting with them, might offer children verbal
tools that will facilitate reflection and awareness of their own response tendencies,
thereby promoting the top-down control that is central to EF (Zelazo, 2004). In support of
this proposal, Bernier, Carlson, and Whipple (2010) reported that maternal sensitivity,
mind-mindedness, and autonomy-support (or scaffolding), assessed between 12 and 15
months, were related to child EF performance (specifically conflict EF) at 18 months and
2 years. When pitted against one another in a multiple regression, autonomy-support and
mind-mindedness explained unique variance in later child EF.
Indeed, the few studies that have examined this question thus far have mainly focused on
autonomy-support or scaffolding. Bibok, Carpendale, and Müller (2009) found that
parental verbal scaffolding during a puzzle task (i.e., utterances that elaborated on the
child’s course of action) was related to the child’s concurrent EF performance at age 2
years. Hughes and Ensor (2009) provided further support for this link, reporting that
maternal verbal scaffolding of the child’s activity during play at 2 years predicted
children’s EF at 4 years of age, above and beyond EF at 2 years.
Page 32 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
A key notion in the caregiving literature is that regulatory processes are first practiced in
the context of attachment relationships (Sroufe, 1996), and that the strategies learned
are then generalized and used outside of the dyadic relationship, such as during tasks
requiring independent self-regulation (i.e., EF; Calkins et al., 2004; Cole et al., 2004).
Furthermore, parenting has been shown to relate to the development of children’s stress-
response systems, which influence the development of frontal brain regions (see the
chapter by Gunnar & Herrera in this handbook; Gunnar et al., 2006; Hane & Fox, 2006).
Hence, secure attachment relationships may favor the healthy development of EF by
facilitating the development of the neural systems that underlie it (Glaser, 2000).
Genetics
Sophisticated models of genetic influences on behavior are making a mark in
developmental psychology, and the area of EF is no exception. For example, using a
traditional behavioral genetics approach, Friedman and colleagues (2007) concluded that
there is a highly heritable common factor as well as additional genetic influences that are
unique to particular components of EF (inhibiting dominant responses, updating WM
representations, and shifting between task sets). Importantly, EF heritability does not
fully overlap with that of IQ or perceptual speed in this high-risk sample. In other work,
Page 33 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
only moderate heritability has been reported for certain components of EF, such as
attention and working memory (Polderman et al., 2007).
Given the challenges associated with interpreting behavioral genetics findings, however,
direct examination of genetic polymorphisms may offer a more straightforward form of
evidence of genetic influences on EF (see the chapters by Lickliter and by Moore in this
handbook). Indeed, growing evidence suggests that genetic polymorphisms implicated in
dopaminergic functioning may play a role in children’s development of attention
problems. In particular, the 7-repeat variant of a variable-nucleotide tandem repeat
(VNTR) polymorphism in the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene has been linked with
higher levels of attention problems, across samples (see Faraone et al., 2005; Gizer, Ficks,
& Waldman, 2009; Thapar, Langley, Asherson, & Gill, 2007) and at multiple points in
development (El-Faddagh, Laught, Maras, Vöhringer, & Schmidt, 2004; Schmidt, Fox,
Perez-Edgar, Hu, & Hamer, 2001).
Page 34 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
negative) of genetic effects on EF may vary across high- and low-quality environmental
experiences.
Sleep
There is mounting evidence for a relation between sleep and the subsequent development
of EF. Bernier and colleagues reported that infant sleep predicted later EF in
toddlerhood, even when SES, cognitive ability, and prior executive ability were controlled
(Bernier, Carlson, Bordeleau, & Carrier, 2010). (p. 728) Infants between 12 and 18
months of age for whom a greater proportion of sleep occurred at night had better EF at
26 months and again at 4 years, particularly in the domain of impulse control (Bernier et
al., in press). This relation was unique to EF, not general cognitive ability. Later in
development, childhood sleep is also a predictor of EF during adolescence, with the
trajectory of parent-reported sleep problems between 4 and 16 years predicting EF in
late adolescence (Friedman, Corley, Hewitt, & Wright, 2009). Specifically, decreasing
sleep problems during childhood, rather than whether they were present initially,
predicted better performance on inhibition and updating tasks at age 17. These
longitudinal findings are consistent with a growing literature documenting the concurrent
association between EF and individual differences in the sleep of healthy infants and
children (e.g., Gómez, Bootzin & Nadel, 2006; Steenari et al., 2003). Neurocognitive
testing of individuals with sleep-disordered breathing, which is abnormal nocturnal
respiratory functioning that includes obstructive sleep apnea, provides additional
evidence for a relation between EF and sleep (see Beebe, 2006, for review). Longitudinal
work highlights changes in key sleep variables throughout development; while more sleep
is generally considered positive with older individuals, the amount of time infants spend
sleeping decreases between the first and second year of life (McLaughlin, Crabtree, &
Williams, 2009). Instead, the proportion of infant nighttime (relative to daytime) sleep
was related to EF, not overall duration or interruptions (Bernier et al., 2010).
Understanding the mechanisms that account for the influence of sleep on subsequent EF
is an exciting direction for future research.
A related line of research has revealed that EF may be particularly sensitive to disruption
by perturbations in circadian rhythms of arousal. Physiological arousal, measured using a
variety of biological and behavioral indices (e.g., core body temperature and sleep–wake
cycles), rises and falls according to a regular circadian rhythm, and there are both
individual and developmental differences in the nature of these rhythms with some
individuals being morning types and others being evening types. A study by Hahn, Cowell,
Wiprzycka, Goldstein, Ralph, Hasher, and Zelazo (2012) found that children between the
ages of 11 and 14 years who were tested in the morning or afternoon performed better on
measures of EF when tested at their optimal versus nonoptimal times of day, even when
controlling for sleep duration on the previous night (see also Goldstein et al., 2007).
Participants tested at their optimal time performed better on a composite measure of EF,
Page 35 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
as well as on individual measures of affective decision making (the Iowa Gambling Task)
and working memory. Understanding the possible role of circadian synchrony on EF
during the transition to adolescence may provide insight into individual differences in
educational achievement and the possible onset of behavioral problems.
Motor Development
Evidence of a predictive relation between early motor development and later EF has been
provided by Murray and colleagues (2006), who followed a large cohort of children
prospectively from the time their mothers were pregnant. These authors selected groups
based on the month that the motor milestone of unsupported standing was achieved.
They found a significant predictive relation between the age of standing and EF
performance at 33 to 35 years of age even when controlling for gender, SES, and
maternal education. Adult EF was measured using a categorization task with and without
WM demand.
The relation between motor ability and cognition, including EF, may be due to common
brain circuitry (see Diamond, 2000; Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Voelker, 2011).
