WH Alley 2009
WH Alley 2009
WH Alley 2009
DOI 10.1007/s00213-008-1424-0
ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION
Abstract
Rationale Imaginative suggestibility, a trait closely related
to hypnotic suggestibility, is modifiable under some
circumstances. Nitrous oxide (laughing gas) is commonly
used for sedation in dentistry and is reported to be more
effective when combined with appropriate suggestions.
Objective The aim of this study was to determine whether
nitrous oxide inhalation alters imaginative suggestibility
and imagery vividness.
Methods Thirty participants were tested twice in a withinsubjects design, once during inhalation of 25% nitrous
oxide and once during inhalation of air plus oxygen. Before
the study, participants expectancies regarding the effects of
nitrous oxide were assessed. Participants were blinded to
drug administration. During each session, participants were
verbally administered detailed measures of imagination and
suggestibility: the SheehanBetts Quality of Mental Imagery scale and the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale
Form C, minus the hypnotic induction.
Results Imaginative suggestibility and imaginative ability
(imagery vividness) were both elevated in the nitrous oxide
condition. This effect was unrelated to participants expectations regarding the effects of the drug.
Conclusions Nitrous oxide increased imaginative suggestibility and imaginative ability. Possible explanations of
these findings are discussed with respect to the effects of Nmethyl-d-aspartate antagonists and to other pharmacological effects upon suggestibility and imagination.
Introduction
Nitrous oxide (laughing gas) inhalation is a form of
conscious sedation and is an analgesic commonly used in
dentistry and also in obstetrics. It has long been noted by
dental practitioners that patients under nitrous oxide
sedation are particularly suggestible and a number of
investigators have noted the clinical advantages of using a
hypnotic voice when administering nitrous oxide (Lippe
1944; Seladin 1947). Bingham (1964) describes a case of
rapid hypnosis by using nitrous oxide and Allen (1972)
notes that during nitrous oxide sedation patients respond
well to suggestions given in a quiet, hypnotic manner.
Hilgard and Hilgard (1975) and Eysenck and Rees (1945)
also informally note that sub-anaesthetic doses of nitrous
oxide will heighten the hypnotic responsiveness of the
patient. If there is a synergistic relationship between nitrous
oxide inhalation and responsiveness to suggestions, then
strategic use of appropriate suggestions for relaxation and
analgesia should enhance the overall clinical effectiveness
of nitrous oxide sedation procedures (Simons et al. 2007).
Suggestibility can also be assessed in the absence of
hypnosis and is termed imaginative suggestibility (Kirsch
and Braffman 2001). Hypnotic inductions are commonly
used to modestly increase suggestibility (Kirsch and
Braffman 2001), and suggestibility can also be modified
by either changing peoples expectations (Vickery and
Kirsch 1991), labeling a situation as hypnotic (Gandhi
and Oakley 2005) or through training (Gorassini and
Spanos 1989). There is disagreement, however, about the
Psychopharmacology
Measures
All participants completed the SheehanBetts Quality of
Mental Imagery Scale (QMI; Sheehan 1967) and the
suggestions from the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility
Scale Form C (SHSS:C; minus the hypnotic induction;
Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard 1962). The QMI is a 35-item
test assessing the vividness of different modalities of mental
imagery (e.g. auditory, olfactory, tactile). It is a shortened
version of Betts (1909) original 150-item questionnaire.
The scale was modified by the authors for verbal
administration, but participants rated each item using the
original seven-point scale (1 = as clear and vivid as the real
thing; 7 = no image present at all, you only know you are
thinking of an object). Testretest reliability for the
SheehanBetts QMI has been found to be 0.75 for females
and 0.72 for males after 2 weeks (Westcott and Rosenstock
1976). Factor analyses of the questionnaire items have
revealed the presence of a large unitary factor corresponding to general vividness of imagery and modality
specific factors (Sheehan 1967; Wagman and Stewart 1974;
White et al. 1974).
The SHSS:C (Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard 1962) is a 12item test, individually administered according to a standardised procedure. The researcher administering the scale
objectively assesses participants responses to each item.
