0% found this document useful (0 votes)
5 views19 pages

Amez-MultitaskingAffectStudents-2021

The study investigates the impact of multitasking on students' academic performance using a longitudinal approach over three years at two Belgian universities. Findings suggest that the positive and negative effects of multitasking on academic outcomes tend to balance each other out, indicating no significant overall impact. The research highlights the need for longitudinal data to better understand the relationship between multitasking behaviors and academic success.

Uploaded by

ouzennka
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
5 views19 pages

Amez-MultitaskingAffectStudents-2021

The study investigates the impact of multitasking on students' academic performance using a longitudinal approach over three years at two Belgian universities. Findings suggest that the positive and negative effects of multitasking on academic outcomes tend to balance each other out, indicating no significant overall impact. The research highlights the need for longitudinal data to better understand the relationship between multitasking behaviors and academic success.

Uploaded by

ouzennka
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 19

IZA - Institute of Labor Economics

Does Multitasking Affect Students’ Academic Performance? Evidence from a Longitudinal


Study
Author(s): Simon Amez, Stijn Baert, Emily Heydencamp and Joey Wuyts
IZA - Institute of Labor Economics (2021)
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/resrep63845
Accessed: 25-02-2025 07:16 UTC

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

IZA - Institute of Labor Economics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to this content.

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
DISCUSSION PAPER SERIES

IZA DP No. 14896

Does Multitasking Affect Students’


Academic Performance?
Evidence from a Longitudinal Study

Simon Amez
Stijn Baert
Emily Heydencamp
Joey Wuyts

NOVEMBER 2021

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
DISCUSSION PAPER SERIES

IZA DP No. 14896

Does Multitasking Affect Students’


Academic Performance?
Evidence from a Longitudinal Study

Simon Amez Emily Heydencamp


Ghent University Ghent University
Stijn Baert Joey Wuyts
Ghent University, University of Antwerp, Ghent University
Université catholique de Louvain and IZA

NOVEMBER 2021

Any opinions expressed in this paper are those of the author(s) and not those of IZA. Research published in this series may
include views on policy, but IZA takes no institutional policy positions. The IZA research network is committed to the IZA
Guiding Principles of Research Integrity.
The IZA Institute of Labor Economics is an independent economic research institute that conducts research in labor economics
and offers evidence-based policy advice on labor market issues. Supported by the Deutsche Post Foundation, IZA runs the
world’s largest network of economists, whose research aims to provide answers to the global labor market challenges of our
time. Our key objective is to build bridges between academic research, policymakers and society.
IZA Discussion Papers often represent preliminary work and are circulated to encourage discussion. Citation of such a paper
should account for its provisional character. A revised version may be available directly from the author.

IZA – Institute of Labor Economics


Schaumburg-Lippe-Straße 5–9 Phone: +49-228-3894-0
53113 Bonn, Germany Email: publications@iza.org www.iza.org

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IZA DP No. 14896 NOVEMBER 2021

ABSTRACT
Does Multitasking Affect Students’
Academic Performance?
Evidence from a Longitudinal Study
Multitasking – alternating between two different tasks at the same time – has become
a daily habit for many university students. However, this may come at a cost since the
existing literature emphasises the negative association between multitasking and academic
performance. Nonetheless, this literature is based on cross-sectional observational data so
that that estimates cannot be given a causal interpretation. To complement these studies,
we opted for a longitudinal design in this study. Specifically, for three consecutive years,
students at two Belgian universities, in more than ten different study programmes, were
surveyed on their multitasking preferences and academic performance. Then, these results
were merged with the students’ exam scores. We exploited the longitudinal character of
the data by running random and fixed effect models. Our results indicate that the positive
and negative aspects of multitasking with respect to academic performance cancel each
other out.

JEL Classification: I23, J24


Keywords: multitasking, academic performance, longitudinal data

Corresponding author:
Stijn Baert
Ghent University
Sint-Pietersplein 6
9000 Ghent
Belgium
E-mail: Stijn.Baert@UGent.be

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
1. Introduction
Multitasking is the ability to screw everything up simultaneously. Jeremy Clarkson

Want to learn faster? Stop multitasking and start daydreaming (Letivin, 2015). Multitasking is actually
kind of a problem (Tsukayama, 2016). Why multitasking doesn t work (Cleveland Clinic, 2021). The
newspapers are filled with concerns about multitasking. Multitasking can be described as the ability to
perform multiple task goals in the same general time period by engaging in frequent switches between
individual tasks (Delbridge, 2000, p. 1). In other words, it is about alternating between different tasks at
the same time (Ajao, 2012). Although multitasking is not a new phenomenon, it has received increased
attention in recent years, which is related to the development of new technologies, as well as new media
(Lee et al., 2011). Not only the number of technological devices but also their addictive traits and the way
people are entangled with them make this an important if challenging topic to study (Burak, 2012).

