86159660
86159660
com
OR CLICK BUTTON
DOWLOAD EBOOK
(Ebook) Biota Grow 2C gather 2C cook by Loucas, Jason; Viles, James ISBN
9781459699816, 9781743365571, 9781925268492, 1459699815, 1743365578, 1925268497
https://ebooknice.com/product/biota-grow-2c-gather-2c-cook-6661374
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/braincomputer-interface-research-a-state-of-the-
art-summary-8-22504520
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/brain-computer-interface-research-a-state-of-the-
art-summary-10-34358984
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/brain-computer-interface-research-a-state-of-the-
art-summary-11-54727042
ebooknice.com
(Ebook) Matematik 5000+ Kurs 2c Lärobok by Lena Alfredsson, Hans Heikne, Sanna
Bodemyr ISBN 9789127456600, 9127456609
https://ebooknice.com/product/matematik-5000-kurs-2c-larobok-23848312
ebooknice.com
(Ebook) SAT II Success MATH 1C and 2C 2002 (Peterson's SAT II Success) by Peterson's
ISBN 9780768906677, 0768906679
https://ebooknice.com/product/sat-ii-success-math-1c-and-2c-2002-peterson-s-sat-
ii-success-1722018
ebooknice.com
(Ebook) Master SAT II Math 1c and 2c 4th ed (Arco Master the SAT Subject Test: Math
Levels 1 & 2) by Arco ISBN 9780768923049, 0768923042
https://ebooknice.com/product/master-sat-ii-math-1c-and-2c-4th-ed-arco-master-
the-sat-subject-test-math-levels-1-2-2326094
ebooknice.com
(Ebook) Cambridge IGCSE and O Level History Workbook 2C - Depth Study: the United
States, 1919-41 2nd Edition by Benjamin Harrison ISBN 9781398375147, 9781398375048,
1398375144, 1398375047
https://ebooknice.com/product/cambridge-igcse-and-o-level-history-
workbook-2c-depth-study-the-united-states-1919-41-2nd-edition-53538044
ebooknice.com
https://ebooknice.com/product/operations-management-a-research-overview-state-
of-the-art-in-business-research-33371476
ebooknice.com
SPRINGER BRIEFS IN
ELEC TRIC AL AND COMPUTER ENGINEERING
Christoph Guger
Brendan Z. Allison
Michael Tangermann Editors
Brain-Computer
Interface
Research
A State-of-the-Art
Summary 9
123
SpringerBriefs in Electrical and Computer
Engineering
Series Editors
Woon-Seng Gan, School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, Nanyang
Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
C.-C. Jay Kuo, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Thomas Fang Zheng, Research Institute of Information Technology, Tsinghua
University, Beijing, China
Mauro Barni, Department of Information Engineering and Mathematics, University
of Siena, Siena, Italy
SpringerBriefs present concise summaries of cutting-edge research and practical
applications across a wide spectrum of fields. Featuring compact volumes of 50
to 125 pages, the series covers a range of content from professional to academic.
Typical topics might include: timely report of state-of-the art analytical techniques, a
bridge between new research results, as published in journal articles, and a contextual
literature review, a snapshot of a hot or emerging topic, an in-depth case study or
clinical example and a presentation of core concepts that students must understand
in order to make independent contributions.
Brain-Computer Interface
Research
A State-of-the-Art Summary 9
Editors
Christoph Guger Brendan Z. Allison
g.tec medical engineering GmbH Department of Cognitive Science
Schiedlberg, Oberösterreich, Austria University of California San Diego
San Diego, CA, USA
Michael Tangermann
Brain State Decoding Lab
University of Freiburg
Freiburg, Baden-Württemberg, Germany
© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature
Switzerland AG 2021
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher, whether
the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse
of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and
transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar
or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication
does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant
protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in this book
are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor the authors or
the editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material contained herein or for any
errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional
claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Contents
v
vi Contents
In the introduction to last year’s book (Guger et al. 2020), we said that we were
preparing for the Tenth Annual Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) Research Award
ceremony as part of the 2020 BCI Meeting in Belgium. Since then, this conference
has been postponed due to COVID. However, many entities have hosted online
conferences, workshops, training sessions, and other events that show a strong
ongoing interest in BCI research. With recent and upcoming online events from
g.tec, NeurotechX, the organizers of the planned BCI Samara Conference, and other
C. Guger (B)
g.tec medical engineering GmbH, Schiedlberg, Austria
e-mail: guger@gtec.at
M. Tangermann
Brain State Decoding Lab, Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg, Freiburg im Breisgau, Germany
B. Z. Allison
Cognitive Science Department, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, USA
organizers, there are still many opportunities to become involved in BCI research or
just learn more about the newest advances in our field.
We have also moved ahead with our ninth book, which is based on the BCI
Research Award 2019. As with earlier books, we invited the authors of projects that
were nominated for a BCI Research Award to contribute chapters describing what
they did in their project, along with discussion and newer work from their group or
other groups. Several authors in this book discuss next steps, future clinical directions,
important challenges, and other issues to add breadth to their chapters.
1 What Is a BCI?
scale projects devoted to new BCI systems meant for healthy users. BCIs for healthy
users are not new, and some applications meant for patients have also been validated
with healthy users (Israel et al. 1980; Jung et al. 1997; Münßinger et al. 2010; Nijholt
et al. 2019). However, most prior efforts have come from small research groups or
companies with relatively limited resources. Hopefully, large-scale BCI efforts will
push the field forward and foster new BCIs for healthy users and patients.
The Annual BCI Research Award is organized through the non-profit BCI Award
Foundation. The Foundation was founded in 2017 in Austria and is chaired by
Drs. Christoph Guger and Dean Krusienski. The BCI Award Foundation has Board
Members to organize the Award. Editor Brendan Allison is also on the Board (Fig. 1).
Jury members may not submit projects. The award is open to any other research
group, regardless of their location, equipment used, etc. The awards procedure this
year followed a procedure like prior years:
• The BCI Award Foundation selects a Chairperson of the Jury from a top BCI
research institute.
• The Chairperson selects a jury of international BCI experts to evaluate all projects
submitted for the Award.
• The Award website1 has instructions, scoring criteria, and the deadline for the
Award.
1 https://www.bci-award.com/Home.
4 C. Guger et al.
The first BCI Research Award was in 2010, and we’ve been producing a book along
with the Award each year. Every year, we reviewed the main purpose of the awards
and books. We want to recognize and encourage the top projects in BCI research
worldwide. Each book contains chapters written by people who were nominated
for that year’s BCI Research Award. After the Awards Ceremony, we invite the
nominees to write chapters about their work. Almost all chapters have reviewed
the work nominated for the award. We provide the authors with several additional
months after the ceremony to add new discoveries or results (from their group or
other research groups), improved tables or figures, major challenges and possible
solutions, future directions, and other commentary.
2 Bcisociety.org.
3 Tugraz.at/institutes/ine/home/.
6 C. Guger et al.
Fig. 3 The Chair of the Jury, Michael Tangermann, and jury member Selina Wriessnegger announce
the projects nominated for the BCI Award at the Awards Ceremony at a BCI conference in Graz
called the 8th Graz BCI Conference 2019
This year, for the first time, we had to leave the formatting entirely to Springer
Publishing and their typesetters, with insufficient changes to proofs. The impact is
obvious, and we hope the quality content shines through nonetheless.
Last year and this year, some chapters have been interviews, providing a different
way to learn more about the nominee’s project and related issues. For example, in
chapter “Towards Brain-Machine Interface-Based Rehabilitation for Patients with
Chronic Complete Paraplegia”, we interviewed Dr. Solaiman Shokur, a Senior
Researcher at Swiss Federal Institute of Technology in Lausanne (EPFL). Dr. Shokur
discussed his team’s research using EEG-based BCIs to help patients with spinal
cord injury (SCI). Their BCI system included locomotion training and VR, and their
results were the first to show that patients with certain types of neurological injuries
could recover some brain function with this approach.
The introduction and discussion chapters are meant to be friendly and straight-
forward. Readers who are new to BCIs, the BCI Research Awards, our book series,
or the chapters in this year’s book can learn more about all these topics. However,
most of the chapters present more challenging material. Readers who are students
or otherwise motivated to understand new terms and topics should be able to learn
about BCI projects that interest them. Experts will also learn about some of the
newest advances and the authors’ perspectives. Interview chapters are often easier
to read.
Brain-Computer Interface Research … 7
The twelve submissions with the highest scores were nominated for the BCI Research
Award 2019. These nominees, affiliations, and project names were:
Junjie Bu1 , Kymberly D. Young2 , Wei Hong1 , Ru Ma1 , Hongwen Song5 , Ying Wang1 ,
Wei Zhang1 , Michelle Hampson3 , Talma Hendler4 , Xiaochu Zhang1,5 .
1
Hefei National Laboratory for Physical Sciences at the Microscale and School
of Life Sciences, University of Science & Technology of China, Hefei, China.
2
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pitts-
burgh, USA.
3
Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, Yale School of Medicine,
New Haven, CT, USA.
4
Functional Brain Center, Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv, Israel.
5
School of Humanities & Social Science, University of Science & Technology of
China, Hefei, China.
3
Department of Biosciences and informatics, Faculty of Science and Technology,
Keio University, Kanagawa, Japan.
4
Keio Institute of Pure and Applied Sciences, Kanagawa, Japan.
5
Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Ontario, Canada.
Peter Yoo1 , Nicholas Opie1 , Thomas Oxley1 , Stephen Ronayne1 , Gil Rind1 , Amos
Meltzer1 .
1
Synchron Inc., Australia.
Farah L. Laiwalla1 , Vincent W. Leung2 , Jihun Lee1 , Patrick Mercier2 , Peter Asbeck2 ,
Ramesh Rao2 , Lawrence Larson1 , Arto Nurmikko1 .
1
Brown University, USA.
2
University of California San Diego, USA.
5 Summary
The subsequent chapters in this book present interviews and research directions that
may interest a myriad of different readers. Like chapters from preceding books, this
year’s chapters include both invasive and non-invasive BCIs, with different system
components, user interaction paradigms, signal processing methods, and goals. Many
chapters present new approaches to help different patient groups.
For example, neurofeedback was prominent in several projects nominated in 2019.
Chapters “BCI-Based Neurofeedback Training for Quitting Smoking” and “Training
with BCI-Based Neurofeedback for Quitting Smoking” present a novel type of
BCI for quitting smoking, with one chapter focused on their project and another
chapter devoted to an interview. Their project used well-established EEG neuro-
feedback principles combined with advanced BCI techniques that was effective in a
double-blind trial. Chapters “Developing a Closed-Loop Brain-Computer Interface
for Treatment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders Using Electrical Brain Stimulation”
and “Closed-Loop BCI for the Treatment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders” describe a
different approach to use neurofeedback for patients with neuropsychiatric disorders.
The project in chapter “Neurofeedback of Scalp Bi-Hemispheric EEG Sensorimotor
Rhythm Guides Hemispheric Activation of Sensorimotor Cortex in the Targeted
Hemisphere” addressed neurofeedback for sensorimotor control. Different chapters
present BCI systems to help patients recover from stroke, produce or understand
speech, or use tactile feedback to support grasping and other activities. Like many
directions presented in different chapters, their work is not yet ready for widespread
clinical application, but could ignite new ideas and follow-up efforts that could help
many people. The last chapter of this book presents the winners of the 2019 BCI
Research Award and some discussion.
References
B.Z. Allison, A. Kübler, J. Jin, 30+ years of P300 brain-computer interfaces. Psychophysiology 57,
e13569–e13569 (2020)
12 C. Guger et al.
J. Bu (B)
School of Biomedical Engineering, Research and Engineering Center of Biomedical Materials,
Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
e-mail: bujunjie@ahmu.edu.cn
J. Bu · X. Zhang
School of Life Sciences, University of Science & Technology of China, Hefei, China
1 Introduction
Nicotine addiction is the leading preventable cause of disease and death worldwide.
With approximately 75% of patients with nicotine dependence not responding fully to
the Gold Standard Programme (a comprehensive intervention consisting of manual-
based teaching sessions together with nicotine replacement therapy) for smoking
cessation interventions (Rasmussen et al. 2017), high relapse rates during long-term
follow-up periods remain a core feature of nicotine addiction. Therefore, there is an
urgent need to develop novel therapeutic approaches for nicotine addiction.
Neurofeedback, a psychophysiological procedure that helps participants self-
regulate their brain activity, has been of growing interest among basic and clinical
neuroscientists (Sitaram et al. 2017). Clinically, neurofeedback has been employed
in many psychiatric disorders, including Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
(Arns et al. 2009), depression (Young et al. 2017), anxiety (Scheinost et al. 2013)
and drug addiction (Sokhadze et al. 2008). Further, recent fMRI-based neurofeed-
back studies indicate preliminary efficacy in reducing cigarette craving in smokers
(Li et al. 2013; Hartwell et al. 2016). However, the feasibility of turning fMRI neuro-
feedback into a widely available clinical intervention is questionable. In contrast,
EEG is a relatively inexpensive and portable brain imaging technique that can be
easily implemented at any location and has more potential for wide-spread clin-
ical implementation than fMRI neurofeedback. Previous EEG-based neurofeedback
protocols, including alpha training, alpha/theta training, and SMR (sensorimotor
rhythm)/beta training, have been employed in drug addiction treatment for more
than four decades (Sokhadze et al. 2008). Using these training protocols, drug-
dependent patients received the power of a single and fixed EEG frequency and
self-regulated that signal (Schmidt et al. 2017). Most of these studies focused on
facilitating relaxation and reducing anxiety. However, the efficacy on drug addiction
has only been classified as “probably efficacious” in reports from the Association
for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback and the International Society for
Neurofeedback and Research (Sokhadze et al. 2008; Schmidt et al. 2017). Addi-
tionally, recent studies question the clinical efficacy of previous EEG neurofeedback
protocols (Thibault and Raz 2016; Fovet et al. 2017; Schabus et al. 2017). Hence, a
new direction for EEG neurofeedback in treating drug addiction is warranted.
The efficacy of these traditional EEG neurofeedback approaches in the treat-
ment of addiction remains dubious, in part, because addiction process involves many
complex cognitive models (e.g. the cue reactivity model (Chiamulera 2005) and
negative reinforcement model Koob [2013]), but previous neurofeedback studies
BCI-Based Neurofeedback Training for Quitting Smoking 15
mainly targeted arousal and/or anxiety. Instead, drug cue reactivity can evoke the
impulse for drug-seeking behavior in addiction (Weiss et al. 2001). Previous studies
from our group and others indicate that smoking cue reactivity is a central charac-
teristic of nicotine addiction (Zhang et al. 2009; Engelmann et al. 2012) and can
predict relapse vulnerability (Janes et al. 2010); thus, there is evidential support for
a causal relationship between cue reactivity and relapse (Parvaz et al. 2011). There-
fore, reducing brain reactivity to smoking cues has the potential to improve smoking
cessation outcomes.
Recent EEG studies have reported that smoking cue reactivity is a complex brain
activity pattern that involves multiple EEG features, including both time domain (e.g.,
P300, slow positive wave) and frequency domain (e.g., alpha oscillation) features
(Cui et al. 2013; Littel and Franken 2007; Littel et al. 2012). Typically, multi-
variate pattern analysis (MVPA) can enhance sensitivity of detecting a particular
brain activity pattern by using multifeature combinations for input to multivariate
patterns (Haynes and Rees 2006). A number of neurofeedback studies combined
with MVPA have been impressively successful at improving attention and percep-
tual learning after only a few sessions (deBettencourt et al. 2015; Shibata et al.
2011), whereas some traditional EEG-based neurofeedback studies require dozens
of sessions for any effects to be detected (Sokhadze et al. 2008).
In the current study, we evaluated a novel EEG neurofeedback paradigm
(cognition-guided neurofeedback) effects on nicotine addiction by double-blind,
randomized, placebo-controlled design.
2 Cognition-Guided Neurofeedback
similar to neutral cue activity patterns, the score decreased, and when the current
activity patterns were more similar to the smoking cue activity patterns, the score
increased.
To improve participants’ vigilance, and help them better self-monitor and evaluate
their brain state during neurofeedback, we used an adaptive closed-loop method in
which the neurofeedback stimulus and decoded brain state influenced each other in
real-time (deBettencourt et al. 2015). Different craving level pictures evoked different
degrees of smoking cue reactivity for smokers (Carter and Tiffany 1999). In the
current study, an approach of continually updating sensory stimuli (e.g., different
craving level pictures) based on changing brain states (e.g., different degrees of
smoking cue reactivity pattern) constituted a “closed-loop” design. The logic of this
closed-loop design is that, when a participant was unsuccessful in “deactivating”
the smoking cue pattern (i.e., the probabilistic score increased), a picture with a
higher craving level was displayed to amplify and externalize the consequences of
the participant’s smoking cue related brain activity pattern (deBettencourt et al. 2015;
deBettencourt and Norman 2016). This made unsuccessful deactivation more salient
and increased the self-monitoring demand of the task. In other words, we amplified
the consequences of their cue pattern, rewarding successful deactivation by reducing
difficulty with a lower craving level picture and punishing unsuccessful deactivation
by increasing difficulty with a higher craving level picture.
The probabilistic score was presented at the bottom half of the screen and trans-
ferred into a picture presented at the top half of the screen at the same time (Fig. 1b).
The association between the probabilistic score and the transferred picture was
controlled by a linear positive correlation function. To reduce fluctuations due to
noise in the EEG signal, the probabilistic score value of each trial was calculated
using a moving average of the current and two preceding values.
After neurofeedback practice, participants were required to identify ten cognitive
strategies that may be effective at de-activating the neurofeedback signal, but it was
emphasized that they should adjust their strategies to find a method that works best
for them during neurofeedback (Instruction: “Make the feedback curve move down
and the picture induce less craving”). Each neurofeedback training session consisted
of 8 cycles, with 40 trials per cycle. Each trial was updated every 2 s including 1
s acquisition and 1 s computing, with a 1 min rest between cycles. At the end of
each cycle, the self-regulation performance during the previous cycle was presented.
After each cycle, participants rated their perceived control over the neurofeedback
signal. The final cumulative performance was translated into an additional money
reward. Both groups received the same money after neurofeedback.
18 J. Bu and X. Zhang
The study was a double blind, randomized, placebo-controlled design. The experi-
mental procedure consisted of four stages (Fig. 1c): (1) baseline session (Visit 1); (2)
two neurofeedback training sessions (Visit 2 and Visit 3); (3) post-training behavioral
session (Visit 4); and (4) follow-up session (Visit 5). Participants were required to be
abstinent from smoking cigarettes for two hours prior to every visit, which ensured
participants had some craving and responsiveness to the cues without the potential
confound of a ceiling effect from prolonged abstinence.
Sixty participants who met the following criteria participated in the experiment:
smoking 10 or more cigarettes per day for 2 years or more, right-handed, between
18 and 40 years of age, normal or corrected to normal vision, and in good mental
and physical health assessed by the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview
(Sheehan et al. 1998).
Participants were randomly assigned to the real-feedback group (n = 30) or the
yoked-feedback group (n = 30). The real-feedback group regulated their own online
brain patterns. The yoked-feedback group regulated the brain activity pattern of a
matched participant in the real-feedback group (deBettencourt et al. 2015).
Fig. 3 Neurofeedback effects on short-term cigarette craving and long-term smoking behavior. *p
< 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.005; ns: not significant. NF: neurofeedback
the real-feedback group showed a significant correlation between the average de-
activated neurofeedback performance and the current cigarette amount at 4-month
follow-up (r = 0.58, p = 0.004) (Fig. 3d). These findings indicated neurofeedback
produced long-term effects on smoking behavior.
Figure 4a shows that the classification accuracy of the pre-neurofeedback clas-
sifier significantly predicted decreased craving scores in the real-feedback group
(r = 0.40, p = 0.03), while the same prediction was not significant in the yoked-
feedback group (r = 0.13, p = .54). Moreover, the correlation analysis revealed that
the degree of de-activation during the first cycle of the first neurofeedback success-
fully predicted the number of cigarettes smoked per day at the 4-month follow-up
(r = 0.45, p = 0.03, Fig. 4b) in the real-feedback group, but not in the yoked-
feedback group (r = 0.16, p = 0.45). These findings indicated short- and long-
term effects were predicted by the classification accuracy at pre-neurofeedback and
neurofeedback performance during the first training cycle, respectively.
BCI-Based Neurofeedback Training for Quitting Smoking 21
Fig. 4 Individual-level prediction of short-term (a) and long-term (b) effects. NF: neurofeedback
6 Conclusion
References
M. Arns et al., Efficacy of neurofeedback treatment in ADHD: the effects on inattention, impulsivity
and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 40(3), 180–189 (2009)
B.L. Carter, S.T. Tiffany, Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction 94(3),
327–340 (1999)
C. Chiamulera, Cue reactivity in nicotine and tobacco dependence: a “multiple-action” model of
nicotine as a primary reinforcement and as an enhancer of the effects of smoking-associated
stimuli. Brain Res. Rev. 48(1), 74–97 (2005)
Y. Cui et al., Alpha oscillations in response to affective and cigarette-related stimuli in smokers.
Nicotine Tob. Res. 15(5), 917–924 (2013)
M.T. deBettencourt, K.A. Norman, Neuroscience: Incepting associations. Curr. Biol. 26(14), R673–
R675 (2016)
22 J. Bu and X. Zhang
M.T. deBettencourt et al., Closed-loop training of attention with real-time brain imaging. Nat.
Neurosci. 18(3), 470–475 (2015)
J.M. Engelmann et al., Neural substrates of smoking cue reactivity: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies.
Neuroimage 60(1), 252–262 (2012)
T. Fovet et al., On assessing neurofeedback effects: should double-blind replace neurophysiological
mechanisms? Brain 140(10), e63 (2017)
D.M. Groppe, T.P. Urbach, M. Kutas, Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields
I: a critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology 48(12), 1711–1725 (2011)
K.J. Hartwell et al., Individualized real-time fMRI neurofeedback to attenuate craving in nicotine-
dependent smokers. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 41(1), 48–55 (2016)
J.D. Haynes, G. Rees, Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
7(7), 523–534 (2006)
A.C. Janes et al., Brain reactivity to smoking cues prior to smoking cessation predicts ability to
maintain tobacco abstinence. Biol. Psychiatry 67(8), 722–729 (2010)
G.F. Koob, Negative reinforcement in drug addiction: the darkness within. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
23(4), 559–563 (2013)
X. Li et al., Volitional reduction of anterior cingulate cortex activity produces decreased cue craving
in smoking cessation: a preliminary real-time fMRI study. Addict. Biol. 18(4), 739–748 (2013)
M. Littel, I.H. Franken, The effects of prolonged abstinence on the processing of smoking cues: an
ERP study among smokers, ex-smokers and never-smokers. J. Psychopharmacol 21(8), 873–882
(2007)
M. Littel et al., Electrophysiological indices of biased cognitive processing of substance-related
cues: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36(8), 1803–1816 (2012)
F. Lotte et al., A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain-computer interfaces: a 10
year update. J. Neural Eng. 15(3), 031005 (2018)
M.A. Parvaz et al., Neuroimaging for drug addiction and related behaviors. Rev. Neurosci. 22(6),
609–624 (2011)
M. Rasmussen, E. Fernandez, H. Tonnesen, Effectiveness of the Gold Standard Programme
compared with other smoking cessation interventions in Denmark: a cohort study. BMJ Open
7(2), e013553 (2017)
M. Schabus, et al., Better than sham? A double-blind placebo-controlled neurofeedback study in
primary insomnia. Brain (2017)
D. Scheinost et al., Orbitofrontal cortex neurofeedback produces lasting changes in contamination
anxiety and resting-state connectivity. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e250 (2013)
J. Schmidt, C. Kärgel, M. Opwis, Neurofeedback in substance use and overeating: current
applications and future directions. Curr. Addict. Rep. 4(2), 116–131 (2017)
D.V. Sheehan, et al., The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development
and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin.
Psychiatry 59(Suppl 20), 22–33;quiz 34–57 (1998)
K. Shibata et al., Perceptual learning incepted by decoded fMRI neurofeedback without stimulus
presentation. Science 334(6061), 1413–1415 (2011)
R. Sitaram et al., Closed-loop brain training: the science of neurofeedback. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
18(2), 86–100 (2017)
T.M. Sokhadze, R.L. Cannon, D.L. Trudeau, EEG biofeedback as a treatment for substance use disor-
ders: Review, rating of efficacy, and recommendations for further research. Appl. Psychophysiol.
Biofeedback 33(1), 1–28 (2008)
R.T. Thibault, A. Raz, When can neurofeedback join the clinical armamentarium? Lancet Psychiatry
3(6), 497–498 (2016)
F. Weiss et al., Compulsive drug-seeking behavior and relapse - Neuroadaptation, stress, and
conditioning factors. Biol. Basis Cocaine Addict. 937, 1–26 (2001)
BCI-Based Neurofeedback Training for Quitting Smoking 23
K.D. Young et al., Randomized Clinical Trial of Real-Time fMRI Amygdala Neurofeedback for
Major Depressive Disorder: Effects on Symptoms and Autobiographical Memory Recall. Am. J.
Psychiatry 174(8), 748–755 (2017)
X. Zhang et al., Masked smoking-related images modulate brain activity in smokers. Hum. Brain
Mapp. 30(3), 896–907 (2009)
Neurofeedback of Scalp Bi-Hemispheric
EEG Sensorimotor Rhythm Guides
Hemispheric Activation of Sensorimotor
Cortex in the Targeted Hemisphere
M. Hayashi · S. Tsuchimoto
School of Fundamental Science and Technology, Graduate School of Keio
University, Kanagawa, Japan
N. Mizuguchi · S. Tsuchimoto
The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan
N. Mizuguchi · J. Ushiba (B)
Department of Biosciences and Informatics, Faculty of Science and Technology,
Keio University, Kanagawa, Japan
e-mail: ushiba@brain.bio.keio.ac.jp
J. Ushiba
Keio Institute of Pure and Applied Sciences, Kanagawa, Japan
shoulder muscles are innervated bihemispherically, whereas hand muscles are mostly
innervated contralaterally.
1 Introduction
In the pre- and post-evaluation blocks, no visual feedback was provided. The aim of
the pre-evaluation block was to evaluate the baseline brain activity and to calibrate
parameters in the neurofeedback settings each day. First, the target frequency was
calibrated for each participant in order to feedback the most reactive frequency. The
target frequency was selected from the alpha band (8–13 Hz) by calculating the mean
intensity of SMR-ERD with a 3-Hz sliding bin and 2-Hz overlap. SMR-ERD in the
alpha band is a reliable EEG biomarker representing increased neuronal excitability
in SM1, corticospinal tract, and thalamocortical systems. Second, the target level of
SMR-ERD during MI was normalized for each participant at the third quartile of the
contralateral or ipsilateral SMR-ERD in the pre-evaluation block. This setting was
empirically approved by the authors as a moderate load for effective operant learning
(Naros et al. 2016).
In the training blocks, participants received visual feedback based on the SMR-
ERDs from both left and right hemispheres. The real-time SMR-ERD intensity in
each hemisphere (relative to the average power of the last 6 s of the resting epoch) was
obtained every 100 ms. To modulate target hemisphere-dependent SMR-ERD, we
Neurofeedback of Scalp Bi-Hemispheric EEG … 29
developed BCI-neurofeedback that displayed both left and right hemispheric SMR-
ERDs concurrently, allowing participants to learn to regulate these two variates at the
same time. Visual feedback was provided on a computer screen in the form of cursor
movements in a two-dimensional coordinate, in which each axis corresponded to the
degree of the contralateral or ipsilateral SMR-ERD (Fig. 1). The axis range was from
−100% (i.e., ERS) to 100%, and the cursors were presented at the origin-position
(x = 0, y = 0) at the initiation of a trial. A key point of this study is that participants
were always instructed to try to move the cursor toward the upper right in the two-
dimensional coordinate during MI in all four neurofeedback training sessions. In the
case of shoulder MI, for example, participants performed the same MI and tried to
move the cursor to the upper right regardless of whether it was a Shoulder-contra
or Shoulder-ipsi session. However, the coordinate systems during the two sessions
differed as follows: in the Shoulder-contra session, the x-axis and y-axis corresponded
to the ipsilateral SMR-ERS and contralateral SMR-ERD, respectively. Conversely,
in the Shoulder-ipsi session, the x-axis and y-axis corresponded to the contralateral
SMR-ERS and ipsilateral SMR-ERD, respectively. Thus, the upper right position
always indicated a reduction in alpha rhythm in the targeted hemisphere with respect
to the baseline (i.e., SMR-ERD) and an increase in the non-targeted side (i.e., SMR-
ERS). Using such a gimmicked environment, we aimed at lateralizing cortical activity
in the sensorimotor cortex, blinding which task was being performed.
A score was calculated when the most recent cursor on the screen reached the
ten blue boxes representing the scoring range (Fig. 1b). The coordinates of each
blue box corresponded to the degree of bilateral SMR-ERDs, with the x-axis set in
steps of 10% SMR-ERS in the non-targeted hemisphere, and y-axis ranged from the
predefined threshold to 100% SMR-ERD in the target hemisphere. At the end of the
trial, the computer cursor returned to the origin position. A score for each segment
(each computer cursor updated every 100 ms) was obtained during the MI epoch to
provide feedback about the overall performance of each trial. The darkest blue box
in the upper left in Fig. 1b had a low score (5 points), whereas the lightest blue box
in the upper right had a high score (15 points). The boxes in the middle were set
in steps of 1 point. Finally, a cumulative sum calculated by adding all scores was
displayed for 5 s in the left side of the screen at the interval period in each trial (range:
0–765 points). To boost learning of self-regulation in sensorimotor cortical activity,
participants were encouraged to get a higher cumulative sum than during the previous
trial. Such screen presentation of the total score at the end of the trial is referred
to as ‘intermittent feedback’ (Johnson et al. 2012). Previous studies demonstrated
that providing intermittent feedback is a useful element for neurofeedback training
(Shibata et al. 2011; Posse et al. 2003) because it probably reduces cognitive loads
during MI.
30 M. Hayashi et al.
3 BCI Performance
Figure 2 illustrates the changes in BCI performance (i.e., total cumulative score) in
each session. In shoulder MI, the total cumulative score was improved in both the
Shoulder-contra session (pre = 1170, post = 1651, difference = 481, Cohen’s d =
0.72, p = 0.007, paired t-test) and the Shoulder-ipsi session (pre = 891, post = 1310,
difference = 419, Cohen’s d = 0.84, p = 0.011, paired t-test). On the other hand,
in hand MI, the total cumulative score was improved in the Hand-contra session
(pre = 1783, post = 1136, difference = 647, Cohen’s d = 1.20, p = 0.018, paired
t-test), but not in the Hand-ipsi session (pre = 1249, post = 1253, difference = 4,
Cohen’s d = 0.01, p = 0.97, paired t-test). We also found that the total cumulative
score in the evaluation blocks was lower than that in the training blocks, which is in
keeping with the well-known information that visual feedback enhances MI-based
BCI performance (Ono et al. 2015; Pichiorri et al. 2015). Differences between the 6
training blocks for cumulative score were not statistically significant (all p > 0.05,
one-way rmANOVA).
Neurofeedback of Scalp Bi-Hemispheric EEG … 31
Spatial patterns of SMR-ERD during the MI epoch in the pre- and post-evaluation
blocks of a representative participant are shown in Fig. 3a, b. The SMR-ERDs were
localized predominantly in the bilateral parieto-temporal regions during the pre-
evaluation block, regardless of whether it was a Shoulder-contra or Shoulder-ipsi
session. During the Shoulder-contra session, the contralateral SMR-ERD increased
after the neurofeedback training session, whereas the ipsilateral SMR-ERD did not
(Fig. 3a). Conversely, during the Shoulder-ipsi session, the contralateral SMR-ERD
did not increase, but the ipsilateral SMR-ERD did (Fig. 3b).
Figure 3c, d show Laterality Index (LI) changes during the Shoulder-contra and
Shoulder-ipsi sessions, respectively. During the Shoulder-contra session, the LI in
the post-evaluation block (−0.113 ± 0.072) was significantly lower than that in the
pre-evaluation block (−0.030 ± 0.089) (difference = 0.083, Cohen’s d = 1.76, p
= 0.023, paired t-test; Fig. 3c). By contrast, during the Shoulder-ipsi session, the
LI in the post-evaluation block (0.017 ± 0.103) was significantly higher than that
in the pre-evaluation block (−0.067 ± 0.103) (difference = 0.084, Cohen’s d =
0.86, p = 0.039, paired t-test; Fig. 3d). Target-hemisphere-dependent SMR-ERDs
were modulated during both the Shoulder-contra and Shoulder-ipsi sessions, even
though participants repeated the same MI under the neurofeedback setting with only
a change in the rule of cursor movement (i.e., reversal of x-axis and y-axis).
We assessed seed-based corrected imaginary part of coherence (ciCOH during the
resting-state in the pre- and post-evaluation blocks to evaluate interregional synchro-
nization (i.e., functional connectivity). Figure 4a, b show significant intrahemispheric
connections in each hemisphere of a representative participant. The number of signif-
icant connections in the contralateral hemisphere increased from the pre- to the post-
epochs during the Shoulder-contra session (Fig. 4a), whereas they increased in the
ipsilateral hemisphere during the Shoulder-ipsi session (Fig. 4b). Intrahemispheric
network intensity changes in the targeted hemisphere during the Shoulder-contra
and Shoulder-ipsi sessions are shown in Fig. 4c and d, respectively. Figure 4e, f
show significant interhemispheric connections of a representative participant, which
increased during both the Shoulder-contra and Shoulder-ipsi sessions. Changes in
interhemispheric network intensity for all participants during the Shoulder-contra
and Shoulder-ipsi sessions are outlined in Fig. 4g and h, respectively. During the
Shoulder-contra session, the interhemispheric network intensity was significantly
higher during the post-evaluation block (2.01 ± 0.28) than during the pre-evaluation
block (1.80 ± 0.19; difference = 0.21, Cohen’s d = 0.88, p = 0.030, paired t-test;
Fig. 4g). Similarly, during the Shoulder-ipsi session, the interhemispheric network
intensity was significantly higher in the post-evaluation block (2.09 ± 0.42) than in
the pre-evaluation block (1.77 ± 0.37) (difference = 0.31, Cohen’s d = 0.81, p =
0.006, paired t-test; Fig. 4h).
Neurofeedback of Scalp Bi-Hemispheric EEG … 33
Fig. 5 Two-way interaction in SMR-ERD during shoulder MI (dark gray) and hand MI (light
gray) tasks
34 M. Hayashi et al.
hemisphere during the shoulder MI task, but no significant difference was observed
during the hand MI task.
6 Discussion
The SMR-ERD contralateral to the imagined limb increased significantly after both
the shoulder-contra and Hand-contra sessions. Previous studies also demonstrated
up-conditioning of the contralateral SM1 using contralateral-based neurofeedback
during hand MI (Birbaumer and Cohen 2007; Ang et al. 2011; Prasad et al. 2010).
Repetitive induction of the SMR-ERD contralateral to the imagined limb through
visual or sensory feedback with neuromuscular electrical stimulation or robotic
movement supports are considered to induce the use-dependent, error-based, and/or
Hebbian-like plasticity of the contralateral SM1 (Gharabaghi et al. 2014; Soekadar
et al. 2015b; Ushiba and Soekadar 2016). As contralateral SMR-ERD is a surro-
gate monitoring marker of contralateral SM1 excitability (Takemi et al. 2013, 2015;
Hayashi et al. 2019), BCI-neurofeedback can promote operant learning of contralat-
eral sensorimotor cortical activity. This is an expected phenomenon because distal
muscles such as the EDC muscle are innervated from the contralateral hemisphere,
which is most influential for muscle contraction (Carson 2005; Colebatch et al. 1990).
However, the BCI-neurofeedback-derived SMR signal from the contralateral
hemisphere does not always guarantee spatially specific activation of the contralat-
eral SM1 because both hemispheres are connected by intrinsic transcallosal projec-
tions and exhibit functional crosstalk (Arai et al. 2011; Waters et al. 2017). Indeed,
conventional contralateral-based BCI-neurofeedback has induced a global increase
including the ipsilateral SMR-ERD, indicating conventional BCI is considered as a
modulation technique without spatial specificity (Pichiorri et al. 2015; Birbaumer
and Cohen 2007; Ono et al. 2014). A key advantage of our study was that the
BCI-neurofeedback that we developed monitored both contralateral and ipsilateral
SMR-ERDs, demonstrating explicitly guided sensorimotor cortical activation in the
targeted contralateral hemisphere alone.
Guiding cortical sensorimotor activation to the targeted hemisphere is also crucial
in the context of neurorehabilitation. For example, it is known that an imbal-
anced interhemispheric inhibition due to excessive suppression from the ipsilateral
(contralesional) to the contralateral side results in further attenuation of the contralat-
eral sensorimotor cortical activity (Shimizu et al. 2002; Murase et al. 2004; Bütefisch
et al. 2008). A temporary guide to down-conditioning in the ipsilateral hemisphere
using non-invasive brain stimulation is also important to reduce interhemispheric
inhibition after stroke (Hummel and Cohen 2006; Takeuchi et al. 2012). Therefore,
the laterality shifting of sensorimotor cortical activity to the contralateral side may
Neurofeedback of Scalp Bi-Hemispheric EEG … 35
contribute to the degree of achievable functional recovery (Askim et al. 2009; Chieffo
et al. 2013).
characteristics and voluntary control of brain activity. Therefore, our findings implied
that intrinsic neuroanatomical properties such as the CST constrains the effective-
ness in BCI-neurofeedback training purported to lateralize sensorimotor cortical
activities. Further work that approaches the further understandings of differences in
BCI-learning is warranted.
7 Conclusion
References
Bundy et al., Contralesional brain-computer interface control of a powered exoskeleton for motor
recovery in chronic stroke survivors. Stroke 48, 1908–1915 (2017)
C.M. Bütefisch, M. Weβling, J. Netz, R.J. Seitz, V. Hömberg, Relationship between interhemispheric
inhibition and motor cortex excitability in subacute stroke patients. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair
22, 4–21 (2008)
A. Caria et al., Chronic stroke recovery after combined BCI training and physiotherapy: a case
report. Psychophysiology 48, 578–582 (2011)
R.G. Carson, Neural pathways mediating bilateral interactions between the upper limbs. Brain Res.
Rev. 49, 641–662 (2005)
R. Chieffo et al., Mapping early changes of cortical motor output after subcortical stroke: a
transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Brain Stimul. 6, 322–329 (2013)
M. Chiew, S.M. LaConte, S.J. Graham, Investigation of fMRI neurofeedback of differential primary
motor cortex activity using kinesthetic motor imagery. NeuroImage 61, 21–31 (2012)
J.G. Colebatch, J.C. Rothwell, B.L. Day, P.D. Thompson, C.D. Marsden, Cortical outflow to
proximal arm muscles in man. Brain 113, 1843–1856 (1990)
K.C. Dodd, V.A. Nair, V. Prabhakaran, Role of the contralesional vs. ipsilesional hemisphere in
stroke recovery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 9 (2017)
A. Gharabaghi et al., Coupling brain-machine interfaces with cortical stimulation for brain-state
dependent stimulation: enhancing motor cortex excitability for neurorehabilitation. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 8, 7 (2014)
S. Halder et al., Prediction of brain-computer interface aptitude from individual brain structure.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 9 (2013)
K. Hasegawa et al., Ipsilateral EEG mu rhythm reflects the excitability of uncrossed pathways
projecting to shoulder muscles. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 14, 11 (2017)
M. Hayashi et al., Two-stage regression of high-density scalp electroencephalograms visualizes
force regulation signaling during muscle contraction. J. Neural Eng. 16, 056020 (2019)
F.C. Hummel, L.G. Cohen, Non-invasive brain stimulation: a new strategy to improve neuroreha-
bilitation after stroke? Lancet Neurol. 5, 708–712 (2006)
K.A. Johnson et al., Intermittent ‘real-time’ fMRI feedback is superior to continuous presentation
for a motor imagery task: a pilot study. J. Neuroimaging 22, 58–66 (2012)
J.G. McPherson et al., Progressive recruitment of contralesional cortico-reticulospinal pathways
drives motor impairment post stroke. J. Physiol. 596, 1211–1225 (2018)
W. Muellbacher et al., Improving hand function in chronic stroke. Arch. Neurol. 59, 1278–1282
(2002)
N. Murase, J. Duque, R. Mazzocchio, L.G. Cohen, Influence of interhemispheric interactions on
motor function in chronic stroke. Ann. Neurol. 55, 400–409 (2004)
G. Naros, I. Naros, F. Grimm, U. Ziemann, A. Gharabaghi, Reinforcement learning of self-regulated
sensorimotor β-oscillations improves motor performance. NeuroImage 134, 142–152 (2016)
C. Neuper, G. Pfurtscheller, Event-related dynamics of cortical rhythms: frequency-specific features
and functional correlates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 43, 41–58 (2001)
C. Neuper, M. Wörtz, G. Pfurtscheller, ERD/ERS patterns reflecting sensorimotor activation and
deactivation. Prog. Brain Res. 159, 211–222 (2006)
T. Ono et al., Brain-computer interface with somatosensory feedback improves functional recovery
from severe hemiplegia due to chronic stroke. Front. Neuroeng. 7, 19 (2014)
T. Ono et al., Multimodal sensory feedback associated with motor attempts alters BOLD responses
to paralyzed hand movement in chronic stroke patients. Brain Topogr. 28, 340–351 (2015)
G. Pfurtscheller, C. Brunner, A. Schlögl, F.H. Lopes da Silva, Mu rhythm (de)synchronization and
EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks. Neuroimage 31, 153–159 (2006)
F. Pichiorri et al., Brain-computer interface boosts motor imagery practice during stroke recovery.
Ann. Neurol. 77, 851–865 (2015)
S. Posse et al., Real-time fMRI of temporolimbic regions detects amygdala activation during single-
trial self-induced sadness. NeuroImage 18, 760–768 (2003)
38 M. Hayashi et al.
1 Introduction
Be it so, then: But is there no better pleasure for him to aim at, and
which he loses by following this; and although a man’s ways, we are
told, be right in his own eyes162; yet, is there no difference in them,
and do not some of them lead through much trouble to
disappointment and death? And is there not a presumption, a
certainty, that the way of the miser is of this sort? when his very
name may admonish him of the light in which the common sense of
mankind regards his pursuit of untasted opulence; and when he
finds, by experience, that his unnatural appetite for it is always
encreasing, be the plenty never so great which is set before him.
But,
But ye shall judge for yourselves. Are not riches, let me ask, sadly
misapplied, when, after having been pursued and seized upon, with
more than a miser’s fury, they are suddenly let go again, on all the
wings163 of prodigality and folly? which scatter their precious load,
not on modest merit, or virtuous industry, or suffering innocence,
but on the flatterers of pride, the retainers of pomp, the panders of
pleasure; in a word, on those miscreants, who imped these harpies,
and sent them forth, for the annoyance of mankind.
And well are these spendthrifts repaid for their good service. For this
profusion brings on more pains and penalties, than I am able to
express; disappointment, regret, disgust, and infamy; and not
uncommonly, in the train of these, that tremendous spectre to a
voluptuous man, Poverty: or, if the source, which feeds this whirlpool
of riotous expence, be yet unexhausted, and flow copiously, these
waters have that baleful quality, that they inflame, instead of
quenching, the drinker’s thirst. All his natural appetites grow nice
and delicate; and ten thousand artificial ones are created, and
become more vexatious to him, than any that are of nature’s
growth. The idolater of riches, the infatuated lover of silver, now
finds, that the power he serves, the mistress he adores, yields him
no other fruit of all his assiduity, but self-abhorrence and distraction;
the loss of all virtuous feelings; and numberless clamorous desires,
which give him no truce of their importunity, and are incapable, by
any gratification, of being quieted and assuaged.
So true is the observation, that he, who, loveth silver, shall not be
satisfied with silver! For, either the passion grows upon us, when the
object is not enjoyed; or, if it be, a new force is given to it, and a
legion of other passions, as impatient and unmanageable as the
original one, start up out of the enjoyment itself.
I know the lovers of money are not easily made sensible of this fatal
alternative. They think, that this, or that sum, will fill164 all their
wishes, and make them as rich, and as happy, as they desire to be.
But they presently feel their mistake; and yet rarely find out, that
the way to content lies through self-command, and that to have
enough of any thing which this world affords, we must be careful not
to grasp at too much of it.
On the entrance into life, higher and more generous motives usually
excite the better part of mankind to labour in those professions, that
are accounted liberal. But, as they proceed in their course, interest,
which was always one spur to their industry, infixes itself deeply into
their minds, and stimulates them more sensibly than any other. It
can scarce be otherwise, considering the influence of example; the
experience they have, or think they have, of the advantages, that
attend encreasing wealth; the fashion of the times, which indulges,
or, as we easily persuade ourselves, requires refined, and therefore
expensive, pleasures; and, above all, the selfishness of the human
mind, which is, and, for wise reasons, was intended to be a powerful
spring of action in us.
Let then the fortune, or the honour (for both are included in the
magical word silver) which eminent worth may propose to itself, be
among the inducements which erect the hopes, and quicken the
application, of a virtuous man. But let him know withal (and I am in
no pain for the effect, which this premature knowledge may have
upon him) that the application, and not the object, is that in which
he will find his account; just as the pursuit, and not the game, is the
true reward of the chace. He who thinks otherwise, and reckons that
affluence is content, or grandeur, happiness, will have leisure, if he
attain to either, to rectify his opinion, and to see that he had made a
very false estimate of human life.
And, now, having thus far commented on my text, I will take leave,
for once, to step beyond it, and shew you, in few words (for many
cannot be necessary on so plain a subject) where and how
satisfaction may be found.
In the abundance of silver, it does not, and cannot lie; nor yet in a
cynical contempt of it: but, in few and moderate desires; in a correct
taste of life, which consults nature more than fancy in the choice of
its pleasures; in rejecting imaginary wants, and keeping a strict hand
on those that are real; in a sober use of what we possess, and no
further concern about more than what may engage us, by honest
means, to acquire it; in considering who, and what we are165; that
we are creatures of a day, to whom long desires and immeasurable
projects are very ill suited; that we are reasonable creatures, who
should make a wide difference between what seems to be, and what
is important; that we are accountable creatures, and should be more
concerned to make a right use of what we possess, than to enlarge
our possessions; that, above all, we are Christians, who are
expected to sit loose to a transitory world, to extend our hopes to
another life, and to qualify ourselves for it.
In this way, and with these reflections, we shall see things in a true
light, and shall either not desire abundant wealth, or shall
understand its true value. The strictest morality, and even our divine
religion, lays no obligation upon us to profess poverty. We are even
required to be industrious in our several callings and stations, and
are, of course, allowed to reap the fruits, whatever they be, of an
honest industry. Yet it deserves our consideration, that wealth is
always a snare, and therefore too often a curse; that, if virtuously
obtained, it affords but a moderate satisfaction at best; and that, if
we WILL be rich, that is, resolve by any means, and at all events, to
be so, we pierce ourselves through with many sorrows166; that it
even requires more virtue to manage, as we ought, a great estate,
than to acquire it, in the most reputable manner; that affluent, and,
still more, enormous wealth secularizes the heart of a Christian too
much, indisposes him for the offices of piety, and too often (though
it may seem strange) for those of humanity; that it inspires a
sufficiency and self-dependance, which was not designed for mortal
man; an impatience of complying with the rules of reason, and the
commands of religion; a forgetfulness of our highest duties, or an
extreme reluctance to observe them.
Yet, with God (our gracious Master adds) all things are possible. I
return, therefore, to the doctrine with which I set out, and conclude;
that riches are not evil in themselves; that the moderate desire of
them is not unlawful; that a right use of them is even meritorious.
But then you will reflect on what the nature of things, as well as the
voice of Solomon, loudly declares, that he who loveth silver, shall not
be satisfied with silver; that the capacity of the human mind is not
filled with it; that, if we pursue it with ardour, and make it the sole
or the chief object of our pursuit, it never did, and never can yield a
true and permanent satisfaction; that, if riches encrease, it is our
interest, as well as duty, not to set our hearts upon them169; and
that, finally, we are so to employ the riches, we any of us have, with
temperance and sobriety, with mercy and charity, as to make
ourselves friends of the mammon of unrighteousness (of the
mammon, which usually deserves to be so called) that, when we fail
(when our lives come, as they soon will do, to an end) they may
receive us into everlasting habitations170.
SERMON XXVI.
PREACHED FEBRUARY 21, 1773.
The topics, he chiefly insists upon, are taken, not from nature, but
the principles of our holy religion, from the right and property, which
God hath in Christians. By virtue of their profession, their bodies and
souls are appropriated to him. Therefore, says he, glorify God in your
body, and in your spirit, which are God’s.
This double attention will give us cause to admire, not the logick
only, but the address, of the learned Apostle. I say, the address;
which the occasion required: for, notwithstanding that no sin is more
opposite to our holy religion, and that therefore St. Paul, in his
epistles to the Gentile converts, gives it no quarter, yet, as became
the wisdom and sanctity of his character, he forgets not of what, and
to whom, he writes.
Let us see, then, how the Apostle acquits himself in these nice
circumstances.
After observing that the sin he had warned the Corinthians to avoid,
was a sin against their own body; that is, was an abuse and
defilement of it, he proceeds, “What! know ye not that your body is
the temple of the Holy Ghost, which is in you, which ye have of God?
And ye are not your own; for ye are bought with a price; therefore,
glorify God in your body, and in your spirit, which are God’s.”
I. First, then, the Apostle asks, What! know ye not that your body is
the temple of the Holy Ghost?—This question refers to that great
Christian principle, that we live in the communion of the Holy
Ghost171; not, in the sense in which we all live and move and have
our being in God; but in a special and more exalted sense; the
Gospel teaching, that God hath given to us Christians the Holy
Spirit172, to be with us, and in us; to purify and comfort us: that we
are baptized by this spirit173, sanctified, sealed by it to the day of
redemption174.
Now this being the case, the body of a Christian, which the Holy
Ghost inhabits and sanctifies by his presence, is no longer to be
considered as a worthless fabrick, to be put to sordid uses, but as
the receptacle of God’s spirit, as the place of his residence; in a
word, as his TEMPLE and sanctuary.
The figure, you see, presents an idea the most august and
venerable. It carried this impression with it both to the Gentile and
Jewish Christians. It did so to the Gentiles, whose superstitious
reverence for their idol-temples is well known: and though many an
abominable rite was done in them, yet the nature of the Deity,
occupying this temple, which was the Holy Ghost, put an infinite
difference between him and their impure deities, the impurest of
which had engrossed the Corinthian worship. So that this contrast of
the object could not but raise their ideas, and impress the reverence,
which the Apostle would excite in them for such a temple, with full
effect on their minds175. And then to Jews, the allusion must be
singularly striking: for their supreme pride and boast was, the
temple at Jerusalem, the tabernacle of the most high, dwelling
between the cherubims, and the place of the habitation of God’s
glory176.
To both Jew and Gentile, the notion of a temple implied these two
things, 1. That the divinity was in a more especial manner present in
it: and, 2. That it was a place peculiarly set apart for his service.
Whence the effect of this representation would be, That the body,
having the Holy Spirit lodged within it, was to be kept pure and
clean for this cælestial inhabitant: and, as being dedicated to his
own use, it was not to be prophaned by any indecencies, much less
by a gross sin, which is, emphatically, a sin against the body, and by
heathens themselves accounted a pollution177 of it.
Further; the Apostle does not leave the Corinthians to collect all this
from the image presented to them, but asserts it expressly; What!
know ye not, that your body is the temple of the Holy Ghost, WHICH
IS IN YOU: Implying, that what they would naturally infer from their
idea of a temple, was true, in fact, that the Holy Ghost was in them;
that his actual occupancy and possession of their bodies
appropriated the use of them to himself, and excluded all sordid
practices in them, as prophane and SACRILEGIOUS. Nay, he further
adds; AND WHICH [Holy Ghost] YE HAVE OF GOD: ye have received this
adorable spirit, which is in you, from God himself; and so are obliged
to entertain this heavenly guest with all sanctity and reverence; not
only for his own sake, and for the honour he does you in dwelling in
you, but for his sake who sent him, and from whose hands ye have
received him.
The expression is, again, figurative; and refers to the notions and
usages that obtained among the heathens, the Greeks especially, in
regard to personal slavery. As passionate admirers, as they were, of
liberty, every government, even the most republican, abounded in
slaves; every family had its share of them. The purchase of them, as
of brute beasts, was a considerable part of their traffick. Men and
women were bought and sold publicly in their markets: the wealth of
states and of individuals, in great measure, consisted in them. Thus
was human nature degraded by the Heathen, and I wish it might be
said, by heathens only. But my present concern is with them. It is
too sad a truth that human creatures sold themselves, or were sold
by their masters, to be employed in the basest services, even those
of luxury and of lust. This infamous practice was common through
all Greece, but was more especially a chief branch of the Corinthian
commerce. Their city was the head-quarters of prostitution, and the
great market for the supply of it.
“To turn now, says the Apostle, from these horrors to a fairer scene;
for I take advantage only of your ideas in this matter, to lead you to
just notions of your present Christian condition. God, the sole
rightful proprietor of the persons of men, left you in the state of
nature, to the enjoyment of your own liberty, with no other restraint
upon it than what was necessary to preserve so great a blessing, the
restraint of reason. Now, indeed, but still for your own infinite
benefit, he claims a stricter property in you, and demands your more
peculiar service. He first made you men, but now Christians. Still he
condescends to proceed with you in your own way, and according to
your own ideas of right and justice. He has bought you with a price:
but, merciful heaven, with what price? With that, which exceeds all
value and estimation, with the BLOOD of his only begotten Son; the
least drop of which is of more virtue than all your hecatombs, and
more precious than the treasures of the East. And for what was this
price paid? Not to enslave, much less to insult and corrupt you (as
ye wickedly served one another), but to redeem you into the
glorious liberty of the sons of God: It was, to restore you from death
to life, from servitude to freedom, from corruption to holiness, to
make to himself a peculiar people, zealous of good works. Say, then,
Is this ransom an equivalent for the purchase of you? And is the end
for which ye are purchased, such as ye dare complain of, or have
reason to refuse? Henceforth, then, ye are not your own: the
property of your souls and bodies is freely, justly, equitably, with
immense benefit to yourselves, and unspeakable mercy on the part
of the purchaser, transferred to God. Your whole and best service is
due to him, of strict right: what he demands of you is to serve him
in all virtue and godliness of living, and particularly to respect and
reverence yourselves; in a word, not to pollute yourselves with
forbidden lusts. In this way ye are required to serve your new lord
and master, who has the goodness to regard such service, as an
honour and glory to himself. Therefore, do your part inviolably and
conscientiously, Glorify God in your body, and in your spirit, which
are God’s.”
This is the the Apostle’s idea, when drawn out and explained at
large. The reasoning is decisive, as in the former case: and the
expression admirably adapted to the circumstances of the persons
addressed. In plain words, the argument is this. God has provided,
by the sacrifice of the death of Christ, for your redemption from all
iniquity, both the service, and the wages of it. By your profession of
Christianity, and free acceptance of this inestimable benefit, freely
offered to you, ye are become in a more especial manner, his
servants: ye are bound, therefore, by every motive of duty and self-
interest to preserve yourselves in all that purity of mind and body,
which his laws require of you; and for the sake of which ye were
taken into this nearer relation to himself. The figure of being bought
with a price, was at once the most natural cover of this reasoning,
as addressed to the Corinthian Christians; and the most poignant
reproof of their country’s inhuman practice of trafficking in the
bodies and souls of men.
The force both of the figure and the reasoning is apparently much
weakened by this minute comment upon the Apostle’s words, which
yet seemed necessary to make them understood.
To draw to a point, then, the substance of what has been said, and
to conclude.
The vice which the Apostle had been arguing against, is condemned
by natural reason. But Christians are bound by additional and
peculiar considerations to abstain from it. Ye, says the Apostle, ARE
THE TEMPLES OF THE HOLY GHOST. To defile yourselves with the sins of
uncleanness is, then, to desecrate those bodies which the Holy
Ghost sanctifies by his presence. It is, in the emphatic language of
scripture, to grieve the holy Spirit, and to do despite to the spirit of
grace. It is like, nay it is infinitely worse, than polluting the
sanctuary: an abomination, which nature itself teaches all men to
avoid and execrate. It is, in the highest sense of the words,
PROPHANENESS, IMPIETY, SACRILEGE.
Again; YE ARE BOUGHT WITH A PRICE: ye are not your own, but God’s;
having been ransomed by him, your souls and bodies, when both
were lost, through the death of his Son: a price, of so immense, so
inestimable a value, that worlds are not equal to it. To dispose of
yourselves, then, in a way which he forbids and abhors: to corrupt
by your impurities that which belongs to God, which is his right and
property; to serve your lusts, when ye are redeemed at such a price
to serve God only, through Jesus Christ; is an outrage which we
poorly express, when language affords no other names for it, than
those of INGRATITUDE, INFIDELITY, INJUSTICE.
Now, it may be said of us, that we are made to POSSESS these sins,
“When we continue under the constant sense and unrepented guilt
of them:” “When we labour under tyrannous habits, which they have
produced:” And, “when we groan under afflictions of various kinds,
which they have entailed upon us.”
In these three respects, I mean to shew how bitter those things are,
which God writeth, that is, decreeth in his justice, against the
iniquities of our youth.
When the young mind has been tinctured in any degree with the
principles of modesty and virtue, it is with reluctance and much
apprehension, that it first ventures on the transgression of known
duty. But the vivacity and thoughtless gaiety of that early season,
encouraged by the hopes of new pleasure, and sollicited, as it
commonly happens, by ill examples, is at length tempted to make
the fatal experiment; by which guilt is contracted, and the sting of
guilt first known. The ingenuous mind reflects with shame and
compunction on this miscarriage but the passion revives; the
temptation returns, and prevails a second time, and a third; still with
growing guilt, but unhappily with something less horror; yet enough
to admonish the offender of his fault, and to embitter his
enjoyments.
In this delirious state he continues for some time. But presently the
scene changes. Although the habit continue, the enjoyment is not
the same: the keenness of appetite abates, and the cares of life
succeed to this run of pleasure.
But neither the cares nor the pleasures of life can now keep him
from reflexion. He cannot help giving way, at times, to a serious turn
of thought; and some unwelcome event or other will strike in to
promote it. Either the loss of a friend makes him grave; or a fit of
illness sinks his spirits; or it may be sufficient, that the companions
of his idle hours are withdrawn, and that he is left to himself in
longer intervals than he would chuse, of solitude and recollection.
“She upbraids him, first, with his loss of virtue, and of that which
died with it, her own favour and approbation. She then sets before
him the indignity of having renounced all self-command, and of
having served ingloriously under every idle, every sordid appetite.
She next rises in her remonstrance; represents to him the baseness
of having attempted unsuspecting innocence; the cruelty of having
alarmed, perhaps destroyed, the honour of deserving families; the
fraud, the perfidy, the perjury, he has possibly committed in carrying
on his iniquitous purposes. The mischiefs he has done to others are
perhaps not to be repaired; and his own personal crimes remain to
be accounted for; and, if at all, can only be expiated by the bitterest
repentance. And what then, concludes this severe monitor in the
awful words of the Apostle, What fruit had ye then in those things
whereof ye are now ashamed? for the end of those things is
death179.”
Suppose now this remonstrance to take effect, and that the sinner is
at length (for what I have here represented in few words, takes
much time in doing; but suppose, I say, that the sinner is at length)
wrought upon by this remonstrance to entertain some serious
thoughts of amendment, still the consciousness of his ill desert will
attend him through every stage of life, and corrupt the sincerity of
all his enjoyments; while he knows not what will be the issue of his
crimes, or whether, indeed, he shall ever be able truly and
effectually to repent of them. For we cannot get quit of our sins, the
moment we resolve to do so: But, as I proposed to shew,
II. In the second place, we are still made to possess the iniquities of
our youth, while we labour under any remains of those tyrannous
habits, which they have produced in us.
Thus he struggles with himself, perhaps for many years, perhaps for
a great part of his life; and in all that time is distracted by the very
inconsistency of his own conduct, and tortured by the bitterest pains
of compunction and self-abhorrence.
But let it be supposed, that the grace of God at length prevails over
the tyranny of his inveterate habits; that his repentance is
efficacious, and his virtue established. Yet the memory of his former
weakness fills him with fears and apprehensions: he finds his mind
weakened, as well as polluted, by his past sins; he has to strive
against the returning influence of them; and thus, when penitence
and tears have washed away his guilt, he still thinks himself
insecure, and trembles at the possible danger of being involved
again in it.
Add to all this, the compunction which such a man feels, when he is
obliged to discountenance in others, perhaps, by his station, to
punish those crimes in which he had so long and so freely indulged
himself: and how uneasy the very discharge of his duty is thus
rendered to him.
To say all upon this head: his acquired habits, if not corrected in due
time, may push him into crimes the most atrocious and shocking;
and, if subdued at length, will agitate his mind with long
dissatisfaction and disquiet. Repentance, if it comes at all, will come
late; and will never reinstate him fully in the serenity and composure
of his lost innocence. But,
III. Lastly, when all this is done (and more to do is not in our power)
we may still possess the iniquities of our youth, in another sense, I
mean, when we groan under the temporal afflictions of many kinds,
which they entail upon us.
When PLEASURE first spreads its share for the young voluptuary, how
little did he suspect the malignity of its nature; and that under so
enchanting an appearance, it was preparing for him pains and
diseases, declining health, an early old-age, perhaps poverty, infamy,
and irreparable ruin? Yet some, or all of these calamities may
oppress him, when the pleasure is renounced, and the sin forsaken.
Youth and health are with difficulty made to comprehend how frail a
machine the human body is, and how easily impaired by excesses.
But effects will follow their causes; and intemperate pleasure is sure
to be succeeded by long pains, for which there is no prevention, and
for the most part, no remedy. Hence it is that life is shortened; and,
while it lasts, is full of languor, disease, and suffering. If by living
fast, as men call it, they only abridged the duration of their
pleasures, their folly might seem tolerable. But the case is much
worse: they treasure up to themselves actual sufferings, from
disorders which have no cure, as well as no name. And not
unfrequently it happens, according to the strong expression in the
book of Job, that a man’s bones are full of the sin of his youth, till
they lie down with him in the grave180.
All this, it may be thought, is very hard. But such is the fact, and
such the order of God’s providence. We have not the making of this
system: it is made to our hands by him who ordereth all things for
the best, how grievous soever his dispensations may sometimes
appear to us. Our duty, and our wisdom is to reflect what that
system is, and to conform ourselves to it.
To conclude: if any thing can rescue unwary youth out of the hands
of their own folly, it must be such a train of reflection as the text
offers to us. Let it sink deep into their minds, that there are indeed
bitter things decreed against the iniquities of that early age; that a
thousand temporal evils spring from that source; that vicious habits
are in themselves vexatious and tormenting; and, that, uncorrected,
and unrepented of, they fill the mind with inutterable remorse and
horror.
When the sins of youth are seen in this light, it is not by giving them
the soft name of infirmities, or by cloathing them with ideas of
pleasure, that we shall be able to reconcile the mind to them. Such
thin disguises will not conceal their true forms and natures from us.
We shall still take them for what indeed they are, for sorcerers and
assassins, the enchanters of our reason and the murderers of our
peace.
Or, if the scorner will not listen to this advice, it only remains to
leave him to his own sad experience; but not till we have made one
charitable effort more to provoke his attention by the caustic
apostrophe of the wise man: Rejoice, O young man, in thy youth,
and let thy heart cheer thee in the days of thy youth, and walk in
the ways of thy heart, and in the sight of thine eyes: but KNOW THOU,
that, for all these things, God will bring thee into judgement185.
SERMON XXVIII.
PREACHED MAY 28, 1769.
I. Take no heed, says the preacher, to all words that are spoken,
LEST THOU HEAR THY SERVANT CURSE THEE. This is the FIRST reason
which he assigns for his advice.
This appears to have been the sentiment of the wise preacher in the
text. Avoid, says he, this impertinent curiosity, lest thou hear thy
servant curse thee; lest the very persons that live under thy roof and
are most obliged to thee, who are reasonably presumed to have the
warmest concern for thy honour and interest, and on whose fidelity
and gratitude the security and comfort of thy whole life more
immediately depends, lest even these be found to make free with
thy character. For there is a time, when even these may be carried
to speak undutifully and disrespectfully of thee.
And would any man wish to make this discovery of those, who are
esteemed to be, and, notwithstanding these occasional freedoms,
perhaps are, his true servants and affectionate friends?
For think not, when this unlucky discovery is made, that the
offended party will treat it with neglect, or be in a condition to
consider it with those allowances, that, in reason and equity, may be
required of him. No such thing: It will appear to him in the light of a
heinous and unpardonable indignity; it will occasion warm
resentments, and not only fill his mind with present disquiet, but
most probably provoke him to severe expostulations; the usual fruit
of which is, to make a deliberate and active enemy of him, who was,
before, only an incautious and indiscreet friend: at the best, it will
engender I know not what uneasy jealousies and black suspicions;
which will mislead his judgment on many occasions; and inspire an
anxious distrust, not of the faulty person himself only, but of others,
who stand in the same relation to him, and, perhaps, of all mankind.
These several ill effects may be supposed, as I said, to flow from the
discovery: and it will be useful to set the malignity of each in its true
and proper light.
II. It now remains that I say one word on the INJUSTICE, and want of
equity, which appears in this practice. For oftentimes also thine own
heart knoweth, that thou thyself, likewise, hast cursed others.
And as in the former case the preacher drew his remonstrance from
his knowledge of the world; so in this, he reasons from his intimate
knowledge of the human heart.
Let the friendliest, the best man living, explore his own conscience,
and then let him tell us, or rather let him tell himself, if he can, that
he has never offended in the instance here given. I suppose, on a
strict inquiry, he will certainly call to mind some peevish sentiment,
some negligent censure, some sharp reflection, which, at times, hath
escaped him, even in regard to his second self, a bosom friend.
Either he took something wrong, and some suspicious circumstance
misled him; or, he was out of health and spirits; or, he was ruffled by
some ungrateful accident; or, he had forgotten himself in an hour of
levity; or a splenetic moment had surprised him. Some or other of
these causes, he will find, had betrayed him into a sudden warmth
and asperity of expression, which he is now ashamed and sorry for,
and hath long since retracted and condemned.
Still further, at the very time when this infirmity overtook him, he
had no purposed unfriendliness, no resolved disaffection towards the
person he allowed himself to be thus free with. His tongue indeed
had offended, but his heart had scarce consented to the offence.
The next day, the next hour, perhaps, he would gladly have done all
service, possibly he would not have declined to hazard his life, for
this abused friend.
Let us then allow for what we cannot well help. And let this
consideration come in aid of the others, employed in the text, to
expell an inveterate folly, which prompts us to lay more stress upon
words, than such frivolous and fugitive things deserve. Let us regard
them, for the most part, but as the shaking of a leaf, or the murmur
of the idle air: they rarely merit our notice, and attention, more: or,
when they do, we should find it better to indulge our charity, than
our curiosity; I mean, to believe well of others, as long as we can,
rather than be at the pains of an anxious inquiry for a pretence to
think ill of them.
Our website is not just a platform for buying books, but a bridge
connecting readers to the timeless values of culture and wisdom. With
an elegant, user-friendly interface and an intelligent search system,
we are committed to providing a quick and convenient shopping
experience. Additionally, our special promotions and home delivery
services ensure that you save time and fully enjoy the joy of reading.
ebooknice.com