Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Dec 29.
Published in final edited form as: Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011 Aug 18;218(1):131–156. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2443-9

Effects of stress on alcohol drinking: a review of animal studies

Howard C Becker 1,2,3,, Marcelo F Lopez 4, Tamara L Doremus-Fitzwater 5
PMCID: PMC3247761  NIHMSID: NIHMS342488  PMID: 21850445

Abstract

Rationale

While stress is often proposed to play a significant role in influencing alcohol consumption, the relationship between stress and alcohol is complex and poorly understood. Over several decades, stress effects on alcohol drinking have been studied using a variety of animal models and experimental procedures, yet this large body of literature has generally produced equivocal results.

Objectives

This paper reviews results from animal studies in which alcohol consumption is evaluated under conditions of acute/sub-chronic stress exposure or models of chronic stress exposure. Evidence also is presented indicating that chronic intermittent alcohol exposure serves as a stressor that consequently influences drinking.

Results

The effects of various acute/sub-chronic stress procedures on alcohol consumption have generally been mixed, but most study outcomes suggest either no effect or decreased alcohol consumption. In contrast, most studies indicate that chronic stress, especially when administered early in development, results in elevated drinking later in adulthood. Chronic alcohol exposure constitutes a potent stressor itself, and models of chronic intermittent alcohol exposure reliably produce escalation of voluntary alcohol consumption.

Conclusions

A complex and dynamic interplay among a wide array of genetic, biological, and environmental factors govern stress responses, regulation of alcohol drinking, and the circumstances in which stress modulates alcohol consumption. Suggestions for future directions and new approaches are presented that may aid in developing more sensitive and valid animal models that not only better mimic the clinical situation, but also provide greater understanding of mechanisms that underlie the complexity of stress effects on alcohol drinking.

Keywords: Stress, Alcohol drinking, Animal models

Introduction

The term stress typically refers to an internal or external (environmental) event that disrupts normal homeostasis (Selye 1936). Stressful events evoke an extensive multi-system and highly integrative physiological response (Goldstein and Kopin 2007; Kopin 1995). The activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis constitutes a major component of the neuroendocrine stress response (Smith and Vale 2006). Alcohol stimulates the HPA axis at several levels, with the magnitude and response profile influenced by a host of variables including genotype, gender, and dosing parameters (Rivier 2000; Wand 2000). Further, both clinical and preclinical studies have documented profound disturbances in HPA axis function following chronic alcohol exposure and withdrawal, and in many instances, these perturbations persist for a protracted period of time. The dysregulation of HPA axis function along with alterations in the activity of extrahypothalamic stress systems in the brain are postulated to significantly influence motivation for alcohol self-administration behavior (Koob and Kreek 2007; Koob and Le Moal 2001).

The relationship between stress and alcohol consumption is complex and poorly understood. On one hand, alcohol is known to be an effective anxiolytic and, hence, motivation for drinking may be guided by its ability to alleviate stress. On the other hand, alcohol is known to activate HPA axis and extrahypothalamic stress systems and, thus, can serve as a stressor itself. Given this reciprocal, yet paradoxical relationship between stress and alcohol, perhaps it is not surprising that there is no clear consensus regarding the circumstances and manner in which stress influences alcohol drinking behavior.

The anxiolytic effects of alcohol are well established in humans and animal models, and this serves as the cornerstone of the tension (stress)-reduction hypothesis (Brady and Sonne 1999; Cappell and Greeley 1987; Pohorecky 1991; Sayette 1999). However, in clinical studies, support for this notion has not been universal (Chutuape and de Wit 1995; Wand et al. 1998). In a similar vein, the self-medication hypothesis stems from the idea that individuals suffering with affective illness use alcohol to relieve dysphoric/stress symptoms associated with the disorder (Carrigan and Randall 2003; Markou et al. 1998). While there is a relatively high prevalence of comorbidity of alcoholism and mood and anxiety disorders, such individuals do not uniformly endorse alcohol use as a means of coping with (medicating) symptoms of the disorder (Battista et al. 2010). Additionally, several clinical studies indicate a dissociation between physiological and subjective effects of alcohol (de Wit et al. 2003; Lewis and Vogeltanz-Holm 2002; Soderpalm and de Wit 2002). Thus, it cannot be said that stress alleviation serves as the sole motivation for alcohol consumption in all individuals. Preexisting psychiatric conditions, the nature and intensity of stress as well as the context in which stress is experienced, and whether stress is associated with and/or resultant from heavy alcohol use are important variables that impact alcohol drinking behavior. Further, there clearly exist individual differences in sensitivity, perception, and responsiveness to stress and, no doubt, these differences will be reflected in drinking behavior. Indeed, both clinical and preclinical evidence indicate that genetic factors play an important role in shaping the nature of the relationship between stress and alcohol drinking (Clarke et al. 2008; Uhart and Wand 2009). Collectively, current evidence suggests that while some individuals may drink alcohol to alleviate perceived stress and dysphoria, many do not drink for this reason.

As previously noted, at the same time that alcohol consumption may be perceived by some as having a calming effect, for other individuals, activation of stress systems along with resultant elevation of circulating corticosteroids may relate to the energizing and euphoric effects of alcohol. There is some evidence indicating that elevated levels of corticosteroid hormones interact with the reinforcing effects of alcohol to enhance propensity to drink. For example, systemic or central administration of corticosterone has been reported to modestly increase alcohol self-administration (Fahlke et al. 1994a, 1996; Fahlke and Hansen 1999), while adrenalectomy or pharmacological inhibition of corticosterone synthesis reduced drinking in rats (Fahlke et al. 1995, 1994b; Lamblin and De Witte 1996). The fact that rats will self-administer corticosterone suggests that this stress hormone may have positive reinforcing effects in its own right (Deroche et al. 1993; Piazza et al. 1993). Further, evidence from studies in animals and humans suggest that stress-related elevations in glucocorticoids interact with mesolimbic reward circuitry to increase drug self-administration behavior (Cleck and Blendy 2008; Melis et al. 2009; Miczek et al. 2008; Sinha 2008; Uhart and Wand 2009). While this is a plausible mechanistic explanation for how stress may increase alcohol drinking, much of the supporting evidence comes predominantly from preclinical and clinical investigations involving psychostimulants. Additionally, it is important to recognize that the ability of alcohol to activate the HPA axis is dose-dependent (Pohorecky 1990, 1991) and, similarly, the ability and manner in which stress modulates neurobiological systems that underlie motivational aspects of alcohol-related behaviors depend on the nature as well as the intensity of the stressor (Kopin 1995; Miczek et al. 2008; Pacak and Palkovits 2001).

The influence of stress on alcohol drinking is further complicated by a host of alcohol-related factors (history of use, level and pattern of drinking, and timing of accessibility of alcohol) and stress-related factors (type, chronicity, intermittency, predictability, and controllability) that intersect with a number of biological variables (genetics, age, and sex). The dynamic interaction of these variables along with experiential factors plays a critical role in defining subjective aspects of stress (i.e., perception and appraisal of a stressful event) as well as how response to stress impacts decisions about alcohol drinking and alcohol consumption alters stress responsiveness. The large number of variables at play along with the dynamic and variable nature in which they interact has certainly contributed to clouding consensus regarding questions of how and when stress influences alcohol drinking in humans. This, in turn, has undoubtedly made the task of developing animal models that accurately reflect the clinical situation especially challenging.

Despite the complexity of the interaction between stress and alcohol reflected in varied clinical study outcomes, it is generally acknowledged that stressful life events play a prominent role in influencing alcohol drinking and, in particular, triggering relapse (Brady and Sonne 1999; Sinha 2001, 2008). This continues to be the major impetus for using animal models to examine stress–alcohol interactions under more controlled conditions as well as elucidate underlying mechanisms. The circumstances and manner in which stress influences drinking behavior in animal models have been extensively studied under a variety of conditions (Pohorecky 1990; Sillaber and Henniger 2004). Unfortunately, this large body of literature has yielded equivocal results, with evidence for stress increasing, decreasing, or not changing alcohol ingestion. This review surveys this literature, with special attention given to the plethora of aforementioned variables that may contribute to discrepancies in results.

An animal model that has been extensively used to study stress–alcohol interactions involves examining the effects of stress on operant alcohol self-administration behavior following a period of abstinence (relapse). In this operant reinstatement model, animals are first trained to respond for alcohol reinforcement. Once stable levels of responding and alcohol intake are established during daily sessions, alcohol-reinforced responding is then extinguished by removing alcohol as a reinforcer, even when the animals appropriately respond for it. Exposing animals to stress (typically footshock) has been shown to reinstate alcohol responding in rats, an effect mediated, at least in part, by extrahypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) activity (Le et al. 1998, 1999, 2000, 2002; Liu and Weiss 2002; Martin-Fardon et al. 2000). While this effect is fairly robust and applicable to several drugs (Shaham et al. 2003; Shalev et al. 2010, 2002), it is important to note that the effect of stress on alcohol/drug responding is examined under conditions of extinction. Since animals are typically not given the opportunity to drink alcohol during test sessions when stress is administered, the reinstatement procedure does not involve examining whether stress alters alcohol consumption. Thus, this model assesses stress effects on alcohol “seeking,” not alcohol drinking.

Since this review is restricted to animal studies investigating stress effects on alcohol consumption, studies involving stress and operant reinstatement procedures are not included in the discussion. Rather, the following highlights results from animal studies in which alcohol consumption is evaluated under conditions when acute/sub-chronic stress exposure is administered during periods when alcohol is available for consumption, and when chronic stress exposure occurs before or during periods of alcohol accessibility. Evidence also is presented from studies indicating that chronic alcohol exposure and withdrawal experience serves as a unique stressor that consequently influences motivation to drink. Finally, suggestions for future directions and new approaches are presented that may aid in developing more sensitive and valid animal models that not only better mimic the clinical situation, but also provide greater understanding of mechanisms that underlie the complexity of stress effects on alcohol drinking.

Acute/sub-chronic stress exposure during alcohol access

A large body of literature has centered around examining the effects of acute or repeated stress exposure on alcohol consumption. Studies of this nature have been organized in Table 1, according to the species employed (i.e., rat or mouse), type of stressor tested, and the direction of the influence of stress on alcohol intake. For the most part, these studies have involved initially providing animals with an opportunity to drink an alcohol solution in order to establish a baseline level of consumption, most commonly in a continuous-access choice situation in which alcohol is offered in an unrestricted fashion as a two-bottle choice, with a control solution such as tap water or saccharin serving as an alternative fluid. Subsequently, stress procedures were administered with alcohol access usually available during this phase to examine alterations in alcohol consumption. In some studies, assessment of intake was continued for a period of time after exposure to the stressor was terminated, allowing for examination of post-stress changes in intake. It should be noted that results from this body of work have been categorized in Table 1 as “Decreased Intake”, “No Change”, or “Increased Intake” based on overall findings in the studies. In several studies, multiple strains, genetic lines, and/or both sexes were examined. When mixed results were reported as a function of these subject variables, results are categorized in the table according to the direction of effects observed for the majority of stressed subjects. Additionally, if in a particular study stress did not significantly alter alcohol intake during the stress phase itself, that study was listed in the “No Change” column of Table 1. Therefore, studies in which stress-related increases in alcohol intake were reported only during a post-stress measurement phase were placed in the “No Change” column. Further, while it would be optimal in studies of this nature to compare alcohol consumption in stressed and non-stressed subjects, in some instances non-stressed controls were not included in the study design (i.e., stress-induced changes in intake were assessed relative to a period of time prior to stress application). Notably, there are some limited studies in which neither baseline intake nor non-stressed controls were included in the study, thus precluding conclusions regarding the influence of stress on alcohol intake—these studies are not included in Table 1.

Table 1.

Acute/sub-chronic stress exposure during alcohol access

Stressor Species Decreased intake No change Increased intake
FS Rat Bond 1978b Casey 1960d Myers and Cicero 1969
Champagne and Kirouac 1987 Powell et al. 1966c Anisman and Waller 1974
Darnaudery et al. 2007b Myers and Holman 1967 Mills et al. 1977
Cicero et al. 1968g Mills and Bean 1978
Von Wright et al. 1971d Kinney and Schmidt 1979g, h
Choca et al. 1977 Volpicelli et al. 1990b, d
Cox and Stainbrook 1977g, h Vengeliene et al. 2003
Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983 Siegmund et al. 2005f
Fidler and LoLordo 1996 Fullgrabe et al. 2007f
Brunell and Spear 2005f
Mouse Chester et al. 2008d, f Racz et al. 2003e
Matthews et al. 2008e
RS Rat Abraham and Gogate 1989h Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983 Derr and Lindblad 1980h
Krishnan et al. 1991 Nash and Maickel 1985d Lynch et al. 1999h
Sprague and Maickel 1994 Rockman et al. 1986b, d Ploj et al. 2003
Haleem 1996 Rockman et al. 1987b, d Roman et al. 2004
Chester et al. 2004d Bowers et al. 1997
Roman et al. 2003
Bertholomey et al. 2011
Mouse Chester et al. 2006h
Tambour et al. 2008
Yang et al. 2008e
FST Rat Vengeliene et al. 2003e Siegmund et al. 2005
Sommer et al. 2008 Fullgrabe et al. 2007f
Mouse Boyce-Rustay et al. 2008e Sillaber et al. 2002d Cowen et al. 2003a
Boyce-Rustay et al. 2007 Cowen et al. 2003b
Lowery et al. 2008d, e Sperling et al. 2010e
Mutschler et al. 2010e
SD Rat van Erp et al. 2001
van Erp and Miczek 2001h
Funk et al. 2005h
Mouse Sillaber et al. 2002d
Croft et al. 2005d
ISO Rat Weisinger et al. 1989 Nash and Maickel 1985d Nunez et al. 1999
Sprague and Maickel 1994 Nunez et al. 2002
Doremus et al. 2005f Mediratta et al. 2003
Other Rat Sandbak and Murison 2001 Rockman and Glavin 1986b
Adams 1995c, e
Turyabahika-Thyen and Wolffgramm 2006
Bertotto et al. 2010
Mouse Little et al. 1999b Sacharczuk et al. 2008e
O'Callaghan et al. 2002b
Schroff et al. 2004d, e

FS footshock, SD social defeat, RS restraint, ISO isolate housing, FST forced swim test

a

Studies in which stressor exposure resulted in an increase in ethanol intake during the stressor phase are listed in the “Increased Intake” column of this table. However, if stress did not significantly alter ethanol intake during the stress phase (even if there was a subsequent increase in intake during the post-stress phase), reports of this nature were listed in the “No Change” column. Finally, if multiple strains and/or both sexes were examined, the article was placed in the column of direction of the effect that the majority of the stressed groups (or the wild-type strain) exhibited (e.g., in the Matthews et al. 2008 paper, two strains showed no change in intake, whereas only one strain showed an increase—therefore, this report was listed in the “No Change” column)

b

Direction of stress effects on ethanol intake depended upon initial preference levels

c

Sex-related differences in stress-induced changes in ethanol intake were observed (paper was placed in the column reflecting the direction of the influence of stress on ethanol consumption among males)

d

Stress-induced increases in ethanol consumption were only apparent during a post-stress observation period

e

Strain differences or differences among genetically manipulated lines were observed with respect to the influence of stress on ethanol consumption

f

Study observed age-related differences in the interaction between stressor exposure and ethanol intake

g

Properties of the shock stressor or the relationship between shock presentation/ethanol availability influenced the relationship between footshock exposure and ethanol drinking

h

Intake was not measured in a continuous-access and/or choice situation

As shown in Table 1, a large number of studies examining the influence of various acute/sub-chronic stress procedures on alcohol consumption have produced equivocal findings. The preponderance of outcomes suggests that stress either does not significantly change or decreases alcohol consumption, although there is some evidence for increased alcohol intake. Notable features of these studies are highlighted below.

Inescapable footshock

Historically, inescapable footshock was one of the first stressors used in which investigators reported that exposure to stress was capable of eliciting an increase in alcohol consumption relative to a group of similarly treated controls without shock exposure and/or relative to a period of unstressed baseline ethanol consumption (e.g., Anisman and Waller 1974; Kinney and Schmidt 1979; Mills et al. 1977; Volpicelli et al. 1990). For example, when adult male Holtzman rats were exposed to either 6 or 12 h alternating periods of unsignalled inescapable footshock presented on either a fixed or random interval schedule, alcohol intake was found to significantly increase relative to non-shocked controls (Anisman and Waller 1974). More recently, other studies utilizing footshock exposure have reported a similar stress-induced increase in alcohol intake (Fullgrabe et al. 2007; Siegmund et al. 2005; Vengeliene et al. 2003).

However, upon closer inspection, many of these outcomes were not especially robust. For example, in the Kinney and Schmidt (1979) study, while cued delivery of inescapable footshock significantly increased alcohol intake compared to non-stressed controls, this increase was only significant during one of the four time periods in which footshock and alcohol access were concurrently experienced. Furthermore, another group of animals in the same study that was exposed to unsignalled footshock failed to significantly increase their alcohol consumption. While Mills et al. (1977) also reported that footshock increased alcohol consumption, this effect was only observed for a short time immediately following stress exposure (there was no difference in overall 24-h intake). Although Vengeliene et al. (2003) also demonstrated that three consecutive days of 10-min exposure to footshock increased alcohol intake among several different strains/lines of rats (Wistar, HAD, P, and AA rats), these increases were only observed on one or maximally 2 days of shock exposure.

In contrast, there have been several studies that have utilized inescapable footshock and subsequently found stress-associated decreases in alcohol intake (Bond 1978; Champagne and Kirouac 1987; Darnaudery et al. 2007). For example, when footshock was administered for 6 days to male Sprague–Dawley rats, alcohol intake significantly decreased, returning to prestress levels after stress exposure was terminated (Bond 1978). While Volpicelli et al. (1990) showed that a short period of footshock increased alcohol intake in rats that initially exhibited low preference for alcohol, other findings indicate that rodents exhibiting a relatively high preference for alcohol prior to stress will exhibit footshock-induced decreases in alcohol consumption (Bond 1978; Volpicelli et al. 1990). Adding to these mixed results, there are instances in which no change in alcohol consumption following inescapable footshock treatment has been reported. For example, several studies examining the effects of foot-shock stress reported no appreciable change in alcohol consumption among adult male rats under a variety of circumstances (e.g., Brunell and Spear 2005; Fidler and LoLordo 1996; Myers and Holman 1967; Powell et al. 1966). Similarly, several strains of mice, including DBA/2 and A/J (Matthews et al. 2008), and the high alcohol preferring selectively bred HAP1 line (Chester et al. 2008) have failed to demonstrate footshock-related changes in alcohol intake.

When analyzing the possible variables that may have contributed to differences across studies in regards to the effect of footshock stress on alcohol consumption, there are virtually no clear patterns that would seemingly explain a reliable increase or decrease in consumption. In considering whether the footshock stressor is administered in a predictable vs. unpredictable manner, for example, in two different rat studies (Cicero et al. 1968; Kinney and Schmidt 1979) and one mouse study (Racz et al. 2003), a cued inescapable footshock elicited significant increases in ethanol consumption, whereas random unsignalled foot-shocks did not. It would be tempting to conclude, therefore, that presentation of a cued inescapable shock is more likely to result in increased alcohol intake. Yet, in most studies that reported increased alcohol consumption following footshock (see Table 1), the shock was not signaled. The same could be said for the duration of footshock exposure: studies reporting footshock-induced increases in alcohol intake have used as few as one (Racz et al. 2003) or three repeated days of footshock exposure (e.g., Siegmund et al. 2005; Vengeliene et al. 2003), while others have reported increases in intake after as many as 27 (Kinney and Schmidt 1979) or even 35 (Myers and Cicero 1969) exposures to the stressor. When examining studies reporting no change in intake, both mouse (Matthews et al. 2008) and rat (Brunell and Spear 2005) studies have failed to observe footshock-related increases in intake with as few as 1 day of stress exposure or as many as 24–30 repeated footshock sessions (Cox and Stainbrook 1977; Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983).

The amount of experience with alcohol consumption prior to stress presentation is another variable that differed greatly across these studies, yet seemed to have no predictive value for determining whether footshock would significantly alter alcohol intake. For instance, studies in which animals had little or no prior opportunity to drink alcohol have reported decreased (e.g., Bond 1978), increased (e.g., Anisman and Waller 1974), or unchanged (e.g., Brunell and Spear 2005; Myers and Holman 1967; Von Wright et al. 1971) alcohol intake following footshock experience. Likewise, studies in which animals were given at least 2 weeks access to alcohol prior to stress administration have reported decreased (Darnaudery et al. 2007), increased (Mills et al. 1977; Vengeliene et al. 2003), or unaltered (Fidler and LoLordo 1996; Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983) alcohol consumption as a function of footshock exposure.

It is tempting to speculate that biological variables (sex, genotype) may play a significant role in determining the outcome of these studies. Unfortunately, when assessing the role that sex of the animal may have on whether or not footshock stress is likely to result in increases or decreases in intake, limited studies are available. While only a few studies included analysis of female subjects, results from those studies were similarly mixed. Specifically, footshock stress in females was reported to increase (Fullgrabe et al. 2007), decrease (Darnaudery et al. 2007), or not significantly alter (Chester et al. 2008; Cox and Stainbrook 1977; Powell et al. 1966) alcohol consumption.

When considering strain of rat, however, there is some evidence to suggest that certain strains may be more prone to demonstrate footshock stress-induced increases in intake. Specifically, the majority of studies reporting stress-related increases in ethanol intake were experiments that utilized Long–Evans (Mills et al. 1977; Mills and Bean 1978; Myers and Cicero 1969) or Wistar rats (Fullgrabe et al. 2007; Siegmund et al. 2005; Vengeliene et al. 2003). In contrast, with the exception of one case (Volpicelli et al. 1990), studies using Sprague–Dawley rats as subjects have most often found either no change in intake (Brunell and Spear 2005; Casey 1960; Cox and Stainbrook 1977; Fidler and LoLordo 1996) or decreased alcohol consumption (Champagne and Kirouac 1987; Darnaudery et al. 2007) during exposure to footshock stress. To our knowledge, the effects of footshock stress on alcohol consumption have not been directly compared among different outbred or inbred strains of rats. Future studies that directly compare some of these strains of rats will be necessary in order to more firmly establish whether a particular genotype confers greater or lesser susceptibility to footshock-induced changes in alcohol consumption. Unfortunately, since so few studies have assessed the effects of footshock stress on alcohol intake among mice, it is premature to draw conclusions as to genetic contributions to the outcome of such studies. Only one study directly compared three inbred mouse strains for the ability of footshock stress to alter drinking. Results from this study indicated that C57BL/6J mice increased alcohol intake in the 24 h following a single acute 15-min footshock exposure, while no change in intake was reported in DBA/2J and A/J mouse strains (Matthews et al. 2008). Given the extensive literature regarding genetic influences on alcohol consumption and stress response in mice (Crabbe 2008; Crabbe et al. 2006; Holmes 2008; Mozhui et al. 2010), systematic evaluation of the influence of footshock stress on alcohol consumption among different genetic mouse models is certainly warranted.

Restraint

Another popularly explored stressor in rodent studies is restraint or immobilization stress. As with footshock stress, some studies employing restraint have reported this stressor to significantly increase alcohol consumption (Derr and Lindblad 1980; Lynch et al. 1999; Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2004). For example, Lynch et al. (1999) found male Wistar rats that experienced repeated immobilization stress increased alcohol intake compared to non-stressed controls. However, in this study, rats were exposed to several phases of forced access to alcohol, which may have impacted the effects of stress on “choice” drinking. Additionally, while 4 days of repeated restraint were shown to significantly increase alcohol intake in both adult male (Ploj et al. 2003) and female (Roman et al. 2004) Wistar rats, this effect was dependent upon whether the animals were earlier exposed to maternal separation stress, and the interaction of this variable on drinking outcome differed for males and females. There are, alternatively, numerous studies in the literature that have reported either restraint stress-induced decreases in alcohol intake (e.g., Chester et al. 2004; Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983; Sprague and Maickel 1994) or no change in alcohol consumption following exposure to this type of stressor (e.g., Bertholomey et al. 2011; Bowers et al. 1997; Rockman et al. 1987; Roman et al. 2003).

Upon examination of the many variables that could potentially influence the manner in which restraint stress impacts alcohol drinking, there appears to be no clear factor that emerges as a primary determinant of the outcome of such studies. For example, if considering the influence of amount of restraint stress exposure, studies have shown increased (Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2004), decreased (Haleem 1996; Sprague and Maickel 1994), or no change (Roman et al. 2003; Tambour et al. 2008) in alcohol consumption with as few as 1–4 days of restraint treatment. Similarly, studies involving more extensive restraint stress exposure (2–8 weeks) have reported increased (Derr and Lindblad 1980; Lynch et al. 1999), decreased (Chester et al. 2004; Krishnan et al. 1991), or unaltered (Nash and Maickel 1985; Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983) alcohol intake.

Another variable to consider is the duration of restraint during each stress session. For this variable, there is again no clear pattern that emerges as to whether duration/intensity of restraint relates to an increase or decrease in alcohol consumption. For example, relatively short exposures (~15–30 min restraint) have been shown to increase (Lynch et al. 1999) or have little effect (Ng Cheong Ton et al. 1983; Roman et al. 2003) on alcohol intake. Likewise, studies that employed relatively long durations of daily restraint (≥18 h) reported increased (Derr and Lindblad 1980) or decreased (Abraham and Gogate 1989) levels of alcohol consumption. Variable unpredictable restraint has been utilized as well, with several studies reporting decreased (Chester et al. 2004; Krishnan et al. 1991; Sprague and Maickel 1994) or unaltered (Nash and Maickel 1985; Rockman et al. 1987) alcohol intake.

As was the case for studies employing footshock stress, there is little indication that the amount of experience with alcohol prior to stress exposure predicts the manner in which restraint stress subsequently modifies drinking behavior. Specifically, studies in which animals were given a relatively short period of “baseline” drinking prior to stress (i.e., ≤1 week) have reported increased (Derr and Lindblad 1980) or no change (Nash and Maickel 1985) in alcohol intake subsequent to restraint exposure. Likewise, studies in which animals were provided access to alcohol for longer periods of time prior to stress exposure (≥2 weeks) report increased (Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2004), decreased (Abraham and Gogate 1989; Chester et al. 2004; Krishnan et al. 1991), or no change (Bertholomey et al. 2011; Roman et al. 2003; Tambour et al. 2008) in alcohol consumption once restraint stress was administered.

Examining the influence of sex on restraint stress-induced changes in alcohol drinking is difficult due to the fact that so few studies have included analysis of female subjects. To our knowledge, only three such studies have been conducted: one demonstrating restraint-induced increases (Roman et al. 2004) and two reporting unchanged alcohol consumption (Bertholomey et al. 2011; Tambour et al. 2008) after restraint stress. Finally, it is difficult to draw conclusions regarding the influence of genetics on the ability of restraint stress to modulate alcohol drinking. While it is the case that three of the four studies reporting restraint stress-induced increases in alcohol intake used Wistar rats (Lynch et al. 1999; Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2004), studies reporting unchanged or decreased intake were conducted across a variety of strains, including Wistar rats. Again, very few studies have systematically examined the effects of restraint stress on alcohol consumption in different strains/lines of mice.

Forced swim

More recently, researchers have begun to explore the potential of other stressors such as forced swim to influence alcohol consumption. Generally, the forced swim test (FST) consists of inescapable exposure to a tank of room temperature water for a session ranging from 6 to 15 min in duration. Currently, there are a limited number of studies in which repeated exposure to the FST is reported to significantly elevate alcohol intake and, in most instances, the change in alcohol consumption was modest and transient in nature. This is true for studies conducted with rats (Fullgrabe et al. 2007; Siegmund et al. 2005) and mice (Cowen et al. 2003a, b; Sperling et al. 2010). On the other hand, there are several instances in which forced swim stress exposure either did not alter alcohol intake during the stress exposure phase in rats (Sommer et al. 2008; Vengeliene et al. 2003) and mice (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2007; Lowery et al. 2008) or even resulted in decreased alcohol intake (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2008). It is important to note that a few reports have observed significant FST-induced increases in alcohol intake long after the stress exposure ended, with these post-stress increases not emerging until at least 3 weeks later and only in certain mouse strains (e.g., BALB/cJ but not C57BL/6N, in Lowery et al. 2008) or genetic models (e.g., CRFR1 knockouts but not wild-type mice, in Sillaber et al. 2002).

Since duration of the swim test is relatively short, generally 5–10 min, and varies little across studies, this variable is unlikely to have contributed to the discordant results noted above. Additionally, it is difficult to conclude that the number of FST stress sessions relates to a particular outcome in these studies since alcohol drinking was typically assessed in the context of FST exposure being repeated over 2–5 days. For example, FST stress-induced increases in alcohol intake have been reported in both rats and mice following two (Cowen et al. 2003a, b) and three (Fullgrabe et al. 2007; Siegmund et al. 2005) days of repeated forced swim testing. Other studies, however, have indicated little change in alcohol consumption after a single acute FST session (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2007), or when FST testing was repeated for 3 days (Sillaber et al. 2002; Sommer et al. 2008; Vengeliene et al. 2003) or five consecutive days (Lowery et al. 2008; Mutschler et al. 2010). Although one study that used a more protracted period of FST exposures (14 consecutive days) reported stress-related decreases in consumption, this reduction was strain dependent, being significant in DBA/2J and BALB/cByJ strains but not in C57BL/6J mice (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2008). Furthermore, in another study by the same group (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2007), the same 14-day regimen of repeated FST exposures did not alter alcohol consumption, suggesting that a longer period of stress presentations in and of itself will not necessarily lead to an observation of FST-induced decreased alcohol intake. Taken together, it is difficult to attribute a specific effect of this stressor on alcohol drinking to the amount of forced swim stress exposure.

Since all of the studies examining FST exposure in Table 1 have involved providing animals with access to alcohol for at least 2 weeks prior to accessing the effects of the stressor on alcohol drinking, it is unlikely that this variable can effectively sort out the discrepant outcomes reported in these studies. Further, as noted above for footshock and restraint stressors, it is difficult to gauge the role of sex in defining the effects of restraint stress on alcohol consumption because so few studies have examined such effects in female subjects. Despite the relatively small number of studies conducted with rats, Wistar rats were shown to exhibit slight increases in alcohol intake following FST exposure (Fullgrabe et al. 2007; Siegmund et al. 2005; Vengeliene et al. 2003). Interestingly, FST exposure did not significantly alter alcohol intake in several lines of rats selectively bred for high alcohol preference (P, HAD, AA lines) (Vengeliene et al. 2003). Therefore, as with sex of the animal, strain of rat used is a variable that has not yet been systematically examined in the literature.

For studies that have utilized different mouse strains/genotypes, again, it is difficult to directly ascertain whether particular lines are more or less susceptible to the effects of FST on alcohol intake since so few studies have been conducted to specifically address this issue. For instance, though results from a few studies would suggest that C57BL/6 mice may be relatively insensitive to the effects of FST exposure on alcohol drinking (e.g., Boyce-Rustay et al. 2007, 2008; Lowery et al. 2008), another study recently reported that FST exposure increased alcohol consumption in this same inbred mouse strain (Sperling et al. 2010). Clearly, more studies are needed that directly compare the effects of FST stress on alcohol drinking in several mouse genotypes.

It is noteworthy that a unique feature of the forced swim stressor is that, as opposed to other stressors such as footshock and restraint, the FST procedure enables assessment of behavioral responsiveness to the stressor during its application. This is attractive because it provides an opportunity to gain insight about how individual differences in coping strategies exhibited during FST exposure may relate to subsequent avidity for alcohol. Unfortunately, to our knowledge, none of the studies examining the effects of an FST stressor on alcohol intake have attempted to relate behavioral (coping) response exhibited during FST exposure with later levels of alcohol intake. In a few instances, behavioral response during FST exposure was recorded in studies involving different age groups (e.g., Fullgrabe et al. 2007) or genetically manipulated lines (e.g., Cowen et al. 2003a, b; Sperling et al. 2010; Vengeliene et al. 2003). However, in these few cases, group differences that were either present or absent in FST-related behavior failed to explain later group differences in alcohol consumption. It is important to note that in these studies, individual relationships between FST behavior and later drinking within a group were not analyzed. Using a different approach, Weiss and his colleagues have shown that rats selectively bred for stress-induced suppression of struggling behavior in the FST procedure (the “stress susceptible” or “SUS” line) demonstrated increased levels of alcohol intake under basal (unstressed) conditions compared to a stress-resistant line and non-selected control line of rats. Further, the increased alcohol consumption in SUS rats, measured in a two-bottle choice situation, approached levels of intake and preference exhibited by rats selectively bred for high alcohol consumption (Weiss et al. 2008; West and Weiss 2006). In a similar vein, another study involving outbred CD1 mice examined the relationship between stress response in a related procedure (tail suspension test; TST) and later alcohol drinking. After characterizing mice as exhibiting high vs. low immobility in the TST, it was shown that the more stress-responsive mice consumed more alcohol in a two-bottle choice situation compared to mice that displayed less immobility (more struggling) in the task (Pelloux et al. 2005). This effect was only observed in females. Nevertheless, these latter findings suggest that more rigorous examination of individual differences in behavioral (coping) response to stressors such as the FST might provide additional insight into the complex relationship between stress responsiveness and propensity to drink. Clearly, this is an area of research that warrants further investigation.

Social defeat

This stressor is particularly ethologically relevant to rodents, containing both physical and psychological components (Miczek et al. 2008). Generally, experimental subjects are placed into the home cage of a “resident” (who has been shown to consistently display aggressive behavior towards intruders) for a short period of time, allowing for the resident to physically chase and attack the experimental subject. In some instances, a perforated divider is then placed in the cage to prevent the animals from physically interacting with each other, yet allowing the intruder to still see, smell, and hear the aggressive resident for a certain period of time. Studies investigating the effects of social defeat stress on the intruder rodent have shown that this stressor reduces both home cage (van Erp and Miczek 2001; van Erp et al. 2001) and operant self-administration (Funk et al. 2005; van Erp and Miczek 2001) of alcohol in rats. A few experiments that have investigated the effects of social defeat stress in mice have generally found little change in alcohol consumption during the stressor phase of the study (Croft et al. 2005; Sillaber et al. 2002). Interestingly, in both of these mouse studies post-stress increases in intake were observed, but not until several weeks after the conclusion of the social defeat experience. Since such a limited number of studies using social defeat stress have been conducted, elucidating the variables responsible for discrepancies across studies is not possible at this time. While evidence from rat studies might suggest that more intense social defeat sessions result in an even greater reduction in ethanol intake (e.g., van Erp et al. 2001), future research would need to validate and expand on this relationship between stressor duration/intensity and ethanol consumption before more firm conclusions are drawn.

Social isolation

Although the effects of individual housing on subsequent alcohol consumption have generally been examined following long-term periods of social isolation (see below), there have been a few instances in which shorter periods of isolation were investigated in rats. Not surprisingly, results regarding the effects of brief periods of social isolation have been mixed as well, with increased (Mediratta et al. 2003; Nunez et al. 2002, 1999), decreased (Doremus et al. 2005; Sprague and Maickel 1994), and unaltered alcohol intake (Nash and Maickel 1985) relative to non-stressed (group-housed) controls reported during the period of stressor exposure. It is important to note that in most of the studies in the literature that have examined the relationship between stress exposure and alcohol consumption, animals are individually housed in order to easily and more accurately assess alcohol intake of individual animals during the experiment. Since the few available studies that have examined the effects of social isolation for more brief periods after weaning have provided such conflicting results, future studies examining the influence of social isolation stress on alcohol consumption would be beneficial as they would help to inform researchers designing future studies regarding the potential influence that isolate housing may have on the results obtained.

Other stressors

In addition to the types of stressors utilized as described above, there are several other stress procedures that researchers have used to examine the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption. For example, tail pinch (Adams 1995), fear-conditioned memories (Bertotto et al. 2010), food restriction (Schroff et al. 2004), shock probe exposure (Sandbak and Murison 2001), saline injections (Little et al. 1999), repeated cage changes (O'Callaghan et al. 2002), ultrasonic noise (O'Callaghan et al. 2002) and overcrowding (Weisinger et al. 1989) are all stressors which have been studied for their ability to influence alcohol consumption, with again, these stressors varying in the direction of their effects on intake (see Table 1 for details).

Post-stress increases in alcohol consumption

When examining the literature concerning the effects of acute/sub-chronic stress exposure on alcohol intake, there have been several instances in which post-stress increases in intake have been observed (e.g., Casey 1960; Croft et al. 2005; Lynch et al. 1999; Nash and Maickel 1985; Volpicelli et al. 1986, 1990). In one study, the effect was reported to last for as many as 6 months after stress exposure (Sillaber et al. 2002). However, other groups have failed to observe delayed effects of stress on alcohol consumption (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2007; Fidler and LoLordo 1996; Tambour et al. 2008) or have found that post-stress elevations in intake are only observed under limited conditions—in certain genotypes and/or only one sex (e.g., Chester et al. 2006; Sillaber et al. 2002). Specifically, of the 78 individual citations listed in Table 1, 59 monitored ethanol intake for a period of time following the termination of stressor exposure. Of these 59 studies, only 41% (24 studies) reported a significant post-stress increase in intake, while the remaining 35 studies reported a decrease or no change in alcohol consumption. Therefore, a more general overview of the literature would indicate that increases in alcohol consumption during a period of time following termination of stress exposure are not observed in as many studies as is often thought.

Summary

Over several decades, a large body of work has been conducted in animals examining the influence of acute/sub-chronic stress experience on alcohol consumption. Various stress procedures have been employed using numerous models and a wide variety of experimental conditions. Despite a general perception in the field that stress is associated with increased alcohol drinking, as shown in Table 1, an overview of this literature reveals equivocal findings, with studies indicating increased, decreased, or no change in alcohol consumption under a variety of stress conditions. There have been multiple proposed hypotheses as to why models of acute/sub-chronic stress exposure do not always increase alcohol intake, and many of these revolve around specific experimental parameters that are thought to favor a particular outcome. These include, but are not limited to, the context in which the stressor is experienced in relation to drinking access (Caplan and Puglisi 1986; Volpicelli et al. 1982), controllability of the stressor (Volpicelli and Ulm 1990), and whether the stressor is signaled or unsignalled (Kinney and Schmidt 1979). While there may be general trends in the influence of these variables on stress-induced changes in alcohol consumption, there are exceptions in nearly every case, and these observations may no longer remain true when considering different species, strains, or stressors.

The factors contributing to stress-induced decreases in alcohol consumption are not entirely clear, although numerous explanations have been hypothesized. In some studies, it has been suggested that rats exhibiting a higher alcohol preference due to such factors as sweetening of the alcohol solution (Bond 1978), induction of drinking via forced consumption (Bond 1978), naturally occurring individual differences in consumption (Darnaudery et al. 2007), or genetic selection for high alcohol intake (Chester et al. 2004) are more likely to express stress-induced reductions in intake. However, this explanation may be too simplistic since there are other circumstances in which initial high alcohol preference was not associated with stress-related suppression of alcohol consumption (Boyce-Rustay et al. 2008; Fidler and LoLordo 1996; Powell et al. 1966). Finally, it should be noted that as indicated in Table 1, there are a substantial number of studies that report no change in alcohol consumption following acute/sub-chronic exposure to various stressors. No doubt, many factors including prior alcohol experience, predictability of the stressor, duration of stressor application, as well as the nature and intensity of the stressor contribute to these equivocal findings in the literature. In surveying this body of work, it is difficult to draw consensus about factors that contribute to a particular outcome (increased vs. decreased alcohol intake) because these variables do not seem to account for differences in results across studies in a systematic or reliable manner.

Chronic stress exposure before and/or during alcohol access

Several studies have centered on evaluation of chronic stress experience, especially during early development, as a risk factor for increased alcohol intake. These studies have mostly focused on consequences of long-term alterations in conditions of socialization, either through early maternal separation, housing manipulations (isolation vs. overcrowding), or evaluation of effects of social status on subsequent alcohol consumption. The main concept guiding these studies is that experience with chronic stress has long-lasting effects on stress- and anxiety-related behaviors which, in turn, play a role in modulating voluntary alcohol intake. These studies are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Chronic stress exposure before and/or during alcohol access

Stressor Species Decreased intake No change Increased intake
MS (short) Rat Weinberg 1987 Huot et al. 2001 Lancaster 1998
Hilakivi-Clarke et al. 1991 Roman et al. 2004
Ploj et al. 2003
Roman et al. 2003
Roman et al. 2005
Mouse Advani et al. 2007
MS (long) Rat Marmendal et al. 2004 Huot et al. 2001
Roman et al. 2004 Ploj et al. 2003
Gustafsson et al. 2005 Roman et al. 2005
Jaworski et al. 2005
Gustafsson and Nylander 2006
Mouse Cruz et al. 2008
Nonhuman primates Higley et al. 1991b
Fahlke et al. 2000
Barr et al. 2004
Barr et al. 2009
ISO Rat Fahlke et al. 1997 Parker and Radow 1974 Deatherage 1972
Pisu et al. 2011 Rockman et al. 1988 Rockman et al. 1987
Adams and Oldham 1996 Schenk et al. 1990
Lodge and Lawrence 2003a, b Wolffgramm 1990
Thorsell et al. 2005c Wolffgramm and Heyne 1991
Roske et al. 1994
Hall et al. 1998
Juarez and Vazquez-Cortes 2003
Deehan et al. 2007
Ehlers et al. 2007
McCool and Chappell 2009
Mouse Yanai and Ginsburg 1976
Advani et al. 2007
Lopez et al. 2011
Nonhuman primates Kraemer and McKinney 1985
McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008
OC Rat Deatherage 1972 Hannon and Donlon-Bantz 1976
Heminway and Furumoto 1972 Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2002
Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2003
Mouse Rodgers and Thiessen 1964
SS Rat Kulkosky et al. 1980 Ellison 1987 Blanchard et al. 1987
Wolffgramm and Heyne 1991
Pohorecky 2006
Pohorecky 2008
Pohorecky 2010
Mouse Kudryavtseva et al. 1991
Hilakivi-Clarke and Lister 1992
Kudryavtseva et al. 2006
Nonhuman primates Crowley and Andrews 1987 McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008
Crowley et al. 1990
SCC Rat Goodwin et al. 1999 Gauvin et al. 1997
Clark et al. 2007
Rosenwasser et al. 2010
Mouse Millard and Dole 1983
Trujillo et al. 2011
CVS Mouse Lopez et al. 2011

MS maternal separation, OC overcrowding, ISO isolate housing, SS social status, SCC shift in circadian cycle, CVS chronic variable stress

Maternal separation

Most of the studies that examined effects of early maternal separation in rodents reported decreased alcohol intake or no change in later drinking (for a review see Roman and Nylander 2005). This is especially the case when the duration of daily maternal separation was relatively short (<60 min; usually about 15 min). For example, except for one study (Lancaster 1998), daily separation of rat pups from their mothers for short time intervals during the first 3 weeks of life have generally resulted in decreased alcohol intake during adulthood (e.g., Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2005, 2003). In contrast, relatively long-term daily maternal separation (60–360 min per day) resulted in increased alcohol intake later in life (Huot et al. 2001; Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2005), although other studies reported no change in alcohol consumption (e.g., Jaworski et al. 2005; Marmendal et al. 2004). In these studies, maternal separation was repeated for several days, which varied across studies (7–20 days). However, it is the duration of each episode of maternal separation (short vs. long) that appears to determine the direction of the effect (increase vs. no change) rather than the number of days that maternal deprivation was experienced by the pups.

Maternal separation studies in rats have included use of several strains (e.g., Wistar, Long–Evans) as well as lines selectively bred for high alcohol intake (alcohol accepting; AA line). The only study that reported elevated alcohol intake following short duration maternal separation was conducted with Long–Evans rats (Lancaster 1998). The remaining studies cited in Table 2 that involved short-duration maternal separation reported either no change or decrease in alcohol intake, and none of these studies used Long–Evans rats. Thus, increased alcohol consumption following short-duration maternal separation may be unique to this particular strain of rats. In the case of long-duration maternal separation, increased alcohol consumption was observed across different strains of rats: Wistar (Ploj et al. 2003), Long–Evans (Huot et al. 2001), and AA (Roman et al. 2005). In most cases, the effect of maternal separation exposure on later alcohol consumption was similar in both male and female offspring. The one exception was an apparent sex-dependent increase in drinking observed only in male AA rats following long-duration maternal separation (Roman et al. 2005).

Studies examining the effects of maternal separation on alcohol self-administration in mice are more limited. In one study, repeated maternal separation prior to weaning did not alter subsequent alcohol intake in C57BL/6J mice, although increased alcohol consumption was reported in female offspring that experienced maternal separation and then were housed in isolation prior to testing (Advani et al. 2007). In another study, daily maternal separation (180 min) in outbred CFW mice produced higher levels of drinking and operant self-administration in adulthood, but progressive ratio testing did not reveal an apparent alteration in the reinforcing efficacy of alcohol (Cruz et al. 2008). Taken together, although there is limited evidence from studies conducted with mice, a similar pattern emerges in that short-duration maternal deprivation produces little change in later alcohol intake (Advani et al. 2007), while more prolonged maternal deprivation favors an outcome of increased alcohol consumption (Cruz et al. 2008).

Studies conducted with nonhuman primates have shown that maternal deprivation has long-lasting effects on behavioral and biological indices of stress and anxiety (Higley et al. 1991b). For example, peer-reared rhesus monkeys show profound alterations in neuroendocrine stress response (chronic elevation of cortisol and ACTH levels) when compared to mother-reared controls. Peer-reared monkeys also showed elevated voluntary alcohol intake in daily 1-h sessions, with intake achieving significantly higher blood alcohol concentrations compared to mother-reared monkeys (Higley et al. 1991a). In another study conducted with rhesus monkeys, although peer-reared and mother-reared subjects did not differ in magnitude of the stress response to social isolation during adulthood, altered stress responsiveness was related to voluntary alcohol consumption. That is, monkeys exhibiting a higher cortisol response to social separation consumed significantly more alcohol in a limited access (1 h) drinking situation (Fahlke et al. 2000). More recently, female macaques that were peer-reared were reported to evidence elevated alcohol intake compared to mother-reared subjects, with the effect modulated by genetic variation in genes related to serotonin regulation of the HPA axis (Barr et al. 2009, 2004).

Chronic isolation

Several studies have examined the effects of prolonged social isolation (typically accomplished by individual housing) on subsequent alcohol consumption. Although one study using rats reports decreased alcohol intake (Fahlke et al. 1997) and a few others report no effect of chronic isolation on later alcohol intake (e.g., Lodge and Lawrence 2003b; Rockman et al. 1988), the large preponderance of studies indicate that chronic isolation increases subsequent alcohol consumption in mouse, rat, and monkey species (see Table 2). For example, several studies conducted with different rat strains (e.g., Hall et al. 1998; Juarez and Vazquez-Cortes 2003) and rats selectively bred for high alcohol preference (Ehlers et al. 2007) have reported that chronic social isolation during adolescence leads to higher alcohol self-administration in adulthood. In a recent study, Long–Evans rats that were chronically isolated during adolescence were shown to exhibit higher levels of alcohol self-administration along with elevated behavioral measures of anxiety (McCool and Chappell 2009). Likewise, studies in mice have shown that chronic social isolation during adolescence results in increased alcohol consumption (Advani et al. 2007). In a recent study conducted in our laboratory, chronic social isolation during early development (14 days, starting at weaning) produced significantly higher voluntary alcohol consumption in male and female C57BL/6J mice compared to group-housed controls (Lopez et al. 2011). A common factor across all of these studies in rats and mice is that chronic social isolation during a critical period of development results in elevated alcohol intake later in adulthood. It is not clear whether chronic isolation during adulthood also affects alcohol intake. To our knowledge, only one study in rats (Schenk et al. 1990) and one study in mice (Lopez et al. 2011) have directly compared the effect of chronic social isolation exposure experience during adolescence vs. adulthood, and these results indicated that isolation housing for the same duration during adulthood did not alter subsequent alcohol intake. This suggests that the timing of chronic isolation stress experience is key, and that interactions with dynamic developmental changes (neural, endocrine) may underlie subsequent enhanced avidity for alcohol in rodents. Clearly, this issue deserves more experimental attention.