Existing multitasking research seems to suggest that multitasking comes at a high cost in that it increases
the number of errors people make and reduces productivity (Mokhtari et al., 2015). Theoretically, an initial
potential explanation of the negative relationship between multitasking and performance is that some
researchers believe that we can only perform multiple tasks at the same time when these tasks are
automated (Kirschner & Karpinski, 2010).1 Since academic tasks, for example, attending a lecture and
taking notes or studying, require focused attention, combining it with other tasks at the same time will lead
to a decrease in performance (Kirschner & Karpinski, 2010). A second reason for poorer performance can
be that multitasking diminishes focus (Hallowell, 2005). This second reason can be attributed to two
mechanisms. First, the time that students spend using media during academic activities may displace the
time spent on the academic activity itself (e.g. Fox et al., 2009). If students do not spend sufficient time on
academic assignments, they may not perform to the best of their abilities (van der Schuur et al., 2015). A
second explanation is in line with what Miller (1956) discovered long ago, that people are restricted in the
quantity of data they are not only able to receive but also process and remember. Since multitasking
involves a greater flow of information, it can lead to the phenomenon called information overload (Lee et
al., 2016; Vorderer et al., 2017). This phenomenon emphasises that people cannot successfully process
multiple information streams at the same time.

Also, empirically, multiple studies have found that multitasking, and more specifically smartphone use, is
effectively associated with poorer performance, especially in university students (Amez & Baert, 2020;
Amez et al., 2021; Baert et al, 2020; Burak, 2012). A more concrete example of this is given by Ellis et al.
(2010): they found that university students grade performances were lower when multitasking occurred
in the learning environment. This might be problematic since it has been discovered that university students
multitask every day while doing homework, in class or while studying (Carrier et al., 2015). Furthermore,

1
Automated tasks refer to tasks where thinking does not play a role, for example, chewing gum, talking and
walking at the same time (Kirschner & Karpinski, 2010).

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Wood et al. (2012) indicated that students who did not use any technologies to multitask during academic
tasks performed better than students who did use technologies to multitask, regardless of the medium that
was used. This kind of multitasking behaviour naturally leads to more distraction, which is something
previous research has found to harm to student performance (Pool et al., 2000). Thus, engaging in different
tasks at the same time tends to impact learning effectiveness (Mayer & Moreno, 2003).

Up until now, most studies exploring the relationship between multitasking and performance have relied
on cross-sectional data, meaning that empirical findings may be biased by an endogeneity problem. That
is, the observed relationship potentially contains (partially) unmeasured characteristics that interact with
multitasking as well as academic performance. In this regard, the literature reveals a clear need for
longitudinal studies (Doleck & Lajoie, 2018; Junco & Cotten, 2012; Uzun & Kilis, 2018). The use of
longitudinal data has some advantages. First, estimators based on longitudinal data allow correction for
time-constant unobserved individual heterogeneity. Second, these estimators are often more efficient
since the data enable the use of both between- and within-individual variations (Bell et al., 2019). For this
reason, this research attempted to understand the relationship between multitasking and university
students academic performance by using a longitudinal design wherein we expected multitasking to
negatively affect students academic outcomes concerning average exam scores.

2. Method
In the following section, we outline the method used to study the relationship between multitasking and
academic performance. First, we discuss our research sample. Then, we explain the different measures that
were used, and finally, we discuss the statistical framework used to analyse the data.

2.1. Research population


For three years in a row, data for several studies were gathered simultaneously by surveying students
attending classes at two major Belgian universities (Ghent University and the University of Antwerp).2 The
students were enrolled in 11 different study programs.3 In the first year of data collection, only first-year
students from both universities were surveyed. The following year, new fist-year students, as well as the
students who participated the previous year, were targeted in the survey. During the last year of data
collection, again, new first-year students were targeted, and we attempted to include every student who had
participated previously. However, this only applies to the University of Antwerp, since only there did we
survey for three years. At Ghent University, we only surveyed for the first two years. The procedure was the
same for both universities. First, during the last week before the Christmas break, a researcher gained access
to students via their curriculum, and entered the classroom during a lecture . The researcher then asked the

2
See Amez et al., 2021 and Baert et al., 2020.
3
At Ghent University, the study programmes included business and economics, commercial sciences, and public
administration and management. At the University of Antwerp, the study programmes included business
economics, economic policy, business engineering, management information systems, communication studies,
political science, social and economic sciences, and sociology.
3

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
students to complete a questionnaire. At the end of the questionnaire, an informed consent clause was
added, asking the students whether they allowed us to cross-reference their questionnaire responses with
the results of their upcoming exams . In general, the Christmas break is used by students to study for these
upcoming exams. Students who filled in the questionnaire and consented to their responses being cross-
referenced with their exam results did not have to provide their results themselves, as the results were
obtained through the faculty administration. To maintain the students anonymity, a third person cross-
referenced the obtained exam results with the data collected from the questionnaires. Originally, the data
collection in 2016, 2017 and 2018 resulted in 2,060 paper and pen questionnaires. However, of those 2060
questionnaires, 104 had to be excluded because the faculty administration was not able to provide the
relevant exam scores, meaning that those 104 students dropped out before they took their exams. Another
83 observations had to be eliminated either because the student did not complete the questionnaire in full
or their responses contained inconsistent information. The final sample comprises 1,873 observations,
among which there are 240 with multiple observations. Besides being longitudinal in nature albeit to a
limited extent this sample is substantially larger than samples used in earlier studies.

2.2. Measures
The questionnaire was divided into two parts, with one part focusing on multitasking and the second part
focusing on academic performance and general socioeconomic features.

The multitasking preference inventory (MPI) was used to measure participants preferences towards
multitasking (Poposki and Oswald, 2010). The questionnaire consisted of four statements regarding
multitasking, namely, when doing a number of assignments, I like to switch back and forth between them
rather than do one at a time , I like to finish one task completely before focusing on anything else , I
prefer to work on several projects in a day, rather than completing one project and then switching to another
and when I have a task to complete, I like to break it up by switching to other tasks intermittently . The
participants indicated the degree to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement on a 5-point scale
ranging from completely disagree to completely agree . A higher score on the MPI means that a student
has a higher preference for multitasking. The mean score on the MPI was 2.684, as can be seen in Panel A
of Table 1.

--- Insert Table 1 here ---

Additionally, students were surveyed with respect to variables that might be correlated with academic
performance. For these control variables, we made a distinction between three types of variables, based on
how they change over time: there are (1) time-invariant variables, (2) predetermined time-varying variables
and (3) time-varying variables. The first participants were asked about time-invariant socioeconomic
variables that could predict academic performance, as suggested by Baert et al. (2015). These variables
were (foreign) origin, gender, father s education level, language spoken at home, household composition and
educational performance before university. Panel B of Table 1 shows the averages for each of these
variables.

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Then data was collected on predetermined time-varying variables, which means variables that can change
over time but which are (generally) set at the beginning of the year. Concerning household composition,
two questions were asked: whether the participant s parents were divorced and whether one of the parents
had passed away. Students were also asked about their living arrangements in college (whether they live
in a student room or not). Additionally, using binary variables, the curriculum backgrounds were
established, indicating the academic programme at the time of the questionnaire. Furthermore,
students were asked how many ECTS credits they aimed to obtain in the upcoming semester and whether
they had had to retake one or more exams from the previous years. 4

Next, students were asked about time-varying variables. In this part of the questionnaire, the college version
of the academic motivation scale by Vallerand et al. (1992) was included. This scale is made up of 28
items, which the students scored on a 7-point scale. This results in an average academic motivation score
between 1 and 7. Lower scores indicate lower academic motivation. Additionally, students were asked how
they perceived their current health, with three possible answers: (a) fairly bad, (b) fairly good or (c) very
good. The last time-varying variable that was included asked the participants if they were currently involved
in a romantic relationship.

Finally, Panel E of Table 1 shows the participants average scores for the two outcome variables. These
variables were constructed based on the exam scores obtained from the faculty students administration. The
first variable (average score: completed exams) was the student s average grade (a score between 0 and
20) based on all the exams the student took in the corresponding semester. The second variable (fraction
of passed exams) was obtained by dividing the number of exams that the students passed (meaning they
obtained at least 50%) by the total number of exams that the students took. As expected, based on the
scientific literature mentioned in the introduction and the literature study, both educational performance
variables are worse in the subsample of participants with an overall above average preference for
multitasking. This correlational analysis, however, does not take into account potential confounding
variables, either observable listed in Panels B, C and D of Table 1 nor unobservable. The random
effects approach, discussed in the following section, does consider these potential confounding variables.

2.3. Used analyses


To analyse these longitudinal data, we could use either the random effects model or the fixed effects model.
The choice of our benchmark model is based on a Hausman test that was performed. Based on the results
of that test, the preferable model for this research is the random effects model. Hence, in our benchmark
analysis, we opted for the random effects approach to identify the relationship between multitasking and
academic performance. This approach exploits both the between- and within-individual variation, which
is more efficient than the fixed effects approach, which only accounts for within-individual variation. We

4
ECTS stands for European credit transfer and accumulation system. A full-time student is expected to complete a
programme of 60 credits per year.

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
refer to the fixed effects model in our robustness analysis.5

3. Results
Table 2 provides the estimations results of our benchmark analysis. In the first model of the analysis, we
regressed the average exam score of the students on their multitasking preference. For this model, we did
not include any additional control variables. Then, for Model 2, we controlled for the aforementioned time-
invariant control variables: gender, language spoken at home, origin, paternal education, number of
siblings and education prior to university. In Model 3, we added the previously mentioned control variables
that are (generally) predetermined at the start of the academic year, namely, whether one of the student s
parents has passed away, whether the student s parents are divorced, whether the student lives in a student
room and, finally, some academic programme characteristics (number of ECTS credits this year and
whether a student has to retake an exam). For the last model, Model 4, we also added the time-varying
control variables. These are academic motivation, relationship status and, finally, general health.

--- Insert Table 2 here ---

When we did not control for additional control variables (Model 1), we found an insignificant negative
coefficient of 0.101 for multitasking preference on exam score. Similarly, no significant coefficient was
found for Models 2 or 3. These models have a negative coefficient of, respectively, 0.019 and 0.047. The
more elaborate model, Model 4, is preferred since this includes all the control variables. This model
resulted in a coefficient of 0.057, but again, this was insignificant. These results contradict the literature
discussed above. Based on this literature, we expected that an increase in multitasking preference would
be associated with a decrease in average score. Nevertheless, these findings could be in line with what van
der Schuur et al. (2015) called the trained attention hypothesis , which says that by constantly alternating
between different media, young people could actually train and improve certain processes, such as filtering
irrelevant information and switching tasks. Thus, some researchers argue that multitasking may also have
positive effects (van der Schuur et al., 2015). Consequently, if we follow this hypothesis, the absence of a
negative effect in our benchmark analysis could indicate that the negative and positive effects associated with
multitasking balance each other out. For example, the negative association between multitasking and the
availability of cognitive resources could be balanced out by the fact that students are better at filtering out
irrelevant information. Thus, students can use their remaining cognitive resources in a more efficient
way. A second explanation could be related to the greater working memory capacity of young people
(Cornelius et al., 2005). The working memory capacity is the mental space in which thinking occurs, and,
according to Willingham (2010), people with more room in their working memory are better at
multitasking. So, since young people have more working memory capacity, they are better at multitasking

5
There were also theoretical reasons to opt for the random effects model as our benchmark approach. First, we did
not have sufficient within-personal information in the data with respect to our independent variable over time.

characteristics. Third, the random effects model exploits both the within-individual and the between-individual
variation more efficiently than the fixed effects model, which focuses mainly on the within-individual variation.
6

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
(Verhaeghen & Salthouse, 1996).

A first robustness check concerned the use of an alternative outcome variable, fractions of passed exams .
As in our benchmark analysis, we ran different regressions with random effects and by increasingly adding
control variables. These results, as can be seen in Table A1, are very similar to our benchmark analysis.
Second, we performed a fixed effects analysis. Also, these estimation results, as presented in Table A2,
are in line with our benchmark analysis.

4. Conclusion
The present study aimed to empirically measure whether multitasking impacts university students
academic performance. As the existing body of literature suggests, this topic is highly relevant since
students are an age group frequently associated with multitasking, and this group s main activity is often
studying, making study performance a major concern in their lives. With this study, we contribute to the
literature, which demonstrates a clear need for longitudinal research concerning this topic. For three
consecutive years, students at two major Belgian universities, in 11 programmes, were surveyed. The data
from these surveys were then merged with the exam scores. These longitudinal data of 1,673
university students were analysed by means of a random effects approach.

Based on our literature review, we hypothesised that multitasking has a negative effect on academic
performance. However, we were unable to accept our hypothesis, that is, no significant association was
found. A possible reason for the absence of a negative effect in our research could be linked to what van
der Schuur et al. (2015) stated, that multitasking could also have positive effects on young people, including
training them in multitasking behaviour, meaning they are more capable of switching between tasks and
filtering out unimportant information. Consequently, the positive and negative effects associated with
multitasking could balance each other out.

We end this article by acknowledging its main limitations, which should be considered when interpreting
the results and taken into account for further research. The first limitation concerns the number of
observations per student in the dataset. Although the dataset covered three consecutive years, only a select
number of students participated multiple times. On top of that, the data is limited to three different moments
in time, which is relatively limited. With this in mind, future research should aim to survey a significant
number of participants but more importantly collect data over more moments in time.

Second, it is challenging to accurately measure students multitasking practices and habits with the use of
an indirect method, such as a questionnaire (Mokhtari et al., 2015). Researchers could minimise the design
limitation of this kind of self-reporting instrument by using other methods, such as time-diary surveys,
which provide a more accurate representation of the time spent multitasking and the time spent on academic
tasks (Mokhtari et al., 2015). This also holds true for the scale measure used in our analyses, the MPI
(Poposki & Oswald, 2010). This inventory measures individual preferences regarding
multitasking rather than actual multitasking behaviour, which could lead to different results. A reason for

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
this is that multitasking behaviour may be affected by demands imposed by the environment; however,
these do not necessarily correspond with personal preference (Poposki & Oswald, 2010). To address this
limitation, future research could choose to include a different scale measure in addition to the MPI for
comparison and to determine if they yield similar results.

Nevertheless, this study provides nuances to previous research reporting a negative association between
multitasking and academic performance. Thus, more research, in line with our previous suggestions, needs
to be conducted to be able to give conclusive suggestions for policies on reducing multitasking behaviour
in academic settings.

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
5. References
Ajao, P. (2012). Multitasking-impact of ICT on learning (Bachelor s thesis). Lahti University of Applied
Sciences, Finland. Retrieved from
http://theseus.fi/bitstream/handle/10024/42889/AjaoPeter.pdf?sequence=1

Amez, S. & Baert, S. (2020). Smartphone use and academic performance: A literature review.
International Journal of Educational Research, 103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2020.101618

performance: First
evidence from longitudinal data. New Media & Society.
https://doi.org/10.1177%2F14614448211012374

Baert, S., Verhaest, D., Vermeir, A. & Omey, E. (2015). Mister Sandman, bring me good marks! On the
relationship between sleep quality and academic achievement. Social Science & Medicine, 130, 91
98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.02.011

Amez, S., Claeskens, M., Daman, T., Maeckelberghe, A., Omey., E. & De Marez, L.
(2020). Smartphone use and academic performance: Correlation or causal relationship? Kyklos, 73(1),
22 46. https://doi.org/10.1111/kykl.12214

Bell, A., Fairbrother, M. & Jones, K. (2019). Fixed and random effects models: Making an informed
choice. Quality & Quantity, 53(2), 1051 1074. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-018-0802-x

Burak, L. (2012). Multitasking in the university classroom. International Journal for the Scholarship of
Teaching and Learning, 6(2), 1 12. https://vc.bridgew.edu/mahpls_fac/74

Carrier, M.L., Rosen, L.D., Cheever, A.N. & Lim, F.A. (2015). Causes, effects, and practicalities of
everyday multitasking. Developmental Review, 34, 64 78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.005

Cleveland Clinic (2021). Why multitasking doesn t work. Retrieved from


https://health.clevelandclinic.org/science-clear-multitasking-doesnt-work/ on 20 April 2021.

Cornelius, J.K., Markus, B. & Murling, G. (2005). Working memory, fluid intelligence, and attention are
predictors of multitasking performance, but polychronicity and extraversion are not. Human
Performance, 18(3), 243 266. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327043hup1803_3

Delbridge, K.A. (2000). Individual differences in multi-tasking ability: Exploring a nomological network.
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Michigan State University, East Lansing.

Doleck, T. & Lajoie, S. (2018). Social networking and academic performance: A review. Educationand
Information Technology, 23(1), 435 465. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-017-9612-3

Ellis, Y., Daniels, B. & Jauregui, A. (2010). The effect of multitasking on the grade performance of
business students. Research in Higher Education Journal, 8(1), 1 10.
https://ssrn.com/abstract=1595375
9

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Ericsson. (2011). More than 50 billion connected devices. (White paper). Retrieved from
http://www.ericsson.com/res/docs/whitepapers/wp-50-billions.pdf on 25 March 2021.

Fox, A.B., Rosen, J. & Crawford, M. (2009). Distractions, distractions: Does instant messaging affect
college students performance on a concurrent reading comprehension task? CyberPsychology &
Behavior, 12(1), 51 53. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0107

Hallowell, E.M. (2005). Overloaded circuits: Why smart people underperform. Harvard Business Review,
83(1), 54 62.

Kirschner, P.A. & Karpinski, A.C. (2010). Facebook® and academic performance. Computers in Human
Behavior, 26(6), 1237 1245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.024

Junco, R., & Cotten, S. R. (2012). No A 4 U: The relationship between multitasking and academic
performance. Computers & Education, 59(2), 505-514.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.12.023

Lee, A.R., Son, S.-M. & Kim, K.K. (2016). Information and communication technology overload and
social networking service fatigue: A stress perspective. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 51 61.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.011

Lee, L., Lin, J. & Robertson, T. (2011). Reading while watching video: The effect of video content on
reading comprehension and media multitasking ability. Journal of Educational Computing Research,
45(2), 183 201. https://doi.org/10.2190%2FEC.45.2.d

Letivin, D.J. (2015). Want to learn faster? Stop multitasking and start daydreaming. Retrieved from
https://www.theguardian.com/education/2015/oct/24/want-to-learn-faster-stop-multitasking-and-
start-daydreaming on 13 April 2021.

Mayer, R.E. & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning.
Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 43 52. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_6

Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for
processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81 97.
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0043158

Mokhtari, K., Delello, J. & Reichard, C. (2015). Connected yet distracted: Multitasking among college
students. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 45(2), 164 180.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10790195.2015.1021880

Pool, M.M., van Der Voort, T.H.A., Beentjes, J.W.J. & Koolstra, C.M. (2000). Background televisionas
an inhibitor of performance on easy and difficult homework assignments. Communication Research,
27(3), 293 326. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F009365000027003002

Poposki, E.M. & Oswald, F.L. (2010). The multitasking preference inventory: Toward an improved
measure of individual differences in polychronicity. Human Performance, 23(3), 247 264.

10

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2010.487843

van der Schuur, W., Baumgartner, S., Sumter, S. & Valkenburg, P. (2015). The consequences of media
multitasking for youth: A review. Computers in Human Behavior, 53, 204 215.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.chb.2015.06.035

Tsukayama, H. (2016). Multitasking is actually kind of a problem for kids and adults. Retrieved from
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-switch/wp/2016/05/04/multitasking-is-actually-kind-of-
problem-for-kids-and-adults/ on 13 April 2021.

Uzun, A.M. & Kilis, S. (2018). Does persistent involvement in media and technology lead to lower
academic performance? Evaluating media and technology use in relation to multitasking, self-
regulation and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 90, 196 203.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.045

Vallerand, R.J., Pelletier, L.G., Blais, M.R., Briere, N.M., Senecal, C. & Vallieres, E.F. (1992). The
Academic Motivation Scale: A measure of intrinsic, extrinsic and amotivation in education.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52(4), 1003 1017.
https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0013164492052004025

Verhaeghen, P. & Salthouse, T.A. (1996). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood:


Estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122(3),
231 249. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.122.3.231

Vorderer, P., Hefner, D., Reinecke, L. & Klimmt, C. (2017). Permanently Online, Permanently
Connected: Living and Communicating in a POPC World. New York, NY: Routledge.

Willingham, D.T. (2010). Have technology and multitasking rewired how students learn? American
Educator, 34(2), 23 28.

Wood, E., Zivcakova, L., Gentile, P., Archer, A., De Pasquale, D. & Nosko, A. (2012). Examining the
impact of off-task multi-tasking with technology on real-time classroom learning. Computers &
Education, 58(1), 365 374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.08.029

11

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
6. Tables
Table 1: Summary Statistics
1 2 3

Subsample: multitasking Subsample: multitasking


preference below preference above
Full sample: average average
N = 1873 N = 1004 N = 869
A. Multitasking
Multitasking preference 2.684 2.032 3.438
B. Time invariant control variables
Female 0.536 0.534 0.537
Foreign origin 0.168 0.172 0.162
Dutch is not the main language at home 0.090 0.099 0.081
: no tertiary education 0.371 0.349 0.398
: tertiary education in college 0.293 0.348 0.322
qualification: tertiary education outside college 0.336 0.304 0.280
Number of siblings: none 0.106 0.100 0.113
Number of siblings: one 0.509 0.498 0.521
Number of siblings: two 0.274 0.286 0.260
Number of siblings: more than two 0.112 0.117 0.106
Programme in secondary education: economics languages 0.133 0.133 0.132
Programme in secondary education: economics maths 0.191 0.182 0.201
Programme in secondary education: ancient languages 0.148 0.151 0.144
Programme in secondary education: exact sciences maths 0.145 0.142 0.147
Programme in secondary education: other 0.383 0.390 0.375
General final mark for secondary education: less than 70% 0.340 0.296 0.390
General final mark for secondary education: between 70% 80% 0.536 0.562 0.505
General final mark for secondary education: more than 80% 0.125 0.142 0.105

12

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
C. Predetermined time-varying control variables
At least one parent passed away 0.029 0.026 0.033
Divorced parents 0.216 0.224 0.206
Living in a student room 0.340 0.338 0.342
Number of ECTS credits in programme 22.774 22.835 22.703
Retaking at least one course 0.021 0.016 0.028
Programme: University of Antwerp 0.472 0.483 0.459
Programme: Ghent University, business and economics 0.224 0.228 0.219
Programme: Ghent University, commercial sciences 0.247 0.241 0.254
Programme: Ghent University, public administration and management 0.057 0.047 0.068
Programme: University of Antwerp, business economics 0.191 0.199 0.181
Programme: University of Antwerp, economic policy 0.025 0.029 0.021
Programme: University of Antwerp, business engineering 0.029 0.030 0;028
Programme: University of Antwerp, management information systems 0.088 0.093 0.081
Programme: University of Antwerp, communication studies 0.032 0.032 0.032
Programme: University of Antwerp, political science 0.013 0.021 0.005
Programme: University of Antwerp, social and economic sciences 0.064 0.050 0.081
Programme: University of Antwerp, sociology 0.022 0.022 0.023
Programme: other 0.008 0.008 0.008
D. Time-varying control variables
Academic motivation scale 4.971 4.981 4.960
General health: fairly bad 0.043 0.052 0.032
General health: fairly good 0.580 0.563 0.600
General health: very good 0.377 0.385 0.368
In a relationship 0.348 0.343 0.353
E. Academic performance
Average score: completed exams 10.997 11.114 10.861
Fraction of exams passed 0.654 0.663 0.644
Note: See Section 3.2 for a description of the data.

13

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Table 2: Estimation Results: Random Effects Analysis
1 2 3 4
Dependent variable Average score: completed exams
Multitasking preference 0.101 (0.083) 0.019 (0.075) 0.047 (0.075) 0.057 (0.0752)
Female 0.195 (0.139) 0.121 (0.142) 0.133 (0.143)
Foreign origin 0.779***(0.225) 0.717***(0.225) 0.705***(0.225)
Dutch is not the main language at home 1.111***(0.287) 1.067***(0.286) 1.073***(0.286)
: tertiary education outside college 0.437**(0.171) 0.436**(0.170) 0.421**(0.169)
: tertiary education in college 0.416**(0.166) 0.434***(0.166) 0.408**(0.166)
Number of siblings: one 0.351 (0.235) 0.300 (0.234) 0.268 (0.234)
Number of siblings: two 0.320 (0.253) 0.279 (0.251) 0.260 (0.251)
Number of siblings: more than two 0.062 (0.301) 0.042 (0.298) 0.031 (0.298)
General final mark for secondary education: between 70% 80% 1.881***(0.150) 1.935***(0.149) 1.922***(0.149)
General final mark secondary education: more than 80% 3.510***(0.235) 3.682***(0.236) 3.687***(0.236)
At least one parent passed away 0.336 (0.410) 0.386 (0.409)
Divorced parents 0.289*(0.168) 0.282*(0.168)
Living in a student room 0.211 (0.142) 0.218 (0.142)
Number of ECTS credits in programme 0.020 (0.019) 0.016 (0.019)
Retaking at least one course 0.356 (0.340) 0.371 (0.340)
Academic motivation scale 0.131 (0.107)
General health: fairly good 0.937***(0.302)
General health: very good 1.054***(0.311)
In a relationship 0.023 (0.136)
Constant 11.070***(0.236) 8.537***(0.347) 9.208***(0.891) 7.750***(1.060)
Controls for programme in secondary education No Yes Yes Yes
Controls for programme in tertiary education No No Yes Yes
Number of observations 1873 1873 1873 1873
Note: The presented results are coefficient estimates, with standard errors in parentheses. ***(**)((*)) indicates significance level at the 1% (5%)((10%)) significance level.

14

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
7. Appendix: Additional tables
Table A1: Estimation Results: Random Effects Analysis With Alternative Outcome Variable
1 2 3 4
Dependent variable Fraction passed exams
Multitasking preference 0.007 (0.009) 0.001 (0.008) 0.003 (0.008) 0.004 (0.008)
Female 0.028*(0.015) 0.017 (0.016) 0.020 (0.016)
Foreign origin 0.043*(0.025) 0.040 (0.025) 0.038 (0.025)
Dutch is not the main language at home 0.126***(0.032) 0.120***(0.032) 0.121***(0.032)
: tertiary education outside college 0.051***(0.019) 0.050***(0.019) 0.047**(0.019)
: tertiary education in college 0.032*(0.018) 0.034*(0.018) 0.030 (0.019)
Number of siblings: one 0.026 (0.026) 0.021 (0.026) 0.017 (0.026)
Number of siblings: two 0.035 (0.028) 0.031 (0.028) 0.028 (0.028)
Number of siblings: more than two 0.003 (0.033) 0.008 (0.033) 0.006 (0.033)
General final mark for secondary education: between 70% 80% 0.186***(0.017) 0.193***(0.017) 0.191***(0.017)
General final mark for secondary education: more than 80% 0.304***(0.026) 0.324***(0.026) 0.325***(0.026)
At least one parent passed away 0.064 (0.045) 0.072 (0.045)
Divorced parents 0.032*(0.019) 0.031*(0.019)
Living in a student room 0.020 (0.016) 0.022 (0.016)
Number of ECTS credits in programme 0.002 (0.002) 0.001 (0.002)
Retaking at least one course 0.066*(0.040) 0.069*(0.040)
Academic motivation scale 0.017 (0.012)
General health: fairly good 0.119***(0.034)
General health: very good 0.146***(0.035)
In a relationship 0.008 (0.015)
Constant 0.655 (0.026) 0.413***(0.039) 0.525***(0.995) 0.338***(0.118)
Controls for programme in secondary education No Yes Yes Yes
Controls for programme in tertiary education No No Yes Yes
Number of observations 1873 1873 1873 1873
Note: The presented results are coefficient estimates, with standard errors in parentheses. ***(**)((*)) indicates significance level at the 1% (5%)((10%)) significance level.

15

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Table A2: Estimation Results: Fixed Effects Analysis
1 2 3
Dependent variable Average score: completed exams
Multitasking preference 0.128 (0.170) 0.180 (0.173) 0.160 (0.174)
Female / /
Foreign origin / /
Dutch is not the main language at home / /
: tertiary education outside college / /
highest qualification: tertiary education in college / /
Number of siblings: one / /
Number of siblings: two / /
Number of siblings: more than two / /
General final mark for secondary education: between 70% 80% / /
General final mark for secondary education: more than 80% / /
At least one parent passed away 1.425 (2.047) 1.559 (2.070)
Divorced parents 0.819 (0.945) 0.847 (0.948)
Living in a student room 0.491 (0.396) 0.480 (0.397)
Number of ECTS credits in programme 0.002 (0.029) 0.001 (0.029)
Retaking at least one course 0.772**(0.381) 0.791**(0.381)
Academic motivation scale 0.159 (0.312)
General health: fairly good 0.009 (0.615)
General health: very good 0.571 (0.646)
In a relationship 0.316 (0.395)
Constant 11.340***(0.458) 11.372***(0.982) 12.047***(1.875)
Controls for programme in secondary education No Yes Yes
Controls for programme in tertiary education No Yes Yes
Number of observations 1873 1873 1873
Note: The presented results are coefficient estimates, with standard errors in parentheses. ***(**)((*)) indicates significance level at the 1% (5%)((10%)) significance level.

16

This content downloaded from 112.209.185.75 on Tue, 25 Feb 2025 07:16:19 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy