Abstract
The understanding of decision-making systems has come together in recent years to form a unified theory of decision-making in the mammalian brain as arising from multiple, interacting systems (a planning system, a habit system, and a situation-recognition system). This unified decision-making system has multiple potential access points through which it can be driven to make maladaptive choices, particularly choices that entail seeking of certain drugs or behaviors. We identify 10 key vulnerabilities in the system: (1) moving away from homeostasis, (2) changing allostatic set points, (3) euphorigenic “reward-like” signals, (4) overvaluation in the planning system, (5) incorrect search of situation-action-outcome relationships, (6) misclassification of situations, (7) overvaluation in the habit system, (8) a mismatch in the balance of the two decision systems, (9) over-fast discounting processes, and (10) changed learning rates. These vulnerabilities provide a taxonomy of potential problems with decision-making systems. Although each vulnerability can drive an agent to return to the addictive choice, each vulnerability also implies a characteristic symptomology. Different drugs, different behaviors, and different individuals are likely to access different vulnerabilities. This has implications for an individual’s susceptibility to addiction and the transition to addiction, for the potential for relapse, and for the potential for treatment.
Keywords: Addiction, decision making, dopamine, frontal cortex, gambling, hippocampus, striatum
1. Introduction
Addiction can be operationally defined as the continued making of maladaptive choices, even in the face of the explicitly stated desire to make a different choice (see the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM-IV-TR], American Psychiatric Association 2000; International Classification of Diseases [ICD-10], World Health Organization 1992). In particular, addicts continue to pursue drugs or other maladaptive behaviors despite terrible consequences (Altman et al. 1996; Goldstein 2000; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Lowinson et al. 1997). Addictive drugs have been hypothesized to drive maladaptive decision-making through pharmacological interactions with neurophysiological mechanisms evolved for normal learning systems (Berke 2003; Everitt et al. 2001; Hyman 2005; Kelley 2004a; Lowinson et al. 1997; Redish 2004). Addictive behaviors have been hypothesized to drive maladaptive decision-making through interactions between normal learning systems and the reward distribution of certain behaviors (Custer 1984; Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Dowling et al. 2005; Parke & Griffiths 2004; Redish et al. 2007; Wagenaar 1988). However, how those interactions drive maladaptive decision-making remains a key, unanswered question.
Over the last 30 years, a number of theories have been proposed attempting to explain why an agent might continue to seek a drug or maladaptive behavior. These theories can be grouped into the following primary categories: (1) opponent processes, based on changes in homeostatic and allostatic levels that change the needs of the agent (Becker & Murphy 1988; Koob & Le Moal 1997; 2001; 2005; 2006; Solomon & Corbit 1973; 1974); (2) reward-based processes and hedonic components, based on pharmacological access to hedonically positive signals in the brain (Kalivas & Volkow 2005; Volkow et al. 2003; 2004; Wise 2004); (3) incentive salience, based on a sensitization of motivational signals in the brain (Berridge & Robinson 1998; 2003; Robinson & Berridge 1993; 2001; 2003; 2004); (4) non-compensable dopamine, based on a role of dopamine as signaling an error in the prediction of the value of taking an action, leading to an overvaluation of drug-seeking (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Di Chiara 1999; Redish 2004); (5) impulsivity, in which users make rash choices, without taking into account later costs (Ainslie 1992; 2001; Ainslie & Monterosso 2004; Bickel & Marsch 2001; Giordano et al. 2002; Odum et al. 2002); (6) situation recognition and categorization, based on a misclassification of situations that produce both gains and losses (Custer 1984; Griffiths 1994; Langer & Roth 1975; Redish et al. 2007; Wagenaar 1988); and (7) deficiencies in the balance between executive and habit systems, in which it becomes particularly difficult to break habits through cognitive mechanisms either through over-performance of the habit system (Robbins & Everitt 1999; Tiffany 1990) or under-performance of flexible, executive, inhibitory systems (Gray & McNaughton 2000; Jentsch & Taylor 1999; Lubman et al. 2004) or a change in the balance between them (Bechara 2005; Bickel et al. 2007; Everitt et al. 2001; Everitt & Wolf 2002). (See Table 1.)
Table 1.
Opponent processes | Changes in allostatic and homeostatic needs | a |
Hedonic processes | Pharmacological access to hedonically positive signals | b |
Incentive salience | Sensitization of motivational signals | c |
Noncompensable DA | Leading to an overvaluation of drug-seeking | d |
Impulsivity | Overemphasis on a buy-now, pay-later strategy | e |
Illusion of control | Misclassification of wins and losses | f |
Shifting balances | Development of habits over flexible systems | g |
Related References:
Although each of these theories has been attacked as incomplete and unable to explain all of the addiction data, the theories are not incompatible with each other. We argue, instead, that each theory explains a different vulnerability in the decision-process system, capable of driving the agent to make an addictive choice. Thus, the set of theories provides a constellation of potential causes for addictive choice behavior. Each different drug of abuse or maladaptive behavior is likely to access a subset of that constellation of potential dysfunction. Individual differences are likely to define the importance of each vulnerability for an individual’s dysfunction. Successful treatment depends on treating those vulnerabilities that are driving the individual’s choice. The identification of addiction as vulnerabilities in the biological decision-making system means that understanding addiction will require an understanding of how animals (including humans) make decisions.
The understanding of decision processes has come together in recent years to form a unified theory of decision-making arising from multiple interacting systems (Cohen & Squire 1980; Daw et al. 2005; Dickinson 1980; 1985; Nadel 1994; O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Redish 1999; Squire 1987). Briefly, a decision can arise from a flexible planning system capable of the consideration of consequences or from a less flexible habit system in which actions are associated with situations (Daw et al. 2005; Redish & Johnson 2007). Behavioral control can be transferred from one system to the other depending on the statistics of behavioral training (Balleine & Dickinson 1998; Colwill & Rescorla 1990; Killcross & Coutureau 2003; Packard & McGaugh 1996). Both systems also require a recognition of the situation in which the agent finds itself (Daw et al. 2006; Redish et al. 2007; Redish & Johnson 2007). These processes provide multiple access points and vulnerabilities through which the decision process can be driven to make maladaptive choices.
2. Scope of the work
Addiction is a complex phenomenon, with causes that can be identified from many perspectives (Volkow & Li 2005a; West 2001), including social (Davis & Tunks 1991), environmental (DeFeudis 1978; Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Maddahian et al. 1986; Morgan et al. 2002), legal (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Kleber et al., 1997; MacCoun 1993), as well as psychological and neurobiological (Goldman et al. 1987; 1999; Heyman 1996; 2000; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Redish 2004; Robinson 2004; Robinson & Berridge 2003; Tiffany 1990), economic (Ainslie 1992; 2001; Becker & Murphy 1988; Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Hursh 1991; Hursh et al. 2005), and genetic (Crabbe 2002; Goldman et al. 2005; Hiroi & Agatsuma 2005) perspectives. All of these perspectives have explanatory power as to the causes of addiction, and all of them provide suggested methods of treatment of addiction. However, a thorough treatment of addiction from all of these perspectives is beyond the scope of a paper such as this one. In this target article, we address an explanation for addictive decisions based on animal learning theory, the neuroscience of learning and memory, human decision-making, and neuroeconomics, which we argue have converged on a unified theory of decision-making as arising from an interaction between two learning systems (a quickly learned, flexible, but computationally expensive-to-execute planning system and a slowly learned, inflexible, but computationally inexpensive-to-execute habit system).
2.1. Our goals
The goal of this target article is to lay out a novel explanation for addiction as “vulnerabilities” in an established decision-making system. Although many of the vulnerabilities that we describe can be identified closely with current theories of addiction (see, e.g., Table 5), those theories have generally arisen from explanations of specific experiments and have all been attacked as incomplete. Our article is the first to identify them as “failure points” in a unified decision-making system. This theory has implications for the taxonomy of addiction, both drug-related and behavioral, as well as implications for prevention and treatment. These implications are addressed at the end of the article.
Table 5.
Current Theory | Related Vulnerabilities | |
---|---|---|
Homeostatic changesa | Deviations from homeostatic set-points drives the system to restore original homeostatic levels. | Vulnerability 1 |
Allostatic changesb | Changes in the homeostatic set-points drives the system to achieve incorrect homeostatic levels. | Vulnerability 2 |
Reward-based processingc | Pharmacological access to reward signals drives the return to those signals | Vulnerability 3 |
Incentive-salienced | Sensitization of motivational signals drives excess motivation for certain events | Vulnerability 4 |
Unmitigated cravinge | Increased expectation of reward with experience drives craving | Vulnerabilities 3, 4 |
Noncompensable dopaminef | Excess positive value-error signals lead to an overvaluation of drug-seeking | Vulnerability 7 |
The illusion of controlg | Incorrect expectations of control of situations leads to a willingness to gamble | Vulnerability 6a |
Impulsivityh | Unwillingness to weigh future events leads to impulsive choices | Vulnerabilities 8, 9 |
Decreased executive functioni | An inability to plan makes it difficult to break habits through cognitive mechanisms | Vulnerabilities 6b, 8 |
Alcohol expectancy theoryj | Expectance of positive rewards are associated with alcohol consumption. These expectancies develop into automated processes under certain conditions | Vulnerabilities 3, 8 |
Becker and Murphy (1988); Koob and Le Moal (1997; 2001; 2005; 2006); Solomon and Corbit (1973; 1974).
Custer (1984); Griffiths (1994); Langer and Roth (1975); Redish et al. (2007); Sylvain et al. (1997); Wagenaar (1988).
Ainslie (1992; 2001); Ainslie and Monterosso (2004); Bickel and Marsch (2001); Giordano et al. (2002); Odum et al. (2002).
Although we do not directly address the social, environmental, or policy-level theories, we believe that our proposed framework will have implications for these viewpoints on addiction. For example, changes in drug price, taxes, legality, and level of policing can change the costs required to reach the addictive substance or behavior (Becker et al. 1994; Grossman & Chaloupka 1998; Liu et al. 1999). The presence of casinos can provide cues triggering learned associations (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006). Acceptability of use and punishments for use will affect the relationship between rewards and costs (Goldman et al. 1987; 1999). Genetics will shape the person’s vulnerabilities to the potential failure points noted further on and will have to be an important part of the individual’s treatment plan (Goldman et al. 2005; Hiroi & Agatsuma 2005).
Before proceeding to the implications of this theory, we first need to lay out the unified model of the decision-making system (sect. 3). As we go through the components of this system, we point out the identifiable vulnerabilities as they arise. In section 4, we then return to each identified vulnerability in turn and discuss the interactions between that vulnerability and specific drugs and problematic behaviors. In section 5, we discuss the implications of this theory for individual susceptibility to addiction, for multiple pathways to relapse, and for the necessity of making available multiple appropriately guided treatment regimens. In section 6, we turn to social, political, and clinical implications, lay out open questions, and suggest future directions for addiction research. Finally, we include an appendix reviewing the known effects of six drugs and problematic behaviors, discussed in the light of the vulnerabilities identified in this article (A: cocaine; B: opiates; C: nicotine; D: alcohol; E: caffeine; and F: gambling).
3. Making decisions
Theories of how animals make decisions have been developed over the last 50 years in the fields of economics (Ainslie 1992, 2001; Becker & Murphy 1988; Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Bickel & Marsch 2001; Glimcher & Rustichini 2004; Petry & Bickel 1998), psychology and neuroscience (Daw 2003; Glimcher 2003; Hastie 2001; Herrnstein 1997; Heyman 1996; Kahneman et al. 1982; Kahneman & Tversky 2000; Sanfey et al. 2006; Slovic et al. 1977), and machine learning (Sutton & Barto 1998). These literatures have converged on the concept that decisions are based on the prediction of value or expected utility of the decision.1 These terms can be defined as the total, expected, future reward, taking into account the probability of receiving the reward and any delay before the reward is received. In these analyses, costs are typically included as negative rewards, but they can also be included separately in some formulations. If the agent can correctly predict the value (total discounted2 reward minus total expected cost) of its actions, then it can make appropriate decisions about which actions to take. The theories of addiction that have been proposed (Table 1) all have the effect of changing the prediction of value or cost in ways that make the agent continue to repeatedly return to seeking of the addictive drug or maladaptive behavior.
There are two potential methods from which one can derive the value of taking some action (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Daw et al. 2005; Redish & Johnson 2007; Sutton & Barto 1998): forward-search and caching. In the first case (forward-search), one considers the possible consequences of one’s actions – the agent realizes that if it takes this action in this situation, this will occur, and it will get this reward, but if it does something else, there will be different consequences, and it will get a different reward. In the other case (caching), the agent has learned to associate a specific action with a given situation – over time, the agent has learned that the best thing to do in this situation is to take this action. The forward-search system takes time to execute (because one has to mentally trace down possible paths), but is very flexible. That flexibility means that it is safe to learn quickly. Learning potential consequences of one’s actions does not commit one to an action; rather it opens the possibility of considering the consequences of an action before selecting that action. In contrast, the caching system is very fast to execute (because one simply has to retrieve the best action for a given situation), but is very rigid. That inflexibility means that it would be dangerous to learn the stimulus-action relationships stored in the habit system too quickly.
This dichotomy can be related to the question of when to stop a search process (Nilsson et al. 1987; Simon 1955). Incomplete search processes may be available in which temporarily cached values are accessed to cut off parts of the search tree, similar to heuristic search processes studied in the classic artificial intelligence literature (Nilsson et al. 1987; Rich & Knight 1991; Russell & Norvig 2002). Similarly, one can imagine that only some of the potential paths are searched in any decision. Finding an optimal solution takes time, and there is a tradeoff between search time and the optimality of the solution found (Simon 1955). From an evolutionary perspective, a quickly found, acceptable solution may be more efficient than a slowly found optimal solution (Gigerenzer 2001; Gigerenzer & Goldstein 1996; Simon 1955). A true caching system, however, does not entail a search process and should not be considered to be equivalent to a single step of the search process (Daw et al. 2005; Gigerenzer 2001). A single step of the search process would identify the consequence of that step, allowing changes in that consequence to change performance without relearning. In contrast, the caching system compares a stored value with an action taken in a given situation and does not identify the consequence during performance, which means that it cannot change its reactions to changes in the value of that consequence. This distinction can be seen in the devaluation literature, discussed further on.
A number of literatures have converged on a division between learning systems that match these two systems. In the animal navigation literature, these two systems are referred to as the cognitive map and route systems,3 respectively (O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Redish 1999). In the animal learning-theory literature, these systems can be identified as three separate systems, a Pavlovian learning system (situation-outcome, ), an instrumental learning system (action-outcome, ), and a habit learning system ().4
They have also been referred to as cognitive and habit learning systems (Mishkin & Appenzeller 1987; Poldrack & Packard 2003; Saint-Cyr et al. 1988; Yin & Knowlton 2006), and match closely the distinction made between declarative and procedural learning (Cohen & Eichenbaum 1993; Cohen & Squire 1980; Redish 1999; Squire 1987; Squire et al. 1984) and between explicit and implicit learning systems (Clark & Squire 1998; Curran 1995; Doyon et al. 1998; Ferraro et al. 1993; Forkstam & Petersson 2005; Knopman & Nissen 1987; 1991; Nissen et al. 1987; Willingham et al. 1989), as well as between controlled and automatic processing theories (Kahneman & Frederick 2002; Schneider & Chein 2003; Schneider & Shiffrin 1977). We argue that these diverse literatures have converged on a pair of decision-making systems, which can be understood as (1) a flexible, cognitive, planning system and (2) a rigid, automatic, habit-based system.
This dichotomy is related to the historical debate on “expectancies” in the classic animal learning theory literature (Bolles 1972; Hull 1943; 1952; Munn 1950; Tolman 1938; 1939; 1948). Tolman (1938; 1939; 1948) argued that animals maintain an expectancy of their potential future consequences (including an expectancy of any rewarding component), and that this provided for latent learning effects as well as fast changes in choices in response to changes in provided needs, whereas Hull (1943; 1952) argued that animals learn simple associations of stimuli and responses, allowing for the slow development of automation (Carr & Watson 1908; Dennis 1932). As noted by Guthrie (1935; see Balleine & Ostlund 2007; Bolles 1972), one implication of Tolman’s cognitive expectancies theories would be a delay in choosing. Just such a delay is seen in early learning, particularly in tasks that require the planning system. At choice points, rats faced with difficult decisions pause and vicariously sample the different choices before committing to a decision (Brown 1992; Meunzinger 1938; Tolman 1938; 1939). This “vicarious trial and error” (VTE) behavior is abolished with hippocampal lesions (Hu & Amsel 1995), and is related to hippocampal activity on hippocampal-dependent tasks (Hu et al. 2006). Recent neural ensemble recording data have found that hippocampal firing patterns transiently represent locations ahead of the animal at choice points during VTE-like behaviors (Johnson & Redish 2007). Once tasks have been overtrained, these VTE movements disappear (Hu et al. 2006; Munn 1950; Tolman 1938), as do the forward representations (Johnson & Redish 2007), suggesting that VTE may be a signal of the active processing in the planning system (Buckner & Carroll, 2007; Johnson & Redish 2007; Tolman 1938; 1939).
These two systems mirror the classical two-process theory in psychology (Domjan 1998; Gray 1975) and the more recent distinction between stimulus-stimulus (SS, S → O), stimulus-outcome (SO, SAO, ), action-outcome (AO, ), and stimulus-response or stimulus-action (SA, ) (Balleine & Ostlund 2007; Dickinson 1985) (see Table 2). The first (S → O) entails the recognition of a causal sequence but does not entail an actual decision. The second () is classical Pavlovian conditioning and entails an action taken in response to a situation in anticipation of a given outcome (Domjan 1998; Pavlov 1927; Rescorla 1988). The third () is classical instrumental conditioning (Balleine & Ostlund 2007; Domjan 1998; Ferster & Skinner 1957) and entails an action taken to achieve an outcome, even in the absence of an immediate stimulus. It is important to note, however, that action-outcome associations do still include stimuli in the form of the context (actions are not taken at all times but rather only within certain facilitating contexts).5 The fourth () entails an association between a situation and an action and denotes habit learning (Domjan 1998; Hull 1943; 1952).
Table 2.
System | Description | Learning Theory | Expectation | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Observation | S → O | Pavlovian | S-S | E(O) | |
Planning | Pavlovian with action | S-O | E(O)→E(V) | ||
Planning | Instrumental | A-O | E(O)→E(V) | ||
Habit | Habit | S-R | E(V) |
These four associations can be differentiated in terms of their expectancies (Table 2). S → O associations entail an expectancy of an outcome, but with no decision, there is no necessary further processing of that outcome, although there is likely to be an emotional preparation of some sort. If an animal can do something to prepare for, produce, or change that outcome, then the association becomes one of situation-action-outcome (). If there is no immediate stimulus triggering the action, then the association becomes an association. Because associations continue to include a contextual gating component, the association is truly an association. Although there are anatomical reasons to separate from associations (Balleine & Ostlund 2007; Ostlund & Balleine 2007; Yin et al. 2005), for our purposes, they can be treated similarly: they both entail an expectancy of an outcome that must be evaluated to produce an expectancy of a value. This means they both require a planning component and can be differentiated from habit learning in which situations are directly associated with actions (). In the association, situation-action pairs entail a direct expectancy of a value, which can then drive the action, even in the absence of a recognition of the outcome.
Following this distinction, we categorize these four association systems into three decision systems: an observation system, which does not make decisions and will not be discussed further; a planning system, which takes a given situation (derived from stimuli, context, or a combination thereof), predicts an outcome, and evaluates that outcome; and a habit system, which takes a given situation (derived from stimuli, context, or a combination thereof) and identifies the best remembered action to take.
These systems, of course, exist within overlapping and interacting structures (Balleine & Ostlund 2007; Corbit et al. 2001; Dayan & Balleine 2002; Devan & White 1999; Kelley 1999a; 1999b; Voorn et al. 2004; Yin et al. 2006; Yin & Knowlton 2006). The flexible planning system involves the entorhinal cortex (Corbit & Balleine 2000), hippocampus (O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Redish 1999), the ventral and dorsomedial striatum (Devan & White 1999; Martin 2001; Mogenson 1984; Mogenson et al. 1980; Pennartz et al. 2004; Schoenbaum et al. 2003; Yin et al. 2005), prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex (Jung et al. 1998; Killcross & Coutureau 2003; Ragozzino et al. 1999), and orbitofrontal cortex (Davis et al. 2006; Padoa-Schioppa & Assad 2006; Schoenbaum et al. 2003; 2006a; 2006b; Schoenbaum & Roesch 2005). The habit system involves the dorsolateral striatum (Barnes et al. 2005; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2004; Yin & Knowlton 2004; 2006), the infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex (Coutureau & Killcross 2003; Killcross & Coutureau 2003) as well as the parietal cortex (DiMattia & Kesner 1988; Kesner et al. 1989) (see Table 3).
Table 3.
Planning System | Habit System | |
---|---|---|
Literature | ||
Animal navigation | Cognitive map | Route, taxon, response |
Animal behavior | S-O, S-A-O, A-O | S-A, S-R |
Memory systems | Cognitive | Habit |
Learning and memory | Episodic (declarative) | Procedural |
Cognition | Explicit | Implicit |
Machine learning | Forward-search | Action-caching |
Properties | ||
Flexibility | Flexible | Rigid |
Execution speed | Slow | Fast |
Learning speed | Quick | Slow |
Devaluation? | Yes | No |
Key Anatomical Structures | ||
Striatum | Ventral, dorsomedial striatum (accumbens, head of the caudate) | Dorsolateral striatum (caudate, putamen) |
Frontal cortex | Prelimbic, orbitofrontal cortex | Infralimbic, other components? |
Hippocampal involvement | Hippocampus (yes) | ——— (no) |
Dopaminergic inputs | Ventral tegmental area | Substantia nigra pars compacta |
3.1. Transitions between decision systems
Behavior generally begins with flexible planning systems but, for repeated behaviors, can become driven by the less-flexible (but also less computationally expensive) habit systems. Examples of this development are well known from our experiences. For example, the first time we drive to a new job, we need a travel plan; we pay attention to street-signs and other landmarks. But after driving that same trip every day for years, the trip requires less and less attention, freeing up resources for other cognitive processes such as planning classes, papers, or dinner. The flexible system, however, generally remains available, as when road construction closes one’s primary route to work and one now needs to identify a new route. Errors can exist within both systems, as for example, a misjudged plan or a trip so automatic, that if one is not paying attention, one might accidentally find oneself having driven to work even though one planned to go somewhere else. This interaction is well-studied in the animal literature, including the overlaying of planning by habit systems (Dickinson 1980; Hikosaka et al. 1999; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2002), restoration of planning in the face of changes (Gray & McNaughton 2000; Isoda & Hikosaka 2007; Sakagami et al. 2006), and conflict between the two systems (Gold 2004; McDonald & White 1994; Packard 1999; Poldrack & Packard 2003; Redish et al. 2000).
Four well-studied examples in the animal literature are the transfer of place strategies to response strategies in the plus-maze (Chang & Gold 2004; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Yin & Knowlton 2004), the development of the regularity of behavioral paths (Barnes et al. 2005; Jog et al. 1999; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2002), the disappearance of devaluation in animal learning studies (Adams & Dickinson 1981; Balleine & Dickinson 1998; Colwill & Rescorla 1985; Tang et al. 2007), and the inhibition of habitual responses in go/no-go tasks (Gray & McNaughton 2000; Husain et al. 2003; Isoda & Hikosaka 2007).
In the plus-maze, animals are trained to take an action that can be solved either by going to a specific place (Tolman et al. 1946) or by taking an action in response to being placed on the maze (Hull 1952). These algorithms can be differentiated by an appropriately designed probe trial (Barnes et al. 1980; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Restle 1957). Rats on this task (and on other similar tasks) first use a place strategy, which then evolves into a response strategy (McDonald & White 1994; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Yin & Knowlton 2004). Place strategies depend on hippocampal, as well as ventral and dorsomedial striatal integrity, while response strategies depend on dorsolateral striatal integrity (Packard & McGaugh 1996; Yin & Knowlton 2004; 2006; Yin et al. 2005).
In tasks in which animals are provided a general task with specific cases that change from day to day or session to session, animals can learn the specific instantiations very quickly. In these tasks, behavioral accuracy improves quickly, followed by a slower development of a regularity in the actions taken by the animal (rats, Jog et al. 1999; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2002; monkeys, Hikosaka et al. 1999; Rand et al. 1998; 2000; humans, Nissen & Bullemer 1987; Willingham et al. 1989). In these tasks, the early (accurate, flexible, and slower) behavior is dependent on hippocampal integrity and correlated to hippocampal activity (Ferraro et al. 1993; Johnson & Redish 2007; Knopman & Nissen 1987), whereas later (also accurate, but inflexible and faster) behavior is dependent on dorsolateral striatal integrity and correlated to dorsolateral striatal activity (Barnes et al. 2005; Doyon et al. 1998; Hikosaka et al. 1998; Jackson et al. 1995; Jog et al. 1999; Knopman & Nissen 1991).
The implication of multiple decision-making systems on the calculation of value can also be seen in the effect of these two decision systems on changes in the valuation of a reward (Adams & Dickinson 1981; Balleine & Dickinson 1998; Colwill & Rescorla 1985; Dickinson 1980; 1985). Classically, these differences are measured by (1) training an agent to take an action (or a sequence of actions) to receive a reward R, and then, (2) changing the value of reward R to the agent, usually in a different context. The value of a reward can be changed by providing excess amounts of the reward (satiation, Balleine & Dickinson 1998) or by pairing the reward with an aversive stimulus, such as lithium chloride (devaluation, Adams & Dickinson 1981; Colwill & Rescorla 1985; Holland & Rescorla 1975; Holland & Straub 1979; Nelson & Killcross 2006; Schoenbaum et al. 2006a). Finally, (3) the agent is provided the chance to take the action. If the action-selection process takes into account the current value of the reward, then the agent will modify its actions in response to the change, but if the action-selection process is an association between the situation and the action (hence does not take into account the value of the reward), the agent will not modify its response. Lesions to ventral striatum (Corbit et al. 2001; Schoenbaum et al. 2006c) and prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex (Killcross & Coutureau 2003) or orbitofrontal cortex (Ostlund & Balleine 2007; Schoenbaum et al. 2006a; 2006b) discourage devaluation, whereas lesions to dorsolateral striatum (Yin et al. 2004; Yin & Knowlton 2004; Yin et al. 2006) and infralimbic cortex (Coutureau & Killcross 2003; Killcross & Coutureau 2003) encourage devaluation processes. Lesions to entorhinal cortex (Corbit & Balleine 2000) and dorsomedial striatum (Adams et al. 2001; Ragozzino et al. 2002a; 2002b; Yin et al. 2005) disrupt flexibility in the face of predictability changes (contingency degradation), whereas lesions to dorsolateral striatum do not (Yin & Knowlton 2006).
It is important to note that not all transitions need be from planning strategies to habit strategies. Because planning strategies are flexible and learned quickly, while habit-based strategies are more rigid and learned more slowly, many tasks are solved in their early stages through the planning system and in their late stages through the habit system (Dickinson 1980; Hikosaka et al. 1999; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Restle 1957). But the habit system can also learn in the absence of an available planning system (Cohen & Squire 1980; Day et al. 1999; Knowlton et al. 1994; Mishkin et al. 1984). Under appropriate conditions, well-developed automated responses can be overridden by controlled (planning-like) systems as in go/no-go tasks (Goldman et al. 1970; Gray & McNaughton 2000; Isoda & Hikosaka 2007; Iversen & Mishkin 1970) or reversal learning (Hirsh 1974; Mackintosh 1974; Ragozzino et al. 2002a). Which system drives behavior at which time depends on parameters of the specific task (Curran 1995; McDonald & White 1994; O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Redish 1999) and may even differ between individuals under identical experimental conditions. In many cases, identical behaviors can be driven by the two systems, and only specialized probe trials can differentiate them (Barnes 1979; Curran 2001; Hikosaka et al. 1999).
3.2. The planning system
The planning system requires recognition of a situation and/or context S, identification of the consequences of taking action a in situation S (recognition of a means of achieving outcome O), and the evaluation of the value of achieving outcome O. This system selects the most appropriate action by considering the potential consequences of that action. The key behavioral parameters involved in this system are fast storage and slow retrieval. As noted earlier, retrieval within this system can be slow because the calculation of value at each step requires processing through the consideration of possibilities. Because the consideration of possibilities does not commit one to a single choice, this system is flexible in its behavioral choices. Because the value of taking action a in situation S is calculated from the value of achieving expected outcome O, which is calculated online from the current needs of the agent, if the desire (need) for the outcome is changed (even in another context), the value calculation can reflect that change.
Computationally, the planning system is likely to require three interacting components: a situation-recognition component, which classifies the complex interaction of contexts and stimuli to identify the situation in which the animal finds itself; a prediction component, which calculates the consequences of potential actions; and an evaluative component, which calculates the value of those consequences (taking into account the time, effort, and probability of receiving reward).
The situation-recognition component entails a categorization process, in which the set of available cues and contexts must be integrated with the agent’s memory so as to produce a classification of the situation. This system is likely to arise in cortical sensory and association systems through competitive learning (Arbib 1995; Grossberg 1976; Redish et al. 2007; Rumelhart & McClelland 1986). Mathematically, the cortical recognition system can be modeled with attractor network dynamics (Durstewitz et al. 1999; 2000; Kohonen 1984; Laing & Chow 2001; Redish 1999; Seamans & Yang 2004; Wilson & Cowan 1972; 1973), in which a partial pattern can be completed to form a remembered pattern through recurrent connections within the structure (Hebb 1949/2002; Hertz et al. 1991; Hopfield 1982). This content addressable memory provides a categorization process transforming the observed set of cues to a defined (remembered) situation that can be reasoned from (Redish et al. 2007).
The prediction component entails a prediction of the probability that the agent will reach situation st+1, given that it takes action a in situation st: P(st+1|st, a). This functionality has been suggested to lie in the hippocampus (Jensen & Lisman 1998; 2005; Johnson & Redish 2007; Koene et al. 2003) or frontal cortex (Daw et al. 2005). The hippocampus has been identified with stimulus-stimulus associations (Devenport 1979; 1980;Devenport & Holloway 1980; Hirsh 1974; Mackintosh 1974; White & McDonald 2002), episodic memory (Cohen & Eichenbaum 1993; Ferbinteanu & Shapiro 2003; Ferbinteanu et al. 2006; Squire 1987), flexible behavior (Devenport et al. 1981b; Gray & McNaughton 2000), including flexible navigation behavior (the cognitive map, i.e., spatial associations between stimuli; O’Keefe & Nadel 1978), as well as in sequence learning (Agster et al. 2002; Cohen & Eichenbaum 1993; Fortin et al. 2002; Levy 1996; Levy et al. 2005) (see Redish [1999] for review). Similar functionality has been proposed to lie in the frontal cortex (Daw et al. 2005), which has long been associated with the ability to recategorize situations (Baddeley 1986; Clark & Robbins 2002; Dalley et al. 2004; Isoda & Hikosaka 2007; Jentsch & Taylor 1999; Quirk et al. 2006; Rushworth et al. 2007) with the storage of delayed events (Baddeley 1986; Fuster 1997; Goldman-Rakic et al. 1990) and the anticipation of reward (Davis et al. 2006; Fuster 1997; Watanabe 2007), as well as with sequence planning (Averbeck & Lee 2007; Kolb 1990; Mushiake et al. 2006; Owen 1997).
The evaluative component allows the calculation of value with each predicted outcome. Anatomically, the evaluative component is likely to include the amygdala (Aggleton 1993; Dayan & Balleine 2002; Phelps & LeDoux 2005; Rodrigues et al. 2004; Schoenbaum et al. 2003), the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) (Daw 2003; Kelley 1999a; 1999b; Kelley & Berridge 2002; Mogenson 1984; Pennartz et al. 1994; Stefani & Moghaddam 2006; Wilson & Bowman 2005) and associated structures (Tindell et al. 2004; 2006), and/or the orbitofrontal cortex (Feierstein et al. 2006; Padoa-Schioppa & Assad 2006; Plassmann et al. 2007; Sakagami & Pan 2007; Schoenbaum et al. 2003; 2006a; Volkow et al. 2003). Neurons in the ventral striatum show reward correlates (Carelli 2002; Carelli et al. 2000; Carelli & Wondolowski 2003; Lavoie & Mizumori 1994; Martin & Ono 2000; Miyazaki et al. 1998) and anticipate predicted reward (Martin & Ono 2000; Miyazaki et al. 1998; Schultz et al. 1992; Yun et al. 2004). Neurons in the ventral pallidum are associated with the identification of hedonic signals (Tindell et al. 2004; 2006). Both the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex project to ventral striatum (Finch 1996; McGeorge & Faull 1989; Swanson 2000), and ventral striatal firing patterns reflect hippocampal and prefrontal neural activity (Goto & Grace 2005a; 2005b; Kalivas et al. 2005; Martin 2001; Pennartz et al. 2004). Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode parameters relating the value of potential choices (Padoa-Schioppa & Assad 2006; Schoenbaum & Roesch 2005).
These structures all receive strong dopaminergic input from the ventral tegmental area. Neurophysiologically, dopamine signals in the ventral striatum – measured by neural recordings from dopaminergic projection neurons (Schultz 1998; 2002) and from voltammetry signals in the ventral striatum itself (Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2005) – show increased firing to unexpected rewards and to unexpected cues predicting rewards. In computational models of the habit system, these signals have been hypothesized to carry value-prediction error information (see further on). Much of the data seems to support a similar role for dopamine from ventral tegmental sources (de la Fuente-Fernandez et al. 2002; Ljungberg et al. 1992; Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2005; Ungless et al. 2004). However, detailed, anatomically instantiated computational models are not yet available for the planning system. Theories addressing dopamine’s role in the planning system have included motivation and effort (Berridge 2006; Berridge & Robinson 1998; 2003; Niv et al. 2007; Robbins & Everitt 2006; Salamone & Correa 2002; Salamone et al. 2005; 2007) and learning (Ikemoto & Panksepp 1999; Reynolds et al. 2001). An important open question, however, is to what extent dopamine is carrying the actual signal of motivation (Berridge 2007) and to what extent dopamine’s effects are dependent on corticostriatal synapses (Anagnostaras et al. 2002; Li et al. 2004; McFarland & Kalivas 2001; McFarland et al. 2003; Nicola & Malenka 1998; Reynolds & Wickens 2002). Finally, dopamine in the prefrontal cortex has also been hypothesized to have a role in controlling the depth of the categorization process (Durstewitz et al. 1999; 2000; Redish et al. 2007; Seamans et al. 2001; Seamans & Yang 2004; Tanaka 2002; 2006).
Neuropharmacologically, these systems, particularly the ventral striatum, are also highly dependent on mechanisms involving opioid signaling. Opioid signaling has been hypothesized to be involved in hedonic processes (Berridge & Robinson 1998; 2003; Kelley et al. 2002). Consistent with these ideas, Levine and colleagues (Arbisi et al. 1999; Levine & Billington 2004) report that opioid antagonists directly interfere with the reported qualia of hedonic pleasure in humans eating sweet liquids, without interfering in taste discrimination. We have suggested that the multiple opioid receptors in the mammalian brain (μ, κ, δ; De Vries & Shippenberg 2002; Herz 1997; 1998) are associated with an evaluation process identifying positive (euphorigenic, signaled by μ-opioid activation) and negative (dysphorigenic, signaled by κ-opioid signaling) evaluations (Redish & Johnson 2007). Whereas μ-receptor agonists are rewarding, euphorigenic, and support self-administration, κ-receptor agonists are aversive, dysphoric, and interfere with self-administration (Bals-Kubik et al. 1989; Chavkin et al. 1982; De Vries & Shippenberg 2002; Herz 1997, 1998; Kieffer 1999; Matthes et al. 1996; Meyer & Mirin 1979; Mucha & Herz 1985).6
We have also proposed that part of the evaluation mechanism occurring during the search process (calculating the expected value from the agent’s needs and the expected outcomes given a relation) may also involve the opioid system (Redish & Johnson 2007). This would predict a release of μ-opioid agonists (e.g., enkephalins) in anticipation of extreme rewards. Rats placed in a drug-associated location show a dramatic increase in released enkephalin in the nucleus accumbens relative to being placed in a saline-associated compartment, presumably in anticipation arising from the drug-associated compartment (Mas-Nieto et al. 2002).7
3.2.1. Potential vulnerabilities in the planning system
The planning system provides potential failure points in changes in the definition of the perceived needs N of the animal, in incorrect identification of satisfaction of that need (mimicking reward), in incorrect evaluation of the expected value of the outcome, and in incorrect search of the relationships themselves, as well as a potential failure point in misclassification of situations.
Vulnerability 1: Homeostatic changes: Changing the definition of the needs N
Vulnerability 2: Allostatic changes: Changing the definition of the needs N
Organisms have evolved to maintain very specific levels of critical biological parameters (temperature, hormonal levels, neurotransmitter levels, etc.) under large challenge variations. Because these specific levels (“set-points”) can change under contextual, biological, social, and other factors, such as with a circadian or seasonal rhythm, some authors have suggested the term allostasis over the more classic term homeostasis, reserving homeostasis for a constant set point (Koob & Le Moal 2006). Drugs and other manipulations can change the needs of an animal either by moving the system away from the homeostatic set-point itself (say, in a withdrawal state after drug use), requiring drugs to return the system to homeostasis, or by changing the system’s desired set-point itself, thus requiring drugs to achieve the new inappropriate set-point (Koob & Le Moal 2006). In either case, these changes will change the perceived needs of the agent, and will thus change the evaluated value of expected outcomes.
Vulnerability 3: Overvaluation of the expected value of a predicted outcome – mimicking reward
As noted earlier, the planning system requires a component that directly evaluates the expected outcome. This evaluation process is, of course, a memory process that must take into account the history of experience with the expected outcome. This means that there must be a biological signal that recognizes the value of outcomes when the agent achieves the outcome itself (thus satisfying the perceived need). This signal is likely to be related to the qualia of euphoric pleasure and dysphoric displeasure (Berridge & Robinson 1998; 2003). We can thus identify this signal with subjective hedonic signals. It is likely that when the agent searches the consequences of the potential action-sequence, the same evaluative process would be used, which could implicate these same signals in craving (Redish & Johnson 2007). This signal is likely to be carried in part by endogenous opioid signals (Berridge & Robinson 1998; 2003; Kelley et al. 2002), potentially in the ventral basal ganglia (Tindell et al. 2004; 2006). Additionally, the memory of value depends on the remembered values of experiences, which tend to be remembered as generally more positive than they really were due to biases in representation (Kahneman & Frederick 2002; Schreiber & Kahneman 2000). Social factors can also affect remembered values of actual dysphoric events (Cummings 2002; Goldman et al. 1999; Jones et al. 2001).
Vulnerability 4: Overvaluation of the expected value of a predicted outcome in the planning system
In fact, any mechanism by which the value of a predicted outcome is increased will have vulnerabilities leading the planning system to over-value certain outcomes. At this point, computational models of the planning system are insufficiently detailed to lead to specific predictions or explanations of the mechanisms by which outcomes are over-valued, but experimental evidence has suggested a role for dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens as a key component (Ikemoto & Panksepp 1999; Robinson & Berridge 1993; Roitman et al. 2004; Salamone et al. 2005; see Robinson & Berridge 2001; 2003, for reviews). The orbitofrontal cortex has also been implicated in the evaluation of potential rewards (Padoa-Schioppa & Assad 2006; Sakagami & Pan 2007; Schoenbaum & Roesch 2005), and incorrect signals arriving from the orbitofrontal cortex could also drive overvaluation of expected drug- or behavior-related outcomes (Kalivas & Volkow 2005; Schoenbaum et al. 2006a; Volkow et al. 2003). Changes in activity in the orbitofrontal cortex (Stalnaker et al. 2006; Volkow & Fowler 2000) and the ventral striatum (Carelli 2002; German & Fields 2007b; Peoples et al. 1999) are likely to play important roles in this vulnerability.
Vulnerability 5: Incorrect search of relationships
The prediction component of the planning system is also a memory process, requiring the exploration of multiple consequences from situation S. If a drug or other process were to increase the likelihood of retrieving a specific relationship, then one would expect this to limit the set of possibilities explored, which would appear as a cognitive blinding to alternatives (Redish & Johnson 2007). Because action-decisions in the planning system must be made through the comparison of available alternatives, this vulnerability would also mean that when an agent is returned to situation S, it would be more likely to remember the availability of outcome O than other potential outcomes, which would make it more likely to remember the high value associated with outcome O (see Vulnerabilities 3 and 4), and thus more likely to experience craving in situation S. This craving would then lead to a recurring search of the same path, which would appear as a cognitive blinding or obsession. This process could also lead to an increase in attention to drug-related cues, which has been seen in both alcohol and heroin addicts (Lubman et al. 2000; Schoenmakers et al. 2007).
Vulnerability 6: Misclassification of situations
In order to retrieve an relation, the agent must recognize that the situation it is in is sufficiently similar to a previous one to successfully retrieve the relation, predict the outcome, and evaluate it. The relations are, of course, dependent on the predictability of the outcome for a given situation, and therfore are sensitive to contingency degradation, in which the predictability of an outcome from a given situation-action pair is decreased (thus changing the relationship; Corbit & Balleine 2000; Corbit et al. 2002; Devenport & Holloway 1980). These relationships can be misunderstood either by over-categorization, in which two situations that are actually identical are miscategorized as different, or by over-generalization, in which two situations that are actually separate are miscategorized as the same.
Over-categorization. Thus, for example, if gambling losses are not recognized as occurring in the same situation as previous gambling wins, an agent can potentially (incorrectly) learn two relations, one leading from situation S1 to a winning outcome and one leading from situation S2 to a losing outcome. If the agent can identify cues that separate situation S1 from situation S2, then it will (incorrectly) predict that it can know when it will achieve a winning outcome. This has been referred to as “the illusion of control” (Custer 1984; Griffiths 1994; Langer & Roth 1975; MacKillop et al. 2006; Redish et al. 2007; Wagenaar 1988).
Over-generalization. An inability to recategorize situations (by recognizing actual changes) can lead to the perseveration of responses and an inability to switch responses in the face of failures and losses. Many drug users and pathological gamblers show failures to reverse or switch action-selection choices in response to novel adverse conditions (Bechara et al. 2001; Clark & Robbins 2002; Everitt et al. 1999; Grant et al. 2000; Jentsch et al. 2002; Verdejo-Garcia et al. 2006). Developing abilities to recategorize situations has been suggested as one means of treating addictions (McCaul & Petry 2003; Sylvain et al. 1997). Simulated agents with deficiencies in the ability to recategorize cues find difficulty in breaking cue-addiction associations8 (Redish et al. 2007).
3.3. The habit system
In contrast to the complexity of the planning system, the habit system entails a simple association between situation and action. Thus, the habit system requires recognition of a situation S, and a single, identified action to take within that situation. This simplicity allows the habit system to react quickly. However, this simplicity also makes the habit system rigid. A learned association essentially commits the agent to take action a in situation S. This means that it would be dangerous to store an association that was not reliable. Therefore, habit associations should only be stored after extensive experience with a consistent relation.
In contrast to the planning system, the habit system does not include any consideration of the potential outcome (i.e., there is no O term in the relation; Table 2). Therefore, in contrast to the planning system, the habit system does not include a prediction of available outcomes and cannot evaluate those potential outcomes online. Hence, it cannot take into account the current perceived needs (desires) of the agent. The habit system is still sensitive to the overall arousal levels of the agent. Thus, a hungry rat will run faster and work harder than a satiated rat (Bolles 1967; Munn 1950; Niv et al. 2007). However, because the habit system does not reflect the current desires of the agent, the habit system will not modify responses when a reward is devalued. Similarly, the habit system cannot select multiple actions in response to a single situation.9 This means that in navigation, the habit system can only take a single action in response to a given situation. For example, a rat with a damaged planning system cannot decide to turn left for water when it is thirsty on one day and turn right for food when it is hungry on the next day.10
Computationally, there are very good models of how the habit system might work. These models have generally been based on the temporal difference instantiation of reinforcement learning (Daw 2003; Daw et al. 2005; 2006; Dayan et al. 2000; Doya 2000b; Montague et al. 1996; Redish 2004; Schultz et al. 1997; Suri & Schultz 1999; Sutton & Barto 1998). In the simplest version of this model, each situation-action pair is associated with a value (termed Q(S, a); Sutton & Barto 1998).11 When an agent takes an action from a situation, the agent can compare the expected value of taking action a in situation S (i.e., Q(S, a)) with the observed value (the reward received minus the cost spent plus the value of being in the state the agent ended up in):
(1) |
where R(t) is the reward observed, C(t) the cost spent, max[Q(Snew, a)] the most value one can get from the situation the agent finds itself in (Snew), and Q(Sold, a) is the estimated value of taking action a in the situation the agent was in (Sold). By updating Q(Sold, a) by δ, the agent moves its estimate Q(Sold, a) closer to its true value; γ is a discounting parameter (γ < 1) which ensures that the time required to reach future rewards is taken into account (Daw 2003; Sutton & Barto 1998). These models can be trained to learn complex situation-action sequences.
The slow development of a habit association has been most studied in contrast to the fast planning system. Lesions or inactivation of the dorsolateral striatum (Yin & Knowlton 2004; Yin et al. 2004; Yin et al. 2006) and the infralimbic cortex (Coutureau & Killcross 2003; Killcross & Coutureau 2003) prevent the loss of devaluation with experience. Lesions or inactivation of the dorsolateral striatum (McDonald &White 1994; Packard & McGaugh 1992; 1996; Potegal 1972; White & McDonald 2002; Yin & Knowlton 2004) or the parietal cortices (DiMattia & Kesner 1988; Kesner et al. 1989) shift rats from response strategy to map strategies in navigation tasks.
As in the planning system, the habit system requires a situation-recognition component, in which the set of cues and contexts is integrated with the agent’s memory to classify the situation, to allow retrieval of the correct association. As with our discussion in the planning system, we suggest that cortical sensory and association systems classify situations through competitive learning processes (Arbib 1995; Grossberg 1976; Redish et al. 2007; Rumelhart & McClelland 1986). Although there are no neurophysiological data suggesting that this situation categorization system is anatomically separate from that used for the planning system, the identification of the habit system with networks involving dorsal and lateral aspects of striatum, and the planning system with networks involving more ventral and medial aspects of striatum, suggest that the specific cortical systems involved may differ. The association itself, including the mechanisms by which the situation signals are finally categorized to achieve a single action decision, have been hypothesized to include the afferent connections from cortex to dorsolateral striatum (Beiser et al. 1997; Houk et al. 1995; Samejima et al. 2005; Wickens 1993; Wickens et al. 2003).
Neuropharmacologically, the habit system receives strong dopamine inputs from the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc). The dopamine signal has been well studied in the primate (Bayer & Glimcher 2005; Ljungberg et al. 1992; Mirenowicz & Schultz 1994; Schultz 1998; 2002; Waelti et al. 2001). Like the dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area, dopamine neurons in SNpc increase firing in response to unexpected increases in expected value (via unexpected rewards or via cues that lead to an increased expectation of reward), and decrease firing in response to unexpected decreases in value (via lack of expected reward or via cues that lead to a decrease in the expectation of reward). This signal has been identified with the value-error signal δ in the temporal difference reinforcement learning algorithm (Barto 1995; Montague et al. 1995; 1996; Schultz et al. 1997), which can provide dopamine a role in training up associations. Dopamine has been shown to be critical to the learning of habitual associations (Faure et al. 2005). However, the role of dopamine in learning and performance is still controversial (Berridge 2007; Cagniard et al. 2006; Frank et al. 2004; Niv et al. 2007).
Cellularly, neurons in the dorsal striatum have been found to represent key parameters of the temporal difference reinforcement learning algorithm: for example, situation-action associations (Barnes et al. 2005; Carelli & West 1991; Daw 2003; Gardiner & Kitai 1992; Hikosaka et al. 1999; Hikosaka et al. 2006; Jog et al. 1999; Kermadi et al. 1993; Kermadi & Joseph 1995; Matsumoto et al. 1999; Miyachi et al. 1997; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2004; Tremblay et al. 1998); reward delivery (Daw 2003; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2004; White & Hiroi 1998;); and value signals (Daw 2003; Kawagoe et al. 2004; Nakahara et al. 2004). These signals develop in parallel with the development of automated behaviors (Barnes et al. 2005; Itoh et al. 2003; Jog et al. 1999; Samejima et al. 2005; Schmitzer-Torbert & Redish 2004; Tang et al. 2007), through an interaction with dopamine signals (Arbuthnott & Wickens 2007; Centonze et al. 1999; Picconi et al. 2003; Reynolds & Wickens 2002). Functional imaging data from humans playing sequential games show similar correlates to value, δ, and other parameters of these models (McClure et al. 2003; 2004; O’Doherty 2004; O’Doherty et al. 2004; Seymour et al. 2004; Tanaka et al. 2004a).
3.3.1. Potential vulnerabilities in the habit system
The primary failure point of the habit system is the overvaluation of a habit association through the delivery of dopamine (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Di Chiara 1999; Redish 2004). As with the planning system, a misclassification of the situation can also provide a potential vulnerability in the habit system (see Vulnerability 6).
Vulnerability 7: Overvaluation of actions
With natural rewards, once the value of the reward has been correctly predicted, the value-error term δ is zero and learning stops (Rescorla & Wagner 1972; Schultz & Dickinson 2000; Waelti et al. 2001). However, when dopamine is produced neuropharmacologically, sidestepping the calculation of δ, each receipt of the drug induces a positive δ signal, the value associated with taking action a in situation S continues to increase, producing an overvaluation (Redish 2004). Because the association is a habitual, automatic association, choices driven by relationships will be unintentional, robotic, perhaps even unconscious.
3.4. Interactions between planning and habit systems
Generally, the planning system is engaged early; but with experience, behavioral control in repetitive tasks is transferred to the habit system. This has been observed in the navigation (O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Packard & McGaugh 1996; Redish 1999), animal conditioning (Balleine & Dickinson 1998; Dickinson 1985; Yin et al. 2006), and human learning (Jackson et al. 1995; Knopman & Nissen 1991; Poldrack et al. 2001) literatures. However, in tasks which require behavioral flexibility, behavioral control can remain with the planning system, even in highly trained animals (Gray & McNaughton 2000; Killcross & Coutureau 2003; McDonald & White 1994; Morris et al. 1982; White & McDonald 2002).
For many tasks, both systems can drive behavior in the absence of the other (Cohen & Squire 1980; Nadel 1994; O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Squire 1987), but some tasks can only be solved by one system or the other. For example, the water maze requires a flexible response to reach a hidden platform and requires hippocampal integrity to be learned quickly (Morris et al. 1982; for a review, see Redish 1999). If the flexibility of the required response is decreased (by, for example, starting the animal in the same location each trial), then the hippocampus no longer becomes necessary to reach the platform (Eichenbaum et al. 1990). Other tasks, such as mirror-writing or the serial reaction time task, require the slow recognition of regularities in situation-action associations, and they are learned at similar speeds by patients with damaged and intact planning systems (Cohen & Squire 1980; Ferraro et al. 1993; Knopman & Nissen 1987). Patients with damaged lateral striatal systems are impaired on these habit-based tasks (Doyon et al. 1998; Ferraro et al. 1993; Knopman & Nissen 1991; Smith et al. 2000; Yin & Knowlton 2006). For some tasks, the planning system can “train up” the habit system, potentially through replay events occurring during subsequent sleep states (Buzsáki 1996; Hoffmann & McNaughton 2002; Marr 1971; Pavlides & Winson 1989; Redish 1999; Redish & Touretzky 1998; Wilson & McNaughton 1994). This transfer of information between systems can explain observations of incomplete retrograde amnesia with certain lesions (consolidation, Cohen & Squire 1980; Nadel & Bohbot 2001; Nadel & Moscovitch 1997; Redish 1999; Squire 1987) but predicts that “consolidated memories” will be less flexible than unconsolidated memories (Redish & Touretzky 1998).
The question of which system drives behavior when the two are put into conflict has only begun to be addressed computationally (Daw et al. 2005) and experimentally (Isoda & Hikosaka 2007), but there is a large literature on behavioral inhibition, in which a changed, novel, or potentially dangerous or costly behavior is inhibited (Gray & McNaughton 2000). This system seems to involve the prefrontal (Sakagami et al. 2006) and/or the hippocampal system (Gray & McNaughton 2000), depending on the specific conditions involved. Whether the interaction entails the planning system overriding a developed habit (Gray & McNaughton 2000) or an external system that mediates control between the two (Isoda & Hikosaka 2007) is still unresolved. Whether such an external mediator can be identified with executive control (presumably, in the prefrontal cortex, Baddeley 1986; Barkley 2001; Barkley et al. 2001) is still a matter of open research.
Vulnerability 8: Selective inhibition of the planning system
The habit system is inflexible, reacts quickly, “without thinking,” whereas the planning system is highly flexible and allows the consideration of possibilities. The habit and planning systems consist of different anatomical substrates. Pharmacological agents that preferentially impair function in structures involved in the planning system or preferentially enhance function in structures involved in the habit system would encourage the automation of behaviors. A shift back from habits to planned behaviors is known to involve the prefrontal cortex (Dalley et al. 2004; Husain et al. 2003; Isoda & Hikosaka 2007; Iversen & Mishkin 1970), and has been hypothesized to involve executive function (Barkley 2001; Barkley et al. 2001; Tomita et al. 1999). If pre-existing dysfunction exists within the inter-system control or if pharmacological agents disrupt this inter-system control, then the agent would develop habits quickly and would have difficulty disrupting those established habits. This vulnerability is distinguishable from the specific planning and habit vulnerabilities by its disruption of function of the planning system and/or its disruption of the inter-system conflict resolution mechanism. Thus, the other vulnerabilities affecting the planning system lead the planning system to make the incorrect choice; Vulnerability 8 makes it difficult for the planning system to correct a misguided habit system (Bechara 2005; Bechara et al. 2001; Bickel et al. 2007; Gray & McNaughton 2000; Jentsch & Taylor 1999; Lubman et al. 2004; Verdejo-Garcia et al. 2006).
3.5. Summary: Decision-making systems
The decision-making system in the mammal is hypothesized to consist of two subsystems: a planning system, based on the evaluation of potential possibilities (e.g., relationships), and a habit system, based on the association of specific actions with specific situations (e.g., associations), both of which require a situation- recognition system, in which observed cues are categorized into situations (e.g., the S terms in the previous formulations). Correct decision-making depends on the integrity of each of these systems (see Figure 1).
3.6. Additional failure points
The eight vulnerabilities identified so far are certainly an incomplete list of the potential failure points of the decision-making system. The description of the decision-making system is, by necessity, incomplete. For example, we have not addressed the question of discounting and impulsivity. Nor have we addressed the question of learning rates.
Vulnerability 9: Over-fast discounting processes
Both the planning and habit systems need to take into account the probability and the delay before an expected goal will be achieved (Ainslie 1992; 2001; Mazur 2001; Stephens & Krebs 1987). In the planning system, this can be calculated online from the expected value of the expected goal given the searched sequence; in the habit system, this would have to be cached as part of the stored value function. The specific mechanism (and even the specific discounting function) are still a source of much controversy (for a review, see Redish & Kurth-Nelson [in press] in Madden et al. [in press]), but the long-term discounting of future rewards is well established. If an agent discounts too strongly, it will overemphasize near-term rewards and underemphasize far-future costs. Because addictions often involve near-term pleasures and far-future costs, faster-than-normal discounting could drive an agent to underestimate the far-future costs and to choose those near-term pleasures. A number of studies have found that addicts discount faster than non-addicts (Alessi & Petry 2003; Bickel & Marsch 2001; Kirby et al. 1999; Madden et al. 1997; Madden et al. 1999; Odum et al. 2002; Petry 2001; Petry & Bickel 1998; Petry et al. 1998; Vuchinich & Simpson 1998).
Vulnerability 10: Changes in learning processes
Other unincorporated components include changes in learning processes (such as over-attention to cues or learning rates being too high or too low). More detailed models of each of the systems will be required before it will be possible to make strong claims about the consequences of such a potential failure point.
However, the hypothesis put forward in this paper that addiction is a consequence of falling victim to vulnerabilities (failure modes) in the decision-making system lays out a research paradigm with important consequences both for what (and how) research questions should be addressed as well as for drug-treatment paradigms and drug-control policies.
These vulnerabilities in the decision-making system may arise from individual predisposition (either due to genetic or social/environmental factors) as well as from drug- or behavior-driven interactions with the decision system. In the second half of this article, we address the interactions and implications of each of the previously identified vulnerabilities with drugs and behaviors of abuse as well as the policy and treatment consequences of the theory.
4. Addiction as vulnerabilities in decision-making
The unified framework for decision-making described above has potential access points through which it can be driven to make maladaptive choices, particularly choices which entail seeking of certain drugs or behaviors. Ten key vulnerabilities can be directly identified with this unified decision-making system as outlined earlier. They are summarized in Table 4 and related to the current theories in Table 5.
Table 4.
Failure Point | Description | Key Systems | Clinical Consequence |
---|---|---|---|
Vulnerability 1 | Moving away from homeostasis | Planning | Withdrawal |
Vulnerability 2 | Changing allostatic set points | Planning | Changed physiological set points, craving |
Vulnerability 3 | Mimicking reward | Planning | Incorrect action-selection, craving. |
Vulnerability 4 | Sensitization of motivation | Planning | Incorrect action-selection, craving. |
Vulnerability 5 | Increased likelihood of retrieving a specific relation | Planning | Obsession |
Vulnerability 6a | Misclassification of situations: overcategorization | Situation-recognition | Illusion of control, hindsight bias |
Vulnerability 6b | Misclassification of situations: overgeneralization | Situation-recognition | Perseveration in the face of losses |
Vulnerability 7 | Overvaluation of actions | Habit | Automated, robotic drug-use |
Vulnerability 8 | Selective inhibition of the planning system | System-Selection | Fast development of habit learning |
Vulnerability 9 | Overfast discounting processes | Planning, habit | Impulsivity |
Vulnerability 10 | Changes in learning rates | Planning, habit | Excess drug-related cue associations |
Note. Because Vulnerability 6 affects the situation S term in both planning and habit systems, we identify it as affecting “situation-recognition.” Vulnerability 8 affects the interaction between the planning and habit systems. Vulnerabilities 9 and 10 can affect components of the planning and habit systems. Detailed models of the effects of these last two vulnerabilities on the systems are as yet unavailable.
Some of these failure modes exist as prior conditions, making an agent more vulnerable to the addictive process, whereas other failure modes are driven by direct interactions with the drugs themselves.
4.1. Vulnerability 1: Deviations from homeostasis
A classic example of deviations from homeostasis that will produce changing needs is the well-known “crash” after the euphoria of an opiate experience (Koob & Le Moal 2006). These negative effects can occur after even a single dose of morphine (Azolosa et al. 1994; Harris & Gewirtz 2005; Koob & Le Moal 2006). Such a negative affect would drive an agent to attempt to compensate by returning to the positive qualia occurring during the drug use. Deviations from homeostasis also lead to the well-known withdrawal symptoms (Altman et al. 1996; Lowinson et al. 1997) seen in reaction to nicotine (Benowitz 1996; Hanson et al. 2003; Hughes & Hatsukami 1986), alcohol (Kiefer & Mann 2005; Littleton 1998; Moak & Anton 1999), opiates (Altman et al. 1996; Koob & Bloom 1988; Schulteis et al. 1997), and caffeine (Daly & Fredholm 1998; Evans 1998) addictions.
4.2. Vulnerability 2: Changes in allostatic set-points
Drug use, particularly repeated drug use, can also produce changes in the set-point itself (referred to as “changes in allostasis,” most likely through long-term changes in receptor levels and changes in levels of endogenous ligands released during normal behaviors; Koob & Le Moal 2006). Animals given prolonged access to drugs, particularly access over many days to long periods of drug availability, develop greatly increased drug-intake levels (Ahmed & Koob 1998; 1999). This has been hypothesized to arise from developing allostatic changes (Ahmed & Koob 2005; Koob & Le Moal 2006).
Pharmacologically, chronic nicotine use changes levels of cholinergic receptors in the brain (Flores et al. 1997; Marks et al. 1992). Chronic alcohol use changes function and expression of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABAA) and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors (Hunt 1998; Littleton 1998; Valenzuela & Harris 1997). Repeated cocaine (Hurd & Herkenham 1993; Steiner & Gerfen 1998), alcohol (Ciraulo et al. 2003), and opiate (Cappendijk et al. 1999; Weissman & Zamir 1987) treatment all produce changes in endogenous opioid release and in opiate receptor expression. Many smokers titrate the number of cigarettes smoked throughout the day, ensuring a relatively constant blood-plasma level of nicotine (Schmitz et al. 1997).
These neurobiological changes change the identified needs of the agent, and thus imply changes in the evaluation of expected outcomes of drug-taking (or abstinence), which will change action-selection in the planning system.12 This vulnerability is identifiable by changes in long-term set-points of physiological variables.
4.3. Vulnerability 3: Overvaluation of the expected value of a predicted outcome – mimicking reward
The planning system requires a signal that directly evaluates the successful achievement of a perceived need (thus leading to the qualia of pleasure). A number of authors have suggested that this may reside within the opiate system (Berridge & Robinson 2003; Redish & Johnson 2007). μ-Opiate agonists (such as heroin, morphine, etc.) are generally highly euphorigenic (Jaffe et al. 1997; Mark et al. 2001; Meyer & Mirin 1979). Even though exogenously delivered μ-opiate agonists (such as heroin or morphine) are not a true reward that the system evolved to recognize, they can mimic the reward system and trick the system into believing that it just received a strong reward, which it will learn to return to. Drugs accessing this vulnerability are likely to be highly euphorigenic, particularly with initial use. Heroin and morphine produce profound euphoria very quickly after injection (Koob & Le Moal 2006). This reward signal will be stored in memories associated with the planning system, which would lead to the recall of highly euphoric signals when the planning system recognizes a path to achieve these reward-mimicking drugs. This vulnerability is recognizable by strong craving when agents recall those euphoric events.
4.4. Vulnerability 4: Overvaluation in the planning system
As reviewed earlier, the planning system consists of recognition (memory), search through, and evaluation of relationships. A fundamental vulnerability of this relationship is in the valuation of the outcome, which is calculated from the level of “need” and the “value” of the outcome satisfying that perceived need, presumably learned through dopaminergic signals (Robinson & Berridge 1993; 2001; 2003; 2004) projecting to the ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex. Dopamine firing patterns in the ventral tegmental area (projecting to the ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex) indicate changes in the amount of expected or just-received reward (Pan et al. 2005; Roesch et al. 2007; Schultz 2002; Schultz et al. 1997), analogous to the δ signal required for the Q-learning algorithm described in the habit system. Although no computational theories are as yet available describing how these dopaminergic signals translate into changes in evaluation in the planning system, voltammetry recordings from the ventral striatum have shown dopamine signals occurring before both cued and self-initiated actions leading to drug receipt (Phillips et al. 2003; Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2004; 2005). These changes presumably modulate the cortico-and hippocampo-ventral-striatal synapses, both during learning (Thomas et al. 2001) and during performance (Goto & Grace 2005a; 2005b; Lisman & Grace 2005; Yun et al. 2004).