A couple of studies have evaluated the effects of chronic isolation on alcohol consumption in nonhuman primates. In one study, adult rhesus monkeys that were socially isolated intermittently (every other week) consumed more alcohol while isolated than during periods of group housing (Kraemer and McKinney 1985). Interestingly, monkeys that experienced intermittent (weekly) separations consumed more alcohol than those continuously isolated prior to and during alcohol access. Despite some methodological issues (alcohol intake when monkeys were together was estimated based on the group consumption from a single bottle), this study showed that social isolation induced higher alcohol intake (Kraemer and McKinney 1985). Other studies conducted with adult male and female squirrel monkeys showed that while acute (20 min) social separation resulted in reduced alcohol intake (and reduced intake of a control fluid) along with anxiety-like behavior, extending social isolation for a week produced selective elevation of alcohol consumption in the male subjects (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008; Miczek et al. 2008). This increase in alcohol intake returned to baseline levels when monkeys returned to their social housing conditions (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008; Miczek et al. 2008). Collectively, results from these studies indicate that the timing and duration of social isolation are critical factors that influence impact on alcohol drinking in adult monkeys. To our knowledge, the effects of chronic social isolation during early developmental periods (adolescence) on subsequent alcohol self-administration have not been systematically studied in monkeys.

Overcrowding

Several studies have evaluated the effect of overcrowding on voluntary alcohol intake in rodents. Results have been mixed and, unfortunately, in some cases methodological problems (e.g., using average group intake as dependent variable) have obscured study outcomes (Deatherage 1972; Heminway and Furumoto 1972). Studies in Wistar rats have shown that 7 or 14 days of overcrowding resulted in elevated alcohol intake when rats were subsequently individually housed for testing (Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2002, 2003). However, the overcrowding housing condition decreased food intake, leaving open the possibility that rats increased their alcohol consumption to compensate for reduced caloric intake (Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2002). Similar results were reported in another study conducted with Sprague–Dawley rats (Hannon and Donlon-Bantz 1976). In this study, overcrowding induced high alcohol intake in both male and female rats. Unlike the previously cited study (Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2002), overcrowding did not affect water or saccharin intake. However, the increase in alcohol intake was transient (5–7 days) and the authors speculate that once rats adapted to the housing situation, alcohol intake returned to baseline levels (Hannon and Donlon-Bantz 1976). Thus, while few studies have examined the effects of overcrowding on alcohol consumption, existing results do not suggest robust or durable effects in rodents.

Social status

A number of studies have examined the influence of dynamic social interactions in group-housed animals on alcohol consumption. In one study, Long–Evans rats housed in a colonial and environmentally enriched housing condition consumed less alcohol than rats housed in groups or individually in standard cages (Kulkosky et al. 1980). Whether an enriched housing environment and/or its interaction with opportunities for social interaction contribute to this outcome was not (and has not been) directly examined. Other studies using Long–Evans rats found that despite similar overall amount of alcohol consumed, intake was much more variable in group-housed rats compared to those individually housed. Further analysis revealed that the greater heterogeneity in alcohol intake among group-housed rats was attributed to higher levels of alcohol consumption in subordinate subjects (Blanchard et al. 1987). These results are congruent with an earlier study showing reduced alcohol intake in dominant Wistar rats (Wolffgramm and Heyne 1991). More recent studies evaluating the effect of social rank on alcohol intake using triads of Long–Evans rats also showed reduced alcohol intake in dominant rats relative to intake in the sub-dominant and subordinate animals (Pohorecky 2006, 2008, 2010). Studies in mice have generally shown similar results. For example, in one study, Swiss mice were housed in groups to determine social rank based on aggressive behavior (mice that evidenced wounds were identified as subordinates). Mice were then individually housed for alcohol intake assessment and results indicated that mice presenting with wounds showed the highest level of alcohol intake (Hilakivi-Clarke and Lister 1992). Similar results were obtained with C57BL/6J mice that were either identified and categorized based on expression of aggressive or submissive behavior (Kudryavtseva et al. 1991) or C57BL/6J mice that were continuously tested for social aggressive interaction while alcohol intake was assessed (Kudryavtseva et al. 2006). In both cases, mice that exhibited more submissive behavior or were defeated in social interaction tests consumed more alcohol. It should be noted that the effect of pairing the aggressive and subordinate mice on intake was not immediate and was observed only during the second week of pair housing (Kudryavtseva et al. 1991). Also, this effect was observed in C57BL/6J mice but not in CBA/lac mice (Kudryavtseva et al. 1991).

Only a very limited number of studies have examined the relation between social dominance and alcohol intake in nonhuman primates, and results have been mixed. Some studies have reported higher alcohol consumption in dominant monkeys (Crowley and Andrews 1987; Crowley et al. 1990), while other studies reported elevated drinking in monkeys with a lower social rank (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008; Miczek et al. 2008). There are several factors that might explain the opposing results observed in this set of studies. For example, two different species of monkeys were used: male Japanese Snow monkeys (Crowley and Andrews 1987; Crowley et al. 1990) and both male and female squirrel monkeys (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008). However, perhaps the most influencing factor was the procedure used to evaluate alcohol intake. In one case, monkeys had shared access to the alcohol bottle and, as might be predicted, dominant monkeys were able to gain more access to the alcohol bottles (Crowley and Andrews 1987; Crowley et al. 1990). However, when competition for alcohol access was relaxed and all monkeys had similar access to the drinking bottles, the differences in alcohol intake between dominant and subordinate monkeys disappeared (Crowley et al. 1990). Studies involving squirrel monkeys involved first determining social rank based on a dominance index and then the animals were individually given access to alcohol (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008).

Shifts in circadian cycle

When rodents are given free 24 h access to alcohol, consumption follows the pattern of water and food intake, with higher levels of consumption during the dark phase of the circadian cycle (Hiller-Sturmhofel and Kulkosky 2001; Spanagel et al. 2005). The introduction of a shift in the normal light/dark cycle has been proven to be stressful in rodents, adversely affecting the overall health of the subjects (Kort and Weijma 1982; Penev et al. 1998; Tsai et al. 2005). Only a few studies have examined the influence of circadian shifts on alcohol consumption. In one study, adult male Sprague–Dawley rats that experienced a single 8-h shift in the light/dark schedule as well as repeated changes in the lights-on schedule showed a significant increase in voluntary alcohol intake (Gauvin et al. 1997). Other studies conducted with male and female Fisher and Lewis rats (Rosenwasser et al. 2010) or male and female HAD1 rats (Clark et al. 2007) showed a significant decrease in alcohol intake when the animals were subjected to 6-h shifts in the light/dark cycle. In another study, male Wistar rats kept constantly in the dark or with the lights on showed reduced alcohol intake as well (Goodwin et al. 1999). Similar results were obtained with C57BL/6J mice that were switched from a 12:12-h light/dark cycle to a 6:6-h light/dark cycle (Millard and Dole 1983). Additionally, C57BL/6J mice, and the selectively bred high alcohol-preferring (HAP2) and low alcohol-preferring (LAP2) mice that experienced significant changes in their light/dark cycle schedules showed reduced alcohol intake (Trujillo et al. 2011). Taken together, most studies examining the effect of shifts in circadian cycles on voluntary alcohol drinking in rats and mice have shown significant reductions in alcohol intake. An explanation for this general outcome remains to be determined.

Chronic variable stress

Several studies that have shown that experience with chronic variable and unpredictable stress has long-lasting effects on HPA axis regulation and stress responsiveness (Flak et al. 2009; Jankord and Herman 2008; Ostrander et al. 2009; Zurita et al. 2000). There is also evidence from studies conducted with rats indicating that experience with chronic variable stress affects later responsiveness to drugs of abuse such as cocaine (Lepsch et al. 2005), morphine, (Molina et al. 1994) and amphetamine (Kabbaj et al. 2002; Lin et al. 2002). However, the effect of chronic variable stress on voluntary alcohol intake remains largely unexplored. In a recent study, exposure to variable and unpredictable mild stressors for 14 days during early development (starting at weaning) resulted in elevated alcohol intake in adult C57BL/6J mice using a two-bottle choice (alcohol vs. water) limited access (2 h/day) testing procedure (Lopez et al. 2011). Additional studies are needed to determine the generality of these effects, and whether similar findings are obtained if chronic variable stress is administered during adulthood.

Summary

A large portion of the studies presented in this section demonstrate that chronic stress exposure, especially when experienced during early life, can subsequently produce increased propensity to self-administer alcohol later in adulthood. Evidence in support of such observations come from studies involving mice, rats, and nonhuman primates, and they are based on the general premise that stressful conditions during early development induce long-lasting epigenetic, physiological, and behavioral alterations that impact motivation to drink alcohol (Higley et al. 1991a; Holmes et al. 2005). A review of this literature indicates that a number of procedural variables have significant modulating effects on the outcome. For example, studies involving maternal separation manipulations suggest that effects on subsequent alcohol drinking are dependent on the duration of the maternal separation episode as well as sex of the test subjects (Ploj et al. 2003; Roman et al. 2005). Most of the studies that used long periods of maternal separation reported higher voluntary ethanol intake, and this effect was observed in rats, mice, and nonhuman primates (see Table 2). In some instances, the strain of rats (Lancaster 1998) or the combination of a particular strain and sex (Roman et al. 2005) appeared to modulate the effect of maternal separation. Additionally, in the case of social isolation, the timing of chronic stress exposure appears important, but may be dependent on the species studied. For example, chronic social isolation in rodents can induce higher alcohol intake when the stress is experienced during early (but not later) development (Lopez et al. 2011; Schenk et al. 1990). However, in nonhuman primates, chronic isolation during adulthood also produced increases in voluntary alcohol intake (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008; Miczek et al. 2008). When considering the social status of rodents housed in groups, most studies have indicated either reduced intake in social dominant subjects (e.g., Pohorecky 2010; Wolffgramm and Heyne 1991) and/or elevated intake in subordinates (e.g., Hilakivi-Clarke and Lister 1992; Kudryavtseva et al. 2006). These results in rodent studies suggest there may be an inverse relationship between social rank and alcohol intake. However, studies conducted with nonhuman primates do not entirely support such a relationship. Specifically, some studies in monkeys have shown that dominant subjects drink more (Crowley and Andrews 1987; Crowley et al. 1990) while other studies indicate subordinate subjects drink more (McKenzie-Quirk and Miczek 2008; Miczek et al. 2008). Differences in monkey species and methods for assessing alcohol consumption may contribute to the mixed results. However, since so few studies have been conducted with nonhuman primates, it is difficult to discern which factors may contribute to higher or lower drinking in dominant subjects. Another issue to be considered relates to the persistence of effects on alcohol drinking. In some cases, the effects of maternal separation and chronic social isolation on later alcohol drinking were transient (Advani et al. 2007; Lancaster 1998; Lopez et al. 2011), while in other cases, the effects were more durable (Ehlers et al. 2007; Roman et al. 2005). At present, it is not known whether it is the characteristics of the stress experience, intervening variables, or an interaction of these factors that play a role in determining not only the direction, but also the persistence of changes in alcohol drinking. As a growing body of evidence suggests that various chronic stress procedures, especially when administered early in development, result in later increased alcohol drinking, more studies focused on examining (epi)genetic, physiological, and neural mechanisms underlying the phenomena are certainly warranted.

Chronic intermittent alcohol exposure as a stressor

As previously noted, it is well known that alcohol activates the HPA axis, and both clinical and experimental studies have documented profound disturbances in HPA axis function following chronic alcohol exposure and withdrawal. For example, in humans and rodents, chronic alcohol consumption results in a general elevation in blood corticosteroid levels, flattening of normal circadian fluctuations, and dampened HPA response to subsequent stress challenge (Errico et al. 1993; Kakihana and Moore 1976; Lee et al. 2000; Rasmussen et al. 2000; Tabakoff et al. 1978; Wand and Dobs 1991). While heightened HPA axis activation associated with withdrawal usually resolves within a few days (Adinoff et al. 1991; Tabakoff et al. 1978; Willenbring et al. 1984), blunted HPA axis responsiveness along with reduced basal levels of circulating corticosteroids appear to persist for a protracted period of time (Adinoff et al. 1990; Costa et al. 1996; Lovallo et al. 2000; Marchesi et al. 1997; Rasmussen et al. 2000; Zorrilla et al. 2001).

In addition to these HPA axis-related effects, alcohol alters CRF activity independent from the HPA axis (Heilig and Koob 2007; Koob and Zorrilla 2010; Uhart and Wand 2009). Increased CRF activity in several brain structures following chronic alcohol exposure represents an important neuroadaptive change that is thought to be key in the emergence of withdrawal-related anxiety and dysphoria, components of the alcohol withdrawal syndrome that are suggested to be intimately tied to alcohol drinking and relapse (Becker 1999, 2009; Heilig and Koob 2007; Koob and Kreek 2007; Koob 2003; Koob and Le Moal 2008). Thus, chronic alcohol exposure and withdrawal experience can be viewed as a potent stressor that disrupts the functional integrity of the HPA axis along with recruiting extrahypothalamic CRF systems, and this perturbation in brain/neuroendocrine stress axes may have significant implications regarding motivation for ethanol self-administration behavior.

Indeed, alcohol dependence has long been postulated to play a significant role in driving and maintaining excessive drinking. As shown in Table 3, numerous studies involving mice and rats have demonstrated escalated alcohol consumption using home cage free choice models and operant-conditioning procedures. In most cases, dependence has been induced by delivering alcohol vapor via inhalation chambers. For example, we have established a mouse model of dependence and relapse drinking that demonstrate repeated cycles of chronic alcohol exposure delivered by inhalation results in an escalation of voluntary alcohol drinking (Becker and Lopez 2004; Lopez and Becker 2005). More detailed analysis of the pattern of alcohol consumption revealed that dependent mice not only consumed a greater overall amount of alcohol compared to non-dependent mice, but also the rate of consumption was faster and progressively increased over successive withdrawal test periods (Griffin et al. 2009b). This enhanced alcohol consumption in dependent mice produced significantly higher and more sustained blood and brain alcohol levels compared to that achieved by more modest (stable) intake in non-dependent mice (Griffin et al. 2009b). Additionally, an increased number of cycles of chronic intermittent alcohol exposure resulted in greater and longer lasting enhancement of voluntary alcohol drinking (Griffin et al. 2009a; Lopez and Becker 2005). The mice are not food or water deprived and, importantly, the effect appears specific to alcohol because repeated cycles of chronic intermittent alcohol exposure did not produce alterations in water or sucrose intake (Becker and Lopez 2004). Others have reported similar results using inhalation procedures in mice (Dhaher et al. 2008; Finn et al. 2007) and in rats (Rimondini et al. 2002, Rimondini et al. 2003; Sommer et al. 2008). Likewise, studies using operant procedures have demonstrated increased alcohol self-administration in mice (Chu et al. 2007; Lopez et al. 2006) and rats (Gilpin et al. 2008b, 2009; O'Dell et al. 2004; Roberts et al. 1996, 2000) with a history of repeated chronic intermittent alcohol exposure, with evidence indicating that the reinforcing efficacy of ethanol is enhanced (Brown et al. 1998; Lopez et al. 2008). Further, studies in mice and rats have demonstrated that significant escalation of alcohol self-administration is facilitated when dependence is induced by delivery of chronic alcohol vapor exposure in an intermittent rather than continuous fashion (Lopez and Becker 2005; O'Dell et al. 2004). These latter findings suggest that repeated experience with alcohol withdrawal plays an important role in favoring excessive alcohol consumption associated with these models of dependence. Hence, it is reasonable to suggest that stress associated with chronic alcohol exposure and, in particular, repeated experience with withdrawal contributes to enhanced motivation to consume alcohol.

Table 3.

