Previewpdf

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 64

FAMILY AND

HUMANDEVELOPMENT
ACROSS CULTURES

A View From the Other Side


This page intentionally left blank
FAMILY AND
HUMAN DEVELOPMENT
ACROSS CULTURES

A View From the Other Side

<;igdem KagItC;lba~1
Koc University

\f ~~I~~~~~i?G9a~P Press
NEW YORK AND LONDON
First published 1996 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Published 2014 by Psychology Press
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY, 10017
and by Psychology Press
27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex, BN3 2FA

Psychology Press is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
Copyright © 1996, by Lawrence Er/baum Associates, Inc.
All rights reserved. No part ofthis book may be reprinted or reproduced or
utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now
known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any
information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from
the publishers.

Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or


registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation
without intent to infringe.

Library of Congress Cata10ging-in-Publication Data

Käglt'i1ba§l, <;igdem
Family and human development across cultures a view from the other side I
<;igdem Käglt'i1ba§1.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and indexes.
ISBN 0-8058-2076-0 (cloth). - ISBN 0-8058-2077-9 (pbk.)
ISBN 978-0-805-82077-5 (pbk)
I. Socialization-Cross-cultural studies. 2. Family-Psychological aspects-Cross-cultural
studies. 3. Developmental psychology-Cross-cultural studies. 4. Cultural relativism.
5. Ethnopsychology. I. TitIe.
HQ783.K34 1996
303.3'2-dc20 95-47534
CIP

Publisher's Note
The publisher has gone to great lengths to ensure the quality
of this reprint but points out that some imperfections in the
original may be apparent.
To the memory oi my mother
This page intentionally left blank
Contents

Foreword M. Brewster Smith ix

Preface xiii

1. Introduction 1

PART I: HUMAN DEVELOPMENT, FAMILY, CULTURE

2. Development in Context 19

3. Socialization for Competence 35

4. Culture and Self 52

5. Family and Family Change 72

PART ,,: INDUCED CHANGE: EARLY ENRICHMENT

6. Induced Change: The Role of Psychology 101

7. Early Childhood Care and Education (ECCE): An Overview 124

8. The Turkish Early Enrichment Project 139

9. Search for Integration and Policy Relevance 170

vii
viii CONTENTS

References 187

Author Index 217

Subject Index 227


Foreword

M. Brewster Smith
University of California, Santa Cruz

This is an extraordinary book by an author who needs no introduction in the


world of international psychology. <;igdem Kaglt<;lba§l (g is silent in Turkish,
lengthening the preceding vowel; <; has the value of eh, § of sh), who is professor
of psychology at Koc University in Istanbul and past president of the Turkish
Psychological Association, is also former president of the International Associa-
tion of Cross-Cultural Psychology and two-term member of the executive com-
mittee of the International Union of Scientific Psychology. In 1993 she received
the American Psychological Association's award for Distinguished Contributions
to the International Advancement of Psychology. This volume represents the
culmination of a distinguished career, wh ich I have been privileged to follow
with admiration ever since her graduate study at the University of California,
Berkeley, some three decades ago. In that sense, as her preface attests, it is a very
personal book.
But it is also an exemplary, up-to-the-moment exposition of a view of human
and social development "from the other side"-from the perspective of what
Kaglt<;lba§l aptly calls the Majority World (no longer sensibly labeled Third
World) of countries that do not participate fully in the benefits and problems of
the industrial and postindustrial West. Further, it is an authoritative presentation
of the cross-cultural perspective, as an essential corrective to the unthinking
culture-boundedness of much Euro-American psychology. It is a searching analy-
sis of perspectives on human development that escape the pitfalls of extreme
relativism to which advocates of a culturally contextual approach are vulnerable,
in terms of criteria of cognitive competence and of developing selfhood in which
autonomy and relatedness are in balance. It is an original contribution to the
theory of family change, opposing the expectation of modernization theory that

ix
X FOREWORD

families in the developing world will converge on the Western individualistic


model. It is a wise and constructive analysis of the role of psychology in inducing
social change, focused on problems of the Majority World but just as relevant to
the Euro-American context. And it is a critical review of research and theory on
early childhood care and education (ECCE) in the United States and in the
Majority World; ECCE is the particular mode of intervention, in the family
context, with which she has been involved. In each of these widely dispersed
topical areas, the breadth and depth of her command of the specialized literature
is amazing. Readers who are already weIl acquainted with some of them (who
else will be familiar with them all?) will respect her guidance to the very exten-
sive bibliography in their own areas of competence and we1come it in the areas
new to them.
All of the foregoing comes to concrete focus in her account of the Turkish
Early Enrichment Project. This Lewin-style action-research, wh ich Kagit<;lba§l
conceived and directed in the shantytowns of Istanbul, is exemplary in a number
of respects. It was planned with sensitivity to the Turkish cultural context, but
with nonrelativistic standards of cognitive and social competence in child devel-
opment in mind. The intervention focused on the family, specifically the mother,
as the primary context of socialization in the preschool years. The research
design permitted appropriately controlled comparisons, with multiple outcome
measures. Strikingly positive short-term results were obtained, expecially on
cognitive and school-related measures, for the effects of training the mothers and
of educational day-care centers. In the almost unprecedented lO-year follow-up,
major effects of mother training persisted (inc1uding empowering benefits to the
mothers themselves), whereas the advantages of day-care center experience dissi-
pated with time. The findings make sense in terms of Kaglt<;lba§l's interpretation
that intervention with the mothers reoriented the functioning of the family sys-
tems of participants in ways that were self-sustaining. She then draws the book
together, making explicit how her project, which has had substantial impact on
Turkish programming for early childhood enrichment, suggests useful ways in
which psychology in the Majority World can become engaged with national and
pan-human objectives of global human development.
I would like to highlight several respects in wh ich Kagit<;lba§l provides intel-
lectualleadership that is much needed by psychology in the Euro-American and
Majority Worlds alike. These are contributions of the book that I enthusiastically
applaud.
For one, I am delighted with the way that she has enlarged on the recent
critique of the individualism of Euro-American culture. Many voices in psychol-
ogy and the social sciences, inc1uding feminists but not restricted to them, would
give higher priority to values of relatedness, as complementing the individualis-
tic values of agency or autonomy. From her Majority World perspective and her
command of the large recent literature documenting the prevalence of "collectiv-
ist" values outside Euro-American cultural precincts, she challenges moderniza-
FOREWORD xi

tion theory when it predicts the supplanting of relatedness values in the less
developed world by Western individualism. True, economic development with
its accompanying urbanization is a strong cOITosive for many aspects of tradition-
al cultures. But the example of Japan and other East Asian "success stories"
indicates that relatedness values can survive economic development. Addressing
her colleagues in the Majority World, she calls for new goals of personal autono-
my in a context that preserves the values of relatedness distinctive of non-
Western cultures. Her mother-training ECCE project demonstrates that it is pos-
sible to apply psychology toward realizing this objective, wh ich she sees as
humanly desirable for the Have"Nots as it is for the Haves.
Recently, a polarity has become salient contrasting the cross-cultural psychol-
ogy of Triandis, Berry, Bond, and others with the cultural psychology of Cole,
Rogoff, Shweder, and their associates. The former is charged by its critics with
continuing the outmoded positivist tradition, employing imposed "etic" dimen-
sions of comparison drawn inappropriately from individualistic Western culture;
the latter is committed to contextual, "ernie" treatment of experience and behav-
ior in each culture's own terms, but is vulnerable to sometimes nihilistic relativ-
ism. Kaglt($lba§l comes from the cross-cultural tradition, of wh ich she is an
acknowledged leader, but she insists, articulately and persuasively, that cultural
and cross-cultural approaches are complementary, not competing; she believes
psychology can give proper attention to cultural context without giving up its
strategies of comparison or its aims to discover principles that transcend his tory
and culture. (Rogoff, on her part, also does not see the approaches in opposition
to one another.)
Her integrative approach comes to a focus in her insistence on the relevance of
standards of human development that apply across nations and cultures and
across the baITier between Haves and Have-Nots in Euro-American societies.
This attempt to combine cultural contextualism with universalistic standards is a
complex, difficult intellectual maneuver, not an easy political compromise. I
think it is the right choice. It has become politically incoITect to regard school-
related cognitive competences as more "developed" than the less abstractive
competencies of children in premodern rural societies or streetwise culture; the
presently indispensable value of formal schooling is even disparaged. Kaglt-
($lba§l reminds us that in the Majority World, universal schooling and literacy is a
consensual/objective engraved in the Universal Declaration of Human Rights
and the Convention on Rights of the Child but only very partially attained,
expecially for girls and women. "Middle-class" cognitive competencies are very
much needed to cope with the complexities of the contemporary world, whether
on the part of disadvantaged minorities in the United States or the hordes of rural
immigrants to the exploding cities of the Majority World. As she observes, it is
only a step from the extreme relativism of some postmodern admirers of tradi-
tional culture to implicit advocacy of a double standard: first-class preschools
and elementary schools for OUT children to prepare them for OUT world, but, for
xii FOREWORD

instance, rote learning in Koranic schools (or no schools, for girls) as preserving
the valued culture of Those Others. The ethicallpolitical issues are difficult, but
KagltC;lba§l's position is courageous and clear.
Those who disagree in principle with her stance that calls for contextualism
without complete relativism will not only have to contend with her argument;
they will also have to come to terms with the implications of her intervention
research on early childhood enrichment and mother empowerment. I have recent-
ly been involved in the armchair defense of scientific empiricism in psychology
from postmodernist assault, trying to sustain the human effort to approach truth
and goodness in formulations that transcend the particular historicocultural con-
texts from which they are derived. I am impressed that KagltC;lba§l's demonstra-
tion of the effectiveness of her mother-training intervention in a Majority World
setting is a much stronger answer to the extreme cultural relativists than any
philosophical metatheoretical argument. The concrete example is persuasive:
relevant culturally contextual scientific research can be done and it is useful and
influential on public policy. Articulate elites in the developing countries will
want to support interventions such as she exemplified; so will the participants
themselves as they encounter such programs. This is the way to go!
This volume will immediately become obligatory reading and a valuable
resource to the band of cultural and cross-cultural psychologists, still too few,
who are committed to reshaping mainstream Euro-American psychology so as to
make it more truly universal, less unwittingly culturebound. It should have equal
immediate interest to psychologists in the Majority World and Euro-American
psychologists who want to collaborate with them in challenging enterprise of
applying psychology to the problems of societal development. But the appro-
priate audience for this book as textbook and resource is much broader.
KagltC;lba§l's view of family and human development "from the other side" is
very relevant to the concerns of mainstream psychologists on "this" side of the
great divide between Have and Have-Not societies. We Euro-Americans have
parallel problems in relating to internal Have-Not minorities. In our current well-
motivated wave of political correctness, we may be bemused by similar qualms
of relativism concerning the relevance of "middle-class" cognitive standards to
children and adults who have been systematically excluded from middle-class
opportunities and benefits. The wave of support for early intervention projects
peaked with President 10hnson's Great Society programs, and these projects
mostly neglected the family context of early child development. Perhaps
KagltC;lba§l's hope-inspiring example from Istanbul may stimulate blase Ameri-
can psychologists to take heart, and once more seriously address the problem of
bringing excluded minorities into full citizenship. Certainly, she has given pro-
fessors and students of developmental psychology and of family relations much
to ponder.
Preface

A PERSONAL ACCOUNT

Interpersonal Relations
A few years aga I was invited to participate in a symposium at an international
congress of cross-cultural psychology. I was asked to present a personal account
of how I became a cross-cultural psychologist, mainly in terms of how I came to
be involved in cross-cultural research. That task helped me look back and delve
into the background of my present academic interests. It was a process of recon-
struction. As I tried to pinpoint the beginning of my academic interest in cross-
cultural or cultural psychology, I found myself going back further and further.
So, an attempted academic reconstruction turned into an autobiographical recon-
struction. Obviously, everyone's work must reflect personal experience, though
this is rarely made explicit. I would like to start this book by making this personal
experience explicit. This is because I believe this exercise might help put what I
have to say into some perspective-an international perspective at that.
I was a graduate student in the social psychology program at the University of
California, Berkeley during the 1960s. I was wen versed in the social psycho-
logicalliterature of the 1950s and 1960s. But, what intrigued me more than the
highly popular cognitive dissonance theory and the lure of the experimental
laboratory was the social, political, and psychological implications of the then-
no-Ionger-in-vogue authoritarian personality theory (Adorno, Frenkel-Bruns-
wick, Levinson, & Sanford; 1950). This was despite the influential methodologi-
cal critique of Christie and Jahoda (1954). So, against my supervisor and mentor
M. Brewster Smith's good advice, I undertook a cross-cultural comparative

xiii
xiv PREFACE

study, instead of a clean laboratory experiment, for my doctoral research to test


the cross-cultural generality of the authoritarian personality theory. Although I
called myself a social psychologist, I was in fact a cross-cultural psychologist at
heart at a time when nobody I knew was doing "cross-cultural psychology."
Yet, this was only a "natural" development given the fact that I was a foreign
student in the United States and was using, almost automatically, a cultural filter
in my reading. Thus, my study emerged out of the realization when I read the
Authoritarian Personality (Adomo et al. , 1950) that some of the characteristics
of the so-called authoritarian personality were in fact social norms in Turkey. The
following is the opening paragraph of the resultant publication, which describes
the general view underlying the study.

Some findings of social psychology may refer to general panhuman relationships,


others to relationships that hold only within specific sociocultural settings. Only
systematic cross-cultural comparison can separate these or identify the limits within
which particular generalizations hold. An example of findings that seem Iikely to
be culturally specific are those in support of a general syndrome of "authoritarian-
ism." In cultures in which social norms bear differentially on the components of
this syndrome, one should expect different patterns of relationship to obtain. Such
contrasts were anticipated between the United States and Turkey. This studyexam-
ines the assumed inherent dynamic organization of personality attributes and attitu-
dinal variables underlying "authoritarianism" in the light of a cross-cultural com-
parison. (KagJt~!ba§!, 1970, p. 444).

I maintain the above view in a general sense some 25 years later. I believe it
fonns a raison d'etre of cross-cultural psychology, which sheds important light
on human behavior.
This early realization of cross-cultural variability in some "basic" personality
characteristics, assumed to be universal, had something to do with my own early
experiences in culture contact, in addition to being a foreign student at Berkeley.
When they occurred, they were simple events; only much later have I been able
to put them into perspective, attributing to them a culturally situated meaning. I
want to relate some of these experiences, going further back in time.
As a teenager I was a boarding student at an American school for girls in
Istanbul, Turkey. There was much physical contact among girls (kissing on both
cheeks, embracing, walking ann in ann in the corridors, in the garden, court-
yard, etc.) as a natural part of interpersonal affection and wann peer relations.!
We used to get a kick out of the shocked glances of the new American teachers
(mostly young women) before they got acculturated to "the ways ofthe natives."

IThere is still much more physical contact among same-sex friends and kin in "contact" cultures
such as Turkey than in "noncontact" Western cultures. However, the absolute amount of physical
contact is probably less today than before, especially among the educated middle-class groups in
those societies due to "cultural diffusion," especially through the media, of the Western models.
A PERSONAL ACCOUNT xv

Some mischievous girls used to overdo the show of affection for its shock value
in the presence of these teachers.
After I graduated from the American school in Istanbul and was accepted at
Wellesley College (in Massachusetts, USA), an important part of my self-
induced preparation/orientation to life in the United States was to restrain myself
from showing physical affection.
During my last years at the American school in Istanbul I studied British and
American literature and 20th-century philosophy, which focused on phenome-
nology and existentialism. Apart from scholarly work by Kierkegaard, Jaspers,
Husserl, Heidegger, and Sartre, I also read some plays by Sartre. These were
my earliest scholarly contacts with the pervasive individualistic perspective in
Western philosophy and literary tradition. The closing words of Sartre's No
Exit, "Hell is other people" were stamped in my memory. This statement in-
trigued me in its utmost strangeness. Today I see it as a reflection of extreme
individualism.
My first impression of "suburban America" (Newton Center, Massachusetts),
where I stayed with an American family, was beautiful hornes, spacious gardens
with lovely trees and flowers, clean streets, and no people. I often wondered
where the people were. Not once did I meet any neighbors or see my host family
visiting them.
I had a close friend at Wellesley, whom I visited and stayed with at her
family's horne during some short vacations. One day when she was very sad, I
asked her what was wrong. She said, "It is a personal matter." I was shattered.
This was clearly a rejection for me; obviously she did not consider me very close
if she could not confide in me. In my understanding there could not be anything
that I would withhold from my best friend; I would at least let her know the
nature of the problem even if I did not reveal the details. What for her was a
simple assertion of her privacy was for me a declaration of rejection. The memo-
ry is still vivid after more than 30 years.
These everyday events and experiences have all had to do with what I consider
today abasie aspect of interpersonal relations showing cross-cultural variation,
that is, interpersonal connectedness (relatedness) or separateness. From another
perspective, they have to do with the self, again in terms of its level of individua-
tion (separateness, boundedness) or connectedness with others. Obviously, there
were many other experiences involving misunderstanding or readjustment during
my student years in the United States, and there have been others since then in
my international contacts. However, I find it important that most of the episodes
I remember, like those I have mentioned, have to do with the connectedness-
separateness dimension of interpersonal relations.
Some episodes may be eye opening even if one is no longer a "naive student."
Consider a conversation I had a few years ago with a well-known North Ameri-
can cross-cultural psychologist friend and his wife. When I asked after their son,
who was about 21 years old at the time, my friend said he was staying with them
xvi PREFACE

in their horne, but they were not charging hirn rent. I couldn't believe what I
heard and wondered if he was joking; he was not. 2
Yet another episode occured while I was on a sabbatical leave at Harvard
University and Radcliffe College (Bunting Institute) some ten years ago. I be-
came acquainted with a renowned anthropologist, several years my senior. One
day when we were getting into the back seat of a car I attempted to help her in
first. My behavior offended her; she said she was not old enough to need help
getting into a car. My behavior was a reflection of my respect and appreciation
for her age and accomplishments. She took it as an insult to her independence
and autonomy. The interesting point here is that I, the cross-cultural psycholo-
gist, let my old country values take precedence over my knowledge of American
values, and she, the highly experienced anthropologist, did not recognize that.
Experiences like these remind one of the cross-cultural diversity in the inter-
personal relations sphere that goes deep into cultural meaning systems and con-
ventions. This diversity exists side by side with a remarkable commonality,
deriving from our common biologically based human nature and our immense
intercultural-intemational communication systems, including similar educational
experiences, working as strong converging and unifying forces. If even among
educated people, culturally sensitive and intemationally minded social scientists
at that, there could be such differences in understanding, there would naturally be
greater differences among common people immersed in their own cultures.
The aforementioned experiential examples demonstrate how living in another
cultural context or acculturation through early exposure to another culture sens i-
tizes one to culture, as such. It is very much like "the fish in water"; you "see"
culture when you get out of it. What is probably even more important, however,
is that once you become conscious of it, you cannot ignore it. This is probably
the summary of my first involvement with cultural and cross-cultural psycholo-
gy. It has important implications for the kind of psychology I practice and that I
think should be pursued.
Of particular significance to me is to understand the underlying dimensions of
interpersonal relations, their variation across cultures, and their antecedents. The
interpersonal connectedness-separateness dimension, as reflected in the previous
personal episodes, signals a clue to an understanding of the self and its development
in context. This context is the family and, moving out from it, the sociocultural
environment. The observed variations in self-construals and self-other relations
appear to be deeply rooted in the cross-cultural diversities in contexts (Kaglt<;lba~l,
1990; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). How and why these variations come about is,
for me, the key question to understanding an important aspect of human reality.

