0% found this document useful (0 votes)
33 views

Meta Intro 141

This document provides an introduction to meta-analysis, including: 1) A brief history of meta-analysis and how it was pioneered in the 1970s as a method to statistically aggregate findings across multiple studies. 2) Definitions of key concepts in meta-analysis such as effect sizes, homogeneity testing, and how it differs from traditional literature reviews by focusing on the direction and magnitude of effects rather than just statistical significance. 3) Guidance on which types of studies are suitable for meta-analysis, including that they must produce quantitative results that can be configured as comparable effect sizes, and discuss the parts of a meta-analysis such as effect sizes representing the dependent variable and study attributes as independent variables

Uploaded by

madihaadnan1
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPT, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
33 views

Meta Intro 141

This document provides an introduction to meta-analysis, including: 1) A brief history of meta-analysis and how it was pioneered in the 1970s as a method to statistically aggregate findings across multiple studies. 2) Definitions of key concepts in meta-analysis such as effect sizes, homogeneity testing, and how it differs from traditional literature reviews by focusing on the direction and magnitude of effects rather than just statistical significance. 3) Guidance on which types of studies are suitable for meta-analysis, including that they must produce quantitative results that can be configured as comparable effect sizes, and discuss the parts of a meta-analysis such as effect sizes representing the dependent variable and study attributes as independent variables

Uploaded by

madihaadnan1
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPT, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 21

Introduction to Meta-Analysis

• a bit of history
• definitions, strengths & weaknesses
• what studies to include ???
• “choosing” vs. “coding & comparing” studies
• what information to code along with each effect size ???
What got all this started?

The two events that seem to have defined &


stimulated meta-analysis in Psychology
• In 1952, Hans J. Eysenck reviewed the available
literature and concluded that there were no favorable
effects of psychotherapy – guess how that went
over…
– 20 additional years of empirical research “failed to
resolve the debate”
• In 1978, Gene V. Glass statistically aggregated the
findings of 375 psychotherapy outcome studies
– Glass (and colleague Smith) concluded that
psychotherapy did indeed work
– Glass called his method “meta-analysis”
The Emergence of Meta-Analysis
The statistical ideas behind meta-analysis predate Glass’s work…
R. A. Fisher (1944)
• “When a number of quite independent tests of significance have been
made, it sometimes happens that although few or none can be
claimed individually as significant, yet the aggregate gives an
impression that the probabilities are on the whole lower than would
often have been obtained by chance”
• Source of the idea of aggregating probability values
W. G. Cochran (1953)
• Discusses a method of averaging means across independent studies
• Laid-out much of the statistical foundation that modern meta-analysis
is built upon (e.g., inverse variance weighting and homogeneity
testing)
The Logic of Meta-Analysis

• Traditional methods of review focus on statistical


significance testing to decide “whether or not” there is an
effect (though we really don’t “believe” in the H0:”)
• Significance testing is not well suited to this task
– highly dependent on sample size
– Most errors are Type II errors (e.g., Butcher’s 59%)
– question of comparability of studies of “same study”
• Meta-analysis changes the focus to the direction and
magnitude of the effects across studies
– Isn’t this what we are interested in anyway?
– Direction and magnitude represented by the effect size
When is meta-analysis applicable?

• Meta-analysis is applicable to collections of research that…


– are empirical, rather than theoretical
– produce quantitative results, rather than qualitative findings (need
means and variances)
– have findings that can be configured in a comparable statistical
form (e.g., as effect sizes, correlation coefficients, odds-ratios,
etc.)
– examine constructs and relationships that are “comparable” given
the question at hand
– Can compute, approximate, or estimate an effect size (ES)
Kinds of Research Amenable to Meta-Analysis

• Central Tendency Research


– prevalence rates & averages
• Between Group Contrasts
– Experimental designs
– Non-experimental & Natural Groups designs
• Within-Groups Contrasts
– Experimental designs
– Non-experimental & Pre-Post designs
• Studies of Statistical Association Between Variables
– measurement research (e.g., reliability & validitty)
– individual differences research
The “Parts” of a meta-analysis

• Each study / analysis is a “case” in the meta analysis


– simple studies will have single analysis giving a single ES
– more complex studies may yield several ESs
• Effect Size (ES) is the “dependent variable” in the meta analysis
– is comparable across studies
– represents the magnitude & direction of the effect of interest
– is independent of sample size
• Other “important” attributes of the study / analysis producing the
effect size are the “independent variables” in the meta analysis
– these have to be coded into the database
What are the strengths of meta-analysis ?
• A disciplined and quantitative approach to combining and
comparing empirical research findings
• Is a non-hierarchical approach – doesn’t favor earlier or later
studies as a “starting place” to which we compare other
studies
• Protects against over-interpreting differences across studies
• Can handle a large numbers of studies (this would
overwhelm traditional approaches to review)
• Allows us to evaluate what attributes of a study are related
to smaller vs. larger effect sizes
• Allows us to better balance concerns about “maximum effect
size” and “maximum representativeness” when designing
studies
• Allows us to plan smarter, more sensitive, and more useful
studies!
What are the weaknesses* of meta-analysis ?
• Requires a huge amount of effort
• “Apples and oranges”; comparability of studies is often in the
“eye of the beholder” (Wilson)
• Most meta-analyses include “blemished” studies
• Various forms of subjectivity…
– What studies to include in the meta analyses
– What study attributes to code
– Coding of those attributes
• Often can’t obtain study results or can’t summarize as effect
sizes
• Analysis of between study differences is fundamentally
correlational

* None of these should impress you!


Which Studies to Include?

A bit of an aside…
• The main meta analytic question used to be…
“What is the size of the effect under study?”
• Leading to the question  “What studies should we include?”
• The answer used to be “all comparable studies”
• You might imagine that answer led to much argument…
• Are studies using… … comparable?
– …different operationalizations / measures of the DV…
– …experimental and non-experimental designs…
– …different populations (or subpopulations)…
– …different  tasks … stimuli … equipment… settings …
The Replication Continuum

Pure Conceptual
Replications Replications
You have to be able to argue that the collection of studies
chosen for meta-analysis examine the same relationship.
This may be at a broad level of abstraction, such as the
relationship between criminal justice interventions and
recidivism or between school-based prevention programs
and problem behavior. Alternatively it may be at a narrow
level of abstraction and represent pure replications.

The closer to pure replications your collection of


studies, the easier it is to argue comparability.
(Lipsey & Wilson, 1993)
Which Studies to Include?

• The main meta analytic question is now more commonly…


“What things influence the size of the effect under study?”
• Leading to the answer  Every study of “the effect”
• Leading to the question  “What attributes should we include?”
• The answer is  “all important attributes”
• Lots of coding, from careful methodological evaluation of each study!!!
This is often the hardest part of the meta analysis!!!!

Said differently…
Meta analyses were primarily used in the past to “combine effect
sizes from comparable studies,” usually to ask if the effect was
“non-zero” (e.g., Glass & Smith).
Meta analyses are primarily used currently to “examine
relationships between how a study is conducted and the effect
sizes obtained from that study.”
So (finally) … Which Studies to Include?
• You must have explicit criteria for what studies you include
• Those criteria must conform to the “standards of your people”
• “Published studies” won’t hack it !
– Because studies retaining Null are less likely to be published,
including only published studies biases effect size estimates
away from 0.
• Potential sources for identification of documents
– computerized bibliographic databases
– authors working in the research domain
– conference programs
– dissertations
– review articles
– hand searching relevant journals
– government reports, bibliographies, clearinghouses
Which Information to Include about each study?
Coding your database, so that you know “all the important stuff” about each study has 4
purposes. The coding will help you ,,,
•identify groups of studies that are “replications”
•compare studies to understand what design elements are related to the size of the effect
found
•Adjust/correct individual effect sizes to give more useful values
•It will give you a better understanding of the research literature than you can possibly
imagine!!!
– Folks who survive the meta-analysis process often say that this was the most valuable
result of their study
– You will see details, similarities, differences, genus & mistakes in a literature that you
thought you knew!!!!

What attributes of the study to code ???


Everything that may be influencing the results & effect size !!!
On the next several pages are summaries of materials we’ve
used in previous classes to characterize and evaluate
research designs – any of the attributes listed could add value
to your meta-analysis.

Put differently…
Everything that can influence study results & statistical
conclusion validity can also influence the effect size found!!!

•All-the-Words page – organizing the design/validity jargon


•Validity Net – organizing the jargon around article critiquing
•Researcher Choices – organizing jargon around designing
studies
•Relationships among the types of research validity
•Variance sources in research designs & procedures
Suggested Data to Code Along with the Effect Size
Suggested Data to Code Along with the Effect Size
Suggested Data to Code Along with the Effect Size
Suggested Data to Code Along with the Effect Size
Measurement Validity
External Validity
Do the measures/data of
Do the who, where, what & our study represent the
when of our study represent characteristics & behaviors
what we intended want to we intended to study?
study?
Internal Validity

Are there confounds or 3rd


variables that interfere with the
characteristic & behavior
relationships we intend to study?

Statistical Conclusion Validity


Do our results represent the relationships between characteristics and
behaviors that we intended to study?
• did we get non-representative results “by chance” ?
• did we get non-representative results because of external, measurement or
internal validity flaws in our study?
Suggested Data to Code Along with the Effect Size

SSTotal = SSIV + SSsubcon+ SSproccon + SSIndif+ SSwcsubinf + SSwcprocinf

Sources of variability…
SSIV  IV 
SSsubcon  subject variable confounds (initial eq problems)
SSproccon  procedural variable confounds (ongoing eq pbms)

SSindif  population individual differences 


SSwcsubinf  within-condition subject variable influences
SSwcprocinf  within-condition procedural variable influences
Suggested Data to Code Along with the Effect Size

1. A label or ID so you can backtrack to the exact analysis from


the exact study – you will be backtracking!!!
2. Sample size for each group *
3. Sample attributes (mean age, proportion female, etc.) #
4. DV construct & specific operationalization / measure #
5. Point in time (after/during TX) when DV was measured #
6. Reliability & validity of DV measure *
7. Standard deviation of DV measure *
8. Type of statistical test used *#
9. Between group or within-group comparison / design #
10.True, quasi-, or non-experimental design #
11.Details about IV manipulation or measurement #
12.External validity elements (pop, setting, task/stimulus) #
13.“Quality” of the study #
– better yet  data about attributes used to eval quality!!!

* Used to “adjust” effect sizes # Used to compare studies

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy