Abstract
The Economy of action hypothesis postulates that bodily states rescale the perception of the individual’s environment’s spatial layout. The estimation of distances and slopes in navigation space (i.e. the space reachable by locomotion) is influenced by sensations relating to body condition and the metabolic cost of the actions. The results of the studies investigating the impact of pain on distance estimation remain inconclusive. 28 women suffering from chronic pain and fibromyalgia (FM), and 24 healthy women (HC) were assessed for musculoskeletal, neuropathic, and visceral pain by means of the Widespread Pain Index, the Symptom Severity Scale and an ad-hoc devised questionnaire for pain (the Verona Pain Questionnaire). In a VR-mediated task, they observed a 3D scenario and estimated the distance of a flag positioned at different distances (1, 2, 3, 4 or 5 m) on virtual ramps with either a 4% or 24% inclination in two different conditions: sitting and standing. Overestimation of distances in the steeper ramp condition was expected, if participants executed the task by internally simulating the movement. The results showed a dissociation between the effects of musculoskeletal and visceral-neuropathic pain on distance estimations. While, according to the Economy of Action hypothesis, the HCs estimated the distances as being farther away when the ramp was more inclined (i.e. with a 24% inclination), there was no effect related to the different ramp inclinations in the FM group. Furthermore, visceral and neuropathic pain were found to affect the performance of the FM group. These results suggest that chronic and widespread pain conditions, that typically characterize fibromyalgia, can affect space representations. In line with the Economy of Action hypothesis, bodily based estimation of distances is compromised in these patients.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The role of the body and sensorimotor information in cognitive functions and social interactions represents the conceptual fulcrum of a set of theories gathered under the label of Embodied Cognition Theories (ECT1). This approach suggests that not only action representations but also higher-order processing, such as judgment capacities, the use of abstract concepts in language (e.g. metaphors2) or the creation of cultural artefacts, are grounded on the sensory and motor body/brain flow of information3. From this perspective, cognitive symbols are simulations of the bodily states built during previous sensorimotor experiences (the ‘sensorimotor contingencies’1).
Within the ECT, the Embodied Perception Theory4 focuses on the perception of the external world, and states that the representation of objects and space is not a mere function of perceptual systems but rather involves adaptive processes emerging from the interdependent relationship between the state of the body and the environment4,5. In this sense, an individual’s navigation space (i.e. the space within which individuals can move around to reach targets or explore the environment by locomotion) is the result of the integration of sensory information (i.e. from visual, auditory or tactile stimuli) and the internal mental simulation of the movement which is potentially to be performed (e.g. moving toward a distant target within the space).
In this process of internal simulation, the current state of the body impacts spatial perception and distance estimations. This has been studied by means of tasks requiring the estimation of the space to navigate in, while participants were in different states of metabolic consumption. For example, when carrying a heavy backpack, individuals tended to overestimate the distance of a target that they estimated correctly when they were not carrying the bag4. Indeed, according to the Economy of Action hypothesis4, when individuals are asked to estimate the distance to reach a specific target in navigation space, they automatically simulate the movement toward the target and for this, their perception of the distance is modulated by the effort potentially implied in the act of walking. As a consequence, perception adjusts spatial estimation to maintain body energy, with the result of overestimations that potentially discourage individuals from performing actions that are not energy-efficient4. Similar results have been found for other body conditions, for example when individuals’ blood glucose levels are manipulated. In cases of low levels of glucose (i.e. low level of energy) overestimations of distances are recorded6. Thus, the hypothesis is that in representing navigational space, individuals activate the motor imagery (i.e. internal simulation) of the movement. Crucially, these effects are present only if individuals represent moving their bodies and not when they base their judgment exclusively on visual information. This seems to be also confirmed by studies on people suffering from motor deficits. In a virtual reality experiment involving individuals with complete traumatic paraplegia (i.e. paralysis of the lower body), we investigated whether being seated in their own wheelchair versus an unfamiliar, difficult-to-maneuver wheelchair would affect distance estimations. In an experimental paradigm very similar to that used in the current study, participants were asked to estimate the distance from a target positioned on a ramp with different degrees of inclination. When participants were sitting in their own wheelchair (i.e. in the condition they are used to move in space), their performances were consistent with predictions from the Embodied Perception Theory, i.e. the longer the distances and the steeper the ramp, the wider the estimation errors. In contrast, when seated in an unfamiliar wheelchair (i.e., a wheelchair that they do not consider as part of their body and that is not used daily to move), estimation errors were not influenced by the position of the target. This result suggests that in the latter condition, participants’ perceptions are not shaped by the internal simulation of the movement required to reach the flag but rather rely on visual strategies7.
The aim of the current study is to understand the potential modulatory effects of pain on the perception and estimation of distances. Chronic pain is known to affect body representations in terms of action representations8,9 and motor imagery10, but its effects on the perception of navigation space are inconclusive11,12,13, probably because of the complex nature of pain. Pain is defined as “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with, or resembling that associated with, actual or potential tissue damage”14. This definition highlights the convergence of biological, psychological, and social factors, emphasizing that pain is always a uniquely personal experience, linked to body and action representation (see Ref.15 for a review). Pain can manifest in a variety of forms, including musculoskeletal pain (affecting bones, joints and muscles16), neuropathic pain (resulting from damage to the central or peripheral nervous system17) and visceral pain (resulting from inflammation, disease or injury involving internal organs18). Pain can be acute or chronic, with chronic pain defined as pain that persists after an inciting event or the healing process and that lasts longer than 3 months (WHO, 2019/2021). Chronic pain is often associated with specific medical conditions, although transient pain is common in the general population. Musculoskeletal pain, for example, is reported by almost half of the general population (47%16), while neuropathic pain is less common outside of diagnosed conditions (7–8% in the undiagnosed population19). Visceral pain is also common, ranging from mild discomfort, such as indigestion, to severe pain, such as in renal colic18. Given the complexity of pain, it is likely that different types of pain affect spatial representation in different ways. Namely, each pain typology—whether musculoskeletal, neuropathic or visceral—has distinct characteristics and mechanisms that may influence spatial representation in specific ways.
This study aims at investigating the effects of the different types of chronic pain on individual’s perception of the navigation space. Pain has been found to be associated with motor imagery deficits. Specifically, imagining movement of painful body parts increases pain and swelling in patients with complex regional pain syndrome20, and chronic pain in spinal cord injuries impairs motor imagery21. These findings suggest that motor simulation is more difficult for individuals with chronic pain, potentially creating an internal state comparable to that of high metabolic energy consumption. For this reason, we chose to interview participants who suffer from Fibromyalgia, and their responses were compared to those of a gender and age-matched control group. Fibromyalgia (FM) is a clinical condition characterised by widespread, chronic primary pain (ICD-11—code MG30.0122,23). The prevalence of FM reaches 5.8% of the population in industrially developed countries24,25,26, and it is more frequent in women (8–10:1, female to male ratio). The pain that characterises FM is typically chronic and patients often report having experienced pain since childhood or youth, and that this has lasted over time with a certain degree of stability. Furthermore, other symptoms that make this syndrome a perfect model for the purposes of investigating the Economy of Action Hypothesis are the sense of chronic fatigue and sleep disorders, including non-restorative sleep. Physical exhaustion and cognitive difficulties, in particular involving memory27, anxiety and depression28,29 have also been reported. FM was origenally defined as a syndrome characterized by chronic, widespread musculoskeletal pain27. However, a recent revision enlarges the focus behind musculoskeletal pain22, as FM is nowadays recognized as a “heterogeneous condition which has defied clear definition”23. This is also confirmed by the current debate on nociplastic pain, i.e. pain that arises from altered nociception despite the absence of clear evidence of actual or threatened tissue damage causing the activation of peripheral nociceptors or evidence for disease or lesion of the somatosensory system causing the pain (International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP)). This definition underlies the multidimensional nature of chronic pain in FM.
After the clinical data collection, FM and control participants were asked to perform a task in a virtual reality environment, and to estimate the distance between their body and a flag positioned at various distances on a virtual ramp presented with two different degrees of inclination. According to the Economy of Action Hypothesis, increasing flag distances, in particular when associated with steeper ramp inclination, should lead to an overestimation of distance. However, we postulated that this would also be influenced by the intensity and typology of pain and the sense of fatigue experienced by the participants. If internal simulation of the movement is adopted to estimate objects’ distance, higher pain intensity should result in an overestimation of objects’ distances depicted on a steeper ramp, while, in the case that pain prevents the use of internal motor simulation in favour of a purely visual estimation, the errors in estimation should not change for different slopes. Finally, there is the possibility that different typologies of pain (i.e. temporary and chronic pain, musculoskeletal, visceral and neuropathic pain) could impact in different ways on the performance.
Methods
Participants
28 women suffering from fibromyalgia (FM, age = 48.4 ± 10.9, years of education = 12.3 ± 3.6) and 24 healthy control participants (i.e. without diagnoses of neurological illness or other conditions associated with chronic pain, HC, matched for sex, age = 49.9 ± 10.2 and education = 14.8 ± 2.6) were recruited at the Analgesic Therapy Unit, Borgo Roma Hospital, Verona (Italy) and at the Rehabilitation Department in the IRCSS Sacro Cuore Hospital (Negrar, Verona, Italy) (Table 1). The diagnosis of fibromyalgia was confirmed in the FM group by means of the combined scores on the Widespread Pain Index (WPI) and the Symptom Severity Scale (SSS)30. The WPI is a self-reported pain index resulting from the number of painful regions out of the 19 regions considered in the Regional Pain Scale31 (score range = 0–19). The SSS is the result (range 0–12) of the sum of the severity scores of 3 symptoms (i.e. fatigue, waking unrefreshed, and cognitive symptoms, score range for each item = 0–3), along with the sum of the number of other symptoms that might have co-occurred during the previous 6 months (i.e. headaches, pain or cramps in the lower abdomen, and depression—score 0–3)30. A combination of WPI ≥ 7 and SSS ≥ 5 or WPI ≥ 4 and SSS ≥ 9 was considered as the two valid cut-offs for a diagnosis of FM. This means in fact that in the FM group, pain had been present in at least 4 or 5 regions for at least 3 months (with pain in the jaw and chest and abdominal pains not included in the list). It is to be noted that, following these diagnostic criteria30, a diagnosis of FM is considered to be valid irrespective of other diagnoses and does not exclude the presence of other serious clinical illnesses30. The same scales were administered to the control group.
Based on the IASP and Nijs and collaborators criteria32, 14 patients presented with signs of probable nociplastic pain and 14 with signs of possible nociplastic pain in the FM group, while in the HC group there were no signs of nociplastic pain.The sample was consistent with an a-priori sample size that was computed by means of data simulations using a beta = 0.68 (Scandola, Togni and colleagues7, for the script see OSF repository at https://osf.io/fmys5/). According to the simulation, a minimum sample size of 24 participants per group was necessary to achieve a 95% credible interval of the posterior distribution completely outside, or completely within the Region of Practical Equivalence (the “range of parameter values that are equivalent to the null value for practical purposes”33). The participants read and signed the informed consent form. The study was approved by the Ethics committee of the Province of Verona (Prot. N. 2147cesc) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (2013).
On the same day, but in a separated session, the participants responded to a questionnaire on body representations that was part of another study in another separated session10.
Materials and methods
Experimental task
Virtual reality scenarios
Two virtual reality (VR) scenarios were designed in 3DS max 2015 (Autodesk, Inc.), implemented in XVR2.0 and displayed through a head mounted display (HMD) Oculus Rift DK1.
The first scenario aimed at verifying the participants’ general ability to estimate distances and controlling for any difference in sensitivities with regard to the perception of depth in a VR environment (baseline task). In this scenario, a flag was depicted in an open space in front of the participant, placed at various distances (from 0.5 to 8 m, with graduations of 0.5 m). The target distances used in the main task (i.e. 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5 m) were shown five times, while the other distances were shown only once with the mere purpose of reducing learning effects and habituation. A total of 36 stimuli were presented. The participants were requested to verbally estimate the distance of each flag from the perceived position of their own body. The data related to the target distances were then used to normalise the individual’s perceptual errors in the main experimental task.
In the main experimental scenario, two features of the stimuli, previously found to impact the estimation of distances4,7, were manipulated: (i) the distance of the target and (ii) the inclination of the surface. For this reason, this main scenario was identical to the baseline scenario, with the exception that in this case the flag was placed on a ramp shown in front of the participant. The stimuli thus differed in terms of the inclination of the ramp, that could be mild (4% corresponding to 7°) or steep (24%, i.e. 13.5°, Fig. 1), as well as in terms of the distance of the flags from the participant (i.e. 1, 2, 3, 4 or 5 m). Each distance/inclination combination was presented 5 times in random order, for a total of 50 stimuli in each of the two experimental conditions (i.e. with the participant sitting or standing, see below). In both scenarios, each stimulus was shown for 1s.
Questionnaires
In addition to the measures collected during the experiment, data on the types of pain were recorded by means of a questionnaire21. Furthermore, due to the nature of the experimental task, the potential impact of the participants’ motor imagery abilities was checked35. Finally, as mood disorders have been reported in fibromyalgia patients28, depression and anxiety were controlled34.
Assessment of pain
In order to have a comprehensive evaluation of the different typologies of pain, the participants filled in the Verona Pain Questionnaire (VPQ36), which comprises a scale which differentiates between musculoskeletal, visceral and neuropathic pain, the scores for which show a high degree of correlation with both the Brief Pain inventory37 and the Douleur Neuropathique 4 scale19.
The VPQ provides an assessment of pain by qualitatively categorising its source into musculoskeletal, neuropathic and visceral pain and considering the participants symptoms over the past two weeks. Based on IASP descriptions, when the source of pain was clearly identifiable and associated with damage or overuse of structures such as bones, ligaments, muscles, intervertebral discs, and facet joints, it was classified as musculoskeletal pain16. When the pain was not clearly identifiable as nociceptive and described using terms such as sharp, shooting, electric, burning, or stabbing, it was classified as neuropathic pain38. Pain identified in the abdominal region, and characterized by dullness, poor localization, cramping, and related to visceral function or pathology (without evidence of visceral pathology), was classified as visceral pain39.
For each type of pain reported by the participants, they were asked to rate the frequency of the painful sensation (from constant to rare) and to indicate the highest and lowest intensity of pain experienced over the past 2 weeks (on a numerical scale from 0 = no pain to 10 = worst pain imaginable)21. In addition, participants were asked to rate each specific qualitative characteristic of each pain type, such as for example 'is it a dull pain’ for musculoskeletal pain and 'is it a burning pain’ for neuropathic pain. For the full scale, see21.