Suggestions on the test are progressively more difficult,
enabling the researcher to terminate the test after the failure
of three consecutive items. The SHSS:C was chosen for a
number of reasons. Firstly, it contains proportionately more
difficult items than other scales (Perry et al. 1992; Bertrand
1989), which is important to minimise any potential ceiling
effects. Secondly, it contains a high proportion of cognitive
items which we theorised might be less affected by
lethargic feelings brought about by nitrous oxide sedation.
As well as removing the hypnotic induction, we made a
number of other modifications to the SHSS:C in order to
increase its suitability for administering to participants
inhaling nitrous oxide. For the age regression item, the
American reference to school grades was omitted and
replaced with the terms junior school and infant school.
The anosmia to ammonia item was omitted to avoid having
to remove nose mask used for administration of the nitrous
oxide. A test of post-hypnotic suggestion modified from the
Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale Form A (SHSS:A;
Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard 1959) was added to replace the
anosmia item. Post-hypnotic amnesia was assessed by joint
criteria considering both initial amnesia and subsequent
reversibility. Amnesia was scored as present only if the
participants recalled both three or fewer critical items
initially and two or more additional items following the
reversal cue. The instructions for the reversibility test ask
subjects to report all items they remember. These mod-
Psychopharmacology
possible, then to open their eyes and rate the item using the
scale. After the final item of the QMI, participants were
asked to open their eyes and the investigator checked that
they were feeling alright.
In the present investigation, the hypnotic induction that
normally precedes the suggestions on the SHSS:C was
omitted, making it a test of imaginative suggestibility
(Kirsch and Braffman 2001). Administration of suggestions
was conducted according to the manual. During the count
down to normal alertness at the end of the SHSS:C, which
was timed to last for 2 min, 100% oxygen was administered. This was to prevent diffusion hypoxia during
emergence since the inhaled air (21% oxygen) is diluted
by the rapid excretion of nitrous oxide, nitrogen being
absorbed only slowly (Cass and Cass 1994).
The QMI was always delivered first because of the
relative complexity of the SHSS:C. During the alerting
phase of the final item of the SHSS:C, the delivery of
nitrous oxide or oxygen was stopped and the participant
was questioned about their memory for items in the test,
followed by the SHSS:C post-experimental interview.
Counterbalanced administration of the tests would have
required restarting delivery of nitrous oxide after this
alerting phase, and it was considered more feasible to
deliver the tests in a single order with nitrous oxide delivery
uninterrupted. At the end of the second session, participants
were asked during which session they thought had received
the nitrous oxide.
Results
Four patients dropped out after completing one session;
they were replaced and their data were not used in the
analysis. Imaginative ability measured by the Sheehan
Betts QMI was greater in the nitrous oxide condition
(85.83, SD = 37.63) than the oxygen alone condition
(111.63, SD=37.77; lower scores on the QMI indicate
higher imaginative ability). Imaginative suggestibility measured by the SHSS:C was greater in the nitrous oxide
condition (7.33, SD=2.80) than the oxygen alone condition
(6.16, SD=2.47).
To test for potential order effects, we included order of
drug administrations as a factor in our analysis (group 1:
first session = drug, second session = no drug; group 2: first
session = no drug, second session = drug). Groups did not
differ with respect to age (t(28)=0.253, p=0.802). A
mixed-model analysis of variance with drug as a withinsubjects factor (nitrous oxide vs. oxygen) and group as a
between-subject factor was conducted for scores on the
SHSS:C and the QMI. For scores on the SHSS:C, there was
no main effect of group (F(1,28)=0.483, p=0.493) and no
drug group interaction (F(1,28)=1.128, p=0.297). For
Psychopharmacology
Discussion
This investigation demonstrates, for the first time with a
standardised test, that inhalation of 25% nitrous oxide
produces objectively assessed increases in imaginative
suggestibility. Nitrous oxide also increased imaginative
Table 1 Percentage pass rates and Z and p values (Wilcoxon-related samples) for items of the SHSS:C when participants were inhaling either
nitrous oxide or oxygen
SHSS:C item
Item class
Auditory hallucination
Moving hands apart
Post-hypnotic suggestion
Hand lowering
Dream
Taste hallucination
Arm rigidity
Post-hypnotic amnesia
Negative visual hallucination
Age regression
Mosquito hallucination
Arm immobilisation
Cognitive (production)
Motor (inhibition)
Cognitive
Motor (production)
Cognitive
Cognitive
Challenge
Cognitive
Cognitive (inhibition)
Cognitive
Cognitive
Motor (inhibition)
Percentage pass
oxygen
Percentage
pass N20
Difference
(N20oxygen)
6.6
96.6
30
90
56.6
43.3
63.3
40
10
56.6
63.3
53.3
3.3
96.6
33.3
93.3
63.3
50
70
53.3
26.6
73.3
80
80
-3.3
0
3.3
3.3
6.7
6.7
6.7
13.3
16.6
16.7
16.7
26.7
0.277
1
0.333
1
0.577
0.707
1.414
1.155
2.236
2.236
1.89
2.53
p (1-tailed)
0.282
0.5
0.3695
0.1585
0.282
0.24
0.0785
0.124
0.0125*
0.0125*
0.0295*
0.0055**
Psychopharmacology
4
3.5
Nitrous Oxide
Oxygen
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Visual
Auditory
Touch
Movement
Taste
Smell
Sensation
0.54
36
27.5
SSS:A/B Stanford Suggestibility Scale Form A/B, SHSS:A/B Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale Form A/B, HGSHS Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility
a
Weitzenhoffer and Sjoberg (1961)
b
Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard (1959)
c
Shor and Orne (1962)
0.06
1.8
1.1
2.56
3.3
2.4
2.5
1.5
1.3
No
No
No
Control (no drug)
Nitrous oxide
Nitrous oxide
Barber et al (1979)
Kelly et al (1978)
Gibson et al (1977)
18
20
20
HGSHSc
Purpose built
Purpose built (excluding
analgesia)
11
5
4
22.6
2.49
4.18
1.69
No
17
HGSHSc
11
12.7
12.8
0.66
11.75
0.91
0.41
2.16
1.54
0.08
1.41
0.11
0.05
5.5
5.16
3.54
4.75
4.55
5.29
3.33
3.62
3.45
3.33
4.44
5.24
No
No
No
No
Yes
Yes
Mescaline
LSD-25
Psilocybin
Combination
Diazepam
Placebo (nicotinic
acid)
Cannabis
Sjoberg and Hollister
(1965)
5 mg/kg
1.5 mcg/kg
225 mcg/kg
1/3 of each of above
5 mg
50 mg
24
24
24
24
34
37
SSS:A/Ba
SSS:A/Ba
SSS:A/Ba
SSS:A/Ba
SHSS:A/Bb
SHSS:A/Bb
17
17
17
17
12
12
Absolute
Nondrug
Drug
Suggestibility change
Suggestibility
No.
items
Hypnotic
induction?
Suggestibility scale
Number
Dose
Drug
Study
Percentage
Psychopharmacology
Psychopharmacology
References
Allen WA (1972) Relative analgesia: an introductory note. Br Dent J
133:2526
Barber J, Donaldson D, Ramras S, Allen GD (1979) The relationship
between nitrous oxide conscious sedation and the hypnotic state.
J Am Dent Assoc 99(4):624626
Benham G, Bowers S, Nash M, Muenchen R (1998) Self-fulfilling
prophecy and hypnotic response are not the same thing. J Pers
Soc Psychol 75:16041613. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1604
Bertrand LD (1989) The assessment and modification of hypnotic
susceptibility. In: Spanos NP, Chaves JF (eds) Hypnosis: the
cognitivebehavioral perspective. Prometheus Books, Buffalo
(NY), pp 1831
Betts GH (1909) The distribution and functions of mental imagery.