2Another North American colleague interpreted this episode to mean that I was surprised my
friend was not charging rent to his son! In case other Western readers also misinterpret what I have
written, let me explain: I was surprised that it would even occur to my friend to charge rent to his own
son.
A PERSONAL ACCOUNT xvii

Social Relevance
Another thread of influence running through both my personal life and profes-
sional career is a deep concern for and a commitment to social well-being. The
roots of this commitment go back to my early socialization in family and school,
which for me coincided. Both my parents were teachers with a mission to
contribute to the education and development of a modem secular society out of
the ashes of an old one based on tradition and religion. 3 They started their own
private school with very limited funds. At age 2 I found mys elf in school and I
have been there all my life. I was brought up with the ideal of "doing something
worthwhile for society," an idea nourished especially by my mother.
In retrospect, such ideals were taken seriously by many young people of my
generation, especially among the children of the educated teachers and civil
servants who carried considerable responsibility for "building a modem nation."
Indeed, early studies conducted in the post-World War 11 period among youth in
developing countries point to much higher "patriotism" and reveal a great value
for "doing something good for one's family and country," as compared with
American youth. For example, Gillespie and Allport (1955) talked about the
strong national loyalties of youth in newly emergent nations in the process of
nation building.
The historical context of nation building probably did make the loyalty feIt to
the nation more salient in young persons' values. However, as evidenced by a
great deal of subsequent research and current work, this is not the whole story.
For example, in my 1966 comparative study of Turkish and American adoles-
cents, I found the same high level of nationalloyalty among the Turkish sampie,
for whom nation building was not relevant. In contrast, American adolescents
valued personal achievement and happiness (Kaglt<;lba§l, 1970). Furtherrnore, in
a later study with Turkish adolescents (Kaglt<;lba§l, 1973), I found patriotism
(loyalty to the country) to fit into a "modem" outlook and to be associated with
belief in internal control of reinforcement, optimism, and achievement motiva-
tion. It was negatively associated with a more traditional outlook, characterized
by religiosity, authoritarianism, and belief in external control of reinforcement.
Even when achievement motivation is studied, which is often assumed to
focus on the self, the same loyalty to society can be seen. Thus Phalet and Claeys
(1993) found Turkish adolescents (both in Turkey and in Belgium) to combine
individual and group loyalties (Kaglt<;lba§l, 1987b) into a "social achievement
motivation," contrasted with the individualistic achievement motivation of the
Belgian youth. Similar findings of socially oriented achievement motivation have
been reported for the Japanese (DeVos, 1968), the Indians (Agarwal & Misra,

3The republican secularist reforms were in full swing. They had been started by Atatürk and the
founders of the Turkish Republic after the war for independence was won in early 1920s, following
the collapse of the Ottoman Empire after World War I. The ties with the six centuries of Ottoman past
were severed and by 1940s the reforms were consolidated.
xviii PREFACE

1986; Misra & Agarwal, 1985), and the Chinese (Bond, 1986, p. 36; K-S. Yang,
1986, pp. 113,114).
The previous examples point to the continuing pervasiveness of the loyalty
and commitment to entities transcending the self in the so-called collectivistic
cultures. In the individualistic culture, however, it has been claimed that "the
primary loyalty is to the self-its values, autonomy, pleasure, virtue and actual-
ization" (Kagan, 1984). I do not mean to infer here value judgments about what
is good and what is bad, but rather to point to differences in emphasis in focusing
on the self or on the larger collectivity in which the self is embedded. As
becomes apparent in the following chapters, this is one of the central themes in
the book.
There has been some recent questioning of the vulnerability of "lives orga-
nized around self-actualization and the pursuit of gratification" and a recognition
of the fact that "human lives seem most meaningful and satisfying when they are
devoted to projects and guided by values that transcend the self" (M. B. Smith,
1994, p.407). Other critics have also expressed their concerns with too much
individualism, especially in the Uni ted States (e.g., Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan,
Swidler, & Tipton, 1985; Cushman, 1990; Lasch, 1979, 1984; Sampson, 1987;
Schwartz, 1986; Taylor, 1989). There have been pleas for a greater commitment
to society (Etzioni, 1993; Sarason, 1981, 1988; M. A. Wallach & L. Wallach,
1983, 1990).
To some extent these recent developments have had an effect in making my
own commitments more salient for me, which I have expressed here. The differ-
ence between the individualistic and the collectivistic concerns appears to be
continuing. It is understandable, therefore, that much of Western academic psy-
chology is still somewhat oblivious of societal problems ,4 but in contrast there is
a loud cry from the collectivistic Majority World 5 for a more socially relevant
psychology that assumes responsibility for societal development (e.g.,
Kaglt~lba§l, 1991d, 1994b; Nsamenang, 1992; D. Sinha, 1983; D. Sinha & Kao,
1988). Some Western cross-cultural psychologists have also joined in, as in an
early call by lahoda (1975).
The stress on the social relevance and applied significance of psychology is a
key to my general orientation to it. This orientation is deep seated in both my
personal and academic background and cultural context. Thus I see psychologi-
cal inquiry not only as an important tool in understanding behavior but also in
changing it, at a macrolevel, to improve the human condition. This may be seen

4There are some signs that this may be changing . See note 23 below.
51 am using the "Majority WorId." instead of "Developing Countries" or the "Third WorId." The
developing countries are not getting any cIoser to the developed countries (if anything, the gap is
widening), and with the coIIapse of the "Second World," the "Third" does not make much sense.
Majority WorId, referring in fact to the majority of the worId's population, emerges as a preferable
term.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS xix

as an overly ambitious or presumptuous view of the field, if not also a naive one.
Even though I am weH aware of some truth in such an objection, as weH as of the
multiple causation of human phenomena, including to a large extent non-
psychological causes, I am, nevertheless, of the opinion that psychology does
have the potential to contribute to the improvement of the human condition.
My work during the last 15 years has involved research along two different but
related paths. One of these has been theoretical in orientation, whereas the other
has been more problem oriented with an applied emphasis. These seemingly
disparate research interests have been quite integrated in my own thinking, and I
hope to reflect this integration in this book.
Thus, on the one hand, I study self-family-culture interfaces and their mod-
ifications across time and space and, on the other hand, I study planned change
through an applied intervention project. This project, which is "action research"
in the Lewinian tradition, is presented here both in its own right and as a case
study demonstrating the applied significance and policy relevance of psychology.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This book was written during a sabbaticalleave in the 1993-1994 academic year
at the Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study (NIAS). I appreciate this leave
granted to me by Bogazici University. I am truly grateful for the perfect atmo-
sphere and the superb support at NIAS, which contributed greatly to my work.
Pilar van Breda-Burgueno of NIAS ably typed several revisions of the manu-
script.
Ype Poortinga read the first version of the manuscript and gave me much
valuable feedback. I also benefited from the comments of lohn Berry, Pierre
Dasen, Patricia Greenfield, and anonymous reviewers, as weH as the support of
Walt Lonner. I am thankful to Marc Bomstein for taking the initiative to contact
Lawrence Erlbaum for me, acting as a liaison. I appreciate the assistance of
ludith Amsel and Sondra Guideman of Lawrence Erlbaum Associates with the
editing and the production of the book.
My students Asli Carkoglu, Nurcan KaramoHa, Didem Gurbey, Ayse Uskul,
Sahika Ayhan, Ozge Koca, and Esin Uzun helped in various ways. Throughout
my work on the manuscript, my husband, Oguz Kaglt<;lba§l, provided me with
invaluable moral support and encouragement, without which I could not have
finished this demanding task. I am indebted to all. Thanks also to my colleagues
and students at Bogazici University who have considerably influenced my think-
ing on human development, the family, and cross-cultural psychology. Many
years were spent at Bogazici before my recent move to Koc University. I feel a
deep sense of gratitude to Bogazici.
This page intentionally left blank
1 Introd uction

ABOUT THIS VOLUME

This volume is, on the one hand, about the psychology of human development
and human relations within the cultural context. The development of human
relations and the self is situated within the family and society. In my thinking, the
links between the person, family, and society are crucial for an understanding of
global human psychology. On the other hand, this volume is also about the
integration of theory and practice. Specifically, I make an attempt to find out
whether a culturally sensitive conceptualization of individual-family-culture
links has any relevance for applications and policies designed to promote human
well-being. Clearly, there are two types of linkages that need c1arification. The
first one has to do with some of the intersections between the levels of analysis-
the individual (self), the group (family), and the larger context in which both
exist (culture and society). The second one relates theory and application.

Linking Self, Family, Society


My purpose here is to look into some limited aspects of family dynamics and
family socialization within varying sociocultural contexts with a view to discover
their functional (or causal) links with human development. Thus, a contextual-
developmental-functional approach is undertaken here.
The approach is contextual in that the study of the person and human develop-
ment automatically implicates the family as the context, and thus figures the
family explicitly in the conceptualization. Similarly, when the family is under
2 1. INTRODUCTION

focus, it is automatically situated in its sociocultural environment. This approach


is very much in line with the ecological orientation of Bronfenbrenner (1979),
with traces of the classical field theory of Lewin (1951), and symbolic interac-
tionism of G. H. Mead (1934), on the one hand, and with the current contextual-
ist models, on the other (e.g., Featherman & R. M. Lerner, 1985; Hurrelmann,
1988; R. M. Lerner, 1983, 1989). Much thinking in cross-cultural psychology is
also contextualistic, almost by definition, and the approach here is akin to the
current theorizing in this field (e.g., Berry, 1976; Berry, Poortinga, SegaIl, &
Dasen, 1992, p. 12; Eckensberger, 1990; Price-Williams, 1980).
The approach here is also developmental. This is because it is not enough to
note or even to establish with some certainty differences across contexts. The
way these differences emerge is just as important for psychological inquiry. More
and more, the significance of a developmental approach is being recognized, and
a developmental orientation is seen as inherently complementing a cross-cultural
one (Bornstein, 1984; Bornstein & Bruner, 1989; Eckensberger, 1990; Heron &
Kroeger, 1981; Jahoda, 1986; Rogoff, Gauvain, & Ellis, 1984; Rogoff & Mo-
relli, 1989). This is not to say that all cross-cultural work takes cognizance of
developmental processes, but that the recognition of the need to do so is increas-
ing. It is interesting to note in this context that in her invited address at the
centennial convention of the American Psychological Association, Anastasi
(1992) pointed to the recent progress of cross-cultural psychology and life-span
developmental psychology as the two most important developments of the last
decades.
Finally, the present approach is functional because social and psychological
adaptive mechanisms are invoked to explain why a particular type of develop-
ment occurs. I should note, however, that this functional approach is not deter-
ministic and allows for flexibility and feedback mechanisms. It also tries to stay
away from teleological reasoning. Adaptive mechanisms are, rather, used as
clues to understand why self-family-culture linkages get established in particu-
lar ways, showing variability as weIl as similarity across cultures. The contex-
tual, developmental, and functional approaches are elaborated on when neces-
sary throughout.
In forming the links among the self, family, and the larger sociocultural
environment, I work from a cultural and cross-cultural perspective. A cultural
approach is presupposed by contextualism, and a cross-cultural approach is
required for the unambiguous interpretation of the observed cultural differences
(Van de Vijver & Poortinga, 1990). To understand the functional relations among
the society, the family, and the development of the self, the underlying dynamics
need to be discovered. A cross-cultural comparative orientation provides the
grounds for such an endeavor, as it supplies more variation than can be obtained
in a single culture study (Berry et al., 1992; Rogoff et al., 1984; 1. W. Whiting &
Child, 1953).
A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 3

Linking Theory and Application


The second basic task I undertake in this volume is to integrate theory and
application. By application I do not me an individual-focused psychological prac-
tice, wh ich readily comes to mind, but rather the use of psychology in large-scale
efforts to improve human well-being and to contribute to societal development.
Psychologists' contribution to development efforts focuses on its human aspects;
therefore, any applied work would benefit from a knowledge of the cultural
context in wh ich human phenomena occur. Intervention attempts in developing
countries need to be especially sensitive to the human relations in the "culture of
relatedness" (Kaglt<;lba§l, 1985a) prevalent in these societies. Thus, interven-
tions may be expected to work better if they take into consideration and build on
the existing human connectedness, as reflected in closely knit family, kinship,
and community ties, rather than counteracting them, for example, in building
individualistic independence and competition.
The emphasis on applied research occupies a central place in my orientation to
psychology (Kaglt<;lba§l, 1994b). Theory that is not put to the test of application
has limited utility, and applications not informed by theory tend to be haphazard
and expensive "shots in the dark" that can't be afforded, especially in the Major-
ity World (see footnote 5 in Preface) countries with limited resources. In other
words, I believe that psychology need not choose between theory and scientific
rigor on the one hand and relevance on the other, and it is incumbent on the
psychologist, especially on one who lives in the Majority World, to be involved
in efforts to uphold human well-being. Culturally sensitive and both "socially
and scientifically responsible" (Drenth, 1991) psychological research can go a
long way toward contributing to social development efforts.
This book presents, in some detail, an applied research project that I have
carried out with my colleagues in Istanbul, Turkey. I believe it deserves attention
both for demonstrating the integration of theory and practice on the one hand,
and the potential of psychology for contributing to human development on the
other. The Turkish Early Enrichment Project and its follow-up study together
spanned a lO-year period. I attempt to bridge the gap between theory and appli-
cation by using this applied research as a case in point.

A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE

This topic covers a wide scope and includes extensive research and theory
spanning the fields of cultural and cross-cultural psychology, anthropology, and
sociology. I would like to give here a rather brief and general overview of the
issues involved and examine the relevant concepts. My approach here is basically
psychological, though Iresort to anthropological and sociological conceptualiza-
tions where appropriate.
4 1. INTRODUCTION

Recent Developments in Cross-Cultural Perspective

Human development always occurs within culture, but it is rarely studied as such
by academic psychology. The issue does not concern only developmental psy-
chology but is true of all psychology whose unit of analysis is typically the
individual. This outlook is in line with the goal of discovering universal regu-
larities in psychological processes and behavior, which psychology inherited
from physics. Accordingly, a physical science model adhering to a positivistic
philosophy of science is typically adopted. This implies a methodological orien-
tation isolating the behavior from its natural context to control for "unwanted"
variation. Thus, social and cultural factors are often absent in analyses.
This is noticeable from a cursory glance at popular developmental psychology
textbooks. They tend not to include cultural differences, or they treat them as
extraneous variables (noise), and they view the individualistic trajectory as the
normal way of developing. These textbooks influence how development is
viewed in American psychology and abroad.
This state of affairs has been noted by critics both within and outside psychol-
ogy. For example, focusing on human development, T. Schwartz (1981) stated:
"Developmental psychology has largely missed the opportunity to consider the
child in the cultural milieu, which is the sine qua non of the developmental
completion of a human nature" (p. 4). Similarly, lahoda and Dasen (1986), in
their introduction to the special issue of the International Journal oj Behavioral
Development, called for a "Cross-cultural developmental psychology ...
[which] is not just comparative [but] essentially is an outlook that takes culture
seriously" and deplored the fact that "theories and findings in developmental
psychology originating in the First world tend to be disseminated to the Third
World as gospel truth" (p. 413).
In his influential work on the ecology of human development, Bronfenbrenner
(1979) complained about the "marked asymmetry: a hypertrophy of theory and
research focusing on the properties of the person and only the most rudimentary
conception and characterization of the environment in wh ich the person is found"
(p. 16), and claimed that "developmental psychology . . . is the science of the
strange behavior of children in strange situations with strange adults for the
briefest possible periods of time" (p. 19).
These views are echoed by those who believe that a noncontextual approach to
behavior in general and to human development in particular is inadequate (e.g.,
Bornstein, 1991; Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Dasen, 1984; lahoda, 1986; Ka-
glt<;lba§l, 1984, 1992b; R. M. Lerner, 1989; MacDonald, 1986; Pepitone, 1987;
Price-Williams, 1980; Rogoff et al., 1984; Rogoff & Morelli, 1989; Shweder &
Bourne, 1984; Tajfel, 1972; Triandis, 1972; Tyler, 1989).
The extensive criticism, substantiated by insightful research, has aimed to be
a corrective to the "narrow" focus of psychology. It has been an outcry, loud and
clear, serving as the basis for the advancement of a wide range of disciplines and
A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 5

critical views spanning cross-cultural and cultural psychology, on the one hand,
and social constructionism and indigenous psychology, on the other.
Most research in mainstream psychology is still going about its usual busi-
ness. Nevertheless, the developments in cultural and cross-cultural psychology
over the past 25 years are substantial, and they do challenge the established
scientific traditions of psychological research though probably not yet strongly
enough to shake them (Bond, 1988; Lonner, 1989). As a crude indicator of the
notable growth of the field, several publications and textbooks could be cited. A
number of journals are devoted to cross-cultural psychological research, among
them are the Journal 0/ Cross-Cultural Psychology, International Journal 0/
Psychology, International Journal o/Intercultural Relations, International Jour-
nal 0/ Behavioral Development, and Psychology and Developing Societies. Some
others have an international cross-cultural outlook: for example, Journal 0/ So-
cial Psychology, Inter-American Journal 0/ Psychology, European Journal 0/
Social Psychology. To date, there have been four reviews of cross-cultural psy-
chology in the Annual Review 0/ Psychology (Brislin, 1983; Kaglt~lba§l & Berry,
1989; Segall, 1986; Triandis, Malpass, & Davidson, 1973) and one of cultural
psychology (Shweder & Sullivan, 1993). The six-volume Handbook 0/ Cross-
Cultural Psychology appeared in 1980. A second edition of the Handbook is
currently in preparation.
A great number of books, written or edited by cross-cultural psychologists
have been published in the series on Cross-Cultural Research and Methodology
and in the selected volumes from the conferences of the International Association
for Cross-Cultural Psychology. There are also numerous publications that pro-
vide overviews of the field, inc1uding the annual Nebraska Symposium on Mo-
tivation (Berman, 1990) and some textbooks of cross-cultural psychology (Berry
et al. , 1992; Segall, Dasen, Berry, & Poortinga, 1990); cross-cultural social
psychology (Moghaddam, Taylor, & Wright, 1993; P. B. Smith & Bond, 1993;
Triandis 1994), and psychology in cultural context (Brislin, 1993; Lonner &
Malpass, 1994; Matsumoto, 1994). All this activity points to a growing cross-
cultural psychology.
Particularly in the cross-cultural study of human development the affinity to
an anthropological approach emphasizing the specific cultural context is notable.
Starting with the pioneering work of the Whitings and their associates on child
rearing in six cultures (Minturn & Lambert, 1964; B. B. Whiting, 1963; B. B.
Whiting & J. W. Whiting, 1975), much work has been conducted by psycholo-
gists and anthropologists at times working together. Several books on cross-
cultural child development, inc1uding a handbook (R. L. Munroe, R. H.
Munroe, & B. B. Whiting, 1981) provide overviews of this work (e.g., Borns-
tein, 1991; Greenfield & Cocking, 1994; R. L. Munroe & R. H. Munroe, 1975;
Stigler, Shweder, & Herdt, 1990; Valsiner, 1989; Wagner, 1983; Wagner &
Stevenson, 1982; Werner, 1979).
Research and conceptualization regarding the importance of the cultural con-
6 1. INTRODUCTION

text for psychology have also been emerging from the non-Western world. This is
significant when we consider the fact that psychology has traditionally been a
Western, and to a large extent, American preoccupation. The rest of the world
has typically followed suit, demonstrating a remarkable degree of "traditional
acquiescence" (Kaglt~lba§l, 1994a, 1994b). More recently, however, the pro-
gress of cross-cultural psychology has benefited from scholarship in non-Western
countries, particularly in Asia. In addition to a growing number of contributions
to journals and books containing cross-cultural psychological work from non-
Western psychologists, some volumes have come out dealing specifically with
psychology and human development in the Majority World (e.g., Curran, 1984;
Nsamenang, 1992; ühuche & ütaala, 1981; üppong, 1980; Pandey, 1988; Sar-
aswathi & Dutta, 1987; Saraswathi & Kaur, 1993; D. Sinha, 1981; D. Sinha &
Kao, 1988; Suvannathat, Bhanthumnavin, Bhuapirom, & Keats, 1985).
A new development emerging mainly from non-Western contexts is the so-
called indigenous psychology (Adair, 1992; Bond, 1986; G. E. Enriquez, 1990;
Heelas & Locke, 1981; Kaglt~lba§l & Berry, 1989; Kim & Berry, 1993; D.
Sinha, 1986, 1992). It purports that each culture should be studied within itself,
as it forms the all-important context of psychological phenomena. In this ap-
proach "from within," the historical-cultural characteristics, symbols and arti-
facts are used as materials to construct a meaningful portrait of a people. "Natu-
ral," rather than "imposed," categories are utilized, reminiscent of the typically
"emic" approach of anthropologists.
"Indigenization," or indigenous psychology, has been proposed to be an an-
tithesis of the universalist orientation, typical of much of cross-cultural psycholo-
gy. However I believe they are complementary approaches (Kaglt~lba§l, 1992a),
each providing feedback for the other. If indigenization is seen as an approach,
rather than a goal in and of itself, then it is likely to be followed by a comparative
approach. And when commonalities emerge out of such comparison among
different indigenous realities, we beg in to approach universality (Berry, 1989).
This point is discussed further later.

Benefits of the Cross-Cultural Perspective

The greatly increasing volume of cross-cultural research is a c1ear indication of


the growing appreciation of the value of a cross-cultural perspective. The advan-
tages involved have been repeatedly voiced by those conducting such research.
The following are some of the oft-quoted benefits within a developmental per-
spective.
A cross-cultural developmental approach uncovers a greater range of variation
than any single culture study. With a more comprehensive coverage of diversity,
a wider perspective emerges according to which what is typical and what is
atypical may need to be redefined (Bornstein, 1984).
A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 7

With increased coverage of variation, it also becomes more possible to distin-


guish between biological and environmental influences. That is, the greater the
commonality found in a developmental sequence or psychological process over
highly varied cultural contexts, the greater the likelihood of its biological roots,
though shared sodal structures mayaiso be a cause. With a finding of increased
diversity in a psychological phenomenon across cultures, environmental causa-
tion is implicated.
In view of the aforementioned two points regarding increased degree of vari-
ability in cross-cultural study, theories based on research with more limited
sampies may need to be revised if they are to hold up to their claims of uni ver-
sality.
The theory-testing potential of cross-cultural research is thus very important.
Any psychological theory claiming universality, as they all do, must be demon-
strated to hold cross-culturally. Obviously, a theory can never be proven in
absolute terms, as there is always the likelihood of one disconfirming case.
Nevertheless, if a theory finds supportive evidence in highly diverse cultural
contexts, its claim to "extern al validity" and universality would be a lot stronger
than if it is tested in only a single cultural context. The more a theory receives
cross-cultural confirmation, the more closely it approximates universal gener-
ality. Indeed, most cross-cultural research in human development has had such a
theory-testing goal. This research has served theory very weil; for example,
Piagetian and Vygotskian theories have enjoyed cross-cultural extensions.
Such testing also helps refine theory. For example, as cross-cultural research
showed that the "formal operations" stage of Piaget was very rare among illiter-
ate adults, he changed his orientation and accepted that formal operations may
not be a universal stage but may occur only in specific familiar domains. This
reformulation was subsequently supported by research conducted in the United
States (D. Kuhn & Brannock, 1977; reported in Rogoff et al., 1984). Cross-
cultural developmental research, which has contributed significantly to theoreti-
cal advancement, cannot boast the same degree of success in serving the well-
being ofthe world's children (Dasen & Jahoda, 1986; Kaglt<;lba§l, 1991a; 1992b;
Wagner, 1983) for that has not been its intention. Much work needs to be done
which is both informed by culturally relevant theory and is problem oriented in
order to promote children's well-being. This is a basic theme in this volume.
Another advantage of cross-cultural comparative research is the possibility of
disentangling some variables highly associated in one culture by going to another
society where this is less so. This allows for refined analysis by unconfounding
variables. For example, it is difficult to study separately the effects of age
(maturation) and the experience of schooling in Western contexts because these
two variables are highly confounded (all children are at school). By conducting
studies in cultures where this is not the case, the effects of maturation, as separate
from schooling, can be studied (Rogoff, 1981; Rogoff et al. , 1984).
As in the previous example, cross-cultural research has the potential to study
8 1. INTRODUCTION

naturally occurring cause-effect relations that cannot be manipulated experimen-


tally, by utilizing natural quasi-experimental studies. To follow-up the previous
example, it is not possible to deprive some children of schooling in order to study
the effects of age on cognitive development independently of schooling. How-
ever, if such is naturally the case, cause-effect relations can be pursued accord-
ingly.
Cross-cultural study can also provide comprehensive descriptions of psycho-
logical phenomena. With increased range of variation covered-for example, in
age-specific human development-we can get a fuller spectrum of development,
which is aprerequisite for explanation (Bornstein, 1984). Especially in the hands
of anthropologists, who have contributed greatly to the study of cross-cultural
human development, rich description can be a valuable source of knowledge and
understanding.
Such comparative description and cross-cultural work, in general, provide
insight into human adaptation (Rogoff et al., 1984). It brings into focus variation
in ecological/environmental factors and how they are experienced by the people
being studied. Functional relations among the ecological, economic, and socio-
political contextual variables and the psychological behavioral characteristics of
people reflect biological/ cultural adaptations (Berry, 1976; Berry et al. , 1992).
Cross-cultural psychological study also works as a corrective for the re-
searcher's ethnocentrism. This is mainly because it sensitizes researchers to the
cultural basis of their own beliefs. Cross-cultural psychologists are likely to
realize that psychology, as it has been constructed historically, is indeed an
indigenous psychology of the Western world. It needs to be tried out for validity
in the non-Western world if its claim to universality is to be substantiated by
evidence. A lot of psychological theories are cultural constructions, reflecting a
particular orientation to and interpretation of "reality."
There are important implications of this view in both the explanation of
human phenomena and in applications across cultures. For example, the way sex
roles or human competence are conceptualized within the prevalent folk theories
of a people may be quite different from those of the psychologist armed with
Western theory. It takes much sensitivity to work out differences in interpretation
and to avoid blunders in applied work that may entail interventions to change
behaviors. Here a psychologist with a cross-cultural orientation and experience
would have a definite advantage over the one with a unicultural background.
Apart from these substantive contributions of the cross-cultural approach to
improve theory and understanding, it has also had a different type of impact on
mainstream thinking in psychology in general, and developmental psychology in
particular. This has had to do with the potential of some cross-cultural research to
unravel socioeconomic development and culture.
A great deal of earlier cross-cultural research was conducted in preindustrial
and some in preliterate societies, or using the Human Relations Area Files,
wh ich consist of anthropological information on the same types of societies. As
A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 9

Azuma (1986) noted, "A limitation of such studies is that cultural variables
covary with the degree of industrialization of the society" (p. 3). This confound-
ing of socioeconomic structural factors (such as education, standard of living,
etc.)-that is, level of societal development-with cultural beliefs and values
blurs interpretation. Also as these societies are often remote and very different
from contemporary societies, findings from them tend to be ignored by main-
stream psychologists as irrelevant (obscure anthropologie al description), though
the cognitive area may be an exception.
More recently, with the increased volume of comparative research emerging
from contemporary nation states, it has been possible to keep socioeconomic
level characteristics rather similar in cross-cultural comparison and to focus on
other cultural differences. It is more difficult to ignore this type of research, for it
often involves socioeconomically similar sampies, such as urban educated
groups, university students, and so on, with nevertheless different cultural orien-
tations. For example, with the recent economic growth in the "Pacific Rim" and
the high mathematics-science achievement levels of Japanese and Chinese chil-
dren (both in their own countries and in the United States), the West has become
interested in Eastern and especially the Japanese culture (Japanese management,
Japanese childrearing, Japanese education, etc.). Such interest leads to a greater
appreciation of cross-cultural research.
Some significant social problems that require solutions are amenable to cross-
cultural study. Among these, ethnic issues and global development efforts cur-
rently have high priority. Thus, with the need to improve ethnic relations and to
contribute to the human aspects of global development, there is increasing al-
location of resources for applied intervention research both in the Majority World
and also involving ethnic minorities in the Western world. This attracts the
attention of mainstream psychologists to ethnic and cross-cultural research as
weIl.
Finally, and as related to all of the previous points, probably a most important
benefit that cross-cultural orientation is providing to general psychology is a
"sensitization to culture." In a way, one could say that it has taken the demonstra-
tion of cultural differences in comparative research to get the psychologists to
take culture seriously. Both cross-cultural and ethnic psychological research has
played an important role here (Berry, 1985). The integration of culture into
psychological analysis promises to widen the scope of our understanding; it can
be a breakthrough for psychology.

The Culture Concept


Anthropologists have been studying culture from the very beginning. It is often
noted that there are some 164 definitions of culture (Kroeber & Kluekhohn,
1952). Obviously there are different views about how best to conceptualize
culture and what aspects to emphasize. There appears to be agreement, however,
10 1. INTRODUCTION

regarding its comprehensive nature. Thus, the following characterizations, and


others, have been proposed: "traditional ideas and especially their attached val-
ues," "the mass of learned behaviour passing through generations," "shared
symbols and meanings," "different experiences of groups that lead to predictable
and significant differences in behavior," "a 'gestalt' of ideas, practices, norms
and meanings that organize behavior as a system," "a superordinate organizer
with a pervasive influence on its constituent elements," "a system, a set of
interrelated and inextricably linked elements," "mental programming or soft-
ware."
Psychologists tend to adopt Herskovits' (1948) all-inclusive definition of cul-
ture as "the man-made part of the environment" (Segall et al., 1990), including
both "physical culture" and "subjective culture" (subjective responses to what is
man made) (Triandis, 1972). Various cultural conceptions have been elaborately
discussed recently (e.g., Berry et al., 1992; Van de Vijver & Hutschemaekers,
1990).
The important point here is situating the psychological phenomenon in its
cultural context. This appears as "obvious" because we know that "it is rare
(perhaps even impossible) for any human being ever to behave without respond-
ing to some aspect of culture" (Segall et al., 1990, p. 5). Nevertheless, what
appears obvious turns out to be less so when an attempt is made to integrate
culture into psychological analysis (Van de Vijver & Hutschemaekers, 1990).
First of all, the diffuse, all-inclusive nature of culture presents a problem in
research. As a superordinate entity, it cannot serve as an explanation or an
independent variable (Segall, 1983). Such explanations can turn into empty
tautologies, such as "Chinese are this way because of their culture." Thus,
attempts are made by psychologists to define culture in less molar and more
molecular ways or to operationalize it (Poortinga, Van de Vijver, Joe, & Van de
Koppel, 1987; Segall, 1984; B. B. Whiting, 1976). In this more molecular
conceptualization, culture is treated as a "set of conditions" (Segall, 1984) that is
quite different from culture as a system (Rohner, 1984). It is also conceptualized
as "shared constraints that limit the behavior repertoire available to members of a
certain socio-cultural group" (Poortinga, 1992, p. 10).
Second, and as related to the previous issue of definition, a perennial meth-
odological problem and a long-standing issue is how to study behavior in culture.
The two basic approaches here, having traditional counterparts in psychology
and sociology, are those that prescribe studying the phenomenon either from
within or from without. This distinction has been the basis of "cultural" and
"cross-cultural" psychology, where the former studies human phenomena from
within and the latter from without. It has also formed a current debate on the
issue of cultural versus cross-cultural psychology, which is discussed later. The
two views have been expressed in different labels in different disciplines and
orientations, but the basic similarities among them prompt me to group them
together. The views from within and from without have their paralleis in the
A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 11

idiographic and nomothetic approaches in psychology, going back to Cronbach's


characterization of "the two disciplines of scientific psychology" (1957), and
before hirn to Allport (1937), who ascribed the distinction to the German philoso-
pher Windelband. They also found paralleis in the qualitative and quantitative
research traditions in sociology and the hermeneutic versus positivistic ap-
proaches in anthropology. In cross-cultural psychology the emic-etic debate and
the indigenous (or relativist) versus universalist orientations (Berry et al., 1992),
respectively, appear analogous. Finally, the current debate between cultural and
cross-cultural psychology refIects the same basic distinction.
What is common in the idiographic, hermeneutic, emic, indigenous, rela-
tivist, cultural approaches is an emphasis on the uniqueness of concepts in each
cultural context, because they derive their meanings from these contexts. There
is also a stress on the variability and the uniqueness of the individual case
(person, culture, etc.) that requires its study from within and in its own right,
defying comparison. In contrast, the nomothetic, positivist, etic, universalist,
cross-cultural approaches study the "typical," not the unique, which can be
compared using a common standard or measure. The emphasis is on the underly-
ing similarities that render comparison possible. There appears to be a basic
conflict between the emic and the etic, if accepted as exc1usive orientations,
because being stuck in one would negate the other (Kaglt<;lba§l, 1992a).
As psychology ventures into integrating culture into its analyses and strives to
account for cultural diversity while c1aiming generality (Dasen & Jahoda, 1986,
p. 413), it is bound to "move out" from individuals into their interaction with the
environment. Yet, this is where this basic conflict comes in. It is between what
Lightfoot and Valsiner (1992, p. 394) called "the need to conceptualize 'context
dependency' of psychological phenomena and the 'context-eliminating' theoreti-
cal traditions of psychology." It can be resolved if there is a genuine understand-
ing that eliminating the context or abstracting behavior from its environment is
not the only route toward reaching generality. Indeed, it may be a route that does
not lead to real generality but to pseudo-generality. This type of generality is
assumed but not empirically demonstrated. The assumption is that the causal
relation that is found in the behavior abstracted from the environment in the pure
controlled laboratory condition holds across cultures or environments because it
is independent of the latter. Yet, even the most ardent experimental ist knows that
complete abstraction from the environment is impossible in research with human
beings who bring their "culture" into the laboratory, in the form of expectations,
habits, values, and so on.
The other, and surer, route toward generality, though laborious, is integrating
context into psychological study and examining its impact. When this type of
context-dependent study, uncovering causal relations, is conducted in other con-
texts also, the obtained similarities and differences can weave the path toward
generalities.
The recent debate about whether cultural or cross-cultural psychology pro-
12 1. INTRODUCTION

vides the better conceptual scheme for studying human phenomena (Markus &
Kitayama, 1992; Shweder, 1990, 1991; Shweder & Sullivan, 1993; Van de
Vijver & Hutschemaekers, 1990) has once again brought the issue to the fore.
The discussion revolves about some more basic methodological and conceptual
issues such as whether a comparative (decontextualizing) or a wholistic, contex-
tualizing (situated) methodology is to be used in the study of human psychologi-
cal phenomena· and whether universalism or relativism of psychological func-
tioning is to be assumed. It is reminiscent of the earlier etic-emic debate (Berry,
1969, 1989; Jahoda, 1977, 1983) and the current discussion on universalist
orientation versus indigenous psychology (Berry et al. , 1992; G. E. Enriquez,
1990; Kim & Berry, 1993; D. Sinha, 1989, 1992).
I do not subscribe to the either-or stance of the debate and believe that the two
approaches can be, and should be, complementary (KagJt<;lba§l, 1992a). To my
understanding, cultural psychology is psychology within the cultural context,
and as such all human psychology should indeed be cultural psychology, as
human phenomena always take place within culture. However, as we are far
from this ideal, psychological inquiry that takes cognizance of the cultural con-
text can be labeled cultural psychology. If in such inquiry a comparative ap-
proach is used and thus at least two cultures are implicated, even if implicitly, we
are in the realm of cross-cultural psychology. It is important to note that a
comparative approach does not predude a contextualistic orientation. Indeed, a
contextualistic orientation and a comparative orientation are basic to my think-
ing, as already made dear. In this, I am in agreement with Eckensberger (1990)
and with Price-Williams, who observed that "contexts are not necessarily unique;
they can be compared" (1980, p. 82). That is why in this book I see myself
involved in both cultural and cross-cultural psychology. However, for the sake of
simplicity and not to use both terms together, I use "cultural psychology" unless
specifically referring to cross-cultural comparison.

Similarities and Differences


This brings me to a consideration of the meaning attributed to similarities and
differences obtained in cross-cultural comparisons. It does not make much sense
to insist on an either-or position (universalism or relativism) because this is an
empirical issue. It is the researcher's task "to know when it makes sense to
emphasize likeness, rand when] difference ... " (to quote a phrase from Shwe-
der, 1984, p. 60 which he later appears to abandon by rejecting likenesses)
(Shweder, 1990, 1991; Shweder & Sullivan, 1993). As I mentioned earlier, with
the greater comprehensive coverage of variation in cross-cultural study, common
and variable characteristics and biological and environmental factors can be
distinguished more effectively than when a smaller range of variation is entailed.
However, it should also be noted that similarity across cultures dos not neces-
sarily imply genetic determination. It can be due to some universal (or commonly
A CULTURAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 13

shared) psychological or structural factors. For example, all societies have devel-
oped rules and social control mechanisms for maintaining intragroup harmony,
care of the young, and socialization of children. Thus, there are similarities, as
weIl as differences, across cultures. These similarities may be due to analogous
functionallinks among behaviors in different cultures, rather than to biological
commonalities (see also Lonner, 1980 and Van de Vijver & Poortinga, 1982).
Yet, when similarities are found between cultures an ethological explanation
is commonly invoked (H. Papousek & M. Papousek, 1991; Sigman & Wachs,
1991), and they are attributed to culture only when differences are found. One
reason for this is the assumption of the uniqueness of each culture. This is a view
derived from descriptive anthropology and is readily accepted by cross-cultural
psychologists. It leads to the expectation that cross-cultural comparison should
uncover differences in behavior. Thus, cross-cultural research reports are replete
with statements such as "the Indian self ... , the Japanese mother ... "; or "the
Greek philotimo," the "Latin American simpatia," the "Japanese amae" or the
"Mexican historic-sociocultural premises." Yet study after study finds similar
characteristics among behavior patterns in countries such as India, Korea, Mexi-
co, Greece, Japan, and so forth, which remain implicit. Indeed, Triandis (1989)
noted sentiments in other collectivistic cultural groups similar to the "Greek
philotimo," and a Turkish researcher (Ay~i~egi, 1993) found the "Mexican
historic-sociocultural premises" (Diaz-Guerrero, 1991) regarding sex roles to be
typical in Turkey!
There is a need to go beyond the descriptive psychological portrayal of differ-
ent peoples toward discovering underlying reasons for behavior that may be
shared to some extent among them. Thus the current two-way thinking (differ-
ence implying cultural, similarity biological causation) needs to be expanded to
at least a three-way thinking. When a difference is found in cross-cultural com-
parison, a contextual (environmental/cultural) interpretation would be implied,
except for few known race differences. When a similarity is found, however,
there is an ambiguity because either shared biology or shared structure (psycho-
logical, ecological, social or cultural) may be the cause. Or, there might even be
the further possibility of a combination of the two. The challenge is unraveling
these influences.
If we are interested in possible generalization, the comparison of two or more
emic portrayals is needed (Berry, 1969, 1989; Triandis, 1978). This is where an
etic or cross-cultural comparative approach comes into the picture. It is important
to note here that the ultimate aim of this approach should be more the discovery
of shared characteristics than of differences, that is, if psychological theory
aspires for universality. But interestingly enough, a focus on differences is preva-
lent here. Of course, there are exceptions, as exemplified in the weIl-known
studies utilizing data from a great number of countries in order to discover
patterns of beliefs or values (e.g., Hofstede, 1980; S. H. Schwartz, 1992; S. H.
Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987, 1990). However, in these studies structures are typ-
14 1. INTRODUCTION

ically imposed from outside rather than discovered in cultural (emic) study. What
appears to be lacking are studies conducted within the cultural context that reveal
functionalladaptive links among phenomena that may, in turn, repeat themselves
in different contexts, thus pointing to some fundamental causal relations.