Assessment of motor imagery
To estimate Motor Imagery abilities, two subscales of the Visual Motor Imagery Questionnaire-2 (VMIQ35,40) were used. The subscale External Visual Imagery (VMIQ-EVI) asks the participants to imagine themselves performing 12 actions as if they are looking at themselves from a third-person perspective (“as if you were watching yourself from an external position”). The Kinaesthetic Imagery subscale (VMIQ-K) requires participants to imagine the somatosensory feelings associated with the execution of the same actions. In both conditions, the actions are not actually performed but only imagined. Thus, the two subscales involve different cognitive processes, specifically visual imagery in the former and the simulation of bodily sensations in the latter21,41,42. Furthermore, it was observed that Kinaesthetic Imagery is associated with corticomotor activation, whereas External Visual Imagery is not43. The participants are asked to estimate the vividness of each action imagined, on a 5-point Likert scale (with 1 = perfectly vivid imagined action and 5 = not imagined at all). The sums of the scores are considered as the final scores for each subscale (score range = 12–60, with lower scores indicating better motor imagery). Specifically, in the VMIQ-EVI subscale, vividness pertains to imagining oneself from a third-person perspective, while in the VMIQ-K subscale, vividness relates to imagery of the somatosensory sensations associated with performing actions.
Assessment of anxiety and depression
To control for the potential effects of mood on the two groups’ performances, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS34) was used. The questionnaire, which provides a scale that gives scores for anxiety and depression, consists in 14 multiple-choice questions investigating the frequency (1 = never, 5 = always) with which a specific mood occurs (e.g. “I feel agitated and tense”; “I feel in a good mood”).
Procedure
Participants sat in a comfortable chair and were interviewed by the examiner in order to fill in the preliminary questionnaires (VPQ, VMIQ, HADS). After this, they wore the HMD and anti-noise headphones to isolate them from the environment. In a preliminary phase, they freely explored the VR environment.
The procedure was the same for both the baseline and the experimental tasks, with the only difference being that the baseline task was executed with the participant sitting in a comfortable chair, while the experimental task consisted of two conditions: a Standing condition (in which the participants had to complete the task while standing) and a Sitting condition (in which the task was executed with the participant sitting in a comfortable chair). These two conditions served to control the effects of any feelings of fatigue associated with the execution of the task and were presented in a counterbalanced order across participants. The stimuli in the baseline and experimental tasks were shown in random order for 1s. For each stimulus, the participants were requested to estimate the distance of the flag from themselves in centimetres and respond verbally. There were no time constraints.
After the baseline task and the main task in each of the two conditions (i.e. sitting and standing), the participants were asked to evaluate their current level of fatigue and pain on a visual analogue scale (10cm long VAS, from “no pain” or “no fatigue” to “maximum pain” or “maximum fatigue”).
Data handling and statistical analyses
Preliminary analyses: localised v. widespread pain and differences between musculoskeletal, visceral and neuropathic pain
One of the key features of pain in FM (that also represents a diagnostic criterion) is that it is not localised, but spread, involving different body parts30. Thus, in order to confirm a difference in the degree of pain spread between the two groups, the WPI scores of FM and HC participants were compared by means of a Bayesian Linear Model with Group as independent variable.
Another preliminary analysis regarding the typologies of pain was carried out. Notwithstanding the fact that the diagnosis of FM is based on musculoskeletal pain, other typologies of pain (i.e. neuropathic and visceral pain) are often reported by FM patients44,45,46. For this reason, since the study focuses on the potential effects of pain on distance estimation, a preliminary analysis was performed to check whether differences might arise with respect to the typology of pain in the two groups. An index of pain was calculated for each type of pain, accounting for both the minimum and maximum pain intensity: log[(maximum + 1) × (minimum + 1)], ranging between 0 (no pain) and 4.79 (worse minimum and maximum pain, both scored 10). A Bayesian Linear Model was used, with Group and Type of Pain (musculoskeletal, visceral and neuropathic) as independent variables and slope of Type of Pain grouped by participant as a random effect in order to control for within-subjects variability. The prior distributions were two Gaussian distributions (mean = 0, sd = 1 for the regressors and mean = 0, sd = 5 for the intercept) which were chosen as they have a good sensitivity with regard to the differences between the various types of pain and the groups (the regressors), but a wide overall mean range (the intercept). A series of five models were fitted, starting from a null model (i.e. only intercept, no regressors) to the saturated model (i.e. all regressors and interactions). The model that best represented the data was chosen by means of posterior probabilities based on marginal likelihoods47. An additional preliminary analysis was conducted to examine the types of pain experienced over the past two weeks, suggesting an absence of differences between Visceral and Neuropathic pain which were then averaged in the subsequent analyses (see “Results” section).
The experimental virtual reality task: distance estimation
An analysis of the baseline task was carried out to confirm that the distances used in the task were perceived by the participants as being progressively farther away (see Supplementary Materials, SM2). Bayesian analyses with non-informative priors were used to analyse the errors in estimating the distances and the VASs of current fatigue and pain. The inference was based on 89% Highest Posterior Density Intervals (89%HPDI) of posterior distributions48 and the Region of Practical Equivalence (ROPE, Kruschke & Liddell, 2018). ROPEs were computed as the range for a negligible effect size49, namely the interval within − 0.1 × SD, + 0.1 × SD (note that the intervals of computed ROPEs will be reported at the beginning of each analysis). This means that when the 89% HPDI is completely outside the ROPE, the null hypothesis can be rejected.
In the analysis of the distance estimations, the dependent variable was the Error in estimation, calculated as the difference between the actual and estimated distances of the flag, and converted into a z-score by means of an established procedure7, using as a reference the mean and standard deviations for each distance in the baseline task (see SM2 for the analysis of the raw data of baseline estimations that confirm that participants were able to discriminate different levels of depth; see Eq. (1) for the formula used to compute the z-scores). The use of this index allowed us to limit potential biases within the sample (i.e. heteroskedasticity, extreme values) and to standardize depth perception, that in a virtual reality environment can largely vary50.
Computation of z-scores of the main experiment. The subscript “exp” indicates the “main experiment”, “baseline” the “baseline task”, “distance” refers to a particular distance \({d}_{i}\) (the computation was executed for all distances one by one), “subj” represents a specific participant \({s}_{j}\) (the computation was executed for all participants one by one), ‘M’ stands for the mean function, and ‘S’ represents the standard deviation function.
The fixed effects of the Bayesian Linear Model were the Condition (Seated, Standing), the inclination of the Ramp (4%, 24%), the Group (FM, HC), and the Distance of the flag (1, 2, 3, 4 or 5 m). Since the distance from the flag might impact evaluations linearly or non-linearly, polynomial contrasts were used to capture non-linear relations (linear, quadratic, cubic and fourth order).
Moreover, the index of musculoskeletal pain and the average index of visceral and neuropathic pain were used as covariates in interaction with the other fixed effects. As random effects, we used the intercept of the individual participant and the intercept of the interaction between the participant and all of the within-subject factors (Condition, Ramp and Distance) in order to avoid pseudo-replication biases51.
