Abstract
Rationale
Previous studies have shown that rats trained to self-administer heroin and cocaine exhibit opposite preferences, as a function of setting, when tested in a choice paradigm. Rats tested at home prefer heroin to cocaine, whereas rats tested outside the home prefer cocaine to heroin. Here, we investigated whether drug history would influence subsequent drug preference in distinct settings. Based on a theoretical model of drug-setting interaction, we predicted that regardless of drug history rats would prefer heroin at home and cocaine outside the home.
Methods
Rats with double-lumen catheters were first trained to self-administer either heroin (25 μg/kg) or cocaine (400 μg/kg) for 12 consecutive sessions. Twenty-six rats were housed in the self-administration chambers (thus, they were tested at home), whereas 30 rats lived in distinct home cages and were transferred to self-administration chambers only for the self-administration session (thus, they were tested outside the home). The rats were then allowed to choose repeatedly between heroin and cocaine within the same session for seven sessions.
Results
Regardless of the training drug, the rats tested outside the home preferred cocaine to heroin, whereas the rats tested at home preferred heroin to cocaine. There was no correlation between drug preference and drug intake during the training phase.
Conclusion
Drug preferences were powerfully influenced by the setting but, quite surprisingly, not by drug history. This suggests that, under certain conditions, associative learning processes and drug-induced neuroplastic adaptations play a minor role in shaping individual preferences for one drug or the other.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Drug use is rarely limited to a single substance, polydrug use being more of a rule than an exception (e.g., Sample 1977; Brecht et al. 2008; Badiani et al. 2015; John et al. 2018). In particular, it is well known that most individuals with heroin use disorder also use cocaine and vice versa (Leri et al. 2003, 2005; Kosten et al. 1986; Levin et al. 1996). Yet, polydrug use does not preclude the forming of preferences for one drug or another (Harford 1978; Gossop and Connell 1975). The mechanisms responsible for these preferences are still poorly understood, leaving aside obvious constraints, such as those deriving from law, market availability, and street price (e.g., Jofre-Bonet and Petry 2008). However, we have shown that the context of use (the setting) plays an important role in modulating drug preference in an animal model of drug self-administration (Caprioli et al. 2009). In this experiment, we trained two groups of rats to self-administer heroin and cocaine on alternate days and then we gave them the opportunity to choose repeatedly between the two drugs within the same session. One of the two groups self-administered the drugs at home (that is, these rats resided in the self-administration chambers). The other group was transferred to the self-administration chambers only for the test sessions (thus, these rats self-administered the drug outside the home). Despite the fact that the self-administration environment was physically identical for the two groups, we found striking differences in their drug preferences. Most rats tested at home preferred heroin to cocaine. In contrast, most rats tested outside the home preferred cocaine to heroin. There are at least three possible explanations for this phenomenon. The first one relates to basic associative learning processes that have been shown to play an important role in drug craving (Childress et al. 1993; Grimm et al. 2001). Since heroin and cocaine were paired with different levers (and their respective light cues) during training, the rats might have developed a setting-dependent bias for one of the two levers. This bias might have expressed itself during the choice phase. A second possibility relates to the fact that during training the rats took different amounts of cocaine versus heroin, as function of setting (see also Caprioli et al. 2007, Caprioli et al. 2008). This might have induced distinct neuroadaptations in the two settings, as it has been shown that repeated exposure to opiates such as morphine produces neuroplastic changes opposite to those produced by psychostimulants such as cocaine (Robinson and Kolb 2004; Becker et al. 2017). Opposite neuroplastic adaptations might have resulted, during the choice phase, in greater craving for heroin at home and for cocaine outside the home. Finally, it is possible that drug preferences were the result of a more fundamental interaction between drug and setting. We have in fact proposed (Badiani 2013) that the rewarding effect of any drug is decreased in the presence of a “mismatch” between the interoceptive information produced by central and peripheral drug effects and the exteroceptive information (the setting). In the case of cocaine, for example, a mismatch would occur when this drug, which produces arousal and sympathetic activation (Billman 1995; Sofuoglu and Sewell 2009), is taken in a quiet domestic environment. The opposite would occur when heroin, which produces sedation and parasympathomimetic effects (Haddad and Lasala 1987; Thornhill et al. 1989), is taken outside the home, that is in exciting, potentially dangerous contexts.
The aim of the present study was to investigate the influence of setting on heroin versus cocaine choice as a function of drug history, using an unbiased procedure. Thus, we modified the procedure used by Caprioli et al. (2009) rats in two ways: (1) we tested independent groups of rats with a history of either heroin or cocaine self-administration; (2) during training, both levers were paired with the same drug so that no lever bias could develop before the choice phase. The three hypotheses highlighted above predict different results. If drug preferences were the result of associative learning processes or of neuroplastic changes relating to drug history, heroin-trained rats should prefer heroin to cocaine, regardless of setting, whereas cocaine-trained rats should prefer cocaine to heroin. Based on our working hypothesis, we predicted instead that rats would prefer heroin to cocaine at home, and cocaine to heroin outside the home, regardless of drug history.
Methods and materials
Animals
Seventy-two male Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan, Italy), weighing 250–275 g at their arrival, were used in this study. Eleven rats were excluded because of catheter clogging (i.e., they failed the catheter patency test, see below) or sickness. Five rats that did not reach the criterion for the acquisition of self-administration (see “Data analysis and statistics”) were also excluded. Thus, only the data from 56 rats were included in the final analysis (N’s refer to these rats). Final sample size was determined based on a previous report with similar experimental design (Caprioli et al. 2009).