Other researchers have suggested that this evaluation process may arise in the orbitofrontal cortex (Padoa-Schioppa & Assad 2006; Plassmann et al. 2007; Schoenbaum et al. 2006a) and that overvaluation in the orbito-frontal cortex can lead to overvaluation of expected rewards (Kalivas & Volkow 2005; Volkow et al. 2003). In rats with a past history of cocaine intake, the orbitofrontal cortex becomes less capable of predicting adverse outcomes than in normal rats (Stalnaker et al. 2006), implying a potential difficulty in identifying negative consequences. Nevertheless, an overvaluation of expected drug outcomes would produce craving (Redish & Johnson 2007) and an increased likelihood of taking actions leading to those expected drug outcomes (German & Fields 2007a).
4.5. Vulnerability 5: Incorrect search of relationships
As noted earlier, the prediction component of the planning system is also a memory process, requiring the exploration of multiple consequences from a given situation S. This prediction process has been suggested to require the hippocampus (Jensen & Lisman 1998; 2005; Johnson & Redish 2007) and the prefrontal cortex (Daw et al. 2005), but the specific mechanism is still unknown.
Although the specific mechanisms of storage and access of relationships are unknown within the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, Vulnerability 5 can occur due to maladaptive and often subtle modifications of system storage and access functions rather than system failure. These systemic modifications may occur as a result of changes in the cell morphology within these areas and plasticity mechanisms within the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. A number of addictive substances produce such changes. Both hippocampus and prefrontal cortex receive dopaminergic inputs, which are known to change the sensitivity to synaptic plasticity (Huang et al. 1995; Seamans & Yang 2004) and to modulate representations (Kentros et al. 2004; Seamans & Yang 2004) and performance (Arnsten et al. 1994; Murphy et al. 1996). Similarly, morphine and other opiate agonists increase synaptic spine formation in cell culture (Liao et al. 2005) and development (Hauser et al. 1987; 1989). Long-term drug exposure also increases the spine formation in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in vivo (Robinson & Kolb 1999; Robinson et al. 2001; 2002). These changes may affect the prediction process, possibly driving it to preferentially search the drug-related potential choices, which would appear clinically as an over-sensitivity to drug-related cues and obsessive consideration of choices leading to drug receipt.
4.6. Vulnerability 6: The illusion of control
When faced with reward distributions that change over time, agents can react in one of two ways: The agent can identify itself as being in a new situation with a different reward distribution, or the agent can identify itself as being in the same situation, but change the expectation of the likelihood of receiving reward. If the agent incorrectly classifies the same situation as different or different situations as the same, the agent may find itself making incorrect decisions.
Misclassification of situations has been primarily identified as a potential cause for problem gambling, in which agents incorrectly identify a statistically unlikely sequence of wins as a separate situation from more-commonly experienced losses (Custer 1984; Langer & Roth 1975; Redish et al. 2007; Wagenaar 1988). This provides the agent with the illusion that certain cues can identify winning situations while other cues identify losing situations (referred to as the “illusion of control”; Langer & Roth 1975; Redish et al. 2007; Sylvain et al. 1997; Wagenaar 1988). Problem gamblers tend to have experienced a statistically unlikely sequence of wins followed by devastating losses (Custer 1984; Wagenaar 1988). This misclassification may arise from excessive recognition of cue changes between the winning and losing experiences (Redish et al. 2007). Problem gamblers are often observed to “explain away” losses by post hoc identification of differences in cues between the losses and memories of wins (also referred to as “hindsight bias”; Custer 1984; Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Wagenaar 1988). Similarly, near misses, in which gamblers lose but come close to the winning situation, encourage continued play (Parke & Griffiths 2004). These near misses may provide illusory support for the hypothesis that certain noisy cues have a relationship to the predictability of the reward.
4.7. Vulnerability 7: Overvaluation in the habit system
In the habit () system, phasic (bursting) dopamine signals are correlated with the value-prediction error signal δ, needed by the temporal difference reinforcement learning algorithm to learn situation-action sequences (Barto 1995; Daw 2003; Montague et al. 1995; 1996). With natural rewards, as the value-prediction system learns to predict those rewards correctly, the value-prediction compensates for the reward, and dopamine at the time of correctly predicted reward decreases to zero with learning (Schultz 1998). Drugs that produce dopamine neuropharmacologically (like cocaine or amphetamine) will bypass that value-compensation system, providing a constant “better-than-expected” δ signal. This non-compensable dopamine signal leads to overvaluation in the system (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Redish 2004). Cocaine and many other abused drugs produce large increases of dopamine pharmacologically throughout the striatum (Ito et al. 2002; Kuhar et al. 1988; Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2005). This mechanism can lead to the formation of habits, which have been suggested as a key process in late stages of drug addiction (Altman et al. 1996; Di Chiara 1999; Everitt & Robbins 2005; Robbins & Everitt 1999; Tiffany 1990). Clinically, such users would be unlikely to show strong craving and would manifest a robotic drug use, without conscious planning or statements of drug-seeking. Habit-based drug use could well be uncorrelated to the qualia of pleasure.
4.8. Vulnerability 8: Selective inhibition of the planning system
Exposure to drugs can shift the normal balance between systems, emphasizing one system over the other. For example, pretreatment with amphetamine shifts rats to preferentially use systems that do not show devaluation (i.e., habit-based over planning-based systems) (Nelson & Killcross 2006). Alcohol, as another example, has been hypothesized to preferentially impair hippocampal (Hunt 1998; White 2003) and prefrontal (Oscar-Berman & Marinkovic 2003) function, which would shift the normal balance from the planning to the habit systems. Dickinson et al. (2002) found that alcohol-seeking in rats is driven primarily by mechanisms and does not show devaluation. Similarly, Miles et al. (2003) found that including cocaine in a sucrose solution prevents devaluation. Such distinctions would appear as a fast increase in habitual responses over planning-based responses.
Following from the hypothesis that prefrontal (executive) systems are involved in the shift from habit back to planning systems (Baddeley 1986; Barkley 2001; Barkley et al. 2001; Dalley et al. 2004; Isoda & Hikosaka 2007), deficits in this executive system would lead to a difficulty in breaking habits (Bechara et al. 2001; Jentsch & Taylor 1999; Lubman et al. 2004). Following from the hypothesis that extinction follows from a reinterpretation of situations (Bouton 2002; Capaldi 1957; Quirk et al. 2006; Redish et al. 2007), this would suggest a difficulty in extinguishing drug-taking. It is certainly possible to extinguish drug-taking in animals (Kalivas et al. 2006; Olmstead et al. 2001), but those extinguished behaviors are particularly susceptible to relapse and reinstatement (McFarland & Kalivas 2001; Shalev et al. 2002). Whether it is more difficult to extinguish some drug-taking behavior in certain agents due to selective inhibition of the planning system or excitation of the habit system is still unknown. Agents falling victim to this vulnerability would show a particularly strong, uncontrolled relapse, likely cue-dependent, and possibly independent of explicitly identified cravings.
4.9. Vulnerability 9: Overfast discounting
As reviewed earlier, there is strong evidence that addicts discount faster than non-addicts (Bickel & Marsch 2001; Reynolds 2006). An important question that is still unresolved is whether these faster discounting factors exist as preconditions or develop with experience. Impulsivity shows a strong heritability that has been hypothesized to underlie a pre-existing factor in addiction (Kreek et al. 2005). Impulsivity has been identified with changes in neuromodulators, particularly serotonin13 (Chamberlain et al. 2006); changing serotonin levels can lead to online changes in discounting rates (Schweighofer et al. 2004; Tanaka et al. 2004b). (Computationally, serotonin has been explicitly modeled as controlling the discounting factor in temporal-difference learning; Doya 2000a; 2002.) Many drugs of abuse, such as cocaine, directly affect serotonin levels (Paine et al. 2003; Ritz et al. 1987), while in other substances, such as alcohol, self-administration levels reflect serotonin levels (Chastain 2006; Valenzuela & Harris 1997). It is currently unknown whether the excess discounting seen in addicts is a pre-existing condition or a consequence of the addictive process itself (Reynolds 2006). As with many of these vulnerabilities, it is possible to have a positive feedback, in which pre-existing conditions support the entrance to addiction (Kreek et al. 2005; Perry et al. 2005; Poulos et al. 1995), and then post-addictive consequences arising from pharmacology or experience exacerbate it (Paine et al. 2003).
4.10. Vulnerability 10: Changes in learning rates
The decision-making system reviewed earlier depends on learning associations among situations, outcomes, and actions. These systems depend on specific learning rates. Neuromodulators such as acetylcholine and dopamine have been hypothesized to control learning rate parameters (Doya 2000a; 2002; Gutkin et al. 2006; Hasselmo 1993; Hasselmo & Bower 1993; Yu & Dayan 2005). Pharmacological substances that manipulate these learning rates can produce enhanced associations, leading to overdeveloped expectations or habits. For example, nicotine enhances the presence of already available phasic dopaminergic signals in vitro (Rice & Cragg 2004). Following from a hypothesized role of phasic dopamine signals in identifying high-value associations to be stored (Montague et al. 1996; Schultz et al. 1997), this would predict that nicotine would enhance small learning signals, further increasing the likelihood of making cue-related associations. Although there is as yet no direct evidence for a general role of nicotine in learning, if nicotine did generally enhance learning signals, this would make smokers particularly susceptible to cue-driven associations (Chiamulera 2005). Multiple drugs taken simultaneously may interact with each other, and drugs may interact with natural rewards as well.
5. Drugs and the taxonomy of vulnerabilities
The 10 vulnerabilities listed here provide a taxonomy of potential problems with decision processes.14 Because neuromodulators (such as acetylcholine, serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine) are involved throughout the decision-making system (learning relations, storing and evaluating relations, recognition of situations S, etc.), drugs of abuse are unlikely to access only one subsystem. Because there are differences in these vulnerabilities, any specific drug is also unlikely to access all 10 vulnerabilities. Because behavioral control involves the entire decision-making systems, behavioral problems such as gambling are likely to arise from an interaction of vulnerabilities. Although each vulnerability can drive an agent to return to the addictive choice, each vulnerability also produces a characteristic symptomology and can thus be separately identifiable within an agent.
Different drugs are likely to access different vulnerabilities. For example, whereas opiates are generally euphorigenic on initial use (Koob & Le Moal 2006; Vulnerability 3), nicotine is often dysphoric on initial use (Heishman & Henningfield 2000; Perkins 2001; Perkins et al. 1996; making Vulnerability 3 unlikely). However, continued use of nicotine can produce strong allostatic changes (Benowitz 1996; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Vulnerability 2), which produce a very strong need to return levels to normal (Fiore 2000). Nicotine also produces increases in the firing of dopaminergic neurons (Balfour et al. 2000; Dani & Heinemann 1996; Pidoplichko et al. 1997), which suggests that it can also accesses the overvaluation vulnerability (Vulnerability 7). Cocaine use automates particularly quickly (Miles et al. 2003) and produces a very strong cued relapse (Altman et al. 1996; Childress et al. 1992; 1993; O’Brien et al. 1992), suggesting that it also accesses the overvaluation vulnerability (Redish 2004; Vulnerability 7), presumably through its direct effects on dopamine (Kuhar et al. 1988; Ritz et al. 1987; Stuber et al. 2004; 2005). However, chronic cocaine use can also produce long-term changes in μ- and κ-opiate receptor levels (Shippenberg et al. 2001), suggesting it can also access the allostatic vulnerability (Vulnerability 2). The appendix provides more details on drugs and the potential vulnerabilities involved with each.
5.1. Individual vulnerabilities
One of the main thrusts of addiction research right now is the question of why some people become addicted and some do not (Deroche-Gamonet et al. 2004; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Tarter et al. 1998; Volkow & Li 2005b). These individual differences arise from an interaction between the genetics of the individual, the development environment (social and physical), the developmental stage of the individual, and the behavioral experience with the addictive substance (Koob & Le Moal 2006; Kreek et al. 2005; Volkow & Li 2005a; 2005b). Laying out the complete details of individual vulnerabilities are beyond the scope of this article (and are in large part still unknown), but the multiple-vulnerabilities hypothesis put forward here suggests a plan of attack to the problem. Addiction research has been historically aimed at problems with a single drug (e.g., nicotine, heroin, alcohol, etc.) or at unifying parameters across drugs (e.g., the role of dopamine). The multiple-vulnerabilities hypothesis suggests that we should look, instead, at the potential vulnerabilities within the natural learning system.
These vulnerabilities are likely to depend on a number of specific individual parameters. For example, imagine an individual who was particularly sensitive to rewards and punishments. That individual would be more susceptible to Vulnerability 3 in which a drug of abuse produced a euphorigenic signal. Imagine an individual who was more likely to treat a slightly new situation as new. Such an individual would be more susceptible to Vulnerability 6 in which wins and losses are not matched. Or imagine an individual in which the effect of nicotine on dopamine was increased. Such an individual would receive a strong dopamine kick with each puff of a cigarette and become particularly vulnerable to Vulnerability 7. Perhaps, in some individuals, nicotine produces excessive dopamine release (Vulnerability 7), leading to a habit-like addiction, whereas in others, nicotine produces allostatic changes (Vulnerability 2), leading to a maintenance-of-levels addiction. Other individuals may have neither of these vulnerabilities, leaving them more resistant to becoming addicted to nicotine.
5.2. Interactions among vulnerabilities
The failure points identified here are not mutually exclusive; they can co-occur. For example, excess dopamine delivered simultaneously to the ventral striatal regions (hypothesized to be involved in the planning system), to the dorsal striatal regions (hypothesized to be involved in the habit system), and to the frontal cortices and hippocampus (hypothesized to be involved in situation-categorization mechanisms) could drive an individual into a host of vulnerabilities. Increased dopamine in the planning system has been hypothesized to lead to increased motivational salience (Robinson & Berridge 2001; 2003; 2004; Vulnerability 4). Increased dopamine to the habit system has been hypothesized to lead to overvaluation of associations (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Redish 2004; Vulnerability 7). Increased dopamine to the situation-categorization system has been hypothesized to change the stability of categorization systems (Redish et al. 2007; Seamans & Yang, 2004; Vulnerability 6). Thus, a single effect of a single drug can access multiple failure points.
Drugs can also produce multiple effects, which can lead to multiple vulnerabilities all leading to maladaptive decisions. For example, cocaine, amphetamine, and methamphetamine pharmacologically block the dopamine transporter (Kuhar et al. 1988; Ritz et al. 1987) leading to increases in dopamine in the ventral striatum (Stuber et al. 2005), but long-term exposure also leads to changes in dopamine receptor levels (Letchworth et al. 2001; Porrino et al. 2004a; 2004b), to a decrease in dopamine release caused by other mechanisms (Martinez et al. 2007), and to changes in long-term depression (LTD) and long-term potentiation (LTP) in ventral (Thomas et al. 2001) and dorsal (Nishioku et al. 1999) striatum. But long-term exposure to cocaine also produces changes in opioid-receptor distributions (Shippenberg et al. 2001). Each of these effects can lead to an agent falling victim to a different vulnerability, which can lead to separate mechanisms driving maladaptive decisions.
Similarly, nicotine has multiple access points throughout the brain (Ikemoto et al. 2006). Repeated nicotine use can lead to allostatic changes in response to the flooding of the system with cholinergic agonists (Benowitz 1996; Koob & Le Moal 2006), but it also leads directly to dopamine release (Pidoplichko et al. 1997), increases the effect of glutamatergic inputs to the ventral tegmental area (Mansvelder & McGehee 2000), and strengthens the effect of already present phasic dopaminergic signals (Rice & Cragg 2004). This combination of vulnerabilities could lead to the subject falling victim to Vulnerability 2 (allostasis), to Vulnerability 7 (overvaluation in the habit system), and to Vulnerability 10 (increased learning rates of drug-related cues) simultaneously. Treatment of only the allostatic component (such as through nicotine replacement therapy, Hanson et al. 2003; Rose et al. 1985) would not treat the simultaneous problem arising from the other vulnerabilities.
The fact that these vulnerabilities can interact implies interactions between drugs which can lead to polydrug abuse. Drugs may be synergistic in their effects on a single vulnerability or they may involve multiple vulnerabilities simultaneously. Cocaine and heroin both affect the opiate system, and allostatic changes made in response to one drug may affect the neurobiological response to another (Leri et al. 2003). Nicotine enhances already present dopaminergic signals (Rice & Cragg 2004), thus the presence of nicotine (potentially changing learning rates, Vulnerability 10) may enhance the ability of other drugs to drive dopamine-produced overvaluation in the planning (Vulnerability 4) or habit systems (Vulnerability 7). Similarly, amphetamine can sensitize cue-driven motivational signals (Wyvell & Berridge 2000), which may explain some of the interaction between cocaine and methamphetamine addiction and sexual behavior (Schneider & Irons 2001). Our theory suggests that polydrug abuse arises from the same causes as drug abuse: Interactions between the agent’s environment (drugs, cues, and experience) and the agent’s internal decision-making system (genetics, planning, and habit systems) lead to the agent falling victim to vulnerabilities in the decision-making system, leading to the continued use of problematic drugs and behaviors.
We are not the first to suggest that decisions to take drugs or to gamble can arise as a consequence of multiple processes. These multiple-process theories are generally discussed in terms of a transition sequence from more cognitive, “planned” processes to less cognitive, more “automatic” processes. For example, Everitt and Robbins (2005) suggest a transition sequence of “actions to habits to compulsion.” Oei and Baldwin (2002) suggest a transition in alcohol consumption from a controlled process to a more automatic habit-based process.
In contrast, it is our contention that there are many paths through these vulnerabilities. It is not always a transition from flexible planning strategies to automated habitual strategies.
Animal experiments have found numerous methods through which animals can appear to lose control over drug-taking, including escalation due to extended exposure to drug availability (Ahmed & Koob 1998; 1999; 2004; 2005; Vanderschuren & Everitt 2004), incubation by separation after exposure to drugs (Bossert et al. 2005; Grimm et al. 2001), relapse due to stress (Shaham et al. 2000; Shalev et al. 2000), relapse due to reinstatement (de Wit & Stewart 1981; McFarland & Kalivas 2001), and even that susceptibility time-courses can change between individuals due to unknown (potentially genetic) factors (Deroche-Gamonet et al. 2004; Goldman et al. 2005; Hiroi & Agatsuma 2005; Ranaldi et al. 2001).
Agents can show addictive decisions through vulnerabilities in planning systems or through vulnerabilities in habit systems or through vulnerabilities in the interaction between them. Our suggestion is that there are many vulnerabilities in the decision-making system and thus many ways for an agent to become addicted. This means that there are many transition sequences as well.
5.3. Transitions
Clinically, the transition to addiction is usually described in terms of three stages: initial exploratory or trial use, subsequent maintenance of drug use associated with the beginning of strong desires (craving), followed in some users by a strong, habitual use in which the user loses control of the drug use (Altman et al. 1996; Everitt & Robbins 2005; Kalivas & Volkow 2005; Lowinson et al. 1997; Oei & Baldwin 2002; Robbins & Everitt 1999). This sequence can be described as a path through the vulnerabilities of the decision systems: once the drug or behavior has been sampled, it will be repeated due to euphorigenic, pharmacological, or socially positive effects. Euphorigenic effects will drive repeated use due to associated reward signals (Vulnerability 3). Pharmacological effects will drive repeated use due to fast homeostatic changes (Vulnerability 1). It is also possible for drugs that are not euphorigenic to be driven by associated socially positive associations, such as has been hypothesized for tobacco (Bobo & Husten 2001; Cummings 2002), alcohol (Bobo & Husten, 2001; Goldman et al. 1987; 1999), and caffeine (Greden & Walters 1997), which we might categorize under Vulnerability 3. However, repeated use will lead to potentiation of the relationship in the planning system (Vulnerability 4) and to the development of allostatic changes (Vulnerability 2), which will lead to strong desires and craving. With sufficient habitual use, actions leading to drug use can become over-valued in the habit system through increased value associated with an relationship (Vulnerability 7). This sequence parallels many examples of normal learning, proceeding from ventral to dorsal striatal systems (Balleine & Dickinson 1998; Everitt et al. 2001; Haber et al. 2000; Letchworth et al. 2001; Packard & McGaugh 1996).
This sequence will not be followed by all individuals or via all drugs of abuse. The timeline with which individuals make these transitions from vulnerability to vulnerability likely depends on a complex interaction between the genetics, development, and drug experience of the individual. We do not expect all addicts to take the same path through this maze of vulnerabilities.
Just as different tasks entail different interactions between planning and habit systems (some tasks entail transitions from planning to habit, other tasks always require the planning system, other tasks require the habit system, and other tasks can entail transitions from habit to more flexible planning systems), we expect different agents (with different genetics, different experiences, etc.) to take different paths through these vulnerabilities. In addition, just as some tasks entail an overlaying of automated habit-like strategies on top of planning-based strategies (e.g., Packard & McGaugh 1996), treating the habit-based vulnerability of a patient may uncover earlier planning-based vulnerabilities. Other agents can show addictive decisions through vulnerabilities in habit or interactive systems without ever passing through planning systems. It is also possible that habit-based addictive decisions may shift to planning-based addictive decisions (e.g., when obstacles are put into place). We argue that, in order to understand and treat the issue of addiction, we need to know not only where the patient is in his or her trajectory through these vulnerabilities, but also which vulnerability (vulnerabilities) the patient has fallen victim to.
5.4. Relapse
The fundamental issue with addiction is that of relapse, which can be defined as drug-seeking or the making of the addictive choice, even after a period of abstinence.
Relapse has been studied both clinically by measuring populations remaining abstinent from drug use and in animals identifying the return to responding for drug after forced removal (extinction, forced abstinence). In humans, relapse can occur after re-exposure to the drug, to cues associated with drug-taking and drug-seeking, and to stress (Self & Nestler 1998; Shalev et al. 2002). Relapse to behavioral addictions (such as gambling) has not been studied in the same detail, but we have suggested that gambling addiction may be related to the reinstatement of responding seen after extinction of normal rewards (Redish et al. 2007, see also Bouton 2002; 2004). In animals, a return to responding can occur due to acute re-exposure to the drug, to cues associated with drug-taking and drug-seeking, and to stress (Bossert et al. 2005; Kalivas et al. 2006; Shaham et al. 2003). The validity of the reinstatement paradigm as a model of abstinence and relapse is still controversial (Kalivas et al. 2006; Katz & Higgins 2003); nevertheless, the reinstatement paradigm can provide an understanding of mechanisms by which relapse could occur (Epstein & Preston 2003).
All of the vulnerabilities noted earlier can potentially drive relapse to the addictive behavior, but the path to relapse will differ depending on the vulnerabilities involved.
For example, relapse driven via homeostatic needs (Vulnerability 1) should occur through the natural time-course of the homeostatic change. Relapse driven through allostatic needs (Vulnerability 2) can be driven by changes in physiological set-points, driven in part by cues or by circadian or other rhythmic changes. The natural time-course of these changes can be seen in some smokers who show a circadian time-course of craving (Benowitz 1996; Perkins 2001; Schmitz et al. 1997). Allostatic set-points driving expectation can also be cue-driven (Ehrman et al. 1992; Hunt 1998; Meyer & Mirin 1979; Siegel 1988). Experienced users can show preparation tolerance with cues associated with heroin (Meyer & Mirin 1979; O’Brien et al. 1977; Siegel 1988). Similarly, alcohol users show fewer coordination deficits under the influence of alcohol in alcohol-associated environments (such as bars) than in non-alcohol-associated environments (such as offices) (Hunt 1998). A number of authors have suggested that relapse under stress may be due to cue-driven deviations from homeostatic set-points (Ahmed & Koob 1997; Shaham et al. 2000; Shalev et al. 2000; Weiss et al. 2001).
Relapse caused by expectation (overvaluation in the planning system, Vulnerabilities 3 and 4) can be identified by “craving.” Such relapse can be triggered by a recall of an association, in which the agent recognizes an action-sequence that can get the agent from the situation the agent is currently in (S) to an over-valued outcome (O). The recognition can be cue-driven or may arise spontaneously, but in either case will entail an expectancy of the outcome. As noted earlier, correct decision-making within the planning component requires a search of multiple possibilities. It is likely that once a pathway to a highly valued outcome is recognized, the search will keep returning to that possibility, producing cognitive blinding and obsession (Vulnerability 5). Both craving and obsession are common to pre-relapse conditions in some (but not all) patients (Altman et al. 1996; Childress et al. 1988; Grant et al. 2006; MacKillop & Monti 2007; O’Brien 2005; Sayette et al. 2000).
Note that our theory predicts that craving should be clinically separable from relapse: Because the planning system is flexible, recognition of a path to an outcome (in our theory, craving is recognition of a path to a high-valued outcome) does not necessarily lead to taking that path. Thus, craving can occur without relapse. Because the habit system does not include recognition of an outcome, in our theory, it does not produce craving. Thus, relapse caused by overvaluation in the habit system (Vulnerability 7) may be robotic, without craving, perhaps without even conscious recognition (Everitt & Robbins 2005; Robbins & Everitt 1999). (Retrospectively, the addict may believe he or she craved the drug, whether or not any actual craving occurred prospectively; Sayette et al. 2000.)
Multiple vulnerabilities can cause a relapse to the addictive choice, but the pathway to that relapse may be different, depending on the vulnerability involved. Therefore, prevention of relapse will also depend on treating the vulnerabilities involved.
5.5. Treatment
Each vulnerability drives the decision-making process towards the addictive choice and provides a potential access-point for the addiction to relapse, but each vulnerability is a different failure-point of the decision process and leads decision-making to error through a different mechanism. Thus, each vulnerability is likely to require a different treatment regimen. This concept (of different treatments for different vulnerabilities) has enjoyed some recent success and been used to explain historical treatment failures. For example, in a recent study (Grant et al. 2006), significant success was found in treating a subset of pathological gamblers that showed strong urges (craving). Irvin and colleagues (Irvin & Brandon 2000; Irvin et al. 2003) suggest that the well-documented decreasing success of smoking cessation in tobacco studies is due to the presence of available over-the-counter cessation-aid products and thus a changing distribution of smokers participating in the studies.
Treatment of the homeostatic deviations and allostatic changes in nicotine through nicotine replacement therapy has been extremely successful (Balfour & Fagerström 1996; Benowitz 1996; Hanson et al. 2003; O’Brien 2005; Rose et al. 1985). However, long-term relapse after these treatments is notoriously high (Balfour & Fagerström 1996; Hanson et al. 2003; Monti & MacKillop 2007). This is likely a consequence of the fact that nicotine replacement therapy does not address the other vulnerabilities involved in nicotine addiction (e.g., Vulnerability 7).
Treatment of the planning vulnerabilities (Vulnerabilities 3, 4, and 5), which lead to excess expectation of positive outcomes (see above) and may be identifiable through craving and obsession (Redish & Johnson 2007), may depend on blocking the misevaluation process. Opiate antagonists have been used to reduce craving in alcohol addictions (Kiefer & Mann 2005; O’Brien et al. 1996) and in gambling (Grant et al. 2006). Many heroin abusers on naltrexone report no craving (Meyer & Mirin 1979; O’Brien 2005, but see Halikas 1997 for another view). Whether this is due to controlling allostatic effects (Koob & Le Moal 2006) or to the blocking of craving and the recognition of future rewards (O’Brien 2005) is still unresolved. Naltrexone treatment of cocaine addicts failed to find a significant effect on craving (Schmitz et al. 2001). It is clear that there is still work to be done to completely elucidate the specific relationship among clinically tested treatments, the qualia identified as craving, and the potential vulnerabilities identified in this article.
Treatment of each vulnerability requires a regimen specifically designed to address that vulnerability. For example, the homeostatic and allostatic vulnerabilities (Vulnerabilities 1 and 2) likely require pharmacological treatment to rebalance the system. Overvaluation in the planning system (Vulnerabilities 3, 4, and 5) likely requires treatment to change the recall and re-evaluation processes, either through pharmacological means or through cognitive behavioral re-training, or some combination of the two. Overvaluation and over-strengthening in the habit system (Vulnerabilities 7 and 8) likely require mechanisms with which to strengthen alternative choices available in the planning system. Miscategorization of situations (Vulnerability 6) likely requires treatments aimed at executive function and its role in re-categorizing situations. Although we have not proposed specific treatments for any of these vulnerabilities, it is our contention that these failure modes are treatable and that treatments aimed at these specific modes are more likely to be successful than general treatments aimed at the general addicted population.
In general, we propose that the clinical treatment of addiction should not be addressed to the general addicted population, or to specific drugs of abuse. Instead, we propose that treatment should first entail the identification of which vulnerabilities have been triggered within the individual, and then treatment should be addressed to the specific constellation of vulnerabilities into which the addicted patient has fallen.
6. Future work is still needed
The thesis of this review is that addiction arises from vulnerabilities inherent in the decision-making system within the brain. Susceptibility to these vulnerabilities arises through an interaction among the genetics of the individual, the development environment, the social milieu, and the behavioral experience of the individual. We have outlined several vulnerabilities that arise from current theories of the mammalian decision-making system. However, it is important to note that the understanding of that decision-making system is still incomplete.
Exactly what differentiates the planning and habit systems is still being debated (e.g., Daw et al. 2005; Dayan & Balleine 2002; Redish & Johnson 2007). Detailed computational models of the habit system have been developed (Montague et al. 1996; Samejima et al. 2005; Suri & Schultz 1999, but see Berridge 2007, for an alternate view), including how those systems could produce addiction-like behavior (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Redish 2004). But computational models of the planning system are still in their earliest stages (Daw et al. 2005; Johnson & Redish 2005; Zilli & Hasselmo, 2008). How these systems interact to produce behavior is still unknown.
A number of open questions still remain. For example, the decision-making theories discussed in this article are primarily about reinforcement (delivery of unexpected reward) and disappointment (non-delivery of expected reward). The role of aversion (delivery of punishment) and relief (non-delivery of expected punishment) in these decision-making systems is still unresolved. Negative symptoms clearly play important roles in addiction (Gawin 1991; Jaffe 1992; Koob & Le Moal 2001; 2005; 2006; O’Brien et al. 1992). How to incorporate those negative symptoms beyond homeostatic (Vulnerability 1) and allostatic (Vulnerability 2) effects is still unclear. Detailed decision-making models in the face of aversion and relief may help elucidate these issues. The fear-conditioning and extinction literature (Domjan 1998; Myers & Davis 2002; 2007) and the roles of the amygdala (Paré et al. 2004; Phelps & LeDoux 2005; Rodrigues et al. 2004) and prefrontal cortex (Milad & Quirk 2002; Quirk et al. 2006) therein are likely to be important starting points for these models.
Similarly, the key parameters that underlie individual differences are still unknown, including whether those key parameters are genetic, environmental, or some combination thereof (Kreek et al. 2005; Volkow & Li 2005b). Models of decision-making can provide candidate variables that may vary across the population, which may change susceptibilities to specific vulnerabilities and would lead to individual reactions to drugs of abuse or potentially addictive behaviors.
The key social definition of a problem addiction relates to the cost to the individual and to society of the addiction. Whereas methamphetamine addiction is a terrible burden on society and thus leads to extreme measures taken to prevent it, caffeine addiction leads to a minor inconvenience to an individual and little or no burden on society. In part, we believe that these differences arise from the different vulnerabilities impacted by these drugs. Problem gambling is often classified as an addiction due in large part to the extreme costs paid by “addicted” individuals. Whether other behaviors, such as shopping or Internet use, should be counted as addictions is an open question (Holden 2001). In this article, we have proposed a new framework for understanding addiction. This new framework provides a new definition of addiction itself as decisions made due to failure modes in the decision-making system. How serious each failure mode is, whether it should be treated, and how it should be treated, are clinical and policy issues that need to be addressed in the future.
The list of vulnerabilities laid out in this target article are certainly incomplete. There are likely to be other processes beyond decision-making that can drive errors, including errors in probability recognition (Kahneman et al. 1982), different responses to gains and losses (Kahneman & Tversky 2000), and errors in memory itself (Schacter 2001). We have not fully explored the potential interactions between the decision-making vulnerabilities, nor have we fully explored the interaction between decision-making vulnerabilities and other memory-based errors.
Clinically, we cannot yet relate these potential vulnerabilities to other action-selection and decision-making disorders such as obsessive-compulsive disorder, Tourette’s syndrome, depression, mania disorders, anxiety disorders, impulsivity disorders, and so on. However, we believe that the paradigm laid out in this article (taking a basic-science understanding of action-selection and decision-making and identifying failure modes) is likely to be fruitful for understanding many other psychiatric disorders beyond addiction.
More importantly, however, we do not yet have specific clinical instruments with which to identify the presence or absence of each vulnerability within an individual, nor do we have specific clinical treatments (pharmacological, behavioral, or otherwise) to suggest. Our hope, however, is that the framework laid out in this article and the identification of these vulnerabilities can lead to research aimed at identification and treatment.
More work elucidating an understanding of the mammalian decision-making system is clearly needed, but we believe that the current understanding of this system can already illuminate addictive processes. It is our belief that an interaction between basic science research on decision-making, basic science research on the neurophysiological effects of addictive substances and behaviors, and the clinical consequences of addiction will illuminate both processes and will provide new avenues for the treatment of addiction.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Daniel Smith, John Ferguson, Jadin Jackson, Zeb Kurth-Nelson, Serge Ahmed, Warren Bickel, Boris Gutkin, Jon Grant, Suck-Won Kim, Antonio Rangel, Steven Grant, Bernard Balleine, Geoff Schoenbaum, and Matthijs van der Meer for helpful discussions. This work was supported by a Sloan Fellowship to A. David Redish, by a Career Development Award from the University of Minnesota TTURC to Redish (NCI/NIDA P50 DA01333), and by a graduate fellowship from the Center for Cognitive Sciences at the University of Minnesota to Adam Johnson (T32HD007151).
Biographies
A. David Redish is Associate Professor of Neuroscience at the University of Minnesota. Dr. Redish has published computational and theoretical papers on neural mechanisms of navigation, memory, decision-making, and addiction, as well as experimental papers on neural information processing patterns in hippocampus and striatum during behavioral tasks. He is the author of Beyond the Cognitive Map: From Place Cells to Episodic Memory (MIT Press, 1999).
Steven L. Jensen is a graduate student in Computer Science and Neuroscience at the University of Minnesota, and Principal Scientist in the Neuromodulation Research Group at Medtronic, Inc. He holds several patents related to implantable medical device technology and has several publications related to Computational Neuroscience.
Adam Johnson is a graduate student at the University of Minnesota and a member of the Graduate Program in Neuroscience and the Center for Cognitive Sciences. He has contributed to several publications on the search for cognitive function within neural circuits.
APPENDIX
The vulnerabilities hypothesis laid out in this target article provides a taxonomy of addictive processes. This means that it should be possible to characterize the effects of drugs of abuse (and problematic behaviors) in terms of these vulnerabilities in the decision-making system. In this appendix, we address the clinical and neurophysiological effects of known drugs of abuse in the light of the vulnerabilities identified in the text: cocaine and psychostimulants (A), opiates (B), nicotine (C), alcohol (D), and caffeine (E). Finally, we discuss problem gambling (F).
A. Cocaine and the psychostimulants
The primary neurobiological effect of cocaine and the psychostimulants is to produce large increases of dopamine pharmacologically, by blocking dopamine reuptake (cocaine, Chen et al. 2006; Kuhar et al. 1988; Ritz et al. 1987)15 or releasing dopamine-containing vesicles (amphetamine, Sulzer et al. 2005). These can be measured quantitatively throughout the dorsal and ventral striatum, and continue to appear, even after cocaine is well predicted (Ito et al. 2002; Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2005). This dopamine release bypasses the brain’s computational systems, which direct when and how dopamine should be released (Schultz 1998; 2002). This non-compensable dopamine release (Di Chiara 1999) has been hypothesized to lead to an overvaluation within the habit system (Bernheim & Rangel 2004; Redish 2004; Vulnerability 7). However, cocaine also leads to dopamine release in the ventral striatum (Ito et al. 2000; Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2005), which can lead to development of over-valuation in the planning system as well (Vulnerability 4).
Cocaine intake also produces transient euphoric highs (Balster 1973; Gold 1997; Volkow et al. 2003), which implies a component that can mimic reward (Vulnerability 3), followed by a very strong post-high crash (Gawin 1991; Koob & Le Moal 2006), which may imply a role of homeostatic mechanisms (Vulnerability 1). One potential issue is that psychostimulants can enhance performance of simple tasks and are sometimes used in reaction to fatigue and boredom (Koob & Le Moal 2006), again implying a potential vulnerability in the relief of homeostatic deviations (Vulnerability 1). With repeated use, users become tolerant to dosages and the subjective high becomes harder to reach with a given dose (suggesting a role for allostasis, Koob & Le Moal 2006; Vulnerability 2).
Cocaine craving is extremely cue-sensitive, in that cues associated with cocaine use lead to strong cravings (Childress et al. 1988; 1992; O’Brien et al. 1992), involving memory circuits including the hippocampus, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex (Childress et al. 1999; Garavan et al. 2000; Grant et al. 1996; Volkow et al. 2003). These circuits are key components of the planning system and suggest an involvement of excess associations, possibly driven by dopamine in the limbic structures (implying a role for Vulnerabilities 4 and 5). However, cocaine craving can be separated from drug-seeking behavior (Dudish-Poulsen & Hatsukami 1997), suggesting that, for some patients, drug-seeking depends on the non-craving-producing vulnerabilities. Anecdotal descriptions suggest the presence of cue-induced “robotic” relapse in the absence of identified craving (Altman et al. 1996, implying a role for Vulnerability 7).
Some researchers have found that cocaine-seeking is goal-directed (Olmstead et al. 2001), implying an involvement of the planning system (Vulnerabilties 1 to 5). However, cocaine and amphetamine have long been associated with motor stereotypies associated with habit-system structures such as the dorsal striatum (Johanson & Fischman 1989; Koob & Le Moal 2006), implying an involvement of Vulnerability 7. Other researchers have found that prior treatment with amphetamine can lead to a faster development of automated behaviors, even during navigation and food-seeking (Nelson & Killcross 2006; O’Tuatheigh et al. 2003), implying an involvement of Vulnerability 8. That many vulnerabilities are accessed by cocaine may be one of the reasons why successful treatment has been so elusive.
B. Opiates
Opiates have been noted as a drug of choice since ancient times (Koob & Le Moal 2006). Modern opiates include the processed forms of the opium poppy, including opium, morphine, heroin, meperidine (Demerol), oxycodone (OxyContin), and codeine. Abused opiate drugs are all strong agonists of the μ-opioid receptor (Jaffe et al. 1997; Negus et al. 1993; van Ree et al. 1999), in contrast to drugs that have strong κ-agonist properties (Jaffe et al. 1997).
Five components of the reaction to opioid intake have been identified (Koob & Le Moal 2006): a euphoric rush of intense pleasure, often characterized by analogy to sexual orgasm, followed by a general feeling of well-being, followed then by a detached, separated state which can include virtual unconsciousness, and finally, a fade back to an appearance of normality. This is then followed by a fifth, highly dysphoric withdrawal state (Jaffe et al. 1997; Koob & Le Moal 2006).
The presence of the intense euphorigenia suggests a relationship to Vulnerability 3. The development of tolerance and the presence of a withdrawal state suggest a potential implication of Vulnerability 2. The fact that the withdrawal can occur after only a single use (Azolosa et al. 1994; Harris & Gewirtz 2005; Koob & Le Moal 2006) suggests the presence of homeostatic changes (Vulnerability 1) as well as long-term allostatic changes (Vulnerability 2). However, relapse can also occur well after all identified withdrawal symptoms have subsided (Shalev et al. 2002).
One potential explanation for relapse long after obvious withdrawal symptoms have faded is changes in expectations arising from associations with environmental stimuli (Meyer & Mirin 1979). Homeostatic expectation can also be cue-driven (Ehrman et al. 1992; Meyer & Mirin 1979; Siegel 1988). Experienced users can show preparation tolerance with cues associated with heroin (Meyer & Mirin 1979; O’Brien et al. 1977; Siegel 1988). However, opiate addicts are not generally described as “robotic” (Altman et al. 1996), and opiate addiction generally involves a strong craving component (Meyer & Mirin 1979). These data suggest that the environmental stimuli are more related to associations, rather than associations, implying a stronger involvement of the planning system (Vulnerabilities 4 and 5) than of the habit system (Vulnerability 7).
Many studies have reported that heroin delivery leads to dopamine activity in vitro (Johnson & North 1992) and dopamine release in the accumbens shell in vivo (Caillé & Parsons 2003; Hemby et al. 1995; Kiyatkin 1994; Kiyatkin & Rebec 1997; Tanda et al. 1997; Wise et al. 1995; Xi et al. 1998). Although cocaine and psychostimulants always increase dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens and striatum, no matter how well predicted (Stuber et al. 2005), Hemby et al. (1995) have reported that heroin only increases dopamine in unpredicted conditions. This finding, however, has not been replicated by other labs (Caillé & Parsons 2003; Wise et al. 1995; Xi et al. 1998) which have found that heroin self-administration does increase dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens. Kiyatkin and Rebec (1997; 2001) report an increase in dopamine in the nucleus accumbens on initiation of self-administration, in preparation for self-administration, and in response to presentation of a heroin-associated cue, but a sudden drop in response to the actual delivery of heroin during self-administration maintenance. This sequence is very similar to that seen in the lever-press for food (Kiyatkin & Gratton 1994; Schultz 1998; 2002), but very different from cocaine (Roitman et al. 2004; Stuber et al. 2005). Kiyatkin (1994) also reports that passive delivery of heroin led to long-term increases in dopamine similar to that reported by Hemby et al. (1995). Recently Georges et al. (2006) found no effect of morphine on dopamine cells in vivo in morphine-dependent rats. We suggest that re-examining these data in light of the hypothesized roles of the dopamine and opiate systems (Berridge & Robinson, 1998; 2003; Montague et al. 1996; Redish, 2004; Redish & Johnson 2007; Schultz 1998; 2002; and see target article earlier) may be fruitful, and we believe that these data suggest that opiate addiction is not likely to involve Vulnerability 7.
C. Nicotine
Nicotine is the primary addictive substance in tobacco products, including cigarettes, as well as smokeless tobacco products (Schmitz et al. 1997). Nicotine is extremely addictive, with a very large proportion of teenagers who sample cigarettes eventually succumbing to long-term regular use (Russell 1990). The neurobiological effects of nicotine are well reviewed elsewhere (Benowitz 1996; Koob & Le Moal 2006) and therefore not reviewed here. Nicotine treatment has primarily been through prevention education (Fiore 2000; Schmitz et al. 1997) and nicotine replacement therapy (Balfour & Fagerström 1996; Benowitz 1996; Hanson et al. 2003; O’Brien 2005; Rose et al. 1985). However, replacement therapy is susceptible to relapse (Balfour & Fagerström 1996; Fiore 2000; Hanson et al. 2003), and current treatments are becoming less successful over time, possibly due to differences in the population still smoking (Irvin & Brandon 2000; Irvin et al. 2003).
Nicotine is, however, dysphoric on initial use (Heishman & Henningfield 2000; Perkins 2001; Perkins et al. 1996). Thus, it is unlikely to access Vulnerability 1 or Vulnerability 3. However, attitudes towards nicotine products can drive positive views of use, which may lead to social pressures that can support initial usage (Cummings 2002).
Nicotine also leads to very large changes in allostatic levels of acetylcholine, dopamine, and other neuromodulators (Flores et al. 1997; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Marks et al. 1992), which would access Vulnerability 2. These allostatic effects may be due to changes in levels of cholinergic receptors in the brain (Flores et al. 1997; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Marks et al. 1992). Deviations from allostatic levels lead to very powerful withdrawal effects (Schmitz et al. 1997), which presumably reflect changes in the perceived needs of an agent, which would lead to strong cravings aimed at restoring those deviations. These deviations can be seen in a daily cycle in the reaction to the initial cigarette of the day (Perkins et al. 1996). It is likely that nicotine replacement therapy can affect these allostatic levels, which may suggest that replacement therapy is treating Vulnerability 2.
However, nicotine increases the activity of dopamine neurons through the activity of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors on dopamine neurons (Mansvelder & McGehee 2002; Pidoplichko et al. 1997). In addition, nicotine increases the effectiveness of associated dopaminergic signals (Rice & Cragg 2004). These effects could lead to non-compensable value-prediction-error signals (Redish 2004), which would suggest that nicotine use is likely to access Vulnerability 7. This vulnerability would lead to excess cue-related triggers. Nicotine shows a particularly high cue-related susceptibility to relapse (Chiamulera 2005; Kenny & Markou 2005; LeSage et al. 2004). Extinction and behavioral treatments potentially aimed at Vulnerability 7 have had limited success so far (Monti & MacKillop 2007; Schmitz et al. 1997). Providing valuable alternatives has had some success (Higgins et al. 2002).
D. Alcohol
Alcohol has long been identified as a drug of abuse, and it may be one of the first drugs to have been regularly abused by humans (Goodwin & Gabrielli 1997). The neurobiological effects of alcohol are well reviewed elsewhere (Hunt 1998; Koob & Le Moal 2006; Valenzuela & Harris 1997) and hence not reviewed here. Alcohol has extensive neurobiological effects, both in terms of acute effects on membrane lipids and on ion channels as well as long-term changes in expression of GABAA and NMDA receptors (Hunt 1998; Littleton 1998; Valenzuela & Harris 1997). This may be indicative of allostatic changes (Vulnerability 2). Supporting these hypotheses, alcohol intake leads to very strong withdrawal symptoms (Goodwin & Gabrielli 1997; Hunt 1998), both in terms of acute intake (e.g., a hangover, Swift & Davidson 1998, suggesting an involvement of Vulnerability 1) and after chronic, long-term intake (Saitz 1998, suggesting involvement of Vulnerability 2).
Much of the theoretical drive behind an understanding of alcohol addiction has arisen from the relationship between cognitive expectancies and alcohol consumption (“alcohol expectancy theory”; Goldman et al. 1987; 1999; Jones et al. 2001). These expectancies can be related to the “if-then” cognitive component of the planning system. Thus, early consumption can be due to positive expectations in the planning system (Vulnerability 3). There is a strong interaction between alcohol and the endogenous opioid systems (Herz 1997). Some success has been found from pharmacological treatment with opioid antagonists such as naltrexone in alcoholic subjects, particularly in reducing craving (O’Brien et al. 1996; Sinha & O’Malley 1999). Alcohol addiction shows a strong cue-driven craving and desire (Childress et al. 1993; Hunt 1998; MacKillop & Monti 2007; Sinha & O’Malley 1999), suggesting involvement of the planning system. Alcohol users show fewer coordination deficits under the influence of alcohol in alcohol-associated environments (such as bars) than in non-alcohol-associated environments (such as offices), suggesting a cue-driven preparation due to expectation of alcohol intake (Hunt 1998). However, Dickinson et al. (2002) found that alcohol intake did not show devaluation even when a comparably trained food-reward did, suggesting a developing involvement of the habit system. Some theories of alcohol consumption have been explicitly tied to the transition from cognitive to automatic learning (Oei & Baldwin 2002). Neurobiologically, the effects of heavy drinking are concentrated on hippocampal and prefrontal cortical function (Devenport et al. 1981a; Hunt 1998; Oscar-Berman & Marinkovic 2003; White 2003), which may lead to an imbalance between the planning and habit systems (Vulnerability 8).
Genetic effects on alcoholism have been well studied (Dick et al. 2006; Herz 1997; Stewart & Li 1997), in particular, in the relationship between genes involved in negative consequences of drinking alcohol (Nurnberger & Bierut 2007). Not surprisingly, people who experience more negative consequences during early drinking experiences are less likely to become addicted to alcohol (Goldman et al. 1999).
Alcohol addiction is clearly a spectrum disorder, with a wide variety of paths to dependence (Nurnberger & Bierut 2007). Alcohol intake stimulates dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra; however, these effects are dependent on the intermediate release of opioid peptides (Di Chiara 1997). It is an interesting (and open) question whether the dopamine release due to alcohol intake is more akin to the non-compensable effect of cocaine (Stuber et al. 2005, suggesting influence of Vulnerability 7; Redish 2004) or to the compensable effect of food (Schultz 1998, suggesting influence of Vulnerability 3; Redish & Johnson 2007).
E. Caffeine
Although caffeine is often not treated as a typical drug of abuse (Koob & Le Moal 2006), and is not regulated legally at this time, it does have strong psychopharmacological effects and has been identified as leading to a measurable drug-dependence (Daly & Fredholm 1998; Evans 1998; Greden & Walters 1997). The most noticeable affect of caffeine related to abuse is the well-identified caffeine withdrawal syndrome (Evans 1998; Nehlig 1999), which can last for several days once caffeine intake has been stopped. However, after that, there is a very low level of subsequent relapse, and neither craving for caffeine nor robotic automatic caffeine-ingestion behaviors appear. Subjects showing strong withdrawal symptoms are significantly more likely to self-administer caffeine than subjects not showing strong withdrawal symptoms, suggesting that the re-establishment of homeostasis underlies much of the caffeine addiction. This suggests that the primary effect of caffeine is due to easily reversible homeostatic (Vulnerability 1) or allostatic (Vulnerability 2) effects. Evidence suggests that large doses of caffeine can lead to dopamine release in both accumbens and caudate nucleus, but only in doses much higher than typically seen in human consumption (Nehlig 1999; Nehlig & Boyet 2000). This suggests that caffeine is unlikely to access the other vulnerabilities, which may explain the ease with which caffeine intake can often be stopped.
F. Gambling
Although gambling does not entail direct pharmacological manipulation of the decision-making system, it has been suggested to share many properties with the pharmacological addictions (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Potenza et al. 2001), in large part because it entails obvious (and often explicitly acknowledged) problematic decision-making (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Potenza 2006; Raylu & Oei 2002; Toneatto et al. 1997; Walker 1992a). Because the primary argument of our unified framework is that addiction entails vulnerabilities in decision-making, we argue that pathological gambling can also be explained within this framework.
The key to pathological gambling has been suggested to lie in distortions in estimates of the value of certain decisions. Agents in general show deficits and distortions in probability estimates, particularly in the difference between probabilities of wins and losses in the face of noisy variables (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Griffiths 1994). These deficits may lead to the process known as the “illusion of control” in which agents believe they can control probabilistic situations due to a miscalculation of predictability relationships between cues and outcomes (Langer & Roth 1975; Sylvain et al. 1997). This can lead to “hindsight bias,” in which gamblers explain away losses through the back identification of differential cues (Custer 1984; Wagenaar 1988). A number of researchers have argued that these may be the key to the process of “chasing” in which gamblers try to recapture losses by risking larger and larger gambles (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Lesieur 1977; Wagenaar 1988). These descriptions suggest that a large part of the gambling addictive process is due to Vulnerability 6 (Redish et al. 2007).
As noted by Parke and Griffiths (2004), an effective way to create a near miss in a gambling context is to manipulate the “trail” by which the gambler completes the process (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006). This can provide the user with additional cues to identify the situations categorized, providing the user with (incorrect) support for the hypothesis that there is a controllable sequence of situations, which, if the gambler could only control correctly, would lead to the win. Over the last several decades, manufacturers have changed lottery cards, video poker, and slot machines to add additional complexity, providing additional variables and additional cues (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006; Parke & Griffiths 2004), which would increase the likelihood of misclassification of situations (Redish et al. 2007).
This classification component plays a role in both the planning and the habit components, and therefore, we can expect gamblers to potentially show key aspects of the planning or the habit systems or both. Gamblers with problems associated with inputs to the planning system may show the signs of planning system deficits, including explicit expectations, craving, and complex, planned behaviors. Gamblers with problems associated with inputs to the habit system may show a more robotic, less self-recognized gambling behavior. Whether these differences translate to differences in preferred games is unknown but may be a testable avenue for future research.
The opiate antagonists naltrexone (Potenza et al. 2001) and nalmefene (Grant et al. 2006) are effective in the short-term treatment of gambling addiction, but only in subjects with strong gambling urges (i.e., craving, thus suggesting an involvement of the planning system over the habit system). No effective treatment has yet been found for pathological gamblers who do not show strong urges (i.e., suggesting a primary involvement of the habit system over the planning system).
Whether other vulnerabilities (such as an over-release of dopamine with monetary wins) can also lead to pathological gambling is still unknown, but a number of researchers have suggested that there are multiple pathways to pathological gambling (Dickerson & O’Connor 2006).
Footnotes
Some literatures have suggested that the value used is subjective value or subjective expected utility, in which the expected value is modified by (usually concave) functions (Glimcher & Rustichini 2004; Kahneman & Tversky 2000; Kahneman et al. 1982). Although this can explain changes in risk-seeking and risk-aversion, it does not have a major effect on the failure-points proposed in this article. Other literatures have suggested an importance of additional parameters such as risk and uncertainty (Hastie 2001; Preuschoff et al. 2006; Rapoport & Wallsten 1972).
The farther an event is in the future, the more likely it is that unexpected events can disrupt the predicted event (Sozou 1998; Stephens & Krebs 1987). Thus, the farther an event is in the future, the more potential there is for error and the less value one should assign to the event. The reward value of future events should therefore be discounted as a function of the time before reward is expected to be received. Additionally, the more quickly one receives a reward, the more one can invest it, presumably providing a positive return (whether in terms of money, energy, or offspring) – again, providing for the necessity of a discounting function (Frederick et al. 2002; Stephens & Krebs 1987). See Madden et al. (in press) for review.
The route system has also been termed the taxon system (O’Keefe & Nadel 1978; Schöne 1984) or the response system (Packard & McGaugh 1992; Poldrack & Packard 2003).
The S-A system has been termed the S-R (stimulus-response) system (Dickinson 1985; Domjan 1998; Hull 1943; 1952), but we prefer to use the term , which prevents confusion with R as indicating reward. In addition, much of the psychology literature is phrased in terms of “stimulus” rather than “situation,” but we prefer the term situation because that indicates the recognition of context, cue, and interactions between cues, all of which are critical for appropriate behavior. In the machine learning literature, “situation” is referred to as “state” (Daw et al. 2006; Sutton & Barto 1998), but we prefer the term situation because in other literatures, “state” refers to internal parameters of the agent (e.g., “motivation states”; Domjan 1998). The categorization of situation includes both internal and external parameters.
Because actions selected via associations only occur within a context, they too contain situation S components and should probably also be identified as . Contexts can be differentiated from cueing stimuli in that contextual information changes slowly relative to the time-course of action-selection, whereas conditioning stimuli change quickly. Thus, contextual stimuli cannot be seen as driving actions, but actions are still only taken from within identified situations. We do not explore this issue further here, but note that our concept of situations includes categorizations derived from both contextual and driving stimuli.
The role of δ-opiate receptors is more controversial (Broom et al. 2002; Herz 1997; Matthes et al. 1996).
German and Fields (2007a; 2007b) have shown that conditioned place preference (Tzschentke 1998) is in fact due to repeated transitions into the drug-associated location, implying that conditioned place preference is evidence of drug-seeking.
This inability to recategorize situations’ vulnerability will also relate to the interaction-between-systems vulnerability (Vulnerability 8), below.
It is possible for internal states (e.g., hunger) to be incorporated into the situation S term, but this would require separate learning under the different internal-state conditions (e.g., under hungry and thirsty conditions) without generalization.
This inability is seen in rats with fimbria-fornix lesions (Hirsh 1974).
This version in which value is a function of both situation and the subsequent action is called Q-learning (Sutton & Barto 1998). Other instantiations have been proposed as well. However, the differences are subtle and not critical to our needs in this paper. We therefore only describe the very basics of Q-learning.
Although the habit system does not directly take the immediate needs of the agent into account, it is possible that continued positive evaluation of drug-taking (or negative evaluation of abstinence) due to the changed-needs vulnerability could slowly train up the habit system, leading to a shift in drug use from the compulsive, needs-based vulnerability to a more robotic, habit-based vulnerability, independent of changes in homeostatic or allostatic set-points.
Other neuromodulators may be involved as well.
There are certainly going to be other problems that have not yet been identified, but these 10 can provide a starting point for this discussion.
Note that cocaine similarly blocks reuptake of norepinephrine and serotonin through blockage of their respective transporters (Ritz et al. 1987); however, the behavioral/addictive consequences of these effects are not known.
Contributor Information
A. David Redish, Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, redish@umn.edu, http://umn.edu/~redish/.
Steve Jensen, Graduate Program in Computer Science, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, jens0491@umn.edu.
Adam Johnson, Graduate Program in Neuroscience and Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, john5726@umn.edu.
References
- Adams CD, Dickinson A. Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1981;33(B):109–122. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Adams S, Kesner RP, Ragozzino ME. Role of the medial and lateral caudate-putamen in mediating an auditory conditional response association. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2001;76:106–116. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2000.3989. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addolorato G, Leggio L, Abenavoli L, Gasbarrini G the AlcoholismTreatment Study Group. Neurobiochemical and clinical aspects of craving in alcohol addiction: A review. Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30(6):1209–1224. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.12.011. [rADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP. The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trends in Neurosciences. 1993;16(8):328–333. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(93)90110-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agster KL, Fortin NJ, Eichenbaum H. The hippocampus and disambiguation of overlapping sequences. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(13):5760–5768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05760.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Cador M. Dissociation of psychomotor sensitization from compulsive cocaine consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:563–571. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300834. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Neurobiological evidence for hedonic allostasis associated with escalating cocaine use. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5:625–666. doi: 10.1038/nn872. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Cocaine- but not food-seeking behavior is reinstated by stress after extinction. Psychopharmacology. 1997;132(3):289–295. doi: 10.1007/s002130050347. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: Change in hedonic set point. Science. 1998;282:298–300. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5387.298. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Long-lasting increase in the set point for cocaine self-administration after escalation in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146(3):303–312. doi: 10.1007/s002130051121. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Vertical shifts in dose-injection curves reflect reward allostasis not sensitization. Psychopharmacology. 2004;171:354–355. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Transition to drug addiction: A negative reinforcement model based on an allostatic decrease in reward function. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180:473–490. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2180-z. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Lin D, Koob GF, Parsons LH. Escalation of cocaine self-administration does not depend on altered cocaine-induced nucleus accumbens dopamine levels. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2003;86:102–113. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01833.x. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Lutjens R, van der Stap LD, Lekic D, Romano-Spica V, Morales M, Koob GF, Repunte-Canonigo V, Sanna PP. Gene expression evidence for remodeling of lateral hypothalamic circuitry in cocaine addiction. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2005;102:11533–11538. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504438102. [SHA] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainslie G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin. 1975;82:463–496. doi: 10.1037/h0076860. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainslie G. Picoeconomics: The strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the person. Cambridge University Press; 1992. [GA, arADR, GA] [Google Scholar]
- Ainslie G. Breakdown of will. Cambridge University Press; 2001. [CA, GA, arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Ainslie G. Précis of Breakdown of will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28(5):635–673. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000117. [GA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainslie G. Recursive self-prediction as a proximate cause of impulsivity: The value of a bottom-up model. In: Madden G, Bickel W, Critchfield T, editors. Theory, science and neuroscience of discounting. APA Books; (in press) [GA] [Google Scholar]
- Ainslie G, Monterosso J. Behavior: A marketplace in the brain? Science. 2004;306(5695):421–423. doi: 10.1126/science.1104884. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M. Stress and addiction: Biological and psychological mechanisms. Academic; 2007. [MLM] [Google Scholar]
- Alessi SM, Petry NM. Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behavioural Processes. 2003;64(3):345–354. doi: 10.1016/s0376-6357(03)00150-5. [CWL, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander BK. The disease and adaptive models of addiction: A framework evaluation. Journal of Drug Issues. 1987;17(1):47–66. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander BK, Beyerstein BL, Hadaway PF, Coambs RB. Effects of early and later colony housing on oral ingestion of morphine in rats. Psychopharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1981;15(4):571–576. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(81)90211-2. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allegre B, Souville M, Therme P, Griffiths MD. Definitions and measures of exercise dependence. Addiction Research and Theory. 2006;14:631–646. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Abramson LY. Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1979;108:441–485. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.108.4.441. [GA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altman J, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, Glautier S, Markou A, Nutt D, Oretti R, Phillips GD. The biological, social and clinical bases of drug addiction: Commentary and debate. Psychopharmacology. 1996;125(4):285–345. doi: 10.1007/BF02246016. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR™) 4th edition. American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [CWL, arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Anagnostaras SG, Schallert T, Robinson TE. Memory processes governing amphetamine-induced psychomotor sensitization. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26(6):703–715. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00402-X. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreou C. Going from bad (or not so bad) to worse: On harmful addictions and habits. American Philosophical Quarterly. 2005;42(4):323–331. [CA, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Andreou C. Understanding procrastination. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour. 2007;37(2):183–193. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- Anthony JC, Warner LA, Kessler RC. Comparative epidemiology of dependence on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances, and inhalants: Basic findings from the National Comorbidity Survey. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1994;2:244–268. [MLM] [Google Scholar]
- Arbib M, editor. The handbook of brain theory and neural networks. MIT Press; 1995. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Arbisi PA, Billington CJ, Levine AS. The effect of naltrexone on taste detection and recognition threshold. Appetite. 1999;32(2):241–249. doi: 10.1006/appe.1998.0217. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arbuthnott GW, Wickens J. Space, time and dopamine. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30(2):62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.12.003. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ariely D. Predictably irrational: The hidden forces that shape our decisions. HarperCollins; 2008. [VGH, ELK] [Google Scholar]
- Arndt J, Greenberg J, Cook A. Mortality salience and the spreading activation of worldview-relevant constructs: Exploring the cognitive architecture of terror. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2002;131:307–324. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.131.3.307. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AFT, Cai JX, Murphy BL, Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 1994;116:143–151. doi: 10.1007/BF02245056. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auld MC, Grootendorst P. An empirical analysis of milk addiction. Journal of Health Economics. 2004;23:1117–1133. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2004.02.003. [RJM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck BB, Lee D. Prefrontal neural correlates of memory for sequences. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(9):2204–2211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4483-06.2007. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azolosa JL, Stitzer ML, Greenwald MK. Opioid physical dependence development: Effects of single versus repeated morphine pretreatments and of subjects’ opioid exposure history. Psychopharmacology. 1994;114(1):71–80. doi: 10.1007/BF02245446. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD. Working memory. Oxford University Press; 1986. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin AS, Rothman AJ, Hertel AW, Linde JA, Jeffery RW, Finch EA, Lando HA. Specifying the determinants of the initiation and maintenance of behavior change: An examination of self-efficacy, satisfaction, and smoking cessation. Health Psychology. 2006;25:626–634. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.626. [DTN] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balfour DJK, Fagerström KO. Pharmacology of nicotine and its therapeutic use in smoking cessation and neurodegenerative disorders. Pharmacology and Therapeutics. 1996;72(1):51–81. doi: 10.1016/s0163-7258(96)00099-x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balfour DJK, Wright AE, Benwell MEM, Birrell CE. The putative role of extra-synaptic mesolimbic dopamine in the neurobiology of nicotine dependence. Behavioural Brain Research. 2000;113(1–2):73–83. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00202-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW. Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends on incentive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1992;18:236–250. [SBO] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mowrer RR, Klein SB, editors. Handbook of contemporary learning theories. Erlbaum; 2001. Incentive processes in instrumental conditioning; pp. 307–366. [SBO, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Whishaw IQ, Kolb B, editors. The behavior of the laboratory rat: A handbook with tests. Oxford University Press; 2004. Incentive behavior; pp. 436–446. [SBO, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Ball J, Dickinson A. Benzodiazepine-induced outcome revaluation and the motivational control of instrumental action in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1994;108:573–589. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.3.573. [SBO] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Dickinson A. Instrumental performance following reinforcer devaluation depends upon incentive learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1991;43B:279–296. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Dickinson A. Goal-directed instrumental action: Contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37(4–5):407–419. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Ostlund SB. Still at the choice-point: Action selection and initiation in instrumental conditioning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007;1104:147–171. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.006. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bals-Kubik R, Herz A, Shippenberg T. Evidence that the aversive effects of opioid antagonists and κ-agonists are centrally mediated. Psychopharmacology. 1989;98:203–206. doi: 10.1007/BF00444692. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balster RL. Fixed-interval schedule of cocaine reinforcement: Effect of dose and infusion duration. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1973;20(1):119–129. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1973.20-119. [arADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clouet D, Asghar K, Brown R, editors. Mechanisms of cocaine abuse and toxicity. National Institute on Drug Abuse; 1988. Pharmacological effects of cocaine relevant to its abuse; pp. 1–13. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Bardo MT, Donohew RL, Harrington NG. Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behavioral Brain Research. 1996;77(1–2):23–43. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00203-0. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bargh JA, Morsella E. The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2008;3:73–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00064.x. [RWW] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkley RA. The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review. 2001;11(1):1–29. doi: 10.1023/a:1009085417776. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkley RA, Edwards G, Laneri M, Fletcher K, Metevia L. Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29(6):541–556. doi: 10.1023/a:1012233310098. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes CA. Memory deficits associated with senescence: A neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1979;93:74–104. doi: 10.1037/h0077579. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes CA, Nadel L, Honig WK. Spatial memory deficit in senescent rats. Canadian Journal of Psychology. 1980;34(1):29–39. doi: 10.1037/h0081022. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes TD, Kubota Y, Hu D, Jin DZ, Graybiel AM. Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature. 2005;437:1158–1161. doi: 10.1038/nature04053. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron J. Thinking and deciding. 4th ed. Cambridge University Press; 2008. [ELK] [Google Scholar]
- Barto AG. Adaptive critics and the basal ganglia. In: Houk JC, Davis JL, Beiser DG, editors. Models of information processing in the basal ganglia. MIT Press; 1995. pp. 215–232. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF. Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychological Inquiry. 1996;7:1–15. [SHA] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF, Tice DM. Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. Academic Press; 1994. [MLM] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. Guilford Press; 2004. [MLM] [Google Scholar]
- Bayer HM, Glimcher P. Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron. 2005;47:129–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.020. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(11):1458–1463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584. [WKB, MLM, aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision-making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW. Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:428–437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00428.1998. [MLM] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275:1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [KRC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Andersen SW, Nathan PE. Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:376–389. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00136-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS. The economic approach to human behavior. University of Chicago Press; 1976. [ELK, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS, Grossman M, Murphy KM. Rational addiction and the effect of price on consumption. In: Loewenstein G, Elster J, editors. Choice over time. Sage; 1992. pp. 361–370. [GA] [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS, Grossman M, Murphy KM. An empirical analysis of cigarette addiction. The American Economic Review. 1994;84(3):396–418. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS, Murphy KM. A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy. 1988;96(4):675–700. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS, Murphy KM, Grossman M. The economic theory of illegal goods: The case of drugs. 2004 NBER Working Paper No. W10976. [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- Beiser DG, Hua SE, Houk JC. Network models of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1997;7(2):185–190. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80006-2. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belin D, Everitt BJ. Cocaine-seeking habits depend upon dopamine-dependent serial connectivity linking the ventral and dorsal striatum. Neuron. 2008;57:432–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.019. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell DE. Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research. 1982;30(5):961–982. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Bem DJ. Self-perception theory. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. vol. 6. Academic; 1972. pp. 1–62. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Ari M. Just a theory: Exploring the nature of science. Prometheus; 2005. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Benowitz NL. Pharmacology of nicotine: Addiction and therapeutics. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology. 1996;36:597–613. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.36.040196.003121. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berke JD. Learning and memory mechanisms involved in compulsive drug use and relapse. In: Wang J, editor. Drugs of abuse: Analysis of neurological effects. Humana Press; 2003. pp. 75–102. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin SI. The hedgehog and the fox. Simon & Schuster; 1953. [WKB] [Google Scholar]
- Bernheim BD, Rangel A. Addiction and cue-triggered decision processes. The American Economic Review. 2004;94(5):1558–1590. doi: 10.1257/0002828043052222. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC. Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiology and Behavior. 2004;81(2):179–209. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.02.004. [KCB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191(3):391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [GA, arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews. 1998;28:309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences. 2003;26(9):507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9. [arADR, JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC, Schulkin J. Palatability shift of a salt-associated incentive during sodium depletion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B. 1989;41(2):121–138. [KCB] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevins RA. Altering the motivational function of nicotine through conditioning processes. In: Bevins RA, Caggiula AR, editors. The motivational impact of nicotine and its role in tobacco use: The 55th Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Springer; (in press) [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Bevins RA, Palmatier MI. Extending the role of associative learning processes in nicotine addiction. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews. 2004;3:143–158. doi: 10.1177/1534582304272005. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, DeGrandpre RJ, Higgins ST. The behavioral economics of concurrent drug reinforcers: A review and reanalysis of drug self-administration research. Psychopharmacology. 1995;118:250–259. doi: 10.1007/BF02245952. [RJM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Marsch LA. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Addiction. 2001;96:73–86. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x. [WKB, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Miller ML, Yi R, Kowal BP, Lindquist DM, Pitcock JA. Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: Competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;90(S1):S85–S91. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.016. [aADR, WKB] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloor R. The influence of age and gender on drug use in the United Kingdom – a review. American Journal of Addiction. 2006;15(3):201–207. doi: 10.1080/10550490600625269. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bobo JK, Husten C. Sociocultural influences on smoking and drinking. Alcohol Research and Health. 2001;24(4):225–232. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolles RC. Theory of motivation. Harper & Row; 1967. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Bolles RC. Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review. 1972;79(5):394–409. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova MA, Lejuez CW, Daughters SB, Rosenthal MZ, Lynch TR. Impulsivity as a common process across borderline personality and substance use disorders. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:790–812. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.05.005. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bossert JM, Ghitza UE, Lu L, Epstein DH, Shaham Y. Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: An update and clinical implications. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2005;526(1–3):36–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.030. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourgois P. In search of respect: Selling crack in El Barrio. 2nd edition. Cambridge University Press; 2002. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:976–986. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01546-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning and Memory. 2004;11(5):485–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breland K, Breland M. The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist. 1961;16(11):682–684. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Broom DC, Jutkiewicz EM, Folk JE, Traynor JR, Rice KC, Woods JH. Nonpeptidic δ-opioid receptor agonists reduce immobility in the forced swim assay in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:744–755. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00413-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown MF. Does a cognitive map guide choices in the radial-arm maze? Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1992;18(1):56–66. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.18.1.56. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brownell KD, Marlatt GA, Lichtenstein E, Wilson GT. Understanding and preventing relapse. American Psychologist. 1986;41(7):765–782. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.41.7.765. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruehl AM, Lende DH, Schwartz M, Sterk CE, Elifson K. Craving and control: Methamphetamine users’ narratives. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 2006;38(4) SARC Suppl. 3:385–392. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2006.10400602. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Carroll DC. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11(2):49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G. The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6(2):81–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.81. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cagniard B, Beeler JA, Britt JP, McGehee DS, Marinelli M, Zhuang X. Dopamine scales performance in the absence of new learning. Neuron. 2006;51(5):541–547. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.026. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caillé S, Parsons LH. SR141716A reduces the reinforcing properties of heroin but not heroin-induced increases in nucleus accumbens dopamine in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;18(11):3145–3149. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.02961.x. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldu X, Dreher JC. Hormonal and genetic influences on processing reward and social information. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007;1118:43–73. doi: 10.1196/annals.1412.007. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi EJ. The effect of different amounts of alternating partial reinforcement on resistance to extinction. American Journal of Psychology. 1957;70(3):451–452. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cappendijk SL, Hurd YL, Nylander I, van Ree JM, Terenius L. A heroin-, but not a cocaine-expecting, self-administration state preferentially alters endogenous brain peptides. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1999;365(2–3):175–182. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(98)00874-7. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli RM. Nucleus accumbens cell firing during goal-directed behaviors for cocaine vs--“natural” reinforcement. Physiology and Behavior. 2002;76(3):379–387. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00760-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli RM, Ijames SG, Crumling AJ. Evidence that separate neural circuits in the nucleus accumbens encode cocaine versus“natural” (water and food) reward. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(11):4255–4266. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04255.2000. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli RM, West MO. Representation of the body by single neurons in the dosolateral striatum of the awake, unrestrained rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1991;309:231–249. doi: 10.1002/cne.903090205. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli RM, Wondolowski J. Selective encoding of cocaine versus natural rewards by nucleus accumbens neurons is not related to chronic drug exposure. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23(35):11214–11223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11214.2003. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr H, Watson JB. Orientation in the white rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology. 1908;18:27–44. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll KM, Kosten TR, Rounsaville BJ. Choosing a behavioral therapy platform for pharmacotherapy of substance users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2004;75(2):123–134. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.02.007. [rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll ME. The economic context of drug and non-drug reinforcers affects acquisition and maintenance of drug-reinforced behavior and withdrawal effects. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1993;33(2):201–210. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(93)90061-t. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll ME, Lac ST, Nygaard SL. A concurrently available non-drug reinforcer prevents the acquisition or decreases the maintenance of cocaine-reinforced behavior. Psychopharmacology. 1989;97(1):23–29. doi: 10.1007/BF00443407. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter BL, Tiffany ST. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999;94(3):327–340. [JS] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:111–126. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.010. [rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centonze D, Gubellini P, Picconi B, Calabresi P, Giacomini P, Bernardi G. Unilateral dopamine denervation blocks corticostriatal LTP. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;82(6):3575–3579. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.6.3575. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Neuropharmacological modulation of cognition. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2006;19(6):607–612. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000247613.28859.77. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA. Animal modeling and neurocircuitry of dual diagnosis. Journal of Dual Diagnosis. 2007;3(2):19–29. doi: 10.1300/J374v03n02_04. [RAC] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA, Bickel WK, Potenza MN. A scale-free systems theory of motivation and addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2007;31(7):1017–1045. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.005. [WKB, RAC, rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA, Jones RM, Brown S, Taylor JR. Natural reward related learning in rats with neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions and prior cocaine exposure. Psychopharmacology. 2005;179(2):470–478. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2042-0. [RAC] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA, Potenza MN. Neurodevelopment, impulsivity, and adolescent gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies. 2003;19(1):53–84. doi: 10.1023/a:1021275130071. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA, Self DW. Motivational responses to natural and drug rewards in rats with neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions: An animal model of dual diagnosis schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27(6):889–905. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00365-2. [RAC] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160(6):1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. [RAC, rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang Q, Gold PE. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum impairs acquisition of response learning in cue-deficient, but not cue-available, conditions. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118(2):383–388. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.383. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charney DS, Nestler EJ, Bunney BS, editors. Neurobiology of mental illness. Oxford University Press; 1999. [RAC] [Google Scholar]
- Chastain G. Alcohol, neurotransmitter systems, and behavior. Journal of General Psychology. 2006;133(4):329–335. doi: 10.3200/GENP.133.4.329-335. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chavkin C, James IF, Goldstein A. Dynorphin is a specific endogenous ligand of the kappa opioid receptor. Science. 1982;215(4531):413–415. doi: 10.1126/science.6120570. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen R, Tilley MR, Wei H, Zhou F, Zhou F-M, Ching S, Quan N, Stephens RL, Hill ER, Nottoli T, Han DD, Gu HH. Abolished cocaine reward in mice with a cocaine-insensitive dopamine transporter. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2006;103(24):9333–9338. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600905103. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiamulera C. Cue reactivity in nicotine and tobacco dependence: A“multiple-action” model of nicotine as a primary reinforcement and as an enhancer of the effects of smoking-associated stimuli. Brain Research Reviews. 2005;48(1):74–97. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.08.005. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiel HJ, Beer RD. The brain has a body: Adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment. Trends in Neurosciences. 1997;20(12):553–537. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01149-1. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, Ehrman R, Rohsenow DJ, Robbins SJ, O'Brien CP. Classically conditioned factors in drug dependence. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1992. pp. 56–69. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, Hole AV, Ehrman RN, Robbins SJ, McLellan AT, O’Brien CP. Cue reactivity and cue reactivity interventions in drug dependence. NIDA Research Monographs. 1993;137:73–94. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R, O’Brien CP. Classically conditioned responses in opioid and cocaine dependence: A role in relapse? NIDA Research Monographs. 1988;84:25–43. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O’Brien CP. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:11–18. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.1.11. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiu PH, Lohrenz TM, Montague PR. Smokers’ brains compute, but ignore, a fictive error signal in a sequential investment task. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:514–520. doi: 10.1038/nn2067. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciraulo DA, Piechniczek-Buczek J, Iscan EN. Outcome predictors in substance use disorders. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2003;26:381–409. doi: 10.1016/s0193-953x(02)00106-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark A. Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press; 1997. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Clark DB, Parker AM, Lynch KG. Psychopathology and substance-related problems during early adolescence: A survival analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1999;28(3):333–341. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp280305. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark L, Robbins TW. Decision-making deficits in drug addiction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2002;6(9):361–363. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01960-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark NK, Stephenson GM. Social remembering: Individual and collaborative memory for social information. European Review of Social Psychology. 1995;6:127–160. [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- Clark RE, Squire LR. Classical conditioning and brain systems: The role of awareness. Science. 1998;280:77–81. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5360.77. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen NJ, Eichenbaum H. Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system. MIT Press; 1993. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen NJ, Squire LR. Preserved learning and retention of patternanalyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science. 1980;210:207–210. doi: 10.1126/science.7414331. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colwill RM, Rescorla RA. Post-conditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1985;11:120–132. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Colwill RM, Rescorla RA. Effect of reinforcer devaluation on discriminative control of instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1990;16(1):40–47. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comer SD, Collins ED, Fischman MW. Choice between money and intranasal heroin in morphine-maintained humans. Behavioural Pharmacology. 1997;8:677–690. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199712000-00002. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comer SD, Collins ED, Wilson ST, Donovan MR, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effects of an alternative reinforcer on intravenous heroin self-administration by humans. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1998;345:13–26. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(97)01572-0. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper A. Delay discounting and problem gambling. Analysis of Gambling Behavior. 2007;1:21–22. [CWL] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Balleine BW. The role of the hippocampus in instrumental conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(11):4233–4239. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04233.2000. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Balleine BW. The role of prelimbic cortex in instrumental conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research. 2003;146:145–157. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.023. [SBO] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Muir JL, Balleine BW. The role of the nucleus accumbens in instrumental conditioning: Evidence of a functional dissociation between accumbens core and shell. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(9):3251–3260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-09-03251.2001. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Sensitivity to instrumental contingency degradation is mediated by the entorhinal cortex and its efferents via the dorsal hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(24):10976–10984. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10976.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coulombe A, Ladouceur R, Desharnais R, Jobin J. Erroneous perceptions and arousal among regular and occasional video poker players. Journal of Gambling Studies. 1992;8:235–244. doi: 10.1007/BF01014651. [KRC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coutureau E, Killcross S. Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2003;146:167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.025. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coventry KR. Rationality and decision making: The case of gambling and the development of gambling addiction. In: Marotta JJ, Cornelius JA, Eadington WR, editors. The downside. Problem and pathological gambling. University of Nevada Press; 2002. pp. 43–68. [KRC] [Google Scholar]
- Coventry KR, Norman AC. Arousal, erroneous verbalisations, and the illusion of control during a computer-generated gambling task. British Journal of Psychology. 1998;69:629–645. [KRC] [Google Scholar]
- Crabbe JC. Genetic contributions to addiction. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53(1):435–462. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135142. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings KM. Programs and policies to discourage the use of tobacco products. Oncogene. 2002;21(48):7349–7364. doi: 10.1038/sj.onc.1205810. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran T. On the neural mechanisms of sequence learning. Psyche. 1995;2(12) (Online publication). Available at: http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v2/psyche-2–12-curran.html [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Curran T. Implicit learning revealed by the method of opposition. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2001;5(12):503–504. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01791-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Custer RL. Profile of the pathological gambler. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1984;45(12) Suppl. 2:35–38. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: Neural and neurochemical substrates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28(7):771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daly JW, Fredholm BB. Caffeine – an atypical drug of dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1998;51:199–206. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00077-5. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR. Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. Putnam; 1994. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, Damasio H, Ponto LL, Parvizi J, Hichwa RD. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:1049–1056. doi: 10.1038/79871. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dani JA, Heinemann S. Molecular and cellular aspects of nicotine abuse. Neuron. 1996;16:905–908. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80112-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danner UN, Aarts H, de Vries NK. Habit and intention in the prediction of behaviors: The role of frequency, stability and accessibility of past behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology. doi: 10.1348/014466607X230876. (in press) [DTN] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darke PR, Chattopadhyay A, Ashworth L. The importance and functional significance of affective cues in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research. 2006;33:322–328. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Davies JB. The myth of addiction: An application of the psychological theory of attribution to illicit drug use. Harwood Academic; 1992. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Davis JB, Donahue RJ, Discenza CB, Waite AA, Ramus SJ. Hippocampal dependence of anticipatory neuronal firing in the orbitofrontal cortex of rats learning an odor-sequence memory task. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2006 Program No. 66.7 [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Davis JR, Tunks E. Environments and addiction: A proposed taxonomy. The International Journal of the Addictions. 1991;25(7A & 8A):805–826. doi: 10.3109/10826089109071025. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND. Reinforcement learning models of the dopamine system and their behavioral implications. Pittsburgh, PA: Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie Mellon University; 2003. [arADR, DR] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Courville AC, Touretzky DS. Representation and timing in theories of the dopamine system. Neural Computation. 2006;18:1637–1677. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1637. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Kakade S, Dayan P. Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Networks. 2002;15:603–616. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00052-7. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1704–1711. doi: 10.1038/nn1560. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day LB, Weisend M, Sutherland RJ, Schallert T. The hippocampus is not necessary for a place response but may be necessary for pliancy. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1999;113(5):914–924. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.113.5.914. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Balleine BW. Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron. 2002;36:285–298. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00963-7. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Kakade S, Montague PR. Learning and selective attention. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:1218–1223. doi: 10.1038/81504. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Niv Y, Seymour B, Daw ND. The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will. Neural Networks. 2006;19:1153–1160. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.002. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFeudis FV. Environmental theory of drug addiction. General Pharmacology. 1978;9(5):303–306. doi: 10.1016/0306-3623(78)90065-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Phillips AG, Zamburlini M, Sossi V, Calne DB, Ruth TJ, Stoessl AJ. Dopamine release in human ventral striatum and expectation of reward. Behavioural Brain Research. 2002;136:359–363. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00130-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delamater AR. Outcome-selective effects of intertrial reinforcement in Pavlovian appetitive conditioning with rats. Animal Learning and Behavior. 1995;23:31–39. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis W. Multiple visual discrimination in the block elevated maze. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1932;13:391–396. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Depue RA, Morrone-Strupinsky JV. A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28(3):313–350. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000063. discussion 350–95 [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deroche-Gamonet V, Belin D, Piazza PV. Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science. 2004;305(5686):1014–1017. doi: 10.1126/science.1099020. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deutsch R, Gawronski B, Strack F. At the boundaries of automaticity: Negation as reflective operation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:385–405. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.385. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deutsch R, Strack F. Reflective and impulsive determinants of addictive behavior. In: Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction. Sage; 2006. pp. 45–57. [RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Devan BD, White NM. Parallel information processing in the dorsal striatum: Relation to hippocampal function. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19(7):2789–2798. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-07-02789.1999. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devenport LD. Superstitious bar pressing in hippocampal and septal rats. Science. 1979;205(4407):721–723. doi: 10.1126/science.462183. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devenport LD. Response-reinforcer relations and the hippocampus. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1980;29(1):105–110. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(80)92526-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devenport LD, Devenport JA, Holloway FA. Necessity of the hippocampus for alcohol’s indirect but not behavioral action. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1981a;33(4):476–487. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(81)91851-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devenport LD, Devenport JA, Holloway FA. Reward-induced stereotypy: Modulation by the hippocampus. Science. 1981b;212(4500):1288–1289. doi: 10.1126/science.7195073. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devenport LD, Holloway FA. The rat’s resistance to superstition: Role of the hippocampus. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology. 1980;94(4):691–705. doi: 10.1037/h0077703. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vries TJ, Shippenberg TS. Neural systems underlying opiate addiction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(9):3321–3325. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03321.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Stewart J. Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1981;75(2):134–143. doi: 10.1007/BF00432175. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Chiara G. Alcohol and dopamine. Alcohol Research and Health. 1997;21(2):108–113. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Chiara G. Drug addiction as dopamine-dependent associative learning disorder. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1999;375(1–3):13–30. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(99)00372-6. [aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Chiara G, Imperato A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 1988;85(14):5274–5278. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5274. [TS] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dick DM, Jones K, Saccone N, Hinrichs A, Wang JC, Goate A, Bierut L, Almasy L, Schuckit M, Hesselbrock V, Tischfield J, Foroud T, Edenberg H, Porjesz B, Begleiter H. Endophenotypes successfully lead to gene identification: Results from the collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism. Behavior Genetics. 2006;36(1):112–126. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-9001-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson M, O’Connor J. Gambling as an addictive behavior. Cambridge University Press; 2006. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A. Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge University Press; 1980. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A. Actions and habits: The development of behavioural autonomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London B. 1985;308:67–78. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A, Balleine B. Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning and Behavior. 1994;22:1–18. [SHA] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A, Balleine B, Watt A, Gonzalez F, Boakes RA. Motivational control after extended instrumental training. Animal Learning and Behavior. 1995;23:197–206. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A, Wood N, Smith JW. Alcohol seeking by rats: Action or habit? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B. 2002;55(4):331–348. doi: 10.1080/0272499024400016. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiMattia BVD, Kesner RP. Spatial cognitive maps: Differential role of parietal cortex and hippocampal formation. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1988;102(4):471–480. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.102.4.471. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diskin KM, Hodgins DC. Narrowing of attention and dissociation in pathological video lottery gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies. 1999;15(1):17–28. doi: 10.1023/a:1023062912062. [KRC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diskin KM, Hodgins DC. Narrowed focus and dissociative experiences in a community sample of experienced video lottery gamblers. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science. 2001;33(1):58–64. [KRC] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon L. Dual diagnosis of substance abuse in schizophrenia: Prevalence and impact on outcomes. Schizophrenia Research. 1999;35(Suppl.):S93–100. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(98)00161-3. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon MR, Jacobs EA, Sanders S. Contextual control of delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2006;39:413–422. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2006.173-05. [CWL] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon MR, Marley J, Jacobs EA. Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2003;36:449–458. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449. [CWL] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domjan M. The principles of learning and behavior. 4th edition. Brooks/Cole; 1998. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Doty P, de Wit H. Effect of setting on the reinforcing and subjective effects of ethanol in social drinkers. Psychopharmacology. 1995;118:19–27. doi: 10.1007/BF02245245. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dowling N, Smith D, Thomas T. Electronic gaming machines: Are they the “crack cocaine” of gambling? Addiction. 2005;100(1):33–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.00962.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doya K. Metalearning, neuromodulation, and emotion. In: Hatano G, Okada N, Tanabe H, editors. Affective minds. Elsevier; 2000a. pp. 101–104. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Doya K. Reinforcement learning in continuous time and space. Neural Computation. 2000b;12:219–245. doi: 10.1162/089976600300015961. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doya K. Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Networks. 2002;15(4–6):495–506. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00044-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyon J, Laforce R, Bouchard G, Gaudreau D, Roy J, Poirer M, Bedard PJ, Bedard F, Bouchard JP. Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36(7):625–641. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00168-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drummond DC. Theories of drug craving, ancient and modern. Addiction. 2001;96(1):33–46. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961333.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudish-Poulsen SA, Hatsukami DK. Dissociation between subjective and behavioral responses after cocaine stimuli presentations. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1997;47(1):1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(97)00054-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan SC, Duncan TE, Biglan A, Ary D. Contributions of the social context to the development of adolescent substance use: A multivariate latent growth modeling approach. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1998;50(1):57–71. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00006-4. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durstewitz D, Kelc M, Gunturkun O. A neurocomputational theory of the dopaminergic modulation of working memory functions. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19(7):2807–2822. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-07-02807.1999. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durstewitz D, Seamans JK, Sejnowski TJ. Dopamine-mediated stabilization of delay-period activity in a network model of prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83(3):1733–1750. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1733. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrman R, Ternes J, O’Brien CP, McLellan AT. Conditioned tolerance in human opiate addicts. Psychopharmacology. 1992;108(1–2):218–224. doi: 10.1007/BF02245311. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, Stewart C, Morris RGM. Hippocampal representation in place learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10(11):3531–3542. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-11-03531.1990. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eiser J. Smoking: The social learning of an addiction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1985;3(4):446–457. [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- Elsmore TF, Fletcher GV, Conrad DG, Sodetz FJ. Reduction of heroin intake in baboons by an economic constraint. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior. 1980;13(5):729–731. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90018-0. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein DH, Preston KL. The reinstatement model and relapse prevention: A clinical perspective. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168:31–41. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1470-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein JM. Generative social science: Studies in agent-based computational modeling. Princeton University Press; 2007. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:299–312. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005891. [SHA] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans JStBT. In two minds: Dual process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:454–459. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012. [KRC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans JStBT, Coventry K. A dual process approach to behavioral addiction: The case of gambling. In: Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction. Sage; 2006. pp. 29–43. [KRC, RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Evans SM. Behavioral pharmacology of caffeine. In: Tarter RE, Ammerman RT, PJOtt RT, Ott PJ, editors. Handbook of substance abuse: Neurobehavioral pharmacology. Plenum; 1998. pp. 69–96. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K, Baker NB, Hunter J, Carthy T, Booker E, London M, Deakin JF, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20:322–339. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00091-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Dickinson A, Robbins TW. The neuropsychological basis of addictive behavior. Brain Research Reviews. 2001;36:129–138. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00088-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(11):1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [RAC, arADR, RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Stacey P. Studies of instrumental behavior with sexual reinforcement in male rats (Rattus norvegicus): II. Effects of preoptic area lesions, castration and testosterone. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1987;101:407–419. [SBO] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Wolf ME. Psychomotor stimulant addiction: A neural systems perspective. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(9):3312–3320. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03312.2002. [MLM, aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faure A, Haberland U, Fran CC, Massioui NE. Lesion to the nigrostriatal dopamine system disrupts stimulus-response habit formation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:2771–2780. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3894-04.2005. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feierstein CE, Quirk MC, Uchida N, Sosulski DL, Mainen ZF. Representation of spatial goals in rat orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron. 2006;60(4):495–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.032. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferbinteanu J, Kennedy PJ, Shapiro ML. Episodic memory – from brain to mind. Hippocampus. 2006;16(9):704–715. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20204. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferbinteanu J, Shapiro ML. Prospective and retrospective memory coding in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2003;40(6):1227–1239. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00752-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson E, Bibby PA. Predicting future blood donor returns: Past behavior, intentions, and observer effects. Health Psychology. 2002;21:513–518. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.21.5.513. [DTN] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ. Cannabis use and other illicit drug use: Testing the cannabis gateway hypothesis. Addiction. 2006;101:556–569. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01322.x. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Early onset cannabis use and psychosocial adjustment in young adults. Addiction. 1997;92(3):279–296. [JMB] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferraro FR, Balota DA, Connor LT. Implicit memory and the formation of new associations in non-demented Parkinson’s disease individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: A serial reaction time (SRT) investigation. Brain and Cognition. 1993;21(2):163–180. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1993.1013. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferster CB, Skinner BF. Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1957. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Field M, Eastwood B. Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology. 2005;183:350–357. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0202-5. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field M, Franken I, Munafo M. Attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. doi: 10.1037/a0015843. (in preparation) [RWW] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field M, Mogg K, Bradley BP. Eye movements to smoking-related cues: Effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology. 2004;173:116–123. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1689-2. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field M, Mogg K, Bradley BP. Craving and cognitive biases for alcohol cues in heavy drinkers. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2005;40:504–510. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agh213. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field M, Santarcangelo M, Sumnall H, Goudie A, Cole J. Delay discounting and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: The effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology. 2006;186:255–263. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0385-4. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finch DM. Neurophysiology of converging synaptic inputs from rat prefrontal cortex, amygdala, midline thalamus, and hippocampal formation onto single neurons of the caudate/putamen and nucleus accumbens. Hippocampus. 1996;6:495–512. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:5<495::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-I. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finlay JM, Zigmond MJ. The effects of stress on central dopaminergic neurons: Possible clinical implications. Neurochemical Research. 1997;22(11):1387–1394. doi: 10.1023/a:1022075324164. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiore MC, editor. Treating tobacco use and dependence. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service; 2000. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer S, Smith GT. Binge eating, problem drinking, and pathological gambling: Linking behavior to shared traits and social learning. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44(4):789–800. [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- Flores CM, Dávila-García MI, Ulrich YM, Kellar KJ. Differential regulation of neuronal nicotinic receptor binding sites following chronic nicotine administration. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1997;69:2216–2219. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1997.69052216.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foltin RW, Fischman MW, Brady JV, Capriotti RM, Emurian CS. The regularity of smoked marijuana self-administration. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 1989;32:483–486. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90184-6. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forkstam C, Petersson KM. Towards an explicit account of implicit learning. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2005;18(4):435–441. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000171951.82995.c4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5(5):458–462. doi: 10.1038/nn834. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MJ, Moustafa AA, Haughey HM, Curran T, Hutchison KE. Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2007;104(41):16311–16316. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706111104. [rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O’Reilly RC. By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science. 2004;306(5703):1940–1943. doi: 10.1126/science.1102941. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken IHA. Drug craving and addiction: Integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2003;27:563–579. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00081-2. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’Donoghue T. Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature. 2002;40(2):351–401. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman AS, Meltzer LT, Bunney BS. Firing properties of substantia nigra dopaminergic neurons in freely moving rats. Life Sciences. 1985;36(20):1983–1994. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(85)90448-5. [JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freud S. Beyond the pleasure principle. In: Strachey J, Freud A, editors. The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud. vol. 18. Hogarth Press; 1920/1956. (Original work published 1920) [GA] [Google Scholar]