Teachers College, Columbia University, New York
Bingham GD (1964) Rapid hypnosis by using nitrous oxide. Am J
Clin Hypn 7:226228
Brown RJ, Oakley DA (2004) An integrative cognitive theory of
hypnosis and high hypnotisability. In: Heap M, Brown RJ,
Oakley DA (eds) The highly hypnotizable person. Routledge,
London, pp 152186
Cass N, Cass L (1994) Pharmacology for anaesthetists. Churchill
Livingstone, Edinburgh
Crawford HJ (1982) Hypnotizability, daydreaming styles, imagery
vividness, and absorption: a multidimensional study. J Pers Soc
Psychol 42:915926
de Groh M (1989) Correlates of hypnotic susceptibility. In: Spanos
NP, Chaves JF (eds) Hypnosis: the cognitivebehavioural
perspective. Prometheus, Buffalo, NY, pp 3263
Derbyshire SWG, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA (2004)
Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined
pain. Neuroimage 23:392401. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2004.04.033
Psychopharmacology
Dienes Z, Perner J (2007) Executive control without conscious
awareness: the cold control theory of hypnosis. In: Jamieson
GA (ed) Hypnosis and conscious states. Oxford University Press,
Oxford
Eysenck HJ, Rees WL (1945) States of heightened suggestibility:
narcosis. J Ment Sci 91:301310
Faymonville ME, Mambourg PH, Joris J, Vrijens B, Fissette J, Albert
A, Lamy M (1997) Psychological approaches during conscious
sedation. Hypnosis versus stress reducing strategies: a prospective randomized study. Pain 73:361367. doi:10.1016/S03043959(97)00122-X
Franks NP, Lieb WR (1998) A serious target for laughing gas. Nat
Med 4(4):383384. doi:10.1038/nm0498-383
Gandhi B, Oakley DA (2005) Does hypnosis by any other name
smell as sweet? The efficacy of hypnotic inductions depends on
the label hypnosis. Conscious Cogn 14:304315. doi:10.1016/j.
concog.2004.12.004
Gibson HB, Corcoran ME, Curran JD (1977) Hypnotic susceptibility
and personality: the consequences of diazepam and the sex of the
subjects. Br J Psychol 68:5159
Glisky ML, Tataryn DJ, Kihlstrom JF (1995) Hypnotizability and
mental imagery. Int J Clin Exp Hypn 43:3454
Gorasini DR (2004) Enhancing hypotisability. In: Heap M, Brown RJ,
Oakley DA (eds) The highly hypnotizable person. Routledge,
London, pp 213239
Gorassini DR, Spanos NP (1989) The Carleton Skill Training
Programme for modifying hypnotic suggestibility: original
version and variations. In: Kirsch I, Capafons A, Amig S,
Cardea-Buelna E (eds) Clinical hypnosis and self-regulation
therapy: a cognitivebehavioural perspective. American Psychological Association Books, Washington, DC, pp 14177
Hilgard ER (1986) Divided consciousness: multiple controls in human
thought and action (expanded edition). Wiley, New York
Hilgard ER, Hilgard JR (1975) Hypnosis in the relief of pain.
Brunner/Mazel, Levittown, PA
Holmes EA, Matthews A (2005) Mental imagery and emotion: a
special relationship? Emotion 5:489497. doi:10.1037/15283542.5.4.489
Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Todorovic SM, Mennerick S, Powell S, Dikranian
K, Benshoff N, Zorumski CF, Olney JW (1998) Nitrous oxide
(laughing gas) is an NMDA antagonist, neuroprotectant and
neurotoxin. Nat Med 4:460463. doi:10.1038/nm0498-460
Kelly SF, Fisher S, Kelly RJ (1978) Effects of cannabis intoxication
on primary suggestibility. Psychopharmacology 56:217219
Kihlstrom JF (2007) Modified version of the SHSS:C. Retrieved
September 30th, 2008, from http://socrates.berkeley.edu/
~kihlstrm/PDFfiles/Hypnotizability/SHSSC%20Script.pdf
Kirsch I (1985) Response expectancy as a determinant of experience
and behaviour. Am Psychol 40:11891202
Kirsch I, Braffman W (2001) Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 10(2):5761. doi:10.1111/14678721.00115
Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Constantini-Ferrando MF, Alpert NM,
Speigel D (2000) Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing
in the brain. Am J Psychiatr 157:12791284. doi:10.1176/appi.
ajp.157.8.1279
Kosslyn SM, Ganis G, Thompson WL (2001) Neural foundations of
imagery. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:635642
Lippe HT (1944) Nitrous oxide analgesia in cavity preparation.
Temple Dental Review 14:7