Chronic intermittent alcohol exposure effects on alcohol drinking

a

No drinking before inhalation

b

Liquid diet

Indeed, work in our laboratory and others have demonstrated that these models involving chronic intermittent alcohol exposure constitute potent stressors, as evidenced by initial HPA axis activation and then dysregulation of HPA axis activity (Lopez et al. 2010; Richardson et al. 2008). Additionally, several studies have shown that a history of dependence not only engenders elevated drinking, but also alters stress responsiveness as measured by several behavioral procedures (Breese et al. 2005; Gehlert et al. 2007; Sommer et al. 2008). This latter effect may be critical in rendering subjects more vulnerable to relapse and return to uncontrolled, harmful levels of alcohol consumption. Further, several studies have shown these models to engage various stress-related neuropeptides and modulators within brain stress–reward pathways that are postulated to contribute to drive/mediate excessive levels of alcohol drinking. For example, CRF (extrahypothalamic) (Finn et al. 2007; Gilpin et al. 2008a; Roberto et al. 2010; Sommer et al. 2008; Thorsell et al. 2005a), NPY (Gilpin et al. 2008c, 2011; Thorsell et al. 2005a, b), and opioid (Gilpin et al. 2008a; Walker and Koob 2008; Walker et al. 2011) systems have been implicated in excessive drinking that follows chronic intermittent alcohol exposure. Recent work in our laboratory indicates that with increased cycles of chronic intermittent alcohol exposure there is an apparent blunting of HPA axis activation, as measured by reduced levels of plasma corticosterone, while at the same time an accentuation of changes in CRF mRNA/peptide expression and release capacity in extrahypothalamic brain regions that are implicated in motivational effects of alcohol (Lopez et al. 2010; Griffin et al. 2011). These changes also have been accompanied by altered expression of the neuroactive steroid allopregnanolone in brain (Morrow et al. 2009). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) has been implicated in stress and addiction processes (Briand and Blendy 2010; Chourbaji et al. 2011; Davis 2008), and there is recent evidence indicating that brain regional changes in BDNF expression/activity following chronic alcohol exposure play a role in mediating withdrawal-related anxiety and regulation of alcohol consumption (Logrip et al. 2009; Pandey et al. 1999, 2006). Other stress-responsive systems (e.g., adrenergic, substance P, and orexin/hypocretin) have been shown to influence alcohol consumption (Ciccocioppo et al. 2009; Heilig et al. 2010; Sinha et al. 2011), but their role in mediating excessive drinking associated with dependence has not been specifically examined. Most of this work has focused on elucidating the role of these neurotransmitters and modulators within corticolimbic circuitry. Recently, basal ganglia circuitry, particularly the striatum, has been implicated in the transition to habit-like, inflexible patterns of behavior that result from chronic stress and chronic alcohol exposure, including excessive levels of drinking associated with dependence (Chen et al. 2011; Dias-Ferreira et al. 2009). Clearly, identifying neuro-adaptive changes within relevant motivational and stress pathways associated with dependence that promote/mediate excessive drinking is key to better understand the complex reciprocal relationship between stress and alcohol and conditions in which stress modulates drinking in the context of dependence.

Future directions: toward developing better animal models

As reviewed above, despite an extensive amount of research using animal models aimed at examining the effects of stress on alcohol drinking, results from this large body of work have been generally equivocal. Thus, while it is frequently argued that stress plays an important role in triggering relapse and maintaining harmful levels of drinking in the “real” world, this scenario has been difficult to reliably model in preclinical studies. Stress appraisal and response, as well as alcohol drinking are very complex and multifaceted behavioral phenotypes themselves. The biological underpinnings and behavioral processes regulating these phenomena are equally complex and dynamic. Nonetheless, in reviewing this relatively large body of literature, several general principles emerge that not only offer explanation for the diversity of outcomes reported, but also provide insight and guidance for future work that will be valuable in designing better animal models for studying interactions of stress and alcohol consumption.

Several genetic and biological variables play a clear role in modulating stress–alcohol interactions. In humans, an interplay between individual differences in stress appraisal and ability to mount effective coping strategies and individual differences in response to alcohol intoxication are likely to contribute to unique stress–alcohol interactions (Childs et al. 2011; Claessens et al. 2011; Williams et al. 2009). While the use of subhuman primates has several advantages as models of the human condition (e.g., genetic, endocrine, and cognitive), these studies have been relatively limited. Most of this work has been conducted using rodent models. However, there are important species-specific differences in response to stress and rarely has this issue been considered in evaluating the effects of a particular stressor on alcohol drinking in mice vs. rats. It is possible (and quite likely) that a distinct set of experimental conditions for studying stress effects on alcohol drinking may be optimal for one species but not for the other. In this vein, consideration of the ethological relevance of stimuli/events may be critical in formulating models to study stress effects on alcohol drinking in a given species. Additionally, there are known differences in stress responsiveness among different strains within a species, such as rats (Giorgi et al. 2003; Walker et al. 2009) and mice (Belzung and Griebel 2001; Millstein and Holmes 2007; Tannenbaum and Anisman 2003). This indicates that genetic factors play an important role in defining responses to stress and, presumably, impact on drinking. Physiological and/or behavioral measures of stress response should be incorporated in study designs to enable addressing this general issue as well as facilitate more informed comparisons across studies and different models.

In a variety of circumstances and testing conditions, genetic-defined differences in alcohol preference (either through comparison of inbred genotypes or selective breeding) have been shown to influence stress–alcohol drinking interactions. Unfortunately, there is no general consensus on whether propensity (high or low) to consume alcohol confers a unique response to the modulatory effects of stress on drinking. It is interesting that studies have not focused on genetic models that demonstrate differences in stress responsiveness (vulnerability or resiliency) as it relates to the capacity of stress to alter alcohol consumption. This may be a fruitful and complimentary avenue to pursue in examining genetic factors that shape stress–alcohol interactions. Sex/gender is a variable that has frequently been included in animal studies, yet a clear picture regarding the influence of this factor has not emerged from the extant literature. While age of the subjects has not been systematically examined, it is clear that stress experience early in life (especially during critical windows of development) can have long-lasting effects on both stress responsiveness and alcohol drinking. Such models appear useful in approximating the impact of early life trauma on propensity to later drink. Studies designed to identify underlying biological mediators of this effect will be important in developing interventions that most effectively prevent later vulnerability to excessive drinking.

As described in this review, a wide array of stress procedures has been employed to study the effects of stress on alcohol drinking in animals. It is now appreciated that not all stressors produce the same biological and behavioral response (Koolhaas et al. 2011). The ethological relevance and qualitative nature of a stressor (i.e., where it lies along a physical vs. psychogenic spectrum), along with physiological and behavioral responses evoked are important aspects of a stressor that undoubtedly contribute to its impact on alcohol consumption. Hence, it is not surprising that study outcomes related to alcohol drinking vary as a function of the stressor used. There is a clear need for systematic comparison of physiological (and when appropriate, behavioral) responses to different stress procedures, and how such effects impact the capacity of different stressors to exert unique modulatory effects on alcohol consumption. Aside from qualitative differences in stress (i.e., the nature of the stressor), there are also significant quantitative differences that are important to consider as well. Intensity, chronicity, and intermittency dimensions of stress have all been addressed in the literature. However, systematic evaluation of these characteristics using an experimental paradigm in which other factors are held constant is needed to help tease apart specific conditions and aspects of a given stressor that engender reliable changes in alcohol drinking. Likewise, predictability and controllability of stress has not been systematically examined in the context of animal models studying stress effects on alcohol self-administration. This represents another important opportunity for future efforts in addressing the role of cognitive aspects of stress that heretofore have not been adequately examined with regard to impact on motivation to drink in animal models. Clearly, the capacity of an organism to mount effective and appropriate coping mechanisms is critical for not only handling a stressful challenge at hand, but it is also likely to bear on motivation to engage in behaviors such as alcohol drinking that may be perceived, at least to some extent, as part of a coping response. Related to this issue is a key role for learning in stress–alcohol interactions. In general, there is a paucity of studies that focus on the role of associative factors in examining stress effects on alcohol drinking. Conditioned responses to alcohol and stress-related stimuli/events are critical for guiding future behavior, yet few studies have examined the relevance of this behavioral plasticity in relation to alcohol drinking. This includes analyses of the role of conditioning (learning) in both stress-modulating effects of alcohol and alcohol-modulatory effects on stress perception and response.

Alcohol drinking history is another important factor that influences the outcome of studies examining stress–alcohol interactions. Surprisingly, this variable has not been systematically examined. Although duration of prestress alcohol experience has typically not been examined, correlation analyses have attempted to relate prestress drinking levels with subsequent changes in drinking following stress exposure. The limited data available do not indicate a clear relationship. In contrast, a history of substantial alcohol experience has been shown to not only constitute a potent stressor itself, but also reliably engender elevated alcohol drinking. These studies have predominantly involved the use of inhalation to induce dependence and various self-administration models to assess subsequent effects on drinking in mice and rats. A general guiding principle that has emerged from these studies postulates that stress associated with chronic alcohol exposure and withdrawal experience continually challenges the physiological integrity of the organism through progressive dysregulation of peripheral and brain stress and reward systems beyond normal homeostatic limits. This, in turn, results in an allostatic state, which refers to a situation in which the organism attempts to maintain physiological stability in the face of continual perturbation (McEwen 2000). In this case, the allostatic state that arises as a result of chronic intoxication is characterized by neuroadaptive changes in brain reward and stress pathways that drive/promote escalation of drinking as well as alter stress responsiveness. In this vein, alcohol dependence may be viewed as a persistent allostatic state, with the organism rendered ill equipped to exert appropriate behavioral control over alcohol consumption, as well as appropriately respond to other (additional) stressful events that may provoke return to excessive drinking. Studies aimed at elucidating underlying neurobiological mechanisms in these models will be critical for advancing our understanding of how stress associated with prolonged alcohol exposure (i.e., dependence) influences motivation to drink, impacts physiological and behavioral response to stress, and influences the ability of stress to modulate alcohol drinking in the context of dependence.

There still remains the question as to why stress effects on alcohol drinking in animal models have produced such equivocal outcomes, but it has proven to be a very effective provocateur of relapse behavior in operant reinstatement paradigms. The latter findings involving reinstatement models have been reviewed elsewhere (Shaham et al. 2003; Shalev et al. 2002, 2010). As previously noted, an important component of operant reinstatement procedures is that stress-induced activation of responding for alcohol (and other drugs) is tested under extinction conditions. Thus, the use of such models does not afford the opportunity to examine stress effects on alcohol consumption per se. While operant self-administration procedures can effectively separate alcohol “seeking” and “taking” (consumption) components, few studies have applied this approach to study stress effects on alcohol self-administration behavior. Further, as advances have been made in identifying neural pathways involved in various aspects of alcohol/drug addiction and relapse, it will be valuable for future efforts to focus on elucidating overlapping vs. distinct neural circuitries that underlie alcohol “seeking” and drinking behaviors. This, in turn, may reveal unique mechanisms by which stress modulates motivational processes involved in procuring alcohol compared to those underlying cognitive and regulatory processes that govern control over alcohol consumption.

Finally, a number of methodological issues deserve attention. Most of the studies reviewed in this paper do not report whether a particular stress procedure employed was, in fact, an effective stressful challenge to the organism. Since inferences are made that exposure to stress produces change in alcohol consumption, it behooves investigators to demonstrate the effectiveness of the stress challenge using physiological and/or behavioral indices. This is especially relevant for studies examining chronic stress effects on alcohol drinking since animals may habituate to certain stressors when repeatedly exposed. Such outcomes may point to potential dissociation between physiological responses to a stressor and its ability to modify alcohol drinking. Thus, while measures of HPA axis activity (plasma corticosteroid levels) may habituate with repeated stress exposure, adaptive changes in other brain stress pathways may become of greater significance with regard to influences on motivation to consume alcohol. Another important consideration is the manner in which alcohol intake is assessed, as it relates to the extent of stress-induced changes in alcohol consumption. In many cases, stress is reported to produce a statistically significant change in alcohol intake but the alteration in consumption may be of minimal or marginal biological relevance. Relatively small, albeit statistically significant changes in alcohol intake over long access periods (e.g., 24 h) may be of questionable relevance if it cannot be demonstrated that a large proportion of the change occurs within a specific time frame, thereby producing a significant elevation in blood alcohol concentrations. Thus, it is critical for such studies to incorporate measures of blood alcohol levels associated with alcohol intake so that the magnitude of stress-modulated drinking can be gauged in proper context. Another issue concerns the temporal relationship between stress exposure and access to alcohol. Many studies examine effects of stress on alcohol drinking where the stress experience occurs concurrent with alcohol access. In other cases, alcohol consumption is assessed during a period after stress exposure is terminated. These scenarios provide valuable information regarding the acute vs. protracted effects of stress on alcohol consumption. Also, timing alcohol access to occur either before or after stress exposure enables analysis of whether changes in drinking manifest as a “coping” strategy (i.e., drinking in anticipation of stress vs. drinking as a consequence of stress). Systematic analysis of this issue with inclusion of appropriate control conditions deserves more experimental attention in animal models. Finally, some studies have reported changes in drinking after long delays since stress experience. Such outcomes suggest underlying long-lasting adaptations that eventually become expressed through altered alcohol consumption. Identifying the nature of these changes will be critical for understanding factors that mediate and influence stress–alcohol interactions. This, in turn, will help guide more refined models that better approximate the complex reciprocal relationship between stress and alcohol drinking in clinical settings.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the NIH/NIAAA-sponsored Integrative Neuroscience Initiative on Alcoholism (INIAstress) Consortium (grant U01 AA014095).

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest The authors do not have any conflicts of interest to report in connection with this manuscript.

Contributor Information

Howard C. Becker, Email: beckerh@musc.edu, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Charleston Alcohol Research Center, Center for Drug and Alcohol Programs, 67 President Street, MSC 861, Charleston, SC 29425, USA; Department of Neurosciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC 29425, USA; RHJ Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Charleston, SC 29425, USA.

Marcelo F. Lopez, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Charleston Alcohol Research Center, Center for Drug and Alcohol Programs, 67 President Street, MSC 861, Charleston, SC 29425, USA

Tamara L. Doremus-Fitzwater, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Charleston Alcohol Research Center, Center for Drug and Alcohol Programs, 67 President Street, MSC 861, Charleston, SC 29425, USA

References

  1. Abraham ME, Gogate MG. Effect of stress on behaviour in rats. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 1989;33:84–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adams N. Sex differences and the effects of tail pinch on ethanol drinking in Maudsley rats. Alcohol. 1995;12:463–468. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(95)00032-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adams N, Oldham TD. Seminatural housing increases subsequent ethanol intake in male Maudsley Reactive rats. J Stud Alcohol. 1996;57:349–351. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1996.57.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adinoff B, Martin PR, Bone GH, Eckardt MJ, Roehrich L, George DT, Moss HB, Eskay R, Linnoila M, Gold PW. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning and cerebrospinal fluid corticotropin releasing hormone and corticotropin levels in alcoholics after recent and long-term abstinence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:325–330. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810160025004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Adinoff B, Risher-Flowers D, De Jong J, Ravitz B, Bone GH, Nutt DJ, Roehrich L, Martin PR, Linnoila M. Disturbances of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning during ethanol withdrawal in six men. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:1023–1025. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.8.1023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Advani T, Hensler JG, Koek W. Effect of early rearing conditions on alcohol drinking and 5-HT1A receptor function in C57BL/6J mice. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;10:595–607. doi: 10.1017/S1461145706007401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Anisman H, Waller TG. Effects of inescapable shock and shock-produced conflict on self selection of alcohol in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1974;2:27–33. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(74)90131-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barr CS, Newman TK, Lindell S, Shannon C, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Suomi SJ, Goldman D, Higley JD. Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variation and rearing condition in alcohol preference and consumption in female primates. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:1146–1152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barr CS, Dvoskin RL, Gupte M, Sommer W, Sun H, Schwandt ML, Lindell SG, Kasckow JW, Suomi SJ, Goldman D, Higley JD, Heilig M. Functional CRH variation increases stress-induced alcohol consumption in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:14593–14598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902863106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Battista SR, Stewart SH, Ham LS. A critical review of laboratory-based studies examining the relationships of social anxiety and alcohol intake. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. 2010;3:3–22. doi: 10.2174/1874473711003010003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Becker HC. Alcohol withdrawal: neuroadaptation and sensitization. CNS Spectr. 1999;4:38–65. [Google Scholar]
  12. Becker HC. Alcohol dependence, withdrawal and relapse. Alcohol Res Health. 2009;31:348–361. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Becker HC, Lopez MF. Increased ethanol drinking after repeated chronic ethanol exposure and withdrawal experience in C57BL/6 mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2004;28:1829–1838. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000149977.95306.3a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Belzung C, Griebel G. Measuring normal and pathological anxiety-like behavior in mice: a review. Behav Brain Res. 2001;125:141–149. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00291-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bertholomey ML, Henderson AN, Badia-Elder NE, Stewart RB. Neuropeptide Y (NPY)-induced reductions in alcohol intake during continuous access and following alcohol deprivation are not altered by restraint stress in alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2011;97:453–461. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bertotto ME, Bussolino DF, Molina VA, Martijena ID. Increased voluntary ethanol consumption and c-Fos expression in selected brain areas induced by fear memory retrieval in ethanol withdrawn rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20:568–581. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Blanchard RJ, Hori K, Tom P, Blanchard DC. Social structure and ethanol consumption in the laboratory rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1987;28:437–442. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(87)90502-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bond NW. Shock induced alcohol consumption in rats: role of initial preference. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1978;9:39–42. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(78)90010-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bowers WJ, Sabongui AG, Amit Z. The role of ethanol availability on stress-induced increases in ethanol consumption. Alcohol. 1997;14:551–556. doi: 10.1016/s0741-8329(96)00046-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Boyce-Rustay JM, Cameron HA, Holmes A. Chronic swim stress alters sensitivity to acute behavioral effects of ethanol in mice. Physiol Behav. 2007;91:77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Boyce-Rustay JM, Janos AL, Holmes A. Effects of chronic swim stress on EtOH-related behaviors in C57BL/6J, DBA/2J and BALB/cByJ mice. Behav Brain Res. 2008;186:133–137. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Brady KT, Sonne SC. The role of stress in alcohol use, alcoholism treatment, and relapse. Alcohol Res Health. 1999;23:263–271. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Breese GR, Overstreet DH, Knapp DJ, Navarro M. Prior multiple ethanol withdrawals enhance stress-induced anxiety-like behavior: inhibition by CRF1- and benzodiazepine-receptor antagonists and a 5-HT1a-receptor agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1662–1669. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Briand LA, Blendy JA. Molecular and genetic substrates linking stress and addiction. Brain Res. 2010;1314:219–234. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Brown G, Jackson A, Stephens DN. Effects of repeated withdrawal from chronic ethanol on oral self-administration of ethanol on a progressive ratio schedule. Behav Pharmacol. 1998;9:149–161. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Brunell SC, Spear LP. Effect of stress on the voluntary intake of a sweetened ethanol solution in pair-housed adolescent and adult rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1641–1653. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000179382.64752.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Caplan MA, Puglisi K. Stress and conflict conditions leading to and maintaining voluntary alcohol consumption in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1986;24:271–280. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90350-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cappell H, Greeley J. Guilford, New York: 1987. Alcohol and tension reduction: an update on research and theory. [Google Scholar]
  29. Carrigan MH, Randall CL. Self-medication in social phobia: a review of the alcohol literature. Addict Behav. 2003;28:269–284. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00235-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Casey A. The effect of stress on the consumption of alcohol and reserpine. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1960;21:208–216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Champagne F, Kirouac G. Effects of unavoidable electric shocks on voluntary alcohol consumption in the rat. Percept Mot Skills. 1987;64:335–338. doi: 10.2466/pms.1987.64.1.335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chen G, Cuzon Carlson VC, Wang J, Beck A, Heinz A, Ron D, Lovinger DM, Buck KJ. Striatal involvement in human alcoholism and alcohol consumption, and withdrawal in animal models. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35 doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01520.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Chester JA, Blose AM, Zweifel M, Froehlich JC. Effects of stress on alcohol consumption in rats selectively bred for high or low alcohol drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2004;28:385–393. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000117830.54371.7a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Chester JA, de Paula BG, DeMaria A, Finegan A. Different effects of stress on alcohol drinking behaviour in male and female mice selectively bred for high alcohol preference. Alcohol Alcsm. 2006;41:44–53. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agh242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Chester JA, Barrenha GD, Hughes ML, Keuneke KJ. Age- and sex-dependent effects of footshock stress on subsequent alcohol drinking and acoustic startle behavior in mice selectively bred for high-alcohol preference. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:1782–1794. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00763.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Childs E, O’Connor S, de Wit H. Bilateral interactions between acute psychosocial stress and acute intravenous alcohol in healthy men. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35 doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01522.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Choca JP, Wilson AS, Garcia TJ. Effects of shock and darkness on alcohol consumption by rats. J Stud Alcohol. 1977;38:2184–2187. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1977.38.2184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Chourbaji S, Branwein C, Gass P. Altering BDNF expression by genetics and/or environment: impact for emotional and depression-like behaviour in laboratory mice. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:599–611. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Chu K, Koob GF, Cole M, Zorrilla EP, Roberts AJ. Dependence-induced increases in ethanol self-administration in mice are blocked by the CRF1 receptor antagonist antalarmin and by CRF1 receptor knockout. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86:813–821. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Chutuape MA, de Wit H. Preferences for ethanol and diazepam in anxious individuals: an evaluation of the self-medication hypothesis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;121:91–103. doi: 10.1007/BF02245595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ciccocioppo R, Gehlert DR, Ryabinin A, Kaur S, Cippitelli A, Thorsell A, Le AD, Hipskind PA, Hamdouchi C, Lu J, Hembre EJ, Cramer J, Song M, McKinzie D, Morin M, Economidou D, Stopponi S, Cannella N, Braconi S, Kallupi M, de Guglielmo G, Massi M, George DT, Gilman J, Hersh J, Tauscher JT, Hunt SP, Hommer D, Heilig M. Stress-related neuropeptides and alcoholism: CRH, NPY, and beyond. Alcohol. 2009;43:491–498. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Cicero TJ, Myers RD, Black WC. Increase in volitional ethanol consumption following interference with a learned avoidance response. Physiol Behav. 1968;3:657–660. [Google Scholar]
  43. Claessens SE, Daskalakis NP, van der Veen R, Oitzl MS, de Kloet ER, Champagne DL. Development of individual differences in stress responsiveness: an overview of factors mediating the outcome of early life experiences. Psychopharmacology. 2011;214:141–154. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2118-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Clark JW, Fixaris MC, Belanger GV, Rosenwasser AM. Repeated light-dark phase shifts modulate voluntary ethanol intake in male and female high alcohol-drinking (HAD1) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1699–1706. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00476.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Clarke TK, Treutlein J, Zimmermann US, Kiefer F, Skowronek MH, Rietschel M, Mann K, Schumann G. HPA-axis activity in alcoholism: examples for a gene-environment interaction. Addict Biol. 2008;13:1–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2007.00084.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Cleck JN, Blendy JA. Making a bad thing worse: adverse effects of stress on drug addiction. J Clin Invest. 2008;118:454–461. doi: 10.1172/JCI33946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Costa A, Bono G, Martignoni E, Merlo P, Sances G, Nappi G. An assessment of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning in non-depressed, early abstinent alcoholics. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1996;21:263–275. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(96)00001-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Cowen MS, Schroff KC, Gass P, Sprengel R, Spanagel R. Neurobehavioral effects of alcohol in AMPA receptor subunit (GluR1) deficient mice. Neuropharmacology. 2003a;45:325–333. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(03)00174-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Cowen MS, Schumann G, Yagi T, Spanagel R. Role of Fyn tyrosine kinase in ethanol consumption by mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003b;27:1213–1219. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000081630.14159.02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Cox WM, Stainbrook GL. Stress-induced alcohol consumption: a new paradigm. Alcohol Alcsm. 1977;12:23–29. [Google Scholar]
  51. Crabbe JC. Neurogenetic studies on alcohol addiction. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363:3201–3211. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Crabbe JC, Phillips TJ, Harris RA, Arends MA, Koob GF. Alcohol-related genes: contributions from studies with genetically engineered mice. Addict Biol. 2006;11:195–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00038.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Croft AP, Brooks SP, Cole J, Little HJ. Social defeat increases alcohol preference of C57BL/10 strain mice; effect prevented by a CCKB antagonist. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:163–170. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0165-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Crowley TJ, Andrews AE. Alcoholic-like drinking in simian social groups. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987;92:196–205. doi: 10.1007/BF00177915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Crowley TJ, Williams EA, Jones RH. Initiating ethanol drinking in a simian social group in a naturalistic setting. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1990;14:444–455. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb00502.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Cruz FC, Quadros IM, Planeta Cda S, Miczek KA. Maternal separation stress in male mice: long-term increases in alcohol intake. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;201:459–468. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1307-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Darnaudery M, Louvart H, Defrance L, Leonhardt M, Morley-Fletcher S, Gruber SH, Galietta G, Mathe AA, Maccari S. Impact of an intense stress on ethanol consumption in female rats characterized by their pre-stress preference: modulation by prenatal stress. Brain Res. 2007;1131:181–186. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Davis MI. Ethanol-BDNF interactions: still more questions than answers. Pharmacol Ther. 2008;118:36–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. de Wit H, Soderpalm AH, Nikolayev L, Young E. Effects of acute social stress on alcohol consumption in healthy subjects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27:1270–1277. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000081617.37539.D6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Deatherage G. Effects of housing density on alcohol intake in the rat. Physiol Behav. 1972;9:55–57. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(72)90264-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Deehan GA, Jr, Cain ME, Kiefer SW. Differential rearing conditions alter operant responding for ethanol in outbred rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1692–1698. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00466.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Deroche V, Piazza PV, Le Moal M, Simon H. Individual differences in the psychomotor effects of morphine are predicted by reactivity to novelty and influenced by corticosterone secretion. Brain Res. 1993;623:341–344. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91451-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Derr R, Lindblad S. Stress-induced consumption of ethanol by rats. Life Sci. 1980;27:2183–2186. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(80)90382-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Dhaher R, Finn D, Snelling C, Hitzemann R. Lesions of the extended amygdala in C57BL/6J mice do not block the intermittent ethanol vapor-induced increase in ethanol consumption. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:197–208. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00566.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Dias-Ferreira E, Sousa JC, Melo I, Morgado P, Mesquita AR, Cerqueira JJ, Costa RM, Sousa N. Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science. 2009;325:621–625. doi: 10.1126/science.1171203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Doremus TL, Brunell SC, Rajendran P, Spear LP. Factors influencing elevated ethanol consumption in adolescent relative to adult rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1796–1808. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000183007.65998.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ehlers CL, Walker BM, Pian JP, Roth JL, Slawecki CJ. Increased alcohol drinking in isolate-housed alcohol-preferring rats. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121:111–119. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.1.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ellison G. Stress and alcohol intake: the socio-pharmacological approach. Physiol Behav. 1987;40:387–392. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90066-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Errico AL, Parsons OA, King AC, Lovallo WR. Attenuated cortisol response to biobehavioral stressors in sober alcoholics. J Stud Alcohol. 1993;54:393–398. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1993.54.393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Fahlke C, Hansen S. Effect of local intracerebral corticosterone implants on alcohol intake in the rat. Alcohol Alcsm. 1999;34:851–861. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/34.6.851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Fahlke C, Engel JA, Eriksson CJ, Hard E, Soderpalm B. Involvement of corticosterone in the modulation of ethanol consumption in the rat. Alcohol. 1994a;11:195–202. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(94)90031-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Fahlke C, Hard E, Thomasson R, Engel JA, Hansen S. Metyrapone-induced suppression of corticosterone synthesis reduces ethanol consumption in high-preferring rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994b;48:977–981. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90208-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Fahlke C, Hard E, Eriksson CJ, Engel JA, Hansen S. Consequence of long-term exposure to corticosterone or dexamethasone on ethanol consumption in the adrenalectomized rat, the effect of type I and type II corticosteroid receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;117:216–224. doi: 10.1007/BF02245190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Fahlke C, Hard E, Hansen S. Facilitation of ethanol consumption by intracerebroventricular infusions of corticosterone. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;127:133–139. doi: 10.1007/BF02805986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Fahlke C, Hard E, Eriksson CJ. Effects of early weaning and social isolation on subsequent alcohol intake in rats. Alcohol. 1997;14:175–180. doi: 10.1016/s0741-8329(96)00141-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Fahlke C, Lorenz JG, Long J, Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Higley JD. Rearing experiences and stress-induced plasma cortisol as early risk factors for excessive alcohol consumption in nonhuman primates. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24:644–650. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Fidler TL, LoLordo VM. Failure to find postshock increases in ethanol preference. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:110–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01053.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Finn DA, Snelling C, Fretwell AM, Tanchuck MA, Underwood L, Cole M, Crabbe JC, Roberts AJ. Increased drinking during withdrawal from intermittent ethanol exposure is blocked by the CRF receptor antagonist D-Phe-CRF(12–41) Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:939–949. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00379.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Flak JN, Ostrander MM, Tasker JG, Herman JP. Chronic stress-induced neurotransmitter plasticity in the PVN. J Comp Neurol. 2009;517:156–165. doi: 10.1002/cne.22142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Fullgrabe MW, Vengeliene V, Spanagel R. Influence of age at drinking onset on the alcohol deprivation effect and stress-induced drinking in female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86:320–326. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Funk CK, Koob GF. A CRF(2) agonist administered into the central nucleus of the amygdala decreases ethanol self-administration in ethanol-dependent rats. Brain Res. 2007;1155:172–178. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Funk D, Harding S, Juzytsch W, Le AD. Effects of unconditioned and conditioned social defeat on alcohol self-administration and reinstatement of alcohol seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:341–349. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0194-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Funk CK, O'Dell LE, Crawford EF, Koob GF. Corticotropin-releasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats. J Neurosci. 2006;26:11324–11332. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3096-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Gauvin DV, Baird TJ, Vanecek SA, Briscoe RJ, Vallett M, Holloway FA. Effects of time-of-day and photoperiod phase shifts on voluntary ethanol consumption in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1997;21:817–825. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Gehlert DR, Cippitelli A, Thorsell A, Le AD, Hipskind PA, Hamdouchi C, Lu J, Hembre EJ, Cramer J, Song M, McKinzie D, Morin M, Ciccocioppo R, Heilig M. 3-(4-Chloro-2-morpholin-4-ylthiazol-5-yl)-8-(1-ethylpropyl)-2,6-dimethyl-imidazo[1,2-b]pyridazine: a novel brain-penetrant, orally available corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 antagonist with efficacy in animal models of alcoholism. J Neurosci. 2007;27:2718–2726. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4985-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Gilpin NW, Richardson HN, Koob GF. Effects of CRF1-receptor and opioid-receptor antagonists on dependence-induced increases in alcohol drinking by alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008a;32:1535–1542. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00745.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Gilpin NW, Richardson HN, Lumeng L, Koob GF. Dependence-induced alcohol drinking by alcohol-preferring (P) rats and outbred Wistar rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008b;32:1688–1696. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00678.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Gilpin NW, Stewart RB, Badia-Elder NE. Neuropeptide Y administration into the amygdala suppresses ethanol drinking in alcohol-preferring (P) rats following multiple deprivations. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008c;90:470–474. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Gilpin NW, Smith AD, Cole M, Weiss F, Koob GF, Richardson HN. Operant behavior and alcohol levels in blood and brain of alcohol-dependent rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33:2113–2123. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01051.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Gilpin NW, Misra K, Herman MA, Cruz MT, Koob GF, Roberto M. Neuropeptide Y opposes alcohol effects on gamma-aminobutyric acid release in amygdala and blocks the transition to alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:1091–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Giorgi O, Lecca D, Piras G, Driscoll P, Corda MG. Dissociation between mesocortical dopamine release and fear-related behaviours in two psychogenetically selected lines of rats that differ in coping strategies to aversive outcomes. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:2716–2726. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02689.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Goldstein DS, Kopin IJ. Evolution of concepts of stress. Stress. 2007;10:109–120. doi: 10.1080/10253890701288935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Goodwin FL, Amir S, Amit Z. Environmental lighting has a selective influence on ethanol intake in rats. Physiol Behav. 1999;66:323–328. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00302-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Griffin WC, Lopez MF, Becker HC. Intensity and duration of chronic ethanol exposure is critical for subsequent escalation of voluntary ethanol drinking in mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009a;33(11):1893–1900. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01027.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Griffin WC, Lopez MF, Yanke AB, Middaugh LD, Becker HC. Repeated cycles of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure in mice increases voluntary ethanol drinking and ethanol concentrations in the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009b;201:569–580. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1324-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Griffin WC, Overstreet MP, Becker HC. Chronic intermitternt ethanol exposure alters CRF release in the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:69A. [Google Scholar]