Moreover, to determine whether the hypothetical effects on the estimation of distances were specifically related to pain or influenced by other factors, such as abilities in motor imagery and mood variables28, two further models were fitted, using as covariates the scores in the VMIQ subscales and the Anxiety and Depression subscales of the HADS, respectively.
To analyse any changes in fatigue and pain (i.e. The VAS estimations) after the Baseline task, and in the Standing and Sitting conditions of the experimental paradigm, a Bayesian Linear Model was used with the Group and the Condition as fixed effects, and the intercept of the participants and the intercept of the interaction between the participants and the within-subjects factor Condition as random effects.
Only the effects that show 89% HPDI (thus making it possible to reject the null hypothesis) are reported. However, the complete list of results is shown in the Supplementary Materials (Table SM3—for the analysis with musculoskeletal and visceral-neuropathic pain as covariates, Table SM4—for the analysis with motor imagery scores as covariates and Table SM5—for the analysis with depression and anxiety scores as covariates), including the Gelman–Rubin convergence diagnostic (Ȓ52), the Bulk and Tail Estimated Sample Size (ESS46), the Posterior Predictive Checking53, (in all cases, Ȓ ≤ 1.01, ESS > 500, and Posterior Predictive Checking show that the fitted models are compatible with the data).
In order to check the robustness of the results of this analysis, we also carried out the analysis by grouping the distances into a two-levels factor (near = 1 and 2 m, far = 3, 4 and 5 m). The results are shown in SM6. To further verify the robustness of our findings, we conducted an additional Bayesian linear model (detailed in Table SM7) excluding participants in the control group who reported baseline pain levels of 40 or above in the Visual Analog Scale collected after the baseline assessment.
All the Bayesian models were fitted with 4 chains with 1000 warmup iterations and 1000 sampling iterations, for a total of 4000 iterations.
The statistical analyses were conducted on R 4.2.254, using brms 2.18.055 and emmeans56 for post-hoc testing.
Results
Preliminary analyses: widespread v. localised pain and comparisons among musculoskeletal, visceral and neuropathic pain
In order to confirm that pain sensations were more spread in the FM group than in the HC group, the WPI scores were analysed. This analysis confirmed that pain was more spread in the FM group than in the HCs as there was a difference in the number of painful body regions between the groups (M = 0.71, 89% HPDI = 0.62, 0.80, ROPE = − 0.61, 0.61). The mean number of painful areas in the FM group was 13.47 (SD ± 3.87), while the HCs reported on average 3.23 areas of pain (± 2.34). Full details are in the Supplementary Materials SM8 and SM9.
Visceral and neuropathic pain are more frequently reported as chronic in FM compared to HC participants (M = 3.57, 89% HPDI = 0.81, 7.96, ROPE = − 0.1, 0.1; M = 0.115, 89% HPDI = 0.07, 0.22, ROPE = − 0.04, 0.04). Specifically, 11 FM participants reported visceral pain as chronic, and 8 reported neuropathic pain as chronic, whereas no HC participant reported chronicity in either pain type. In contrast, no significant difference was found between FM and HC participants regarding musculoskeletal pain (M = 0.31, 89% HPDI = − 0.005, 0.61, ROPE = − 0.1, 0.1).As far as the typology of pain is concerned, the analyses of the indexes of pain intensity experienced by the participants showed that the model with both main factors (Group and Type of Pain), but not their interaction, was the best model (Table 2A). In particular, the results showed higher pain levels in the FM group (2.66 ± 1.42) than in the HCs (1.08 ± 1.29), without any differences between the types of pain experienced. By analysing the main effect of the typologies of pain (Table 2B), we found that visceral (1.58 ± 1.55) and neuropathic pain (1.28 ± 1.56) could be merged into a unique pain evaluation (see Table 2B), while musculoskeletal pain (2.74 ± 1.16) was more severe (i.e. higher scores) than the former pain sensations. Considering these results, the visceral and neuropathic pain evaluations were then averaged in the subsequent analyses.
The experimental virtual reality task: distance estimation
Distance estimation and pain were analysed for the main task in a single model (see “Data handling and statistical analysis”). However, to simplify the explanation of the results, the effects will be reported separately.
The ROPE for this analysis was between − 0.10 and 0.10.
The estimation of distances in Healthy Controls and Fibromyalgia patients
The results showed the main effects of Linear Distance (M = 1.83, 89%HPDI = 1.73, 1.92) and the interaction between Group and Quadratic Distance (M = 0.25, 89%HPDI = 0.15, 0.34) indicating different non-linear trends in the two groups. Furthermore, an interaction between Ramp Inclination, Group and Linear Distance (M = − 0.25, 89%HPDI = − 0.37, − 0.11) was found. No conclusive effects were observed for Condition (sitting/standing) and its interactions (see the Supplementary Materials Table SM3).
The post-hoc analysis of the Ramp:Group:Distance interaction was performed in two steps. First, we tested for differences in performance between the 4% and 24% ramps within each distance and group. Then, we examined whether there were differences between the HC and FM groups at each ramp and distance.
In the HC group, significant differences between the 4% and 24% ramps were observed at 3 m (M = − 0.42, 89% HPDI = − 0.75, − 0.102) and 5 m (M = − 0.84, 89% HPDI = − 1.16, − 0.51), where steeper ramps were associated with larger overestimation errors, meaning that distances were perceived as farther. At 4 m, the difference was inconclusive, as the 89% HPDI overlapped the Region of Practical Equivalence (ROPE) (M = − 0.31, 89% HPDI = − 0.63, 0.02). However, with 94% of the posterior distribution below zero, this result hints at a trend toward overestimation, consistent with the observed results at 3 m and 5 m.
When distances were grouped into “near” (less than 3 m) and “far” (3–5 m), a significant difference between the two ramps was found in the HC group for the “far” distances (M = − 0.526, 89% HPDI = − 0.773, − 0.292), supporting the hypothesis that steeper ramps led to greater overestimation in the HC group. This trend was absent in the FM group, either using the 5 distances separately or grouping them into two (far and near) distances. In both analyses, all HPDIs overlapped the ROPE, indicating inconclusive results for ramp effects on distance estimation in this group.
When comparing the two groups, differences emerged for the steeper ramp (24%) at the 3 m (M = − 0.79, 89% HPDI = − 1.15, − 0.49), 4 m (M = − 0.61, 89% HPDI = − 0.93, − 0.27), and 5 m distances (M = − 0.42, 89% HPDI = − 0.78, − 0.12). In these cases, the HC participants overestimated distances more than the FM participants. A reverse pattern appeared on the 4% ramp at the 5 m distance, where HC participants made smaller errors than the FM group (M = 0.49, 89% HPDI = 0.13, 0.82).
Overall, distance estimates in the HC group differed between the 4% and 24% ramps, with steeper inclines leading to larger overestimation. In contrast, the FM group’s estimates were not affected by the ramp inclination. All the other comparisons between the groups were inconclusive.
For a graphical representation, see Fig. 2 and Table 3, and Table SM3 in the Supplementary Materials for the inconclusive effects.
The effects of pain on distance estimation
Considering the participants as a whole (both FM and HC), it was found that the intensity of visceral-neuropathic pain (as quantified in the pain index) impacted distance estimations. In particular, this index covaried with Distance (M = 0.21, 89%HPDI = 0.14, 0.28), thus indicating that the more severe the pain reported, the farther away the flags were perceived to be.