- Fudim OK. Sensory preconditioning of flavors with a formalin-produced sodium need. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1978;4(3):276–285. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.4.3.276. [KCB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. 3rd edition. Lippincott-Raven; 1997. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Galea S, Nandi A, Vlahov D. The social epidemiology of substance use. Epidemiology Review. 2004;26(1):36–52. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh007. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, McMahan RW, Schoenbaum G. Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:6610–6614. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06610.1999. [SBO] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallistel C, Gibbon J. Time, rate and conditioning. Psychological Review. 2000;107:289–344. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.289. [DR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallistel C, Gibbon J. The symbolic foundations of conditioned behavior. Erlbaum; 2002. [DR] [Google Scholar]
- Gambino B, Shaffer H. The concept of paradigm and the treatment of addiction. Professional Psychology. 1979;10:207–223. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan H, Pankiewicz J, Bloom A, Cho J-K, Sperry L, Ross TJ, Salmeron BJ, Risinger R, Kelley D, Stein EA. Cue-induced cocaine craving: Neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157(11):1789–1798. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1789. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardiner TW, Kitai ST. Single-unit activity in the globus pallidus and neostriatum of the rat during performance of a trained head-movement. Experimental Brain Research. 1992;88:517–530. doi: 10.1007/BF00228181. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garris PA, Kilpatrick M, Bunin MA, Michael D, Walker QD, Wightman RM. Dissociation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens from intracranial self-stimulation. Nature. 1999;398:67–69. doi: 10.1038/18019. [JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawin FH. Cocaine addiction: Psychology and neuropsychology. Science. 1991;251(5001):1580–1586. doi: 10.1126/science.2011738. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawronski B, Deutsch R, Mbirkou S, Seibt B, Strack F. When “Just say no” is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2008;44:370–377. [RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Georges F, Moine CL, Aston-Jones G. No effect of morphine on ventral tegmental dopamine neurons during withdrawal. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:5720–5726. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5032-05.2006. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerdeman GL, Partridge JG, Lupica CR, Lovinger DM. It could be habit forming: Drugs of abuse and striatal synaptic plasticity. Trends in Neuroscience. 2003;26(4):184–192. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00065-1. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- German PW, Fields HL. How prior reward experience biases exploratory movements: A probabilistic model. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007a;97(3):2083–2093. doi: 10.1152/jn.00303.2006. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- German PW, Fields HL. Rat nucleus accumbens neurons persistently encode locations associated with morphine reward. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007b;97(3):2094–2106. doi: 10.1152/jn.00304.2006. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer G. The adaptive toolbox: Toward a Darwinian rationality. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 2001;47:113–146. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer G, Goldstein DG. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review. 1996;103:650–669. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.4.650. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert DG, Sharpe JP, Ramanaiah NV, Detwiler FR, Anderson AE. Development of a situation x trait response (STAR) model-based smoking motivation questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2000;29:65–84. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D, editors. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgement. Cambridge University Press; 2002. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Giordano LA, Bickel WK, Loewenstein G, Jacobs EA, Marsch L, Badger GJ. Mild opioid deprivation increases the degree that opioid-dependent outpatients discount delayed heroin and money. Psychopharmacology. 2002;163(2):174–182. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1159-2. [AJG, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glimcher PW. Decisions, uncertainty, and the brain: The science of neuroeconomics. MIT Press; 2003. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Glimcher PW, Rustichini A. Neuroeconomics: The consilience of brain and decision. Science. 2004;306(5695):447–452. doi: 10.1126/science.1102566. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold MS. Cocaine (and crack): Clinical aspects. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 181–199. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Gold P. Coordination of multiple memory systems. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2004;82(3):230–242. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.07.003. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman D, Oroszi G, Ducci F. The genetics of addictions: Uncovering the genes. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2005;6(7):521–532. doi: 10.1038/nrg1635. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman MS, Boca FKD, Darkes J. Alcohol expectancy theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In: Leonard KE, Blane HT, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 203–246. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman MS, Brown SA, Christiansen BA. Expectancy theory: Thinking about drinking. In: Blaine HT, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. Guilford Press; 1987. pp. 181–226. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman MS, Del Boca FK, Darkes J. Alcohol expectancy theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In: Leonard KE, Blane HT, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. Guilford Press; 1999. [RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman PS, Rosvold HE, Mishkin M. Evidence for behavioral impairment following prefrontal lobectomy in the infant monkey. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1970;70(3):454–463. doi: 10.1037/h0028701. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS, Funahashi S, Bruce CJ. Neocortical memory circuits. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. 1990;55:1025–1038. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.097. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein A. Addiction: From biology to drug policy. Oxford University Press; 2000. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:1642–1652. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642. [AJG, MLM] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman A. Addiction: Definition and implications. British Journal of Addiction. 1990;85:1403–1408. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01620.x. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman A. Neurobiology of addiction: An integrative review. Biochemical Pharmacology. 2008;75:266–322. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.07.030. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin DW, Gabrielli WF. Alcohol: Clinical aspects. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 142–148. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Goto Y, Grace AA. Dopamine-dependent interactions between limbic and prefrontal cortical plasticity in the nucleus accumbens: Disruption by cocaine sensitization. Neuron. 2005a;47(2):255–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.017. [RAC, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goto Y, Grace AA. Dopaminergic modulation of limbic and cortical drive of nucleus accumbens in goal-directed behavior. Nature Neuroscience. 2005b;8(6):805–812. doi: 10.1038/nn1471. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goto Y, O’Donnell P. Delayed mesolimbic system alteration in a developmental animal model of schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(20):9070–9077. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-09070.2002. [RAC] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JE, Potenza MN, Hollander E, Cunningham-Williams R, Nurminen T, Smits G, Kallio A. Multicenter investigation of the opioid antagonist nalmefene in the treatment of pathological gambling. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163(2):303–312. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.303. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant S, Contoreggi C, London ED. Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(8):1180–1187. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00158-x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, Margolin A. Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 1996;93(21):12040–12045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.21.12040. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA. Elements of a two-process theory of learning. Academic Press; 1975. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford University Press; 2000. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Graybiel AM. The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1998;70(1–2):119–136. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3843. [rADR, RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greden JF, Walters A. Caffeine. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 294–307. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science. 2001;293:2105–2108. doi: 10.1126/science.1062872. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grenard JL, Ames SL, Wiers RW, Thush C, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Working memory moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.426. (in press) [RWW] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths MD. The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. British Journal of Psychology. 1994;85(3):351–370. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths MD. Sex addiction on the Internet. Janus Head: Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies in Literature, Continental Philosophy, Phenomenological Psychology and the Arts. 2004;7(2):188–217. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths MD. A “components” model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use. 2005;10:191–197. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Plante T, editor. Mental disorders of the new millennium. vol. 1. Greenwood; 2006. An overview of pathological gambling; pp. 73–98. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Willoughby T, Wood E, editors. Children’s learning in a digital world. Blackwell; 2008. Videogame addiction: Fact or fiction? pp. 85–103. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths MD, Delfabbro P. The biopsychosocial approach to gambling: Contextual factors in research and clinical interventions. Journal of Gambling Issues. 2001;5:1–33. Available at: http://www.camh.net/egambling/issue5/feature/index.html. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y. Neuroadaptation: Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature. 2001;412:141–142. doi: 10.1038/35084134. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen HJ, Wright CI, Beijer AV, Voorn P. Convergence and segregation of ventral striatal inputs and outputs. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999;877:49–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09260.x. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S. Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: I. Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors. Biological Cybernetics. 1976;23:121–134. doi: 10.1007/BF00344744. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman M, Chaloupka FJ. The demand for cocaine by young adults: A rational addiction approach. Journal of Health Economics. 1998;17:427–474. doi: 10.1016/s0167-6296(97)00046-5. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber J, Koszegi B. Is addiction“rational”? Theory and evidence. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2001;116:1261–1305. [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- Guroglu B, Haselager GJ, van Lieshout CF, Takashima A, Rijpkema M, Fernandez G. Why are friends special? Implementing a social interaction simulation task to probe the neural correlates of friendship. Neuroimage. 2008;39(2):903–910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.007. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guthrie ER. The psychology of learning. Harpers; 1935. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Gutkin BS, Dehaene S, Changeux J-P. A neurocomputational hypothesis for nicotine addiction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2006;103(4):1106–1111. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510220103. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN. The primate basal ganglia: Parallel and integrative networks. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy. 2003;26:317–330. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.10.003. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, Fudge JL, McFarland NR. Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(6):2369–2382. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-02369.2000. [RAC, aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halikas JA. Craving. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. 3rd edition. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 85–90. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson K, Allen S, Jensen S, Hatsukami D. Treatment of adolescent smokers with the nicotine patch. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2003;5(4):515–526. doi: 10.1080/14622200307243. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris AC, Gewirtz JC. Acute opioid dependence: Characterizing the early adaptations underlying drug withdrawal. Psychopharmacology. 2005;178(4):353–366. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2155-0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart CL, Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Alternative reinforcers differentially modify cocaine self-administration by humans. Behavioral Pharmacology. 2000;11(1):87–91. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200002000-00010. [CLH, rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart CL, Haney M, Vosburg SK, Comer SD, Foltin RW. Reinforcing effects of oral ∆9-THC in male marijuana smokers in a laboratory choice procedure. Psychopharmacology. 2005;181:237–243. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2234-2. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart CL, Haney M, Vosburg SK, Rubin E, Foltin RW. Smoked cocaine self-administration is decreased by modafinil. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:761–768. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301472. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasselmo ME. Acetylcholine and learning in a cortical associative memory. Neural Computation. 1993;5:32–44. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hasselmo ME, Bower JM. Acetylcholine and memory. Trends in Neurosciences. 1993;16(6):218–222. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(93)90159-j. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hastie R. Problems for judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:653–683. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.653. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatsukami DK, Thompson TN, Pentel PR, Flygare BK, Carroll ME. Self-administration of smoked cocaine. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1994;2(2):115–125. [CLH, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hauser KF, McLaughlin PJ, Zagon IS. Endogenous opioids regulate dendritic growth and spine formation in developing rat brain. Brain Research. 1987;416(1):157–161. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91509-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hauser KF, McLaughlin PJ, Zagon IS. Endogenous opioid systems and the regulation of dendritic growth and spine formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1989;281(1):13–22. doi: 10.1002/cne.902810103. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath D. Drinking occasions: Comparative perspectives on alcohol and culture. Psychology Press; 2000. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Hebb DO. The organization of behavior. Erlbaum; 1949/2002. (Original work published in 1949) [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Heishman SJ, Henningfield JE. Tolerance to repeated nicotine administration on performance, subjective, and physiological responses in nonsmokers. Psychopharmacology. 2000;152(3):321–334. doi: 10.1007/s002130000541. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemby S, Martin T, Co C, Dworkin S, Smith J. The effects of intravenous heroin administration on extracellular nucleus accumbens dopamine concentrations as determined by in vivo microdialysis. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1995;273(2):591–598. [arADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendersen RW, Graham J. Avoidance of heat by rats: Effects of thermal context on the rapidity of extinction. Learning and Motivation. 1979;10:351–363. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Henningfield JE, Keenan RM. Nicotine delivery kinetics and abuse liability. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61(5):743–750. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.5.743. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrnstein RJ. The matching law. Harvard University Press; 1997. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hertz J, Krogh A, Palmer RG. Introduction to the theory of neural computation. Addison-Wesley; 1991. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Herz A. Endogenous opioid systems and alcohol addiction. Psychopharmacology. 1997;129:99–111. doi: 10.1007/s002130050169. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hertz J, Krogh A, Palmer RG. Opioid reward mechanisms: A key role in drug abuse? Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology. 1998;76(3):252–258. doi: 10.1139/cjpp-76-3-252. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heyman GM. Resolving the contradictions of addiction. Brain and Behavioral Sciences. 1996;19(4):561–574. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Heyman GM. An economic approach to animal models of alcoholism. Alcohol Research and Health. 2000;24(2):132–139. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ST, Alessi SM, Dantona RL. Voucher-based incentives: A substance abuse treatment innovation. Addictive Behaviors. 2002;27:887–910. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(02)00297-6. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ST, Bickel WK, Hughes JR. Influence of an alternative reinforcer on human cocaine self-administration. Life Sciences. 1994;55:179–187. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(94)00878-7. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ST, Heil SH, Lussier JP. Clinical implications of reinforcement as a determinant of substance use disorders. Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55(1):431–461. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142033. [CLH, rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O, Miyashita K, Miyachi S, Sakai K, Lu X. Differential roles of the frontal cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum in visuomotor sequence learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1998;70(1–2):137–149. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3844. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O, Nakahara H, Rand MK, Sakai K, Lu X, Nakamura K, Miyachi S, Doya K. Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends in Neurosciences. 1999;22(10):464–471. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01439-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O, Nakamura K, Nakahara H. Basal ganglia orient eyes to reward. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;95:567–584. doi: 10.1152/jn.00458.2005. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hiroi N, Agatsuma S. Genetic susceptibility to substance dependence. Molecular Psychiatry. 2005;10:336–344. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001622. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsh R. The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: A theory. Behavioral Biology. 1974;12:421–444. doi: 10.1016/s0091-6773(74)92231-7. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann KL, McNaughton BL. Coordinated reactivation of distributed memory traces in primate neocortex. Science. 2002;297(5589):2070–2073. doi: 10.1126/science.1073538. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman WF, Moore M, Templin R, McFarland B, Hitzemann RJ, Mitchell SH. Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:162–170. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0494-0. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogarth L, Duka T. Human nicotine conditioning requires explicit contingency knowledge: Is addictive behaviour cognitively mediated? Psychopharmacology. 2006;184:553–566. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0150-0. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holden C. “Behavioral” addictions: Do they exist? Science. 2001;294:980–982. doi: 10.1126/science.294.5544.980. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC. Relations between Pavlovian-instrumental transfer and reinforcer devaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2004;30:104–117. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104. [SBO] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC, Rescorla RA. The effect of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after first- and second-order appetitive conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1975;1:355–363. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.1.4.355. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC, Straub JJ. Differential effects of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after Pavlovian appetitive conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1979;5:65–78. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.5.1.65. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollander E, Wong CM. Obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1995;56:3–6. [CWL] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt DD, Green L, Myerson J. Is discounting impulsive? Evidence from temporal and probability discounting in gambling and non-gambling college students. Behavioural Processes. 2003;64:355–367. doi: 10.1016/s0376-6357(03)00141-4. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hommer DW. Functional imaging of craving. Alcohol Research and Health. 1999;23(3):187–196. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopfield JJ. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 1982;79:2554–2558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Wiers RW. Assessing mplicit alcohol associations with the IAT: Fact or artifact? Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31:1346–1362. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.10.009. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houk JC, Adams JL, Barto AG. A model of how the basal ganglia generate and use neural signals that predict reinforcement. In: Houk JC, Davis JL, Beiser DG, editors. Models of information processing in the basal ganglia. MIT Press; 1995. pp. 249–270. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Howitt D. Concerning psychology. Oxford University Press; 1991. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Hruschka D, Lende DH, Worthman C. Biocultural dialogues: Biology and culture in psychological anthropology. Ethos. 2005;33(1):1–19. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Hu D, Amsel A. A simple test of the vicarious trial-and-error hypothesis of hippocampal function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 1995;92:5506–5509. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.12.5506. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu D, Xu X, Gonzalez-Lima F. Vicarious trial-and-error behavior and hippocampal cytochrome oxidase activity during Y-maze discrimination learning in the rat. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;116(3):265–280. doi: 10.1080/00207450500403108. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang Y-Y, Kandel ER, Vashavsky L, Brandon EP, Qi M, Idzerda RL, McKnight GS, Bourtchouladze R. A genetic test of the effects of mutations in PKA on mossy fiber LTP and its relation to spatial and contextual learning. Cell. 1995;83:1211–1222. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90146-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes JR, Hatsukami D. Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1986;43(3):289–294. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800030107013. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull CL. Principles of behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1943. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hull CL. A behavior system: An introduction to behavior theory concerning the individual organism. Yale University Press; 1952. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt WA. Pharmacology of alcohol. In: Tarter RE, Ammerman RT, Ott PJ, editors. Handbook of substance abuse: Neurobehavioral pharmacology. Plenum; 1998. pp. 7–22. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Hurd YL, Herkenham M. Molecular alterations in the neostriatum of human cocaine addicts. Synapse. 1993;13(4):357–369. doi: 10.1002/syn.890130408. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hursh SR. Behavioral economics of drug self-administration and drug abuse policy. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1991;56(2):377–393. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.56-377. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hursh SR, Galuska CM, Winger G, Woods JH. The economics of drug abuse: A quantitative assessment of drug demand. Molecular Interventions. 2005;5:20–28. doi: 10.1124/mi.5.1.6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Husain M, Parton A, Hodgson TL, Mort D, Rees G. Self-control during response conflict by human supplementary eye field. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:117–118. doi: 10.1038/nn1005. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchison KA. Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2003;10:785–813. doi: 10.3758/bf03196544. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SE. Addiction: A disease of learning and memory. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1414–1422. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1414. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SE, Malenka RC. Addiction and the brain: The neurobiology of compulsion and its persistence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:695–703. doi: 10.1038/35094560. [RAC, MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikemoto S, Panksepp J. The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews. 1999;31(1):6–41. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00023-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikemoto S, Qin M, Liu ZH. Primary reinforcing effects of nicotine are triggered from multiple regions both inside and outside the ventral tegmental area. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:723–730. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4542-05.2006. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irvin JE, Brandon TH. The increasing recalcitrance of smokers in clinical trials. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2000;2(1):79–84. doi: 10.1080/14622200050011330. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irvin JE, Hendricks PS, Brandon TH. The increasing recalcitrance of smokers in clinical trials: II. Pharmacotherapy trials. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2003;5(1):27–35. doi: 10.1080/1462220031000070534. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irvine JM. Reinventing perversion: Sex addiction and cultural anxieties. Journal of the History of Sexuality. 1995;5:429–450. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Isoda M, Hikosaka O. Switching from automatic to controlled action by monkey medial frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:240–248. doi: 10.1038/nn1830. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito R, Dalley JW, Howes SR, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Dissociation in conditioned dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and shell in response to cocaine cues and during cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(19):7489–7495. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07489.2000. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito R, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Dopamine release in the dorsal striatum during cocaine-seeking behavior under the control of a drug-associated cue. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(14):6247–6253. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-06247.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itoh H, Nakahara H, Hikosaka O, Kawagoe R, Takikawa Y, Aihara K. Correlation of primate caudate neural activity and saccade parameters in reward-oriented behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89(4):1774–1783. doi: 10.1152/jn.00630.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iversen SD, Mishkin M. Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Experimental Brain Research. 1970;11(4):376–386. doi: 10.1007/BF00237911. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson GM, Jackson SR, Harrison J, Henderson L, Kennard C. Serial reaction time learning and Parkinson’s disease: Evidence for a procedural learning deficit. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33(5):577–593. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00010-z. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffe JH. Current concepts of addiction. In: O’Brien CP, Jaffe JH, editors. Research publications: Association for research in nervous and mental disease. vol. 70. Raven; 1992. pp. 1–21. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffe JH, Knapp CM, Ciraulo DA. Opiates: Clinical aspects. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrood JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 158–166. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins JE. The influence of peer affiliation and student activities on adolescent drug involvement. Adolescence. 1996;31(122):297–306. [JMB] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen O, Lisman JE. An oscillatory short-term memory buffer model can account for data on the Sternberg task. Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18(24):10688–10699. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-24-10688.1998. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen O, Lisman JE. Hippocampal sequence-encoding driven by a cortical multi-item working memory buffer. Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28(2):67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.12.001. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Olausson P, Garza RDL, Taylor JR. Impairments of reversal learning and response perseveration after repeated, intermittent cocaine administrations to monkeys. Neuropsychologia. 2002;26:183–190. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00355-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeong H, Mason SP, Barabasi AL, Oltvai ZN. Lethality and centrality in protein networks. Nature. 2001;411(6833):41. doi: 10.1038/35075138. [WKB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ji M, Wood W. Purchase and consumption habits: Not necessarily what you intend. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 2007;17:261–276. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Jog MS, Kubota Y, Connolly CI, Hillegaart V, Graybiel AM. Building neural representations of habits. Science. 1999;286:1746–1749. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5445.1745. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johanson CE, Fischman MW. The pharmacology of cocaine related to its abuse. Pharmacological Reviews. 1989;41(1):3–52. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson A, Redish AD. Hippocampal replay contributes to within session learning in a temporal difference reinforcement learning model. Neural Networks. 2005;18(9):1163–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2005.08.009. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson A, Redish AD. Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(45):12176–12189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3761-07.2007. [arADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson A, van der Meer MAA, Redish AD. Integrating hippocampus and striatum in decision-making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17(6):692–697. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.01.003. [rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SW, North RA. Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyper-polarization of local interneurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12:483–488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-02-00483.1992. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones BT, Corbin W, Fromme K. A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction. 2001;96:57–72. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961575.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung MW, Qin Y, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA. Firing characteristics of deep layer neurons in prefrontal cortex in rats performing spatial working memory tasks. Cerebral Cortex. 1998;8:437–450. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.5.437. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D. A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist. 2003;58:697–720. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Frederick S. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D, editors. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press; 2002. pp. 49–81. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A, editors. Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press; 1982. . [ELK, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47(2):263–292. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Tversky A, editors. Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge University Press; 2000. [ELK, aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW, McFarland K. Brain circuitry and the reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168:44–56. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1393-2. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW, Peters J, Knackstedt L. Animal models and brain circuits in drug addiction. Molecular Interventions. 2006;6:339–344. doi: 10.1124/mi.6.6.7. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The neural basis of addiction: A pathology of motivation and choice. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(8):1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403. [aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW, Volkow ND, Seamans J. Unmanageable motivation in addiction: A pathology in prefrontal-accumbens glutamate transmission. Neuron. 2005;45(5):647–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.005. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, Yamaguchi K, Chen K. Stages of progression in drug involvement from adolescence to adulthood: Further evidence for the gateway theory. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1992;53:447–457. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1992.53.447. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplow JB, Curran PJ, Dodge KA Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Child, parent, and peer predictors of early-onset substance use: A multisite longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30(3):199–216. doi: 10.1023/a:1015183927979. [JMB] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S, Mizrahi R, Li M. From dopamine to salience to psychosis-linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophrenic Research. 2005;79(1):59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.003. [JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz JL, Higgins ST. The validity of the reinstatement model of craving and relapse. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168:21–30. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1441-y. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawagoe R, Takikawa Y, Hikosaka O. Reward-predicting activity of dopamine and caudate neurons – a possible mechanism of motivational control of saccadic eye movement. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91(2):1013–1024. doi: 10.1152/jn.00721.2003. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE. Functional specificity of ventral striatal compartments in appetitive behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999a;877:71–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09262.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE. Neural integrative activities of nucleus accumbens subregions in relation to learning and motivation. Psychobiology. 1999b;27(2):198–213. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE. Memory and addiction: Shared neural circuitry and molecular mechanisms. Neuron. 2004a;44:161–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.016. [aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE. Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: Role in ingestive behavior and reward related learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004b;27:765–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.015. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: Relevance to addictive drugs. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(9):3306–3311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03306.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE, Bakshi VP, Haber SN, Steininger TL, Will MJ, Zhang M. Opioid modulation of taste hedonics within the ventral striatum. Physiology and Behavior. 2002;76(3):365–377. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00751-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE, Schochet T, Landry CF. Risk taking and novelty seeking in adolescence: Introduction to part I. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:27–32. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.003. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenny PJ, Markou A. Conditioned nicotine withdrawal profoundly decreases the activity of brain reward systems. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25(26):6208–6212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4785-04.2005. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kentros CG, Agnihotri NT, Streater S, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER. Increased attention to spatial context increases both place field stability and spatial memory. Neuron. 2004;42:283–295. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00192-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kermadi I, Joseph JP. Activity in the caudate nucleus of monkey during spatial sequencing. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995;74(3):911–933. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.911. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kermadi I, Jurquet Y, Arzi M, Joseph J. Neural activity in the caudate nucleus of monkeys during spatial sequencing. Experimental Brain Research. 1993;94:352–356. doi: 10.1007/BF00230305. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner RP, Farnsworth G, DiMattia BV. Double dissociation of egocentric and allocentric space following medial prefrontal and parietal cortex lesions in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1989;103(5):956–961. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.5.956. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC. The epidemiology of dual diagnosis. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:730–737. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.034. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khalil EL. Buridan’s ass, uncertainty, risk, and self-competition: A theory of entrepreneurship. Kyklos. 1997;50:147–163. [ELK] [Google Scholar]
- Khalil EL. The problem of creativity: Distinguishing technological action and cognitive action. Revue de Philosophie Economique. 2007;8:33–69. [ELK] [Google Scholar]
- Kiefer F, Mann K. New achievements and pharmacotherapeutic approaches in the treatment of alcohol dependence. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2005;526(1–3):163–171. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.028. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kieffer BL. Opioids: First lessons from knockout mice. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 1999;20(1):19–26. doi: 10.1016/s0165-6147(98)01279-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killcross S, Coutureau E. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cerebral Cortex. 2003;13(8):400–408. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.4.400. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim SJ, Lyoo IK, Hwang J, Sung YH, Lee HY, Lee DS, Jeong DU, Renshaw PF. Frontal glucose hypometabolism in abstinent methamphetamine users. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1383–1391. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300699. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby KN, Herrnstein RJ. Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed rewards. Psychological Science. 1995;6(2):83–89. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1999;128(1):78–87. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.128.1.78. [CWL, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiviniemi MT, Bevins RA. Affect-behavior associations in motivated behavioral choice: Potential transdisciplinary links. In: Zelick PR, editor. Issues in the psychology of motivation. Nova; 2007. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Kiviniemi MT, Voss-Humke AM, Seifert AL. How do I feel about the behavior? The interplay of affective associations with behaviors and cognitive beliefs as influences on physical activity behavior. Health Psychology. 2007;26:152–158. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.2.152. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyatkin EA. Behavioral significance of phasic changes in mesolimbic dopamine-dependent electrochemical signal associated with heroin self-injections. Journal of Neural Transmission, General Section. 1994;96(3):197–214. doi: 10.1007/BF01294787. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyatkin EA, Gratton A. Electrochemical monitoring of extracellular dopamine in nucleus accumbens of rats lever-pressing for food. Brain Research. 1994;652:225–234. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90231-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyatkin EA, Rebec GV. Activity of presumed dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area during heroin self-administration. NeuroReport. 1997;8(11):2581–2585. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199707280-00032. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyatkin EA, Rebec GV. Impulse activity of ventral tegmental area neurons during heroin self-administration in rats. Neuroscience. 2001;102(3):565–580. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00492-9. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleber HD, Califano JA, Demers JC. Clinical and societal implications of drug legalization. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 855–864. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiman MAR. Controlling drug use and crime with testing, sanctions, and treatment. In: Heymann & William PB, Brownsberger N, editors. Drug addiction and drug policy: The struggle to control dependence. Harvard University Press; 2001. [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- Knopman DS, Nissen MJ. Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1987;37(5):784–788. doi: 10.1212/wnl.37.5.784. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knopman DS, Nissen MJ. Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington’s disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29(3):245–254. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90085-m. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knowlton BJ, Squire LR, Gluck MA. Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. Learning and Memory. 1994;1(2):106–120. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koene RA, Gorchetchnikov A, Cannon RC, Hasselmo ME. Modeling goal-directed spatial navigation in the rat based on physiological data from the hippocampal formation. Neural Networks. 2003;16(5–6):577–584. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(03)00106-0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohonen T. Self-organization and associative memory. Springer-Verlag; 1984. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb B. Prefrontal cortex. In: Kolb B, Tees RC, editors. The cerebral cortex of the rat. MIT Press; 1990. pp. 437–458. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, Bloom FE. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. Science. 1988;242:715–723. doi: 10.1126/science.2903550. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, LeMoal M. Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997;278(5335):52–58. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52. [MLM, aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, LeMoal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24(2):97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [CWL, MLM, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, LeMoal M. Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the ‘dark side’ of drug addiction. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(11):1442–1444. doi: 10.1038/nn1105-1442. [MLM, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, LeMoal M. Neurobiology of addiction. Elsevier Academic; 2006. [MTK, MLM, arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, LeMoal M. Drug addiction: Pathways to the disease and pathophysiological perspectives. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;17:377–393. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.10.006. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, LeMoal M. Addiction and the brain anti-reward system. Annual Review of Psychology. 2008;59:29–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotler M, Cohen H, Segman R, Gritsenko I, Nemanov L, Lerer B, Kramer I, Zer-Zion M, Kletz I, Ebstein RP. Excess dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III seven repeat allele in opioid-dependent subjects. Molecular Psychiatry. 1997;2:251–254. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000248. [JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krank MD, Swift R. Unconscious influences of specific memories on alcohol outcome expectancies. Alcoholism: Experimental and Clinical Research. 1994;18:423. (Abstract) [RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, LaForge KS. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1450–1457. doi: 10.1038/nn1583. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreps DM. A course in microeconomic theory. Princeton University Press; 1990. [ELK] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan-Sarin S, Reynolds B, Duhig AM, Smith A, Liss T, McFertridge A, Cavallo DA, Carroll KM, Potenza MN. Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in a smoking cessation program for adolescent smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88:79–82. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.006. [CWL] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruse JM, Overmier JB, Konz WA, Rokke E. Pavlovian conditioned stimulus effects upon instrumental choice behavior are reinforcer specific. Learning and Motivation. 1983;14:165–181. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhar MJ, Ritz MC, Sharkey J. Cocaine receptors on dopamine transporters mediate cocaine-reinforced behavior. In: Clouet D, Asghar K, Brown R, editors. Mechanisms of cocaine abuse and toxicity. National Institute on Drug Abuse; 1988. pp. 14–22. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuley NB, Jacobs DE. The relationship between dissociative-like experiences and sensation seeking among social and problem gamblers. Journal of Gambling Behavior. 1987;4:197–207. [KRC] [Google Scholar]
- Laing CR, Chow CC. Stationary bumps in networks of spiking neurons. Neural Computation. 2001;13(7):1473–1514. doi: 10.1162/089976601750264974. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langer EJ, Roth J. Heads I win, tails it’s chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1975;32(6):951–955. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Lasser K, Boyd JW, Woolhandler S, Heimmerlstein D, McCormick D, Bor D. Smoking in mental illness: A population-based prevalence study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2000;284(20):2606–2610. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.20.2606. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laviolette SR, Gallegos RA, Henriksen SJ, van der Kooy D. Opiate state controls bi-directional reward signaling via GABAA receptors in the ventral tegmental area. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(2):160–169. doi: 10.1038/nn1182. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavoie AM, Mizumori SJY. Spatial-, movement- and reward-sensitive discharge by medial ventral striatum neurons in rats. Brain Research. 1994;638:157–168. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90645-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee D. Game theory and neural basis of social decision making. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:404–409. doi: 10.1038/nn2065. [rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lende DH. Colombia y la prevención sociocultural del uso de droga [Colombia and the sociocultural prevention of drug use] Humanidades. 2005a;8(11–12):9–30. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Lende DH. Wanting and drug use: A biocultural analysis of addiction. Ethos. 2005b;33(1):100–124. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Lende DH. Evolution and modern behavioral problems. In: Trevathan W, Smith EO, McKenna JJ, editors. Evolutionary medicine and health: New perspectives. Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 277–290. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Lende DH, Leonard T, Sterk CE, Elifson K. Functional methamphetamine use: The insiders’ perspective. Addiction Research and Theory. 2007;15(5):465–477. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Lende DH, Smith EO. Evolution meets biopsychosociality: An analysis of addictive behavior. Addiction. 2002;97(4):447–458. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00022.x. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenoir M, Ahmed SH. Supply of a nondrug substitute reduces escalated heroin consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301602. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenoir M, Serre F, Cantin L, Ahmed SH. Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(8):e698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000698. [SHA, rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leone G, editor. Remembering together: Some thoughts on how direct or virtual social interactions influence memory processes. Erlbaum; 2006. [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- Leri F, Bruneau J, Stewart J. Understanding polydrug use: Review of heroin and cocaine co-use. Addiction. 2003;98(1):7–23. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00236.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeSage MG, Burroughs D, Dufek M, Keyler DE, Pentel PR. Reinstatement of nicotine self-administration in rats by presentation of nicotine-paired stimuli, but not nicotine priming. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2004;79(3):507–513. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.09.002. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leshner AI. Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science. 1997;278(5335):45–47. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.45. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesieur H. The chase: Career of the compulsive gambler. Anchor; 1977. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Letchworth SR, Nader MA, Smith HR, Friedman DP, Porrino LJ. Progression of changes in dopamine transporter binding site density as a result of cocaine self-administration in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(8):2799–2807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02799.2001. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine AS, Billington CJ. Opioids as agents of reward-related feeding: A consideration of the evidence. Physiology and Behavior. 2004;82:57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.032. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy DA, Stark CEL, Squire LR. Intact conceptual priming in the absence of declarative memory. Psychological Science. 2004;15:680–686. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00740.x. [RWW] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy WB. A sequence predicting CA3 is a flexible associator that learns and uses context to solve hippocampal-like tasks. Hippocampus. 1996;6(6):579–591. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:6<579::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-C. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy WB, Sanyal A, Rodriguez P, Sullivan DW, Wu XB. The formation of neural codes in the hippocampus: Trace conditioning as a prototypical paradigm for studying the random recoding hypothesis. Biological Cybernetics. 2005;92:409–426. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0568-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, Acerbo MJ, Robinson TE. The induction of behavioural sensitization is associated with cocaine-induced structural plasticity in the core (but not shell) of the nucleus accumbens. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;20(6):1647–1654. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03612.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao D, Lin H, Law PY, Loh HH. Mu-opioid receptors modulate the stability of dendritic spines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2005;102(5):1725–1730. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406797102. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipska BK, Lerman DN, Khaing ZZ, Weinberger DR. The neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion model of schizophrenia: Effects on dopamine and GABA mRNA markers in the rat midbrain. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;18:3097–3104. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03047.x. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman JE, Grace AA. The hippocampal-VTA loop: Controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron. 2005;46(5):703–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.002. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Littleton J. Neurochemical mechanisms underlying alcohol withdrawal. Alcohol Research and Health. 1998;22(1):13–24. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu JL, Liu JT, Hammit JK, Chou SY. The price elasticity of opium in Taiwan, 1914–1942. Journal of Health Economics. 1999;18:795–810. doi: 10.1016/s0167-6296(99)00023-5. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;67(1):145–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.1.145. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lohrenz T, McCabe K, Camerer CF, Montague PR. Neural signature of fictive learning signals in a sequential investment task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2007;104(22):9493–9498. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608842104. [rADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez M, Balleine BW, Dickinson A. Incentive learning and the motivational control of instrumental performance by thirst. Animal Learning and Behavior. 1992;20:322–328. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. 3rd edition. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Lubman DI, Peters LA, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Deakin JF. Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychological Medicine. 2000;30:169–175. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001269. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubman DI, Yücel M, Pantelis C. Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction. 2004;99:1491–1502. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00808.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacAndrew C, Edgerton RB. Drunken comportment: A social explanation. Walter de Gruyter; 1969. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- MacCoun RJ. Drugs and the law: A psychological analysis of drug prohibition. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;113(3):497–512. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.497. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacCoun RJ. In what sense (if any) is marijuana a gateway drug? [Retrieved March, 9, 2005];FAS Drug Policy Analysis Bulletin. 1998 from http://www.fas.org/drugs/issue4.htm#gateway [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- MacCoun RJ. Is the addiction concept useful for drug policy? In: Vuchinich R, Heather N, editors. Choice, behavioural economics and addiction. Elsevier; 2003. pp. 383–408. [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- MacCoun RJ, Reuter P. Drug war heresies: Learning from other vices, times, and places. Cambridge University Press; 2001. [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- MacKillop J, Anderson EJ, Castelda BA, Mattson RE, Donovick PJ. Convergent validity of measures of cognitive distortions, impulsivity, and time perspective with pathological gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2006;20(1):75–79. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.1.75. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKillop J, Monti PM. Advances in the scientific study of craving for alcohol and tobacco. In: Miller PM, Kavanagh D, editors. Translation of addiction science into practice. Ch. 10. Elsevier; 2007. pp. 187–207. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh NJ. The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press; 1974. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Maddahian E, Newcomb MD, Bentler PM. Adolescents’ substance use: Impact of ethnicity, income, and availability. Advances in Alcohol and Substance Abuse. 1986;5(3):63–78. doi: 10.1300/J251v05n03_05. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madden GJ, Bickel WK, Critchfield T, editors. Impulsivity: Theory, science, and neuroscience of discounting. APA Books; (in press) [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Madden GJ, Bickel WK, Jacobs EA. Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients exponential or hyperbolic discounting functions? Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1999;7(3):284–293. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.7.3.284. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madden GJ, Petry NM, Badger GJ, Bickford WK. Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control patients: Drug and monetary rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1997;5(3):256–262. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.5.3.256. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manski C, Pepper J, Petrie C, editors. Informing America’s policy on illegal drugs: What we don’t know keeps hurting us. 2001. Available at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309072735/html/ [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- Mansvelder HD, McGehee DS. Long-term potentiation of excitatory inputs to brain reward areas by nicotine. Neuron. 2000;27:349–357. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00042-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansvelder HD, McGehee DS. Cellular and synaptic mechanisms of nicotine addiction. Journal of Neurobiology. 2002;53(4):606–617. doi: 10.1002/neu.10148. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantsch JR, Yuferov V, Mathieu-Kia AM, Ho A, Kreek MJ. Effects of extended access to high versus low cocaine doses on self-administration, cocaine-induced reinstatement and brain mRNA levels in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2004;175:26–36. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1778-x. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mark TL, Woody GE, Juday T, Kleber HD. The economic costs of heroin addiction in the United States. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2001;61(2):195–206. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(00)00162-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marks MJ, Pauly JR, Gross SD, Deneris ES, Hermans-Borgmeyer I, Heinemann SF, Collins AC. Nicotine binding and nicotinic receptor subunit RNA after chronic nicotine treatment. Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12:2765–2784. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-07-02765.1992. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marlatt GA. Alcohol, the magic elixir? In: Peele S, Grant M, editors. Alcohol and pleasure: A health perspective. Psychology Press; 1999. pp. 233–248. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Marlatt GA, Baer JS, Donovan DM, Kivlahan DR. Addictive behaviors: Etiology and treatment. Annual Review of Psychology. 1988;39:233–252. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.001255. [MDG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr D. Simple memory: A theory of archicortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. 1971;262(841):23–81. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin PD. Locomotion towards a goal alters the synchronous firing of neurons recorded simultaneously in the subiculum and nucleus accumbens of rats. Behavioral Brain Research. 2001;124(1):19–28. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00209-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin PD, Ono T. Effects of reward anticipation, reward presentation, and spatial parameters on the firing of single neurons recorded in the subiculum and nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2000;116:23–38. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00249-7. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez D, Narendran R, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Broft A, Huang Y, Cooper TB, Fischman MW, Kleber HD, Laruelle M. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release: Markedly blunted in cocaine dependence and predictive of the choice to self-administer cocaine. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(4):622–629. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.622. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mas-Nieto M, Wilson J, Cupo A, Roques BP, Noble F. Chronic morphine treatment modulates the extracellular levels of endogenous enkephalins in rat brain structures involved in opiate dependence: A microdialysis study. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:1034–1041. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-01034.2002. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto N, Hanakawa T, Maki S, Graybiel AM, Kimura M. Role of nigrostriatal dopamine system in learning to perform sequential motor tasks in a predictive manner. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;82(2):978–998. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.2.978. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthes HWD, Maldonado R, Simonin F, Valverde O, Slowe S, Kitchen I, Befort K, Dierich A, Meur ML, Dolĺe P, Tzavara E, Hanoune J, Roques BP, Kieffer BL. Loss of morphine-induced analgesia, reward effect, and withdrawal symptoms in mice lacking the µ-opioid-receptor gene. Nature. 1996;383:819–823. doi: 10.1038/383819a0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayr E. This is biology: The science of the living world. Belknap; 1998. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Mazur JE. Hyperbolic value addition and general models of animal choice. Psychological Review. 2001;108(1):96–112. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.96. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCaul ME, Petry NM. The role of psychosocial treatments in pharmacotherapy for alcoholism. The American Journal on Addictions. 2003;12:S41–S52. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2003.tb00495.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure SM, Berns GS, Montague PR. Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron. 2003;38(2):339–346. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00154-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure SM, Daw N, Montague R. A computational substrate for incentive salience. Trends in Neuroscience. 2003;26:423–428. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(03)00177-2. [DR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306(5695):503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907. [SHA, WKB, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald RJ, White NM. Parallel information processing in the water maze: Evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampus. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1994;61:260–270. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(05)80009-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarland K, Kalivas PW. The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(21):8655–8663. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-21-08655.2001. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarland K, Lapish CC, Kalivas PW. Prefrontal glutamate release into the core of the nucleus accumbens mediates cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23(8):3531–3537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03531.2003. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGeorge AJ, Faull RL. The organization of the projection from the cerebral cortex to the striatum in the rat. Neuroscience. 1989;29(3):503–537. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90128-0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMurran M. The psychology of addiction. Taylor and Francis; 1994. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Meunzinger KF. Vicarious trial and error at a point of choice. I. A general survey of its relation to learning efficiency. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 1938;53:75–86. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer R, Mirin S. The heroin stimulus. Plenum; 1979. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature. 2002;420:70–74. doi: 10.1038/nature01138. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miles FJ, Everitt BJ, Dickinson A. Oral cocaine seeking by rats: Action or habit? Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;117(5):927–938. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.927. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millar A, Navarick D. Self-control and choice in humans. Learning and Motivation. 1984;15:203–218. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- Mirenowicz J, Schultz W. Importance of unpredictability for reward responses in primate dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994;72(2):1024–1027. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.2.1024. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishkin M, Appenzeller T. The anatomy of memory. Scientific American. 1987;256(6):80–89. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0687-80. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishkin M, Malamut B, Bachevalier J. Memories and habits: Two neural systems. In: Lynch G, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM, editors. Neurobiology of learning and memory. Guilford: 1984. pp. 65–77. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Miyachi S, Hikosaka O, Miyashita K, Kárádi Z, Rand MK. Differential roles of monkey striatum in learning of sequential hand movement. Experimental Brain Research. 1997;115:1–5. doi: 10.1007/pl00005669. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyazaki K, Mogi E, Araki N, Matsumoto G. Reward-quality dependent anticipation in rat nucleus accumbens. NeuroReport. 1998;9:3943–3948. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199812010-00032. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moak DH, Anton RF. Alcohol. In: McCrady BS, Epstein EE, editors. Addictions: A comprehensive textbook. Oxford University Press; 1999. pp. 75–95. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller F, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogenson GJ. Limbic-motor integration – with emphasis on initiation of exploratory and goal-directed locomotion. In: Bandler R, editor. Modulation of sensorimotor activity during alterations in behavioral states. Liss; 1984. pp. 121–138. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Mogenson GJ, Jones DL, Yim CY. From motivation to action: Functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Progress in Neurobiology. 1980;14:69–97. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(80)90018-0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molina JC, Bannoura MD, Chotro MG, McKinzie DL, Arnold HM, Spear NE. Alcohol-mediated tactile conditioned aversions in infant rats: Devaluation of conditioning through alcohol-sucrose associations. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1996;66:121–132. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0053. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montague PR, Dayan P, Person C, Sejnowski TJ. Bee foraging in uncertain environments using predictive Hebbian learning. Nature. 1995;377(6551):725–728. doi: 10.1038/377725a0. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montague PR, Dayan P, Sejnowski TJ. A framework for mesence-phalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16(5):1936–1947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01936.1996. [arADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monterosso JR, Flannery BA, Pettinati HM, Oslin DW, Rukstalis M, O’Brien CP, Volpicelli JR. Predicting treatment response to naltrexone: The influence of craving and family history. American Journal on Addictions. 2001;10(3):258–268. doi: 10.1080/105504901750532148. [TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monti PM, MacKillop J. Advances in the treatment of craving for alcohol and tobacco. In: Miller PM, Kavanagh D, editors. Translation of addiction science into practice. Ch. 11. Elsevier; 2007. pp. 209–235. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan D, Grant KA, Gage HD, Mach RH, Kaplan JR, Prioleau O, Nader SH, Buchheimer N, Ehrenkaufer RL, Nader MA. Social dominance in monkeys: Dopamine D2 receptors and cocaine self-administration. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5(2):169–174. doi: 10.1038/nn798. [DHL, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RGM, Garrud P, Rawlins JNP, O’Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982;297:681–683. doi: 10.1038/297681a0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mucha RF, Herz A. Motivational properties of kappa and mu opioid receptor agonists studied with place and taste preference conditioning. Psychopharmacology. 1985;86:274–280. doi: 10.1007/BF00432213. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munn NL. Handbook of psychological research on the rat. Houghton Mifflin; 1950. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Muraven M, Baumeister RF. Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:247–259. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247. [DTN] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy BL, Arnsten AFT, Goldman-Rakic PS, Roth RH. Increased dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 1996;93(3):1325–1329. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.3.1325. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray KB, Häubl G. Explaining cognitive lock-in: The role of skill-based habits of use in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research. 2007;34:77–88. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Mushiake H, Saito M, Sakamoto K, Itoyama Y, Tanji J. Activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex reflects multiple steps of future events in action plans. Neuron. 2006;50(4):631–641. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.045. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers KM, Davis M. Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron. 2002;36(4):567–584. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01064-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers KM, Davis M. Mechanisms of fear extinction. Molecular Psychiatry. 2007;12:120–150. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001939. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L. Multiple memory systems: What and why, an update. In: Schacter DL, Tulving E, editors. Memory systems 1994. MIT Press; 1994. pp. 39–64. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L, Bohbot V. Consolidation of memory. Hippocampus. 2001;11:56–60. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2001)11:1<56::AID-HIPO1020>3.0.CO;2-O. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1997;7:217–227. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80010-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nader MA, Woolverton WL. Cocaine vs. food choice in rhesus monkeys: Effects of increasing the response cost for cocaine. NIDA Research Monographs. 1990;105:621. [rADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nader MA, Woolverton WL. Effects of increasing the magnitude of an alternative reinforcer on drug choice in a discrete-trials choice procedure. Psychopharmacology. 1991;105:169–174. doi: 10.1007/BF02244304. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakahara H, Itoh H, Kawagoe R, Takikawa Y, Hikosaka O. Dopamine neurons can represent context-dependent prediction error. Neuron. 2004;41:269–280. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00869-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neal DT, Pascoe T, Wood W. Effects of regulatory depletion on the implementation and inhibition of habits in everyday life. Duke University; 2008. Unpublished manuscript [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Neal DT, Wood W. Automaticity in situ: Direct context cuing of habits in daily life. In: Bargh JA, Gollwitzer P, Morsella E, editors. Psychology of action, vol. 2: Mechanisms of human action. Oxford University Press; (in press) [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Neal DT, Wood W, Quinn JM. Habits: A repeat performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:198–202. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Negus S, Henriksen S, Mattox A, Pasternak G, Portoghese P, Takemori A, Weinger M, Koob G. Effect of antagonists selective for mu, delta and kappa opioid receptors on the reinforcing effects of heroin in rats. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1993;265(3):1245–1252. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nehlig A. Are we dependent upon coffee and caffeine? A review on human and animal data. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1999;23(4):563–576. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00050-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nehlig A, Boyet S. Dose-response study of caffeine effects on cerebral functional activity with a specific focus on dependence. Brain Research. 2000;858(1):71–77. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)02480-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neiss R. The role of psychobiological states in chemical dependency: Who becomes addicted? Addiction. 1993;88(6):745–756. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02089.x. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson A, Killcross S. Amphetamine exposure enhances habit formation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(14):3805–3812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4305-05.2006. [SBO, aADR, HY] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestler EJ. Is there a common molecular pathway for addiction? Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1445–1449. doi: 10.1038/nn1578. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newell A. Unified theories of cognition. Harvard University Press; 1990. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Nicola SM, Malenka RC. Modulation of synaptic transmission by dopamine and norepinephrine in ventral but not dorsal striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;79:1768–1776. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.4.1768. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson OG, Shapiro ML, Gage FH, Olton DS, Bjorklund A. Spatial learning and memory following fimbria-fornix transection and grafting of fetal septal neurons to the hippocampus. Experimental Brain Research. 1987;67:195–215. doi: 10.1007/BF00269466. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishioku T, Shimazoe T, Yamamoto Y, Nakanishi H, Watanabe S. Expression of long-term potentiation of the striatum in methamphetamine-sensitized rats. Neuroscience Letters. 1999;268(2):81–84. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00386-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology. 1987;19:1–32. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Nissen MJ, Knopman DS, Schacter DL. Neurochemical dissociation of memory systems. Neurology. 1987;37(5):789–794. doi: 10.1212/wnl.37.5.789. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation. The Hebrew University of Jerusalem; 2006. The effects of motivation on habitual instrumental behavior. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y. Cost, benefit, tonic, phasic. What do response rates tell us about dopamine and motivation? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007;1104(1):357–376. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.018. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y, Daw ND, Dayan P. How fast to work: Response vigor, motivation and tonic dopamine. In: Weiss Y, Schölkopf B, Platt J, editors. Advances in neural information processing systems 18. MIT Press; 2006a. pp. 1019–1026. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y, Joel D, Dayan P. A normative perspective on motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006b;10(8):375–381. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.010. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y, Daw ND, Joel D, Dayan P. Tonic dopamine: Opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191(3):507–520. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0502-4. [DTN, arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nooteboom B. Learning and innovation in organizations and economies. Oxford University Press; 2000. [ELK] [Google Scholar]
- Nurnberger JI, Bierut L. Seeking the connections: Alcoholism and our genes. Scientific American. 2007;296(4):46–53. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0407-46. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien CP. Anticraving medications for relapse prevention: A possible new class of psychoactive medications. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1423–1431. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1423. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R. A learning model of addiction. In: O’Brien CP, Jaffe JH, editors. Research publications: Association for research in nervous and mental disease, vol. 70. Raven; 1992. pp. 157–177. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien CP, McLellan AT. Myths about the treatment of addiction. The Lancet. 1996;347:237–240. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)90409-2. [RJM, rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien CP, Testa T, O’Brien TJ, Brady JP, Wells B. Conditioned narcotic withdrawal in humans. Science. 1977;195:1000–1002. doi: 10.1126/science.841320. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien CP, Volkow N, Li TK. What’s in a word? Addiction versus dependence in DSM-V. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:764–765. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.764. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien CP, Volpicelli LA, Volpicelli JR. Naltrexone in the treatment of alcoholism: A clinical review. Alcohol. 1996;13(1):35–39. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(95)02038-1. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Doherty JP. Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:769–776. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.016. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston K, Dolan RJ. Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science. 2004;304(5669):452–454. doi: 10.1126/science.1094285. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donnell P, Greene J, Pabello N, Lewis BL, Grace AA. Modulation of cell firing in the nucleus accumbens. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999;877:157–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09267.x. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donoghue T, Rabin M. Doing it now or later. The American Economic Review. 1999a;89(1):103–123. [CA, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donoghue T, Rabin M. Incentives for procrastinators. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1999b;114(3):769–816. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donoghue T, Rabin M. Choice and procrastination. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2001;116(1):121–160. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Clarendon Press; 1978. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- O’Tuatheigh CMP, Salum C, Young AMJ, Pickering AD, Joseph MH, Moran PM. The effect of amphetamine on Kamin blocking and overshadowing. Behavioral Pharmacology. 2003;14:315–322. doi: 10.1097/01.fbp.0000080416.18561.3e. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odum AL, Madden GJ, Bickel WK. Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never-and ex-smokers of cigarettes. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2002;4:295–303. doi: 10.1080/14622200210141257. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oei TPS, Baldwin AR. Expectancy theory: A two-process model of alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;55:525–534. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.525. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olmstead MC, Lafond MV, Everitt BJ, Dickinson A. Cocaine seeking by rats is a goal-directed action. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;115(2):394–402. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olmstead TA, Sindelar JL, Petry NM. Cost-effectiveness of prize-based incentives for stimulant abusers in outpatient psychosocial treatment programs. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;16:175–182. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.08.012. [GA] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K. Alcoholism and the brain: An overview. Alcohol Research and Health. 2003;27(2):125–134. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund S, Balleine BW. Orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome encoding in Pavlovian but not instrumental conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(18):4819–4825. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5443-06.2007. [aADR, SBO] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouellette JA, Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124:54–74. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Owen AM. Cognitive planning in humans: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neuropharmacological perspectives. Progress in Neurobiology. 1997;53(4):431–450. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(97)00042-7. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG. Glutamate infused post-training into the hippocampus or caudate-putamen differentially strengthens place and response learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 1999;96(22):12881–12886. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.22.12881. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, McGaugh JL. Double dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: Further evidence for multiple memory systems. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1992;106(3):439–446. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.3.439. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, McGaugh JL. Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1996;65:65–72. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0007. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa-Schioppa C, Assad JA. Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature. 2006;441:223–226. doi: 10.1038/nature04676. [arADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paine TA, Dringenberg HC, Olmstead MC. Effects of chronic cocaine on impulsivity: Relation to cortical serotonin mechanisms. Behavioural Brain Research. 2003;147(1–2):135–147. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(03)00156-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan WX, Schmidt R, Wickens JR, Hyland BI. Dopamine cells respond to predicted events during classical conditioning: Evidence for eligibility traces in the reward-learning network. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25(26):6235–6242. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1478-05.2005. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press; 1998. [DR] [Google Scholar]
- Paré D, Quirk GJ, Ledoux JE. New vistas on amygdala networks in conditioned fear. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92:1–9. doi: 10.1152/jn.00153.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parke J, Griffiths M. Gambling addiction and the evolution of the “near miss”. Addiction Research and Theory. 2004;12(5):407–411. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Paterson NE, Markou A. Increased motivation for self-administered cocaine after escalated cocaine intake. Neuroreport. 2003;14:2229–2232. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200312020-00019. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus MP. Decision-making dysfunctions in psychiatry – altered homeostatic processing? Science. 2007;318(5850):602–606. doi: 10.1126/science.1142997. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlides C, Winson J. Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9(8):2907–2918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-08-02907.1989. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov I. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press; 1927. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Pennartz CMA, Groenewegen HJ, Lopes da Silva FH. The nucleus accumbens as a complex of functionally distinct neuronal ensembles: An integration of behavioural, electrophysiological, and anatomical data. Progress in Neurobiology. 1994;42:719–761. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(94)90025-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennartz CMA, Lee E, Verheul J, Lipa P, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. The ventral striatum in off-line processing: Ensemble reactivation during sleep and modulation by hippocampal ripples. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(29):6446–6456. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0575-04.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peoples LL, Uzwiak AJ, Gee F, West MO. Tonic firing of rat nucleus accumbens neurons: Changes during the first two weeks of daily cocaine self-administration sessions. Brain Research. 1999;822:231–236. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)01271-2. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins KA. Reinforcing effects of nicotine as a function of smoking status. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2001;9(8):250. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.9.3.243. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins KA, Grobe JE, Weiss D, Fonte C, Caqquila A. Nicotine preference in smokers as a function of smoking abstinence. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1996;55(2):257–263. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00079-2. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry JL, Larson EB, German JP, Madden GJ, Carroll ME. Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacology. 2005;178(2–3):193–201. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1994-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petraitis J, Flay BR, Miller TQ, Torpy EJ, Greiner B. Illicit substance use among adolescents: A matrix of prospective predictors. Substance Use and Misuse. 1998;33(13):2561–2604. doi: 10.3109/10826089809059341. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM. Pathological gamblers, with and without substance abuse disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110(3):482–487. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.3.482. [CWL, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM. Pathological gambling: Etiology, comorbidity and treatment. American Psychological Association; 2005. [CWL] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Alessi SM, Carroll KM, Hanson T, MacKinnon S, Rounsaville B, Sierra S. Contingency management treatments: Reinforcing abstinence versus adherence with goal-related activities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:555–567. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.592. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Bickel WK. Polydrug abuse in heroin addicts: A behavioral economic analysis. Addiction. 1998;93(3):321–335. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.9333212.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Bickel WK, Arnett M. Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. Addiction. 1998;93(5):729–738. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.9357298.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Casarella T. Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1999;56:25–32. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00010-1. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelps EA, LeDoux JE. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron. 2005;48(2):175–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips PEM, Stuber GD, Heien MLAV, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking. Nature. 2003;422:614–618. doi: 10.1038/nature01476. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza PV, Deminiére JM, Le Moal M, Simon H. Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science. 1989;245:1511–1513. doi: 10.1126/science.2781295. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza PV, Le Moal M. Pathophysiological basis of vulnerability to drug abuse: Role of an interaction between stress, glucocorticoids, and dopaminergic neurons. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology. 1996;36:359–378. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.36.040196.002043. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picconi B, Centonze D, Håkansson K, Bernardi G, Greengard P, Fisone G, Cenci MA, Calabresi P. Loss of bidirectional striatal synaptic plasticity in L-DOPA-induced dyskinesia. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(5):501–506. doi: 10.1038/nn1040. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, Holland PC. Orbitofrontal lesions impair use of cue-outcome associations in a devaluation task. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:317–322. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.317. [SBO] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Setlow B, Gallagher M, Holland PC, Schoenbaum G. Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:11078–11084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11078.2003. [SBO] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pidoplichko VI, DeBiasi M, Williams JT, Dani JA. Nicotine activates and desensitizes midbrain dopamine neurons. Nature. 1997;390:401–404. doi: 10.1038/37120. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plassmann H, O’Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(37):9984–9988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA, Clark J, Paré-Blagoev EJ, Shohamy D, Moyano JC, Myers C, Gluck MA. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature. 2001;414:546–550. doi: 10.1038/35107080. [DTN, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA, Packard MG. Competition among multiple memory systems: Converging evidence from animal and human studies. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:245–251. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00157-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polkinghorne DE. Postmodern epistemology of practice. In: Kvale S, editor. Psychology and postmodernism. Sage; 1992. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Porrino LJ, Daunais JB, Smith HR, Nader MA. The expanding effects of cocaine: Studies in a nonhuman primate model of cocaine self-administration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004a;27(8):813–820. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.013. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porrino LJ, Lyons D, Smith HR, Daunais JB, Nader MA. Cocaine self-administration produces a progressive involvement of limbic, association, and sensorimotor striatal domains. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004b;24(14):3554–3562. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5578-03.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potegal M. The caudate nucleus egocentric localization system. Acta Neurobiological Experiments. 1972;32:479–494. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potenza MN. Should addictive disorders include non-substance-related conditions? Addiction. 2006;101(S1):142–151. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01591.x. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potenza MN, Kosten TR, Rounsaville BJ. Pathological gambling. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2001;286(2):141–144. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.2.141. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulos CX, Le AD, Parker JL. Impulsivity predicts individual susceptibility to high levels of alcohol self-administration. Behavioral Pharmacology. 1995;6(8):810–814. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuschoff K, Bossaerts P, Quartz SR. Neural differentiation of expected reward and risk in human subcortical structures. Neuron. 2006;51:381–390. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.024. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk GJ, Garcia R, González-Lima F. Prefrontal mechanisms in extinction of conditioned fear. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):337–343. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.010. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- RachBeisel J, Scott J, Dixon L. Co-occurring severe mental illness and substance use disorders: A review of recent research. Psychiatric Services. 1999;50(11):1427–1434. doi: 10.1176/ps.50.11.1427. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachlin H. The science of self-control. Harvard University Press; 2004. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Rachlin H, Green L. Commitment, choice, and self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1972;17:15–22. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1972.17-15. [CWL] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragozzino ME, Detrick S, Kesner RP. Involvement of the prelimbic-infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cortex in behavioral flexibility for place and response learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:4585–4594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04585.1999. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragozzino ME, Jih J, Tzavos A. Involvement of the dorsomedial striatum in behavioral flexibility: Role of muscarinic cholinergic receptors. Brain Research. 2002a;953(1–2):205–214. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03287-0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragozzino ME, Ragozzino KE, Mizumori SJY, Kesner RP. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in behavioral flexibility for response and visual cue discrimination learning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2002b;116:105–115. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.1.105. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus SJ, Davis JB, Donahue RJ, Discenza CB, Waite AA. Interactions between the orbitofrontal cortex and hippocampal memory system during the storage of long-term memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007;1121:216–231. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.038. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranaldi R, Bauco P, McCormick S, Cools AR, Wise RA. Equal sensitivity to cocaine reward in addiction-prone and addiction-resistant rat genotypes. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2001;12(6–7):527–534. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200111000-00014. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rand MK, Hikosaka O, Miyachi S, Lu X, Miyashita K. Characteristics of a long-term procedural skill in the monkey. Experimental Brain Research. 1998;118:293–297. doi: 10.1007/s002210050284. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rand MK, Hikosaka O, Miyachi S, Lu X, Nakamura K, Kitaguchi K, Shimo Y. Characteristics of sequential movements during early learning period in monkeys. Experimental Brain Research. 2000;131:293–304. doi: 10.1007/s002219900283. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapoport A, Wallsten TS. Individual decision behavior. Annual Review of Psychology. 1972;23:131–176. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Raskin MS, Daley DC, editors. Introduction and overview of addiction. Sage; 1991. [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- Raylu N, Oei TPS. Pathological gambling. A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2002;22(7):1009–1061. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00101-0. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reason JT. Human error. Cambridge University Press; 1990. [DTN] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD. Beyond the cognitive map: From place cells to episodic memory. MIT Press; 1999. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD. Addiction as a computational process gone awry. Science. 2004;306(5703):1944–1947. doi: 10.1126/science.1102384. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD, Jensen S, Johnson A, Kurth-Nelson Z. Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: Implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling. Psychological Review. 2007;114(3):784–805. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.784. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD, Johnson A. A computational model of craving and obsession. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007;1104(1):324–339. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.014. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD, Kurth-Nelson Z. Neural models of temporal discounting. In: Madden G, Bickel W, Critchfield T, editors. Impulsivity: Theory, science, and neuroscience of discounting. APA Books; (in press) [GA, aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD, Rosenzweig ES, Bohanick JD, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA. Dynamics of hippocampal ensemble realignment: Time vs. space. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(24):9289–9309. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09298.2000. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD, Touretzky DS. The role of the hippocampus in solving the Morris water maze. Neural Computation. 1998;10(1):73–111. doi: 10.1162/089976698300017908. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist. 1988;43(3):151–160. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.43.3.151. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. Associative relations in instrumental learning: The Eighteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1991;43:1–23. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. Transfer of instrumental control mediated by a devalued outcome. Animal Learning and Behavior. 1994;22:27–33. [SBO] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black AH, Prokesy WF, editors. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. Appleton Century Crofts; 1972. pp. 64–99. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Restle F. Discrimination of cues in mazes: A resolution of the “place-vs-response” question. Psychological Review. 1957;64:217–228. doi: 10.1037/h0040678. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Revusky S, Taukulis HK, Coombes S. Dependence of the Avfail effect on the sequence of training operations. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1980;29:430–445. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(80)92616-3. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds BR. A review of delay-discounting research with humans: Relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2006;17(8):651–667. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3280115f99. [CWL, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds BR, Ortengren A, Richards JB, de Wit H. Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;40(2):305–315. [AJG, CWL, rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JNJ, Hyland BI, Wickens JR. A cellular mechanism of reward-related learning. Nature. 2001;413:67–70. doi: 10.1038/35092560. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JNJ, Wickens JR. Dopamine-dependent plasticity of corticostriatal synapses. Neural Networks. 2002;15(4–6):507–521. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00045-x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds KD, West SG. Attributional constructs: Their role in the organization of social information in memory. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 1989;10(2):119–130. [JMB] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes JS, Gammie SC, Garland T., Jr Neurobiology of mice selected for high voluntary wheel-running activity. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2005;45(3):438–455. doi: 10.1093/icb/45.3.438. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes JS, Garland T, Jr, Gammie SC. Patterns of brain activity associated with variation in voluntary wheel-running behavior. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;117(6):1243–1256. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1243. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ME, Cragg SJ. Nicotine enhances reward-related dopamine signals in striatum. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(6):583–584. doi: 10.1038/nn1244. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rich E, Knight K. Artificial intelligence. McGraw-Hill; 1991. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Richard R, van der Pligt J, de Vries N. Anticipated affect and behavioral choice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 1996;18:111–129. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Richards JB, Sabol KE, de Wit H. Effects of methamphetamine on the adjusting amount procedure, a model of impulsive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:432–439. doi: 10.1007/pl00005488. [CWL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritz MC, Lamb RJ, Goldberg SR, Kuhar MJ. Cocaine receptors on dopamine transporters are related to self-administration of cocaine. Science. 1987;237:1219–1223. doi: 10.1126/science.2820058. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Drug addiction: Bad habits add up. Nature. 1999;398(6728):567–570. doi: 10.1038/19208. [aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. A role for mesencephalic dopamine in activation: Commentary on Berridge. Psychopharmacology. 2006;191(3):433–437. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0528-7. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts WA. Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:473–489. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.3.473. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts WA, Feeney MC, Macpherson K, Petter M, McMillan N, Musolino E. Episodic-like memory in rats: Is it based on when or how long ago? Science. 2008;320(5872):113–115. doi: 10.1126/science.1152709. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE. Neuroscience: Addicted rats. Science. 2004;305(5686):951–953. doi: 10.1126/science.1102496. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews. 1993;18(3):247–336. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p. [KCB, JS, aADR, RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction. 2000;95(S2):S91–S117. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111681. [TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Mechanisms of action of addictive stimuli: Incentive sensitization and addiction. Addiction. 2001;96:103–114. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9611038.x. [GA, DHL, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annual Reviews of Psychology. 2003;54(1):25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237. [KCB, arADR, RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Incentive-sensitization and drug “wanting” (Reply) Psychopharmacology. 2004;171:352–353. [aADR, JS] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Gorny G, Mitton E, Kolb B. Cocaine self-administration alters the morphology of dendrites and dendritic spines in the nucleus accumbens and neocortex. Synapse. 2001;39(3):257–266. doi: 10.1002/1098-2396(20010301)39:3<257::AID-SYN1007>3.0.CO;2-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Gorny G, Savage VR, Kolb B. Widespread but regionally specific effects of experimenter- versus self-administered morphine on dendritic spines in the nucleus accumbens, hippocampus, and neocortex of adult rats. Synapse. 2002;46(4):271–279. doi: 10.1002/syn.10146. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Kolb B. Alterations in the morphology of dendrites and dendritic spines in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex following repeated treatment with amphetamine or cocaine. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;11(5):1598–1604. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00576.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocha BA. Stimulant and reinforcing effects of cocaine in monoamine transporter knockout mice. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2003;479(1–3):107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2003.08.061. [TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocha BA, Fumagalli F, Gainetdinov RR, Jones SR, Ator R, Giros B, Miller GW, Caron MG. Cocaine self-administration in dopamine-transporter knockout mice. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1(2):132–137. doi: 10.1038/381. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues SM, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms underlying emotional learning and memory in the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2004;44(1):75–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.014. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roesch MR, Calu DJ, Schoenbaum G. Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1615–1624. doi: 10.1038/nn2013. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roesch MR, Takahashi Y, Gugsa N, Bissonette GB, Schoenbaum G. Previous cocaine exposure makes rats hypersensitive to both delay and reward magnitude. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(1):245–250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4080-06.2007. [TS] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roitman MF, Stuber GD, Phillips PEM, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Dopamine operates as a subsecond modulator of food seeking. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(6):1265–1271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3823-03.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosado J, Sigmon SC, Jones HE, Stitzer ML. Cash value of voucher reinforcers in pregnant drug-dependent women. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2005;13:41–47. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.1.41. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose JE, Herskovic JE, Trilling Y, Jarvik ME. Transdermal nicotine reduces cigarette craving and nicotine preference. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics. 1985;38(4):450–456. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1985.203. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen MI, Rosenheck RA, Shaner A, Eckman T, Gamache G, Krebs C. Veterans who may need a payee to prevent misuse of funds for drugs. Psychiatric Services. 2002;53(8):995–1000. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.53.8.995. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosse RB, Fay-McCarthy M, Collins JP, Risher-Flowers D, Alim TN, Deutsch SI. Transient compulsive foraging behavior associated with crack cocaine use. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150:155–156. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.1.155. [JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rumelhart DE, McClelland JL. Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. MIT Press; 1986. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MFS, Buckley MJ, Behrens TEJ, Walton ME, Bannerman DM. Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17(2):220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.001. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell MAH. The nicotine addiction trap: A 40-year sentence for four cigarettes. British Journal of Addiction. 1990;85:293–300. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb03085.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell SJ, Norvig P. Artificial intelligence: A modern approach. Prentice Hall; 2002. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Saint-Cyr JA, Taylor AE, Lang AE. Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. Brain. 1988;111:941–959. doi: 10.1093/brain/111.4.941. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saitz R. Introduction to alcohol withdrawal. Alcohol Research and Health. 1998;22(1):5–12. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakagami M, Pan X. Functional role of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in decision making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17(2):228–233. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.008. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakagami M, Pan X, Uttl B. Behavioral inhibition and prefrontal cortex in decision-making. Neural Networks. 2006;19(8):1255–1265. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.05.040. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone JD, Correa M. Motivational views of reinforcement: Implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behavioural Brain Research. 2002;137(1–2):3–25. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00282-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone JD, Correa M, Farrar A, Mingote SM. Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191(3):461–482. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0668-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone JD, Correa M, Mingote SM, Weber SM. Beyond the reward hypothesis: Alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Current Opinion Pharmacology. 2005;5(1):34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2004.09.004. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samejima K, Ueda Y, Doya K, Kimura M. Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science. 2005;310(5752):1337–1340. doi: 10.1126/science.1115270. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson HH, Cunningham CL, Czachowski CL, Chappell A, Legg B, Shannon E. Devaluation of ethanol reinforcement. Alcohol. 2004;32:203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.02.002. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanfey AG, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, Cohen JD. Neuroeconomics: Crosscurrents in research on decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10(3):108–116. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.009. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayette MA, Shiffman S, Tiffany ST, Niaura RS, Martin CS, Shadel WG. The measurement of drug craving. Addiction. 2000;95(Suppl. 2):S189–S210. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111762. [arADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL. The seven sins of memory. Houghton Mifflin; 2001. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Tulving E, editors. Memory systems 1994. MIT Press; 1994. [rADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schmetzer AD. Deinstitutionalization and dual diagnosis. Journal of Dual Diagnosis. 2006;3:95–101. [RAC] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz JM, Schneider NG, Jarvik ME. Nicotine. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 276–294. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz JM, Stotts AL, Rhoades HM, Grabowski J. Naltrexone and relapse prevention treatment for cocaine-dependent patients. Addictive Behaviors. 2001;26(2):167–180. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(00)00098-8. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitzer-Torbert NC, Redish AD. Development of path stereotypy in a single day in rats on a multiple-T maze. Archives Italiennes de Biologie. 2002;140:295–301. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitzer-Torbert NC, Redish AD. Neuronal activity in the rodent dorsal striatum in sequential navigation: Separation of spatial and reward responses on the multiple-T task. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91(5):2259–2272. doi: 10.1152/jn.00687.2003. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider J, Irons R. Assessment and treatment of addictive sexual disorders: Relevance for chemical dependency relapse. Substance Use and Misuse. 2001;36(13):1795–1820. doi: 10.1081/ja-100108428. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider W, Chein JM. Controlled & automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science. 2003;27(3):525–559. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review. 1977;84(1):1–66. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Roesch M. Orbitofrontal cortex, associative learning, and expectancies. Neuron. 2005;47(5):633–636. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.018. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Roesch M, Stalnaker TA. Orbitofrontal cortex, decision making, and drug addiction. Trends in Neurosciences. 2006a;29(2):116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.12.006. [arADR, TS] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Setlow B, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M. Encoding changes in orbitofrontal cortex in reversal-impaired aged rats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006b;95(3):1509–1517. doi: 10.1152/jn.01052.2005. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Stalnaker TA, Roesch MR. Ventral striatum fails to represent bad outcomes after cocaine exposure. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2006c Program No. 485.16. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Setlow B. Cocaine makes actions insensitive to outcomes but not extinction: Implications for altered orbitofrontal-amygdalar function. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15(8):1162–1169. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh216. [HY] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Setlow B, Ramus SJ. A systems approach to orbitofrontal cortex function: Recordings in rat orbitofrontal cortex reveal interactions with different learning systems. Behavioural Brain Research. 2003;146(1–2):19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.013. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenmakers T, Wiers RW, Jones BT, Bruce G, Jansen ATM. Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addiction. 2007;102:399–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01718.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schöne H. In: Spatial orientation. Strausfeld C, translator. Princeton University Press; 1984. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiber CA, Kahneman D. Determinants of the remembered utility of aversive sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2000;129(1):27–42. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.1.27. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuckit MA, Smith TL, Pierson J, Danko GP, Allen RC, Kreikebaum S. Patterns and correlates of drinking in offspring from the San Diego Prospective Study. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. 2007;31(10):1681–1691. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00465.x. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulteis G, Heyser CJ, Koob GF. Opiate withdrawal signs precipitated by naloxone following a single exposure to morphine: Potentiation with a second morphine exposure. Psychopharmacology. 1997;129(1):56–65. doi: 10.1007/s002130050162. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1. [arADR, JS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron. 2002;36:241–263. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00967-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Apicella P, Scarnati E, Ljungberg T. Neuronal activity in monkey ventral striatum related to the expectation of reward. Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12(12):4595–4610. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04595.1992. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Dickinson A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23(1):473–500. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweighofer N, Tanaka SC, Asahi S, Okamoto Y, Doya K, Yamawaki S. An fMRI study of the delay discounting of reward after tryptophan depletion and loading: I. Decision-making. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2004 Program Number 776.14. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Seamans JK, Gorelova N, Durstewitz D, Yang CR. Bidirectional dopamine modulation of GABAergic inhibition in prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(10):3628–3638. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-10-03628.2001. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seamans JK, Yang CR. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology. 2004;74:1–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.05.006. [aADR, DR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Searle JR. Rationality in action. MIT Press; 2001. [SHA] [Google Scholar]
- See RE, Fuchs RA, Ledford CC, McLaughlin J. Drug addiction, relapse and the amygdala. In: Shinnick-Gallagher P, Pitkanen A, Shekhar A, Cahill L, editors. The amygdala in brain function: Basic and clinical approaches. New York Academy of Sciences; 2003. pp. 294–307. (Series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 985). [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seibt B, Häfner M, Deutsch R. Prepared to eat: How immediate affective and motivational responses to food cues are influenced by food deprivation. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2007;37:359–379. [RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Self DW, Nestler EJ. Relapse to drug-seeking: Neural and molecular mechanisms. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1998;51:49–60. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00065-9. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seymour B, O’Doherty JP, Dayan P, Koltzenburg M, Jones AK, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ, Frackowlak RS. Temporal difference models describe higher-order learning in humans. Nature. 2004;429:664–667. doi: 10.1038/nature02581. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer HJ, LaPlante DA, LaBrie RA, Kidman RC, Donato AN, Stanton MV. Toward a syndrome model of addiction: Multiple expressions, common etiology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2004;12:367–374. doi: 10.1080/10673220490905705. [MLM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaham Y, Erb S, Stewart J. Stress-induced relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking in rats: A review. Brain Research Reviews. 2000;33:13–33. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(00)00024-2. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, de Wit H, Stewart J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: History, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168:3–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1224-x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shalev U, Grimm JW, Shaham Y. Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: A review. Pharmacological Reviews. 2002;54(1):1–42. doi: 10.1124/pr.54.1.1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shalev U, Highfield D, Yap J, Shaham Y. Stress and relapse to drug seeking in rats: Studies on the generality of the effect. Psychopharmacology. 2000;150(3):337–346. doi: 10.1007/s002130000441. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe L. A reformulated cognitive-behavioral model of problem gambling: A biopsychosocial perspective. Clinical Psychology Review. 2002;22(1):1–25. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(00)00087-8. [KRC, CWL, rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherwin CM. Voluntary wheel running: A review and novel interpretation. Animal Behaviour. 1998;56(1):11–27. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0836. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiffman S, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JA, Hickcox M. First lapses to smoking: Within-subjects analysis of real-time reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:66–379. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.2.366. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shippenberg TS, Chefer VI, Zapata A, Heidbreder CA. Modulation of the behavioral and neurochemical effects of psychostimulants by kappa-opioid receptor systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;937(1):50–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03558.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel S. Drug anticipation and the treatment of dependence. NIDA Research Monographs. 1988;84:1–24. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver M, Sabini J. Procrastinating. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior. 1981;11(2):207–221. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- Silvia PJ. Interest – the curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17(1):57–60. [DHL] [Google Scholar]
- Simon H. A behavioral model of rational choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1955;69:99–118. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Simons J, Carey KB. A structural analysis of attitudes toward alcohol and marijuana use. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1998;24:727–735. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Simons RL, Simons LG, Burt CH, Drummund H, Stewart E, Brody GH, Gibbons FX, Cutrona C. Supportive parenting moderates the effect of discrimination upon anger, hostile view of relationships, and violence among African American boys. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2006;47:373–389. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700405. [MTK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer M, Valentín F, Baer H, Jia Z. Why does Juan Garcia have a drinking problem: The perspective of critical medical anthropology. Medical Anthropology. 1992;14(1):77–108. doi: 10.1080/01459740.1992.9966067. [DHL] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R, O’Malley S. Craving for alcohol: Findings from the clinic and the laboratory. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 1999;34(2):223–230. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/34.2.223. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skog OJ, Melberg HO. Becker’s rational addiction theory: An empirical test with price elasticities for distilled spirits in Denmark 1911–31. Addiction. 2006;101:1444–1450. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01551.x. [RJM] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S. Behavioral decision theory. Annual review of Psychology. 1977;28:1–39. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Brandt J, Shadmehr R. Motor disorder in Huntington’s disease begins as a dysfunction in error feedback control. Nature. 2000;403:544–549. doi: 10.1038/35000576. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solnick J, Kannenberg C, Eckerman D, Waller M. An experimental analysis of impulsivity and impulse control in humans. Learning and Motivation. 1980;11:61–77. [CA] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation: II. Cigarette addiction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1973;81(2):158–171. doi: 10.1037/h0034534. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review. 1974;81(2):119–145. doi: 10.1037/h0036128. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sozou PD. On hyperbolic discounting and uncertain hazard rates. The Royal Society London B. 1998;265:2015–2020. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Spanagel R, Weiss F. The dopamine hypothesis of reward: Past and current status. Trends in Neurosciences. 1999;22(11):521–527. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01447-2. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR. Memory and brain. Oxford University Press; 1987. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR, Cohen NJ, Nadel L. The medial temporal region and memory consolidation: A new hypothesis. In: Weingartner H, Parker ES, editors. Memory consolidation: Psychobiology of cognition. Erlbaum; 1984. pp. 185–210. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Stacy AW. Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marihuana use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:61–73. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.61. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stacy AW, Wiers RW. An implicit cognition, associative memory framework for addiction. In: Munafo MR, Albero IP, editors. Cognition and addiction. Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 31–71. [RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Roesch MR, Franz TM, Burke KA, Schoenbaum G. Abnormal associative encoding in orbitofrontal neurons in cocaine-experienced rats during decision-making. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24(9):2643–2653. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05128.x. [aADR, HY] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefani MR, Moghaddam B. Rule learning and reward contingency are associated with dissociable patterns of dopamine activation in the rat prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and dorsal striatum. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(34):8810–8818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1656-06.2006. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiner H, Gerfen CR. Role of dynorphin and enkephalin in the regulation of striatal output pathways and behavior. Experimental Brain Research. 1998;123(1–2):60–76. doi: 10.1007/s002210050545. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephens DW, Krebs JR. Foraging theory. Princeton; 1987. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart RB, Li TK. The neurobiology of alcoholism in genetically selected rat models. Alcohol Research and Health. 1997;21(2):169–176. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stitzer ML, McCaul ME, Bigelow GE, Liebson IA. Oral methadone self-administration: Effects of dose and alternative reinforcers. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics. 1983;34:29–35. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1983.124. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strack F, Deutsch R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2004;3:220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuber GD, Roitman MF, Phillips PEM, Carelli RM, Wightman RM. Rapid dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens during contingent and noncontingent cocaine administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004:1–11. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300619. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuber GD, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Extinction of cocaine self-administration reveals functionally and temporally distinct dopaminergic signals in the nucleus accumbens. Neuron. 2005;46:661–669. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.036. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Heath Services Administration. Results from the 2002 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. Rockville, MD: 2003. [MLM] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Heath Services Administration. Office of Applied Studies. Rockville, MD: 2007. Results from the 2006 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. NSDUH Series H-32, DHHS Pub. No. SMA 7–4293. [CLH] [Google Scholar]
- Suddendorf T, Busby J. Mental time travel in animals? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7(9):391–396. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00187-6. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suddendorf T, Corballis MC. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2007;30:298–313. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001975. [SHA] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sulzer D, Sonders MS, Poulsen NW, Galli A. Mechanisms of neurotransmitter release by amphetamines: A review. Progress in Neurobiology. 2005;75(6):406–433. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.04.003. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sunderwirth SG, Milkman H. Behavioural and neurochemical commonalities in addiction. Contemporary Family Therapy. 1991;13:421–433. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Suri RE. TD models of reward predictive responses in dopamine neurons. Neural Networks. 2002;15:523–533. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00046-1. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suri RE, Schultz W. A neural network model with dopamine-like reinforcement signal that learns a spatial delayed response task. Neuroscience. 1999;91(3):871–890. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00697-6. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton RS, Barto AG. Reinforcement learning: An introduction. MIT Press; 1998. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson LW. Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behavior. Brain Research. 2000;886(1–2):113–164. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02905-x. [RAC, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swift R, Davidson D. Alcohol hangover: Mechanisms and mediators. Alcohol Research and Health. 1998;22(1):54–60. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sylvain C, Ladouceur R, Biosvert JM. Cognitive and behavioral treatment of pathological gambling: A controlled study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65(5):727–732. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.5.727. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka SC. Dopamine controls fundamental cognitive operations of multi-target spatial working memory. Neural Networks. 2002;15(4–6):573–582. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00050-3. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka SC. Dopaminergic control of working memory and its relevance to schizophrenia: A circuit dynamics perspective. Neuroscience. 2006;139(1):153–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.070. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S. Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nature Neuroscience. 2004a;7:887–893. doi: 10.1038/nn1279. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka SC, Schweighofer N, Asahi S, Okamoto Y, Doya K. An fMRI study of the delay discounting of reward after tryptophan depletion and loading: II. Reward-expectation. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2004b Program Number 776.17. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Tanda G, Pontieri FE, Chiara GD. Cannabinoid and heroin activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by a common µ1 opioid receptor mechanism. Science. 1997;276(5321):2048–2050. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5321.2048. [aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang C, Pawlak AP, Volodymyr P, West MO. Changes in activity of the striatum during formation of a motor habit. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25(4):909–1252. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05353.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarter RE, Ammerman RT, Ott PJ, editors. Handbook of substance abuse: Neurobehavioral pharmacology. Plenum; 1998. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Terrell F, Miller AR, Foster K, Watkins CE., Jr Racial discrimination induced anger and alcohol use among black adolescents. Adolescence. 2006;40:485–492. [MTK] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thaler RH, Shefrin HM. An economic theory of self-control. Journal of Political Economy. 1981;89:392–406. [SHA] [Google Scholar]
- Thewissen R, Havermans RC, Geschwind N, van den Hout M, Jansen A. Pavlovian conditioning of an approach bias in low-dependent smokers. Psychopharmacology. 2007;194:33–40. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0819-7. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas MJ, Beurrier C, Bonci A, Malenka R. Long-term depression in the nucleus accumbens: A neural correlate of behavioral sensitization to cocaine. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4(12):1217–1223. doi: 10.1038/nn757. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thush C, Wiers RW, Ames SL, Grenard JL, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2008;94:116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.10.019. [RWW] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffany ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review. 1990;97(2):147–168. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.147. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tindell AJ, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW. Ventral pallidal representation of pavlovian cues and reward: Population and rate codes. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(5):1058–1069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1437-03.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tindell AJ, Berridge KC, Zhang J, Peciña S, Aldridge JW. Ventral pallidal neurons code incentive motivation: Amplification by mesolimbic sensitization and amphetamine. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005a;22(10):2617–2634. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04411.x. [KCB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tindell AJ, Smith KS, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW. Ventral pallidal neurons integrate learning and physiological signals to code incentive salience of conditioned cues. Paper presented at the Society for Neuroscience conference; November 12 2005; Washington, DC. 2005b. [KCB] [Google Scholar]
- Tindell AJ, Smith KS, Pecina S, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW. Ventral pallidum firing codes hedonic reward: When a bad taste turns good. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;96(5):2399–2409. doi: 10.1152/jn.00576.2006. [KCB, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toates F. Motivational systems. Cambridge University Press; 1986. [KCB] [Google Scholar]
- Toates F. The determiners of behavior at a choice point. Psychological Review. 1938;45(1):1–41. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Toates F. Prediction of vicarious trial and error by means of the schematic sowbug. Psychological Review. 1939;46:318–336. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Toates F. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review. 1948;55:189–208. doi: 10.1037/h0061626. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolman EC, Ritchie BF, Kalish D. Studies in spatial learning. II. Place learning versus response learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1946;36:221–229. doi: 10.1037/h0060262. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomita H, Ohbayashi M, Nakahara K, Hasegawa I, Myashita Y. Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval. Nature. 1999;401:699–703. doi: 10.1038/44372. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toneatto T, Blitz-Miller T, Calderwood K, Dragonetti R, Tsanos A. Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies. 1997;13(3):253–266. doi: 10.1023/a:1024983300428. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trafimow D, Sheeran P. Some tests of the distinction between cognitive and affective beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1998;34:378–397. [MTK] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay L, Hollerman JR, Schultz W. Modifications of reward expectation-related neuronal activity during learning in primate striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:964–977. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.964. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truan F. Addiction as a social construction: A postempirical view. Journal of Psychology. 1993;127:489–499. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1993.9914886. [MDG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng KY, Lewis BL, Lipska BK, O’Donnell P. Post-pubertal disruption of medial prefrontal cortical dopamine-glutamate interactions in a developmental animal model of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62:730–738. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.012. [RAC] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981;211:453–458. doi: 10.1126/science.7455683. [ELK] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzschentke TM. Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference paradigm: A comprehensive review of drug effects, recent progress and new issues. Progress in Neurobiology. 1998;56:613–672. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00060-4. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungless MA, Magill PJ, Bolam JP. Uniform inhibition of dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area by aversive stimuli. Science. 2004;303(5666):2040–2042. doi: 10.1126/science.1093360. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valenzuela CF, Harris RA. Alcohol: Neurobiology. In: Lowinson JH, Ruiz P, Millman RB, Langrod JG, editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Williams and Wilkins; 1997. pp. 119–142. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderschuren LJMJ, Everitt BJ. Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science. 2004;305(5686):1017–1019. doi: 10.1126/science.1098975. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderschuren LJMJ, Kalivas P. Alterations in dopaminergic and glutamatergic transmission in the induction and expression of behavioral sensitization: A critical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology. 2000;151:99–120. doi: 10.1007/s002130000493. [RAC] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandrey R, Bigelow GE, Stitzer ML. Contingency management in cocaine abusers: A dose-effect comparison of goods-based versus cash-based incentives. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2007;15:338–343. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.4.338. [CLH] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Ree JM, Gerrits MAFM, Vanderschuren LJMJ. Opioids, reward and addiction: An encounter of biology, psychology, and medicine. Pharmacological Reviews. 1999;51(2):341–396. [aADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo-Garcia A, Bechara A, Recknor EC, Perez-Garcia M. Negative emotion-driven impulsivity predicts substance dependence problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;91:213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.025. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo-Garcia A, Rivas-Pérez C, López-Torrecillas F, Pérez-Garcia M. Differential impact of severity of drug use on frontal behavioral symptoms. Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31(8):1373–1382. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.11.003. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verplanken B, Aarts H, van Knippenberg A, Moonen A. Habit versus planned behaviour: A field experiment. British Journal of Social Psychology. 1998;37:111–128. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1998.tb01160.x. [DTN] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS. Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: Involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10(3):318–325. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.318. [aADR, TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. Role of dopamine in drug reinforcement and addiction in humans: Results from imaging studies. Behavioral Pharmacology. 2002;13(5):355–366. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200209000-00008. [GA, rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain: Insights from imaging studies. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2003;111(10):1444–1451. doi: 10.1172/JCI18533. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Swanson JM. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: Results from imaging studies and treatment implications. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9(6):557–569. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001507. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Li TK. Drugs and alcohol: Treating and preventing abuse, addiction and their medical consequence. Pharmacological and Therapeutics. 2005a;108(1):3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2005.06.021. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Li TK. The neuroscience of addiction. Nature Neuroscience. 2005b;8(11):1429–1430. doi: 10.1038/nn1105-1429. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, Jayne M, Ma Y, Wong C. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: Mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6583–6588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006. [RAC] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voorn P, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Groenewegen HJ, Robbins TW, Pennartz CMA. Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends in Neurosciences. 2004;27(8):468–474. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.06.006. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vuchinich R, Heather N. Choice, behavioural economics and addiction. Elsevier Science; 2003. [RJM] [Google Scholar]
- Vuchinich RE, Simpson CA. Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1998;6(3):292–305. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.6.3.292. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waelti P, Dickinson A, Schultz W. Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature. 2001;412:43–48. doi: 10.1038/35083500. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar WA. Paradoxes of gambling behavior. Erlbaum; 1988. [KRC, aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Walker MB. Irrational thinking among slot machine players. Journal of Gambling Studies. 1992a;8(3):245–261. doi: 10.1007/BF01014652. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker MB. The psychology of gambling. Pergamon Press; 1992b. [KRC] [Google Scholar]
- Walker-Andrews AS, Bahrick LE. Perceiving the real world: Infants’ detection of and memory for social information. Infancy. 2001;2(4):469–481. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0204_05. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter H, Abler B, Ciaramidaro A, Erk S. Motivating forces of human actions. Neuroimaging reward and social interaction. Brain Research Bulletin. 2005;67(5):368–381. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.016. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wanberg KW, Horn JL. Assessment of alcohol-use with multi-dimensional concepts and measures. American Psychologist. 1983;38:1055–1069. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.38.10.1055. [MDG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe M. Role of anticipated reward in cognitive behavioral control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17(2):213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.007. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson JB. Behaviorism. Norton: The People’s Institute; 1924. [GA] [Google Scholar]
- Weingardt KR, Stacy AW, Leigh BC. Automatic activation of alcohol concepts in response to positive outcomes of alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1996;20:25–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01038.x. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss F, Ciccocioppo R, Parsons LH, Katner S, Liu X, Zorrilla EP, Valdez GR, Ben-Shahar O, Angeletti S, Richter RR. Compulsive drug-seeking behavior and relapse: Neuroadaptation, stress, and conditioning factors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;937(1):1–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03556.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman BA, Zamir N. Differential effects of heroin on opioid levels in the rat brain. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1987;139(1):121–123. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(87)90506-1. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West R. Theories of addiction. Addiction. 2001;96:3–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.96131.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White AM. What happened? Alcohol, memory blackouts, and the brain. Alcohol Research and Health. 2003;27(2):186–196. [aADR] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White NM, Hiroi N. Preferential localization of self-stimulation sites in striosomes/patches in the rat striatum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 1998;95:6486–6491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.11.6486. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White NM, McDonald RJ. Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2002;77:125–184. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2001.4008. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside S, Lynam D. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:669–689. [AJG] [Google Scholar]
- Wickens J. A theory of the striatum. Pergamon; 1993. [aADR] [Google Scholar]
- Wickens JR, Reynolds JNJ, Hyland BI. Neural mechanisms of reward-related motor learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2003;13:685–690. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2003.10.013. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widyanto L, Griffiths MD. Internet addiction: Does it really exist? (Revisited) In: Gackenbach J, editor. Psychology and the Internet: Intrapersonal, interpersonal and transpersonal applications. 2nd edition. Academic Press; 2006. pp. 1141–1163. [MDG] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RCME, Sher KJ, Grenard J, Ames SL, Stacy AW. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2007;86:263–283. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021. [AJG, RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction. Sage; 2006a. [KRC, RWW] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editors. Implicit cognition and addiction. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006b;15:292–296. [KRC, RWW] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, van de Luitgaarden J, van den Wildenberg E, Smulders FTY. Challenging implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in young heavy drinkers. Addiction. 2005;100:806–819. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01064.x. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, van Woerden N, Smulders FTY, De Jong PJ. Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:648–658. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.648. [RWW] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilcox P. An ecological approach to understanding youth smoking trajectories: Problems and prospects. Addiction. 2003;98(S1):57–77. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.98.s1.5.x. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilens TE, Faraone SV, Biederman J, Gunawardene S. Does stimulant therapy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder beget later substance abuse? A meta-analytic review of the literature. Pediatrics. 2003;111:179–185. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.1.179. [CLH] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willingham DB, Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1989;15(6):1047–1060. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.15.6.1047. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Windle M, Cleary SD. Temperament and novelty seeking in adolescent substance use: Convergence of dimensions of temperament with constructs from Cloninger’s theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74(2):387–406. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.2.387. [JMB] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson DIG, Bowman EM. Rat nucleus accumbens neurons predominantly respond to the outcome-related properties of conditioned stimuli rather than their behavioral-switching properties. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94(1):49–61. doi: 10.1152/jn.01332.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HR, Cowan JD. Excitatory and inhibitory interactions in localized populations of model neurons. Biophysical Journal. 1972;12(1):1–24. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(72)86068-5. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HR, Cowan JD. A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic tissue. Kybernetik. 1973;13:55–80. doi: 10.1007/BF00288786. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–679. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windmann S, Kirsch P, Mier D, Stark R, Walter B, Gunturkun O, Vaitl D. On framing effects in decision making: Linking lateral versus medial orbitofrontal cortex activation to choice outcome processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18(7):1198–1211. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1198. [rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA. Dopamine, learning, and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:1–12. doi: 10.1038/nrn1406. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA, Bozarth MA. A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychological Review. 1987;94(4):469–492. [TS] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA, Leone P, Rivest R, Leeb K. Elevations of nucleus accumbens dopamine and DOPAC levels during intravenous heroin self-administration. Synapse. 1995;21:140–148. doi: 10.1002/syn.890210207. [arADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittman M, Paulus MP. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;12:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004. [AJG] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood W, Neal DT. A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psychological Review. 2007;114:843–863. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.843. [DTN, rADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood W, Tam L, Guerrero Witt M. Changing circumstances, disrupting habits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:918–933. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.918. [DTN] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. International classification of diseases, ICD-10. World Health Organization; 1992. [arADR] [Google Scholar]
- Wyvell CL, Berridge KC. Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the conditioned incentive salience of sucrose reward: Enhancement of reward “wanting” without enhanced “liking” or response reinforcement. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(21):8122–8130. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-21-08122.2000. [SBO, aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyvell CL, Berridge KC. Incentive-sensitization by previous amphetamine exposure: Increased cue-triggered “wanting” for sucrose reward. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(19):7831–7840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07831.2001. [KCB] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xi Z-X, Fuller SA, Stein EA. Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens during heroin self-administration is modulated by kappa opioid receptors: An in vivo fast-cyclic voltammetry study. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1998;284(1):151–161. [arADR] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yi R, Mitchell SH, Bickel WK. Delay discounting and substance abuse. In: Madden G, Bickel WK, editors. Impulsivity: Theory, science, and neuroscience of discounting. American Psychological Association; (in press) [WKB] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. Contributions of striatal subregions to place and response learning. Learning and Memory. 2004;11(4):459–463. doi: 10.1101/lm.81004. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919. [RAC, aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;19:181–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action-outcome learning in instrumental conditioning. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;22(2):505–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04219.x. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the action-outcome contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006;166(2):189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.012. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu AJ, Dayan P. Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron. 2005;46(4):681–692. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.026. [aADR] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yun IA, Wakabayashi KT, Fields HL, Nicola SM. The ventral tegmental area is required for the behavioral and nucleus accumbens neuronal firing responses to incentive cues. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(12):2923–2933. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5282-03.2004. [aADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zack M, Poulos CX. Amphetamine primes motivation to gamble and gambling-related semantic networks in problem gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(1):195–207. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300333. [TS] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang J, Tindell A, Berridge K, Aldridge J. Profile analysis of integrative coding in ventral pallidal neurons. Paper presented at the Society for Neuroscience conference; November 12, 2005; Washington, DC. 2005. [KCB] [Google Scholar]
- Zilli EA, Hasselmo ME. Modeling the role of working memory and episodic memory in behavioral tasks. Hippocampus. 2008;18(2):193–209. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20382. [arADR] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zinberg NE. Drug, set, and setting: The basis for controlled intoxicant use. Yale University Press; 1984. [MDG, JS] [Google Scholar]