  97. Gustafsson L, Nylander I. Time-dependent alterations in ethanol intake in male wistar rats exposed to short and prolonged daily maternal separation in a 4-bottle free-choice paradigm. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30:2008–2016. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00247.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Gustafsson L, Ploj K, Nylander I. Effects of maternal separation on voluntary ethanol intake and brain peptide systems in female Wistar rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;81:506–516. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Haleem DJ. Adaptation to repeated restraint stress in rats: failure of ethanol-treated rats to adapt in the stress schedule. Alcohol Alcsm. 1996;31:471–477. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.alcalc.a008181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Hall FS, Huang S, Fong GW, Pert A, Linnoila M. Effects of isolation-rearing on voluntary consumption of ethanol, sucrose and saccharin solutions in Fawn Hooded and Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998;139:210–216. doi: 10.1007/s002130050706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Hannon R, Donlon-Bantz K. Effect of housing density on alcohol consumption by rats. J Stud Alcohol. 1976;37:1556–1563. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1976.37.1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Heilig M, Koob GF. A key role for corticotropin-releasing factor in alcohol dependence. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:399–406. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Heilig M, Egli M, Crabbe JC, Becker HC. Acute withdrawal, protracted abstinence and negative affect in alcoholism: are they linked? Addict Biol. 2010;15:169–184. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00194.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Heminway DA, Furumoto L. Population density and alcohol consumption in the rat. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1972;33:794–799. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Higley JD, Hasert MF, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M. Nonhuman primate model of alcohol abuse: effects of early experience, personality, and stress on alcohol consumption. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1991a;88:7261–7265. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.16.7261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M. CSF monoamine metabolite concentrations vary according to age, rearing, and sex, and are influenced by the stressor of social separation in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1991b;103:551–556. doi: 10.1007/BF02244258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Hilakivi-Clarke L, Lister RG. Social status and voluntary alcohol consumption in mice: interaction with stress. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1992;108:276–282. doi: 10.1007/BF02245112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Hilakivi-Clarke LA, Turkka J, Lister RG, Linnoila M. Effects of early postnatal handling on brain beta-adrenoceptors and behavior in tests related to stress. Brain Res. 1991;542:286–292. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91580-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Hiller-Sturmhofel S, Kulkosky P. Chronobiological regulation of alcohol intake. Alcohol Res Health. 2001;25:141–148. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Holmes A. Genetic variation in cortico-amygdala serotonin function abd risk for stress-related disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:1293–1314. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Holmes A, le Guisquet AM, Vogel E, Millstein RA, Leman S, Belzung C. Early life genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors shaping emotionality in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1335–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Meaney MJ, Plotsky PM. Development of adult ethanol preference and anxiety as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in Long Evans rats and reversal with antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;158:366–373. doi: 10.1007/s002130100701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Jankord R, Herman JP. Limbic regulation of hypothalamopituitary-adrenocortical function during acute and chronic stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1148:64–73. doi: 10.1196/annals.1410.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Jaworski JN, Francis DD, Brommer CL, Morgan ET, Kuhar MJ. Effects of early maternal separation on ethanol intake, GABA receptors and metabolizing enzymes in adult rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;181:8–15. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2232-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Juarez J, Vazquez-Cortes C. Alcohol intake in social housing and in isolation before puberty and its effects on voluntary alcohol consumption in adulthood. Dev Psychobiol. 2003;43:200–207. doi: 10.1002/dev.10133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Kabbaj M, Isgor C, Watson SJ, Akil H. Stress during adolescence alters behavioral sensitization to amphetamine. Neuroscience. 2002;113:395–400. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00188-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Kakihana R, Moore JA. Circadian rhythm of corticosterone in mice: the effect of chronic consumption of alcohol. Psychopharmacologia. 1976;46:301–305. doi: 10.1007/BF00421118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Kinney L, Schmidt H., Jr Effect of cued and uncued inescapable shock on voluntary alcohol consumption in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1979;11:601–604. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(79)90249-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Koob GF. Alcoholism: allostasis and beyond. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27:232–243. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000057122.36127.C2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Koob G, Kreek MJ. Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1149–1159. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05030503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:29–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Koob GF, Zorrilla EP. Neurobiological mechanisms of addiction: focus on corticotropin-releasing factor. Curr Opin Investig Drugs. 2010;11:63–71. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Koolhaas JM, Bartolomussi A, Buwalda B, de Boer SF, Flugge G, Korte SM, Meerlo P, Murison R, Olivier B, Palanza P, Richter-Levin G, Sgoifo A, Steimer T, Stiedl O, van Dijk G, Wöhr M, Fuchs E. Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1291–1301. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Kopin IJ. Definitions of stress and sympathetic neuronal responses. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1995;771:19–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb44667.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Kort WJ, Weijma JM. Effect of chronic light-dark shift stress on the immune response of the rat. Physiol Behav. 1982;29:1083–1087. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(82)90302-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Kraemer GW, McKinney WT. Social separation increases alcohol consumption in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1985;86:182–189. doi: 10.1007/BF00431706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Krishnan S, Nash JF, Jr, Maickel RP. Free-choice ethanol consumption by rats: effects of ACTH4-10. Alcohol. 1991;8:401–404. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(91)90663-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Kudryavtseva NN, Madorskaya IA, Bakshtanovskaya IV. Social success and voluntary ethanol consumption in mice of C57BL/6J and CBA/Lac strains. Physiol Behav. 1991;50:143–146. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90511-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Kudryavtseva N, Gerrits MA, Avgustinovich DF, Tenditnik MV, Van Ree JM. Anxiety and ethanol consumption in victorious and defeated mice; effect of kappa-opioid receptor activation. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006;16:504–511. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Kulkosky PJ, Zellner DA, Hyson RL, Riley AL. Ethanol consumption of rats in individual, group, and colonial housing conditions. Physiol Psychol. 1980;8:56–60. [Google Scholar]
  132. Lamblin F, De Witte P. Adrenalectomy prevents the development of alcohol preference in male rats. Alcohol. 1996;13:233–238. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(95)02043-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Lancaster FE. Sex differences in voluntary drinking by Long Evans rats following early stress. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:830–836. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Le AD, Quan B, Juzytch W, Fletcher PJ, Joharchi N, Shaham Y. Reinstatement of alcohol-seeking by priming injections of alcohol and exposure to stress in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998;135:169–174. doi: 10.1007/s002130050498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Le AD, Poulos CX, Harding S, Watchus J, Juzytsch W, Shaham Y. Effects of naltrexone and fluoxetine on alcohol self-administration and reinstatement of alcohol seeking induced by priming injections of alcohol and exposure to stress. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21:435–444. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00024-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Le AD, Harding S, Juzytsch W, Watchus J, Shalev U, Shaham Y. The role of corticotrophin-releasing factor in stress-induced relapse to alcohol-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000;150:317–324. doi: 10.1007/s002130000411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Le AD, Harding S, Juzytsch W, Fletcher PT, Shaham Y. The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in the median raphe nucleus in relapse to alcohol. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7844–7849. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-07844.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Lee S, Schmidt D, Tilders F, Cole M, Smith A, Rivier C. Prolonged exposure to intermittent alcohol vapors blunts hypothalamic responsiveness to immune and non-immune signals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24:110–122. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Lepsch LB, Gonzalo LA, Magro FJ, Delucia R, Scavone C, Planeta CS. Exposure to chronic stress increases the locomotor response to cocaine and the basal levels of corticosterone in adolescent rats. Addict Biol. 2005;10:251–256. doi: 10.1080/13556210500269366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Lewis BA, Vogeltanz-Holm ND. The effects of alcohol and anxiousness on physiological and subjective responses to a social stressor in women. Addict Behav. 2002;27:529–545. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00190-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Lin D, Bruijnzeel AW, Schmidt P, Markou A. Exposure to chronic mild stress alters thresholds for lateral hypothalamic stimulation reward and subsequent responsiveness to amphetamine. Neuroscience. 2002;114:925–933. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00366-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Little HJ, O'Callaghan MJ, Butterworth AR, Wilson J, Cole J, Watson WP. Low alcohol preference among the "high alcohol preference" C57 strain of mice; preference increased by saline injections. Psychopharmacology. 1999;147:182–189. doi: 10.1007/s002130051159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Liu X, Weiss F. Additive effect of stress and drug cues on reinstatement of ethanol seeking: exacerbation by history of dependence and role of concurrent activation of corticotropin-releasing factor and opioid mechanisms. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7856–7861. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-07856.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Lodge DJ, Lawrence AJ. The CRF1 receptor antagonist antalarmin reduces volitional ethanol consumption in isolation-reared fawn-hooded rats. Neuroscience. 2003a;117:243–247. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00793-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Lodge DJ, Lawrence AJ. The effect of isolation rearing on volitional ethanol consumption and central CCK/dopamine systems in Fawn-Hooded rats. Behav Brain Res. 2003b;141:113–122. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00328-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Logrip ML, Janak PH, Ron D. Escalating ethanol intake is associated with altered corticostriatal BDNF expression. J Neurochem. 2009;109:1459–1468. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06073.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Lopez MF, Becker HC. Effect of pattern and number of chronic ethanol exposures on subsequent voluntary ethanol intake in C57BL/6J mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;181:688–696. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Lopez MF, Ralston LA, Becker HC. Ethanol seeking and drinking behaviors: comparison of female and male C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30:188A. [Google Scholar]
  149. Lopez MF, Anderson RI, Becker HC. Repeated cycles of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure increase both self-administration and the reinforcing value of ethanol in C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:163A. [Google Scholar]
  150. Lopez MF, Griffin WC, Becker HC. Ethanol intake, plasma corticosterone levels and brain region CRF levels in ethanol-dependent C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:200A. [Google Scholar]
  151. Lopez MF, Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Becker HC. Chronic social isolation and chronic variable stress during early development induce later elevated ethanol intake in adult C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol. 2011;45:355–364. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Lovallo WR, Dickensheets SL, Myers DA, Thomas TL, Nixon SJ. Blunted stress cortisol response in abstinent alcoholic and polysubstance-abusing men. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24:651–658. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Lowery EG, Sparrow AM, Breese GR, Knapp DJ, Thiele TE. The CRF-1 receptor antagonist, CP-154,526, attenuates stress-induced increases in ethanol consumption by BALB/cJ mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:240–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00573.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Lynch WJ, Kushner MG, Rawleigh JM, Fiszdon J, Carroll ME. The effects of restraint stress on voluntary ethanol consumption in rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1999;7:318–323. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.7.4.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Marchesi C, Chiodera P, Ampollini P, Volpi R, Coiro V. Beta-endorphin, adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol secretion in abstinent alcoholics. Psychiatry Res. 1997;72:187–194. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(97)00101-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Markou A, Kosten TR, Koob GF. Neurobiological similarities in depression and drug dependence: a self-medication hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:135–174. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00113-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Marmendal M, Roman E, Eriksson CJ, Nylander I, Fahlke C. Maternal separation alters maternal care, but has minor effects on behavior and brain opioid peptides in adult offspring. Dev Psychobiol. 2004;45:140–152. doi: 10.1002/dev.20027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Martin-Fardon R, Ciccocioppo R, Massi M, Weiss F. Nociceptin prevents stress-induced ethanol- but not cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. NeuroReport. 2000;11:1939–1943. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Matthews DB, Morrow AL, O'Buckley T, Flanigan TJ, Berry RB, Cook MN, Mittleman G, Goldowitz D, Tokunaga S, Silvers JM. Acute mild footshock alters ethanol drinking and plasma corticosterone levels in C57BL/6J male mice, but not DBA/2J or A/J male mice. Alcohol. 2008;42:469–476. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. McCool BA, Chappell AM. Early social isolation in male Long-Evans rats alters both appetitive and consummatory behaviors expressed during operant ethanol self-administration. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33:273–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00830.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. McEwen BS. Allostasis and allostatic load: implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:108–124. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00129-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. McKenzie-Quirk SD, Miczek KA. Social rank and social separation as determinants of alcohol drinking in squirrel monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;201:137–145. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1256-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Mediratta PK, Mahajan P, Sharma KK, Bhandari R, Dubey SP. Involvement of GABA-A receptor chloride channel complex in isolation stress-induced free choice ethanol consumption in rats. Indian J Exp Biol. 2003;41:47–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Melis M, Diana M, Enrico P, Marinelli M, Brodie MS. Ethanol and acetaldehyde action on central dopamine systems: mechanisms, modulation, and relationship to stress. Alcohol. 2009;43:531–539. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Miczek KA, Yap JJ, Covington HE., 3rd Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol Ther. 2008;120:102–128. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Millard WJ, Dole VP. Intake of water and ethanol by C57BL mice: effect of an altered light-dark schedule. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1983;18:281–284. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(83)90377-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Mills KC, Bean JW. The caloric and intoxicating properties of fluid intake as components of stress-induced ethanol consumption in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1978;57:27–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00426953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Mills KC, Bean JW, Hutcheson JS. Shock induced ethanol consumption in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1977;6:107–115. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(77)90166-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Millstein RA, Holmes A. Effects of repeated maternal separation on anxiety- and depression-related phenotypes in different mouse strains. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Molina VA, Heyser CJ, Spear LP. Chronic variable stress enhances the stimulatory action of a low dose of morphine: reversal by desipramine. Eur J Pharmacol. 1994;260:57–64. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(94)90009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Morrow AL, Biggio G, Serra M, Becker HC, Lopez MF, Porcu P, Alward SE, O’Buckley TK. The role of neuroactive steroids in ethanol/stress interactions. Alcohol. 2009;43:521–530. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Mozhui K, Karlsson RM, Kash TL, Ihne J, Norcross M, Patel S, Farrell MR, Hill EE, Graybeal C, Martin KP, Camp M, Fitzgerald PJ, Ciobanu DC, Sprengel R, Mishina M, Wellman CL, Winder DG, Williams RW, Holmes A. Strain differences in stress responsivity are associated with divergent amygdala gene expression and glutamate-mediated neuronal excitability. J Neurosci. 2010;30:5357–5367. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5017-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Mutschler J, Bilbao A, von der Goltz C, Demiralay C, Jahn H, Wiedemann K, Spanagel R, Kiefer F. Augmented stress-induced alcohol drinking and withdrawal in mice lacking functional natriuretic peptide-A receptors. Alcohol Alcsm. 2010;45:13–16. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agp065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Myers RD, Cicero TJ. Effects of serotonin depletion on the volitional alcohol intake of rats during a condition of psychological stress. Psychopharmacologia. 1969;15:373–381. doi: 10.1007/BF00403712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Myers RD, Holman RB. Failure of stress of electric shock to increase ethanol intake in rats. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1967;28:132–137. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Nagaraja HS, Jeganathan PS. Voluntary alcohol drinking & caloric intake in rats exposed to crowding stress. Indian J Med Res. 2002;116:111–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Nagaraja HS, Jeganathan PS. Effect of acute and chronic conditions of over-crowding on free choice ethanol intake in rats. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2003;47:325–331. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Nash JF, Jr, Maickel RP. Stress-induced consumption of ethanol by rats. Life Sci. 1985;37:757–765. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(85)90546-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Ng Cheong Ton MJ, Brown Z, Michalakeas A, Amit Z. Stress induced suppression of maintenance but not of acquisition of ethanol consumption in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1983;18:141–144. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(83)90264-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Nunez MJ, Riveiro P, Becerra MA, De Miguel S, Quintans MR, Nunez LA, Legazpi MP, Mayan JM, Rey-Mendez M, Varela M, Freire-Garabal M. Effects of alprazolam on the free-choice ethanol consumption induced by isolation stress in aged rats. Life Sci. 1999;64:PL213–PL217. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(99)00130-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Nunez MJ, Rivas M, Riveiro P, Suarez J, Balboa J, Nunez LA, Rey-Mendez M, Freire-Garabal M. Effects of nefazodone on voluntary ethanol consumption induced by isolation stress in young and aged rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;73:689–696. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00875-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. O'Callaghan MJ, Croft AP, Watson WP, Brooks SP, Little HJ. Low alcohol preference among the "high alcohol preference" C57/BL10 mice; factors affecting such preference. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;72:475–481. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00717-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. O'Dell LE, Roberts AJ, Smith RT, Koob GF. Enhanced alcohol self-administration after intermittent versus continuous alcohol vapor exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2004;28:1676–1682. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000145781.11923.4e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Ostrander MM, Ulrich-Lai YM, Choi DC, Flak JN, Richtand NM, Herman JP. Chronic stress produces enduring decreases in novel stress-evoked c-fos mRNA expression in discrete brain regions of the rat. Stress. 2009;12:469–477. doi: 10.3109/10253890802641966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Pacak K, Palkovits M. Stressor specificity of central neuroendocrine responses: implications for stress-related disorders. Endocr Rev. 2001;22:502–548. doi: 10.1210/edrv.22.4.0436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Pandey SC, Zhang D, Mittal N, Nayyar D. Potential role of the gene transcription factor cyclic AMP-responsive element binding protein in ethanol withdrawal-related anxiety. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1999;288:866–878. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Pandey SC, Zhang H, Roy A, Misra K. Central and medial amygdaloid brain-derived neurotrophic factor signaling plays a critical role in alcohol-drinking and anxiety-like behaviors. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8320–8331. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4988-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Parker LF, Radow BL. Isolation stress and volitional ethanol consumption in the rat. Physiol Behav. 1974;12:1–3. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(74)90060-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Pelloux T, Hagues G, Costentin J, Duterte-Boucher D. Helplessness in the tail suspension test is associated with an increase in ethanol intakle and its rewarding effect in female mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:378–388. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000156123.10298.fa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Penev PD, Kolker DE, Zee PC, Turek FW. Chronic circadian desynchronization decreases the survival of animals with cardiomyopathic heart disease. Am J Physiol. 1998;275:2334–2337. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1998.275.6.H2334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Piazza PV, Deroche V, Deminiere JM, Maccari S, Le Moal M, Simon H. Corticosterone in the range of stress-induced levels possesses reinforcing properties: implications for sensation-seeking behaviors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993;90:11738–11742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.24.11738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Pisu MG, Mostallino MC, Dore R, Maciocco E, Secci PP, Serra M. Effects of voluntary ethanol consumption on emotional state and stress responsiveness in socially isolated rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;21:414–425. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Ploj K, Roman E, Nylander I. Long-term effects of maternal separation on ethanol intake and brain opioid and dopamine receptors in male Wistar rats. Neuroscience. 2003;121:787–799. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00499-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Pohorecky LA. Interaction of ethanol and stress: research with experimental animals-an update. Alcohol Alcsm. 1990;25:263–276. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.alcalc.a045000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Pohorecky LA. Stress and alcohol interaction: an update of human research. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1991;15:438–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1991.tb00543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Pohorecky LA. Housing and rank status of male Long-Evans rats modify ethanol's effect on open-field behaviors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;185:289–297. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0257-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Pohorecky LA. Psychosocial stress and chronic ethanol ingestion in male rats: effects on elevated plus maze behavior and ultrasonic vocalizations. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:432–447. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Pohorecky LA. Acute novel stressors modify ethanol intake of psychosocially stressed rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2010;95:390–400. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Powell BJ, Kamano DK, Martin LK. Multiple factors affecting volitional consumption of alcohol in the Abrams Wistar rat. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1966;27:7–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Racz I, Bilkei-Gorzo A, Toth ZE, Michel K, Palkovits M, Zimmer A. A critical role for the cannabinoid CB1 receptors in alcohol dependence and stress-stimulated ethanol drinking. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2453–2458. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02453.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Rasmussen DD, Boldt BM, Bryant CA, Mitton DR, Larsen SA, Wilkinson CW. Chronic daily ethanol and withdrawal: 1. Long-term changes in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24:1836–1849. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Richardson HN, Lee SY, O'Dell LE, Koob GF, Rivier CL. Alcohol self-administration acutely stimulates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, but alcohol dependence leads to a dampened neuroendocrine state. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28:1641–1653. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06455.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Rimondini R, Arlinde C, Sommer W, Heilig M. Long-lasting increase in voluntary ethanol consumption and transcriptional regulation in the rat brain after intermittent exposure to alcohol. FASEB J. 2002;16:27–35. doi: 10.1096/fj.01-0593com. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Rimondini R, Sommer W, Heilig M. A temporal threshold for induction of persistent alcohol preference: behavioral evidence in a rat model of intermittent intoxication. J Stud Alcohol. 2003;64:445–449. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Rimondini R, Sommer WH, Dall'Olio R, Heilig M. Long-lasting tolerance to alcohol following a history of dependence. Addict Biol. 2008;13:26–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2007.00079.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Rivier C. Effects of alcohol on the neuroendocrine system. In: Noronha A, et al., editors. Review of NIAAA's neuroscience and behavioral research portfolio: NIAAA Research Monograph No 34. Bethesda: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 2000. pp. 61–81. [Google Scholar]
  207. Roberto M, Cruz MT, Gilpin NW, Sabino V, Schweitzer P, Bajo M, Cottone P, Madamba SG, Stouffer DG, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF, Siggins GR, Parsons LH. Corticotropin releasing factor-induced amygdala gamma-aminobutyric acid release plays a key role in alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:831–839. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Roberts AJ, Cole M, Koob GF. Intra-amygdala muscimol decreases operant ethanol self-administration in dependent rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:1289–1298. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, Griffin P, Koob GF. Excessive ethanol drinking following a history of dependence: animal model of allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:581–594. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00167-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Rockman GE, Glavin GB. Activity stress effects on voluntary ethanol consumption, mortality and ulcer development in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1986;24:869–873. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90428-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Rockman GE, Hall A, Glavin GB. Effects of restraint stress on voluntary ethanol intake and ulcer proliferation in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1986;25:1083–1087. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90089-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Rockman GE, Hall A, Hong J, Glavin GB. Unpredictable cold-immobilization stress effects on voluntary ethanol consumption in rats. Life Sci. 1987;40:1245–1251. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(87)90580-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Rockman GE, Hall AM, Markert LE, Glavin GB. Influence of rearing conditions on voluntary ethanol intake and response to stress in rats. Behav Neural Biol. 1988;49:184–191. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(88)90506-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Rodgers DA, Thiessen DD. Effects of population density on adrenal size, behavioral arousal, and alcohol preference of inbred mice. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1964;25:240–247. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Roman E, Nylander I. The impact of emotional stress early in life on adult voluntary ethanol intake-results of maternal separation in rats. Stress. 2005;8:157–174. doi: 10.1080/10253890500188666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Roman E, Hyytia P, Nylander I. Maternal separation alters acquisition of ethanol intake in male ethanol-preferring AA rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27:31–37. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000047352.88145.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Roman E, Ploj K, Nylander I. Maternal separation has no effect on voluntary ethanol intake in female Wistar rats. Alcohol. 2004;33:31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Roman E, Gustafsson L, Hyytia P, Nylander I. Short and prolonged periods of maternal separation and voluntary ethanol intake in male and female ethanol-preferring AA and ethanol-avoiding ANA rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:591–601. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000158933.70242.fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Rosenwasser AM, Clark JW, Fixaris MC, Belanger GV, Foster JA. Effects of repeated light-dark phase shifts on voluntary ethanol and water intake in male and female Fischer and Lewis rats. Alcohol. 2010;44:229–237. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2010.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Roske I, Baeger I, Frenzel R, Oehme P. Does a relationship exist between the quality of stress and the motivation to ingest alcohol? Alcohol. 1994;11:113–124. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(94)90052-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Sacharczuk M, Juszczak G, Sliwa AT, Tymosiak-Zielinska A, Lisowski P, Jaszczak K, Pluta R, Lipkowski A, Sadowski B, Swiergiel AH. Differences in ethanol drinking between mice selected for high and low swim stress-induced analgesia. Alcohol. 2008;42:487–492. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Sandbak T, Murison R. Behavioural responses to elevated plus-maze and defensive burying testing: effects on subsequent ethanol intake and effect of ethanol on retention of the burying response. Alcohol Alcsm. 2001;36:48–58. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/36.1.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Sayette MA. Does drinking reduce stress? Alcohol Res Health. 1999;23:250–255. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Schenk S, Gorman K, Amit Z. Age-dependent effects of isolation housing on the self-administration of ethanol in laboratory rats. Alcohol. 1990;7:321–326. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(90)90090-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Schroff KC, Cowen MS, Koch S, Spanagel R. Strain-specific responses of inbred mice to ethanol following food shortage. Addict Biol. 2004;9:265–271. doi: 10.1080/13556210412331292596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Selye H. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature. 1936;138:32. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.2.230a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, De Wit H, Stewart J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;168:3–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1224-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Shalev U, Grimm JW, Shaham Y. Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: a review. Pharmacol Rev. 2002;54:1–42. doi: 10.1124/pr.54.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Shalev U, Erb S, Shaham Y. Role of CRF and other neuropeptides in stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking. Brain Res. 2010;1314:15–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.07.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  230. Siegmund S, Vengeliene V, Singer MV, Spanagel R. Influence of age at drinking onset on long-term ethanol self-administration with deprivation and stress phases. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1139–1145. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000171928.40418.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Sillaber I, Henniger MS. Stress and alcohol drinking. Ann Med. 2004;36:596–605. doi: 10.1080/07853890410018862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Sillaber I, Rammes G, Zimmermann S, Mahal B, Zieglgansberger W, Wurst W, Holsboer F, Spanagel R. Enhanced and delayed stress-induced alcohol drinking in mice lacking functional CRH1 receptors. Science. 2002;296:931–933. doi: 10.1126/science.1069836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;158:343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. Sinha R. Chronic stress, drug use, and vulnerability to addiction. Ann N YAcad Sci. 2008;1141:105–130. doi: 10.1196/annals.1441.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  235. Sinha R, Fox HC, Hong KI, Hansen J, Tuit K, Kreek MJ. Effects of adrenal sensitivity, stress- and cue-induced craving, and anxiety on subsequent alcohol relapse and treatment outcomes. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011 doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  236. Smith SM, Vale WW. The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in neuroendocrine responses to stress. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2006;8:383–395. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/ssmith. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Soderpalm AH, de Wit H. Effects of stress and alcohol on subjective state in humans. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2002;26:818–826. doi: 10.1097/00000374-200206000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Sommer WH, Rimondini R, Hansson AC, Hipskind PA, Gehlert DR, Barr CS, Heilig MA. Upregulation of voluntary alcohol intake, behavioral sensitivity to stress, and amygdala crhr1 expression following a history of dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Spanagel R, Rosenwasser AM, Schumann G, Sarkar DK. Alcohol consumption and the body's biological clock. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1550–1557. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000175074.70807.fd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  240. Sperling RE, Gomes SM, Sypek EI, Carey AN, McLaughlin JP. Endogenous kappa-opioid mediation of stress-induced potentiation of ethanol-conditioned place preference and self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;210:199–209. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1844-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Sprague JE, Maickel RP. Effects of stress and ebiratide (Hoe-427) on free-choice ethanol consumption: comparison of Lewis and Sprague-Dawley rats. Life Sci. 1994;55:873–878. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(94)90043-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Tabakoff B, Jafee RC, Ritzmann RF. Corticosterone concentrations in mice during ethanol drinking and withdrawal. J Pharm Pharmacol. 1978;30:371–374. doi: 10.1111/j.2042-7158.1978.tb13259.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  243. Tambour S, Brown LL, Crabbe JC. Gender and age at drinking onset affect voluntary alcohol consumption but neither the alcohol deprivation effect nor the response to stress in mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:2100–2106. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00798.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  244. Tannenbaum B, Anisman H. Impact of chronic intermittent challenges in stressor-susceptible and resilient strains of mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:292–303. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01487-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Ehlers CL. Effects of neuropeptide Y and corticotropin-releasing factor on ethanol intake in Wistar rats: interaction with chronic ethanol exposure. Behav Brain Res. 2005a;161:133–140. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Ehlers CL. Effects of neuropeptide Y on appetitive and consummatory behaviors associated with alcohol drinking in wistar rats with a history of ethanol exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005b;29:584–590. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000160084.13148.02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  247. Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Khoury A, Mathe AA, Ehlers CL. Effect of social isolation on ethanol consumption and substance P/neurokinin expression in Wistar rats. Alcohol. 2005c;36:91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2005.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  248. Trujillo JL, Do DT, Grahame NJ, Roberts AJ, Gorman MR. Ethanol consumption in mice: relationships with circadian period and entrainment. Alcohol. 2011;45:147–159. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2010.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Tsai LL, Tsai YC, Hwang K, Huang YW, Tzeng JE. Repeated light-dark shifts speed up body weight gain in male F344 rats. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2005;289:E212–E217. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00603.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  250. Turyabahika-Thyen K, Wolffgramm J. Loss of flexibility in alcohol-taking rats: promoting factors. Eur Addict Res. 2006;12:210–221. doi: 10.1159/000094423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Uhart M, Wand GS. Stress, alcohol and drug interaction: an update of human research. Addict Biol. 2009;14:43–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00131.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  252. van Erp AM, Miczek KA. Persistent suppression of ethanol self-administration by brief social stress in rats and increased startle response as index of withdrawal. Physiol Behav. 2001;73:301–311. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00458-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. van Erp AM, Tachi N, Miczek KA. Short or continuous social stress: suppression of continuously available ethanol intake in subordinate rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2001;12:335–342. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200109000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  254. Vengeliene V, Siegmund S, Singer MV, Sinclair JD, Li TK, Spanagel R. A comparative study on alcohol-preferring rat lines: effects of deprivation and stress phases on voluntary alcohol intake. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27:1048–1054. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000075829.81211.0C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  255. Volpicelli JR, Ulm RR. The influence of control over appetitive and aversive events on alcohol preference in rats. Alcohol. 1990;7:133–136. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(90)90074-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  256. Volpicelli JR, Tiven J, Kimmel SC. The relationship between tension reduction and ethanol consumption in rats. Physiol Psychol. 1982;10:114–116. [Google Scholar]
  257. Volpicelli JR, Davis MA, Olgin JE. Naltrexone blocks the post-shock increase of ethanol consumption. Life Sci. 1986;38:841–847. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(86)90601-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  258. Volpicelli JR, Ulm RR, Hopson N. The bidirectional effects of shock on alcohol preference in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1990;14:913–916. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb01837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  259. Von Wright JM, Pekanmaki L, Malin S. Effects of conflict and stress on alcohol intake in rats. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1971;32:420–433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  260. Walker BM, Koob GF. Pharmacological evidence for a motivational role of kappa-opioid systems in ethanol dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:643–652. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  261. Walker FR, Naicker S, Hinwood M, Dunn N, Day TA. Strain differences in coping behaviour, novelty seeking behaviour, and susceptibility to socially conditioned fear: a comparison between Wistar and Sprague Dawley rats. Stress. 2009;12:507–516. doi: 10.3109/10253890802673134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  262. Walker BM, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF. Systemic kappa-opioid receptor antagonism by nor-binaltorphimine reduces dependence-induced excessive alcohol self-administration in rats. Addict Biol. 2011;16:116–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00226.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  263. Wand G. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: changes and risk for alcoholism. In: Noronha A, et al., editors. Review of NIAAA's neuroscience and behavioral research portfolio: NIAAA Research Monograph No 34. Bethesda: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 2000. pp. 397–415. [Google Scholar]
  264. Wand GS, Dobs AS. Alterations in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in actively drinking alcoholics. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1991;72:1290–1295. doi: 10.1210/jcem-72-6-1290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  265. Wand GS, Mangold D, El Deiry S, McCaul ME, Hoover D. Family history of alcoholism and hypothalamic opioidergic activity. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:1114–1119. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.12.1114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  266. Weinberg J. Effects of early experience on responsiveness to ethanol: a preliminary report. Physiol Behav. 1987;40:401–406. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90068-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  267. Weisinger RS, Denton DA, Osborne PG. Voluntary ethanol intake of individually- or pair-housed rats: effect of ACTH or dexamethasone treatment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1989;33:335–341. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90510-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  268. Weiss JM, West CHK, Emery MS, Bonsall RW, Moore JP, Boss-Williams KA. Rats selectively-bred for behavior related to affective disorders: proclivity for intake of alcohol and drugs of abuse, and measures of brain monoamines. Biochem Pharmacol. 2008;75:134–159. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  269. West CHK, Weiss JM. Intake of ethanol and reinforcing fluids in rats bred for susceptibility to stress. Alcohol. 2006;38:13–27. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2006.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  270. Willenbring ML, Morley JE, Niewoehner CB, Heilman RO, Carlson CH, Shafer RB. Adrenocortical hyperactivity in newly admitted alcoholics: prevalence, course and associated variables. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1984;9:415–422. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(84)90049-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  271. Williams PG, Suchy Y, Rau HK. Individual differences in executive functioning: implications for stress regulation. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:126–140. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9100-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  272. Wolffgramm J. Free choice ethanol intake of laboratory rats under different social conditions. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1990;101:233–239. doi: 10.1007/BF02244132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  273. Wolffgramm J, Heyne A. Social behavior, dominance, and social deprivation of rats determine drug choice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;38:389–399. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90297-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  274. Yanai J, Ginsburg BE. Increased sensitivity to chronic ethanol in isolated mice. Psychopharmacologia. 1976;46:185–189. doi: 10.1007/BF00421390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  275. Yang X, Wang S, Rice KC, Munro CA, Wand GS. Restraint stress and ethanol consumption in two mouse strains. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:840–852. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00632.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  276. Zorrilla EP, Valdez GR, Weiss F. Changes in levels of regional CRF-like-immunoreactivity and plasma corticosterone during protracted drug withdrawal in dependent rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;158:374–381. doi: 10.1007/s002130100773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  277. Zurita A, Martijena I, Cuadra G, Brandão ML, Molina V. Early exposure to chronic variable stress facilitates the occurrence of anhedonia and enhanced emotional reactions to novel stressors: reversal by naltrexone pretreatment. Behav Brain Res. 2000;117:163–171. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00302-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy