SelfIdentity2011Vess
SelfIdentity2011Vess
net/publication/235560347
CITATIONS READS
227 4,191
5 authors, including:
Tim Wildschut
University of Southampton
182 PUBLICATIONS 13,590 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Tim Wildschut on 20 May 2014.
To cite this article: Matthew Vess, Jamie Arndt, Clay Routledge, Constantine Sedikides & Tim
Wildschut (2012): Nostalgia as a Resource for the Self, Self and Identity, 11:3, 273-284
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation
that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any
instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary
sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,
demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or
indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Self and Identity, 11: 273–284, 2012
http://www.psypress.com/sai
ISSN: 1529-8868 print/1529-8876 online
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2010.521452
MATTHEW VESS
Ohio University, Athens, Ohio, USA
JAMIE ARNDT
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
CLAY ROUTLEDGE
North Dakota State University, Fargo, North Dakota, USA
CONSTANTINE SEDIKIDES
TIM WILDSCHUT
University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
This research tested whether nostalgia serves as a positive resource for the self. In
Experiment 1, nostalgia was induced and the accessibility of positive self-attributes
was assessed. Participants who thought about a nostalgic experience, relative to
those who thought about a positive future experience, evidenced heightened
accessibility of positive self-attributes. In Experiment 2, participants received
negative or positive performance feedback and then thought about a nostalgic or
ordinary past experience. Subsequently, they were given the opportunity to make
internal attributions for their performance. Participants displayed a typical pattern
of self-serving attributions if they were not given the opportunity to engage in
nostalgia. Nostalgic engagement, however, attenuated this effect. Nostalgia indeed
functions as a positive resource for the self.
When Rick Blaine (Humphrey Bogart) tells Ilsa Lund (Ingrid Bergman) that they
will ‘‘always have Paris’’ in the movie Casablanca, he showcases the human capacity
to revisit psychologically momentous events in the past. If the movie were to have
followed these characters into the future, it is likely that both would have
experienced periods of sentimental longing for the time they spent in the shadow of
the Eiffel Tower. The purpose of the present research was to explore further the
positive psychological benefits that the experience of sentimentality for the past, or
Received 8 February 2010; accepted 6 August 2010; first published online 15 November 2010.
Correspondence should be addressed to: Matthew Vess, 205 Porter Hall, Department of Psychology,
Ohio University, Athens, OH 45701, USA. E-mail: vessm@ohio.edu
Ó 2012 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
274 M. Vess et al.
nostalgia, confers to the self. Recent perspectives on nostalgia suggest that one key
benefit of nostalgia is its bolstering effect on self-esteem (Wildschut, Sedikides,
Arndt, & Routledge, 2006). The present work builds from this finding to identify two
additional self-relevant functions served by nostalgic reverie. We examine whether
nostalgia increases the cognitive accessibility of positive self-attributes (Experiment
1) and reduces self-serving attributions in response to performance feedback
(Experiment 2).
Early conceptualizations of nostalgia viewed the emotion primarily as a form of
psychological dysfunction (Hofer, 1688/1934) and linked it to negative states such as
depression (McCann, 1941) and homesickness (Davis, 1979). This is perhaps not
surprising, given that many of the ‘‘symptoms’’ thought to be associated with bouts
of nostalgia were negative (e.g., anxiety, sadness). However, evidence that
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 examined the effects of nostalgia on the accessibility of positive self-
attributes. Participants considered either a nostalgic event or a positive event in
their future, and subsequently categorized positive and neutral personality traits as
self-descriptive or not. We used categorization speed as an index of concept
accessibility (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000). Past research has contrasted the effects of
nostalgia with reflections on ordinary events and past autobiographical events, as
in experiments showing that reflections on nostalgic events engender greater
explicit self-esteem than do reflections on ordinary past events (Wildschut et al.,
2006). This research has also often used state nostalgia manipulation checks and
confirmed that the treatment does in fact increase in-the-moment nostalgia
(Wildschut et al., 2006, 2010; Zhou et al., 2008). However, research has yet to
assess whether nostalgic reflection has unique effects relative to the consideration
of a positive event in the future. We remedied this deficiency in Experiment 1.
Moreover, we measured positive and negative affect immediately after the nostalgia
induction to assess whether any observed effects were driven by general affective
consequences of nostalgic reverie. This is a crucial addition to the experimental
procedure, given previous findings that nostalgic reflection improves mood
(Wildschut et al., 2006).
We hypothesized that nostalgic engagement would increase the accessibility of
positive self-attributes relative to the consideration of a future positive event.
Reflecting on a nostalgic event and considering a future positive event may engender
comparable degrees of positivity, but nostalgic reverie has the potential to bring on-
line specific memories where the self is a central player in a positive chain of events
(Wildschut et al., 2006). We expected the activation of these cognitive structures to
276 M. Vess et al.
bring online positive features of the self, thus increasing the speed at which nostalgic
participants categorized positive self-attributes.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Thirty (15 female)1 psychology students (age: M ¼ 18.8, SD ¼ 1.19) participated in a
study on ‘‘personality and categorization speed’’ for course credit. They completed
all materials on computers.
Materials
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Affect. Next, participants completed the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS asks participants to
indicate the extent to which they currently feel a variety of emotions (e.g., ‘‘excited,’’
‘‘scared’’) on a 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely) scale. Both positive
(M ¼ 3.15, SD ¼ 0.85, a ¼ .87) and negative affect (M ¼ 1.60, SD ¼ 0.47, a ¼ .73) are
assessed.
Results
Primary Analyses
‘‘Me’’ categorization. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on
the number of words categorized as ‘‘me’’ revealed no difference between positive
(M ¼ 11.13, SD ¼ 2.08) and neutral (M ¼ 10.07, SD ¼ 3.44) words, F(1, 29) ¼ 2.00,
p ¼ .16, Zp2 ¼ .07. Despite the disproportionate number of neutral traits, participants
still manifested a directional tendency to endorse more strongly positive traits, as
would be expected from past research (Sedikides & Gregg, 2003, 2008). Also,
participants did not differ in the number of positive words, (Mnostalgia ¼ 10.80,
SD ¼ 2.48 vs. Mfuture ¼ 11.47, SD ¼ 1.60); t(28) ¼ 0.87, p ¼ .39, d ¼ 0.32, and neutral
words, (Mnostalgia ¼ 9.73, SD ¼ 3.03 vs. Mfuture ¼ 10.40, SD ¼ 3.88); t(28) ¼ 0.52,
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Ancillary Analyses
A set of analyses focused on affect indicated that participants in the control con-
dition reported more positive affect, (M ¼ 3.45, SD ¼ 0.83 vs. M ¼ 2.85, SD ¼ 0.79);
t(28) ¼ 2.05, p 5 .05, d ¼ 0.74, and marginally more negative affect, (M ¼ 1.76,
SD ¼ 0.54 vs. M ¼ 1.43, SD ¼ 0.33); t(28) ¼ 2.00, p 5 .06, d ¼ 0.73, than participants
in the nostalgia condition. However, controlling for positive and negative affect did
not attenuate the primary results described above, F(1, 25) ¼ 4.10, p ¼ .05, Zp2 ¼ .14.
Furthermore, we conducted analyses controlling for the number of attributes
categorized as ‘‘me.’’ The primary results remained unaltered, F(1, 25) ¼ 4.98,
p 5 .05, Zp2 ¼ .17.
Discussion
Experiment 1 showed that nostalgic reverie amplifies the accessibility of positive self-
attributes. Participants who reflected on a nostalgic (vs. future positive) event were
faster to categorize positive self-attributes, and controlling for individual differences
in categorization speed and affect could not account for this effect. Moreover, the
nostalgic and future positive event conditions did not differ in the overall number of
traits categorized as ‘‘me,’’ helping to rule out an alternative explanation for the
278 M. Vess et al.
observed effects. One might argue that the latency differences emerged due to
participants in the positive event condition categorizing more marginally descriptive
traits as ‘‘me.’’ Such categorization tendencies would consequently take longer to
make, leading to larger response latencies. The data rule out this possibility. Thus,
the results support a previously unrecognized function of nostalgia: the cognitive
activation of positive self-attributes. These results are particularly evocative, given
that they were obtained with a relatively brief inducement of nostalgic engagement
and without specific direction for participants to reflect on positive memories
involving the self.
Experiment 2
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Method
Participants and Procedure
Fifty-six (29 female) psychology students (M ¼ 18.5, SD ¼ 0.91) participated for
course credit in a study ostensibly interested in the ‘‘relationship between personality
and analytical reasoning.’’ We excluded three participants from analyses due to their
suspicions about the legitimacy of the RAT.
Nostalgia and the Self 279
Materials
Feedback manipulation. The experimenter distributed the RAT, and further
described it as an analytic reasoning test that accurately predicts professional and
academic success. The RAT presents participants with sets of three words that are
linked together in some way by a fourth word. The objective is to identify correctly
the linking fourth word for each set. Following previous research (McFarlin &
Blascovich, 1984), participants were randomly assigned to a difficult (negative
feedback) or easy version of the RAT (positive feedback). Participants were allotted
3 minutes to solve 10 RAT items and were then given a scoring key. The scoring key
provided the correct answers to each RAT item and indicated how one’s score
compared to other students’ scores: 0–4 were labeled ‘‘below average for university
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
students,’’ 5–6 were labeled ‘‘average,’’ and 7–10 were labeled ‘‘above-average.’’ Thus,
participants in the difficult RAT condition received negative performance feedback
and participants in the relatively easy condition received neutral to positive
performance feedback.
Affect. Following the RAT, participants completed the PANAS (Watson et al.,
1988) described in Experiment 1 (positive affect, a ¼ .90; negative affect, a ¼ .88).
Event reflection. Participants next completed the event reflection task manipula-
tion. The task was identical to that of Experiment 1, except we used a different
control topic: an ordinary event that occurred during the last week. As noted earlier,
this topic has been used in previous research, which has shown that this
manipulation yields the predicted differences on measures of state nostalgia
(Routledge et al., 2008; Wildschut et al., 2006, 2010; Zhou et al., 2008).
Results
Manipulation Checks
Participants in the difficult RAT condition (M ¼ 1.36, SD ¼ 1.66) solved fewer
problems than those in the easy RAT condition (M ¼ 6.43, SD ¼ 1.83), t(51) ¼ 10.51,
p 5 .001, d ¼ 2.90. These means corresponded to the feedback provided in the RAT
interpretation key (e.g., scores 0–4 were ‘‘below average’’). Moreover, a t-test
comparing the feedback conditions revealed that, as expected, ‘‘failure’’ participants
(M ¼ 1.80, SD ¼ 0.84) reported more negative affect than ‘‘success’’ participants
(M ¼ 1.44, SD ¼ 0.35), t(51) ¼ 2.07, p ¼ .04, d ¼ 0.56. No differences emerged for
positive affect, but the pattern was consistent with expectations: Mfailure ¼ 2.82,
SD ¼ 1.07 versus Msuccess ¼ 3.09, SD ¼ 0.75, t(51) ¼ 1.02, p ¼ .31, d ¼ 0.29. We
conclude that the manipulations were effective.
Primary Analyses
We subjected performance attributions to a 2 (Event Reflection: nostalgia,
ordinary)62 (Feedback: failure, success) ANOVA. There was no main effect of
Event Reflection condition, F(1, 49) ¼ 1.41, p ¼ .24, Zp2 ¼ .03, but the Feedback
280 M. Vess et al.
main effect was significant, F(1, 49) ¼ 61.22, p 5 .001, Zp2 ¼ .56. Participants in the
failure condition (M ¼ 3.29, SD ¼ 1.56) attributed their performance to their ability
less than participants in the success condition (M ¼ 5.64, SD ¼ 0.78). More
importantly, the Event Reflection6Feedback interaction was also significant, F(1,
49) ¼ 12.65, p ¼ .001, Zp2 ¼ .21.
There was a pronounced difference in performance attributions among success
and failure participants who reflected on an ordinary past event, F(1, 49) ¼ 69.18,
p 5 .001, Zp2 ¼ .59. However, within the nostalgia condition, the self-serving
attribution pattern, while still significant, was attenuated, F(1, 49) ¼ 8.56, p ¼ .01,
Zp2 ¼ .15. Indeed, as seen in Table 1, within the failure condition, participants in the
nostalgic event condition attributed their performance more to their ability than
ordinary event participants, F(1, 49) ¼ 10.57, p ¼ .002, Zp2 ¼ .18. In contrast, within
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Discussion
Experiment 2 elucidates an additional way that nostalgia functions as a self resource.
Consistent with previous research (Campbell & Sedikides, 1999; Mezulis et al., 2004),
participants in the failure condition attributed their performance less to their ability
than those in the success condition. However, participants in the failure condition
who engaged in nostalgic reflection attributed their performance more to their ability
than those who considered an ordinary event and nostalgia attenuated self-serving
attributions. These results confirm the guiding hypotheses and indicate that nostalgic
reverie serves as a self-affirming resource that mitigates the deleterious consequences
of self-esteem threat.
General Discussion
Two experiments illustrated how nostalgia serves as a resource for the self. In
Experiment 1, nostalgic reverie led to faster categorizations of positive self-attributes
than did contemplating a future positive event. This finding indicates that nostalgia,
in addition to elevating explicit self-esteem (Wildschut et al., 2006), amplifies the
accessibility of positive self-attributes. In Experiment 2, thinking about a nostalgic,
compared to an ordinary, event reduced self-serving attributions in response to
performance feedback. The use of two different control conditions across the
experiments and the affect measures in Experiment 1 suggest that these results are
not due simply to thinking about general past events, future positive events, or
ego-relevant threats that may provoke destructive responses. Although there might
be affective consequences of not defending the self from evaluative threat,
Experiment 2 suggests that nostalgia may confer self-affirming benefits that serve
to buttress self-integrity and value in the face of negative events.
Future research could further explore the merits of this analysis by examining the
effects of nostalgia on other defensive responses to threats (e.g., social comparison
processes, openness to threatening information) and by further explicating the
dynamics that underlie this process. For example, recent work has demonstrated that
nostalgic reverie elicits a more abstract cognitive mindset (Stephan, Wildschut,
Sedikides, & Robertson, 2010), a mindset also engendered by typical self-affirmation
exercises (Wakslak & Trope, 2009) and associated with less defensive orientations to
self-relevant threats (Freitas, Salovey, & Liberman, 2001). It is possible that this shift
in cognitive abstraction may account for the self-buttressing effects of nostalgia. The
present research provides a foundation for empirical efforts to assess the merits of
this analysis and to harvest the wide range of positive benefits associated with
nostalgia.
The current research also contributes to the understanding of how temporal
thought can be used to maintain positive self-views. For instance, individuals
show a temporal distance bias in their recollection of past success and failure:
they construe negative past events farther away from one’s current self and
construe positive events as relatively recent (Ross & Wilson, 2002). In addition,
individuals will take steps to protect special memories from corruption and decay
(Zauberman, Ratner, & Kyu Kim, 2009). The self-enhancing benefits of nostalgia
are consistent with this literature and point to a new line of research. Nostalgia
may not only influence self-esteem, but self-esteem considerations may also help
to structure the content and temporal priority of the events upon which one
reflects nostalgically. For example, individuals who derive self-worth mostly from
relationships (as opposed to personal achievements) may be particularly likely to
become nostalgic about past interpersonal events (cf. Wildschut et al., 2010). Such
possibilities highlight the integrative potential of nostalgia research with other
central theories of self-esteem (e.g., contingencies of self-worth; Crocker & Wolfe,
2001).
Taken together, the present findings provide further insight into the self-relevant
function of nostalgia. They suggest that reflecting on nostalgic aspects of the past
bolsters the positivity of self-conceptions and prepares individuals to respond less
defensively to the challenges and threats of the present. Thus, the ability to wax
nostalgic about that romantic time in Paris may have served as an ever-present self-
resource for Casablanca’s unforgettable lead duo, Rick and Ilsa.
282 M. Vess et al.
Notes
1. Given that no effects involving gender were observed in either experiment, such effects
will not be discussed further.
2. An outlier (z-residual ¼ 3.0) was excluded from these analyses.
References
Alicke, M., & Sedikides, C. (2009). Self-enhancement and self-protection: What they are and
what they do. European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 1–48.
Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likeableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69,
220–232.
Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is there a universal
positive bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and
cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130,
711–747.
Robinson, M. D. (2007). Lives lived in milliseconds: Using cognitive methods in personality
research. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research
methods in personality psychology (pp. 345–359). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like yesterday: Self-esteem, valence of personal past
experiences, and judgments of subjective distance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 82, 792–803.
Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2008). A blast from the past: The
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 55, 1063–1070.
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. (2006). Nostalgia: Content, triggers,
functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 975–993.
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Cordaro, F. (2010). Nostalgia as a
repository of social connectedness: The role of attachment-related avoidance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 573–586.
Zauberman, G., Ratner, R. K., & Kyu Kim, B. (2009). Memories as assets: Strategic memory
protection in choice over time. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 715–728.
Zhou, X., Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., & Gao, D.-G. (2008). Counteracting loneliness: On the
restorative function of nostalgia. Psychological Science, 19, 1023–1029.
Downloaded by [University of Southampton Highfield] at 06:54 28 June 2012