ORGANIZATION OF THE BOOK

This introductory chapter has focused on cultural and cross-cultural perspectives


in psychology, which provide a general conceptual framework. Given the bene-
fits of a cross-cultural approach in widening the scope of our understanding, I
make frequent use of it in discussions throughout the book. Nevertheless, my
main focus is on human development in cultural context, as mediated by the
family. I ex amine it from a functional-contextual perspective and with both a
theoretical and applied emphasis. Its coverage here is quite varied and spans
wide ground. The different chapters deal with different yet related topics.
The book is composed of two parts. The first part focuses on human develop-
ment, family, and culture. The chapters in this first part mainly cover theory and
research linking phenomena at the individual, group (family), and societallevels
of analyses. Human development in cultural context and the more specific pro-
cess of the development of social/cognitive competence; the relations between
culture and self; and family change through social change are examined.
Throughout the discussions, a cross-cultural, contextual, functional, and devel-
opmental perspective is assumed.
The first part contains four chapters dealing with the central issues discussed
in the introduction. Chapter 2 studies "Development in Context." Numerous
examples from across cultures and diverse theoretical perspectives point to the
necessity of construing human development in context. Though the cultural
context is stressed, an ecological perspective to context is used with different
layers of embedded environmental influences, ranging from the family to soci-
etal values. Context is seen to impart meaning to human experience, as for
example in the construal of "childhood" itself.
Chapter 3 examines "Socialization for Competence" and builds upon the
previous chapter. Theoretical and cultural conceptions of cognitive competence
are reviewed. There is a discussion of cultural variability in the meaning of
"competence" and particularly cognitive competence. It deals with difficult is-
sues of relativism versus comparative standards and the danger of double stan-
dards in the study of competence across cultures. A basic theme, also discussed
in chapter 2 and later in chapter 6, is the culture gap between traditional child-
rearing values and the new environmental demands emerging with social change
and urban lifestyles, which may result in a disadvantage for the child.
Chapter 4 provides a discussion of the concept of self across cultures and
looks into the complex interplay of "Culture and Self." Several theoreticaland
ORGANIZATION OF THE BOOK 15

disciplinary perspectives are examined, inc1uding a review of the recent critique


of individualism in the Western, particularly in the American, context. A distinc-
tion is made between the "relational self" and the "separated self," which is
another basic theme throughout the book. It is also conceptualized as a dimen-
sion of (inter)dependence-independence in interpersonal relations. Autonomy,
control, and achievement, as weil as the development of the self, are interpreted
within this perspective.
Chapter 5 explores "Family and Family Change." Family is studied as the
microcontext of development. Three prototypical family patterns are discussed,
and a model of family change is proposed through social change. The commonly
accepted modernization view of convergence toward the Western model is ques-
tioned, and a shift toward a "model of emotional interdependence" is proposed.
This is a family /human model, as the development of the self within the family is
also studied. There is a proposed shift toward the development of the
"autonomous-relational self" through socioeconomic development within the
culture of relatedness (collectivism).
Part 11 moves from theory to application. It focuses on induced change and the
role of psychology both in general terms and also with regard to early enrich-
ment. It examines the applied significance of psychology in promoting healthy
human development and analyzes the psychology (social science)-social policy
interface.
Chapter 6, "Induced Change: The Role of Psychology," critically appraises
the role of psychology in the light of the foregoing discussions and forms a link
between the two parts of the book. It asks the question of how psychology can
help promote "human development" and assigns a socially, as weil as a scien-
tifically, responsible role to psychology. There is a more extensive discussion
here of the "political" issues of values, standards, and relativism in the study of
human development across cultures. Some controversial topics are confronted,
such as schooling, religious education, school readiness, and the nature-nurture
underpinnings.
Chapter 7, "Early Childhood Care and Education (ECCE)," narrows down the
question to early enrichment. It provides an overview of the early childhood care
and education research and applications, both in the Western and the Majority
World. This is seen as an area where psychological expertise, especially in the
field of developmental psychology, can inform research and applications. Some
issues in ECCE are reviewed, such as the relative effectiveness and cost-
effectiveness of center-based and home-based approaches.
Chapter 8, "The Turkish Early Enrichment Project," is presented as a case in
point. It describes a longitudinal study of early enrichment, coupled with a
follow-up, where both the horne and preschool environment and mother training
are studied in terms of their differential effects on the development of children.
Effects of the project intervention on the overall development and school
achievement of children as weIl as on the mothers and the horne environment are
16 1. INTRODUCTION

discussed. The holistic, interactional, contextual orientation of the project is seen


as its main strength, leading to gains sustained over time.
Chapter 9, "Search for Integration and Policy Relevance," builds the policy
implications of the Turkish Early Enrichment Project and examines the general
issue of psychology and social policy. Policy relevance of psychology (and of
social science) is discussed with a particular focus on global human development
and well-being. An integration of the foregoing topics and discussions is
searched here. The development of the self and of human competence in family
and culture is viewed again, with an "involved" stance. The socially relevant role
of psychology comes to the fore.
All chapters are interrelated, yet each stands on its own. As such, they may be
seen as independent topics, and in particular, the first and the second parts differ
in theoretical and practical emphasis. Nevertheless, in my thinking, the different
parts hang together, and I try to weave them together. Specifically, the theoretical
perspectives set out in Part I serve as the theoretical underpinnings of the inter-
vention presented in Part 11. Similarly, the call for the greater involvement of
psychology in socially relevant research serves as the moral justification for the
intervention. Theory, (applied) research, and policy need to be interrelated, and
this is wh at this book is about.
References
Adair, J. G. (1992). Empirical studies of indigenization and development of the discipline
in developing countries. In S. Iwawaki , Y. Kashima , & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in
cross-cultural psychology (pp. 62–74). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Adorno, T. W. , Frenkel-Brunswik, E. , Levinson, D. J. , & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The
authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.
Agarwal, R. , & Misra, G. (1986). A factor analytical study of achievement goals and
means: An Indian view. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 717–731.
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: a psychological interpretation. New York: Henry Holt.
Allport, G. W. (1959). The historical background of modern social psychology. In G.
Lindzey (Ed.). Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 3–56). Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.
Anandalaksmy, S. (1994). The girl child and the family: An action research study.
Department of Women and Child Development, Ministry of Human Resources
Development, India.
Anandalaksmy, S. , & Bajaj, M. (1981). Childhood in the weawers' community in
Varanasi: socialization for adult roles. In D. Sinha (Ed.), Socialization of the Indian Child
(pp. 31–38). New Delhi: Concept Publishing Company.
Anastasi, A. (1958). Heredity, environment, and the question “How?” Psychological
Review, 65, 197–208.
Anastasi, A. (1992). A century of psychological science. American Psychologist, 47,
842–843.
Anderson, R. C. , & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.),
Comprehension and Teaching: Research Reviews. Newark, NJ: IRA.
Andersson, B. E. (1992). Effects of day-care on cognitive and socioemotional
competence of 13-year-old Swedish schoolchildren. Child Development, 63, 20–36.
Angyal, A. (1951). A theoretical model for personality studies. Journal of Personality, 20,
131— 142.
Applegate, J. L. , Burleson, B. R. , & Delia, J. G. (1992). Reflection enhancing parenting
as an antecedent to children's social-cognitive and communicative development. In I. E.
Sigel , A. V. McGillicuddy DeLisi , & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems (pp.
3–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Aries, P. (1962). Centuries of childhood: A social history of family life ( R. Baldick ,
Trans.). New York: Knopf.
Aries, P. (1980). Two successive motivations for the declining birth rate in the West.
Population and Development Review, 6, 645–650.
Aswad, B. (1974). Visiting patterns among women of the elite in a small Turkish city.
Anthropological Quarterly, 47, 9–27.
Ataman, J. , & Epir, S. (1972). Socio economic status and classificatory behavior among
Turkish children. In L.J.C. Cronbach & P.J.D. Drenth (Eds.), Mental test and and cultural
adaptation. (pp. 329–337). The Hague: Mouton.
Ayçiçeği, A. (1993). The effects of the mother training program. Unpublished master's
thesis, Bogazigi University, Istanbul.
Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer interaction and problem solving: When are two heads better
than one? Child Development, 59, 87–96.
Azuma, H. (1984). Secondary control as a heterogeneous category. American
Psychologist, 39, 970–971.
Azuma, H. (1986). Why study child development in Japan? In H. Stevenson , H. Azuma ,
& K. Hakuta (Eds.). Child development and education in Japan (pp. 3–12). New York:
Freeman.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Bakan, D. (1968). Disease, pain, and sacrifice. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Mental development in the child and the race. New York:
Macmillan.
Baldwin, J. M. (1909). Darwin and the humanities. Baltimore: Review Publishing.
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On
the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626.
Baltes, P. B. , & Brim, O. (Eds.). (1979). Life span development and behavior (Vol. 2).
New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B. , Reese, H. W. , & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental
psychology. Annual review of psychology, 31, 65–110.
Bandura, A. (1962). Social learning through imitation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska
symposium on motivation (Vol. 10, pp. 211–271). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska
Press.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. London: Allen & Unwin.
Barciauskas, R. C. , & Hull, D. B. (1989). Loving and working: Reweaving women's
public and private lives. Bloomington, IN: Meyer-Stone Books.
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Barnett, W. S. (1985). The Perry Preschool Program and its long-term effects: A benefit-
cost analysis. Ypsilanti MI:, High/Scope Educational Research Foundation: Early
Childhood Policy Papers.
Barry, H. , Bacon, M. , & Child, I. (1957). A cross-cultural survey of some sex differences
in socialization. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 327–332.
Barry, H. , Child, I. , & Bacon, M. (1959). Relation of child training to subsistence
economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 31–63.
Basaran, F. A. (1974). Psiko-sosyal geli§im [Psycho-social development]. Ankara,
Turkey: Ankara University Press.
Batson, C. D. (1990). How social an animal? American Psychologist, 45, 336–346.
Baumeister, R. F. (1986). Identity: Cultural change and the struggle for self. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology
Monographs, 4 (1, Pt. 2).
Baumrind, D. (1980). New directions in socialization research. American Psychologist,
35, 639–652.
Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child
development today and tomorrow (pp. 349–378). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Befu, H. (1986). The social and cultural background of child development in Japan and
the United States. In H. Stevenson , H. Azuma , & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development
and education in Japan (pp. 13–25). New York: Freeman.
Bekman, S. (1990). Alternative to the available: Home based vs. centre based programs.
Early Childhood Development and Care, 58, 109–119.
Bekman, S. (1993). The preschool education system in Turkey revisited. OMEP
International Journal of Early childhood, 25, 13–19.
Bellah, R. N. , Madsen, R. , Sullivan, W. M. , Swidler, A. , & Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits
of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Belsky, J. , & Pensky, E. (1988). Developmental history, personality, and family
relationships: Toward an emergent family system. In R. A. Hinde & J.S. Hinde (Eds.),
Relationships within families (pp. 193–217). Oxford, England: Clarendon.
Bendix, R. (1967). Tradition and modernity reconsidered. Comparative studies in society
and history, 9, 292–346.
Benedict, P. (1974). The Kabul Giinii: Structured visiting in an Anatolian provincial town.
Anthropological Quarterly, 47, 28–47.
Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. New York: Mentor.
Benedict, R. (1970). Patterns of the good culture. Psychology Today, 4, 53–55.
Bennett, J. M. (1993). Jomtien revisited: A plea for a differentiated approach. In L.
Eldering & P. Leseman (Eds.), Early intervention and culture (pp. 11–19). Netherlands,
The Hague: UNESCO.
Bennett, J. (1984). The tie that binds: Peasant marriage and families in late Medieval
England. Journal of Interdisciplinary History, 15, 111–129.
Berger, B. , & Berger, P. L. (1984). The war over the family. New York: Anchor.
Berger, P. L. , & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. New York:
Doubleday.
Berman, J. J. (Ed.). (1990). Cross-cultural perspectives: Nebraska symposium on
motivation 1989. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Bernstein, B. (1974). Class, codes, and control: Theoretical studies toward a sociology of
language (rev. ed.). New York: Shocken.
Berrueta-Clement, J. R. , Schweinhart, L. L. , Barnett, W. , Epstein, A. , & Weikart, D.
(1984). Changed lives: The effects of the Perry Preschool Programme on youths through
age 19. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Berry, J. W. (1969). On cross-cultural comparability. International Journal of Psychology,
4, 119–128.
Berry, J. W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style: Comparative studies in cultural
and psychological adaptation. New York: Sage/Halsted.
Berry, J. W. (1979). A cultural ecology of social behaviour. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 177–206). New York:
Academic Press.
Berry, J. W. (1980). Ecological analyses for cross-cultural psychology. In N. Warren
(Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 157–189). New York: Academic
Press.
Berry, J. W. (1984). Toward a universal psychology of cognitive competence.
International Journal Of Psychology, 19, 335–361.
Berry, J. W. (1985). Cultural psychology and ethnic psychology: A comparative analysis.
In I. Reyes-Lagunes & Y. Poortinga (Eds.), From a different perspective (pp. 3–15).
Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Berry, J. W. (1989). Imposed etics-emics-derived etics: The operationalization of a
compelling idea. International Journal of Psychology, 24, 721–735.
Berry, J. W. (1990). Cultural variations in cognitive style. In S. Wapner (Ed.), Bio-psycho-
social factors in cognitive style (pp. 289–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Berry, J. W. , & Bennett, J. A. (1992). Cree conceptions of cognitive competence.
International Journal of Psychology, 27, 73–88.
Berry, J. W. , & Dasen, P. R. (Eds.) (1974). Culture and cognition: Readings in cross-
cutural psychology. London: Methuen & Co Ltd.
Berry, J. W. , Irvine, S. H. , & Hunt, E. B. (Eds.). (1988). Indigenous cognition:
Functioning in cultural context. Dordrecht: Nijhoff.
Berry, J. W. , Poortinga, Y. H. , Seagall, M. H. , & Dasen, P. R. (1992). Cross-cultural
psychology: Research and applications. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
Bertalanffy, L. von . (1968). General systems theory. New York: Braziller.
Bisht, S. , & Sinha, D. (1981). Socialization, family and psychological differentiation. In D.
Sinha (Ed.), Socialization of the Indian child (pp. 41–54). New Delhi: Concept.
Boas, F. (1911). The mind of primitive man. New York: Macmillan.
Bock, P. K. (1988). Rethinking psychological anthropology: Continuity and change in the
study of human actions. New York: Freeman.
Bond, M. H. (Ed.). (1986). The psychology of the Chinese people. Hong Kong: Oxford
University Press.
Bond, M. H. (Ed.). (1988). The cross-cultural challenge to social psychology. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Bond, M. H. , & Cheung, T. S. (1983). The spontaneous self-concept of college students
in Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14,
153–171.
Bornstein, M. H. (1984). Cross-cultural developmental psychology. In M. H. Bornstein &
M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (pp. 231–281).
London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Between caretakers and their young: Two modes of interaction
and their consequences for cognitive growth. In M. H. Bornstein & J. S. Bruner (Eds.),
Interaction in human development (pp. 197–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Bornstein, M. H. (1991). Cultural approaches to parenting. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Bornstein, M. H. , & Bruner, J. S. (Eds.). (1989). Interaction in human development.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bornstein, M. H. , Tal, J. , & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (1991). Parenting in cross-cultural
perspective: The United States, France and Japan. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural
approaches to parenting (pp. 69–89). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bornstein, M. H. , & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (1990). Activities and interactions of mothers
and their first born infants in the first 6 months of life. Child Development, 61,
1206–1217.
Bornstein, M. H. , Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. , Tal, J. , Ludemann, P. , Toda S. , Rahn, C. W.
, Pecheux, M. G. , Azuma, H. , & Vardi, D. (1992). Maternal responsiveness to infants in
three societies: The United States, France, and Japan. Child Development, 63, 808–821.
Boszormenyi-Nagy, I. , & Spark, G. (1973). Invisible loyalties: Reciprocity in inter
generational family therapy. New York: Harper & Row.
Boyden, J. , (1990). Childhood and the policy makers: A comparative perspective on the
globalization of childhood. In A. James & A. Prout (Eds.), Constructing and
reconstructing childhood (pp. 184–215). New York: Palmer Press.
Bradburn, N. M. (1963). Nachievement and father dominance in Turkey. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 464–468.
Brazelton, B. T. (1982). Early intervention: What does it mean? In H. E. Fitzgerald (Ed.),
Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (pp. 1–34). New York: Plenum.
Brim, O. G. , & Kagan, J. (Eds.). (1980). Constancy and change in human development.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brislin, R. W. (1983). Cross-cultural research in psychology. Annual Review of
Psychology, 34, 363–400.
Brislin, R. W. (1993). Culture's influence on behavior. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich.
Britton, E. (1976). The peasant family in 14th-century England. Peasant Studies, 5,
20–27.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). Is early intervention effective! Unpublished Report for
Department of Health Education and Welfare, Washington DC.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature
and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development:
Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723–742.
Bronfenbrenner, U. , & Ceci, S. J. (1993). Heredity, environment, and the question
“how?”—A first approximation. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature-nurture (pp.
313–324). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bronfenbrenner, U. , & Weiss, H. B. (1983). Beyond policies without people: An
ecological perspective on child and family policy. In E. F. Zigler , S. L. Kagan , & E.
Klugman (Eds.), Children, families and government: Perspectives on American social
policy (pp. 393–414). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bruner, J. S. , & Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Interaction in human development. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brunswik, R. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory. Psychological
Review, 62, 236–242.
Bulatao, R. A. (1979). On the nature of the transition in the value of children. Honolulu,
Hawaii: East-West Population Institute.
Caldwell, J. C. (1977). Towards a restatement of demographic transition theory. In J. C.
Caldwell (Ed.), The persistence of high fertility (pp. 25–122). Canberra: Australian
National University.
Caldwell, J. C. (1979). “Education as a factor in mortality decline: An examination of
Nigerian data?” Population Studies, 33, 395–413.
Caldwell, J. C. (1980). “Mass education as a determinant of the timing of fertility
decline?” Population and development Review, 6(2), 225–256.
Campbell, D. T. (1975). On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and
between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 30, 1103–1126.
Carraher, T. N. , Schliemann, A. D. , & Carraher, D. W. (1988). Mathematical concepts in
everyday life. In G. B. Saxe & M. Gearhart (Eds.), Children's mathematics: New
directions in child development (pp. 71–87). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Carvioto, J. (1981). Nutrition, stimulation, mental development and learning. W. O.
Atwater Memorial Lecture presented at the 12th International Congress of Nutrition, San
Diego, CA.
Cashmore, J. A. , & Goodnow, J. J. (1986). Influences on Australian parents' values:
Ethnicity versus socioeconomic status. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 17,
441–454.
Caudill, W. A. , & Frost, L. (1973). A comparison of maternal care and infant behavior in
Japanese-American, American and Japanese families. In W. P. Lebra (Ed.), Mental
health research in Asia and the Pacific. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.
Caudill, W. A. , & Schooler, C. (1973). Child behavior and child rearing in Japan and the
United States: An interim report. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 157, 323–338.
Chamoux, M. N. (1986). Apprendre autrement: Aspects des pedagogies dites
informelles chex les Indiens du Mexique [A different type of learning: Aspects of informal
pedagogy among Mexican Indians]. In P. Rossel (Ed.), Demain Vartisanat? [Tomorrow's
handicraft?]. Paris: Cahiers IUED.
Chao, R. K. (1995). Chinese and European-American mothers' views about the role of
parenting upon children's school success. Journal Cross-Cultural Psychology(in print).
Childs, C. P. , & Greenfield, P. M. (1980). Informal modes of learning and teaching: The
case of Zinacantoco weaving. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural psychology
(Vol. 2, pp. 269–316). London: Academic Press.
Chodorow, N. (1974). Family structure and feminine personality. In M. Z. Rosaldo & L.
Lamphere (Eds.), Women, culture and society (pp. 43–66). Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mothering: Psychoanalysis and the sociology
of gender. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Chodorow, N. (1989). Feminism and psychoanalytic theory. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Choi, S. H. (1992). Communicative socialization processes: Korea and Canada. In S.
Iwawaki , Y. Kashima , & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology (pp.
103–121). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Christie, R. , & Jahoda, M. (1954). Studies in the scope and method of “the Authoritarian
Personality?” Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Cicirelli, V. G. , Evans, J. W. , & Schiller, J. S. (1969). The impact of Head Start: An
evaluation of the effects of Head Start on children's cognitive and affective development.
Washington, DC: Westinghouse Learning Corporation, Ohio University.
Cochrane, S. , & Mehra, K. (1983). Socioeconomic determinants of infant and child
mortality in developing countries. In O. A. Wagner (Ed.), Child development and
international development: Research-policy interfaces (pp. 27–44). San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Cohler, B. , & Geyer, S. (1982). Psychological autonomy and interdependence within the
family. In F. Walsh (Ed.), Normal family processes (pp. 196–227). New York: Guilford.
Cohler, B. , & Grunebaum, H. (1981). Mothers, grandmothers, and daughters:
Personality and child-care in three generation families. New York: Wiley.
Cole, M. , Hood, L. , & McDermott, R. P. (1978). Concept of ecological validity: Their
differing implications for comparative cognitive research. Quarterly Newsletter of the
Institute for Comparative Human Development, 2, 34–37.
Cole, M. , Sharp, D. , & Lave, C. (1976). The cognitive consequences of education:
Some empirical evidence and theoretical misgivings. Urban Review, 9, 218–233.
Coleman, J. S. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Coll, C.T.G. (1990). Developmental outcome of minority infants: A process-oriented look
into our beginnings. Child Development, 61, 270–289.
Consortium for Longitudinal Studies (1983). As the twig is bent: Lasting effects of
preschool programs. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Connolly, K. (1985). Can there be a psychology for the Third World? Bulletin of the
British Psychological Society, 38, 249–257.
Consultative Group on Early Childhood Care and Development (1986). Measuring early
childhood development: A review of instruments and measures. New York: UNICEF.
Copple, C. , Cline, M. , & Smith, A. (1987). Paths to the future: Long term effects of Head
Start in Philadelphia school district. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services.
Cotterell, J. L. (1986). Work and community influences on the quality of child rearing.
Child Development, 57, 362–374.
Cousins, S. (1989). Culture and selfhood in Japan and the U. S. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 56, 124–131.
Cowan, P. A. , Field, D. , Hansen, D. A. , Skolnick, A. , & Swanson, G. E. (Eds.). (1993).
Family, self, and society. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American
Psychologist, 12, 671–684.
Curran, V. H. (Ed.). (1984). Nigerian children: Developmental perspectives. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Cushman, P. (1990). Why the self is empty: Toward a historically situated psychology.
American Psychologist, 45, 599–611.
Dalal, A. K. (1990). India: Psychology in Asia and the Pacific. In G. Shouksmith & E. A.
Shouksmith (Eds.), Status reports on teaching research in eleven countries. Bangkok:
UNESCO.
Damon, W. (Ed.). (1989). Child development today and tomorrow. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
D'Andrade, R. G. , & Strauss, C. (Eds.). (1992). Human motives and cultural models.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Darroch, R. , Meyer, P. A. , & Singarimbun, M. (1981). Two are not enough: The value of
children to Javanese and Sundanese parents. Honolulu: East-West Population Institute.
Dasen, P. R. (1984). The cross-cultural study of intelligence: Piaget and the Baoule.
International Journal of Psychology, 19, 407–434.
Dasen, P. R. (1988a). Developpement psychologique et activites quotidiennes ches des
enfants Africains [Psychological development and daily activities of African children].
Enfance, 41, 3–24.
Dasen, P. R. (1988b). Cultures et developpement cognitif: La recherche et ses
applications [Cultures and cognitive development: Research and its applications]. In R.
Bureau & D. de Saivre (Eds.), Apprentissages et cultures: Les manieres d'apprendre
(Colloque de Cerisy) [Apprenticeship and culture: ways of learning (Cerisy Colloquim)
(pp. 123–141). Paris: Karthala.
Dasen, P. R. , Berry, J. W. , & Sartorius, N. (Eds.), (1988). Health and cross-cultural
psychology: Toward applications. London: Sage.
Dasen, P. R. , & Jahoda, G. (1986). Cross-cultural human development. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 413–416.
Dawson, J.L.M. (1967). Traditional versus Western attitudes in West Africa: The
construction, validation and application of a measuring device. British Journal of Social
and Clinical Psychology, 6, 81–96.
De Silva, M. , Nikapota, A. , & Vidyasagara (1988). “Advocacy and opportunity: Planning
for child mental health in Sri Lanka.” Health, Policy and Planning, 3, 302–307.
Deutsch, M. (1962). Cooperation and trust: Some theoretical notes. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),
Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
De Vos, G. (1968). Achievement and innovation in culture and personality. In E. Norbeck
, D. Price-Williams , & E. W. McCord (Eds.), The study of personality (pp. 348–370). New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
De Vos, G. (1985). Dimensions of the self in Japanese culture. In M. G. De Vos & F.L.K.
Hsu (Eds.), Culture and self. New York: Tavistock.
Diaz-Guerrero, R. (1991). Historic-sociocultural premises (HSCPs) and global change.
International Journal of Psychology, 26(5), 665–673.
Doi, T. (1973). Anatomy of dependence. Tokyo: Kodansha International.
Doi, L. A. (1974). Amae: A Key concept for understanding Japanese personality
structure. In R. A. LeVine (Ed.), Culture and personality (pp. 307–314).
Doob, L. W. (1967). Scales for assaying psychological modernization in Africa. Public
Opinion Quarterly, 31, 414–421.
Dornbusch, S. M. , Ritter, P. L. , Leiderman, P. H. , Roberts, O. F. , & Fraleigh, M. J.
(1987). The relation of parenting style to adolescent school performance. Child
Development, 58, 1244–1257.
Drenth, P.J.D. (1991). Scientific and social responsibility: A dilemma for the psychologist
as a scientist. Paper presented at the Second European Congress, Budapest.
Duben, A. (1982). The significance of family and kinship in urban Turkey. In Ç.
Kağιtçbaşι (Ed.), Sex, roles, family and community in Turkey (pp. 73–99). Bloomington:
Indiana University Press.
DuBois, C. (1944). The people ofAlor. New York: Harper & Row.
Dym, B. (1988). Ecological perspectives on change in families. In H. B. Weiss & F. H.
Jacobs (Eds.), Evaluating family programs (pp. 477–496). New York: Aldine.
Eckensberger, L. (1979). A metamethodological evaluation of psychological theories
from a cross-cultural perspective. In L. Eckensberger , W. Lonner , & Y. H. Poortinga
(Eds.), Cross-cultural contributions to psychology (pp. 255–275). Lisse: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
Eckensberger, L. (1990). On the necessity of the culture concept in psychology: A view
from cross-cultural psychology. In F.J.R. van de Vijver & G.J.M. Hutschemaekers (Eds.),
The investigation of culture (pp. 153–177). Tilburg: Tilburg University Press.
Eisenberg, L. (1982). Conceptual issues on biobehavioral interactions. In D. L. Parron &
L. Eisenberg (Eds.), Infants at risk for developmental dysfunction (pp. 57–68).
Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences, Institute of Medicine.
Eksi, A. (1982). Genglerimiz ve sorunlari [Our youth and their problems]. Istanbul:
Istanbul University Publication, 2790.
Ekstrand, L. H. , & Ekstrand, G. (1987). Children's perceptions of norms and sanctions in
two cultures. In Q. Kagitgiba§i (Ed.), Growth and progress in cross-cultural psychology
(pp. 171–180). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Eldering, L. , & Kloprogge, F. (Eds.). (1989). Different cultures same school. Lisse:
Swets & Zeitlinger.
Eldering, L. , & Vedder, P. (1993). Culture-sensitive home intervention: The Dutch Hippy
experiment. In L. Eldering & P. Leseman (Eds.), Early intervention and culture (pp.
231–252). Paris: UNESCO.
Eliram, T. , & Schwarzwald, J. (1987). Social orientation among Israeli youth. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 18, 31–44.
Engle, P. L. (1986). The intersecting needs of working mothers and their young children:
1980 to 1985. Unpublished manuscript, California Polytechnic State University, CA.
Enriquez, V. G. (1988). The structure of Philippine social values: Towards integrating
indigenous values and appropriate technology. In D. Sinha & H.S.R. Kao (Eds.), Social
values and development: Asian perspectives (pp. 124–148). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Enriquez, G. E. (1990). Indigenous psychology. Philippines: New Horizons Press.
Ercan, S. (1993). The short-term effects of the Home Intervention Program on the
cognitive development of childeren. Unpublished Master's Thesis. Istanbul: Bogazici
University.
Erelçn, F. G. (1988). Collectivistic norms in Turkey: Tendency to give and receive
support. Unpublished master's thesis, Bogazigi University, Istanbul.
Etzioni, A. (1993). Spirit of community. Rights, responsibilities and the communitarian
agenda. New York: Crown.
Evans, J. L. , & Myers, R. G. (1985). Improving program actions to meet the intersecting
needs of women and children in developing countries: A policy and program review. The
Consultative Group on Early Childhood Care and Development, Ypsilanti, MI:
High/Scope Educational Research Foundation.
Evans, J. L. , & Shah, P. M. (1994). Child care programmes as an entry point for
maternal and child health components of primary health care. Geneva: World Health
Organisation.
Fawcett, J. T. (1983). Perceptions of the Value of Children: Satisfactions and costs. In R.
Bulatao , R. D. Lee , P. E. Hollerbach , & J. Bongaarts (Eds.), Determinants of fertility in
developing countries (Vol. 1, pp. 347–369). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Featherman, D. L. , & Lerner, R. M. (1985). Ontogenesis and sociogenesis:
Problematics for theory and research about development and socialization across the
lifespan. American Sociological Review, 50, 659–676.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 8,
117–140.
Fisek, G. O. (1991). A cross-cultural examination of proximity and hierarchy as
dimensions of family structure. Family Process, 30, 121–133.
Forgas, J. P. , & Bond, M. H. (1985). Cultural influences on the perception of interaction
episodes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 75–88.
Fu, V. R. , Hinkle, D. E. , & Hanna, M. A. (1986). A three-generational study of the
development of individual dependence and family interdependence. Genetic, Social and
General Psychology Monographs, 112(2), 153–171.
Fujinaga, T. (1991). Development of personality among Japanese Children. Paper
presented at the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development,
Workshop on Asian Perspectives of Psychology, Ann Arbor, MI.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr . (1966). Industrialization and the American family: A look
backward. American Sociological Review, 31, 326–337.
Gabrenya, W. K. , Wang, Y. , & Latane, B. (1985). Social loafing on an optimizing task:
Cross-cultural differences among Chinese and Americans. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 16, 223–242.
Gaines, A. D. (1984). Cultural definitions, behavior and the person in American
psychiatry. In A. J. Marsella & G. M. White (Eds.), Cultural conceptions of mental health
and therapy (pp. 167–192). Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
Garbarino, J. (1990). The human ecology of early risk. In S. J. Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff
(Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (pp. 78–96). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Garber, H. , & Heber, R. (1983). Modification of predicted cognitive development in high-
risk children through early intervention. In M. K. Oetterman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), How
much can intelligence be increased? (pp. 121–137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Gay, J. , & Cole, M. (1967). The new mathematics and an old culture. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Gectan, E. (1973). Toplumumuz bireylerinde kimlik kavraml [Identity in the individuals of
our society]. Ankara, Turkey: Ankara University, Faculty of Education Publication.
Geertz, C. (1975). On the nature of anthropological understanding. American Scientist,
63, 47–53.
Gellner, E. (1992). Postmodernism, reason and religion. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Georgas, J. (1989). Changing family values in Greece: From collectivistic to
individualistic. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 80–91.
Gerard, H. B. , & Rabbie, J. M. (1961). Fear and social comparison. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 62, 586–592.
Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 26(2), 309–320.
Gergen, K. J. (1985). Social constructionist inquiry: Context and implications. In K. J.
Gergen & K. E. Davis (Eds.), The social construction of the person. New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life. New
York: Basic Books.
Gergen, K. J. , & Davis, K. E. (Eds.). (1985). The social construction of the person. New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Gillespie, J. M. , & Allport, G. W. (1955). Youth's outlook on the future: A cross national
study. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goodnow, J. J. (1984). Parents' ideas about parenting and development: A review of
issues and recent work. In M. E. Lamb , A. L. Brown & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances in
developmental psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 193–242). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Goodnow, J. J. (1985). Change and variation in ideas about childhood and parenting. In
I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Parental belief systems (pp. 235–270). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parents' ideas, actions, and feeling: Models and methods from
developmental and social psychology. Child development, 59, 286–320.
Gorer, G. , & Rickman, J. (1949). The people of great Russia. New York: Norton.
Gottfried, A. , & Gottfried, A. (1984). Home environment and cognitive development in
young children of middle socioeconomic status families. In A. Gottfried (Ed.), Home
environment and early cognitive development (pp. 57–116). New York: Academic Press.
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experimental canalization of behavioral development.
Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13.
Gray, S. , Ramsey, B. , & Klaus, R. (1983). The early training project 1962–1980. In The
Consortium for Longitudional Studies (Ed.), As the twig is bent. (pp. 33–70). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenfield, P. M. (1994). Independence and interdependence as developmental scripts:
implications for theory, research and practice. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking
(Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development, (pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenfield, P. M. , & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (1994). Cross-cultural roots of minority child
development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenfield, P. M. , & Lave, J. (1982). Cognitive aspects of informal education. In D.
Wagner & H. Stevenson (Eds.), Cultural perspectives on child development (pp.
181–207). San Francisco: Freeman.
Greenwald, A. G. , & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.),
Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 129–178). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Grotevant, H. D. (1989). Child development within the family context. In W. Damon (Ed.),
Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 34–51). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Gusfield, J. R. (1967). Tradition and modernity: Misplaced polarities in the study of social
change. American Journal of Sociology, 73, 351–362.
Guerin, P. J. (1976). Family therapy: The first 25 years. In P. J. Guerin (Ed.), Family
therapy: Theory and practice (pp. 2–22). New York: Gardner.
Guisinger, S. , & Blatt, S. J. (1994). Individuality and relatedness: Evolution of a
fundamental dialectic. American Psychologist, 49, 104–111.
Gupta, G. C. (1992). Ecology, cognition, metacognition and mind. New Delhi, India: B. R.
Publishing.
Gutierrez, J. , Sameroff, A. J. , & Karrer, B. M. (1988). Acculturation and SES effects on
Mexican-American parentsM concepts of development. Child Development, 59,
250–255.
Haglund, E. (1982). The problem of the match: Cognitive transition between early
childhood and primary school: Nigeria. Journal of Developing Areas, 17, 77–92.
Halpern, R. (1990). Community based early intervention. In S. J. Meisels & J. P.
Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (pp. 469–498). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Hanawalt, A. A. (1986). The ties that bound: Peasant families in medieval England. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Harkness, S. (1992). Parental ethnotheories in action. In I. E. Sigel , A. V. Mc.Gillicuddy-
DeLisi , & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems (pp. 373–391). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Harkness, S. , & Super, C. (1993). The developmental Niche: Implications for children's
literacy development. In L. Eldering & P. Leseman (Eds.), Early intervention and culture
(pp. 115–132). Paris: UNESCO.
Harrison A. D. , Wilson, M. N. , Pine, C. J. , Chan, S. R. , & Buriel, R. (1990). Family
Ecologies of ethnic minority children. Child Development, 61, 347–362.
Hartman, H. (1958). Ego psychology and the problem of adaptation (D. Rapaport,
Trans.). New York: International Universities Press. (Original work published 1939)
Hatano, G. (1982). Cognitive consequences of practice in culture specific procedural
skills. Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition 4 (1),
15–18.
Hayashi, C. , & Suzuki, T. (1984). Changes in belief systems, quality of life issues and
social conditions over 25 years in post-war Japan. Annals of the Institute of Statistical
Mathematics, 36, 135–161.
Hebbeler, K. (1985). An old and a new question on the effects of early education for
children from low income families. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 7,
207–216.
Heelas, P. , & Lock, A. (Eds.). (1981). Indigenous psychologies: The anthropology of the
self London: Academic Press.
Helling, G. A. (1966). The Turkish village as a social system. Los Angeles, CA:
Occidental College.
Hendrix, L. (1985). Economy and child training reexamined. Ethos, 13, 246–261.
Heron, A. , & Kroeger, E. (1981). Introduction to developmental psychology. In H. C.
Triandis & A. Heron (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–15).
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Herskovits, M. J. (1948). Man and his works: The science of cultural anthropology. New
York: Knopf.
Hess, R. D. , & Shipman, V. C. (1965). Early experience and the socialization of
cognitive modes in children. Child Development, 36, 869–888.
Himes, J. R. , Landers, C. , & Leslie, J. (1992). Women, work and child care. Florence,
Italy: Innocenti, UNICEF.
Hoffman, L. W. (1987). The value of children to parents and childrearing patterns. InÇ.
Kagitçibaşi (Ed.), Growth and progress in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 159–170).
Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Hoffman, L. W. (1988). Cross-cultural differences in childrearing goals. New Directions
for Child Development, 40, 99–122.
Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Moral internalization: Current theory and research. In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 85–133). New York:
Academic Press.
Hoffmann, F. L. (1989). Developmental issues, college students and the 1990s. Journal
of College Student Psychotherapy, 4, 3–12.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hogan, R. (1975). Theoretical egocentrism and the problem of compliance. American
Psychologist, 30, 533–540.
Holtzman, W. H. , Diaz-Guerrero, R. , & Swartz, J. D. (1975). Personality development in
two cultures: A cross-cultural longitudinal study of school children in Mexico and the
United States. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Horowitz, F. D. (1993). The need for a comprehensive new environmentalism. In R.
Plomin & G.E.C. McClearn (Eds.), Nature-nurture (pp. 341–354). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Hoselitz, B. (1965). Economics and the idea of mankind. New York: Columbia University
Press.
Hsu, F.L.K. (Ed.). (1972). Psychological anthropology. (2nd ed:). Cambridge, MA:
Schenkman.
Hsu, F.L.K. (1985). The self in cross-cultural perspective. In A. J. Marsella , G. DeVos , &
F.L.K. Hsu (Eds.), Culture and self: Asian and Western perspectives (pp. 24–55). New
York: Tavistock.
Huesmann, L. R. , Eron, L. D. , Lefkowitz, M. N. , & Walder, L. O. (1983). The stability of
aggression over time and generation. Paper presented at the meeting of the society for
Research in Child Development, Detroit, MI.
Hurrelman, K. (1988). Social structure and personality development: The individual as a
productive processor of reality. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Huston, A. C. (1991). Antecedents, consequences, and possible solutions for poverty
among children. In A. C. Huston (Ed.), Children in poverty: Child development and public
policy (pp. 282–315). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Huynh, C. T. (1979). The concept of endogenous development centered on man. Paris:
UNESCO.
Imamoğlu, E. O. (1987). An interdependence model of human development. In Ç.
Kağitçibşai (Ed.), Growth and progress (pp. 138–145). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Inglehart, R. (1991). Changing human goals and values: A proposal for a study of global
change. In K. Pawlik (Ed.), Perception and assessment of global environmental
conditions and change (Page c, Rep. No. 1).
Inkeles, A. (1969). Making men modern. On the causes and consequences of individual
change in six developing countries. American Journal of Sociology, 75, 208–225.
Inkeles, A. (1977). Understanding and misunderstanding individual modernity. Journal of
Cross Cultural Psychology, 8, 135–176.
Inkeles, A. , & Levinson, D. J. (1954). The study of modal personality and socio-cultural
systems. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 977–1020).
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Inkeles, A. , & Smith, D. H. (1974). Becoming modern: Individual changes in six
developing countries. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Irvine, S. H. (1966). Towards a rational for testing abilities and attainments in Africa.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 36, 24–32.
Irvine, S. H. (1970). Affect and construct—a cross-cultural check on theories of
intelligence. Journal of Social Psychology, 80, 23–30.
Ito, K. L. (1985). Affective bonds: Hawaiian interrelationships of self. In G. M. White & J.
Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Person, self, and experience: Exploring Pacific ethnopsychologies
(pp. 301–311). Berkeley: University of California Press.
Iwawaki, S. (1986). Achievement motivation and socialization. In S. E. Newstead , S. M.
Irvine , & P. L. Dann (Eds.), Human assessment: Cognition and motivation. Boston:
Martinus Nijhoff.
Jahoda, G. (1975). Applying cross-cultural psychology to the Third World. In J. W. Berry
& W. J. Lonner (Eds.), Applied cross-cultural psychology (pp. 3–7). Lisse: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
Jahoda, G. (1977). In pursuit of the emic-etic distinction: Can we ever capture it? In Y. H.
Poortinga (Ed.), Basic problems in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 55–63). Lisse: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
Jahoda, G. (1983). The cross-cultural emperor's conceptual clothes: The emic-etic issue
revisited. In J. B. Deregowski , S. Dziurawiec , & R. A. Annis (Eds.), Expiscations in
cross-cultural psychology (pp. 19–38). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Jahoda, G. (1986). A cross-cultural perspective on developmental psychology.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 417–437.
Jahoda, G. , & Dasen, P. R. (Eds.). (1986). International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 9(4), 413–416.
James, A. , & Prout, A. (1970). Constructing and reconstructing childhood. Lewes:
Falmer Press.
Jansen, H.A.M. (1987). The development of communal living in the Netherlands. In L.
Shamgar-Handelman & R. Palomba (Eds.), Alternative patterns of family life in modern
societies. Rome: Collana Monografie.
Jarrett, R. L. (1993). Voices from below: The value of ethnographic research for
informing public policy. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for
Research in Child Development, New Orleans.
Jolly, R. (1988). Deprivation in the child's environment: Seeking advantage in adversity.
Canadian Journal of Public Health Supplement, 20.
Kagan, J. (1984). Nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.
Kağitçibaşi Ç. . (1970). Social norms and authoritarianism: A Turkish-American
comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 444–451.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1973a). Early childhood education and intervention. UNESCO: Child,
Family, Community. Paris: UNESCO.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1973b). Psychological aspects of modernization in Turkey. Journal of
Cross- Cultural Psychology, 4, 157–174.
Kağitçibaşi, C. (1981). Early childhood education and intervention. UNESCO: Child,
Family, Community.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1982a). The changing value of children in Turkey (Publ. No. 60-E).
Honolulu: East-West Center.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1982b). Sex roles, value of children and fertility in Turkey. In Ç.
Kağitçibaşi (Ed.), Sex roles, family and community in Turkey (pp. 151–180).
Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1982c). Old-age security value of children: Cross-national socio-
economic evidence. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13, 29–42.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1984). Socialization in traditional society: A challenge to psychology.
International Journal of Psychology, 19, 145–157.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. . (1985a). Culture of separateness-culture of relatedness. In C. Klopp
(Ed.), 1984 Vision and Reality. Papers in Comparative Studies (Vol. 4, pp. 91–99).
Columbus, OH: Ohio State University.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1985b). A Model of family change through development: The Turkish
family in comparative perspective. In R. Lagunes & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), From a
different perspective: Studies of behavior across cultures (pp. 120–135). Lisse: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1986). Status of women in Turkey: Cross-cultural perspectives.
International Journal of Middle East Studies, 18, 485–499.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1987a). Child rearing in Turkey: Implications for immigration an
intervention. In L. Eldering & F. Kloprogge (Eds.), Different cultures, same school (pp.
137–152). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1987b). Individual and group loyalties. In Ç. Kağıtçıbaşı (Ed.), Growth
and progress in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 94–103). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1990). Family and socialization in cross-cultural perspective: A model of
change. In J. Berman (Ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives: Nebraska symposium on
motivation, 1989 (pp. 135–200). Lincoln, NE: Nebraska University Press.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1991a). Decreasing infant mortality as a global demographic change: A
challenge to psychology. International Journal of Psychology, 26, 649–664.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1991b). The early enrichment project in Turkey. UNESCO-UNICEF-WFP
Notes Comments … (No. 193). Paris: UNESCO.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1992a). Linking the indigenous and universalist orientations. In S.
Iwawaki , Y. Kashima , & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology (pp.
29–37). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1992b). Research on parenting and child development in cross-cultural
perspective. In M. Rosenzweig (Ed.), International psychological science (pp. 137–160).
Washington, DC: APA.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1994a). A critical appraisal of individualism-collectivism: Toward a new
formulation. In U. Kim , H. Triandis , Ç. Kağıtçıbaşı , S-C. Choi , & G. Yoon (Eds.),
Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method and Applications (pp. 52–65). Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1994b). Human development and societal development. In Bouvy, A-M.
, v. d. Vijver, F.J.R. , Boski, P. (Eds.), Journeys in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 3–24).
Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. In J. W. Berry , M. H. Segali , & Ç.
Kağıtçıbaşı (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, 2nd Ed.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. , Benedek, L. , Dubois, A. , Carney, C. , Jallinoja, A. , &
Vlaardingerbroek, P. (1994). The interaction between the providers of family services.
Strasbourg: Council of Europe.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. & Berry, J. W. (1989). Cross-cultural psychology: Current research and
trends. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 493–531.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. , & Savasir, I. (1988). Human abilities in the eastern mediterranean. In S.
H. Irvine & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Human abilities in cultural context (pp. 232–262).
Cambridge; Cambridge University Press.
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. , Sunar, D. , & Bekman, S. (1988). Comprehensive preschool education
project: Final Report. Ottawa: International Development Research Centre.
Kahl, J. A. (1968). The measurement of modernism: Study of values in Brazil and
Mexico. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Kakar, S. (1978). The inner world: A psychoanalytic study of childhood and society in
India. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Kamerman, S.B. (1991). Child care policies and programs: An international overview.
Journal of Social Issues, 47, 179–196.
Kamerman, S. B. , & Kahn, A. J. (1989). Family policy: Has the United States learned
from Europe? Policy Studies Review, 8, 581–598.
Kao, H.S.R. , & Hong, N. S. (1988). Minimal ‘self’ and Chinese work behavior:
Psychology of the grass-roots. In D. Sinha & H.S.R. Kao (Eds.), Social values and
development: Asian perspectives (pp. 254–272). London: Sage.
Kapp, W. K. (1963). Hindu culture, economic development and economic planning in
India. Bombay: Asia Publishing House.
Kardiner, A. , & Linton, R. (1945). The individual and his society. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Kashima, Y. , Siegel, M. , Tanaka, K. , & Isaka, H. (1988). Universalism in lay
conceptions of distributive justice: A cross-cultural examination. International Journal of
Psychology, 23, 51–64.
Kashima, Y. , & Triandis, H. C. (1986). The self-serving bias in attributions as a coping
strategy: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17, 83–97.
Kassebaum, N. L. (1994). Head start: Only the best for America's children. American
Psychologist, 49, 123–126.
Kaur, B. , & Saraswathi, T. S. (1992). New directions in human development and family
studies: Research, policy, and programme interfaces. International Journal of
Psychology, 27, 333–349.
Keller, H. , Schölmerich, A. , & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1988). Communication patterns in
adult-infant interactions in western and non-western cultures. Journal of cross-cultural
psychology, 19, 427–445.
Keniston, K. (1985). The myth of family independence. In J. M. Henslin (Ed.), Marriage
and family in a changing society (2nd ed., pp. 27–33). New York: Free Press.
Kessen, W. (1991). The American child and other cultural inventions. In M. Woodhead ,
P. Light , & R. Carr (Eds.), Growing up in a changing society (pp. 37–53). London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Kim, U. , & Berry, J. W. (1993). Indigenous psychologies: Research and experience in
cultural context. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Kim, U. , Triandis, H. C. , Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. Choi, S-C. , & Yoon, G. (Eds.). (1944).
Individualism and collectivism: Theory method applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
King, K. , & Myers, R. (1983). Preventing school failure: The relationship between
preschool and primary education. Ottawa, Canada: International Development Research
Center.
Kiray, M. B. (1981). The women of small town. In N. Abadan-Unat (Ed.), Women in
Turkish society (pp. 259–274). Leiden: Brill.
Kirkpatrick, J. , & White, G. M. (1985). Exploring ethnopsychologies. In G. M. White & J.
Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Person, self and experience: Exploring Pacific ethnopsychologies.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Klaus, R. A. , & Gray, S. W. (1968). The early training project for disadvantaged children.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 33(4, Serial No. 120).
Kluckhohn, C. (1957). Mirror for man. New York: Premier Books.
Kohlberg, L. (1981). From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get
away with it in the study of moral development. In L. Kohlberg (Ed.), Essays on moral
development: The philosophy of moral development (Vol. 1, pp. 101–189). San
Francisco: Harper & Row.
Kohn, M. L. (1969). Class and conformity: A study in values. New York: Dorsey.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
Kohut, H. (1984). How does analysis cure? Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kojima, H. (1984). A significant stride toward the comparative study of control. American
Psychologist, 39, 972–973.
Koppitz, E. M. (1968). Psychological evaluation of children's human figure drawings.
New York: Grune & Stratton.
Kornadt, H. J. (1987). The aggression motive and personality development: Japan and
Germany. In F. Halish & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention and volition. Berlin:
Springer-Verlag.
Kotchabhakdi, N. J. , Winichagoon, P. , Smitasiri, S. , Dhanamitta, S. , & Valya-Sevi, A.
(1987). The integration of psychosocial components in nutrition education in
northeastern Thai villages. Asia Pacific Journal of Public Health, 2, 16–25.
Kroeber, A. L. , & Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture, part HI: Papers of the Peabody
Museum of Harvard University. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.
Kuhn, D. , & Brannock, J. (1977). Development of the isolation of variables scheme in
experimental and “natural experiment” context. Developmental Psychology, 13, 9–14.
Kuhn, M. H. , & McPartland, T. S. (1954). An empirical investigation of self-attitudes.
American Sociological Review, 19, 68–76.
Kvale, S. (Ed.). (1992). Psychology and postmodernism. London: Sage.
Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC) (1983). Culture and cognitive
development. In Kessen W. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (14th. ed., Vol. 1, pp.
295–356). New York: Wiley.
Lakoff, G. , & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Lambert, W. E. (1987). The fate of old-country values in a new land: A cross-national
study of childrearing. Canadian Psychology, 28(1), 9–20.
Lamborn, S. D. , Mounts, N. S. , Steinberg, L. , & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Patterns of
competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian,
indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 62, 1049–1065.
Landers, C. (1992). Measuring the development of young children: A comparative review
of screening and assessment techniques. New York: UN/C: Consultative Group on Early
Childhood Care and Development.
Landers, C. , & Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (1990). Measuring the psychosocial development of
young children. Florence: Innocenti/UNICEF.
Laosa, L. M. (1978). Maternal teaching strategies in Chicano families of varied
educational and socioeconomic levels. Child development, 49, 1129–1135.
Laosa, L. M. (1980). Maternal teaching strategies in Chicano and Anglo-American
families: The influence of culture and education on maternal behavior. Child
Development, 51, 759–765.
Laosa, L. M. (1982). Families as facilitators of children's intellectual development at 3
years of age. In L. M. Laosa , & I. E. Sigei (Eds.), Families as learning environments for
children (pp. 1–45). New York: Plenum.
Laosa, L. M. (1984). Ethnic, socioeconomic, and home language influences upon early
performance on measures of abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76,
1178–1198.
Laosa, L. M. , & Sigel I. E. (1982). Families as learning environments for childeren. New
York: Plenum Press.
Larsen, J. M. , & Leigh, G. K. (1977). Early childhood self-concept index: Test manual
and technical report. Provo, UT: Department of Child Development and Family
Relations, Brigham Young University.
Lasch, C. (1978). The culture of narcissism. New York: Norton.
Lasch, C. (1984). The minimal self: Psychic survival in troubled times. New York: Norton.
Laslett, P. (1971). The world we have lost. New York: Scribner's.
Laslett, P. (1977). Characteristics of the Western family considered over time. In P.
Laslett (Ed.), Family and illicit love in earlier generations. London: Cambridge University
Press.
Lau, S. , & Cheung, P. C. (1987). Relations between Chinese adolescents' perception of
parental control and organization and their perception of parental warmth.
Developmental Psychology, 23, 726–729.
Lau, S. , Lew, W.J.F. , Hau, K. T. , Cheung, P. C. , & Berndt, T. J. (1990). Relations
among perceived parental control, warmth, indulgence, and family harmony of Chinese
in Mainland China. Developmental Psychology, 26, 674–677.
Lave, J. (1977). Tailor-made experiments and evaluating the intellectual consequences
of apprenticeship training. Quarterly Newsletter of the Institute for Comparative Human
Development, 1, 1–3.
Lazar, I. , Darlington, R. B. , Murray, H. W. , & Snipper, A. S. (1982). Lasting effects of
early education: A report from the consortium for longitudinal studies. Monographs of
Society for Research in Child Development, (33, Serial No. 120).
Leahy, R. L. (1990). The development of concepts of economic and social inequality. In
V. C. McLoyd & C. A. Flanagan (Eds.), Economic stress: Effects on family life and child
development (pp. 107–120). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Lebra, T. S. (1976). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu: University of Hawaii
Press.
Lenero-Otero, L. (Eds.). (1977). Beyond the nuclear family model: Cross-cultural
perspectives, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lerner, J.V.A. (1983). “Goodness of fit” model of the role of temperament in psyhosocial
adaptation in early adolescents. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 143, 149–157.
Lerner, R. M. (1982). Children and adolescents as producers of their own development.
Developmental Review, 2, 342–370.
Lerner, R. M. (1989). Developmental contextualism and the lifespan view of person-
context interaction. In M. H. Bornstein and J. S. Bruner (Eds.), Interaction in human
development (pp. 217–243). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Lerner, R. M. , & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (1981). Individuals as producers of their
development: A life span perspective. New York: Academic Press.
Lerner, R. M. , Hultsch, D. F. , & Dixon, R. A. (1983). Contextualism and the character of
developmental psychology in the 1970s. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
412, 101–128.
Leseman, P. (1993). How parents provide young children with access to literacy. In L.
Eldering & P. Leseman (Eds.), Early intervention and culture (pp. 149–172). The Hague:
UNESCO.
Lesthaeghe, R. (1980). On the social control of human reproduction. Population and
Development Review, 6, 527–548.
Lesthaeghe, R. (1983). A century of demographic and cultural change in Western
Europe: An exploration of underlying dimensions. Population and Development Review,
9(3), 411–437.
Lesthaeghe, R. , & Surkyn, J. (1988). Cultural dynamics and economic theories of fertility
change. Population and Development Review, 14(1), 1–47.
Leung, K. (1987). Some determinants of reactions to procedural models for conflict
resolution. A cross national study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
898–908.
Levenstein, P. , O'Hara, J. , & Madden, J. (1983). The mother-child home program of the
verbal interaction project. In Consortium for Longitudinal Studies (Ed.), As the twig is
bent (pp. 237–264). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Le Vine, R. A. (1974). Parental goals: A cross-cultural view. Teachers' College Record,
76, 226–239.
Le Vine, R. A. (1983). Fertility and child development: An anthropological approach. In
D. A. Wagner (Ed.), Child development and international development: Research-policy
interfaces (pp. 45–56). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Le Vine, R. A. (1988). Human parental care: Universal goals, cultural strategies,
individual behavior. New Directions in Child Development, 40, 37–50.
Le Vine, R. A. (1989). Cultural environments in child development. In N. Damon (Ed.),
Child development today and tomorrow. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Le Vine, R. A. , & Le Vine, B. (1966). Nyansongo: A Gusii community in Kenya. New
York: Wiley.
Le Vine, R. A. , & White, M. I. (1986). Human conditions: The cultural basis of
educational development. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Le Vine, R. A. , & White, M. I. (1991). Revolution in parenthood. In M. Woodhead , P.
Light , & R. Carr (Eds.), Growing up in a changing society (pp. 5–25). London: Routledge
& Kegan Paul.
Levinger, B. (1992). Promoting child quality: Issues, trends and strategies. Washington,
DC: Academy for Educational Development.
Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1910). Les fonctions mentales dans les sociétés inférieures [How natives
think]. Paris: Alean.
Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1922). Mentalité primitive. [Primitive mentality]. Paris: Alean.
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.
Lewin, L. (1958). Group decision and social change. In E. E. Maccoby , T. M. Newcomb ,
& E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (pp. 197–211). New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Lightfoot, C. , & Valsiner, J. (1992). Parental belief systems under the influence: Social
guidance of the construction of personal cultures. In I. E. Sigel , A. McGillicuddy , & J.
Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems (pp. 393–414). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Lin, C-Y. C. , & Fu, V. R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing practices among
Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Development, 61,
429–433.
Lindzey, G. (1961). Projective techniques and cross-cultural research. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Lombard, A. (1981). Success begins at home. Lexington, MA: Heath.
Lonner, W. J. (1980). The search for psychological uni versais. In H. C. Triandis & W. W.
Lambert (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 143–204). Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Lonner, W. J. (1989). The introductory psychology text and cross-cultural psychology.
Beyond Ekman, Whorf, and Biased I. Q. tests. In D. M. Keats , D. Munro , & L. Mann
(Eds.), Heterogeneity in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 4–22). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Lonner, W. J. , & Malpass, R. S. (Eds.). (1994). Psychology and culture. Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.
Lykes, M. B. (1985). Gender and individualistic vs. collectivistic bases for nations about
the self. Journal of Personality, 53, 356–383.
Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical
overview. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1006–1017.
MacDonald, K. (1986). Developmental models and early experience. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 175–190.
MacFarlane, A. (1978). The origins of English individualism. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Macro (1993). Ailede cocuk egitimi arastirmasi [Child training in the family]. Istanbul:
Mahler, M. (1972). On the first three phases of the separation-individuation process.
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 53, 333–338.
Mahler, M. , Pine, F. , & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth of the human
infant. New York: Basic Books.
Marin, G. (1985). The preference for equity when judging the attractiveness and fairness
of an allocator: The role of familiarity and culture. Journal of Social Psychology, 125,
543–549.
Markus, H. R. , & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition,
emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.
Markus, H. R. , & Kitayama, S. (1992). The what, why and how of cultural psychology: A
Review on Shweder's “Thinking through cultures.” Psychological Inquiry, 3(4), 357–364.
Marsella, A. J. , De Vos, G. , & Hsu, F.L.K. (Eds.). (1985). Culture and self: Asian and
Western perspectives. New York: Tavistock.
Marsella, A. J. , & White, G. M. (Eds.). (1984). Cultural conceptions of mental health and
therapy. Boston: Reider.
Masters, J. C. (1984). Psychology, research and social policy. American Psychologist,
31, 851–862.
Matsumoto, D. (1994). People: Psychology from a cultural perspective. Pacific Grove,
CA: Brooks/Cole.
Mazrui, A. (1968). From social Darwinism to current theories of modernization. World
Politics, 21, 69–83.
McClelland, D. C. (1971). Motivational trend in society. New York: General Learning
Press.
McClelland, D. C. , Atkinson, J. W. , Clark, R. A. , & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The
achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
McClelland, D. C. , & Winter, D. G. (1969). Motivating economic achievement. New York:
Free Press.
McDonald, K. (1986). Developmental models and early experience. International Journal
of Behavioral Development, 9, 175–190.
McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V. (1982). The relationship between parents' beliefs about
development and family constellation, socio-economic status and parents' teaching
strategies. In L. M. Laosa & I. E. Sigei (Eds.), Families as learning environments for
children (pp. 261–299). New York: Plenum.
McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V. , Sigel, I. E. , & Johnson, J. E. (1982). The family as a system
of mutual influences: Parental beliefs, distancing behaviors, and children's
representational thinking. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The child and its family
(pp. 91–106). New York: Plenum.
McGoldrick, M. , & Carter, E. A. (1982). The family life cycle. In F. Walsh (Ed.), Normal
family processes. New York: Guilford.
McKey, R. H. , Condelli, L. , Ganson, H. , Barret, B. J. , McConkey, C. , & Plantz, M. C.
(1985). The impact of Head Start on children, families and communities. Washington,
DC: Final Report of the Head Start Evaluation, Synthesis and Utilization Project.
McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children:
Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development,
61, 311–346.
McLoyd, V. C. , & Wilson, L. (1990). Maternal behavior, social support, and economic
conditions as predictors of distress in children. In V. C. McLoyd & C. Flanagan (Eds.),
Economic stress: Effects on family life and child development (pp. 49–70). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
McNally, S. , Eisenberg, N. , & Harris, J. D. (1991). Consistency and change in maternal
child-rearing practices and values: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 62,
190–198.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: Morrow.
Meisels, S. J. , & Shonhoff, J. P. (Eds.). (1990). Handbook of early childhood
intervention. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961–978.
Miller, J. G. , Bersoff, D. M. , & Harwood, R. L. (1990). Perceptions of social
responsibilities in India and the United States: Moral imperatives or personal decisions?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 33–47.
Miller, S. A. (1988). Parents' beliefs about children's cognitive development. Child
Development, 59, 259–285.
Min, Kan Feng. (1994). Empowering Women Through Formal Education. The Hague:
CESO
Minturn, L. , & Hitchcock, J. T. (1966). The Rajputs of Khalapur, India (Six Cultures
Series, Vol. 3). New York: Wiley.
Minturn, L. , & Lambert, W. W. (1964). Mothers of six cultures. New York: Wiley.
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Misra, G. & Gergen, K. J. (1993). On the place of culture in psychological science.
International Journal of Psychology, 28, 225–243.
Misra, G. , & Agarwal, R. (1985). The meaning of achievement: Implications for a cross-
cultural theory of achievement motivation. In I. R. Lagunes & Ype H. Poortinga (Eds.),
From a different perspective: Studies of behavior across cultures (pp. 250–256). Lisse:
Swets & Zeitlinger.
Mogey, J. (1991). Families: Intergenerational and generational connections-conceptual
approaches to kinship and culture. Marriage and Family Review, 16, 47–66.
Moghaddam, F. M. (1990). Modulative and generative orientations in psychology:
Implications for psychology in the three worlds. Journal of Social Issues, 1, 21–41.
Moghaddam, F. M. , & Taylor, D. M. (1986). What constitutes an “appropriate
psychology” for the developing world? International Journal of Psychology, 21, 253–267.
Moghaddam, F. M. , Taylor, D. M. , & Wright, S. C. (1993). Social psychology in cross-
cultural perspective. New York: Freeman.
Moock, P. , & Leslie, J. (1986). Childhood malnutrition and schooling in the Terai region
of Nepal. Journal of Development Economies, 33–52.
Moos, R. H. , & Moos, B. S. (1981). Family environment scale manual. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists' Press.
Morsbach, H. (1980). Major psychological factors influencing Japanese interpersonal
relations. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 317–344).
London: Academic Press.
Mosley, W. H. , & Chen, L. C. (1984). An analytic framework for the study of child
survival in developing countries. In W. H. Mosley & L. C. Chen (Eds.), Child Survival
strategies for research. Population and Development Review, 10, (suppl.), 25–45.
Mulhall, S. , & Swift, A. (1982). Liberals & Communaitarians. UK: Blackwell Publishers.
Mundy-Castle, A. (1974). Social and technological intelligence in Western and non-
Western cultures. In S. Pilowsky (Ed.), Cultures in collision. Adelaide: Australian National
Association of Mental Health.
Munroe, R. L. , & Munroe, R. H. (1975). Cross-cultural human development. Monterey,
CA: Brooks/Cole.
Munroe, R. L. , Munroe, R. H. , & Shimmin, H. (1984). Children's work in four cultures:
Determinants and Consequences American Anthropologist, 86, 342–348.
Munroe, R. L. , Munroe, R. H. , & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.). (1981). Handbook of cross-
cultural human development. New York: Garland.
Myers, R. (1992). The 12 who survive. London: Routledge.
Naidu, R. K. (1983). A developing program of stress research. Paper presented at the
seminar on Stress, Anxiety and Mental Health. Allahabad, Dec. 1983.
Neki, J. S. (1976). An examination of the cultural relativism of dependence as a dynamic
of social and therapeutic relationships. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 49, 1–10.
Nikapota, A. D. (1990). Case study for Sri Lanka—child development in primary care.
Paper presented at the Innocenti Technical Workshop on Psychosocial Development,
Florence, Italy.
Novikoff, A. B. (1945). The concept of integrative levels of biology. Science, 62,
209–215.
Nsamenang, A. B. (1992). Human development in cultural context: A third world
perspective. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Nsamenang, A. B. (1993). Psychology in Sub-Saharan Africa. Psychology and
Developing Societies, 5, 171–184.
Nunes, T. (1992). The environment of the child. The Bernard van Leer Foundation:
Occasional Paper No. 5, The Hague.
Nunes, T. (1993). Psychology in Latin America: The case of Brazil. Psychology and
Developing societies, 5, 123–134.
Nunes, T. , Schliemann, A. D. , & Carraher, D. W. (1993). Street mathematics and
school mathematics. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ogbu, J. (1988). Cultural diversity and human development. New Directions for child
Development, 42, 11–28.
Ogbu, J. U. (1990). Cultural model, identity, and literacy. In J. W. Stigler , R. A. Shweder
, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development
(pp. 520–541). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Ohuche, R. O. , & Otaala, B. (Eds.). (1981). The African child and his environment.
Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.
Okagaki, L. , & Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Parental beliefs and children' s school
performance. Child Development, 64, 36–56.
Okman, F. (1982). The determinants of cognitive style: An investigation on adolescents
(in Turkish). Unpublished thesis, Bogaziçi University, Istanbul, Turkey.
Olmsted, P. P. , & Weikart, D. P. (1989). How nations serve young children: Profiles of
child care and education in 14 countries. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Olmsted, P. P. , & Weikart, D. P. (Eds.). (1994). Families Speak: Early childhood care
and education in 11 countries. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Oloko, B. A. (1994). Children' s street work in urban Nigeria: Dilemma of modernizing
tradition. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child
development (pp. 197–224). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Olson, E. (1982). Duofocal family structure and an alternative model of husband-wife
relationship. In Ç. Kağitçibaşi (Ed.), Sex roles, family and community in Turkey (pp.
33–72). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Oppong, C. (Ed.). (1980). Child development in African contexts: A collection of
readings. London: Allen & Un win.
Osterweil, Z. , & Nagano, K. N. (1991). Maternal views on autonomy: Japan and Israel.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22, 363–375.
Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Pandey, J. (Ed.). (1988). Psychology in India: The state-of-the-art (Vols. 1–3). New
Delhi: Sage.
Panel (1973a). The experience of separation-individuation in infancy and its
reverbarations through the course of life: 1. Infancy and childhood. Journal of the
American Psychoanalytic Association, 21, 135–154.
Panel (1973b). The experience of separation-individuation in infancy and its
reverbarations through the course of life: 2. Adolescence and maturity. Journal of the
American Psychoanalitic Association, 21, 155–167.
Papousek, H. , & Papousek, M. (1991). Innate and cultural guidance of infants'
integrative competencies: China, The United States, and Germany. In M. H. Bornstein
(Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting (pp. 23–44). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Patterson, G. R. , & Dishion, T. J. (1988). Multilevel family process models: traits,
interactions, and relationships. In R. A. Hinde & J. Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within
families (pp. 283–310). Oxford, England: Clarendon.
Peisner, E. S. (1989). To spare or not to spare the rod. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Child
development in cultural context (pp. 111–141). Toronto: Hogrefe.
Pepitone, A. (1987). The role of culture in theories of social psychology. In Ç. Kağitçibaşi
(Ed.), Growth and progress in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 12–22). Lisse: S wets &
Zeitlinger.
Peristiany, J. C. (1976). Mediterranean family structures. London: Cambridge University
Press.
Phalet, K. , & Claeys, W. (1993). A comparative study of Turkish and Belgian youth.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 24, 319–343.
Phillips, D. A. (1991). With a little help: Children in poverty and child care. In A. C.
Huston (Ed.), Children in poverty: Child development and public policy (pp. 158–189).
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Plomin, R. (1989). Environment and genes: Determinants of behavior. American
Psychologist, 44, 105–111.
Plomin, R. , & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.). (1993). Nature, nurture and psychology.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Publications.
Podmore, V. N. , & St. George R. (1986). New Zealand Maori and European mothers
and their 3-year-old children: Interactive behaviors in pre-school settings. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 7, 373–382.
Pollitt, R. , Gorman, K. S. , Engle, P. L. , Martorell, R. , & Rivera, J. (1993). Early
supplementary feeding and cognition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 58, 1.
Pollitt, E. , & Metallinos-Katsaras, E. (1990). Iron deficiency and behavior: Constructs,
methods and validity of the findings. In Wurtman and Wurtman (Eds.), Nutrition and the
brain: Vol. 8. Behavioral effects of metals, and their biochemical mechanisms (pp.
101–146). New York: Raven.
Pomerlau, A. , Malcuit, G. , & Sabatier, C. (1991). Child-rearing practices and parental
beliefs in three cultural groups of Montreal: Québécois, Vietnamese, Haitian. In M.
Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting (pp. 56–68). London: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Poortinga, Y. H. (1992). Towards a conceptualization of culture for psychology. In S.
Iwawaki , Y. Kashima , & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology (pp.
3–17). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Poortinga, Y. H. , Van de Vijver, F.J.R. , Joe, R. C. , & Van de Koppel, J.M.H. (1987).
Peeling the onion called culture: A sypnosis. In Ç. Kagitçiba§i (Ed.), Growth and
progress in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 22–34). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Price-Williams, D. (1980). Toward the idea of a cultural psychology: A superordinate
theme for study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 11, 75–88.
Provence, S. , & Naylor, A. (1983). Working with disadvantaged parents and their
children: Scientific and practice issues. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Ramey, C. T. , & Campbell, F. A. (1991). Poverty, early childhood education, and
academic competence: The abecedarian experiment. In A. C. Huston (Ed.), Children in
poverty: Child development and public policy (pp. 190–221). London: Cambridge
University Press.
Ramey, C. T. , Yeates, K. O. , & Short, E. T. (1984). The plasticity of intellectual
development: Insights from preventive intervention. Child Development, 55, 1913–1925.
Rank, O. (1929). The trauma of birth. New York: Knopf.
Rank, O. (1945). Will therapy and Truth and reality. New York: Knopf.
Razí, Z. (1993). The myth of the immutable English family. Past and Present: A Journal
of Historical Studies, 140, 3–44.
Rogoff, B. (1981). Schooling and the development of cognitive skills. In H. C. Triandis &
A. Heron (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology'. Vol. 4. Developmental
psychology (pp. 233–294). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.
Rogoff, B. , Gauvain, M. , & Ellis, S. (1984). Development viewed in its cultural context.
In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced
textbook (pp. 533–571). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rogoff, B. , & Lave, L. (1984). Everyday cognition: Its development in social context.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rogoff, B. , Mistry, J. , Göncü, A. , & Mosier, C. (1991). Cultural variation in the role
relations of toddlers and their families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to
parenting (pp. 173–183). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rogoff, B. , & Morelli, G. (1989). Perspectives on children' s development from cultural
psychology. American Psychologist, 44 (2), 343–348.
Rohner, R. (1980). Handbook for the study of parental acceptance and rejection (3rd
ed.) Storrs, CT: University of Connecticut.
Rohner, R. (1984). Toward a conception of culture for cross-cultural psychology. Journal
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 111–138.
Rohner, R. P. , & Pettengill, S. M. (1985). Perceived parental acceptance-rejection and
parental control among Korean adolescents. Child Development, 56, 524–528.
Rohner, R. P. , & Rohner, E. C. (1978). Unpublished research data. Center for the Study
of Parental Acceptance and Rejection, University of Connecticut.
Roland, A. (1988). In search of self in India and Japan. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Roopnarine, J. L. , & Talukder, E. (1990). Characteristics of holding, patterns of play,
and social behaviors between parents and infants in New Delhi, India. Developmental
Psychology, 26, 667–673.
Rosen, B. C. (1962). Socialization and achievement motivation in Brazil. American
Sociological Review, 27, 612–624.
Rotenberg, M. (1977). Alienating-individualism and reciprocal-individualism: A cross-
cultural conceptualisation. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 3, 3–17.
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of
reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, 1–28.
Royce, J. M. , Darlington, R. B. , & Murray, H. W. (1983). Pooled analysis: Ginding
across studies. In Consortium for Longitudional Studies, As the twig is bent
(pp.411–460). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Saal, C. D. (1987). Alternative forms of living and housing. In L. Shamgar-Handelman &
R. Palomba (Eds.), Alternative patterns of family life in modern societies. Rome: Collana
Monografie.
Saavedra, J. M. (1980). Effects of perceived parental warmth and control on the self-
evaluation of Puerto Rican adolescent males. Behavior Science Research, 15, 41–45.
Sabatier, C. (1986). La mère et son bébé: Variations culturelles: Analyse critique de la
littérature [Mother and her baby: Cultural variations: Critical analysis of literature].
Journal of International Psychology, 21, 513–553.
Sameroff, A. J. , & Fiese, B. H. (1992). Family representations of development. In I. E.