The rats were housed and tested in the same dedicated temperature- and humidity-controlled rooms, with free access (except during the test sessions) to food and water under a 14-h dark/10-h light cycle (lights off at 7:00 AM). After their arrival, the rats were housed two per cage for 7–10 days before the surgery. After the surgery, the rats were housed individually (see “General self-administration procedures”). The procedures were approved by the Italian Ministry of Health and were conducted in accordance with the European Community Directives (2010/63/EU), with the Italian Law on Animal Research (Decreto Legislativo 26/2014), and with the guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals issued by the Italian Ministry of Health.
Self-administration apparatus
The apparatus consisted of self-administration chambers (28.5-cm length, 27-cm width, and 32-cm height) made of transparent plastic (front and rear walls), aluminum (side-walls and ceiling), and stainless steel (grid floor). Plastic trays covered with pine wood shavings were placed under the cage floors. Each chamber was equipped with two retractable levers, positioned on the left hand wall 12.5 cm apart and 9 cm above the floor, two sets of three cue lights (red, yellow, and green), positioned above each lever, and a counterbalanced arm holding a double channel liquid swivels. Chambers and accessories were purchased from ESATEL S.r.l. (Rome, Italy). The self-administration chambers were placed within sound- and light-attenuating cubicles. Each chamber was connected via an electronic interface (ESATEL) to a syringe pump (Razel Scientific Instruments, St Albans, VT, USA) and to a Programmable Logic Controller (PLC; Allen Bradley, Milwaukee, WI, USA). Finally, the PLCs were connected to PCs running control software developed by Aries Sistemi S.r.l. (Rome, Italy).
Surgery
The intravenous double-lumen catheters consisted of two 10.5 cm of silicone tubing (0.37-mm inner diameter, 0.94-mm outer diameter) sheathed, at 3.4 cm from its proximal end, by a 5-mm length of heat-shrink tubing. On the day of surgery, the rats received an intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of 2.33 mg of xylazine hydrochloride (Rompun®, Bayer HealthCare) and an intramuscular injection of 14,000 IU of benzylpenicillin (Fournier Pharma, S. Palomba, Italy). The rats were then anesthetized with an i.p. injection of 0.56 ml/kg of Zoletil 100® (Virbac, Carros, France), containing tiletamine (50 mg/ml) and zolazepam (50 mg/ml). Using standard surgical procedures, the double-lumen catheters were inserted into the right jugular vein, so as to reach the right atrium with its proximal end, and was then secured to the surrounding soft tissues with silk thread. The distal end of the double-lumen catheters was passed subcutaneously in front of the left shoulder, externalized through a small incision at the nape of the neck, and connected to two L-shaped 22-gauge cannulas. The cannula was then secured to the rat’s skull using dental cement and stainless steel screws. After surgery, the rats were given 15 mg i.v. enrofloxacin (Baytril®, KVP Pharma + Veterinär produkte Gmbh, Kiel, Germany) in a double i.v. bolus. Catheters were flushed daily (at 1800 h) with 0.1 ml of a sterile saline solution containing 0.4 mg of enrofloxacin and 25 IU heparin (Marvecs Services, Agrate Brianza, Italy). At the end of the experiment, all rats underwent a catheter patency test in which they received two i.v. boluses of 40 mg/kg of thiopental sodium (Pharmacia Italia, Milan, Italy), one in each catheter lumens, with a 15-min interval between the two.
Setting of self-administration
The rats were allowed at least 7 days to recover from the surgery and were then randomly assigned to one of two groups, to be tested at home and outside the home, respectively. Home rats (N = 26) were individually housed in the self-administration chambers where they remained for the entire duration of the experiment. The other rats (N = 30) were individually housed in standard transparent plastic cages (40-cm length, 24.5-cm width, and 18-cm height; with stainless steel tops and flat bottoms covered with ground corncob bedding) and were transferred to the self-administration chambers immediately before the start of each testing session. Thus, although the testing environment was physically identical for the two groups, it represented home for one group and a distinct environment for the other group (see Fig. 1a).
Procedures
General self-administration procedures
The self-administration sessions took place during the dark phase, between 1230 and 1630 h. The rats tested at home were connected to the infusion lines 3 h before the start of each session. The rats tested outside the home were transferred to the self-administration chambers immediately before the start of each session and their catheters were connected to the infusion lines. In both groups, the infusion pumps were activated during the 60 s preceding the start of each session, so as to fill the dead volume of the catheters’ lumina with the appropriate drug solutions (thus, no drug entered the rats’ bloodstream before the beginning of the session). During the training phase (see below), only one lumen was used in any given session, whereas during the choice phase (see below), both lumina were filled (one with heroin and the other with cocaine). At this time, food and water were removed from the chambers of rats tested at home, so that both groups were tested in the absence of food or water. All other husbandry and testing routines were identical for the two groups. Notice that throughout the experiments, the rats were individually housed and tested in the same dedicated testing room (thus, was no transport from one room to another and no disruption of social context or circadian rhythmicity) with ad libitum access to food and water (except during the test sessions).
The experiment included three phases: training (12 days), withdrawal (3 days), and choice (7 days).
Training phase (days 1–12)
The rats were randomly assigned to self-administer either heroin (N = 12 at home and N = 14 outside the home) or cocaine (N = 14 at home and N = 16 outside the home) for 12 consecutive daily 3-h sessions (see Fig. 1b, c). Unit doses of 25 μg/kg for heroin (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) and 400 μg/kg for cocaine (Division of Neuroscience & Behavioral Research, NIDA, Bethesda, MD, USA) were dissolved in sterile saline to an infusion volume of 40 μl. These doses were selected on the basis of previous experiments (Caprioli et al. 2007a, 2008, 2009).
