RCM White Paper - When Words Fail
RCM White Paper - When Words Fail
FAIL
and the brain
GOALS
• Expressing
emotions
• Fostering social ELEMENTS
and interpersonal
relationships
• Demonstrating
proficiency • Pitch
• Rhythm
• Timbre
• Neural basis
Even in everyday speech, language “syllable-timed” languages, such What’s your Style?
has a characteristic rhythm. In English, as French, don’t use this pattern of What’s more, studies have found
different syllables can vary in stress, strong and weak beats, but tend that a composer’s native language
leading to alternation of strong and to time every syllable roughly the can influence the type of music
weak beats. You can see this limerick- same. And this makes a difference to they write. A 2003 study showed
like pattern across whole sentences, listeners. You can tell the difference that English composers were more
but rhythm can also distinguish between languages from the rhythm likely to write music with more
between two words, as in OB-ject vs. alone. In fact, we might have more rhythmic variability compared to
ob-JECT. practice with this than any other French composers. This mirrors the
part of language. Although a fetus greater rhythmic variability in
It turns out that not every language
cannot make out individual words in English than French speech. What’s
has the same rhythm, though.
utero, the rhythm of speech comes incredible is that this holds up even
English is a “stress-timed” language,
through clearly — just like the beat for music without any words! Your
where strong beats tend to come
of a noisy neighbor’s stereo from native language can influence your
in a predictable rhythm. In contrast,
the next apartment. musical style.
There was a young wo- man named Bright whose speed was much fast- er than light she
set out one day in a rel- a- tive way and re- turned on a pre- vi- ous night
Although English does not use pitch led to many embarrassing situations Music to our Ears
alone to distinguish between different for people learning the language).
Have you ever repeated a word so
words, many world languages do. Tone languages can use pitch height
many times that it starts to sound
These languages, called tone to distinguish between words, or they
weird? Well, it turns out that with
languages, include Mandarin, Thai, can use pitch rise and fall within a
some types of speech, listening to it
Yoruba, Navajo, and hundreds of word, as in Mandarin. Speakers of
over and over can actually make it
others. In Mandarin, for example, the languages that use tones are more
sound like singing! This is known
syllable /ma/ can mean horse, mother, likely to develop perfect pitch, showing
as the Speech-to-Song illusion,
scold or hemp depending on the rise yet another crossover between speech
discovered by Diana Deutsch.
and fall of the voice (which must have and language processing.
Composer Steve Reich used this
phenomenon in his Grammy-award
Quickly, with a high pitch and bright timbre Slower, with a lower pitch and darker tone
Even if you’ve never had the opportunity to study music as a child, all is
not lost. A recent study of older adults taking piano lessons for the first
time showed improvements in word recognition in a noisy environment,
accompanied by stronger neural responses to attentive listening. The
upshot? It’s never too late to start taking music lessons.
Mastering language
One of the most robust findings
that scientists have seen is an
improvement in phonological
processing. The ability to recognize
different sounds of the language,
and to connect letters with the
sounds that they make, is crucial to
children mastering language. This
phonological skill is linked to reading
ability, and children with music
training tend to show better reading
abilities as well. Poor phonological
skills are often indicative of speech or
reading disorders, such as dyslexia,
and while music training shouldn’t
be thought of as a cure for these
problems, it is true that basic musical
skills, such as pitch and rhythm,
are associated with enhanced
phonological skills.
O
OVERLAP
P
PRECISION
E
EMOTION
R
REPETITION
A
ATTENTION
114 107
children in our Smart StartTM program, 106
114
group lessons for children age 0-6. 113
105
103
This program also serves as an 111 104
introduction to music theory, and uses 110
102
102
a curriculum developed to promote 109 102
the use of various cognitive skills such 108 102
Pre Post Pre Post
as attention, memory, and cognitive
flexibility. We found that these
children improved in both musical and Identification Phonemic awareness
Imitation Phonemic memory
linguistic ability over the course of a
Reading Rapid symbolic naming
school year, and see benefits to both 100% 120
90%
Percentage Correct
110
You can read more in our 105
80%
publication Music Perception. 100
rcmusic.com/musicandlanguage 70% 95
90
Another recent project at the RCM 60%
85
showed that this improved linguistic
50% 80
ability for musicians carries over into Non- Instru- Vocalist Non- Instru- Vocalist
musician mentalist musician mentalist
adulthood. Our study of university-
level music students, including those
at our Glenn Gould School, showed
than non-musicians — a useful skill a factor that can help language
that they were better at many of the
in a multi-cultural world. comprehension and its use.
same phonological and linguistic skills
that were improved among our Smart A related study also showed that Our research is helping to learn more
StartTM students. A test of ours even musicians were better than non- about the connection between music
showed that musicians could musicians at both discriminating and and language and why musical
understand foreign accents better imitating the pitch of spoken words — training can transfer across domains.
Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., & Lapidis, R. (2011). Illusory transformation from speech to song. The Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 129(4), 2245-2252.
Hutchins, S. (2018). Early childhood music training and associated improvements in music and language abilities.
Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 35(5), 579-593.
Koelsch, S., Skouras, S., Fritz, T., Herrera, P., Bonhage, C., Küssner, M. B., & Jacobs, A. M. (2013). The roles of superficial
amygdala and auditory cortex in music-evoked fear and joy. Neuroimage, 81, 49-60.
Palmer, C., & Hutchins, S. (2006). What is musical prosody?. Psychology of learning and motivation, 46, 245-278.
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and hearing, 30(6),
653-661.
Patel, A. D. (2011). Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of speech? The OPERA hypothesis. Frontiers
in psychology, 2, 142.
Patel, A. D., & Daniele, J. R. (2003). An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music. Cognition, 87(1),
B35-B45.
Patel, A. D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., & Holcomb, P. J. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and
music: An event-related potential study. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 10(6), 717-733.
Ramus, F., & Mehler, J. (1999). Language identification with suprasegmental cues: A study based on speech resynthesis.
The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(1), 512-521.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological science, 15(8), 511-514.
Trainor, L. J. (1996). Infant preferences for infant-directed versus noninfant-directed playsongs and lullabies. Infant
behavior and development, 19(1), 83-92.
Wong, P. C., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., & Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of
linguistic pitch patterns. Nature neuroscience, 10(4), 420-422.
Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., & Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: music and speech. Trends in
cognitive sciences, 6(1), 37-46.
Tel: 416.408.2825
rcmusic.com
18 THE ROYAL CONSERVATORY WHEN WORDS FAIL