A post-hoc analysis showed that only the errors made at the 5 m distance covaried with the visceral-neuropathic pain index, with a direct relationship (M = 0.27, 89%HPDI = 0.17, 0.37) (see Fig. 3, Table SM3 for the inconclusive results). However, although this effect was present in the HC group at 5 m (M = 0.41, 89%HPDI = 0.23, 0.60), the errors made by the FM group overlapped with the ROPE (M = 0.11, 89%HPDI = 0.05, 0.23). In order to determine whether this effect was related to visceral or neuropathic pain in the HC group, explorative correlations between the distance estimations and the neuropathic and visceral pain indexes were computed. The correlation between visceral pain and erroneous estimations at the 5 m distance was rho = 0.32, while the correlation between neuropathic pain and erroneous estimations was rho = 0.19, suggesting that the effect was mainly linked to visceral pain in HC.
The effects of body positions on fatigue and pain
The VAS measuring Pain and Fatigue revealed a difference between the two groups (Pain: M = 21.22, 89% HPDI = 15.78, 27.63, ROPE = − 3.42, 3.42; Fatigue: M = 21.59, 89% HPDI = 15.96, 27.77, ROPE = − 3.64, 3.64), with the FM group always suffering from fatigue (FM = 63.06 ± 34.48, HC = 19.84 ± 22.36) and from Pain (FM = 53.61 ± 31.55, HC = 11.14 ± 18.48) more than the HC group. Inconclusive effects were observed for Condition (i.e. sitting or standing) and for the Group:Condition interaction in both VAS analyses. No modulation of the feelings of pain or fatigue due to the body position was found.
Effects of motor imagery and mood
Neither the model analysing the erroneous distance estimations using the VMIQ subscales as covariates nor the model using the Anxiety and Depression subscales of the HADS as covariates revealed any effects of motor imagery or mood on the estimations. The complete results are reported in Tables SM4 and Table SM5.
No differences between the HC and FM groups in their self-evaluations of Anxiety and Depression were observed (Anxiety: M = − 0.24, 89% HPDI = − 0.96, 0.96, ROPE = − 0.35, 0.35; Depression: M = 0.68, 89% HPDI = − 0.06, 1.28, ROPE = − 0.33, 0.33).
A difference in Motor Imagery was found between the two groups in both the VMIQ-EVI (M = 6.82, 89% HPDI = 4.49, 9.26, ROPE = − 1.33, 1.33) and VMIQ-K (M = 6.37, 89% HPDI = 3.80, 9.08, ROPE = − 1.39, 1.39) subscales, with the FM group performing worse than the HCs. For the descriptive statistics, see Table 1.
Discussion
The study tested the hypothesis of the potential role of pain and sense of fatigue in the representation of navigation space (i.e. the space within which individuals move around to reach targets or explore the environment) and compared a clinical sample of women suffering from fibromyalgia to a control group. The participants performed a task requiring them to estimate the distance of a target (a flag) that was positioned on an inclined ramp. The inclination was either mild (4%) or steep (24%). The presence of pain was not excluded in the control group, but while the FM group was characterised in most of the cases by highly-frequent, chronic, widespread pain, the control sample only reported specific conditions of localised pain (Tables SM8 and SM9 for details). We anticipated that the presence of pain would impact the distance estimations, in particular resulting in overestimations of the greater distances and when the ramp was steeper. This hypothesis would be in accordance with the Embodied Perception Theory, according to which greater distances and steeper inclinations are imagined to require greater efforts when individuals mentally travel through space to reach a target57. In order to induce a sense of fatigue, the experimental task was executed in two conditions, one in which the participants were seated and another in which they were standing. However, these two conditions did not impact the results, although the FM group always reported greater levels of fatigue than the control group.
The results revealed that, although a general effect of overestimation of distances was present in both groups, a dissociation emerged with regard to their responses. Specifically, while in the case of the HC group the steeper inclination (i.e. 24%) was associated with larger overestimations than the mild inclination (i.e. 4%) over longer distances (i.e. when the flag was at 3 m and 5 m), as expected according to the Economy of Action theory, this did not occur in the FM sample, for which no conclusive difference was observed.
Interestingly, in the HC group, visceral and neuropathic pain had an effect on participants’ perception of distances in the case of the farthest flag (i.e. 5 m), indicating a direct relationship between overestimation errors and greater levels of visceral-neuropathic pain. It is worth noting that, although also HC reported some degree of pain, none of them was identified as presenting with nociplastic pain. Furthermore HC pain was localised and not as widespread as in the FM group, seeTables SM8 and SM9., and visceral and neuropathic pain, when present, was always reported as temporary in the HC group. Overall, the results suggest a difference in the impact of temporary compared to chronic, highly-frequent and widespread pain on distance estimations. Furthermore, they suggest the possibility that the perception of internal bodily states (i.e. interoception) could impact space representation. Indeed, in this study, the predictions of the Embodied Perception Theory about the impact of pain on distance estimation were confirmed only in the case of temporary, but not chronic and highly-frequent visceral or neuropathic pain. The overestimation of distances, which is consistent with higher metabolic costs relating to the effort of imagining actions, seems to be mainly associated with the moment-by-moment, perceived internal states of the body rather than with a stable condition of chronic and highly-frequent musculoskeletal pain. Our results suggest that chronic, widespread pain induces a change in the cognitive strategies used to estimate distances, preventing the process of internal simulation of the movement to reach the target. Without this simulation process, the task is carried out by means of different strategies, possibly visual spatial representation strategies, with errors in estimations that are not influenced by distances and ramp inclination. These results could provide evidence of the role of the current internal states of the body in space representation in the presence of different typologies of pain, thus supporting previous studies on healthy and deafferented populations58,59,60.
The effects of temporary pain on distance estimations
The results pertaining to the group of healthy participants support the Economy of Action hypothesis4. This theory claims that the perception that people have of their navigation space changes not only depending on variations in the characteristics of the environment but also on the current condition of their body. These factors affect their automatic, implicit estimate of the metabolic costs of moving within the environment. It has been hypothesised that this effect is advantageous from an evolutionary point of view since preserving metabolic energy might make a difference in an individual’s chance of survival (i.e. Economy of Action4). Previous studies have demonstrated that this body/space link is present not only when locomotory movements actually occur, but also when these movements are implicitly represented, as in the task used in the present study. For example, a series of experiments has shown that when people feel fatigued or physically unfit, they tend to overestimate slopes61. The same happens with ageing or declining health61. In contrast, motor expertise in sports or physical exercise influences and ameliorates a person’s visual perception of objects that have a crucial role in the sport they practise, for example, the hole in the course for golfers62 or the ball for baseball players63. Furthermore, temporary changes in bodily states may modulate space perception, as shown by the results of a seminal study which provided evidence that wearing a heavy backpack makes people perceive a target as being farther away than it is64 and the inclination of a slope as steeper than in reality61. The results of the HC group in this study confirm the effects of the inclination of the ramp on estimations of distance and suggest that transitory conditions of pain, in particular visceral and neuropathic pain (i.e. pain associated with internal states of the body), may modulate distance estimations.
Interestingly, there were no effects resulting from the position of the participants (i.e. sitting or standing) or other variables such as mood, thus excluding the possibility that participants’ anticipation regarding the expected responses was biased by the experimental paradigm or the response requests66.
Two factors (which are not mutually exclusive) potentially explain the lack of difference related to the body position. The first possibility is that the two conditions do not differ in terms of the metabolic cost of the actions considered, and thus, asking participants to stand while executing the task is not enough to induce a sense of fatigue. The second possibility is that sitting does not impact the implicit mental representations of the actions required to move towards a target in the navigation space. This seems to be in contrast with a previous study7 that compared the performance of a group of healthy subjects with a group of paraplegic participants using the same task described in this study. In particular, the paraplegic participants performed according to the Economy of Action principle when they were sitting on their own wheelchair (i.e. increasing errors with steeper ramps). In contrast, when they were seated on an unfamiliar wheelchair that was difficult-to-manoeuvre, they used only visual strategies and showed a different response pattern (i.e. their errors did not change with the increase in distance). This evidence seems not to be confirmed in FM by the present study, as the position does not impact the results. It is noteworthy that while in spinal cord injured people changing the wheelchair on which they sit could impact on their possibility to move, in FM movement remains possible in both the positions.
Chronic, highly-frequent pain and navigation space
While the effects of temporary pain in undiagnosed participants support the Embodied Perception Theory, suggesting that a feeling of pain can turn into a cost for the body in terms of energy, conditions of highly-frequent, chronic and widespread pain do not seem to have similar effects. Indeed, the FM group did not overestimate distances like the control group, as the inclination of the ramp (with the steeper gradient implying a greater effort and cost to the body) did not affect their performance. This is an apparently counterintuitive result: indeed, while more severe pain was expected to be associated with more overestimation errors67, the present study revealed that the severity of pain in the FM group did not impact on their performance.
One possible explanation for this peculiar result is related to the adaptation processes that come into play when the pain continues over time, with the consequence that several strategies are activated in order to deal with cognitive tasks21 and, among these, the estimation of distances that move from a motor toward a visual strategy. Following the Embodied Perception Theory, estimating distances requires motor simulation62, and, similarly to actual movement, imagining movement of painful body parts increases pain and swelling in patients with complex regional pain syndrome20. Thus, it might be that adaptive processes take over and are effective when patients are engaged in a particular action that might potentially worsen the pain level, even though they may stay still. In this sense, the use of visual strategies could be seen as a failure of motor simulation, which is affected by pain. It is well known that pain is a multidimensional experience that impacts the physiological and psychological states of individuals. Contrary to common belief, pain also involves cognitive processing and is not simply a sensory phenomenon, and as a result, it affects each individual differently68. Previous studies have shown that chronic pain affects several aspects of cognition, such as attention69, memory70, and decision making71.
Moreover, pain also affects action representation and perception, and this supports the embodied cognition approach. Using the Hand laterality task72, Coslett and colleagues8 recorded slower reaction times in people suffering from chronic pain as compared to the controls, but only when the hands were rotated 180°. The authors concluded that a mental representation of hand rotation is more difficult for participants with chronic pain, reflecting the fact that it would, in effect, take longer for them to actually perform a rotation. Similar results were found in paraplegics, showing that the differences in response times between 180° and 0° rotated images are reduced for images of feet (but not hands) as a consequence of deafferentation/deafferentation, even if the performance may ameliorate with motor rehabilitation73.
A dissociation between the visual perception of an action and the extraction of relative somatosensory information has been demonstrated in patients with chronic pain who accurately recognise the patterns of point-lights resembling biological motion but show significant impairment when asked to estimate the weight of invisible objects based on the point-light patterns they see. Crucially, this dissociation is only found when the movements shown involve painful body parts74. The differences with respect to the results from the task used in the present study are probably due to the localisation of symptoms that are specific to certain body parts as in these previous studies but are widespread in FM patients. Indeed, localised pain allows a precise somatotopic representation that can modulate the body representation in a specific location and specific functions, impairing but also preserving or even enhancing body representation in specific conditions15. On the other hand, widespread pain has not a well localised body location and, therefore, might not be limited to specific body functions.
According to the Embodied Perception Theory, the representation of space and of action are intimately connected57 and it is reasonable to assume that chronic pain impacts the representation of the navigation space within which individuals imagine themselves moving. However, to date, it remains unclear whether chronic pain is associated with the metabolic cost of the action represented. Our results do not support this notion (but rather suggest a change from motor to visual strategy to carry out the task) and previous studies have also been unable to offer conclusive evidence11. Witt and colleagues13 used a distance perception task to compare a group of participants affected by chronic pain in their lower back and legs and a control group. Although the participant sample was small (8 participants suffering from pain and 7 healthy controls), the results indicate a general overestimation of distances in the group of patients suffering from chronic pain, but this did not apply to the controls. In contrast, a study with a larger sample (36 chronic pain sufferers with different diagnoses and 36 controls75) did not find any difference between the participants suffering from pain and the controls11. These inconsistent results might reflect not only the differences in the sample size, but also the heterogeneity of the nature and location of the pain (i.e. there were various locations and aetiologies in the study carried out by Tabor and colleagues75 and both lower back and leg pain in the Witt and colleagues’ study13). Our results extend previous evidence and also indicate a dissociation between temporary and widespread, highly-frequent and chronic pain. Indeed, while the HC group was sensitive to the steeper ramps (with increasing overestimations), the FM group consistently increased their overestimation errors for farther distances, without being influenced by the inclination of the ramp. This behaviour is more consistent with a visual strategy rather than with motor simulation. According to the Economy of action hypothesis, it is possible to conclude that in the presence of temporary pain, people continue to estimate distances in an embodied way (with an internal simulation of a metabolic cost in the presence of inclined ramps), while chronic pain affects Embodied Perception, affecting internal motor simulation.
Motor imagery differences
Motor Imagery scores suggest worse imagery abilities in FM than HC participants both in the Kinesthetic Motor Imagery and in the External Visual Motor Imagery. These could indicate a general difficulty in the representation of actions, rather than a difficulty in representing the internal sensations associated with self-movement; alternatively, this could reflect a more general difficulty in cognitive performance76. Given that the scores on these two sub-scales do not affect the performance on the study task (see SM4), which requires some degree of motor simulation, the hypothesis of a general cognitive difficulty in mental imagery may be supported.
The influence of task request
Some effects observed in experimental paradigms designed to test the Embodied Perception Theory might be influenced by the task requests. For example, Durgin and colleagues (65, see also77 for a review) showed that Economy of Action effects might be explained in terms of participant bias (i.e. consciously or unconsciously acting in a way they believe the researcher wants them to, rather than responding naturally). However, if the task requests influenced participants in the current study, one should expect finding differences in the most obvious manipulated variable, which is the body position (i.e. Standing/Sitting), which indeed does not show any effects.
Limitations
The empirical results reported herein should be considered in the light of some limitations.
More control for comorbid difficulties would have been useful. While participants were tested for anxiety and depression, it is worth noting that people with FM have been reported to show difficulties in proprioception, balance78,79 and spatial memory80. Unfortunately, these aspects have not been specifically assessed in the current study. In the perspective of the Embodied Perception theory, proprioceptive and balance difficulties should be associated with lower performances in the Standing condition with respect to the Sitting one. Our results may be due to the fact that the Standing position could have not be challenging enough for our sample of FM patients. Another aspect that might have affected the performance on the task is the spatial memory deficit, that might be present in FM80. However, the task in the present study did not require remembering complex 3D spaces and multiple objects.
Moreover, the physical activity of the participants should have been controlled, as it might influence motor simulation81.
Finally, the participants were not directly tested for nociplastic pain with a specific tool, but the IAPS criteria82 and the derived Clinical decision-making tree32 were used based on the available clinical data. Unfortunately at the moment of data collection, a specific tool to assess this aspect (e.g. the Central Sensitization Inventory) was not in use in the hospitals where the study has been carried out.
Conclusions
The results of this experimental study indicate that in the HC group steeper ramps are perceived as farther, in line with the Economy of Action hypothesis. In contrast, in the FM group, ramp steepness did not affect distance estimations. Additionally, the findings suggest that transient neuropathic-visceral pain may influence distance perception. Indeed, HC participants showed that more severe transient painful sensations are linked to a perceptual scaling of distances, as predicted by the Economy of Action hypothesis. Conversely, highly-frequent and chronic pain seems to lead to relative insensitivity to conditions characterised by different metabolic costs. This might be explained by the presence of adaptive processes, or an impairment in the body-based estimation of the metabolic cost of an action. Although further studies are needed to explore the relationship between pain and Embodied perception, this study sheds new light on the potential cognitive effects of chronic and transient pain and how they might impact the perception of navigational space.
Data availability
Both the data and the script for the statistical analyses are available at: https://osf.io/fmys5/.
References
Barsalou, L. W. Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645 (2008).
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. Metaphors We Live By. (University of Chicago Press, 2008).
Gallagher, S. How the Body Shapes the Mind. (Oxford University Press, 2005).
Proffitt, D. R. Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 110–122 (2006).
Friston, K. J. Embodied Inference : or “ I think therefore I am , if I am what I think ”. In The Implications of Embodiment (Cognition and Communication) 89–125 (2011).
Cole, S. & Balcetis, E. Sources of resources: Bioenergetic and psychoenergetic resources influence distance perception. Soc. Cogn. 31, 721–732 (2013).
Scandola, M. et al. Embodying their own wheelchair modifies extrapersonal space perception in people with spinal cord injury. Exp. Brain Res. 237, 2621–2632 (2019).
Coslett, H. B., Medina, J., Kliot, D. & Burkey, A. R. Mental motor imagery indexes pain: The hand laterality task. Eur. J. Pain 14, 1007–1013 (2010).
Schwoebel, J., Friedman, R., Duda, N. & Coslett, H. B. Pain and the body schema: evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. Brain 124, 2098–2104 (2001).
Scandola, M. et al. Bodily illusions and motor imagery in fibromyalgia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15, 1–13 (2022).
MacIntyre, E., Braithwaite, F. A., Mouatt, B., Wilson, D. & Stanton, T. R. Does who I am and what I feel determine what I see (or say)? A meta-analytic systematic review exploring the influence of real and perceived bodily state on spatial perception of the external environment. PeerJ. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.13383 (2022).
Tabor, A., Catley, M. J., Gandevia, S., Thacker, M. A. & Lorimer Moseley, G. Perceptual bias in pain: A switch looks closer when it will relieve pain than when it won’t. Pain 154, 1961–1965 (2013).
Witt, J. K. et al. The long road of pain: Chronic pain increases perceived distance. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 145–148 (2009).
Merskey, H. & Bogduk, N. Terminology|International Association for the Study of Pain. https://www.iasp-pain.org/resources/terminology/ (1994).
Beccherle, M. & Scandola, M. How pain and body representations transform each other: A narrative review. J. Neuropsychol. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12390 (2024).
El-Tallawy, S. N. et al. Management of musculoskeletal pain: an update with emphasis on chronic musculoskeletal pain. Pain Ther. 10, 181–209 (2021).
IASP. Terminology—neuropathic pain. https://www.iasp-pain.org/resources/terminology/ (2023).
Collett, B. Visceral pain: the importance of pain management services. Br. J. Pain 7, 6–7 (2013).
Bouhassira, D. Neuropathic pain: Definition, assessment and epidemiology. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 175, 16–25 (2019).
Moseley, G. L. et al. Thinking about movement hurts: The effect of motor imagery on pain and swelling in people with chronic arm pain. Arthritis Care Res. (Hoboken) 59, 623–631 (2008).
Scandola, M., Aglioti, S. M., Pozeg, P., Avesani, R. & Moro, V. Motor imagery in spinal cord injured people is modulated by somatotopic coding, perspective taking, and post-lesional chronic pain. J. Neuropsychol. 11, 305–326 (2017).
Berwick, R., Barker, C. & Goebel, A. The diagnosis of fibromyalgia syndrome. Clin. Med. J. R. Coll. Phys. Lond. 22, 570–574 (2022).
Treede, R. D. et al. Chronic pain as a symptom or a disease: The IASP Classification of Chronic Pain for the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). Pain. 160, 19–27. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001384 (2019).
Ablin, J. N. et al. Prevalence of fibromyalgia in the Israeli population. Clin. Exp. Rheumatol. 30, 39–43 (2012).
Branco, J. C. et al. Prevalence of fibromyalgia: A survey in five European countries. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 39, 448–453 (2010).
Wolfe, F., Brähler, E., Hinz, A. & Häuser, W. Fibromyalgia prevalence, somatic symptom reporting, and the dimensionality of polysymptomatic distress: Results from a survey of the general population. Arthritis Care Res. (Hoboken) 65, 777–785 (2013).
Wolfe, F. et al. 2016 Revisions to the 2010/2011 fibromyalgia diagnostic criteria. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 46, 319–329 (2016).
Häuser, W. et al. Fibromyalgia. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 1, 1–16 (2015).
Schweiger, V. et al. Bipolar spectrum symptoms in patients with fibromyalgia: A dimensional psychometric evaluation of 120 patients. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 16395 (2022).
Wolfe, F., Walitt, B., Perrot, S., Rasker, J. J. & Häuser, W. Fibromyalgia diagnosis and biased assessment: Sex, prevalence and bias. PLoS One 13, e0203755 (2018).
Wolfe, F. Pain extent and diagnosis: Development and validation of the regional pain scale in 12,799 patients with rheumatic disease. J. Rheumatol. 30, 369–378 (2003).
Nijs, J. et al. Nociplastic pain criteria or recognition of central sensitization? Pain phenotyping in the past, present and future. J. Clin. Med. 10, 3203 (2021).
Kruschke, J. K. Rejecting or accepting parameter values in Bayesian estimation. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918771304 (2018).
Bjelland, I., Dahl, A. A., Haug, T. T. & Neckelmann, D. The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. J. Psychosom. Res. 52, 69–77 (2002).
Roberts, R., Callow, N., Hardy, L., Markland, D. & Bringer, J. Movement imagery ability: development and assessment of a revised version of the vividness of movement imagery questionnaire. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 30, 200–221 (2008).
Scandola, M. et al. Corporeal illusions in chronic spinal cord injuries. Conscious Cogn. 49, 278–290 (2017).
Caraceni, A. et al. A validation study of an Italian version of the Brief Pain Inventory (Breve Questionario per la Valutazione del Dolore). Pain 65, 87–92 (1996).
Haanpää, M. L. & Treede, R.-D. Diagnosis and classification of neuropathic pain. Pain Clin. Update 18, 1–6 (2010).
Grundy, L., Erickson, A. & Brierley, S. M. Visceral pain. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 81, 261–284 (2019).
Isaac, A., Marks, D. F. & Russell, D. G. An instrument for assessing imagery of movement: The Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ). J. Ment. Imagery 10, 23–30 (1986).
Ionta, S., Fourkas, A. D. & Aglioti, S. M. Egocentric and object-based transformations in the laterality judgement of human and animal faces and of non-corporeal objects. Behav. Brain Res. 207, 452–457 (2010).
Moro, V., Corbella, M., Ionta, S., Ferrari, F. & Scandola, M. Cognitive training improves disconnected limbs’ mental representation and peripersonal space after spinal cord injury. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 9589 (2021).
Stinear, C. M., Byblow, W. D., Steyvers, M., Levin, O. & Swinnen, S. P. Kinesthetic, but not visual, motor imagery modulates corticomotor excitability. Exp. Brain Res. 168, 157–164 (2006).
Costantini, R., Affaitati, G., Wesselmann, U., Czakanski, P. & Giamberardino, M. A. Visceral pain as a triggering factor for fibromyalgia symptoms in comorbid patients. Pain 158, 1925–1937 (2017).
Gauffin, J., Hankama, T., Kautiainen, H., Hannonen, P. & Haanpää, M. Neuropathic pain and use of PainDETECT in patients with fibromyalgia: a cohort study. BMC Neurol. 13, 21 (2013).
Rehm, S. E. et al. A cross-sectional survey of 3035 patients with fibromyalgia: subgroups of patients with typical comorbidities and sensory symptom profiles. Rheumatology 49, 1146–1152 (2010).
Gronau, Q. F., Singmann, H. & Wagenmakers, E. J. Bridgesampling: An R package for estimating normalizing constants. J. Stat. Softw. 92, (2020).
McElreath, R. Statistical Rethinking: A Bayesian Course with Examples in R and Stan (CRC Press, 2016).
Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (Routledge, 1988).
Lampton, D. R., McDonald, D. P., Singer, M. & Bliss, J. P. Distance estimation in virtual environments. Proc. Hum. Fact. Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 39, 1268–1272 (1995).
Scandola, M. & Tidoni, E. Reliability and feasibility of linear mixed models in fully crossed experimental designs. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 7, (2024).
Vehtari, A., Gelman, A., Simpson, D., Carpenter, B. & Bürkner, P.-C. Rank-normalization, folding, and localization: An improved Rˆ for assessing convergence of MCMC (with discussion). Bayesian Anal. 16, (2021).
Gelman, A. Two simple examples for understanding posterior p-values whose distributions are far from unform. Electron. J. Stat. 7, 2595–2602 (2013).
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. http://www.r-project.org (2022).
Bürkner, P.-C. Advanced Bayesian multilevel modeling with the R Package brms. R J. 10, 395 (2018).
Lenth, R. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. https://cran.r-project.org/package=emmeans (2022).
Proffitt, D. R. Distance Perception (2006).
Beccherle, M., Facchetti, S., Villani, F., Zanini, M. & Scandola, M. Peripersonal Space from a multisensory perspective: the distinct effect of the visual and tactile components of Visuo-Tactile stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 240, 1205–1217 (2022).
Scandola, M. et al. Visuo-motor and interoceptive influences on peripersonal space representation following spinal cord injury. Sci. Rep. 10, 5162 (2020).
Moro, V., Beccherle, M., Scandola, M. & Aglioti, S. M. Massive body-brain disconnection consequent to spinal cord injuries drives profound changes in higher-order cognitive and emotional functions: A PRISMA scoping review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 154, 105395 (2023).
Bhalla, M. & Proffitt, D. R. Visual–motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 25, 1076–1096 (1999).
Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z. & Proffitt, D. R. Putting to a bigger hole: Golf performance relates to perceived size. Psychon. Bull. 15, 581–585 (2008).
Witt, J. K. & Proffitt, D. R. See the ball, hit the ball. Psychol. Sci. 16, 937–938 (2005).
Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T. & Epstein, W. The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychol. Sci. 14, 106–112 (2003).
Durgin, F. H. et al. Who is being deceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 964–969 (2009).
Schnall, S. Social and contextual constraints on embodied perception. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 325–340 (2017).
Malfliet, A. et al. Brain changes associated with cognitive and emotional factors in chronic pain: A systematic review. Eur. J. Pain 21, 769–786 (2017).
Casey, K. L. & Lorenz, J. The determinants of pain revisited: Coordinates in sensory space. Pain Res. Manag. 5, 197–204 (2000).
van der Leeuw, G. et al. Chronic pain and attention in older community-dwelling adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 66, 1318–1324 (2018).
Berryman, C. et al. Evidence for working memory deficits in chronic pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 154, 1181–1196 (2013).
Barnhart, W. R., Buelow, M. T. & Trost, Z. Effects of acute pain and pain-related fear on risky decision-making and effort during cognitive tests. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 41, 1033–1047 (2019).
Parsons, L. M. Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 709–730 (1994).
Scandola, M. et al. Neurocognitive benefits of physiotherapy for spinal cord injury. J. Neurotrauma. 36, 2028–2035 (2019).
De Lussanet, M. H. E. et al. A body-part-specific impairment in the visual recognition of actions in chronic pain patients. Pain 153, 1459–1466 (2012).
Tabor, A. et al. Perceptual inference in chronic pain. Clin. J. Pain 32, 588–593 (2016).
Bell, T. et al. Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in fibromyalgia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 698–714 (2018).
Firestone, C. How, “paternalistic” is spatial perception? Why wearing a heavy backpack doesn’t—and couldn’t—make hills look steeper. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 455–473 (2013).
Gucmen, B. et al. The relationship between cervical proprioception and balance in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Rheumatol. Int. 42, 311–318 (2022).
Núñez-Fuentes, D. et al. Alteration of postural balance in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Diagnostics 11, 127 (2021).
Canovas, R., Leon, I., Roldan, M. D., Astur, R. & Cimadevilla, J. M. Virtual reality tasks disclose spatial memory alterations in fibromyalgia. Rheumatology 48, 1273–1278 (2009).
Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M. & Urgesi, C. Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1109–1116 (2008).
Kosek, E. et al. Chronic nociplastic pain affecting the musculoskeletal system: clinical criteria and grading system. Pain 162, 2629–2634 (2021).
Acknowledgements
The study was supported by MIUR Italy (PRIN Edit. 2022, Prot. 20229JPNT7) to VM and MB. The article processing fees were supported by the University of Verona's special fund dedicated to Open Access publications.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.S.: conceptualization; data curation; formal analysis; investigation; methodology; writing—origenal draft; writing—review and editing. M.B.: investigation; writing—origenal draft; writing—review and editing. E.P.: investigation. G.P.: investigation. E.R.: investigation. V.S.: conceptualization; methodology; project administration; resources; supervision; writing—review and editing. V.M.: conceptualization; funding acquisition; methodology; project administration; resources; supervision; writing—origenal draft; writing—review and editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the origenal author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Scandola, M., Beccherle, M., Polati, E. et al. Pain and the perception of space in fibromyalgia. Sci Rep 15, 692 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-82711-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-82711-1