Prospective findings are consistent with other work showing that concurrent motor
development and EF are correlated in typically developing children. A large investigation
of Dutch kindergartners revealed a specific relation between motor measures and EF, but
not general cognitive ability, when attention was controlled (Wassenberg et al., 2005). An
Australian sample of the same age range exhibited relations between fine motor and ball
skills in the motor domain and conflict control on the Day/Night Stroop task and
externalizing behavior rated by teachers on the Rowe behavior rating scale (Livesey et
al., 2006). In sum, there is evidence that motor development and EF are related, with
early motor milestones predicting adult EF as well as relations between individual
differences of young children in the motor and executive domains. However, the literature
is variable in terms of the specific motor and executive tasks that have been linked.
Page 36 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Training studies are also useful for examining possible mechanisms of development via
experimental manipulation (see Diamond & Lee, 2011, for a summary of EF
interventions). These can target short-term change by (a) training one EF component and
then observing any immediate changes in other components, (b) providing more general
learning experiences, such as metacognitive instruction, and observing any facilitation of
EF, or (c) providing training in a related domain, such as ToM, and observing effects on
EF. The first approach is illustrated in an investigation of the transfer of training effects
associated with one of four conditions: visual WM training, inhibition training, an active
control condition with commercial videogames, and a passive control condition (Thorell,
Lindqvist, Nutley, Bohlin, & Klingberg, 2009). The computer-based training was provided
in the context of preschool programming for about 15 minutes a day over 5 weeks. The
preschoolers who received working memory training exhibited significant improvements
in untrained verbal and visual WM and attention tasks, while the inhibition training group
did not improve in untrained tasks in these domains. Neither training group improved on
measures of inhibition, reaction time, or visual problem solving. An example of the second
approach is the measurement of the effects of a broad social and emotional learning
curriculum that teaches cognitive and language strategies to enhance self-control and EF.
Specifically, the Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies (PATHS) Curriculum (Kusché
& Greenberg, 1994) promotes inhibition and increases verbal fluency (Riggs, Greenberg,
Kusché, & Pentz, 2006). Of note, the PATHS curriculum is delivered by teachers in
elementary classrooms, thus translating knowledge of neurocognitive development
directly to the classroom. Similarly, the Tools of the Mind curriculum (Bodrova & Leong,
2007) includes activities to enhance self-talk and model the use of external aids to
memory and attention to preschoolers. Low-income preschoolers who received Tools of
the Mind improved on two measures of EF, the Dots and Flanker tasks (Diamond, Barnett,
Page 37 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Thomas, & Munro, 2007). Interestingly, children who attended a Montessori school have
been shown to perform better on tasks of EF than children assigned by lottery to non-
Montessori schools (Lillard, in press; Lillard & Else-Quest, 2006). As with Tools of the
Mind, the Montessori approach is multifaceted. Characteristics of Montessori schools
include multiage classrooms, student-chosen learning activities carried out with minimal
instruction from teachers, and long periods of time designated for uninterrupted pursuit
of these activities. An example of the third approach is training of preschool children on a
ToM skill (i.e., representing false beliefs) and an EF skill (i.e., the DCCS task) (Kloo &
Perner, 2003). Training on each task improved performance on the other, suggesting that
both of these skills may rely on similar, shared mechanisms.
Page 38 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
to help mitigate or even reverse the course of EF deficits and delays in vulnerable, at-risk
populations. More basic psychometric and longitudinal work is needed, as well as more
follow-up to assess the viability of EF interventions over time, in both clinical and
educational environments.
Acknowledgment
The preparation of this chapter was supported in part by NIH R01 HD51495 to SMC.
References
Ackerly, S. S., & Benton, A. L. (1948). Report of a case of bilateral frontal lobe defect.
Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 27, 479–504.
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Kirkwood, H., & Elliott, J. (2009). The cognitive and
behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80,
606–621.
Amodio, D., & Frith, C. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social
cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268–277.
Anderson, P., Anderson, V., & Lajoie, G. (1996). The Tower of London Test: Validation and
standardization for pediatric populations. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(1), 54–65.
Page 39 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Long-term sequelae of
prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology,
18, 281–296.
Apperly, I. A., & Carroll, D. J. (2009). How do symbols affect 3- to 4-year-olds’ executive
function? Evidence from a reverse-contingency task. Developmental Science, 12, 1070–
1082.
Apperly, I. A., Warren, F., Andrews, B. J., Grant, J., & Todd, S. (2011). Developmental
continuity in theory of mind: Speed and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in children
and adults. Child Development, 82, 1691–1703.
Archibald, M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific
language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
41, 675–693.
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents’
educational level on the development of executive functions. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 28, 539–560.
Arts, B., Jabben, N., Krabbendam, L., & van Os, J. (2008). Meta-analyses of cognitive
functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychological
Medicine, 38, 771–785.
Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, P. K., & Rodriguez, M.
(2000). Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with
rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 776–792.
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118.
Badre, D., D’Esposito, M. (2007). FMRI evidence for a hierarchical organization of the
prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(12), 1–18.
Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–122.
Barker, E. D., Seguin, J. R., White, H. R., Bates, M. E., Lacourse, E., Carbonneau, R., &
Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Developmental trajectories of male physical violence and theft.
Relations to neurocognitive performance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 592–599.
Page 40 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive
functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 541–556.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future
consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation of working
memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 18(1), 428–437.
Beck, D. M., Schaefer, C., Pang, K., & Carlson, S. M. (2011). Executive function in
preschool children: Test-retest reliability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 169–
193.
Bell, M. A., Wolfe, C. D., & Adkins, D. R. (2007). Frontal lobe development during infancy
and childhood. In D. J. Coch, K. W. Fischer, & G. Dawson (Eds.), Human behavior learning,
and the developing brain: Typical development (pp. 247–276). New York: Guilford.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to
environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885–908.
Bernier, A., Beauchamp, M., Bouvette-Turcot, A., Carlson, S. M., & Carrier, J. (in press).
Sleep and cognition in the preschool years: Specific links to executive functioning. Child
Development.
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Bordeleau, S., & Carrier, J. (2010). Relations between
physiological and cognitive regulatory systems: Infant sleep regulation and subsequent
executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 1739–1752.
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. (2011). Social precursors of
preschoolers’ executive functioning: A closer look at the early caregiving environment.
Developmental Science, 15, 12–24.
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-
regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child
Development, 81, 326–339.
Berry, D., Deater-Deckard, K., McCartney, K., Wang, Z., & Petrill, S. A. (in press). Gene–
environment interaction between DRD4 7-repeat VNTR and early maternal sensitivity
Page 41 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Beveridge, M., Jarrold, C., & Pettit, E. (2002). An experimental approach to executive
fingerprinting in young children. Infant and Child Development, 11, 107–123.
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind.
Child Development, 70, 636–644.
Bialystok, E. (2009). Claiming evidence from non-evidence: A reply to Morton and Harper.
Developmental Science, 12, 499–501.
Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010). Word mapping and
executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition
and Development, 11, 485–508.
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children:
Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325–
339.
Bibok, M. B., Carpendale, J. I., & Müller, U. (2009). Parental scaffolding and the
development of executive function. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development,
123, 17–34.
Bjorklund, D. F., & Kipp, K. (1996). Parental investment theory and gender differences in
the evolution of inhibition mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 163–188.
Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T.,
Kivlighan, K. T., Fortunato, C. K., & the Family Life Project Investigators (2011). Salivary
cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in childhood.
Child Development, 82, 1970–1984.
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false
belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child
Development, 78, 647–680.
Page 42 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the mind: The Vygotskian approach to early
childhood education (2nd ed.). New York: Merrill/Prentice Hall.
Booth, J. N., Boyle, J. M. E., & Kelly, S. W. (2010). Do tasks make a difference? Accounting
for heterogeneity of performance of children with reading difficulties on tasks of
executive function: Findings from a meta-analysis. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 28(1), 133–176.
Brace, J. J., Morton, J. B., & Munakata, Y. (2006). When actions speak louder than words:
Improving children’s flexibility in a card-sorting task. Psychological Science, 17, 665–669.
Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A
dimensional and developmental study. Developmental and Neuropsychology, 26, 571–593.
Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the
toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664–674.
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002).
Development of frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from
fMRI. Neuron, 33, 301–311.
Bunge, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). A brain-based account of the development of rule use in
childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118–121.
Buss, A., & Spencer, J. P. (2008, July). The emergence of rule-use: A dynamic neural field
model of the DCCS. Paper presented at the 30 th Annual Conference of the Cognitive
Science Society, Washington, DC. Retrieved August 16 th, 2012, from http://
www.cogsci.rpi.edu/csjarchive/proceedings/2008/pdfs/p463.pdf
Page 43 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Byrd, D. L., van der Veen, T. K., McNamara, J. P. H., & Berg, K. (2004). Preschoolers don’t
practice what they preach: Preschoolers’ planning performances with manual and spoken
response requirements. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 427–449.
Calkins, S. D., Dedmon, S., & Hungerford, A. (2004). Mothers’ interactions with
temperamentally frustrated infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 219–239.
Carlson, S. M., & Beck, D. M. (2009). Symbols as tools in the development of executive
function. In A. Winsler, C. Fernyhough, & I. Montero (Eds.), Private speech, executive
functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp.163–175). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Carlson, S. M., & Choi, H. P. (2009, April). Bilingual and bicultural: Executive function in
Korean and American children. In K. G. Millett (Chair), Cognitive Effects of Bilingualism:
A Look at Executive Function and Theory of Mind. Paper symposium presented at the
biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, CO.
Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more: Executive function and
symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16, 609–616.
Carlson, S. M., Faja, S., & Beck, D. M. (in press). Incorporating early development into
measurement approaches: The need for a continuum of measures across development. In
J. A. Griffin, L. S. Freund, & P. McCardle (Eds.), Executive function in preschool age
children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind:
Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122.
Page 44 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in
young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282–298.
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and
children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between
executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working
memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73–92.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive
functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299–319.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Hix, H. R. (1998). The role of inhibitory processes in young
children’s difficulties with deception and false belief. Child Development, 69, 672–691.
Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool
children. Cognitive Development, 22, 489–510.
Carlson, S. M., & White, R. E. (in press). Executive function, pretend play, and
imagination. In M. Taylor (Ed.), Handbook of imagination. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2008). Symbolic thought. In M. M. Haith & J. B. Benson
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development (Vol. 3, pp. 288–297).
London: Elsevier.
Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). The value of control and the influence of values.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(41), 16861–16862.
Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of cognitive behavior. Brain
and Cognition, 20, 51–73.
Casey, B. J., Cohen, J. D., Jezzard, P., Turner, R., Noll, D. C., Trainor, R. J., et al. (1995).
Activation of prefrontal cortex in children during a nonspatial working memory task with
functional MRI. NeuroImage, 2, 221–229.
Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain
development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54, 241–
257.
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., Levita, L., Libby, V., Pattwell, S., Ruberry, E., Soliman, F., &
Somerville, L. H. (2010). The storm and stress of adolescence: Insights from human
imaging and mouse genetics. Developmental Psychobiology, 2, 225–235.
Page 45 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Casey, B. J., Thomas, K. M., Welsh, T. F., Badgaiyan, R. D., Eccard, C. H., Jennings, J. R., &
Crone, E. A. (2000). Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and
expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences USA, 97(15), 8728–8733.
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., & Fossella, J. (2002). Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic
approaches toward a model of cognitive control. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 237–
254.
Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2000). Neighborhood deprivation affects
children’s mental health: Environmental risks identified in a genetic design. Psychological
Science, 11, 338–342
Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. M. (2001). Changes in executive
control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental
Psychology, 37, 715–730. (p. 733)
Chelune, G. J., & Baer, R. A. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 8, 219–228.
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-
rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-
cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677–686.
Christoff, K., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000) The frontopolar cortex and human cognition:
Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex.
Psychobiology, 28(2), 168–186.
Clark, A. S., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Gonadal hormones influence the emergence
of cortical function in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 1287–1295.
Clark, C. A., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning
abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–
1191.
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994). The development of emotion regulation
and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), The development of emotion
regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 59, 73–100.
Cole, P., Martin, S., & Dennis, T. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct:
Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child
Development, 75, 317–333.
Page 46 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Corbett, B. A., Constantine, L. J., Hendren, R., Rocke, D., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Examining
executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 30, 210–222.
Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Saults, J. S., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismajatullina, A., &
Conway, A. R. A. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in
working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 42–100.
Cragg, L., & Nation, K., (2010). Language and the development of cognitive control.
Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 631–642.
Crone, E. A., Bunge, S. A., Latenstein, H., & van der Molen, M. W. (2005).
Characterization of children’s decision making: Sensitivity to punishment frequency, not
task complexity. Child Neuropsychology, 11, 245–263.
Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Donohue, S. E., & Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neural evidence for
dissociable components of task switching. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 475–486.
Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive
neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 97–104.
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). The
return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science,
264, 1102–1105.
Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., Boyle, C. A., Waller, J. L., Miller, P. H., Naglieri, J., et al.
(2007). Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children’s cognitive functioning: A
randomized controlled trial. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 78, 510–519.
DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children.
Science, 238, 1556–1557.
DeLoache, J. S., Sugarman, S., & Brown, A. L. (1985). The development of error
correction strategies in young children’s manipulative play. Child Development, 56, 928–
993.
Dempster, F. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified
theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45–75.
Page 47 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Dennis, M., Agostino, A., Roncadin, C., & Levin, H. (2009). Theory of mind depends on
domain-general executive functions of working memory and cognitive inhibition in
children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 31, 835–847.
DePrince, A. P., Weinzierl, K. M., & Combs, M. D. (2009). Executive function performance
and trauma exposure in a community sample of children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 353–
361.
Dewey, J. (1931/1985). Context and thought. In J. A. Boydston & A. Sharpe (Eds.), John
Dewey: The later works, 1925–1953 (Vol. 6, pp. 3–21). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois
University Press.
Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated
by infants’ performance on AB. Child Development, 56, 868–883.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007) Preschool program improves
cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388.
Diamond, A., Carlson, S. M., & Beck, D. M. (2005). Preschool children’s performance in
task switching on the dimensional change card sort task: Separating the dimensions aids
the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 689–729.
Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of
frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271–
294.
Diamond, A., & Gilbert, J. (1989). Development as progressive inhibitory control of action:
Retrieval of a contiguous object. Cognitive Development, 4, 223–249. (p. 734)
Page 48 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Diamond, A., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2005). Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: Parallels
between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16, 291–297.
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function
development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964.
Diamond, A., Prevor, M., Callender, G., & Druin, D. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cognitive
deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 62, 1–208.
Dodge, K., & Garber, J. (1991). Domains of emotion regulation. In J. Garber & K. Dodge
(Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 3–12). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Dowsett, S. M., & Livesey, D. J. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool
children: Effects of “executive skills” training. Developmental Psychology, 36, 161–174.
Drewe, E. A. (1975). Go-no go learning after frontal lobe lesions in humans. Cortex, 11, 8–
16.
Duckworth, A. L., & Carlson, S. M. (in press). Self-regulation and school success. To
appear in B. W. Sokol, F. M. E. Grouzet, & U. Mueller (Eds.), Self-regulation and
Autonomy: Exploring the Social, Developmental, Educational, and Neurological
Dimensions of Human Conduct. New York: Cambridge.
Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey,
B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental
Science, 9, 1–20.
Durston, S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., Buitelaar, J. K., & Van Engeland,
H. (2001). Anatomical MRI of the developing human brain: what have we learned? Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1012–1020.
Eddy, C. M., Rizzo, R., & Cavanna, A. E. (2009). Neuropsychological aspects of Tourette
syndrome: A review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 67, 503–513.
Eigsti, I. M., Zayas, V., Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., Ayduk, O., Dadlani, M., et al. (2006).
Predicting cognitive control from preschool to late adolescence and young adulthood.
Psychological Science, 17, 478–484.
Page 49 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Murphy, B. C., Guthrie, I. K., Jones, S., et al.
(1997). Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of children’s social functioning
from regulation and emotionality. Child Development, 68, 642–664.
El-Faddagh, M., Laucht, M., Maras, A., et al. (2004) Association of dopamine D4 receptor
(DRD4) gene with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a high-risk
community sample: a longitudinal study from birth to 11 years of age. Journal of Neural
Transmission, 111, 883– 889.
Engle, R. F. (2002). New frontiers for ARCH models . Journal of Applied Econometrics, 17,
425–446.
Eslinger, P. J., Flaherty-Craig, C. V., & Benton, A. L. (2004). Developmental outcomes after
early prefrontal cortex damage. Brain and Cognition, 55, 84–103.
Espy, K. (1997). The shape school: Assessing executive function in preschool children.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 495–499.
Espy, K. A., & Bull, R. (2005). Inhibitory processes in young children and individual
variation in short-term memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 669–688.
Farah, M. J., Betancourt, L., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Giannetta, J. M., Nancy L.,
Brodsky, N. L., Elsa, K., Malmud, E. K., & Hurt, H. (2008). Environmental stimulation,
parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Developmental Science, 15,
793–801.
Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L.,
Malmud, E. K., & Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: Specific associations with
neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110, 166–174.
Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J., Holmgren, M. A., et
al. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological
Psychiatry, 57, 1313–1323.
Friedman, N. P., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., & Wright, K. P. (2009). Individual differences in
childhood sleep problems predict later cognitive executive control. Sleep, 32, 323–333.
Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., &
Hewitt, J. K. (2007). Greater attention problems during childhood predict poorer
executive functioning in late adolescence. Psychological Science, 18(10), 893–900.
Page 50 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K.
(2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201–225.
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (1998). I. Cognitive complexity and control:
Implications for theory of mind in typical and atypical development. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 7, 116–121.
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning.
Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527.
Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of gratification: Some longitudinal
personality correlates. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44, 1198–1213.
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A
review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 31–60.
Gaskins, I. W., & Pressley, M. (2007). Teaching metacognitive strategies that address
executive function processes within a schoolwide curriculum. In L. Meltzer (Ed.),
Executive function in education: From theory to practice (pp. 261–286). New York:
Guilford Press.
Gerstadt, C., Hong, Y., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and
action: performance on children 3½–7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition,
53, 129–153.
Giedd, J. N., Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Casey, B. J., Kozuch, P. L., Viatuzis, A.
C., Vauss, Y. C., Hamburger, S. D., Kaysen, D., & Rapoport, J. L. (1996). Quantitative
magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cerebral Cortex, 6,
551–560.
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of
executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 235–238.
Page 51 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Gizer, I. R., Ficks, C., & Waldman, I. D. (2009). Candidate gene studies of ADHD: A
metaanalytic review. Human Genetics, 126, 51–90.
Gladstone, R. (1969). Age, cognitive control and extinction. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 7, 31–35.
Glaser, D. (2000). Child abuse and neglect and the brain—A review. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 97–116.
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., Nugent
III, T. F., Herman, D. H., Clasen, L. S., Toga, A. W., Rapoport, J. L., & Thompson, P. M.
(2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early
adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 8174–8179.
Goldberg, E., & R. Bilder, R. (1987). Frontal lobes and hierarchic organization of
neurocognitive control. In E. Perecman (Ed.), Frontal lobes revisited (pp. 159–187). New
York: IRBN.
Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M., Astor, B. C., Denckla, M.
B., & Landa, R. J. (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and
children with ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 279–293.
Goldstein, D., Hahn, C., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Time of day,
intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type
adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 431–
440.
Gómez, R. L., Bootzin, R. R., & Nadel, L. (2006). Naps promote abstraction in language-
learning infants. Psychological Science, 17, 670–674.
Gunnar, M. R., Fisher, P. A., and the Early Experience, Stress and Prevention Science
Network (2006). Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to
bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Development and
Psychopathology, 18, 651–677.
Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain:
Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11,
651–659.
Page 52 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Hahn, C., Cowell, J. M., Wiprzycka, U. J., Goldstein, D., Ralph, M., Hasher, L., & Zelazo, P.
D. (2012). Circadian rhythms in executive function during the transition to adolescence:
The effect of synchrony between chronotype and time of day. Developmental Science, 15,
408–416.
Hala, S., Hug, S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Executive functioning and false belief
understanding in preschool children: Two tasks are harder than one. Journal of Cognition
and Development, 4, 275–298.
Hane, A. A., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving of human
infants influence stress reactivity. Psychological Science, 17, 550–556.
Happaney, K., Zelazo, P. D., & Stuss, D. T. (2004). Development of orbitofrontal function:
Current themes and future directions. Brain and Cognition, 55, 1–10.
Henning, A., Spinath, F. M., & Aschersleben, G. (2011). The link between preschoolers’
executive function and theory of mind and the role of epistemic states. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 513–531.
Hughes, C. (1998a). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and
verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.
Hughes, C. (1998b). Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers’ strategic behavior predict
later understanding of mind? Developmental Psychology, 34, 1326–1339.
Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind
and emotion and executive function among “hard to manage” preschoolers. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 981–994.
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2-year-olds: A
family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 645–668.
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive
relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447–1459.
Page 53 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2009). How do families help or hinder the development of
executive function? New Directions in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: Special Issue on
social interaction and the development of executive function, 123, 35–50.
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2011). Individual differences in growth in executive function
across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-
perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
108, 663–676.
Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems
and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131–142.
Hughes, C., White, A., Sharpen, J., & Dunn, J. (2000). Antisocial, angry, and
unsympathetic: “Hard-to-manage” preschoolers’ (p. 736) peer problems and possible
cognitive influences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 169–179.
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C., & van der Molen, M. (2006). Age-related change in executive
function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44,
2017–2036.
Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., & Itakura, S. (in press). East-West differences in context-
sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science.
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children
with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77,
1822–1841.
Isquith, P. K., Crawford, J. S., Espy, K. A., & Goia, G. A. (2005). Assessment of executive
functioning in preschool-aged children. Mental Retardation and Developmental
Disabilities Research Reviews, 11, 209–215.
Isquith, P. K., Gioia, G. A., & Espy, K. A. (2004). Executive function in preschool children:
Examination through everyday behavior. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 403–422.
Page 54 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Jacobson, L. A., & Pianta, R. C. (2007, April). Executive function skills and children’s
academic and social adjustment to sixth grade. Poster presented at the biennial meeting
of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston.
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Language and the development of cognitive flexibility:
Implications for theory of mind. In J. W. Astington and J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language
matters for theory of mind (pp. 144–162). New York: Oxford University Press.
Jacques, S., Zelazo, P. D., Kirkham, N. Z., & Semcesen, T. K. (1999). Rule selection and
rule execution in preschoolers: An error-detection approach. Developmental Psychology,
35, 770–780.
Johnson, M. H., & Munakata, Y. (2005). Processes of change in brain and cognitive
development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(3), 152–158.
Jordan, P. L., & Morton, J. B. (2012). Perseveration and the status of 3-year-olds’
knowledge in a card-sorting task: Evidence from studies involving congruent flankers.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 52–64.
Joseph, M. F., Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., & Soares, J. C. (2008). A quantitative and
qualitative review of neurocognitive performance in pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of
Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 18, 595–605.
Kalkut, E. L., Han, S. D., Lansing, A. E., Holdnack, J. A., & Delis, D. C. (2009).
Development of set-shifting ability from late childhood through early adulthood. Archives
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 565–574.
Karpov, B. A., Luria, A. R., & Yarbus, A. L. (1968). Disturbances of the structure of active
perception n lesions of the posterior and anterior regions of the brain. Neuropsychologia,
6, 157–166.
Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Anthony, L. G., & Wallace, G. L. (2008). Understanding
executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world.
Neuropsychological Review, 18(4), 320–338.
Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The Children’s
Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148–157.
Kesek, A., Cunningham, W. A., Packer, D. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Indirect goal priming is
more powerful than explicit instruction in children. Developmental Science, 14, 944–948.
Page 55 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L. M., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their
knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6,
449–467.
Klahr, D., & Robinson, M. (1981). Formal assessment of problem-solving and planning
processes in preschool children. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 113–148.
Klingberg, T., Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Moseley, M. E., & Hedehus, M. (1999).
Myelination and organization of the frontal white matter in children: a diffusion tensor
MRI study. Neuroreport, 10, 2817–2821.
Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2003). Training transfer between card sorting and false belief
understanding: helping children apply conflicting descriptions. Child Development, 74,
1823–1839.
Kochanska, G., & Askan, N. (1995). Mother-child mutually positive affect, the quality of
child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal control as correlates of early
internalization. Child Development, 66, 236–254.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in
the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1091–1111.
Kochanska, G. Coy, K. C., Tjebkes, T. L., & Husarek, S. J. (1998). Individual differences in
emotionality in infancy. Child Development, 69, 375–390.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood:
Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development.
Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.
Kochanska, G., Padavich, D. L., & Koenig, A. L. (1996). Children’s narratives about
hypothetical moral dilemmas and objective measures of their conscience: Mutual
relations and socialization antecedents. Child Development, 67, 1420–1436.
Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., & Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the
anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 399, 148–151.
Kolb, B., Forgie, M., Gibb, R., Gorny, G., & Rowntree, S. (1998). Age, experience, and the
changing brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 143–159. (p. 737)
Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 6556–6560.
Kray, J., Eber, E., & Lindenberger, U. (2004). Age differences in executive functioning
across the lifespan: The role of verbalization in task preparation. Acta Psychologica, 115,
143–165.
Page 56 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Krikorian, R., & Bartok, J. (1998). Developmental data for the Porteus Maze Test. Child
Neuropsychologist, 12, 305–310.
Kusché, C. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1994). The PATHS (Promoting Alternative Thinking
Strategies) Curriculum. South Deerfield, MA: Channing-Bete.
Kwon, J. (2004). Expressing refusals in Korean and American English. Multilingua, 23,
339–364.
Lahat, A., Todd, R. M., Mahy, C. E. V., Lau, K., & Zelazo, P. D. (2010). Neurophysiological
correlates of executive function: A comparison of European-Canadian and Chinese-
Canadian 5-year-old children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 72.
Lambek, R., Tannock, R., Dalsgaard, S., Trillingsgaard, A., Damm, D., & Thomsen, P. H.
(2010). Executive dysfunction in school-age children with ADHD. Journal of Attention
Disorders, 15, 646–655.
Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links
between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-
cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 108, 677–692.
Lan, X., Ponitz, C., Miller, K., Li, S., & Cortina, K. (2009). Keeping their attention:
Classroom practices associated with behavioural engagement in first grade mathematics
classes in China and the United States. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 198–211.
Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive
functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–
80.
Levin, H. S., Culhane, K. A., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., Mattson, A. J., Harward, H., et
al. (1991). Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal-lobe
functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(3), 377–395.
Leyfer, O., Woodruff-Borden, J., Klein-Tasman, B. P., Fricke, J., & Mervis, C. B. (2006).
Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in 4- to 16-year-olds with Williams syndrome.
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 141B, 615–622.
Lijffijt, M., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2005). A meta-analytic
review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Deficient
inhibitory motor control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 216–222.
Page 57 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Lillard, A. (in press). Academic year change in classic vs. supplemented Montessori vs.
conventional preschool programs. Journal of School Psychology.
Lillard, A., & Else-Quest, N. (2006). Evaluating Montessori education. Science, 313(5795),
1893–1894.
Livesey, D., Keen, J., Rouse, J., & White, F. (2006). The relation between measures of
executive function, motor performance and externalising behaviour in 5- and 6-year-old
children. Human Movement Science, 25, 50–64.
Logie, R. H., & Pearson, D. G. (1997).The inner eye and the inner scribe of visuo-spatial
working memory: Evidence from developmental fractionation. European Journal of
Cognitive Psychology, 9, 241–257.
Loo, S. K., Humphrey, L. A., Terttu, T., Moilanen, I. K., McGough, J. J., McCracken, J. T., et
al. (2007). Executive functioning among Finnish adolescents with attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 46, 1594–1604.
Lukowski, A. F., Koss, M., Burden, M. J., Jonides, J., Nelson, C. A., Kaciroti, N., Jimenez, E.,
& Lozoff, B. (2010). Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years:
evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutritional
Neuroscience, 13, 54–70.
Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P., Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J.,
Keshavan, M. S., Genovese, C. R., Eddy, W. F., & Sweeney, J. A. (2001). Maturation of
widely distributed brain function subserves cognitive development. Neuroimage, 13, 786–
793.
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior
(J. Tizard, Ed.). New York: Liveright Publishing Corporation.
Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.
(Original work published in 1962).
Luton, L. M., Burns, T. G., & DeFilippis, N. (2010). Frontal lobe epilepsy in children and
adolescents: A preliminary neuropsychological assessment of executive function. Archives
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, 762–770.
Page 58 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-
analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297–1313.
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory
control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11,
81–93.
Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of
working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 377–384.
Masten, A. S., Desjardins, C. D., McCormick, C. M., Kuo, S. I-C., & Long, J. D. (2010). The
significance of childhood competence for adult success in work: A developmental cascade
analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 681–696.
Morton, J. B., Bosma, R., Ansari, D. (2009). Age-related changes in brain activation
associated with dimensional shifts of attention: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 46, 249–256.
McAuley, T., & White, D. A. (2011). A latent variables examination of processing speed,
response inhibition, and working (p. 738) memory during typical development. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 453–468.
McClelland, M. M., Piccinin, A., Acock, A. C., & Stallings, M. C. (submitted). Relations
between preschool attention and later school achievement and educational outcomes.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
McEvoy, R., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social
communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 34, 563–578.
McLaughlin Crabtree, V., & Williams, N. A. (2009). Normal sleep in children and
adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 18, 799–811.
McNamara, J. P. H., DeLucca, T. L., & Berg, W. K. (2007, March). Children’s executive
functioning: A longitudinal and microgenetic study. Poster presented at the meeting of the
Society for Research in Child Development, Boston.
Meltzer, L. (2007). Executive functions in education. From theory to practice. New York:
Guilford Press.
Page 59 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Meltzer, L., Pollica, L. S., & Barzillai, M. (2007). Executive function in the classroom:
Embedding strategy instruction into daily teaching practices. In L. Meltzer (Ed),
Executive function in education: From theory to practice (pp. 165–193). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Miller, M. R., Geisbrecht, G. F., Müller, U., McInerney, R. J., & Kerns, K. A. (2012). A latent
variable approach to determining the structure of executive function in preschool
children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 395–423.
Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2011). Toddlers benefit from labeling on an executive
function search task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 580–592.
Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J. M. Warren & K. Akert
(Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behaviour (pp. 313–334). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., Berman, M. G., Casey, B. J., Gotlib, I. H., Jonides, J., Kross, E.,
Teslovich, T., Wilson, N. L., & Shoda, Y. (2010). “Willpower” over the life span:
Decomposing self-regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 252–256.
Mischel, W., & Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 254–261.
Mischel, W., & Rodriguez, M. L., (1993). Psychological distance in self-imposed delay of
gratification. In R. R. Cocking & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), The development and meaning of
psychological distance (pp. 109–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies by
preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–
696.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children.
Science, 244, 933–938.
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual
differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 21, 8–14.
Page 60 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The
unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal
lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., Houts,
R., Poulton, R., Roberts, B. W., Ross, S., Sears, M. R., Thomson, W. M., & Caspi, A. (2011).
A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 108(7), 2693–2698.
Moore, B., Mischel, W., & Zeiss, A. (1976). Comparative effects of the reward stimulus
and its cognitive representation in voluntary delay. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 34(3), 419–424.
Morasch, K. C., & Bell, M. A. (2011). The role of inhibitory control in behavioral and
physiological expressions of toddler executive function. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 108, 593–606.
Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2009). Neural origin of cognitive shifting in young children.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 6017–6021.
Moriguchi, Y., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Itakura, S. (2010). Children perseverate to a
human’s actions but not to a robot’s actions. Developmental Science, 13, 62–68.
Moriguchi, Y., Lee, K., & Itakura, S. (2007). Social transmission of disinhibition in young
children. Developmental Science, 10, 481–491.
Morrison, F. J., Ponitz, C. C., & McClelland, M. M. (2010). Self-regulation and academic
achievement in the transition to school. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Child
development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 203–224). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Morton, J. B., Bosma, R., & Ansari, D. (2009). Age-related changes in brain activation
associated with dimensional shifts of attention: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 46, 249–256.
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual
advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719–726.
Morton, J. B., & Munakata, Y. (2002). Active vs. latent representations: a neural network
model of perseveration, dissociation, and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology, 40,
255–265.
Moses, L. J., & Carlson, S. M. (2004). Self-regulation and children’s theories of mind. In
M. Chandler, C. Lalonde, & C. Lightfoot (Eds.), Changing conceptions of psychological life
(pp. 127–146). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Page 61 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Müller, U., Dick, A. S., Gela, K., Overton, W. F., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The role of negative
priming in preschoolers’ flexible rule use on the dimensional change card sort task. Child
Development, 77, 395–412.
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., & Imrisek, S. (2005). Executive function and children’s
understanding of false belief: How specific is the relation? Cognitive Development, 20,
173–189.
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Leone, T., & Hood, S. (2004). Interference control in a new rule
use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75, 1594–
1609. (p. 739)
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Lurye, L. E., & Liebermann, D. P. (2008). The effect of labeling on
preschool children’s performance in the Dimensional Change Card Sort task. Cognitive
Development, 23, 395–408.
Munakata, Y., & Yerys, B. E. (2001). All together now: When dissociations between
knowledge and action disappear. Psychological Science, 12, 335–337.
Murray, G. K., Veijola, J., Moilanen, K., Miettunen, J., Glahn, D. C., Cannon, T. D., Jones, P.
B., & Isohanni, M. (2006). Infant motor development is associated with adult cognitive
categorization in a longitudinal birth cohort study. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 47, 25–29.
Niendam, T. A., Bearden, C. E., Johnson, J. K., McKinley, M., Loewy, R., O’Brien, M.,
Nuechterlein, K. H., Green, M. F., & Cannon, T. D. (2006). Neurocognitive performance
and functional disability in the psychosis prodrome. Schizophrenia Research, 84, 100–
111.
Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in
ADHD: Do we need a neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57,
1224–1230.
Page 62 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
O’Donnell, S., Noseworthy, M., Levine, B., Brandt, M., & Dennis, M. (2005). Cortical
thickness of the frontopolar area in typically developing children and adolescents.
NeuroImage, 24, 948–954.
Oh, S. & Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its
relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding . Child Development, 79,
80–99.
Ohlsson, S. (1996). Learning from performance errors. Psychological Review, 103(2), 241–
262.
Overman, W. H., Bachevalier, J., Schuhmann, E., & Ryan, P. (1996). Cognitive gender
differences in very young children parallel biologically based cognitive gender differences
in monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, 673–684.
Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three
neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2),
171–177.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-
functioning autistic individuals: Relation to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.
Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006). Multiple cognitive capabilities/
deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its
relation to theory of mind and executive control. Development and Psychopathology, 18,
77–98.
Page 63 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and cognitive control. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337–344.
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2002). What causes 3-year-olds’ difficulty on the dimensional
change card sorting task? Infant and Child Development, 11, 93–105.
Perner, J., Lang, B., & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self-control: More than a
common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73, 752–767.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
Polderman, T. J. C., Derks, E. M., Hudziak, J. J., Verhulst, F. C., Posthuma, D., & Boomsma,
D. I. (2007). Across the continuum of attention skills: A twin study of the SWAN ADHD
rating scale. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1080–1087.
Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). A structured
observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes.
Developmental Psychology, 45, 605–619.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for
the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23.
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker, P. (2011). Control networks and
neuromodulators of early development. Developmental Psychology, 48, 827–835.
Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism
on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 567–
579.
Povinelli, D. J., & O’Neill, D. (2000) Do chimpanzees use their gestures to instruct each
other? In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other
minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 459–487).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Powell, K. B., & Voeller, K. S. (2004). Prefrontal executive function syndromes in children.
Journal of Child Neurology, 19(10), 785–797.
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1996). Why animals lack pedagogy and some cultures have
more of it than others. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of human
development and education (pp. 302–344). Oxford: Blackwell. (p. 740)
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a “theory of mind”?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.
Page 64 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011).
Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 621–637.
Prencipe, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Development of affective decision-making for self and
other: Evidence for the integration of first- and third-person perspectives. Psychological
Science, 16, 501–505.
Qu, L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). The facilitative effect of positive stimuli on 3-year-olds’
flexible rule use. Cognitive Development, 22, 456–473.
Rajji, T. K., Ismail, Z., & Mulsant, B. H. (2009). Age at onset and cognition in
schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195, 286–293.
Riggs, N., Blair, C., & Greenberg, M. (2003). Concurrent and 2-year longitudinal relations
between executive function and the behavior of 1st and 2nd grade children. Child
Neuropsychology, 9, 267–276.
Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Pentz, M. A. (2006). The mediational role
of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in
elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science, 7, 91–
102.
Roditi, B. N., & Steinberg, J. (2007). The strategic math classroom: Executive function
processes and mathematics learning. In L. Meltzer (Ed.), Executive function in education:
From theory to practice (pp. 237–260). New York: Guilford Press.
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile
autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.
Romine, C. B., & Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe
function: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190–201.
Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2011). Modeling a cascade of effects: The
role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic
achievement. Developmental Science, 14, 1161–1175.
Rose, D., & Rose, K. (2007). Deficits in executive function processes: A curriculum-based
intervention. In L. Meltzer (Ed.), Executive function in education: From theory to practice
(pp. 287–308). New York: Guilford Press.
Page 65 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of
temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child
Development, 72, 1394–1408.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., Halparin, J., Gruber, D., Lercari, L. P., McCandliss, B. D., & Posner,
M. I. (2004). Development of attention during childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–
1040.
Rueda, M., Posner, M., Rothbart, M., & Davis-Stober, C. (2004). Development of the time
course for processing conflict: An event-related potentials study with 4-year-olds and
adults. BioMed Central Neuroscience, 5, 39.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, R. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005).
Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 14931–14935.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., Saccamanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training,
maturation and genetic influences on the development of executive attention.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 14931–14936.
Russell, J. (1996). Agency: Its role in mental development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Russell, J. (Ed.) (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The ‘windows task’ as a
measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 9, 331–349.
Rylander, G. (1939). Personality changes after operations on the frontal lobes. Acta
Psychiatrica et Neurologica, 20, 327.
Sabbagh, M. A., & Taylor, M. (2000). Neural correlates of theory of mind reasoning in
adults: An event-related potential study. Psychological Science, 11, 46–50.
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of
executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers.
Psychological Science, 17, 74–81.
Saxe, R., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Special issue: Theory of mind. Social Neuroscience,
1(3,4).
Page 66 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Perez-Edgar, K., Hu, S., & Hamer, D. H. (2001). Association of
DRD4 with attention problems in normal childhood development. Psychiatric Genetics, 11,
25–29.
Seguin, J. R., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Executive function in early physical aggression. In R.
E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp.
307–329). New York: Guilford.
Senn, T. E., Espy, K. A., & Kaufmann, P. M. (2004). Using path analysis to understand
executive function organization in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology,
26, 445–464.
Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P. M., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Parenting quality
interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in
early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 1039–1046.
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-
regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic
conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.
Shonkoff, J. P. (2011). Protecting brains, not simply stimulating minds. Science, 6045,
982–983.
Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in “theory of mind.” Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 463–471.
Simmering, V. R., Schutte, A. R., & Spencer, J. P. (2008). Generalizing the dynamic field
theory of spatial cognition across real and developmental timescales. Brain Research,
1202, 68–86. (p. 741)
Solanto, M. V., Abikoff, H., Sonuga-Barke, E., Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., Wigal, T., et al.
(2001). The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of
impulsivity in ADHD: a supplement to NIMH multimodal treatment study of ADHD.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 215–228.
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Dalen, L., & Remington, B. (2003). Do executive deficits and delay
aversion make independent contributions to preschool attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder symptoms? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
42, 1335–1342.
Page 67 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Sophian, C., & Wellman, H. M. (1983). Selective information use and perseveration in the
search behavior of infants and young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
35, 369–390.
Speltz, M., Deklyen, M., Calderon, R., Greenberg, M., & Fisher, P. (1999).
Neuropsychological characteristics and test behaviors of boys with early onset conduct
problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 315–325.
Spencer, J. P., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the
A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of 2-year-
olds. Child Development, 72, 1327–1346.
Steenari, M. R., Vuontela, V., Paavonen, E. J., Carlson, S., Fjallberg, M., & Aronen, E.
(2003). Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13-year-old school children. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 85–92.
Stevens, C., Fanning, J., Coch, D., Sanders, L., & Neville, H. (2008). Neural mechanisms
of selective auditory attention are enhanced by computerized training:
Electrophysiological evidence from language-impaired and typically developing children.
Brain Research, 1205, 55–69.
Stevens, S. E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Kreppner, J. M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E.,
Groothues, C., Hawkins, A., & Rutter, M. (2008). Inattention/overactivity following early
severe institutional deprivation: presentation and associations in early adolescence.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 385–398.
Steenari, M. R., Vuontela, V., Paavonen, E. J., Carlson, S., Fjallberg, M., & Aronen, E. T.
(2003). Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13-year-old schoolchildren. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 85–92.
Stiles, J. (2009). On genes, brains, and behavior: Why should developmental psychologists
care about brain development? Child Development Perspectives, 3, 196–202.
Strayhorn, J. M., (2002). Self-control: Theory and research. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 7–16.
Page 68 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press.
Talwar, V., Carlson, S. M., & Lee, K. (2011). Effects of a punitive environment on
children’s executive functioning: A natural experiment . Social Development, 20, 805–824.
Thapar, A., Langley, K., Asherson, P., et al. (2007). Gene–environment interplay in
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the importance of a developmental
perspective. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 1–3.
Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: A
field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 1–34.
Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H., & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an
implicit to an explicit understanting of false belief from infancy to preschool age. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 172–187.
Thompson, C., Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1997). The development of future-oriented
prudence and altruism in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 12, 199–212.
Thorell, L. B. Lindqvist, S., Nutley, S. B., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and
transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12,
106–113.
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.
Towse, J. N., Redbond, J., Houston-Price, C. M. T., & Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the
Dimensional Change Card Sort: Perspectives from task success and failure. Cognitive
Development, 15, 347–365.
Tucker, D. M., Luu, P., & Pribram, K. H. (1995). Social and emotional self-regulation.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 213–239.
Vicari, S., Bellucci S., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2003). Visual and spatial working memory
dissociation: Evidence from a genetic syndrome . Developmental Medicine and Child
Neurology, 45, 269–273.
Visu-Petra, L., Benga, O., Tincas, I. & Miclea, M. (2007). Visual-spatial processing in
children and adolescents with Down’s syndrome: a computerized assessment of memory
skills. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 942–952.
Page 69 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Larose, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). Kindergarten disruptive
behaviors, protective factors, and educational achievement by early adulthood. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97, 617–629.
Waber, D. P., & Holmes, J. M. (1985). Assessing children’s copy productions of the Rey-
Osterreith Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7,
264–280.
Walshaw, P. D., Alloy, L. B., & Sabb, F. W. (2010). Executive function in pediatric bipolar
disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of distinct phenotypic
profiles. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 103–120.
Wassenberg, R., Feron, F. J., Kessels, A. G., Hendriksen, J. G., Kalff, A. C., Kroes, M.,
Hurks, P. P., Beeren, M., Jolles, J., & Vles, J. S. (2005). Relation between cognitive and
motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a (p. 742) large-scale cross-
sectional study. Child Development, 76, 1092–1103.
Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Tulsky, D. S., Zelazo, P. D., Bauer, P., et al. (in
press). NIH Toolbox for the assessment of behavioral and neurological function: Cognition
domain instruments. Neurology.
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind
development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75,
523–541.
West, H., & Abravanel, E. (1972). Evidence for class-concept mediation of perceptual sets
in preschool children. Child Development, 43, 1242–1248.
Page 70 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Whitehead, A. N., & Russell, B. (1927). Principia Mathematica. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., Stopp, C., Respass, J., Stewart, P., Jameson, T. R., Gilbert, D. G.,
& Huggenvik, J. I. (2009). Gene-environment interactions across development: Exploring
DRD2 genotype and prenatal smoking effects on self-regulation. Developmental
Psychology, 45, 31–44.
Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A, & Charack, D. (2008). Using confirmatory analysis to understand
executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 44, 575–587.
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. (2011).
The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 108, 436–452.
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity
of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-
analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.
Willcutt, E. G., Sonuga-Barke, E., Nigg, J., & Sergeant, J. (2008). Recent developments in
neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Advances in Biological
Psychiatry, 24, 195–226.
Willoughby, M., & Blair, C. (2011). Test-retest reliability of a new executive function
battery for use in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 17, 564–579.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining
function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13,
103–128.
Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early
childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental
Psychobiology, 44, 68–83.
Yakovlev, P. I., & Lecours, A. R. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of
the brain. In A. Minkowski (Ed.), Regional development of the brain in early life (pp. 3–
70). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Yerys, B. E. (2007). Executive functioning in autism: Examining the role of verbal labeling
and social context. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and
Engineering, 67(9–B), 5431.
Page 71 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Wilson, A. C., Trancik, A., & Bazinet, A. (2011). Emotion
regulation profiles, temperament, and adjustment problems in preadolescents. Child
Development, 82, 951–966.
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing
executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297–301.
Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub,
S. (in press). NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and
attention. In P. D. Zelazo & P. J. Bauer (Eds.), National Institutes of Heath Toolbox—
Cognitive Function Battery (NIH Toolbox CFB): Validation for children between 3 and 15
years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
Zelazo, P. D., & Bauer, P. J. (Eds.) (in press). National Institutes of Health Toolbox—
Cognition Battery (NIH Toolbox CB): Validation for children between 3 and 15 years.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive
function: A problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 198–226.
Zelazo, P. D., Chandler, M., & Crone, E. A. (2010). The birth and early development of a
new discipline: Developmental social cognitive neuroscience. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Chandler,
& E. A. Crone (Eds.). Developmental social cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–9). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. M., & Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the life span.
Acta Psychologica, 115(2–3), 167–183.
Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control: The development of
executive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 121–126.
Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing
rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 37–63.
Zelazo, P. D., Jacques, S., Burack, J., & Frye, D. (2002). The relation between theory of
mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and
Child Development, 11, 171–195. (p. 743)
Page 72 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive functions in typical and atypical development.
In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469).
Oxford: Blackwell.
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive
function: Cognitive complexity and control—revised. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 68(3), 93–119.
Zelazo, P. D., & Reznick, J. S. (1991). Age–related asynchrony of knowledge and action.
Child Development, 62, 719–735.
Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Pinon, D. (1995). Response control and the execution of
verbal rules. Developmental Psychology, 31, 508–517.
Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Spinazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and
response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Developmental Psychology, 34,
203–214.
Stephanie M. Carlson
Philip David Zelazo, Ph.D., is the Nancy M. and John E. Lindahl Professor at the
Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota. His research has been
recognized by numerous awards and honors, he serves on several editorial boards
(e.g., Child Development; Emotion; Development and Psychopathology), and he is
currently the President of the Jean Piaget Society. In 2007, he was the lead Editor of
The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness.
Susan Faja
Page 73 of 73
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use.