Moreno-Núñez Casla Enprensa 2024
Moreno-Núñez Casla Enprensa 2024
Moreno-Núñez Casla Enprensa 2024
Aims
The overall purpose of our research was to explore the characteristics of teachers’
communicative bids during classroom-based group interactions in nursery schools. To this
goal, we explored the quantitative and qualitative variations of teachers’ multimodal
coordination based on (1) the frequency of use of gestures and their different types, (2) the
extent to which they involve the use of objects and are related to the classroom layout, (3) to
whom each communicative bid was addressed (i.e., the whole group or a single child) and, (4)
its relations with the teacher-student ratio. We expected the number of children in the
classroom could be related to the multimodal nature of teachers’ communicative bids, the use
of objects and the frequency of group-directed utterances. We also expected specific
configurations of gestures, the use of objects and the classroom layout to be coordinated with
the verbal utterances directed to the children. In particular, we considered that some
classroom layouts might favor greater opportunities for group-directed communicative bids,
while other layouts might be more inviting to address children in one-to-one interactions.
Method
Participants
This study involved 16 female teachers in charge of 16 classrooms across six public nursery
schools in middle-income urban areas of the Madrid region (Spain) and 161 children
attending the 2–3 years classrooms. Nursery schools were selected from an intentional
sampling based on previous contacts of the research team. Children’s mean age was 29.33
months (SD = 3.9 months). The number of children per classroom ranged from six to 15 (M =
10.06).
Procedure
Prior to data collection, two researchers conducted a participatory observation in each
classroom to familiarize the children with them and the recording equipment. They took field
notes on the way teachers conducted circle time activities that, in Spanish nursery schools, are
considered as a daily opportunity for promoting communication and group bonding. They are
similarly structured in different nursery schools, typically including rounds of presentations,
singing, storytelling and/or book-reading, which ensured that all sessions were comparable.
Circle time activities were selected because as structured routines they account for higher
proportions of CDS and could promote more homogeneous and frequent interactions between
children and teachers than other school activities (Chaparro-Moreno et al., 2019; Soderstrom
et al., 2018).
After the initial observation, the researchers filmed activities by placing two video cameras
attached to a tripod, one oriented towards the group of children and the other to the teacher,
who also wore a wireless microphone. This ensured that the recording angle and sound
allowed for the subsequent identification of their actions. Teachers did not receive any
instructions on how to act so as to preserve the ecological conditions of interaction. Overall,
we collected over 4 hours of footage in videos with a mean duration of 18.13 min (SD = 6.62
min).
Data Coding
Video files were imported into ELAN, an annotation tool for audio and video recordings
(Lausberg & Sloetjes, 2009), where we coded teachers’ communicative bids (i.e. each verbal
or gestural attempt to communicate with the children). Teachers’ verbal speech was
transcribed according to the CHAT program of the CHILDES project (MacWhinney, 2000),
and divided into utterances. Following Bernstein Ratner and Brundage (2015), an utterance or
conversational unit is a string of words that ends with a terminal intonation contour, has a
complete grammatical structure, or is followed by a pause longer than 2 seconds. We also
coded teachers’ communicative bids using predefined categories devised from previous
studies (Murillo et al., 2018; Rodrigo et al., 2006). A detailed list of the categories,
subcategories, and related examples can be found in Table 1. This included the type of
communicative bids, the uses of object and the types of gestures. Additionally, we coded
interactive instances according to whether the utterances were addressed to a single child
(dyadic) or to the group (polyadic), and also accounted for structural variables such as the
classroom layout and the number of children in the classroom. A second researcher
independently coded 15% of the recordings (3 out of 16 sessions). Inter-observer agreement
ranged from 89% to 99%, indicating a substantial or almost perfect degree of agreement
(Landis & Koch, 1977) between coders for the use of gestures (89%), the orientation of the
communicative bid (89%), and the use of objects (99%). Disagreements between the
researchers were discussed to refine the categories for future studies.
Table 1
Type of communicative bid Verbal: Verbal utterances of the teacher. Posing a question to the group such as
“What is the weather like today?”.
Uses of object With object: utterances performed while Showing an object to present it to the
holding an object and in reference to it group or pointing to a character in a
book while shared-reading.
Type of gestures: Pointing: gestures that indicate a distal Pointing to her ear and saying to the
Any meaning-loaded motor referent. We considered as pointing all group "Let's listen to her, please",
actions used to communicate gestures performed either with an index referring to a girl who is intervening at
with others about a referent finger extension, the whole arm, a head that moment.
or event. movement or pointing with the chin.
Symbolic: gestures that represent absent With the arms extended, clapping the
referents or actions. They have a hands in front of the body in
motivate relationship with what they representation of a crocodile’s mouth.
represent.
Orientation of the Dyadic: utterances directed to a single Asking a specific child “Who woke you
communicative bid child. up today?”.
Polyadic: utterances directed to the While looking for a book that she
group. They were defined as polyadic if: wanted to use, the teacher asked the
a) the content of the speech was clearly children “Where's the one about walking
directed to the group, b) there were down the road, kids?”.
vocatives or second person forms in
Categories Subcategories Examples
Note: For classroom layout examples, we used a dark dot to represent the teacher spatial disposition, while the children are
represented in grey color dots.
Data Analysis
Data wrangling and the calculation of descriptive statistics were performed using SPSS. We
analyzed each type of teachers’ utterances by calculating (1) the mean proportion of each type
of communicative bid over the total number of communicative bids, and (2) the mean rate per
minute (rate/min) of each type of communicative bid. While proportions allow for a general
picture of the distribution of each type of bid, their rate/min was considered a better measure
due to the different length of the video files. Furthermore, since this dependent variable
ensures the independence principle, it was used for comparing the three types of
communicative bid (verbal, gestural or a combination of both) through a repeated-measures
ANOVA where each type was considered as an intrasubject factor. The same procedure was
utilized to explore the distribution of uses of object, of each type of gesture, and whether the
utterances were directed to the group or to a single child. In addition, we used Pearson
correlations for exploring the relationship between teachers’ uses of object and the different
types of gestures, as well as with the number of children that were present in each classroom.
Results
Across all 16 classrooms, teachers performed 6575 communicative bids. The most frequent
were verbal utterances (n = 3486, 53.0%), followed by verbal-gestural bids (n = 2911,
44.3%) and gestures (n = 178, 2.7%). This same distribution applies to their rate/min
proportion (Table 2). We found a significant effect of the type of communicative bid, F(2,14)
= 344.388, p < .001, ƞ2 = .980. Pairwise comparisons based on the Bonferroni procedure show
that there are no significant differences between verbal and verbal-gestural utterances, and
that the rate of gestural communicative bids is significantly lower than verbal-gestural (p <
.001) and verbal productions (p < .001). It is worth noting that most teachers’ gesture
production did not involve the use of objects, which was only observed in a small portion of
the behaviors (n = 756, 11.5%).
Table 2. Descriptive Statistics for Teachers’ Behaviors
Behavior Frequency Rate/min (M) Rate/min (SD)
Type of bid
Verbal 3486 12.20 2.95
Gestural 178 0.62 0.33
Verbal-Gestural 2911 10.62 1.98
Uses of object
With object 756 2.14 2.57
Without object 5819 9.88 5.14
Gestures
Pointing 637 2.27 0.69
Instrumental 340 1.73 1.67
Rhythmic 262 1.03 0.67
Conventional 1190 4.58 2.23
Symbolic 660 2.39 1.41
Orientation
Dyadic 1148 4.02 1.75
Polyadic 1941 0.63 0.18
Classroom layout
Circle 10 n/a n/a
Line 1 n/a n/a
L-shape 4 n/a n/a
Undefined 1 n/a n/a
*p < .05. **p < .01.
Teachers performed unequal numbers of each type of gesture. Of the 3089 total gestures
observed (which include both solely produced gestures and verbal-gestural bids), 1190 were
conventional gestures (38.5%), 660 symbolic gestures (21.4%), 637 pointing gestures
(20.6%), 340 instrumental gestures (11.0%) and 262 were rhythmic gestures (8.5%). We
found a significant effect of the type of gesture, F(4,12) = 14.959, p < .001, ƞ2 = .833.
Conventional gestures were more frequent than all other types of gestures, with significant
differences with pointing (p = .014) and rhythmic gestures (p < .001). We also found that the
differences between conventional and instrumental gestures were marginally significant (p =
.053), while there was no significance between conventional gestures and the production of
symbolic gestures (p = .065). Teachers’ use of rhythmic gestures was the least frequent,
showing significant differences with pointing (p < .001) and symbolic gestures (p = .033).
Interestingly, our data showed that, overall, most bids including gestures were directed to the
group (n = 1941, 62.8%), while only around a third of the total gesture production were
addressed to a single child (n = 1148, 37.2%). Moreover, the rate/min of dyadic and polyadic
gestures significantly differ, F(1,15) = 49.733, p < .001, ƞ2 =.768. Regarding the distribution
of each type of gesture, we found a similar trend in gestures addressed to the group and to
single children (Figure 1), as evidenced by the effect of the type of gestures with both dyadic,
F(4,12) = 18.268, p < .001, ƞ2 = .859, and polyadic instances, F(4,12) = 5.061, p = .013, ƞ2 =
.628. This probably results from the higher frequency observed in the production of
conventional gestures. However, certain types of gestures were more frequent in polyadic
than in dyadic interchanges, as observed for rhythmic, F(1,15) = 26.437, p < .001, ƞ2 = .638,
and symbolic gestures, F(1,15) = 33.284, p < .001, ƞ2 = .689.
Figure 1
Distribution of each type of gesture according to the orientation of the communicative bids
(dyadic vs. polyadic)
In order to analyze the relationship between classroom ratios, the type of gesture and the use
of objects while gesturing, we ran a series of Pearson bivariate correlations based on the
rate/min of each type of gesture and the number of children that were enrolled in each
classroom. We found negative and significant correlations between conventional gesture
rate/min and the number of children in the classroom, r(16) = -.547, p = .028. Also, similar
results arise from the relationship between gestures and the use of objects, r(16) = -.551, p =
.027.
Finally, in order to explore the relationship between the classroom layout and the type of
interaction strategies that teachers promote, we classified each participating classroom
according to the participants’ layout (circle, n = 10; lines, n = 1; L-shape, n = 4; undefined, n
= 1). Our results showed that the use of verbal-gestural communicative bids is similar across
all classrooms, except for the classroom with an undefined layout, where the teacher used
more verbal utterances (Figure 2). Furthermore, teachers posed unequal uses of gestures with
objects, which were very rare when children were sitting in a circle or an L-shape distribution.
The distribution of gestures directed to the group and to a single child also differed. Dyadic
gestures were more frequent in classrooms where children were arranged in lines or in an L-
shape. Note that the classrooms in which children were interacting in a circle or in an
undefined layout were the ones that showed the most similar pattern of those observed in the
full sample. However, distribution patterns of each type of gesture varied across classroom
layouts. While, as shown before, the full sample showed a greater proportion of conventional
gestures, we found that this was only observed in those classrooms where children were
sitting in a circle or L-shape. In turn, instrumental gestures were more frequent in lineal and
undefined layouts where, in fact, this comprised more than 60% of the teachers’ gesture
production.
[FIGURE 002]
Figure 2
Proportion of each type of gesture according to the classroom layout
We observed very low frequencies of some types of gestures in some classrooms, that might
be related to the spatial organization of the children. For instance, instrumental gestures were
the least frequent in L-shaped classrooms, and there was a small representation of pointing
gestures in classrooms that were organized in a line or according to an undefined layout (less
than 8%). The use of rhythmic gestures was typically low in all classrooms.
References
<jrn>Alessandroni, N. (2023). The road to conventional tool use: Developmental changes in
children’s material engagement with artifacts in nursery school. Infancy, 28(2), 388–409.
https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12522 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Bautista, A., Moreno-Núñez, A., Vijayakumar, P., Quek, E., & Bull, R. (2020). Gross
motor teaching in preschool education: Where, what, and how do Singapore educators
teach? Infancia y Aprendizaje, 43(2), 443–482.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02103702.2019.1653057</jrn>
<jrn>Berge, A. (2019). ‘I believe that a sofa can be of great help’: Materiality in a
kindergarten room and the impact on social practices. International Journal of Early
Childhood, 51(1), 59–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13158-019-00235-6</jrn>
<eref>Bernstein Ratner, N., & Brundage, S. (2015). A clinician’s complete guide to CLAN
and PRAAT. Talkbank Organization. https://talkbank.org/manuals/Clin-
CLAN.pdf</eref>
<jrn>Cárdenas, K., Moreno-Núñez, A., & Miranda-Zapata, E. (2020). Shared book-reading in
early childhood education: Teachers’ mediation in children’s communicative
development. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 2030.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02030 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Casla, M., Méndez-Cabezas, C., Montero, I., Murillo, E., Nieva, S., & Rodríguez, J.
(2022). Spontaneous verbal repetition in toddler-adult conversations: A longitudinal
study with Spanish-speaking two-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 49(2), 266–301.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000921000015 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Chaparro-Moreno, L. J., Justice, L. M., Logan, J. A. R., Purtell, K. M., & Lin, T. J.
(2019). The preschool classroom linguistic environment: Children’s first person
experiences. PLoS One, 14(8), Article e0220227.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220227 PubMed</jrn>
<bok>Cremin, T., Flewitt, R., Mardell, B., & Swann, J. (2017). Storytelling in early
childhood: Enriching language, literacy, and classroom culture. Routledge.</bok>
<jrn>Dalgaard, N. T., Bondebjerg, A., Klokker, R., Viinholt, B. C. A., & Dietrichson, J.
(2022). Adult/child ratio and group size in early childhood education or care to promote
the development of children aged 0–5 years: A systematic review. Campbell Systematic
Reviews, 18(2), Article e1239. https://doi.org/10.1002/cl2.1239 PubMed</jrn>
<edb>Degotardi, S. (2021). The language environment of infant child care: Issues of quantity,
quality, participation, and context. In O. Saracho (Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on
research on child care in early childhood education (pp. 85-107). Information Age
Publishing.</edb>
<jrn>Degotardi, S., Han, F., & Torr, J. (2018). Infants’ experience with ‘near and clear’
educator talk: Individual variation and its relationship to indicators of quality.
International Journal of Early Years Education, 26(3), 278–294.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2018.1479632</jrn>
<jrn>García, J. L., Heckman, J. J., Leaf, D. E., & Prados, M. J. (2020). Quantifying the life-
cycle benefits of an influential early-childhood program. Journal of Political Economy,
128(7), 2502–2541. https://doi.org/10.1086/705718 PubMed</jrn>
<ths>Hansen, J. E. (2018). Educational language practices and language development in
early childhood education and care [Doctoral dissertation, University of Stavanger].
https://hdl.handle.net/11250/2712081</ths>
<jrn>Hindman, A. H., Farrow, J. M., Anderson, K., Wasik, B. A., & Snyder, P. A. (2021).
Understanding child-directed speech around book reading in toddler classrooms:
Evidence from Early Head Start programs. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 719783.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.719783 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Iverson, J. M., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain: The dynamic emergence of
speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11-12), 19–40.</jrn>
<jrn>Justice, L. M., Jiang, H., & Strasser, K. (2018). Linguistic environment of preschool
classrooms: What dimensions support children’s language growth? Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 42(1), 79–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.09.003</jrn>
<jrn>Kärtner, J. (2018). Beyond dichotomies—(M)others’ structuring and the development of
toddlers’ prosocial behavior across cultures. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 6–10.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.040 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for
categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310
PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Lausberg, H., & Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-
ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, 41(3), 841–849.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.3.841 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Løkken, G., & Moser, T. (2012). Space and materiality in early childhood pedagogy –
Introductory notes. Education Inquiry, 3(3), 303–315.
https://doi.org/10.3402/edui.v3i3.22036</jrn>
<bok>MacWhinney, B. (2000). El Proyecto CHILDES: Herramientas para el análisis del
habla. Lawrence Erlbaum.</bok>
<jrn>Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & del Olmo, M. J. (2017). Rhythmic ostensive
gestures: How adults facilitate babies’ entrance into early triadic interactions. Infant
Behavior and Development, 49, 168–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.09.003
PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Murillo, E., Ortega, C., Otones, A., Rujas, I., & Casla, M. (2018). Changes in the
synchrony of multimodal communication in early language development. Journal of
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 61(9), 2235–2245.
https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0402 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Perniss, P. (2018). Why we should study multimodal language. Frontiers in Psychology,
9, Article 1109. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01109 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Perry, L. K., Prince, E. B., Valtierra, A. M., Rivero-Fernández, C., Ullery, M. A., Katz,
L. F., Laursen, B., & Messinger, D. S. (2018). A year in words: The dynamics and
consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom. PLoS One, 13(7),
Article e0199893. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199893 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Pianta, R. C. (2016). Teacher-student interactions: Measurement, impacts, improvement,
and policy. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(1), 98–105.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215622457</jrn>
<jrn>Poveda, D. (2003). Paths to participation in classroom conversations. Linguistics and
Education, 14(1), 69–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0898-5898(03)00007-X</jrn>
<jrn>Rodrigo, M. J., González, A., Ato, M., Rodríguez, G., de Vega, M., & Muñetón, M.
(2006). Co-development of child–mother gestures over the second and the third years.
Infant and Child Development, 15(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.412</jrn>
<jrn>Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later
language learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 182–187.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00764.x PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Rowe, M. L., & Snow, C. E. (2020). Analyzing input quality along three dimensions:
Interactive, linguistic, and conceptual. Journal of Child Language, 47(1), 5–21.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000655 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Schaffer, H. R., Hepburn, A., & Collis, G. M. (1983). Verbal and nonverbal aspects of
mothers’ directives. Journal of Child Language, 10(2), 337–355.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900007807 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Schmidt, C. L. (1996). Scrutinizing reference: How gesture and speech are coordinated
in mother-child interaction. Journal of Child Language, 23(2), 279–305.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008801 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Soderstrom, M., Grauer, E., Dufault, B., & McDivitt, K. (2018). Influences of number
of adults and adult: Child ratios on the quantity of adult language input across childcare
settings. First Language, 38(6), 563–581.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723718785013</jrn>
<jrn>Soderstrom, M., & Wittebolle, K. (2013). When do caregivers talk? The influence of
activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare
environments. PLoS One, 8(11), Article 80646.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080646 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Suanda, S. H., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2016). The multisensory nature of verbal
discourse in parent–toddler interactions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 41(5-8), 324–
341. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2016.1256403 PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Tamis-LeMonda, C., Song, L., Smith Leavell, A., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Yoshikawa,
H. (2012). Ethnic differences in mother–infant language and gestural communications are
associated with specific skills in infants. Developmental Science, 15(3), 384–397.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01136.x PubMed</jrn>
<jrn>Torr, J. (2019). Infants’ experiences of shared reading with their educators in early
childhood education and care centres: An observational study. Early Childhood
Education Journal, 47(5), 519–529. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-019-00948-2</jrn>
<jrn>Vermeer, H. J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Cárcamo, R. A., & Harrison, L. J. (2016).
Quality of child care using the environment rating scales: A meta-analysis of
international studies. International Journal of Early Childhood, 48(1), 33–60.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13158-015-0154-9</jrn>
<jrn>White, E. J., Peter, M., & Redder, B. (2015). Infant and teacher dialogue in education
and care: A pedagogical imperative. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30, 160–173.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.10.008</jrn>
<jrn>Wu, Z., & Gros-Louis, J. (2014). Infants’ prelinguistic communicative acts and maternal
responses: Relations to linguistic development. First Language, 34(1), 72–90.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723714521925</jrn>
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the staff and families from the participating nursery schools for their
invaluable cooperation with our studies.
Funding
This research was supported by project PID2019-108845GA-
I00/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and project PID2021-123907NB-I00, both co-funded by
the State Research Agency (Ministry of Science and Innovation, Spain) and the European
Social Fund (ESF).
Competing Interests
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or
financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Biographical Statement
Ana Moreno-Núñez is Associate Professor in the Department of Developmental and
Educational Psychology, UAM. She leads the research group “Musical dynamics and early
triadic interactions” (MusicalETI), where she investigates early cognitive and communicative
development and, in particular, infants' engagement in interactions around materiality under
the mediation of adults.
Marta Casla is Professor in the Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology,
UAM. She is interested in early linguistic development from a multimodal point of view, also
considering the role of adults and group interactions in educational contexts. She has
participated in different studies using both experimental and qualitative methods, and
naturalistic data.