Sigei , A. V. McGillicuddy-DeLisi , & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems (pp.
347–369). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sameroff, A. J. , Seifer, R. , Barocas, B. , Zax, M. , & Greenspan, S. (1987). IQ scores of
4-year-old children: Social environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 79, 343–350.
Sampson, E. E. (1977). Psychology and the American ideal. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 35, 767–782.
Sampson, E. E. (1985). The decentralization of identity: Towards a revised concept of
personal and social order. American Psychologist, 40, 1203–1211.
Sampson, E. E. (1987). Individuation and domination: Undermining the social bond. In Ç.
Ka-ğιtçibaşι (Ed.), Growth and progress in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 84–93). Lisse:
Swets & Zeitlinger.
Sampson, E. E. (1988). The debate on individualism: Indigenous psychologies of the
individual and their role in personal and societal functioning. American Psychologist, 43,
15–22.
Sampson, E. E. (1989). The challenge of social change for psychology: Globalization
and psychology’s theory of the person. American Psychologist, 44, 914–921.
Santerre, R. (1973). Pedagogie Musulmane d’Afrique noire [Muslim schooling in black
Africa]. Montréal: Les Presses de l'Université de Montréal.
Sarason, S. B. (1981). Psychology misdirected. New York: Free Press.
Sarason, S. B. (1988). The making of an American psychologist: An autobiography. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Saraswathi, T. S. , & Dutta, R. (1987). Developmental psychology in India, 1975–1986:
An annotated bibliography. New Delhi: Sage.
Saraswathi, T. S. , & Kaur, B. (1993). Human development and family studies in India.
New Delhi: Sage.
Savaşir, I. , & Şahin, N. (1988). Weschler çocuk zekâ ölçeği (WISC-R) [Weschler
Intelligence Scale for Children]. Ankara: Milli Egitim Basimevi.
Savasir, I. , Sezgin, N. , & Erol, N. (1992). 0–6 Yaş Cocuklari icin gelişim tarama
envanteri gelistirilmesi. [Devising a developmental screening inventory for 0–6 year old
children]. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 3, 33–38.
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s. Child development, 63, 1–19.
Schorr, L. B. (1991). Effective programs for children growing up in concentrated poverty.
In A. C. Huston (Ed.), Children in poverty: Child development and public policy (pp.
260–281). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Schwartz, B. (1986). The battle for human nature. New York: Norton.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. , & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a psychological structure of human values.
Journal of Personality and social Psychology, 53, 550–562.
Schwartz, S. H. , & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and
structure of values: Extensions and cross-cultural replications. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 58, 878–891.
Schwartz, T. (1981). The acquisition of culture. Ethos, 9, 4–17.
Schweinhart, L. J. , Barnes, H. V. , Weikart, D. P. , Barnett, W. S. , & Epstein, A. S.
(1994). Significant Benefits: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27.
Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Schweinhart, L. J. , & Weikart, D. P. (1980). Young children grow-up (Monograph no. 7).
Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Scribner, S. , & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Segali, M. H. (1983). On the search for the independent variable in cross-cultural
psychology. In S. H. Irvine & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Human assessment and cultural factors
(pp. 127–138). New York: Plenum.
Segali, M. H. (1984). More than we need to know about culture, but are afraid not to ask.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 153–162.
Segali, M. H. (1986). Culture and behavior: Psychology in global perspective. Annual
Review of Psychology, 37, 523–564.
Segali, M. H. , Dasen, P. R. , Berry, J. W. , & Poortinga, Y. H. (1990). Human behavior in
global perspective. New York: Pergamon.
Seitz, V. , & Provence, S. (1990). Caregiver-focused models of early intervention. In S. J.
Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (pp. 400–427).
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Seitz, V. , Rosenbaum, L. K. , & Apfel, N. H. (1985). Effects of family support
intervention: A 10-year follow-up. Child Development, 56, 376–391.
Selman, R. L. (1989). Fostering intimacy and autonomy. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child
development today and tomorrow (pp. 409–436). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Semin, R. (1975). Failure in school
Serpell, R. (1977). Strategies for investigating intelligence in its cultural context.
Quarterly Newsletter, Institute for Comparative Human Development, 3, 11–15.
Serpell, R. (1979). How specific are perceptual skills? A cross-cultural study of pattern
reproduction. British Journal of Psychology. 70, 365–380.
Serpell, R. (1993). Significance of schooling. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
Seshadri, S. , & Gopaldas, T. (1989). Impact of iron supplementation on cognitive
functions in preschool and school-aged children: The Indian experience. American
Journal of Clinical Nutrition, Supplement.
Shand, N. (1985). Culture’s influence in Japanese and American maternal role
perception and confidence. Psychiatry, 48, 52–61.
Shwalb, D. W. , Shwalb, B. J. , & Murata, K. (1991). Individualistic striving and group
dynamics of fifth and eighth grade Japanese boys. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
22, 347–361.
Shweder, R. A. (1984). Anthropology’s romantic rebellion against the enlightenment, or
there is more to the thinking than reason and evidence. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. Le
Vine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion, (pp. 1–27). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology— What is it? In J. W. Stigler , R. A. Shweder
& G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp.
1–43). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Shweder, R. A. , & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person vary cross-
culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. Le Vine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self
and emotion (pp. 158–199). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R. A. , & LeVine, R. (1984). Culture theory. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Shweder, R. A. , Mahapatra, M. , & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development.
In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 1–90).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Shweder, R. A. , & Sullivan, M. A. (1993). Cultural Psychology: Who needs it? Annual
Review of Psychology, 44, 497–523.
Sigel, I. E. (1985). A conceptual analysis of beliefs. In I. E. Sigei (Ed.), Parental belief
systems (pp. 345–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sigel, I. E. (1992). The belief-behavior connection: A resolvable dilemma? In I. E. Sigel ,
A. McGillicuddy-Delisi , & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems (pp. 433–456).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sigel, I. E. , McGillicuddy-Delisi, A. , & Goodnow, J. (1992). Parental belief systems.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sigman, M. , & Wachs, T. D. (1991). Structure, continuity, and nutritional correlates of
caregiver behavior patterns in Kenya and Egypt. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural
Approaches to Parenting (pp. 123–136). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Silverman, L. H. , & Weinberger, J. (1985). Mommy and I are one: Implications for
psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 40 (12), 1296–1308.
Simons, R. L. , Whitbeck, L. B. , Conger, R. D. , & Chyi, I. W. (1991). Intergenerational
transmission of harsh parenting. Developmental Psychology, 27, 159–171.
Sinha, D. (Ed.). (1981). Socialization of the Indian child. New Delhi: Naurang Rai.
Sinha, D. (1983). Cross-cultural psychology: A view from the Third World. In J. B.
Deregowski , S. Dziuraviec , & R. C. Annis (Eds.), Expiscations in cross-cultural
psychology (pp. 3–17). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Sinha, D. (1986). Psychology in a third world country: The Indian experience. New Delhi:
Sage.
Sinha, D. (1988). The family scenario in a developing country and its implications for
mental health: The case of India. In P. R. Dasen , J. W. Berry , & N. Sartorious (Eds.),
Health and cross-cultural psychology: Toward applications (pp. 48–70). Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Sinha, D. (1989). Cross-cultural psychology and the process of indigenisation: A second
view from the Third World. In D. M. Keats , D. M. Munro , & L. Mann (Eds.),
Heterogeneity in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 24–40). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Sinha, D. (1992). Appropriate indigenous psychology in India: A search for new identity.
In S. Iwawaki , Y. Kashima , & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology
(pp. 38–48). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Sinha, D. , & Kao, H.S.R. (1988). Social values and development: Asian perspectives.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Sinha, D. , & Tripathi, R. C. (1994). Individualism in a collectivist culture: A case of
coexistence of opposites. In U. Kim , H. C. Triandis , Ç. Kagitçibaşi , S-C. Choi , & G.
Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, Method and Applications (pp.
123–136). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Sinha, J.B.P. (1985). Collectivism, social energy, and development in India. In I. R.
Lagunes & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), From a different perspective: Studies of behavior
across cultures (pp. 120–135). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Sinha, J.B.P. (1988). Reorganizing values for development. In D. Sinha & S. R. Kao
(Eds.), Social values and development: Asian perspectives. New Delhi: Sage.
Sinha, J.B.P. (1993). The bulk and the front of psychology in India. Psychology and
Developing Societies, 5, 135–150.
Sinha, S. R. (1985). Maternal strategies for regulating children’s behavior. Journal of
Cross Cultural Psychology, 16, 27–40.
Slaughter, D. T. (1983). Early intervention and its effects upon maternal and child
development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48.
Slaughter, D. T. (1988). Black children, schooling, and educational interventions. In D. T.
Slaughter (Ed.), Black children and poverty: A developmental perspective (pp. 109–116).
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Slobin, D. I. (1972). Children and language: They learn the same way all around the
world. Psychology Today, 6, 71–74, 82.
Smilansky, M. (1979). Priorities in education: Preschool evidence and conclusions
(World Bank Staff Working Paper, No. 323). Washington, DC: World Bank.
Smith, M. B. (1968). Competence and socialization. In J. A. Clausen (Ed.), Socialization
and Society (pp. 270–320). Boston: Little, Brown.
Smith, M. B. (1978). Perspectives on selfhood. American Psychologist, 33, 1053–1063.
Smith, M. B. (1990). Psychology in the public interest: What have we done? What can
we do? American Psychologist, 45, 530–536.
Smith, M. B. (1991). Values, self, and society. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Publishers.
Smith, M. B. (1994). Selfhood at risk: Post-modern perils and the perils of post-
modernism. American Psychologist, 49, 405–411.
Smith, P. B. , & Bond, M. H. (1993). Social psychology across cultures. Hartfordshire,
England: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Smith, R. M. (1979). Kin and neighbours in a 13th-century Suffolk community. Journal of
Family History, 4, 79089.
Snow, Ce . (1991). The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy
in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 5–10.
Snow, C. E. (1993). Linguistic development as related to literacy. In L. Eldering & P.
Leseman (Eds.), Early intervention and culture (pp. 133–148). Netherlands: UNESCO.
Sockalingam, S. , Zeitlin, M. , & Satoto, C. N. (1990). Study to encourage positive
indigenous care taking behaviour in improving child nutrition and health. Paper available
from the Consultative Group on Early Child Care and Development, New York.
Spence, J. T. (1985). Achievement American style. American Psychologist, 12,
1285–1295.
Staub, S. , & Green, P. (Eds.). (1992). Psychology and social responsibility: Facing
global challenges. New York: New York University Press.
Steinberg, L. , Elmen, J. D. , & Mounts, N. S. (1989). Authoritative parenting,
psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development,
60, 1424–1436.
Stevens, J. H. (1988). Social support, locus of control and parenting in three low-income
groups of mothers: Black teenagers, black adults, and white adults. Child Development,
59, 635–642.
Stevenson, H. , Azuma, H. , & Hakuta, K. (Eds.). (1986). Child development and
education in Japan. New York: Freeman.
Stigler, J. W. , Shweder, R. A. , & Herdt, G. (1990). Cultural psychology. Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Suina, J. H. , & Smolkin, L. B. (1994). From natal culture to school culture to dominant
society culture: Supporting transitions for Pueblo Indian students. In P. M. Greenfield &
R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 115–132).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sun, L. K. (1991). Contemporary Chinese culture: Structure and emotionality. Australian
Journal of Chinese Affairs, 26, 1–41.
Super, C. M. (1981). Behavioral development in infancy. In R. H. Munroe , R. L. Munroe
, & B. B. Whiting (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development. New York:
Garland.
Super, C. M. (1983). Cultural variation in the meaning and uses of children's
“intelligence”. In J. Deregowski , S. Dziuraviec , & R. Annis (Eds.), Expiscations in cross-
cultural psychology (pp. 199–212). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Super, C. M. , & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at
the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9,
545–570.
Suvannathat, C. , Bhanthumnavin, D. , Bhuapirom, L. , & Keats, D. M. (1985). Handbook
of Asian child development and child rearing practices. Bangkok: Behavioral Science
Research Institute.
Suzuki, T. (1984). Ways of life and social milieus in Japan and the United States: A
comparative study. Behaviormetrika, 15, 77–108.
Swaminathan, I. (1986). Coordinators Notebook, 2, 16. New York: The Consultative
Group on Early Childhood Care and Development.
Szapocznik, J. , & Kurtines, W. M. (1993). Family psychology and cultural diversity.
American Psychologist, 48, 400–407.
Tajfel, H. (1972). “Experiments in a vacuum.” In J. Israel & H. Tajfel (Eds.), The context
of social psychology: A critical assessment (pp. 66–119). London: Academic Press.
Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the self: The making of the modern identity. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Teasdale, G. R. , & Teasdale, J. I. (1992). Culture and curriculum: Dilemmas in the
schooling of Australian Aboriginal children. In S. Iwawaki , Y. Kashima , & K. Leung
(Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 442–457). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Thadani, V. N. (1978). The logic of sentiment: The family and social change. Population
and Development Review, 4(3), 457–499.
Thorton, A. (1984). Modernization and family change. In Social change and family
policies. Proceedings of the 20th International CFR Seminar. Melbourne: Australian
Institute of Family Studies.
Thorton, A. , & Fricke, T. E. (1987). Social change and the family: Comparative
perspectives from the West, China, and South Asia. Sociological Forum, 2, 746–779.
Tizard, B. (1991). Working mothers and the care of young children. In M. Woodhead , P.
Light , & R. Caar (Eds.), Growing up in a changing society (pp. 61–77). London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Toennies, F. (1957). Community and society. ( C.P. Loomis , translator) East Lansing:
Michigan State Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture. New York: Wiley.
Triandis, H. C. (1978). Some universals of social behavior. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 4, 1–16.
Triandis, H. C. (1988). Collectivism and individualism: A reconceptualization of a basic
concept in cross-cultural psychology. In G. K. Verma & C. Bagley (Eds.), Personality,
attitudes, and cognitions (pp. 60–95). London: Macmillan.
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts.
Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In J. J.
Berman (Ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives: Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp.
41–134). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Triandis, H. C. , Leung, K. , Villareal, M. V. , & Clack, F. L. (1985). Allocentric versus
idiocentric tendencies: Convergent and discriminant validation. Journal of Research in
Personality, 19, 395–415.
Triandis, H. C. , Malpass, R. , & Davidson, A. R. (1973). Psychology and culture. Annual
Review of Psychology, 24, 355–378.
Triandis, H. C. , McCusker, C. , & Hui, C. H. (1990). Multimethod probes of individualism
and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1006–1020.
Trickett, P. K. , & Susman, E. J. (1988). Parental perceptions of childrearing practices in
physically abusive and nonabusive families. Developmental Psychology, 24, 270–276.
Tripathi, R. C. (1988). Aligning development to values in India. In D. Sinha & H.S.R. Kao
(Eds.), Social values and development (pp. 315–333). Newburg Park, CA: Sage.
Trommsdorf, G. (1985). Some comparative aspects of socialization in Japan and
Germany. In I. R. Lagunes & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), From a different perspective:
Studies of behavior across cultures (pp. 231–240). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
UNESCO (1982). Different theories and practices of development. Paris: UNESCO.
UNESCO (1991). World education report. Paris: UNESCO.
UNICEF (1991). The state of the world's children. New York: UNICEF.
UNICEF/India (1988). Annual Report. New Delhi: UNICEF.
Valsiner, J. (1989). Child development in cultural context. Toronto: Hogrefe.
Van de Vijver, F.J.R. , & Hutschemaekers, G.J.M. (Eds.). (1990). The investigation of
culture. Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.
Van de Vijver, F.J.R. , & Poortinga, Y. H. (1990). A taxonomy of cultural differences. In J.
R. van de Vijver & G.J.M. Hutschemaekers (Eds.), The investigation of culture: Current
issues in cultural psychology (pp. 91–114). Tilburg: Tilburg.
Vannoy, D. (1991). Social differentiation, contemporary marriage, and human
development. Journal of Family Issues, 12, 251–267.
Van Oudenhoven, N. (1989). Children at risk and community response. Notes,
Comments (No. 187). UNESCO.
Verhoeven, L. , Rood, v. P. , & v. d. Laan, . (Eds.). (1991). Attaining functional literacy: A
cross-cultural perspective. The Hague: UNESCO.
Von Bertalanffy, L. (1933). Modern theories of development. London: Oxford University
Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wachs, T. D. (1987). Specificity of environmental action as manifested in environmental
correlates of infant's mastery motivation. Development Psychology, 23, 782–790.
Wachs, T. D. (1993). Determinants of intellectual development: Single determinant
research in a multideterminant universe. Intelligence, 17, 1–10.
Wachs, T. D. , & Gruen, G. (1982). Early experience and human development. New
York: Wiley.
Wagner, D. A. (Ed.). (1983). Child development and international development:
Research-policy interfaces. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Wagner, D. A. (Ed.). (1986). Child development research and the Third World. American
Psychologist, 41, 298–301.
Wagner, D. A. (1988). Appropriate education and literacy in the Third World. In P. R.
Dasen , J. W. Berry , & N. Sartorious (Eds.), Health and cross-cultural psychology (pp.
93–111). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Wagner, D. A. , & Spratt, J. E. (1987). Cognitive consequences of contrasting
pedagogies: The effects of Quranic preschooling in Morocco. Child Development, 58,
1207–1219.
Wagner, D. A. , & Stevenson, H. W. (Eds.). (1982). Cultural perspectives on child
development. San Francisco: Freeman.
Wallach, M. A. , & Wallach, L. (1983). Psychology's sanction for selfishness: The error of
egoism in theory and therapy. New York: Freeman.
Wallach, M. A. , & Wallach, L. (1990). Rethinking goodness. Albany, NY: State University
of New York Press.
Washington, V. (1988). Historical and contemporary linkages between black child
development and social policy. In D. T. Slaughter (Ed.), Black children and poverty: A
developmental perspective (pp. 934–938). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Wassik, B. H. , Ramey, C. T. , Bryant, D. M. , & Sparling, J. J. (1990). A longitudinal
study of two early intervention strategies: Project care. Child Development, 61,
1682–1696.
Weber, M. (1958). The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism ( Talcott Parsons ,
Trans.). New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.
Weil, S. (1987). Proximal households as alternatives to joint families in Israel. In L.
Shamgar-Handelman & R. Palomba (eds.), Alternative patterns of family life in modern
societies. Rome: Collana Monografie.
Weiss, H. B. (1988). Family support and education programs: Working through
ecological theories of human development. In H. B. Weiss & F. H. Jacobs (Eds.),
Evaluating family programs (pp. 3–36). New York: Aldine.
Weiss, H. B. , & Jacobs, F. H. (1988). Family support and education programs:
challenges and opportunities. In H. B. Weiss & F. H. Jacobs (Eds.), Evaluating family
programs (pp. xix–xxix). New York: Aldine.
Weisz, J. R. , Rothbaum, F. M. , & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing out and standing in.
American Psychologist, 39, 955–969.
Werner, E. E. (1979). Cross-cultural child development: A view from the planet Earth.
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Werner, E. E. , & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Westen, D. (1985). Self and society. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
White, G. , & Kirkpatrick, J. (Eds.). (1985). Person, self and experience: Exploring Pacific
ethnopsychologies. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Whiting, B. B. (Ed.). (1963). Six cultures: Studies in child rearing. New York: Wiley.
Whiting, B. B. (1976). The problem of the packaged variable. In K. Riegel & J. Meacham
(Eds.), The developing individual in a changing world (Vol. 1, pp. 303–309). The Hague:
Mouton.
Whiting, B. B. , & Whiting J. W. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psychocultural
analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Whiting J. W. , & Child, I. (1953). Child training and personality. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Whiting J. W. (1974). A model for psychocultural research. Annual Report. Washington,
DC: American Anthropological Association.
Witkin, H. , & Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural
perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 4–87.
Wober, M. (1974). Towards an understanding of the Kiganda concept of intelligence. In
J. W. Berry & P. Dasen (Eds.), Culture and cognition (pp. 119–128). London: Methuen.
Woodhead, M. (1985). Pre-school education has long term effects: But can they be
generalized? Oxford Review of Education, 77(2), 133–155.
Woodhead, M. (1988). When psychology informs public policy. American Psychologist,
43, 443–454.
Woodhead, M. (1991). Psychology and the cultural construction of children's needs. In
M. Woodhead , P., Light , & R. Carr (Eds.), Growing up in a changing society (pp.
37–57). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Woodhead, M. , Light, R. C. , & Carr, R. (Eds.). (1991). Growing up in a changing
society. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
World Bank (1988). Education in Sub-Saharan Africa, policies for adjustment,
revitalization, and expansion. Washington, DC: World Bank.
World Bank (1993). Best practices in addressing micronutrient malnutrition. Washington,
DC: World Bank Population, Health and Nutrition Department.
World Health Organization (1986). Protocols for the development and field testing of
techniques for monitoring physical growth and psychosocial development.
(WHO/MCH/MNH/86. 1). Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.
World Health Organization (1990). [Progress report on the activities of physical growth
and psychosocial development (September 1988–April 1990)]. Programme of Maternal
and Child Health, WHO, Geneva, Switzerland.
Yamaguchi, E. (1985). Culture and experience. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
Yang, C. F. (1988). Familism and development: An examination of the role of family in
contemporary China Mainland, Hong Kong, and Taiwan. In D. Sinha & H.S.R. Kao
(Eds.), Social values and development: Asian perspectives (pp. 93–123). London: Sage.
Yang, K-S. (1986). Chinese personality and its change. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The
psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 106–170). New York: Oxford University Press.
Yang, K-S. (1988). Will societal modernization eventually eliminate cross-cultural
psychological differences? In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The cross-cultural challenge to social
psychology (pp. 67–85). London: Sage.
Young, M. E. (1993). Integrated child development — Challenges and opportunities.
Report prepared for the World Bank.
Young, N. (1992). Postmodern self-psychology mirrored in science and the arts. In S.
Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and postmodernism (pp. 135–145). London: Sage.
Yu, A-B. , & Yang, K-S. (1994). The Nature of achievement motivation in collectivistic
societies. In U. Kim , H. C. Triandis , Ç. Kağıtçıbaşı , S-C. Choi , & G. Yoon (Eds.),
Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 239–250).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Zeitlin, M. (1991). Nutritional resilience in a hostile environment: Positive deviance in
child nutrition. Nutrition Review, 49(9), 259–268.
Zeitlin, M. , Ghassemi, H. , & Mansour, M. (1990). Positive deviance in child nutrition,
with emphasis on psychosocial and behavioral aspects and implications for
development. Tokyo: United Nations University.
Zelizer, V. A. (1981). Pricing the priceless child. New York: Basic Books.
Zigler, E. , & Berman, W. (1983). Discerning the future of early childhood intervention.
American Psychologist, 38, 894–906.
Zigler, E. , & Styfco, S. J. (1994). Head Start: Criticism in a constructive context.
American Psychologist, 49, 127–132.
Zigler, E. , & Weiss, H. (1985). Family support systems: an ecological approach to child
development. In R. Rapaport (Ed.), Children, youth, and families: The action-research
relationship (pp. 166–205). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Zimmerman, B. J. , & Rosenthal, T. L. (1974). Observational learning of rule-governed
behavior by children. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 29–42.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy