Mervis1981 Categorization of Natural Objects
Mervis1981 Categorization of Natural Objects
REVIEWS Further
Quick links to online content
CATEGORIZATION OF +341
NATURAL OBJECTS
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Carolyn B. Mervis
Eleanor Rosch
CONTENTS
89
0066-4308/8110201-0089$01.00
90 MERVIS & ROSCH
ple, Locke 1 690), the ideas that people have of objects (concepts) consist
of an intension (meaning) and an extension (the objects in the class). The
intension is a specification of those qualities that a thing must have to be
a member of the class; the extension consists of things that have those
qualities. Thus, qualities (attributes) connect concepts to the world. Con
cept formation research within learning theory, by the very nature of its
research paradigm, presupposes a British empiricist stand on these issues
(e.g. see Fodor 198 1).
In a classical concept formation experiment, stimuli are typically sets
of items varying orthogonally on a limited number of sensory qualities
such as color and form. Concepts are complexes composed of and decom
posable into the defining qualities and logical relations between those qual
ities (e.g. red and square) which are their elements. Originally, the passive
and gradual learning of common defining elements was emphasized in re
search (Hull 1920). However, since Bruner (Bruner et al 1956), research
has concentrated on subjects' active hypothesis testing in the learning
of relevant features and the logical rules combining them (see Bourne et
al 1979).
The newer categorization research has raised for debate at least six
empirical and theoretical issues, none of which had been considered debat
able by the earlier approach. They are listed below, beginning with the
somewhat more concrete structural problems.
(a) those with fur and mouths, which move about primarily on foot;
(b) those with fur and mouths, which move about primarily by flying;
(c) those with fur and beaks, which move about primarily on foot;
(d) those with fur and beaks, which move about primarily by flying;
(e) those with feathers and mouths, which move about primarily on foot;
(j) those with feathers and mouths, which move about primarily by flying;
(g) those with feathers and beaks, which move about primarily on foot;
(h) those with feathers and beaks, which move about primarily by flying.
It is not immediately obvious how to assign these (hypothetical) creatures
to categories; any of several schemes (e.g. by coat type, by oral opening type)
would be equally plausible, and none seems particularly reasonable a priori.
Thus, given the total set type of organization, it makes sense that category
assignments should be originally arbitrary. However, it hardly requires
research to demonstrate that the perceived world of objects is not structured
in this manner. Just two of the eight theoretically possible combinations of
attribute values, types a and h (mammals and birds, respectively), comprise
92 MERVIS & ROSCH
the great majority of existent species in the world that are possible based
on this total set. This correlated attribute structure of the perceived world
has been used as the basis for several programs of research concerning
natural category structure.
the basis of linguistic and cultural evidence, that one of these levels is more
fundamental than the others. Psychologists (Rosch et a1 1976a) have argued
that the most cognitively efficient, and therefore the most basic level of
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
One of the most pervasive research findings is that basic level categories
are acquired before categories at other hierarchical levels. Rosch et al
(1976a) and Daehler et al ( 1 979) found that young children can solve simple
sorting problems at the basic level before solving them at the superordinate
level. Mervis & Crisafi (198 1), using artificial category hierarchies, found
that 21;2-year-olds were able to sort basic level triads correctly, but could
not sort either superordinate or subordinate level triads. With regard to
language acquisition, Stross ( 1 973) and Dougherty ( 1 978) have both shown
that the first botanical labels that children learn are names for basic level
categories. Other studies have yielded similar results for selected nonbotani
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
predict infant behavior. Ricciuti ( 1 965) examined the behavior of 12-, 18-,
and 24-month-olds who were given two types of toys to play with, and
found considerable evidence of categorization abilities in even the 1 2-
month-olds. K. Nelson ( 1 973), using 20-month-olds, has replicated this
result for other basic level categories. In summary, then, there is now
substantial evidence that basic level categorization should be considered a
basic process.
In a classical concept formation experiment, any one stimulus which fits the
definition of the concept (possesses the relevant attributes in the correct
combination) is as good an example of the concept as any other. More
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
1 975, 1 977; Lakoff 1 977; Bowerman 1 978; Bates & MacWinney 1 980;
deVilliers 1 9 80; Maratsos & Chalkley 1 980; Coleman & Kay 1 98 1 ).
Representativeness of items within a category has been shown to affect
virtually all of the major dependent variables used as measures in psycho
logical research. In this section we consider speed of processing, free pro
duction of exemplars, natural language use of category terms, asymmetries
in similarity relationships between category exemplars, and learning and
development.
Speed of processing (reaction time) has been extensively investigated in
category verification tasks. Subjects are usually asked to verify statements
of the form "An [exemplar] is a [category name]" as rapidly as possible.
Response times are shorter for verification of the category membership of
representative exemplars than nonrepresentative exemplars; these effects
are robust and appear in a variety, of experimental paradigms (see reviews
in E. E. Smith et al 1 974, E. E. Smith 1 978, Hampton 1979, Danks &
Glucksberg 1980, Kintsch 1980). Rosch et al ( 1976b) have also demon
strated this effect for three types of artificial categories, where representa
tiveness was defined by family resemblance, by mean values of attributes,
or by degree of distortion from the prototype (random dot pattern). These
differences in response times are amplified when a prime (prior mention of
the category name) is provided. Priming reduces response times to verify
the category membership of representative exemplars but increases re
sponse times to verify the membership of nonrepresentative exemplars. This
result has been obtained for colors (Rosch 1 975c), superordinate semantic
categories (Rosch 1975b, MacKenzie & Palermo 198 1), and for the artificial
categories just described (Rosch et al 1 976b).
Order and probability of exemplar production have been investigated
primarily for superordinate semantic categqries. Battig & Montague ( 1969)
asked subjects to list exemplars·.ofeacli 0[-,56 superordinate categories.
Frequency of mention of an exemplar was found to. be significantly corre-
CATEGORIZATION OF OBJECTS 97
acceptable to say "A sparrow is a true bird," but not "A penguin is a true
bird." Correspondingly, the sentence "A penguin is technically a bird" is
acceptable, but "A sparrow is technically a bird" is not. Similarly, Rosch
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
(1 975a) has shown that when subjects are given sentence frames such as "[x]
is virtually [y)," they reliably place the more representative member of a
pair in the referent (y) slot. In addition, representativeness ratings for
members of superordinate categories predict the extent to which the mem
ber term is substitutable for the superordinate word in sentences (Rosch
1 977). Finally, Newport & Bellugi (1 978) have shown that in American
Sign Language, when superordinate terms are denoted by a short list of
exemplars only the more representative exemplars may be used.
Asymmetry in similarity ratings between members that vary in represen
tativeness is another way in which members of a category fail to be equiva
lent. Tversky & Gati ( 1 978) and Rosch ( 1975a) have shown that less
representative exemplars are often considered more similar to more repre
sentative exemplars than vice versa. For example, subjects felt that Mexico
was more similar to the United States than the United States was to Mexico.
This phenomenon helps to explain the asymmetries which Whitten et al
( 1979) found in similarity ratings of pairs of "synonyms." It also helps to
explain Keller & Kellas's (1978) finding that release from proactive inhibi
tion is significantly greater if the change is from typical to atypical members
of a category than if the change is from atypical to typical members. In
addition, asymmetry in similarity ratings has implications for inductive
reasoning. Rips (1975) found that new information about a category mem
ber was generalized asymmetrically; for example, when told that the robins
on an island had a disease, subjects were more likely to decide that ducks
would catch it than that robins would catch a disease which the ducks
had.
In the learning and development of categories, representativeness appears
to be a major variable. Representativeness gradients have two basic implica
tions for category acquisition (Mervis & Pani 1 980). The first implication
is that category membership is established (for the set of exemplars to which
a person has been exposed) first for the most representative exemplars and
98 MERVIS & ROSCH
last for the least representative exemplars. One of the most robust findings
from research using statistically generated categories is that correct classifi
cation of novel exemplars is strongly negatively correlated with degree of
distortion of the exemplar from the prototype pattern. This result has been
obtained using both random dot pattern categories (e.g. Posner & Keele
1968, Homa et al 1973, Homa & Vosburgh 1 976) and random polygon
categories (e.g. Aiken & Williams 1973, Williams et al 1977). Similarly,
when subjects are asked to indicate which of a series of categorically related
stimuli have been seen previously, percentage of false recognition responses
and degree of confidence that the (novel) pattern has been seen previously
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
are both negatively correlated with degree of distortion from the prototype
(e.g. Franks & Bransford 1 97 1 ; Neumann 1 974, 1 977; Posnansky & Neu
mann 1 976). When subjects are given explicit feedback concerning the
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
worse than exposure to good examples only. However, three other studies
found training on good examples superior to training on a range of examples
(Mervis & Pani 1980, Hupp & Mervis 198 1 , Mervis & Mirman 198 1).
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
in fact, insofar as category members are the same higher level "sort of
thing" they will share such higher level, and therefore necessary, attributes
as "animate" or "solid object"; see Keil 1 979, E. E. Smith & Medin 1 981.)
We can now see how a family resemblance structure of categories might
make sense of the findings that the most representative members of catego
ries are established first as category members and are the most useful basis
for learning categories. Because basic level categories maximize within
category similarity relative to between-category similarity, it is reason
able that they were found to be learned first-before categories subordinate
and superordinate to them. Correspondingly, the most representative exem
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
fuzzy, and the theoretical issue of how well defined one wants to consider
the category "representation" itself.
Two experimental approaches have been used to demonstrate that cate
gory boundaries are not well defined. The first involves demonstrations that
there are between-subject disagreements concerning which categories cer
tain (poor) exemplars belong to. Berlin & Kay ( 1 969) found substantial
disagreement among subjects concerning the location of color category
boundaries, even when native language was controlled. This result has been
replicated by Labov ( 1973), using schematic drawings of cups, by McClos
key & Glucksberg (1978), using superordinate semantic categories, by Can
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
findings.
Other models incorporate greater indeterminacy into the representation
itself. In the two-stage model of E. E. Smith et al (1 974) and the single-stage
models of McClosky & Glucksberg (1979) and Hampton ( 1979), concepts
are represented by sets of weighted non-necessary features. Processing deci
sions about category membership are made on a probabilistic basis. Collins
& Loftus's (1975) model also includes a probabilistic decision process, but
it is allowed to operate on more varied types of structural information than
simply attributes.
The issue of determinacy has been approached in a slightly different
fashion in research on context effects. Both Collins & Loftus (1975) and
McClosky & Glucksberg (1 979) invoke Bayesian inference procedures spe
cifically to deal with context; however, all of the probabilistic models have
an unchanging (and, in that sense, determinate) representation of some sort.
Context has been employed to criticize just this sort of assumption. In the
psychological literature, changes in meaning, comprehension, or memory
of particular terms as a function of differing contexts have been used to
question the adequacy of semantic memory models (see e.g. Barclay et al
1 974, R. C. Anderson & Ortony 1975, R. C. Anderson et al 1 976, Potter
& Faulconer 1979). These studies tend to be primarily critical rather than
to offer formulations of their own. In addition, context has been used as the
basis of more far reaching criticisms of determinate views; for authors in
the hermeneutic tradition, context becomes the basis of arguments against
representations and other noninteractive accounts of meaning (see for ex
ample the papers in Rabinow & Sullivan 1979).
in fact, where telling a subject the rule may retard performance. If only
specific exemplars are stored in memory, categorization of novel items could
occur by matching the new instance to the most similar item in memory.
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
perceived objects (Hochberg & Galper 1 967, Yin 1 969, Rock 1 973, Brad
shaw 1 976). A second development which has offered the opportunity to
view categories as wholes is the possibility for spatial representation of
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
1 978; Schank 1973; Fodor et al 1980). The recent suggestion that the
capacity to match figures holistically may be a cognitive strategy which
shows individual differences (Cooper 1 976, Cooper & Podgorny 1 976) is
intriguing; for contrasting work on individual differences see Day ( 1 976).
Two important points have been made in this discussion of decomposabil
ity. First, although the tendency in cognitive models is to decompose almost
automatically, the evidence of holistic processing of some stimuli or at some
stages suggests that we be more thoughtful about decompositional models.
Second, findings concerning decomposition appear to be dependent on the
level under consideration. Some general principles of decomposition are
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
needed.
However, as Palmer (1978) and E. E. Smith & Medin (1981) have pointed
out, the difference between features and dimensions may apply more to the
surface form of the representation than to the underlying information that
is represented. It can be shown that features may be extended to handle
quantitative properties, and dimensions may be extended to handle most
(but perhaps not all; see Beals et a1 1968) qualitative properties. (But note
that missing values communicate different information for features vs for
dimensions; see Garner 1 978a,b.) Features, transformations, and nonmetric
dimensions can be integrated reasonably into the same representation.
One of the first psychologists to question the nature of attributes (whether
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
present, this work is largely confined to formal systems (Miller & Johnson
Laird 1 976) and to artificial intelligence (see Winograd 1975).
SUMMARY
vide constraints on alternative views. Other issues that research and theory
in the modeling of categorization have brought into focus are the nature of
the abstractive process, the question of decomposition of categories into
elements, and the nature of the attributes into which categories are often
decomposed. In short, current research on categories could be said to
represent a kind of experimental epistemology.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank Doug Medin, Elissa Newport, Emilie Roth, and
especially John Pani for their thoughtful criticisms of previous drafts of this
paper. Preparation of the manuscript was supported by grant No. BNS79-
1 5 1 20 from the National Science Foundation and by grant No. 6-400-
76-01 1 6 from the National Institutes of Education, both to the University
of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign; and by grant No. I RO I MH243 16-03 from
the National Institutes of Mental Health to the University of California,
Berkeley.
Literature Cited
Aiken, L. S., Williams, T. M. 1973. A devel terms. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav.
opmental study of schematic concept 1 5 :667-79
formation. Dev. Psycho!. 8 : 1 62-67 Anglin, J. M. 1977. Word, Object and Con
Anderson, N. H. 1974. Algebraic models in ceptual Development. New York: Nor
perception. In Handbook of Perception, ton. 302 pp.
ed. E. C. Carterctte, M. P. Fried Attneave, F. 1957. Transfer of experience
man, 2:21 5-99. New York: Academic. with a class-schema to identification
556 pp. learning of patterns and shapes. J. Exp.
Anderson, R. C., Ortony, A. 1975. On put Psychol. 54:81-88
ting apples into bottles-A problem of Barclay, J. R., Bransford, J. D., Franks, J. J.,
polysemy. Cognit. Psycho!. 7:1 67-80 McCarrell, N. S., Nitsch, K. 1974.
Anderson, R. C., Pichert, J. W., Goetz, E. T., Comprehension and semantic flexibil
SchaJlert, D. L., Stevens, K. V., Trollip, ity. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 13:
S. R. 1 976. Instantiation of general 471-81
1 10 MERVIS & ROSCH
Barresi, 1., Robbins, D., Shain, K. 1975. Role B. B. Lloyd, pp. 1 69-2 1 5. Hillsdale, NI:
of distinctive features in the abstraction Erlbaum. 328 pp.
of related concepts. 1. Exp. Psychol. Brown, C. H., Kolar, J., Torrey, B. I.,
Hum. Learn. Mem. 104:360--68 Truong-Quang, T., Volkman, P. 1976.
Bartlett, F. C. 1932. Remembering: A Study Some general principles of biological
in Experimental and Social Psychology. and non-biological folk classification.
Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. Am. Ethnol. 3:73-85
3 1 7 pp. Brown, R. 1958. How shall a thing be called?
Bates, E., MacWinney, B. 1980. Functional Psychol. Rev. 65: 14-21
ist approaches to grammar. In Lan Brown, R. 1976. Reference: In memorial
guage Acquisition: The State of the tribute to Eric Lenneberg. Cognition
Art. ed. L. Gleitman, E. Wanner. 4: 1 25-53
Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. Bruner, I. S., Goodnow, I. I., Austin, J. G.
In press 1956. A Study of Thinking. New York:
Battig, W. F., Montague, W. E. 1969. Cate Wiley. 330 pp.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
gory norms for verbal items in 56 cate Calder, A. 1979. Constructive mathematics.
gories: A replication and extension of Sci. Am. 24 1 : 1 46-69
the Connecticut category norms. J. Cantor, N., Mischel, W. 1979. Prototypes in
Exp. Psychol. Monogr. 80: 1--46 person perception. In Adv. Exp. Soc.
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
Daehler, M. W., Lonardo, R., Bukatko, D. motion and Structure. New York: Wi
1979. Matching and equivalence judg ley. 203 pp.
ments in very young children. Child Garner, W. R. 1978a. Aspects of a stimulus:
Dev. 50: 1 70-79 Features, dimensions, and configura
Danks, J. H., Glucksberg, S. 1980. Experi tions. See Brooks 1978, pp. 99-133
mental psycholinguistics. Ann. Rev. Garner, W. R. 1 978b. Selective attention to
Psychol. 3 1 :391-417 attributes and to stimuli. J. Exp. Psy
Day, R. 1976. Language-bound and lan chol.-General 107:287-308
guage-optional processing. Presented at Gelman, R. 1978. Cognitive development.
Psychon. Soc., St. Louis Ann. Rev. Psychol 29:297-332
DeValois, R. L., Jacobs, F. H. 1968. Primate Glass, A. L., Holyoak, K. J. 1 975. Alterna
color vision. Science 162:533-40 tive conceptions of semantic memory.
deVilIiers, J. 1 980. The process of rule learn Cognition 3 : 3 1 3-39
ing in child speech: A new look. In Chil Goldman, D., Homa, D. 1977. Integrative
and metric properties of abstracted in
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
66-80
Dreyfus, H. L. 1979. What Computers Can 't Hampton, J. A. 1979. Polymorphous con
Do: A Critique ofArtificial Reason. New cepts in semantic memory. J. Verb.
York: Harper & Row. 354 pp. 2nd ed . Learn. Verb. Behav. 1 8 :441-61
Erickson, J. R., Jones, M. R. 1978. Thinking. Hayes-Roth, B., Hayes-Roth, F. 1977. Con
cept learning and the recognition and
Ann. Rev. Psychol. 29:6 1-90
classification of exemplars. J. Verb.
Erreich, A., Valian, V. 1979. Children's inter
Learn. Verb. Behav. 16:32 1-38
nal organization of locative categories.
Heider, E. R. 1972. Universals in color nam
Child Dev. 50: 107 1-77
ing and memory. J. Exp. Psychol 93:
Fillmore, C. D. 1975. An alternative to
1 0-20
checklist theories of meaning. Proc. Jst
Hochberg, J., Galper, R. E. 1967. Recogni
Ann. Meet. Berkeley Ling. Soc. 1 :
tion of faces I: An exploratory study.
123-31
Bull. Psychon. Soc. 9:619-20
Fillmore, C. D. 1977. Topics in lexical se
Homa, D., Cross, J., Cornell, D., Goldman,
mantics. In Current Issues in Linguistic D., Shwartz, S. 1973. Prototype ab
Theory, ed. R. W. Cole, pp. 76-138.
straction and classification of new in
Wilmington, Ind: Indiana Univ. Press.
stances as a function of number of in
303 pp.
stances defining the prototype. J. Exp.
Flavell, J. H. 1977. Cognitive Development. Psychol 101 : 1 1 6-22
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Homa, D., Vosburgh, R. 1976. Category
286 pp. breadth and the abstraction of
Flavell, J. H., Wellman, H. M. 1976. Meta prototypical information. J. Exp. Psy
memory. In Perspectives on the Develop chol-Hum. Learn. Mem. 2:322-30
ment ofMemory and Cognition, ed. R. I:Iull, C. L. 1 920. Quantitative aspects of the
V. K ail Jr., J. W. Hagen, pp. 3-33. evolution of concepts. Psychol. Monogr.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 498 pp. Vol. 28 (whole No. 123). 86 pp.
Fodor, J. 1970. Three reasons for not deriv Hunn, E. 1 975. Cognitive process�s in folk
ing 'kill' from 'cause to die'. Ling. Inq. ornithology: The identification of gulls.
1 :429-38 Work. pap. No. 42. Lang. Behav. Res.
Fodor, J. 1 9 8 1 . The current status of the in Lab., Univ. Calif., Berkeley
nateness controversy. In Representa Hunn, E. 1976. Toward a perceptual model
tions, ed. J. Fodor. Vermont: Bradford of folk biological classification. Am.
Books. In press Ethnol. 3:508-24
Fodor, J., Garrett, M., Walker, E., Parkes, R. Hupp, S. C., Mervis, C. B. 1 98 1 . Acquisition
1980. Against definitions. Cognition of Basic Object Categories by Severely
Vol. 8. In press Handicapped Children. Submitted for
Franks, J. J., Bransford, J. D. 1 97 1 . Abstrac publication.
tion of visual patterns. J. Exp. Psychol. Husaim, J. S., Cohen, L. B. 1980. Infant
90:65-74 learning of ill-defined categories. Pre
Gamer, W. R. 1970. The stimulus in infor sented at Int. Conf. Infant Stud. New
mation processing. Am. PsychoL 25: Haven
3 50-58 Hutchinson, J. W., Lockhead, G. R. 1 977.
Gamer, W. R. 1974. The Processing ofInfor- Similarity as distance: A structural
1 12 MERVIS & ROSCH
Kay, P., McDaniel, C. K. 1978. The linguis Effect of Context on Semantic Catego
tic significance of the meanings of basic rization. Submitted for publication
color terms. Language 54:610-46 Maratsos, M. P., Chalkley, M. A. 1980. The
Keil, F. C. 1979. Semantic and Conceptual internal language of children's syntax:
Development: An Ontological Perspec The ontogenesis and representation of
syntactic categories. See deVilliers
tive. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard Univ.
1 980. In press
Press. 2 1 4 pp.
McCauley, c., Stitt, C. L. 1978. An individ
Keller, D. , Kellas, G. 1978. Typicality as a
ual and quantitative measure of stereo
dimension of encoding. J. Exp. Psy
types. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36:929-40
chol.-Hum. Learn. Mem. 4:78-85
McCauley, c., Stitt, C. L., Segal, M. 1 980.
Kemler, D. G., Smith, L. B. 1978. Is there a
Stereotyping: From prejudice to predic
developmental trend from integrality to
tion. Psychol. Bull 87: 195-208
separability in perception? J. Exp. Child
McCawley, J. 1971. Prelexical syntax.
PsychoL 26:498-507
Georgetown Univ. Monogr. Ser. Lang.
Kemler, D. G., Smith, L. B. 1979. Accessing
Ling. 2 3 : 1 9-33
similarity and dimensional relations:
McCawley, J. 1978. Conversational implica
Effects of integrality and separability on
ture and the lexicon. In Syntax and Se
the discovery of complex concepts. J. mantics, ed. P. Cole, 9:245-59. New
Exp. Psychol.-General 108:1 33-50 York: Academic. 340 pp.
Kempton, W. 1978. Category grading and McCloskey, M. E., Glucksberg, S. 1978. Nat
taxonomic relations: A mug is a sort of ural categories: Well defined or fuzzy
a cup. Am. Ethnol. 5:44-65 sets? Mem. Cognit. 6 :462-72
Kinchla, R. A., Wolf, J. M. 1979. The order McCloskey, M. E., Glucksberg, S. 1979. De
of visual processing: "Top-down," "bot cision processes in verifying category
tom-up," or "middle-out." Percept. Psy membership statements: Implications
chophys. 25:225-31 for models of semantic memory. Cognit.
Kintsch, W. 1 9 80. Semantic memory: A Psychol. 1 1 : 1-37
tutorial. In Attention and Performance Medin, D. L., Schaffer, M. M. 1978. Context
VIIL ed. R. S. Nickerson. Hillsdale, theory of classification learning. Psy
NJ: Erlbaum. In press chol. Rev. 85:207-38
Kogan, N. 1 97 1 . Educational implication of Mervis, C. B. 1980. Category structure and
cognitive styles. In Psychology and Edu the development of categorization. In
cational Practice, ed. G. Lesser, pp. Theoretical Issues in Reading Compre
242-92. Glenview, Ill: Scott, Foresman. hension, ed. R. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, W.
580 pp. F. Brewer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. In
Labov, W. 1973. The boundaries of words press
and their meanings. In New Ways of Mervis, C. B., Catlin, J., Rosch, E. 1975. De
Analyzing Variation in English, ed. C. J. velopment of the structure of color cate
Bailey, R. Shuy, pp. 340-73. Washing gories. Dev. PsychoL 1 1 :54-60
ton: Georgetown Univ. Press. 373 pp. Mervis, C. B., Catlin, J., Rosch, E. 1976. Re
Lakotf, G. 1973. Hedges: A study in meaning lationships among goodness-of-exam
criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts. pie, category norms, and word fre
J. Philos. Logic 2:458-508 quency. Bull Psychon. Soc. 7:283-84
CATEGORIZAnON OF OBJECTS 1 13
Mervis, C. B., Crisafi, M. A. 1981. The Per ration in Cognition, ed. D. A. Norman,
ceptual-Cognitive Primacy of Basic D. E. Rumelhart, LNR Research
Level Categories. In preparation Group, pp. 279-307. San Francisco:
Mervis, C. B., Mirman, M. C. 1 9 8 1 . The Freeman. 430 pp.
Effects of Category Size and Distortion Palmer, S. E. 1978. Fundamental aspects of
Level on Concept Abstraction. In prepa cognitive representation. See Brooks
ration 1978, pp. 259-303
Mervis, C. B., Pani, J. R. 1 980. Acquisition Piagei:, J. 1 970. Piaget's theory. In Carmicha
of basic object categories. Cognit. Psy el's Manual of Child Psychology, ed. P.
chol. Vol. 12. In press H. Mussen, 1 :703-32. New York: Wi
Meyer, D. E. 1970. On the representation and ley. 1 5 19 pp. Piaget, J. 1972. The Princi
retrieval of stored semantic infonna pies of Genetic Epistemology. New
tion. Cognit. PsychoL 1 :242-300 York: Basic Books. 98 pp.
Miller, G. A., Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1976. Pittenger, J. B., Shaw, R. E. 1975. Aging
Language and Perception. Cambridge, faces as visual-elastic events: Implica
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Mass: Harvard .Univ. Press. 760 pp. tions for a theory of nonrigid shape per
Mirman, M. C. 1978. The Effects ofNumber ception. J. Exp. Psychol. -Hum. Percept.
and Distortion Level of Category Exem Perform. 1 :374-82
plars on Concept Abstraction. Honors Pittenger, J. B., Shaw, R. E., Mark, L. S.
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.
Ricciuti, H. N. 1965. Object grouping and 2 year olds. Presented at Bien Meet.
selective ordering behavior in infants 12 Soc. Res. Child Dev., New Orleans
to 24 months old. Merrill-Palmer Q. Ross, J. 1972. The category squish: Endsta
1 1 : 1 29-48 tion Hauptwort. Chicago Ling. Soc.
Riley, D. A., Lamb, M. R. 1979. Stimulus Pap. 8:3 1 6-29
generalization. In Perception and Its De Saltz, E., Dixon, D., Klein, S., Becker, G.
velopment: A Tribute to Eleanor J. Gib 1 977. Studies of natural language con
son, ed. A. D. Pick, pp. 7-37. Hillsdale, cepts: III. Concept overdiscrimination
NJ: Erlbaum. 258 pp. in comprehension between two and four
Rips, L. J. 1975. Inductive judgments about years of age. Child Dev. 48:1 682-85
natural categories. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Saltz, E., Soller, E., Sigel, I. E. 1972. The
Behav. 14:665-8 1 development of natural language con
Rips, L. J., Shoben, E. J., Smith, E. E. 1973. cepts. Child Dev. 43: 1 19 1 - 1 202
Semantic distance and the verification Schank, R. C. 1973. Identification of concep
tualizations underlying natural lan
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1981.32:89-115. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Tversky, A. 1 977. Features of similarity. Psy scription from examples. In The Psy
chol. Rev. 84:327-52 chology of Computer Vision. ed. P. H .
Tversky, A., Gati, I. 1978. Studies of simi Winston, pp. 1 57-2 10. New York:
larity. See Brooks 1978, pp. 79-98 McGraw-Hill. 282 pp.
by University of Sussex on 10/12/12. For personal use only.