At the start of each training session, only one lever was extended and the respective cue lights were turned on. The fixed ratio (FR), that is, the number of consecutive lever presses required to obtain a single infusion, increased during training from 1 (FR1) in sessions 1–4, to FR2 in sessions 5–8, and then to FR5 in sessions 9–12 (see Fig. 2). The 40 μl of drug solution was infused at a rate of 10 μl/s. After each infusion, the cue lights turned off and the lever retracted. The cue lights turned on and the lever extended again after a time-out (T) period. In order to achieve, by the end of the training phase, the schedule of reinforcement to be used during the choice sessions (see next subsection), the duration of the TO period was progressively increased over test sessions: 40 s in sessions 1–2, 60 s in sessions 3–4, 120 s in sessions 5–6, 180 s in sessions 7–8, 300 s in sessions 9–10, and 600 s in sessions 11–12. A synopsis of the schedule of self-administration during training is illustrated in Fig. 2.
It is important to stress that in each group, both levers were alternatively paired with the drug and the starting lever was counterbalanced across rats. That is, some rats started with the right lever on session 1 and ended with the left lever on session 12. The opposite occurred for the other rats (see Fig. 1c). Furthermore, the two lumina of the catheter were used alternatively, in a counterbalanced manner across sessions, to deliver the drug solution.
At the end of each session, food and water were given back to the rats at home and the rats in the outside-the-home condition were returned to their home cages.
Withdrawal phase (days 12–15)
After the end of the training phase, the rats were withdrawn from self-administration for 3 days (from the afternoon of day 12 to the morning of day 15). During this period, the rats were left undisturbed in their respective housing to ensure complete cocaine and heroin clearance (see Caprioli et al. 2009).
Choice phase (days 15–21)
After the withdrawal period, the rats underwent a choice phase consisting of seven sessions (3 h each) during which the rats were allowed to choose repeatedly between heroin (25 μg/kg) and cocaine (400 μg/kg) within session (see Fig. 1d). Notice that the choice phase was the first time cocaine-trained rats had access to heroin and heroin-trained rats had access to cocaine.
At the beginning of each choice session, both levers were extended and the respective cue lights turned on. One lever was paired with heroin and the other lever with cocaine, in a counterbalanced manner across rats. As during the last training sessions, the choice sessions were conducted on an FR5 schedule of reinforcement but in this case pressing on the cocaine lever reset the counter of the heroin lever and vice versa. Once the animal completed the FR5 task on one of the levers, the drug was administered, the respective cue lights turned off, and both levers retracted for a 600-s TO period, after which the levers were extended again and the lights turned on for the next trial. Thus, the rats could self-administer a maximum of 18 infusions per session.
To ensure that the choice procedure was completely unbiased, the levers were also counterbalanced with respect to the last training session (see Fig. 1c, d). Thus, for some of the rats that had ended the training (session 12) with the right lever, the choice was between cocaine on the right and heroin on the left, whereas for the other rats, cocaine was on the left and heroin on the right. The same was done for the rats that had ended the training (session 12) with the left lever. Furthermore, the infusion lines were counterbalanced across rats and sessions; that is, on the first choice sessions, some rats received cocaine through the anterior lumen and heroin through the posterior lumen, the opposite occurred on the following session, and so on; the reverse sequence was used for the other rats.
Catheter patency test
At the end of the experiment, all rats underwent a catheter patency test in which they received two i.v. boluses of 40 mg/kg of thiopental sodium (Pharmacia Italia, Milan, Italy), one in each catheter lumens, with a 15-min interval between the two. Five rats that failed the test (that is, that did not became ataxic within 5 s after thiopental) were excluded from the analyses.
Data analysis and statistics
Training data
Five rats did not reach the criterion for self-administration (an average of at least three infusions of cocaine or heroin per session on the last four sessions on FR5) and were excluded from the analysis. Self-administration data for the training phase were analyzed using a three-way ANOVA for the between-subject factors setting (two levels: home vs. outside the home) and drug (two levels: cocaine vs. heroin) and the repeated-measure factor training session (12 levels). The effect of setting on heroin or cocaine self-administration for each session was estimated using one-tailed Student’s t tests (as the direction of the difference was predicted by the working hypothesis). The effect size was estimated by calculating partial eta-squared (η2), with critical values: 0.01–0.059 = small effect size, 0.06–0.13 = medium effect size, and > 0.14 = large effect size (Cohen 1988).
Choice data
The total number of choices was analyzed using a three-way ANOVA for the between-subject factors setting (two levels: home vs. outside the home) and drug history (two levels: cocaine and heroin) and the repeated-measure factor choice session (seven levels). The number of cocaine versus heroin choices could not be compared directly because the two variables were inversely related. Thus, we first calculated, for each rat, the ratio between the number of cocaine infusions and the total number of infusions (cocaine choice ratio) for the seven choice sessions. After arcsine transformation, these data were analyzed using a three-way ANOVA for the between-subject factors setting (two levels: home vs. outside the home) and drug history (two levels: cocaine and heroin) and the repeated-measure factor choice session (seven levels). Effect size was estimated by calculating η2. Furthermore, based on the proportion of cocaine infusions on the last three choice sessions, the rats were individually classified, using a straightforward bootstrapping procedure (Wilson 1927; Newcombe 1988), as cocaine-preferring, heroin-preferring, or non-preferring (p’s < 0.05).
The relationships between drug intake during training and drug preferences were assessed using linear correlation analysis.
Results
Drug self-administration training
Figure 2 illustrates the number of infusions during training for the heroin (left panel) and cocaine (right panel) groups, as a function of the setting. The ANOVA showed a significant drug × setting interaction (F1, 52 = 12.074, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.188) but no main effect of drug (F1, 52 = 3.263, p = 0.077, η2 = 0.059) or setting (F1, 52 = 0.131, p = 0.719, η2 = 0.003). There was also a significant training session × drug × setting interaction (F11, 52 = 2.044, p = 0.023, η2 = 0.038). Follow-up analysis showed a significant effect of setting for both heroin (F1, 24 = 6.954, p = 0.014, η2 = 0.225) and cocaine (F1, 28 = 6.101, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.179) groups, indicating that, consistent with previous findings (and despite the constraints in the maximum number of infusions), rats at home self-administered more heroin than rats outside the home whereas rats outside the home self-administered more cocaine than rats at home. The results of pair-wise comparisons for the effect of setting in each session are illustrated in Fig. 2. Notice that the narrowing of group differences in the last training sessions was the result of a ceiling effect due to the constraints in the maximum number of infusions (see “Methods and materials” section and Fig. 2).
Drug choice
Figure 3 illustrates the number of cocaine versus heroin infusions, during the seven sessions of the choice phase, as a function of the setting. The rats tested outside the home made a slightly higher number of total choices relative to the rats tested at home (109.2 ± 2.1 vs. 101.7 ± 4.4) but this difference was not significant (p = 0.13). As detailed in the “Data analysis and statistics” section, the comparison of cocaine versus heroin infusions would be meaningless because the two variables were inversely related. However, Fig. 4 illustrates the cocaine preference ratio (the ratio between the number of cocaine infusion and the total number of infusions) calculated on the same data. During the first choice session, the rats self-administered more or less the same amounts of heroin and cocaine infusions, indicating that no lever bias had developed during training. However, on the following sessions, preferences emerged. The rats tested outside the home exhibited higher cocaine preference ratio than the rats tested at home, as indicated by a significant main effect of setting (F1, 52 = 5.718; p = 0.02, η2 = 0.099). These differences grew larger over test session, even though the interaction between choice session and setting only approached significance (F6, 312 = 2.078; p = 0.056, η2 = 0.038). In contrast, drug history appeared to exert only a modest influence on drug choice, as indicated by the lack of a main effect of drug history (F1, 52 = 1.835, p = 0.18, η2 = 0.034), with no setting × drug history (F1, 52 = 0.050, p = 0.825, η2 = 0.001), choice session × drug history (F6, 312 = 0.622, p = 0.713, η2 = 0.012), or choice session × setting × drug history (F6, 312 = 0.606, p = 0.725, η2 = 0.012) interaction. Finally, as shown in Fig. 5, there was no significant correlation between preference ratios and total amounts of heroin or cocaine taken during the training phase either at home (r2 = 0.03, p = 0.47 for heroin; r2 = 0.12, p = 0.23 for cocaine) or outside the home (r2 = 0.03, p = 0.20 for heroin; r2 = 0.002, p = 0.87 for cocaine).
Figure 6 illustrates drug preferences in individual rats. Most rats (80.4%) expressed a significant preference for either heroin or cocaine (see “Data analysis and statistics”). Individual preferences were influenced by the setting: outside the home, there were many more cocaine-preferring rats (60.0%) than heroin-preferring rats (16.7%), whereas the at home, there were many more heroin-preferring rats (57.7%) than cocaine-preferring rats (23.1%), with a near tenfold shift in cocaine versus heroin preference as a function of setting.
Discussion
An unbiased choice procedure was used here to determine drug preferences in rats previously trained to self-administer heroin or cocaine in different settings. We found that individual differences in drug preference were powerfully influenced by the setting of drug taking but in a substance-specific manner. At home, two and a half times more rats chose heroin than cocaine. The opposite occurred outside the home: four times more rats chose cocaine than heroin. Overall, there was a nearly tenfold shift in drug preferences as a function of setting. Most important, we found that drug history had a negligible effect on drug preference, that is, at home the rats preferred heroin even if they had previously self-administered only cocaine whereas outside the home the rats preferred cocaine even if they had previously self-administered only heroin.
Individual differences in heroin versus cocaine preference
Research done in the last three decades has stressed the existence of shared neural substrates for the rewarding effects of addictive drugs (Di Chiara and Imperato 1988; Nestler 2004; Badiani et al. 2011; Covey et al. 2014). Psychostimulants and opiates, for example, despite their very different pharmacodynamic profiles, share the ability to increase dopamine levels in the terminal regions of the mesostriatal dopamine system. Cocaine and amphetamine do so by binding the dopamine transporter located on dopaminergic terminals (Harris and Baldessarini 1973; Rothman and Baumann 2003). Opiates act indirectly by activating mu opioid receptors (Inturrisi et al. 1983; Selley et al. 2001) located on GABAergic interneurons, which exert tonic inhibitory control on dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area (Gysling and Wang 1983; Di Chiara and Imperato 1986; Johnson and North 1992). The evidence of shared mechanisms of action has led to the widespread assumption that the rewarding effects of psychostimulants and opiates are substantially the same. It is also thought that psychostimulants and opiates induce partly overlapping neuroplastic changes that are responsible for the transition to abuse and the vulnerability to relapse (Robinson and Berridge 1993; Berridge and Robinson 2016; Nestler 2001, 2004; Pickens et al. 2011). However, unified models of drug reward cannot easily provide a satisfactory explanation for the existence of drug preferences of the type reported here. In particular, it would be difficult to explain why some of the rats that had previously self-administered only cocaine should then prefer heroin, and, vice versa, some of the rats that had previously self-administered only heroin should then prefer cocaine. It is reasonable to conclude that heroin and cocaine produce distinct internal states and that the rewarding values of these internal states differ substantially from one individual to another.
Further research is necessary to determine the neurobiological correlates of cocaine versus heroin reward in rats. However, it is of some interest that within-subject single-unit electrophysiology experiments in rats have shown that heroin and cocaine self-administration engage distinct neuronal populations in the terminal regions of the mesostriatal dopamine system (Chang et al. 1998). Moreover, studies using chemical lesions (Pettit et al. 1984; Gerrits and Van Ree 1996), dopamine receptor antagonists (Ettenberg et al. 1982), and RNA-interference of dopamine D1 receptors (Pisanu et al. 2015) have shown differential involvement of the dopaminergic system in cocaine versus heroin self-administration.
Heroin versus cocaine preference as a function of setting
The findings reported here indicate that drug preferences are a function not only of individual differences but also of the setting of drug use, confirming a previous report (Caprioli et al. 2009).
We have previously shown that the setting can influence in opposite directions virtually all aspects of cocaine versus heroin reward, including drug intake, motivation to work for the drug, drug discrimination, drug affect, and vulnerability to relapse into heroin or cocaine seeking after a period of abstinence (Paolone et al. 2004; Caprioli et al. 2007a, b, 2008; Celentano et al. 2009; Montanari et al. 2015; Avvisati et al. 2016). In contrast, earlier studies focusing on drug-induced psychomotor sensitization have shown that the magnitude of sensitization to both psychostimulants and opiates is much greater when these drugs are repeatedly administered outside the home than when administered at home (Badiani et al. 1995, a, b, 1997, 2000; Browman et al. 1998a, b; Crombag et al. 1996, 2000; Fraioli et al. 1999; Ostrander et al. 2003; Paolone et al. 2003, 2007). This suggests a fundamental dissociation between the rewarding effects of opiates and their psychomotor-activating effects and cautions against the long-standing notion that the latter necessarily reflect the former (Wise and Bozarth 1987). We also found that the analgesic response to morphine and development of tolerance to this effect is the same at home and outside the home (Paolone et al. 2003), consistent with the notion that tolerance and sensitization to different drug effects can develop independent of each other (Stewart and Badiani 1993). Taken together, these findings indicate that the influence of setting on drug reward is not the consequence of a general facilitation or reduction in drug efficacy due to changes in pharmacokinetics or pharmacodynamics.
As discussed in the “Introduction,” to account for the ability of the setting to influence in opposite ways the reinforcing effects of heroin and cocaine, we have proposed that the overall rewarding effects of addictive drugs are the result of a complex interaction between their central and peripheral effects and the setting of drug use (Badiani 2013). In the presence of mismatch between exteroceptive information (setting) and interoceptive information generated by central and peripheral drug actions, the affective valence of drug experience would be more negative than in conditions in which there was no such a mismatch. Self-administration experiments with other classes of drugs with sedative or activating effects lend support to our hypothesis (Testa et al. 2011; De Luca and Badiani 2011; De Luca et al. 2012).
The major aim of the present study was to test a crucial implication of the mismatch theory, that is, that rats would tend to prefer heroin to cocaine at home, and cocaine to heroin outside the home, regardless of whether they had previously self-administered only heroin or only cocaine. Overall, our findings are consistent with this hypothesis. Yet, the extent to which drug choice was independent of drug history was somewhat surprising, as it suggests that, under certain conditions, associative learning processes (Childress et al. 1993; Grimm et al. 2001) and drug-induced neuroplastic adaptations (for reviews, see Robinson and Berridge 1993; Nestler 2001; Pickens et al. 2011) play a relatively minor role in shaping the preference of an individual for one drug or the other.
The neurobiological mechanisms through which the setting influences drug preferences in the rat are not known. However, it has been shown that the setting modulates in different, sometimes opposite manner the activity of reward areas of the brain in response to psychostimulants, such as amphetamine and cocaine (Badiani et al. 1998, 1999; Uslaner et al. 2001a, b; Ostrander et al. 2003; Hope et al. 2006), versus opiates, such as morphine and heroin (Ferguson et al. 2004; Paolone et al. 2007; Celentano et al. 2009). In particular, it appears that cocaine increases (relative to vehicle) the activity of D2+/enkephalin+ medium spiny neurons (MSN) of the striatum, to a much greater extent outside the home than at home (Uslaner et al. 2001b), whereas the opposite occurs with morphine (Ferguson et al. 2004). These earlier findings might have some bearing to the results reported here given that D2+/enkephalin+ MSN indirectly disinhibit the subthalamic nucleus (STN), which has been implicated in reward and decision-making (Zenon et al. 2016; Pelloux et al. 2018). Further work is necessary to explore the role of the STN in drug preferences.
Conclusions
The findings reported here have important implications for addressing a crucial issue in drug addiction research. That is, to what extent “the risk factors for the use or misuse of a particular class of psychoactive substances are specific to that class or are nonspecific in that they predispose the individual to the use or misuse of a wide range of such compounds” (Kendler et al. 2003, p. 687). Studies in twins (Tsuang et al. 1998; Kendler et al. 2003) have shown that the substance specificity of drug abuse is almost entirely due to environmental influences but very little is known about their nature (Zinberg 1984). Thus, it is remarkable that translational studies in humans have shown that the setting can influence drug reward in a manner similar to that observed in the rat. Indeed, polydrug users with a diagnosis of substance use disorder (SUD) reported a preference for using heroin at home and cocaine outside the home (Caprioli et al. 2009; Badiani and Spagnolo 2013). This was true regardless of the route of administration or of social context. A recent study has shown that such preferences may depend on the fact that heroin and cocaine produce different affective states in different settings (De Pirro et al. 2018). In particular, it was found that in cocaine and heroin users with SUD, the affective valence of heroin was greater at home than outside the home, whereas the opposite was seen for cocaine. Furthermore, in these individuals, the setting exerted a substance-specific influence also on the activity of brain regions implicated in processing drug reward and contextual information (De Pirro et al. 2018). Taken together, our work suggests that unitary constructs of drug reward and drug addiction should be revised in the light of mounting evidence indicating distinct neurobiological underpinnings for the response to different classes of drugs (see Badiani et al. 2011; Badiani 2013; Badiani et al. 2018; Peters et al. 2013; Nutt et al. 2015).
References
Avvisati R, Contu L, Stendardo W, Michetti C, Montanari M, Scattoni ML, Aldo Badiani A (2016) Ultrasonic vocalization in rats self-administering heroin and cocaine in different settings: evidence of substance-specific interactions between drug and setting. Psychopharmacology 233:1501–1511
Badiani A (2013) Substance-specific environmental influences on drug use and drug preference in animals and humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23(4):588–596
Badiani A, Spagnolo PA (2013) Role of environmental factors in cocaine addiction. Curr Pharm Des 19(40):6996–7008
Badiani A, Morano MI, Akil H, Robinson TE (1995) Circulating adrenal hormones are not necessary for the development of sensitization to the psychomotor activating effects of amphetamine. Brain Res 673(1):13–24
Badiani A, Anagnostaras SG, Robinson TE (1995a) The development of sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine is enhanced in a novel environment. Psychopharmacology 117(4):443–452
Badiani A, Browman KE, Robinson TE (1995b) Influence of novel versus home environments on sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of cocaine and amphetamine. Brain Res 674(2):291–298
Badiani A, Camp DM, Robinson TE (1997) Enduring enhancement of amphetamine sensitization by drug-associated environmental stimuli. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 282(2):787–794
Badiani A, Oates MM, Day HEW, Watson SJ, Akil H, Robinson TE (1998) Amphetamine-induced behavior, dopamine release and cfos mRNA expression: modulation by environmental novelty. J Neurosci 18:10579–10593
Badiani A, Oates MM, Day HEW, Watson SJ, Akil H, Robinson TE (1999) Environmental modulation of amphetamine-induced c-fos expression in D1 versus D2 striatal neurons. Behav Brain Res 103:203–209
Badiani A, Oates MM, Robinson TE (2000) Modulation of morphine sensitization in the rat by contextual stimuli. Psychopharmacology 151(2–3):273–282
Badiani A, Belin D, Epstein D, Calu D, Shaham Y (2011) Opiate versus psychostimulant addiction: the difference do matter. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:685–700
Badiani A, Boden JM, De Pirro S, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Harold GT (2015) Tobacco smoking and cannabis use in a longitudinal birth cohort: evidence of reciprocal causal relationships. Drug Alcohol Depend 150:69–76
Badiani A, Berridge KC, Heilig M, Nutt DJ, Robinson TE (2018) Addiction research and theory: a commentary on the Surgeon General's Report on alcohol, drugs, and health. Addict Biol 23(1):3–5
Becker JAJ, Kieffer BL, Le Merrer J (2017) Differential behavioral and molecular alterations upon protracted abstinence from cocaine versus morphine, nicotine. THC and alcohol Addict Biol 22(5):1205–1217
Berridge KC, Robinson TE (2016) Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am Psychol 71(8):670–679
Billman GE (1995) Cocaine: a review of its toxic actions on cardiac function. Crit Rev Toxicol 25(2):113–132
Brecht ML, Huang D, Evans E, Hser YI (2008) Polydrug use and implications for longitudinal research: ten-year trajectories for heroin, cocaine, and methamphetamine users. Drug Alcohol Depend 96:93–201
Browman KE, Badiani A, Robinson TE (1998a) The influence of environment on the induction of sensitization to the psychomotor activating effects of intravenous cocaine in rats is dose-dependent. Psychopharmacology 137(1):90–98
Browman KE, Badiani A, Robinson TE (1998b) Modulatory effect of environmental stimuli on the susceptibility to amphetamine sensitization: a dose-effect study in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 287(3):1007–1014
Caprioli D, Paolone G, Celentano M, Testa A, Nencini P, Badiani A (2007a) Environmental modulation of cocaine self-administration in the rat. Psychopharmacology 192:397–406
Caprioli D, Celentano M, Paolone G, Badiani A (2007b) Modeling the role of environment in addiction. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 31:1639–1653
Caprioli D, Celentano M, Paolone G, Lucantonio F, Bari A, Nencini P, Badiani A (2008) Opposite environmental regulation of heroin and amphetamine self-administration in the rat. Psychopharmacology 198(3):395–404
Caprioli D, Celentano M, Dubla A, Lucantonio F, Nencini P, Badiani A (2009) Ambience and drug choice: cocaine- and heroin-taking as a function of environmental context in humans and rats. Biol Psychiatry 65:893–899
Celentano M, Caprioli D, di Pasquale P, Cardillo V, Nencini P, Gaetani S, Badiani A (2009) Drug context differently regulates cocaine versus heroin self-administration and cocaine- versus heroin-induced Fos mRNA expression in the rat. Psychopharmacology 204:349–360
Chang JY, Janak PH, Woodward DJ (1998) Comparison of mesocorticolimbic neuronal responses during cocaine and heroin self-administration in freely moving rats. J Neurosci 18:3098–3115
Childress AR, Hole AV, Ehrman RN, Robbins SJ, McLellan AT, O’Brien CP (1993) Cue reactivity and cue reactivity interventions in drug dependence. NIDA Res Monogr 137:73–95
Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale
Covey DP, Roitman MF, Garris PA (2014) Illicit dopamine transients: reconciling actions of abused drugs. Trends Neurosci 37(4):200–210
Crombag HS, Badiani A, Robinson TE (1996) Signalled versus unsignalled intravenous amphetamine: large differences in the acute psychomotor response and sensitization. Brain Res 722(1–2):227–231
Crombag HS, Badiani A, Maren S, Robinson TE (2000) The role of contextual versus discrete drug-associated cues in promoting the induction of psychomotor sensitization to intravenous amphetamine. Behav Brain Res 116(1):1–22
De Luca MT, Badiani A (2011) Ketamine self-administration in the rat: evidence for a critical role of setting. Psychopharmacology 214:549–556
De Luca MT, Meringolo M, Spagnolo PA, Badiani A (2012) The role of setting for ketamine abuse: clinical and preclinical evidence. Rev Neurosci 23:769–780
De Pirro S, Galati G, Pizzamiglio L, Badiani A (2018) The affective and neural correlates of heroin versus cocaine use in addiction are influenced by environmental setting but in opposite directions. J Neurosci 38(22):5182–5195
Di Chiara G, Imperato A (1986) Preferential stimulation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens by opiates, alcohol, and barbiturates: studies with transcerebral dialysis in freely moving rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci 473:367–381
Di Chiara G, Imperato A (1988) Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 85:5274–5278
Ettenberg A, Pettit HO, Bloom FE, Koob GF (1982) Heroin and cocaine intravenous self-administration in rats: mediation by separate neural systems. Psychopharmacology 78:204–209
Ferguson SM, Thomas MJ, Robinson TE (2004) Morphine-induced c-fos mRNA expression in striatofugal circuits: modulation by dose, environmental context, and drug history. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:1664–1674
Fraioli S, Crombag HS, Badiani A, Robinson TE (1999) Susceptibility to amphetamine-induced locomotor sensitization is modulated by environmental stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology 20(6):533–541
Gerrits MA, Van Ree JM (1996) Effect of nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion on motivational aspects involved in initiation of cocaine and heroin self-administration in rats. Brain Res 713:114–124
Gossop MR, Connell PH (1975) Attitudes of oral and intravenous multiple drug users toward drugs of abuse. Int J Addict 10(3):453–466
Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y (2001) Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412(6843):141–142
Gysling K, Wang RY (1983) Morphine-induced activation of A10 dopamine neurons in the rat. Brain Res 277:119–127
Haddad GG, Lasala PA (1987) Effect of parasympathetic blockade on ventilatory and cardiac depression induced by opioids. Respir Physiol 67:101–114
Harford RJ (1978) Drug preferences of multiple drug abusers. J Consult Clin Psychol 46(5):908–912
Harris JE, Baldessarini RJ (1973) Uptake of (3H)-catecholamines by homogenates of rat corpus striatum and cerebral cortex: effects of amphetamine analogues. Neuropharmacology 12:669–679
Hope BT, Simmons DE, Mitchell TB, Kreuter JD, Mattson BJ (2006) Cocaine-induced locomotor activity and Fos expression in nucleus accumbens are sensitized for 6 months after repeated cocaine administration outside the home cage. Eur J Neurosci 24(3):867–875
Inturrisi CE, Schultz M, Shin S, Umans JG, Angel L, Simon EJ (1983) Evidence from opiate binding studies that heroin acts through its metabolites. Life Sci 33(Suppl 1):773–776
Jofre-Bonet M, Petry NM (2008) Trading apples for oranges? Results of an experiment on the effects of heroin and cocaine price changes on addicts’ polydrug use. J Econ Behav Organ 66:281–311
John WS, Zhu H, Mannelli P, Schwartz RP, Subramaniam GA, Wu LT (2018) Prevalence, patterns, and correlates of multiple substance use disorders among adult primary care patients. Drug Alcohol Depend 187:79–87
Johnson SW, North RA (1992) Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyperpolarization of local interneurons. J Neurosci 12:483–488
Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC (2003) Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. Am J Psychiatry 160:687–695
Kosten TR, Gawin FH, Rounsaville BJ, Kleber HD (1986) Cocaine abuse among opioid addicts: demographic and diagnostic factors in treatment. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 12(1–2):1–16
Leri F, Bruneau J, Stewart J (2003) Understanding polydrug use: review of heroin and cocaine co-use. Addiction 98(1):7–22
Leri F, Stewart J, Fischer B, Jürgen R, Marsh DC, Brissette S, Bruneau J, El-Guebaly N, Noël L, Tyndall MW, Wild TC (2005) Patterns of opioid and cocaine co-use: a descriptive study in a Canadian sample of untreated opioid-dependent individuals. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 13(4):303–310
Levin FR, Foltin RW, Fischman MW (1996) Pattern of cocaine use in methadone-maintained individuals applying for research studies. J Addict Dis 15(4):97–106
Montanari C, Stendardo E, De Luca MT et al (2015) Differential vulnerability to relapse into heroin versus cocaine-seeking as a function of setting. Psychopharmacology 232:2415–2424
Nestler EJ (2001) Molecular basis of long-term plasticity underlying addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:119–128
Nestler EJ (2004) Historical review: molecular and cellular mechanisms of opiate and cocaine addiction. Trends Pharmacol Sci 25:210–218
Newcombe RG (1988) Two-sided confidence intervals for the single proportion: comparison of seven methods. Stat Med 17:857–872
Nutt DJ, Lingford-Hughes A, Erritzoe D, Stokes PR (2015) The dopamine theory of addiction: 40 years of highs and lows. Nat Rev Neurosci 16(5):305–312
Ostrander MM, Badiani A, Day HE, Norton CS, Watson SJ, Akil H, Robinson TE (2003) Environmental context and drug history modulate amphetamine-induced c-fos mRNA expression in the basal ganglia, central extended amygdala, and associated limbic forebrain. Neuroscience 120:551–571
Paolone G, Burdino R, Badiani A (2003) Dissociation in the modulatory effects of environmental novelty on the locomotor, analgesic, and eating response to acute and repeated morphine in the rat. Psychopharmacology 166(2):146–155
Paolone G, Palopoli M, Marrone MC, Nencini P, Badiani A (2004) Environmental modulation of the interoceptive effects of amphetamine in the rat. Behav Brain Res 152:149–155
Paolone G, Conversi D, Caprioli D, Bianco PD, Nencini P, Cabib S, Badiani A (2007) Modulatory effect of environmental context and drug history on heroin-induced psychomotor activity and fos protein expression in the rat brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 32(12):2611–2623
Pelloux Y, Degoulet M, Tiran-Cappello A, Cohen C, Lardeux S, George O, Koob GF, Ahmed SH, Baunez C (2018) Subthalamic nucleus high frequency stimulation prevents and reverses escalated cocaine use. Mol Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0080-y
Peters J, Pattij T, De Vries TJ (2013) Targeting cocaine versus heroin memories: divergent roles within ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Trends Pharmacol Sci 34(12):689–695
Pettit HO, Ettenberg A, Bloom FE, Koob GF (1984) Destruction of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens selectively attenuates cocaine but not heroin self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology 84:167–173
Pickens CL, Airavaara M, Theberge F, Fanous S, Hope BT, Shaham Y (2011) Neurobiology of the incubation of drug craving. Trends Neurosci 34(8):411–420
Pisanu A, Lecca D, Valentini V, Bahi A, Dreyer JL, Cacciapaglia F, Scifo A, Piras G, Cadoni C, Di Chiara G (2015) Impairment of acquisition of intravenous cocaine self-administration by RNA-interference of dopamine D1-receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell. Neuropharmacology 89:398–411
Robinson T, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 18(3):247–291
Robinson TE, Kolb B (2004) Structural plasticity associated with exposure to drugs of abuse. Neuropharmacology 47:33–46
Rothman RB, Baumann MH (2003) Monoamine transporters and psychostimulant drugs. Eur J Pharmacol 479(1–3):23–40
Sample CJ (1977) The epidemiology of drug abuse: current issues. Concept of polydrug use. NIDA Res Monogr 1977(10):19–31
Selley DE, Cao CC, Sexton T, Schwegel JA, Martin TJ, Childers SR (2001) [mu] Opioid receptor-mediated G-protein activation by heroin metabolites: evidence for greater efficacy of 6-monoacetylmorphine compared with morphine. Biochem Pharmacol 62:447–455
Sofuoglu M, Sewell RA (2009) Norepinephrine and stimulant addiction. Addict Biol 14(2):119–129
Stewart J, Badiani A (1993) Tolerance and sensitization to the behavioral effects of drugs. Behav Pharmacol 4(4):289–312
Testa A, Nencini P, Badiani A (2011) The role of setting in the oral self-administration of alcohol in the rat. Psychopharmacology 215:749–760
Thornhill JA, Townsend C, Gregor L (1989) Intravenous morphine infusion (IMF) to drug-naive, conscious rats evokes bradycardic, hypotensive effects, but pressor actions are elicited after IMF to rats previously given morphine. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 67(3):213–222
Tsuang MT, Lyons MJ, Meyer JM, Doyle T, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True W, Lin N, Toomey R, Eaves L (1998) Co-occurrence of abuse of different drugs in men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:967–972
Uslaner J, Badiani A, Day HE, Watson SJ, Akil H, Robinson TE (2001a) Environmental context modulates the ability of cocaine and amphetamine to induce c-fos mRNA expression in the neocortex, caudate nucleus and nucleus accumbens. Brain Res 920:106–116
Uslaner J, Badiani A, Norton CS, Day HE, Watson SJ, Akil H, Robinson TE (2001b) Amphetamine and cocaine induce different patterns of c-fos mRNA expression in the striatum and subthalamic nucleus depending on environmental context. Eur J Neurosci 13:1977–1983
Wilson EB (1927) Probable inference, the law of succession, and statistical inference. J Am Stat Assoc 22:209–212
Wise RA, Bozarth MA (1987) A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychol Rev 94(4):469–492
Zinberg NE (1984) Drug, set, and setting the basis for controlled intoxicant use. Yale University Press, New Haven, Connecticut
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Dr. Silvana De Pirro and Dr. Terry Robinson for their helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. The authors also thank the National Institute for Drug Abuse (NIDA; Bethesda, MD, USA) for providing the cocaine used in this study.
Funding
This work was supported by grants from Sapienza University of Rome (C26A12LN24) and from the University of Sussex (SDF-SA027-05).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
About this article
Cite this article
De Luca, M.T., Montanari, C., Meringolo, M. et al. Heroin versus cocaine: opposite choice as a function of context but not of drug history in the rat. Psychopharmacology 236, 787–798 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-5115-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-5115-1