(Richard Colwell) MENC Handbook of Musical Cognition
(Richard Colwell) MENC Handbook of Musical Cognition
(Richard Colwell) MENC Handbook of Musical Cognition
Richard Colwell,
Editor
Edited by
Richard Colwell
1
2006
1
Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further
Oxford Universitys objective of excellence
in research, scholarship, and education.
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Printed in the United States of America
on acid-free paper
Preface
The use of research findings is critical to the success of music teaching and
learning. There is probably no area of greater importance in the 21st century
than research involving music and its impact on the development and use
by the human brain. The popular press has reported partial findings of re-
search indicating the importance of music in human development. It is im-
portant that teachers and researchers have a full understanding of the find-
ings of valid studies in order that our knowledge not be misused. Andreas
Lehmann, a German scholar, assembled an outstanding team that wrote
chapters on our knowledge of perception and cognition for the New Hand-
book of Research on Music Teaching and Learning published by Oxford
University Press in 2002. In this text, we provide an up-date of that material
with the addition of a chapter on Music and Neuroscience by John W. Flohr
and Donald A. Hodges.
This up-date is the result of the efforts of Ms. Kim Robinson and Ms.
Eve Bachrach of Oxford University Press and Mike Blakeslee of the National
Association for Music EducationMENC, believers in the importance of
disseminating research findings to the profession. It is our hope that the
publication of separate, small, economical, books on specialized research
topics will make the material more accessible to users in a variety of fields.
Music cognition is a vital topic for scholars in medicine, psychology, in
educational psychology, and in music theory, as well as for music educators.
One will note that we have selected authors from all of these fields and
authors from continental Europe, Great Britain, Canada, and the United
States. It has been my pleasure to work through with them with the assis-
tance of Professor Lehmann issues of language, definitions, and concepts to
make the material clear to not only English speakers but those who use
English as a research tool. Accurate definitions apply to the topic of each
chapter although the authors have coordinated their writing to avoid dupli-
cation and to cover the material of music perception and cognition as suc-
cinctly as possible in only seven chapters. Although the chapters stand alone
vi PREFACE
Contributors ix
3 Listening to Music 72
William Forde Thompson and E. Glenn Schellenberg
Index 259
This page intentionally left blank
Contributors
john w. flohr received his doctorate in music education from the University
of Illinois-Urbana. He has performed as a professional musician and taught at
the college, public school, and preschool level. He is professor of music at Texas
Womans UniversityDenton, serves as current president of the Texas Music Ed-
ucators Conference, and recently was faculty fellow at the national Arts Eduction
Partnership. He specializes in early childhood music education and has authored
research papers, books, videos, audio recordings, and computer programs in
music and music education. His most recent book is Musical Lives of Young
Children.
heiner gembris holds a degree in music education from the Academy of Music
Detmold and in musicology from the Technische Universitat Berlin. He worked
as a music teacher in secondary public schools for several years. In 1985, he
earned his doctorate with a study on music listening and relaxation. After an
employment at the University of Augsburg, he was appointed professor for sys-
tematic musicology at the University of Munster (1991), and later at the Uni-
versity of Halle/Wittenberg (1998). Since 2001 he has been head of music edu-
ix
x CONTRIBUTORS
cation and Music Psychology at the University of Paderborn and has directed
the Institute for Research on Musical Ability (IBFM). Heiner Gembris has oc-
cupied numerous leading positions in scientific organisations and is member of
the scientific board of national and international journals. His main research
interests concern the effects of music listening, music preferences, and the nature
and development of music abilities across the lifespan. In 1998, he published a
textbook in German on musical talent and development (2nd edition 2002).
reinhard kopiez has been professor of music psychology at the School of Mu-
sic and Drama in Hannover, Germany since 1998. He earned his PhD in 1990
from the Technical University of Berlin. After appointments as assistant professor
for musicology at the Technical University of Berlin and later for systematic
musicology at the school of music in Wuerzburg. Since 1998 he has been vice
president of the German Society for Music Psychology (DGM) and since 2002
its president. He is also a member of the executive council of the European
Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Kopiez has written several
books and numerous chapters and articles for national and international
publications. His main areas of research interest are performance research,
rhythm perception, and music in everyday-life. Some recent publications include:
(1998) Fussball-Fangesaenge [Singing at soccer games], Wuerzburg: Koenigshau-
sen; (2003a) Intonation of harmonic intervals: adaptibility of expert musicians
to equal temperament and just intonation, Music Perception, 20(4), 383410;
(2003b) Tempo and loudness analysis of a continuous 28-hour performance of
Erik Saties composition Vexations, Journal of New Music Research, 32(3),
243258; (2003c) Stability of motor programs during a state of meditation:
electrocortical activity in a pianist playing Vexations by Erik Satie continuously
for 28 hours, Psychology of Music, 31(2), 173186.
william forde thompson is director of the CCIT and a professor in the De-
partment of Psychology at the University of Toronto at Mississauga. He is also
an associate member of the Unit for the Study of Musical Skill and Development.
After receiving his doctorate in psychology from Queens University, he began
an exemplary career in music perception and cognition. He is an accomplished
composer, writing music for the theater that has been staged throughout Canada
and on CBS Radio. He is an officer in the Society for Music Perception and
Cognition and heads Experiment Creator, which tests and teaches concepts re-
lated to audio-visual content. His research interests are in decoding speech pros-
ody, acoustic cues and speech, visual influences on perception, and recognition
xii CONTRIBUTORS
of emotion in cross cultural situations. His most recent book is Music Thought
and Feeling: The Psychology of Music, Oxford University Press, 2006.
andreas c. lehmann
and feelings enter into the perception and cognition game? As a result, and
in order to be of maximal use for music educators, here we did not adhere
to a strong division of perception and cognition but rather understood cog-
nition in its broader sense, namely, how it applies in the context of experi-
ence, training, development, and culture.
This book with its seven chapters does not purport to cover the whole
range of topics relevant to music perception and cognition. We have tried
to capitalize on emergent issues and research done since the publication of
the first Handbook (Colwell, 1992). In our experience, music educators as
a group are likely to look at the research presented in the following 7 chap-
ters. We should keep in mind, however, there is probably not the music
educator, as there is not the music psychologist. Any choice of issues dis-
cussed, references cited, or references omitted (due to space limitations) will
have to be the result of subjective decisions. A review of the literature is
never objective, because the writer has an agenda, which is to introduce a
personal view on a topic. With the help of the many reviewers who made
thoughtful suggestions to improve the draft chapters, the printed chapters
should now match the needs of aspiring or in-service music educators and
music education researchers.
The chapters follow a certain logic in that the first five chapters proceed
from the basic neurological and cognitive processes to a panoramic view of
musical development and the theories behind research on learning. The last
two chapters concentrate on music performance skills, musical expression,
and the audience. A chapter that was not originally part of the Musical
Cognition and Development part of the 2002 Handbook is the one by Flohr
and Hodges on Music and Neuroscience. Here the authors circumscribe the
research methods and results currently available in the area of neuroscience.
It is obvious that we are starting to better understand how music is processed
in the brain, and how nature and nurture interact. This chapter in fact pro-
vides the necessary backdrop to some of the others presented in this volume.
Wilfried Gruhn and Frances Rauscher introduce neuropsychological and
neurophysiological research as it relates to learning. They also introduce
some learning theories that essentially can be viewed as theories of cognition,
and they clarify one hotly debated topic in music education, namely, the
question of transfer of learning, which sometimes serves as a justification for
music education in schools. William Forde Thompson and E. Glenn Schel-
lenberg are experts in basic music cognition with all its developmental and
cultural implications. Their chapter abounds with pointers to current re-
search methodology and brings to our attention the processes in music per-
ception we often take for granted (e.g., melody perception, timbre, rhythm).
Heiner Gembris gives a more panoramic survey of topics and issues in de-
velopmental psychology. While emphasizing cognitive aspects, he also in-
corporates sociological and cultural aspects. This chapter is closely tailored
to the questions frequently asked by music educators. Bruce Torff comes
from the general area of educational psychology. His chapter places research
on music perception and cognition into the larger context of research on
INTRODUCTION 5
REFERENCES
Levitin, D. (1999). [Review of the book The psychology of music (2nd ed.),
edited by Diana Deutsch]. Music Perception, 16, 495505.
Colwell, R., & Richardson, C. (Eds.) (2002). The new handbook of research on
music teaching and learning. New York: Oxford University Press.
Davidson, J. W. (Ed.) 2004. The music practitioner: Research for the music per-
former, teacher and listener. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.
This page intentionally left blank
Music and Neuroscience
1
john w. flohr
donald a. hodges
Relationships among the brain, music, and musical abilities are of interest
to musicians, psychologists, and neuroscientists. The purpose of this chapter
is to provide a detailed and critical overview of the neuroscientific research
dealing with music and music education. Unfortunately, a direct translation
from neuroscience research into music education at this time is very prob-
lematic. Although our understanding of brain and behavior has increased at
an exponential rate over the past 10 years, theories of brain functioning and
our understanding of the neurobiological forces that shape musical behavior
are still in their infancy. The focus will be on ideas helpful to music education
from what is known about music and the brain. The chapter is organized
into three sections.
How does one go about studying the phenomenon of music in the brain?
The brains immense complexity, in combination with the subtleties and in-
7
8 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Animal Research
Although studying animals may seem like an unusual place to begin, this
has been a standard approach in psychology (e.g., Pavlovs dog) and contin-
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 9
soon after that can pick out the sounds of the mothers voice (Trehub, Schel-
lenberg, & Hill, 1997; Trehub & Trainor, 1993). A significant amount of
the interactions between a newborn and its caretakers is based on two-way
sound manipulations. The caretakers sing lullabies and talk baby talk,
and there are musical crib mobiles and toys. Motherese, a term psychol-
ogists have coined to refer to the type of baby talk typically spoken to
infants, emphasizes pitch, timbre, dynamic inflections, and rhythm patterns
in order to convey meaning (Dissayanake, 2000). Clearly, the baby cannot
interpret the meaning of words but does learn to interpret the emotional
content. Likewise, the baby learns early on to communicate by manipulating
the same sonic elements to express mood states such as hunger, pain, fear,
happiness, love, and so on. From this line of research, it is clear that infant
musical behaviors are exhibited primarily because of inherited mechanisms
(Imberty, 2000; Trehub, 2004). While learning takes place from the outset,
babies do not need systematic, formal instruction in order to respond to
music, speech, and other sounds.
the left hemisphere, and vice versa (Kimura, 1961; Robinson & Solomon,
1974). Contralateral response times are faster than ipsilateral (from one in-
ner ear to the auditory cortex on the same side) (Majkowski, Bochenek,
Bochenek, Knapik-Fijalkowska, & Kopec, 1971) and can occlude impulses
arriving along ipsilateral pathways (Kimura, 1967). In a typical experiment,
subjects might hear a nonsense syllable such as bleh in the right ear and
teh in the left ear (stimuli must be nonsensical, as real words would be
immediately recognized). They are then presented with four foils and asked
to identify the one they heard. Over a number of trials, consistency in pick-
ing out the right ear signal would indicate processing dominance by the left
hemisphere and vice versa.
A major limitation of this technique for music research is that the stimuli
can be no longer than 2 seconds. If they are longer, the brain can go back
and forth, picking up enough information from each ear to make recognition
of both stimuli possible. Obviously, only limited musical meaning can be
found in 2-second fragments. Also, stimulus variables (the type of sound/
music being used), task variables (whether subjects are asked to make global
or local decisions), and subject variables (amount of training, gender, hand-
edness, etc.) can make dramatic differences in the results. Sergent was highly
critical of this line of research and felt that all such data could be discarded
(Sergent, 1993; for an extended review of this literature and discussion, see
Hodges [1996b, pp. 222232]).
For a period of time, primarily during the 1970s, much was made of
music being in the right side of the brain. This oversimplification has since
been modified. Music is not in the right side of the brain alone; both sides
are involved. In fact, sophisticated musical processing most likely involves
the front-back, top-bottom, and left and right sides of the brain in widely
distributed but locally specialized neural networks. Furthermore, selectively
changing the focus of attention radically alters brain activation patterns (Pla-
tel et al., 1997). Further implications of this line of research are discussed
subsequently.
Reaction Time
3
Brain
2 ERP. EEG
PET
Map 1
Log Size (mm)
Column 0
Functional MRI
Layer -1
Dendrite -3
Synapse
-4
Electron Microscopy
-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Millisecond Second Minute Hour Day
Log Time (sec)
Figure 1.1. Schematic illustration of the spatial (vertical axis) and temporal (horizontal axis)
resolution of techniques used to study brain function in humans. The hazy edges of each
box suggest that these are approximations and that these techniques may not be limited to
these boundaries in the future. This figure emphasizes the different spatial and temporal
characteristics of methods used to study the neurobiology of behaviors, none of which alone
is sufficient to understand the neural basis of a behavior or its development. (From Janowsky
& Carper in Sameroff & Haith, eds., 1996.)
EEG and ERP. Due to neural activity, the brain constantly produces a small
amount of electrical current that can be measured; EEG measures the
summed activity of millions of brain cells under electrodes placed in various
places around the skull (Gur & Gur, 1994). Data are interpreted in terms
of frequency (Hz), amplitude (microvolts), form, and distribution (some-
times converted into a brain map). Most often reported are frequency com-
ponents, including delta (0.54.0 Hz), theta (4.58.0 Hz), alpha (8.512.0
Hz), and beta waves (12.532.0 Hz). Electroencephalography has been used
for some time to study different levels of arousal and is now being employed
to study cognitive processes in general and music processing specifically (Al-
tenmuller, 1993; Barber, 1999; Faita & Besson, 1994).
While EEG tracks the brains electrical activity over time, ERP examines
the brains immediate response to a stimulus in millisecond (ms) intervals.
A computer averages EEG readings following multiple presentations of a
stimulus. This allows extraneous aspects of the EEG to be canceled out,
while electrical activity occurring in time-locked response to the stimulus is
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 15
revealed (Brown, Marsh, & Ponsford, 1985). Data are evaluated for direc-
tional changes in the wave pattern (either positive or negative), intensity level
(amplitude), and latency (time lapse). A P300 wave indicates a positive wave
whose maximum amplitude occurred approximately 300 ms after the stim-
ulus, while a N400 wave is a negative wave form occurring approximately
400 ms after the stimulus. The P300 has been more frequently studied in
relation to music. It is hypothesized to be an indicator of working mem-
ory, comparing incoming stimuli to stored memories, and has been linked
to the detection of musical events (Cohen & Erez, 1991; Frisina, Walton, &
Crummer, 1988; Hantz & Crummer, 1988; Paller, McCarthy, & Wood,
1992; Paulus, 1988; Schwent, Snyder, & Hillyard, 1976).
MEG and SQUID. The brains electrical activity produces a magnetic field
that can be measured just outside the skull through MEG, giving location
information about neural activity. SQUID provides refined spatial informa-
tion, in millimeters, and precise temporal resolution, in milliseconds (Hari,
1990). This approach has been used in only a few studies related to music
(Kaufmann, Curtis, Wang, & Williamson, 1991; Williamson & Kaufmann,
1988).
MRI and fMRI. MRI provides very precise information about anatomical
structures under the skin but does not provide information about function
(Ackerman, 1992). It has been used to show structural features of musicians
brains (Amunts et al., 1997; Pantev et al., 1998; Schlaug, Jancke, Huang,
& Steinmetz, 1994, 1995). A newer development, fMRI does provide data
about both location and function. Currently, there is considerable interest
in finding a way to use fMRI to study music. The difficulty is that both MRI
and fMRI are very noisy environments for the subjects. The cameras motion
within the scanner generates rhythmical noise that competes with musical
perception. While it is possible to extract speech or other nonmusical sounds
from the ambient noise, it is not so easily done with music. Regular head-
phones cannot be used, because of the strong magnet. Researchers are at-
tempting to deliver musical stimuli through pneumatic tubes or to develop
better antinoise cancellation devices. A recently developed technique causes
the cameras to pause for up to 10 seconds, which allows stimuli to be pre-
sented during the resulting silent period; staggering the camera start time
allows for full acquisition of the hemodynamic response. If these problems
are solved, fMRI should prove to be a very valuable approach for studying
music cognition.
(i.e., less active than during rest) also provide useful information. Because
PET provides information about function but not location, it is mapped onto
MRI data. The combination of the two tells neuroscientists what is going
on where. PET is a powerful technique that is revealing important infor-
mation about music processing (Fox et al., 1995; Parsons, Fox, & Hodges,
1998; Zatorre, 1994; Zatorre, Evans, & Myer, 1994).
Taken collectively, these various brain imaging techniques are opening up
new understandings about the brain in general and about music cognition spe-
cifically. The most rapid advancements are being made in this field, and music
psychologists, music educators, and music therapists should be aware of find-
ings as they are reported (see Avanzini et al., 2003, Avanzini, Lopez, Koelsch,
& Majno, 2005, and Zatorre & Peretz, 2001, for recent compilations).
Neuromotor Research
Musical responses are both expressive (i.e., performing) and receptive (i.e.,
listening). Musical performance activates motor control areas in the brain
to such a high degree that musicians may be considered small-muscle athletes
(Wilson, 1986). A PET study of eight professional pianists confirmed this as
motor systems in the brain were strongly activated during performance (Fox
et al., 1995; Parsons, 2001). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), a
technique for mapping neuromotor pathways, was used with 15 subjects to
show that the motor cortex controlling the fingers increased in response to
piano exercises, both actual and imagined (Pascual-Leone et al., 1995). Re-
searchers used magnetic source imaging to compare 9 string players with 6
nonmusicians; the main finding was that the string players had greater neu-
ronal activity and a larger area in the right primary somatosensory cortex
that controls the fingers of the left hand than controls (Elbert, Pantev, Wien-
bruch, Rockstrub, & Taub, 1995). These effects were greater for those who
started playing at a young age.
Highly precise and rhythmically coordinated movements are critical for
musical performance, and investigators are beginning to identify timing
mechanisms in the brain (Freund & Hefter, 1990; Miller, Thaut, & Aunon,
1995; Moore, 1992; Wilson, 1991, 1998). A related issue is focal dystonia,
a neuromotor problem in which the brain and hands (or other body parts,
such as mouth structures used in embouchure formation) fail to communi-
cate properly (Wilson, 1988, 1992; Wilson & Roehmann, 1992). Several
concert pianists have had major careers curtailed by a focal dystonia in one
hand. Highly practiced movements seem to be most affected, while other
uses of the hand remain functional.
In the receptive mode, Thaut and colleagues have produced an impressive
body of work on how Parkinsonian and stroke patients can regain motor
function (e.g., walking or grasping) through rhythmic entrainment (McIn-
tosh, Thaut, & Rice, 1996; Thaut, Brown, Benjamin, & Cooke, 1995;
Thaut, McIntosh, Prassas, & Rice, 1993). Rhythmic timing embedded in
music serves as a cue to motor system timing mechanisms in the brain.
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 17
Affective Research
Data-gathering techniques for studying affective responses to music fall into
three categories: verbal reports, behavioral observations, and physiological
responses (Abeles & Chung, 1996). In terms of finding out the brains role
in emotional responses to music, current strategies are quite limited. Re-
cently, PET scans of 10 amateur musicians indicated that different brain
regions were activated in response to positive and negative music listening
experiences (Blood, Zatorre, Bermudez, & Evans, 1999). Another avenue of
approach is found in biochemical analyses of blood samples (for a review
see Bartlett, 1996). Music can elicit changes in such biochemicals as endor-
phins, cortisol, adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), interleukin-1, and se-
cretory immunoglobin A. The brain-music-biochemical relationship is not
yet well understood but does hold some promise. In particular, studies in
psychoneuroimmunology are being used in music medicine to document the
physiological effects (e.g., changes in blood chemistry) that music has on the
body (Pratt & Spingte, 1996; Reilly, 1999; Spingte & Droh, 1992). Fear
and anxiety can be reduced in many clinical situations through the use of
music.
Each of these research strategies has advantages and disadvantages. It is
important to integrate findings from the various approaches into a more
coherent whole. Where data from different techniques are in disagreement,
efforts must be undertaken to resolve discrepancies. Wherever possible, it
would be helpful to attack the same problem with more than one strategy.
Particularly in the area of neuroimaging, it is important to keep up with
new technologies and possibilities (e.g., with development of better sound
delivery systems in fMRI).
Overall Development
To what extent do genes specify the intricate working of the human brain?
To what extent are the intricate workings of the brain acquired as a result
of experiences? New advances in neuroscience have addressed the ancient
nature versus nurture debate. In general, neuroscience research has shown
that neither nature nor nurture alone determines brain development. Human
brains at the prenatal stages are already interacting with the environment
(Standley, 1998). The brain appears to be more plastic and malleable during
the first decade of life than in adulthood. According to Thatcher studies have
shown that 40% of short-term and 70% of long-term connections in the
brain are influenced by heredity. Therefore, 30% to 60% of the brains con-
nections come from environmental influences or an interaction of heredity
and environmental influences (Thatcher, 1998). Nelson and Bloom (1997)
cite numerous demonstrations that show how positive or negative early ex-
periences can alter both the structure and function of the brain. It is also
18 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
important to remember that a childs brain is not the same as an adult brain.
There is agreement that during the first decade of life a child typically has
up to twice as much neural activity and connections as an adult (Bates, Thal,
& Janowsky, 1992; Chugani, 1998; Chugani, Phelps, & Mazziotta, 1987).
The brain makes connections during the prenatal period and throughout life
(Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 1999; Janowsky & Carper, 1996). Some con-
nections are found to be predetermined genetically, and others develop from
environmental influences (Cossu, Faienza, & Capone, 1994; Drayna, et al.,
2001; Flohr, 2004a; Ibatoullina, Vardaris, & Thompson, 1994; Schlaug, et
al., 2004; van Baal, de Geus, & Boomsma, 1998), Ibatoullina, Vardaris, &
Thompson, 1994; van Baal, de Geus, & Boomsma, 1998).
Children as young as 1 day old are able to make cognitive choices about
their environment (Atkins & Flohr, 2000; Flohr, Atkins, Bower, & Aldridge,
2000; Woodward, Fresen, Harrison, & Coley, 1996). There are clear addi-
tive and regressive events in brain development. As shown in figure 1.2, a
child at birth may have the largest number of neurons that she or he will
ever have. After the age of 2 years, the number of neurons remains nearly
the same until 65 years of age. Synapses, the connection between neurons,
form in the brain and change as the child develops. (For an explanation
directed to educators see Sylwester, 1995.) During early childhood the per-
cent of adult cortical levels of myelin and glia are increasing and may be
150
Percent of Adult Cortical Levels
100
50 Neurons
Synapses
Metabolism
Myelin
Glia
0
Gestation Birth 2 4 6 8 10 12
Age in Years
Figure 1.2. Approximation of the time course of additive and regressive events in brain de-
velopment. (From Janowsky & Carper in Sameroff & Haith, eds., 1996.)
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 19
experiences influence neural wiring, and the neural wiring has an effect on
recognition or interpretation of new sensory data. It is presently unknown
if the critical period is due to biological clock mechanisms, the brain struc-
tures that have developed, or an interaction of the two. There may be critical
periods in musical development, and the search for these periods provides a
fertile ground for research.
Authors often write about optimal periods as if they were critical periods,
although presently there are no identified critical periods in musical devel-
opment. An optimal period is used to refer to those periods in which devel-
opment will be faster or easier. For example, it is easier to learn to sing in
tune during the ages of 36 years than at 2528 years of age. It may also
be easier or more efficient to learn the language of classical, jazz, or any
style of music before the age of 6. The period in all these cases is optimal
rather than critical. There is no evidence to support the notion that a child
cannot learn to sing if she or he does not have music experiences before, for
example, 6 years of age.
There are indications of possible critical periods in music. For example,
the music educator Gordon advanced the idea of developmental music ap-
titude (Gordon, 1979, 1990). He has found that childrens scores on mea-
sures of musical aptitude do not change significantly after the age of ap-
proximately 9 years. A few studies indicate optimal periods and point
toward possible critical periods for music training. A group of adults with
a history of violin training and a group of adults without violin training had
their brains mapped using MEG (Elbert et al., 1995). The area of the so-
matosensory cortex representing the fingers of the left fingering hand was
larger than that in the contralateral hemisphere representing the right bow
hand and also larger than the corresponding area in nonmusicians. This
finding was consistent with reports of adult human amputees (Nelson &
Bloom, 1997). A possible critical period was indicated by a trend for the
effect to be larger for individuals who had begun music training before the
age of 10. Another optimal period and possible critical period was seen in
a study of violin training, where in a sample of 60 musicians and nonmu-
sicians, those who started training before the age of 7 years exhibited in-
creased corpus callosum size (Schlaug, Janke, Huang, Steiger, & Steinmetz,
1995).
Windows of opportunity refers to the idea that there are general time
frames in which optimal or critical development will take place. The fact
that writers often do not specify the window of opportunity as critical or
optimal leads to much confusion. It is important to underline the difference
between optimal and critical periods when talking about windows of op-
portunity for brain development. For example, it is an overstatement of
neuroscientific research to say that there is a window of opportunity for
music during the ages of 37 years and if a parent does not give the child a
chance during those ages, the child will not be musical. Bruer writes: For
most learning, particularly learning culturally transmitted skills and knowl-
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 21
of several theories see Sylwester [1995, pp. 1424, 3946]. For models of
brain functioning see Hodges [1996b, pp. 201205].) An examination of
current theories and studies that support them reveals that while some em-
phasize brain structure, others emphasize function. Many of these theories
will undoubtedly be proven incorrect with time, some may be combined with
others or new theories, many will be modified, and a few may even stand
the test of time. The following theories have been selected from among many
in the field because each has possible application to music education. (The
theories and theoretical areas are presented in alphabetical order of key-
word.)
Developmental Shifts
The theory concerning development shift refers to the idea that at certain
times of significant changes, shifts, or brain spurts, occur in brain devel-
opment (Epstein, 1978, 1986). These changes are thought to be part of the
human biological development program. The shifts can be modified by the
environment but are part of a normal developmental sequence. The notion
finds its roots in cognitive theory (e.g., Piaget, 1950), and the neurological
basis for at least one developmental shift from the ages of 5 to 7 has been
related to cognitive theory. Sameroff and Haith compiled several articles that
point to the efficacy of the idea that a childs brain chemistry and electrical
activity inherently change in significant ways between the ages of 5 and 7
(Janowsky & Carper, 1996). Flohr and Miller (1993, 1995) lend support to
this idea in relation to music with a series of EEG studies with 13 young
children. One finding was that children at age 7 exhibited significant EEG
brain alpha activity differences in response to contrasting styles of music
that were not present when the same children were 5 years of age. Music
by Vivaldi stimulated different brain responses from the children from Irish
folk song music while the children listened to the music and tapped rhythm
sticks. These differences occurred in the motor strip and the temporal areas.
The differences lend support to the idea of a developmental shift in music
processing. Barber et al. (1997) also found brain activity differences in re-
sponse to different styles of music. Subjects in their study were 21 adult
musicians and 25 nonmusicians.
Future research investigating the theory of developmental shift will prob-
ably have much to say to music educators and educators in general. For
example, when are the neurological attributes of a child best able to process
sounds from music or language? Are most 7-year-old children ready (in the
neurological sense) for abstract symbols in music, or is it more appropriate
at an earlier or later age? Developmental research since Zimmermans pio-
neering cognitive research (1971) has affected music education. Develop-
mental shift and development research are fertile fields for music educa-
tion and neurological researchers to collaborate in.
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 23
Expert or Habituation
Another theory, expert or habituation (or even efficiency) theory,
seeks to explain observed decreases in brain activity during certain tasks.
One might expect to observe an increase in thinking brain activity of a
subject during a cognitive task. The opposite phenomenon has been found;
experts and students after training show decreases in brain activity as op-
posed to novices engaged in the same cognitive task. Some studies have
supported the idea that less energy or brain electrical activity may be used
to perform a task in an experts brain than in a novices brain (Languis &
Miller, 1992; Miller & Flohr, 1995). The idea is that the brain of a novice
learner is less efficient and expends more energy when confronted with a
challenging task than the brain of an expert learner (Jensen, 1998). Thus,
music instruction should enable children to expend less energy during mu-
sical tasks.
There is, however, an alternative idea of why the decreases in activity are
observed. Petsche and colleagues suggested that the reduced temporal lobe
activity in the beta frequency bands of 75 healthy college students was re-
lated to habituation from listening to music (Petsche, Lindner, Rappelsberger,
& Gruber, 1988). It can be posited that the subjects developed a habituated
type of cortical activity in their listening to music after they were involved
in a period of music training. Habituation in an expert musician can also
be thought of as the musician going on automatic pilot while performing
the music. She or he has learned the piece so well that the motor systems
and cerebellum take over the performance, and other areas in the cerebral
cortex are deactivated, thereby producing less electrical or chemical activity.
Parsons and colleagues have found a similar result in their PET scan studies
with musicians (Parsons, 2000, 2001; Parsons et al., 1998). Cerebral blood
flow in a musicians frontal cortex decreased as the musician performed
music on a keyboard as opposed to rest. The reduction in PET scan activity
may lend support to the idea that the expert (or a person with training) uses
less energy in a well-learned task. However, the expert performer also ex-
hibits greater activation in other areas during performance, and the current
data do not show if there is less energy expended in the motor cortex among
expert performers, which is what the expert theory would predict. The iden-
tified decreases in the research are clearly linked to expert musicians and
students involved in music instruction. Future research will help determine
the mechanisms involved.
activity. However, in the second study by Flohr and others, results showed
an increase in some areas of the brain in contrast to a decrease in other
areas. The module theory does not specify whether the submodules produce
increased or decreased electrical activity. In addition, an increase in one mod-
ule along with a decrease in another part of the brain may represent the way
the brain relegates processing resources.
The term coherence may be viewed within the modular theory, al-
though coherence has been used to support connectionism. Connectionist
models postulate that cognition occurs through a network of linked nodes.
The nodes integrate activation through their excitatory and inhibitory links
(Carpenter, Akira, & Just, 1995, p. 92). Coherence reflects the number and
strength of coordination between different brain locations. Some authors
believe that coherence demonstrates evidence of anatomical connections and
information exchange between two brain locations (Fein, Raz, & Merrin,
1988). Brain imaging studies have shown a relationship between music lis-
tening or music instruction and increased coherence activities in children
(deBeus, 1999; Malyarenko et al., 1996; Sarnthein et al., 1997).
Coherence studies lend support to the idea that music instruction for
children at an early age will promote more profuse and efficient connections.
For example, Malyrenko et al. (1996) found that an exposure to music of
1 hour per day over a 6 month period had an effect on the brain electrical
activity in a sample of 43 4-year-old preschool children. Brain bioelectric
activity parameters indicated that listening to music resulted in an enhance-
ment of the coherence function. DeBeus (1999) ran a coherence analysis on
20 preschool childrens EEG data. In a baseline, resting condition and while
listening to music, deBeus found increased connectivity for a music-training
group versus a no-music-training group. Coherence patterns differentiated
children with and without music training during a resting condition, and
showed patterns similar to those identified by other researchers comparing
trained to untrained musicians. The listening-to-music condition identified
connections that included a topographical pattern of auditory analysis, in-
creased working memory activity, increased activity among musically sen-
sitive areas, and increased activity between hemispheres. Two limiting points
about coherence are important to note. First, coherence values are not well
accepted by all neuroscientists; second, skilled performers in comparison
with low-level performers in other fields (e.g., a high-level chess player with
a nonchess player, a high-level mathematician with a mathematically un-
skilled person) would presumably show differences.
Multiple Intelligences
In Frames of Mind (1985) and later in Intelligence Reframed (1999), Gard-
ner describes the concept of multiple intelligences. Performing, listening to
music, improvising, or composing music may require the use of at least eight
intelligences. The eight types of intelligence used in music activities may
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 27
include: music; visual-spatial (in the sense that instruments, e.g., cello, per-
cussion, or piano, have a visual-spatial element); bodily kinesthetic (the way
in which fine and gross movement functions in the perception of musical
motion and is used in performing music); interpersonal (e.g., conductor and
orchestral member); intrapersonal (e.g., expression of feelings through mu-
sic); language, as in singing; spiritual (e.g., in performance of sacred music,
deriving spiritual meaning and interpretation in music); and logical-
mathematical (e.g., rhythm durations). Neurological research has indicated
that many parts of the brain are utilized as children are engaged in music-
making activities. The multiple intelligences idea may help explain why other
types of intelligence are apparently affected by music instruction. The theory
that several kinds of thinking are required to learn and make music has been
called a near transfer theory (Hetland, 2000a). The quality of music in-
struction (Elliott, 1995) or combination of activities used in music instruc-
tion could help facilitate the transfer. One should not expect transfer from
music to other skills unless significant learning has occurred in music. The
idea of multiple intelligences and near transfer may help explain the ways
brain activity is affected by music instruction (Flohr, Miller, & deBeus,
2000).
Trion Theory
Shaw (2000) proposed a trion cortical model for the coding of certain
aspects of musical structure and perception and defines the trion model as
a highly structured mathematical realization of the Mountcastle organiza-
tion principle in which the cortical column is the basic neural network of
the cortex and is comprised of sub-unit minicolumns, our idealized trions
(p. 229; for more information on this theory see Leng & Shaw, 1991, 2000).
The cortex is to a large extent organized into a vertical column of neurons.
Shaw proposed that when the brain patterns predicted by his theoretical
model were mapped onto pitches and instruments, recognizable human
styles of music were found. Shaws trion model provided a theoretical back-
ground for the much-publicized and controversial Mozart effect studies
(Rauscher, Shaw, & Ky, 1993; Rauscher, Shaw, Ky, & Wright, 1994). This
trion theory suggests that the musical and spatial processing areas of the
brain are in some way shared, proximal, or overlapping. Thus, the relation-
ship of music and spatial processing are linked by neurological connections
in the cortex. Shaw and others write that a prewired neural connection is
suggested because of the short-term effect of music instruction on spatial
abilities (Graziano, Peterson, & Shaw, 1999).
Any theory including the trion theory is difficult to test with current brain
imaging techniques. However, an EEG study and a study outlining a new
method of analysis for EEG data have been used to empirically test the trion
theory (Sarnthein et al., 1997; Bodner et al., 1999). In the EEG study a carry
over of brain activity from listening to music to a spatial test was observed
28 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
in 3 of 7 subjects. The lack of effect in the other 4 subjects was perhaps due
to individual differences, problems in application of the theory, the lack of
a general effect, or other unknown causes. Comparing and combining trion
theory with other theories will be an interesting area for future research.
General Findings
Future Directions
Future directions for neuroscience and music research probably will and
should include the following:
allows us to study the human brain with the least possible effect of prior
environmental influence.
3. Music and neuroscience research may reveal activities that will help chil-
dren develop musical skills more efficiently and effectively. Families and
parents will be given information on specific enjoyable, interactive music
strategies.
4. More projects dealing with the relation of music to emotion and tem-
perament.
5. Continuing focus on music cognition. How does the human brain or-
ganize musical sounds into meaningful experiences? What are the devel-
opmental sequences in music cognition? Is music cognition completely
autonomous, or are there relationships among various intelligences?
6. Increased use of more than one type of brain imaging to test theories
through improvements in brain imaging technology and neurochemistry
(Nakamura et al., 1999).
7. Increased collaboration between cognitive neuroscientists, neurobiolo-
gists, philosophers, and musicians; there is an increasing interest among
neuroscientists in the study of music and the brain. Neuroscientists are
interested in using music as a way in which other brain activity may be
studied as well as examining music by itself. For example, music is dif-
ferent from language and can be used as a contrast in experiments de-
signed to investigate brain activity and language (Besson, 2000).
8. Continued movement toward research with greater external validity; for
example, focus on musical stimuli and musically valid tasks.
9. Better education of the general public and music education profession so
that patience is developed for basic research and sophisticated findings
are not reduced to promotional sound bites.
10. Music educators and philosophers may bring to future neuroscience re-
search the appreciation for the fluidity, creative constructs, and context-
specificity of music. Music is more than sonorous stimuli; it is connected
to cognitive, affective, kinesthetic, and social processes of each individual.
STUDY QUESTIONS
1. In what ways does neuroscience research influence your thoughts on
the nature versus nurture debate?
2. Identify and discuss the differences between optimal and critical pe-
riods. In what ways are the two terms misused?
3. List and discuss three important summary statements from the chapter.
Do you agree or disagree with those statements? Why?
4. Compare and contrast two theories or theoretical frameworks.
5. Describe two brain imaging techniques and discuss their strengths and
weaknesses in imaging brain activity.
6. Compare and contrast animal and human research.
7. Briefly outline and describe brain development from infancy through
adulthood, noting landmarks on the way. Where do most of these landmarks
seem to occur?
8. In what ways might musical experiences change the brain?
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 31
REFERENCES
Abeles, H., & Chung, J. (1996). Responses to music. In D. Hodges (Ed.), Hand-
book of music psychology (2nd ed., pp. 285342). San Antonio, TX: IMR
Press.
Abrams, R., & Gerhardt, K. (1997). Some aspects of the fetal sound environ-
ment. In I. Deliege & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Perception and cognition of music
(pp. 83101). East Sussex, England: Psychology Press.
Ackerman, S. (1992). Discovering the brain. Washington, DC: National Acad-
emy Press.
Altenmuller, E. (1993). Psychophysiology and EEG. In E. Niedermeyer & F. L.
da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography (pp. 597614). Baltimore: Williams
and Wilkins.
Altenmuller, E., Gruhn, W., Parlitz, D., & Kahrs, J. (1996). Music learning pro-
duces changes in brain activation patterns: A longitudinal DC-EEG study.
International Journal of Applied Music, I, 2833.
Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus,
A., & Zilles, K. (1997). Motor cortex and hand motor skills: Structural com-
pliance in the human brain. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 206215.
Atkins, D., & Flohr, J. W. (2000, June). Music style ratings in relation to emo-
tional tone of the music and individual variation in emotionality. Paper pre-
sented at the twelfth annual American Psychological Society Conference, Mi-
ami.
Avanzini, G., Faienza, C., Minciacchi, D., Lopez, L. & Majno, M. (Eds.). (2003).
The neurosciences and music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
vol. 999.
Avanzini, G., Lopez, L., Koelsch, S., & Majno, M. (Eds.). (2005). The Neuro-
sciences and Music II: From Perception to Performance, Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 1060.
Barber, B. (1999). An investigation of neural processes in music cognition using
quantitative electroencephalography. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni-
versity of Melbourne.
Barber, B., McKenzie, S., & Helme, R. (1997). A study of brain electrical re-
sponses to music using quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG). Inter-
national Journal of Arts Medicine, 5(2), 1221.
Bartlett, D. (1996). Physiological responses to music and sound stimuli. In D.
Hodges (Ed.), Handbook of music psychology (2nd ed., pp. 343385). San
Antonio, TX: IMR Press.
Bates, E., Thal, D., & Janowsky, J. S. (1992). Early language development and
its neural correlates. In F. Boller, J. Grafman, S. J. Segalowitz, & I. Rapin
(Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 69110). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Besson, M. (2000, June). Comparisons between language and music. Paper pre-
sented at Mapping Music in the Brain, a satellite symposium of the annual
conference for Human Brain Mapping. San Antonio, Texas.
32 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Emotional
responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paral-
imbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 382387.
Bodner, M., Shaw, G. L., Gabriel, R., Johnson, J., Murias, M., & Swanson, J.
(1999). Detecting symmetric patterns in EEG data: A new method of analysis.
Clinical Electroencephalography, 30(4), 143148.
Brody, J. (1991, April 9). Not just music, bird song is a means of courtship and
defense. New York Times, pp. C1, C9.
Brown, W., Marsh, J., & Ponsford, R. (1985). Hemispheric differences in event-
related potentials. In D. Benson & E. Zaidel (Eds.), The dual brain:
Hemispheric specialization in humans (pp. 163180). New York: Guildford
Press.
Bruer, J. T. (1999). The myth of the first three years. New York: Free Press.
Caine, R. N., & Caine, G. (1994). Making connections: Teaching the human
brain. New York: Addison-Wesley.
Carpenter, P. A., Akira, M., & Just, M. A. (1995). Language comprehension:
Sentence and discourse processing. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 92.
Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho, K., &
Naatanen, R. (1998). Development of language-specific phoneme represen-
tations in the infant brain. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 351353.
Chugani, H. T. (1998). A critical period of brain development: Studies of cere-
bral glucose utilization with PET. Preventive Medicine, 27(2), 184188.
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. F., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron emission to-
mography study of human brain functional development. Annals of Neurol-
ogy, 22(4), 487497.
Cohen, D., & Erez, A. (1991). Event-related potential measurements of cognitive
components in response to pitch patterns. Music Perception, 8(4), 405430.
Cossu, G., Faienza, C., & Capone, C. (1994). Infants hemispheric computation
of music and speech. In C. Faienza (Ed.), Music, speech and the developing
brain. Italy: Edizioni Angelo Guerini.
Crystal, H., Grober, E., & Masur, D. (1989). Preservation of musical memory
in Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
52, 14151416.
DAmato, M. (1988). A search for tonal pattern perception in cebus monkeys:
Why monkeys cant hum a tune. Music Perception, 5(4), 453480.
deBeus, R. (1999). The effects of music training on electroencephalographic co-
herence of preschool children. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60(09B),
4952.
Deliege, I., & Sloboda, J. (1996). Musical beginnings: Origins and development
of music competence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Diamond, M., Scheibel, A., Murphy, G., & Harvey, T. (1985). On the brain of
a scientist: Albert Einstein. Experimental Neurology, 88, 198204.
Dissanayake, E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts. In N. Wallin, B. Mer-
ker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 387410). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Drayna, D., Manichaikul, A., de Lange, M., Snieder, H. Spector, T. (2001). Sci-
ence, 291, 19691972.
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstrub, B., & Taub, E. (1995). In-
creased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players.
Science, 270(5234), 305307.
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 33
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the phys-
iological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology,
206, 419436.
Hutchinson, S., Lee, L., Gaab, N. & Schlaug, G. (2003) Cerebellar volume of
musicians. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 943949.
Ibatoullina, A. A., Vardaris, R. M., & Thompson, L. (1994). Genetic and envi-
ronmental influences on the coherence of background and orienting response
EEG in children. Intelligence 19, 6578.
Imberty, M. (2000). The question of innate competencies in musical communi-
cation. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music
(pp. 449462). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Janowsky, J. S., & Carper, R. (1996). Is there a neural basis for cognitive tran-
sitions in school-age children? In A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The
five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility (pp. 3360).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Jensen, E. (1998). Teaching with the brain in mind. Alexandria, VA: Association
for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
Johnson, J., & Ulatowska, H. (1995). The nature of the tune and text in the
production of songs. In R. Pratt & R. Spintge (Eds.), Music medicine: Vol. 2
(pp. 153168). St. Louis, MO: MMB Music.
Juslin, P., & Sloboda, J. (Eds.). (2001). Music and emotion: Theory and research.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Kaufmann, L., Curtis, S., Wang, J., & Williamson, S. (1991). Changes in cortical
activity when subjects scan memory for tones. Electroencephalography and
Clinical Neurophysiology, 82, 266284.
Kimura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166171.
Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening.
Cortex, 3, 163178.
Krause, B. (1987). The niche hypothesis: How animals taught us to dance and
sing [On-line]. Available: http://www.wildsanctuary.com.
Krumhansl, C. (2002). Music: A link between cognition and emotion. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 11(2), 4549.
Languis, M. K., & Miller, D. C. (1992). Lurias theory of brain functioning: A
model for research in cognitive psychophysiology. Educational Psychologist,
27, 493511.
Lee, D., Chen, Y. & Schlaug, G. (2003). Corpus callosum: Musician and gender
effects. NeuroReport, 14, 205209.
Leng, X., & Shaw, G. L. (1991). Toward a neural theory of higher brain function
using music as a window. Concepts in Neuroscience, 2(2), 229258.
Leonhard, C., & House, R. (1972). Foundations and principles of music edu-
cation. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Levitin, D., & Bellugi, U. (1998). Musical abilities in individuals with Williams
syndrome. Music Perception, 15(4), 357389.
McIntosh, G., Thaut, M., & Rice, R. (1996). Rhythmic auditory stimulation as
an entrainment and therapy technique: Effects on gait of stroke and Parkin-
sonians patients. In R. Pratt & R. Spintge, (Eds.), Music medicine: Vol. 2
(pp. 145152). St. Louis, MO: MMB Music.
Majkowski, J., Bochenek, Z., Bochenek, W., Knapik-Fijalkowska, D., & Kopec,
J. (1971). Latency of averaged evoked potentials to contralateral and ipsi-
36 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Pascual-Leone, A., Dand, N., Cohen, L., Braskil-Neto, J., Cammarota, A., &
Hallett, M. (1995). Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial
magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. Journal
of Neurophysiology, 74(3), 10371045.
Paulus, W. (1988). Effect of musical modelling on late auditory evoked poten-
tials. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 237(5),
307311.
Payne, K. (2000). The progressively changing songs of humpbackwhales: A win-
dow on the creative process in a wild animal. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S.
Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 135150). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Peretz, I. (2001). Brain specialization for music: New evidence from congenital
amusia. In Zatorre, R. & Peretz, I. (Eds.), The biological foundations of music.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 930, pp. 153165).
Petsche, H., Lindner, K., Rappelsberger, P., & Gruber, G. (1988). The EEG: An
adequate method to concretize brain processes elicited by music. Music Per-
ception, 6, 133159.
Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge and Kegan
Paul.
Platel, H., Price, C., Baron, J. C., Wise, R., Lambert, J., Frackowiak, R., Lech-
evalier, B., & Eustache, F. (1997). The structural components of music per-
ception: A functional anatomical study. Brain, 20(2), 229243.
Posner, M., & Raichle, M. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific Amer-
ican Library.
Pratt, R., & Spintge, R. (1996). Music medicine: Vol. 2. St. Louis, MO: MMB
Music.
Raichle, M. 1994. Visualizing the mind. Scientific American, 270(4), 5864.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1993). Music and spatial task per-
formance. Nature, 365, 611.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Ky, K. N., & Wright, E. L. (1994, August). Music
and spatial task performance: A causal relationship. Paper presented at
the American Psychological Association 102nd annual convention, Los An-
geles.
Reilly, M. (1999). Music: A cognitive behavioral intervention for anxiety and
acute pain control in the elderly cataract patient. Dissertation Abstracts In-
ternational, 60(07B), 3195.
Reimer, B. (1999). Facing the risks of the Mozart effect. Music Educators
Journal, 86, 3743.
Restak, R. (1994). The Modular Brain. New York: Scribners.
Robinson, G., & Solomon, D. (1974). Rhythm is processed by the speech hemi-
sphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(3), 508511.
Sarnthein, J., vonStein, A., Rappelsberger, P., Petsche, H., Rauscher, F., & Shaw,
G. (1997). Persistent patterns of brain activity: An EEG coherence study of
the positive effect of music on spatial-temporal reasoning. Neurological Re-
search, 19, 107116.
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H. (1994). In vivo morphom-
etry of interhemispheric asymmetry and connectivity in musicians. In I. De-
liege (Ed.), Proceedings of the third international conference for music per-
ception and cognition (pp. 417418), University of Liege, Belgium.
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, L., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of
structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267(5198), 699701.
38 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Schlaug, G., Janke, L., Huang, Y., Staiger, J. F., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). In-
creased corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1047
1055.
Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Overy, K., Cronin, K., Lee, D.J. & Winner, E. (2004).
Effects of music training on childrens brain and cognitive development. Pro-
ceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception and Cog-
nition, Evanston, IL.
Schwent, V., Snyder, E., & Hillyard, S. (1976). Auditory evoked potentials dur-
ing multichannel selective listening: Role of pitch and localization cues. Jour-
nal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2(3),
313325.
Sergent, J. (1993). Mapping the musician brain. Human Brain Mapping, 1(1),
2038.
Shaw, G. L. (2000). Keeping Mozart in mind. San Diego: Academic Press.
Slater, P. (2000). Birdsong repertoires: Their origins and use. In N. Wallin, B.
Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 4964). Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Sluming, V., Barrick, T., Howard, M., Cezayirli, E., Mayes, A. & Roberts, N.
(2002) Voxel-based morphometry reveals increased gray matter density in
Brocas Area in male symphony orchestra musicians. Neuroimage, 17, 1613
1622.
Snowdon, D. A. (1997). Aging and Alzheimers disease: Lessons from the nun
study. Gerontologist, 37(2), 150156.
Snowdon, D. (2001). Aging with grace: What the nun study teaches us about
leading longer, healthier, and more meaningful lives. New York: Bantam
Books.
Spintge, R., & Droh, R. (1992). Music medicine. St. Louis, MO: MMB Music.
Standley, J. (1998). Pre and perinatal growth and development: Implications of
music benefits for premature infants. International Journal of Music Educa-
tion, 31, 213.
Sylwester, R. (1995). A celebration of neurons: An educators guide to the human
brain. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Devel-
opment.
Thatcher, R. (1998, September 11). EEG database guided neurotherapy: Prac-
tical applications. Presentation at the annual conference of the Society for the
Study of Neuronal Regulation, Austin, Texas.
Thaut, M., Brown, S., Benjamin, J., & Cooke, J. (1995). Rhythmic facilitation
of movement sequencing: Effects on spatio-temporal control and sensory mo-
dality dependence. In R. Pratt & R. Spintge (Eds.), Music medicine: Vol. 2
(pp. 104112). St. Louis, MO: MMB Music.
Thaut, M., McIntosh, G., Prassas, S., & Rice, R. (1993). Effect of rhythmic
cuing on temporal stride parameters and EMG patterns in hemiparetic gait
of stroke patients. Journal of Neurologic Rehabilitation, 7, 916.
Thulborn, K. R., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1999). Plasticity of language-
related brain function during recovery from stroke. Stroke, 30(4), 749
754.
Trehub, S. (2004). Music Perception in Infancy. In J. Flohr, Musical lives of
young children (pp. 2429). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Trehub, S., Bull, D., & Thorpe, L. (1984). Infants perception of melodies: The
role of melodic contour. Child Development, 55, 821830.
MUSIC AND NEUROSCIENCE 39
Trehub, S., Schellenberg, E., & Hill, D. (1997). The origins of music perception
and cognition: A developmental perspective. In I. Deliege & J. Sloboda (Eds.),
Perception and cognition of music (pp. 103128). East Sussex, England: Psy-
chology Press.
Trehub, S., & Trainor, L. (1993). Listening strategies in infancy: The roots of
language and musical development. In S. McAdams & E. Bigand (Eds.),
Thinking in sound: Cognitive perspectives on human audition (pp. 278327).
New York: Oxford University Press.
van Baal, G. C. M., de Geus, E. J. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (1998). Genetic influ-
ences on EEG coherence in 5-year-old twins. Behavior Genetics, 28(1), 919.
Wallin, N., Merker, B., & Brown, S. (Eds). (2000). The origins of music. Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Warren, R. (1993). Perception of acoustic sequences: Global integration versus
temporal resolution. In S. McAdams & E. Bigand (Eds.), Thinking in sound:
Cognitive perspectives on human audition (pp. 3768). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Whaling, C. (2000). Whats behind a song? The neural basis of song learning
in birds. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music
(pp. 6576). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Williamson, S., & Kaufman, L. (1988). Auditory evoked magnetic fields. In A.
Jahn & J. Santos-Sacchi (Eds.), Physiology of the ear (pp. 497505). New
York: Raven Press.
Wilson, F. (1986). Tone deaf and all thumbs? New York: Viking Press.
Wilson, F. (1988). Brain mechanisms in highly skilled movements. In F. Roeh-
mann (Ed.), The biology of music making (pp. 9299). St. Louis, MO: MMB
Music.
Wilson, F. (1991). Music and the neurology of time. Music Educators Journal,
77(5), 2630.
Wilson, F. (1992). Digitizing digital dexterity: A novel application for MIDI
recordings of keyboard performance. Psychomusicology, 11, 7995.
Wilson, F. (1998). The hand. New York: Vintage Books.
Wilson, F., & Roehmann, F. (1992). The study of biomechanical and physiolog-
ical processes in relation to musical performance. In R. Colwell (Ed.), Hand-
book of research on music teaching and learning (pp. 509524). New York:
Schirmer Books.
Woodward, S., Fresen, J., Harrison, V. C., & Coley, N. (1996). The birth of
musical language. Proceedings of the seventh International Seminar of the
Early Childhood Commission, Winchester, U.K.
Zatorre, R. (1994). Musical processing in the nonmusicians brain: Evidence for
specialized neural networks. In I. Deliege (Ed.), Proceedings of the third in-
ternational conference for music perception and cognition (pp. 3940), Uni-
versity of Liege, Belgium.
Zatorre, R., Evans, A., & Meyer, E. (1994). Neural mechanisms underlying
melodic perception and memory for pitch. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(4),
19081919.
Zatorre, R., & Peretz, I. (Eds.) (2001). The biological foundations of music.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 930.
Zimmerman, M. P. (1971). Musical characteristics of children. Washington, DC:
Music Educators National Conference.
The Neurobiology of Music
Cognition and Learning
2
wilfried gruhn
frances rauscher
The 20th century has provided a wealth of important data about cognition
and learning. However, with the cognitive revolution in developmental psy-
chology and the rise in Piagetian theory, the emphasis shifted from learning
to thinking. Consequently, substantial knowledge about the development of
childrens thinking at different ages has been collected since then, but not as
much knowledge has been provided about how children learn. This move-
ment away from studying childrens learning reflected more than a shift in
interest; it rather reflected an assumption that cognitive development and
learning are different processes. However, learning and cognition are two
sides of the same coin. What one knows is largely based upon what one has
learned, and learning, of course, generates knowledge. Therefore, any theory
of cognitive development that has little to say about learning provides a
limited perspective of development.
Over the past decades, the emergence of interdisciplinary studies in cog-
nitive neuroscience produced evidence of neural correlates for cognition and
learning. To the same degree neuroscientific research has extended our un-
derstanding of the basic structures of cognition and learning. This chapter
reviews current research on the neural mechanisms of music learning and
cognition. We will also refer the reader to relevant chapters elsewhere in this
text for a greater degree of technical detail.
A brief survey of the literature on learning and cognition shall provide
the framework for our particular neurobiological perspective. Four different
approaches to investigating learning are reviewed: behaviorism (Watson
40
NEUROBIOLOGY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND LEARNING 41
Behaviorism
The behaviorists viewed developmental changes in behavior as relying on
several basic principles of learning, particularly classical conditioning (Pav-
lov, 1927) and operant conditioning (Skinner, 1953), as initially demon-
strated by animal experiments. For example, Ivan Pavlovs experiments with
dogs revealed that a neutral stimulus (a bell) begins to elicit a response
(salivation) after being repeatedly paired with another stimulus (food) that
already elicits that response. A stimulusresponse chain (S R) can be
strengthened by repetition and reinforcement, and the response will gradu-
ally generalize. Learning, according to the behaviorists, occurs when a be-
havioral change can be linked to a stimulus presumed to have caused that
change and can thus be objectively measured. However, the behaviorists
failed to do justice to the organization of human behavior and the complex
inner processes that are responsible for generating it. For example, although
the neuronal activity of learning in the human brain cannot be observed
directly, it clearly plays a major role in behavior. From a behaviorist point
of view, however, the mind remains a black box about which one can
only speculate and which therefore cannot contribute very much to the sci-
entific study of behavior. Any introspection into the processes of learning
was seen by the behaviorists as neither reliable nor relevant to the under-
standing of behavioral processes. This limitation in the behaviorist view,
along with the emergence of computer science, encouraged investigators to
attempt to describe the cognitive processes that are necessary to generate
and control complex human behavior. This event became known as the
cognitive revolution.
Cognitive Psychology
The cognitive revolution represented a qualitative shift from an emphasis on
behavior toward an emphasis on understanding the inner processes involved
in cognition and intellectual growth. The constructivist perspective empha-
sizes the active role of the child in constructing advanced forms of cognition
that transcend less adequate earlier forms (Baldwin, 1894/1968). Construc-
tivists believe that one should begin the study of childrens cognitive devel-
opment by exploring the foundational concepts with which children come
equipped at birth and then go on to document any change that may take
place in these concepts with age. Jean Piaget was perhaps the most influential
developmental psychologist to carefully observe childrens development.
Based on his own observations of his three young children, Piaget built a
cognitive theory of the awakening of intelligence in children. According to
Piaget, children progress through a series of five universal stages of devel-
42 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Sociohistorical Theory
Contextual models, sometimes called systems views, emphasize that the
transformation from infant to adult takes place via a complex, multidirec-
tional system of influences (Gottlieb, 1991). These theorists are concerned
with understanding how the broad range of biological, physical, and soci-
ocultural settings affect learning and development. For example, Lev S. Vy-
gotskys sociohistorical theory from the 1930s stresses the importance of
cultural tools, symbols, and ways of thinking that the child acquires from
more knowledgeable members of the community (1934/1962). Development
is viewed as a dynamic, never-ending transaction that involves continuing,
reciprocal exchanges: People and settings transform the child, who in turn
affects the people and settings that surround him or her, which further re-
shape the child, in an endless progression. According to the sociohistorical
view, knowledge does not originate in the environment alone (as the behav-
iorists claimed) or in the interaction between the individual and the envi-
ronment (as the constructivists maintained). Rather, knowledge originates in
the social, linguistic, and material history of the individuals culture and its
NEUROBIOLOGY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND LEARNING 43
Connectionism
Connectionism offers a fresh perspective to the understanding of learning
by focusing research on the microstructure of cognition. Connectionist the-
ory is extremely rich in terms of its implications for brain development.
Highly sophisticated brain-imaging techniques, such as electro- and
magneto-encephalography (EEG and EMG), event-related potential (ERP),
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), computer tomography (CT), and posi-
tron emission tomography (PET), permit a new view of the active brain. The
topography of brain areas involved in aural perception and learning has been
elaborated and transcribed into brain maps. The study of highly complex
network structures and interconnections has laid the foundation for a con-
nectionist model of parallel distributed processing (PDP) (Rumelhart &
McClelland, 1986). The theory behind PDP is bound to the hypothesis of
the modularity of mind (Fodor, 1983). Here the input systems refer to dif-
ferent brain areas that are highly specialized in processing particular prop-
erties of the incoming auditory stimulation, such as pitch, loudness, location
of the sound source, melodic contour, and so forth. The possibility of exactly
measuring the neural activation, the intensity and distribution of activation
patterns, and the localization and lateralization of domain-specific process-
ing tasks across the cortex have enabled new avenues for the investigation
of the physiological foundations of music cognition and music learning
(Hodges, 1996).
Along with the development and implementation of the imaging tech-
niques, computer models of artificial neural networks have been devel-
oped to investigate strategies in problem-solving and decision-making pro-
cesses (Fiske, 1993; Griffith & Todd, 1999; Todd & Loy, 1991). The
connectionist approach can be seen as a neurally inspired model of infor-
mation processing, in which groupings of neurons are interconnected in in-
put layers (by which signals enter the system), output layers (which represent
the outcome of the network), and hidden layers (which compute the more
complex nonlinear relationships within the network). (For a more detailed
44 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
ticular deficits; (2) animal experiments that study neuronal brain reactions;
(3) the measurement of infants information processing, especially sensory
and auditory temporal processing; and (4) the implementation of brain-
imaging techniques for a clear and precise identification and localization of
changes in brain activation.
Neurons are highly specialized to respond to particular qualities of stim-
uli, for example, to a section of a band of frequencies (pitch), to a movement
of sounds up and down (direction), to the intensity of sound (loudness), and
so forth. Neurons of a particular brain area represent different features.
Unfortunately, little research exists on the auditory sensory mapping of the
associative cortex. In cognition, distributed processing of sensory informa-
tion must be coordinated for the creation of what is eventually perceived by
the conscious mind. As M. E. Martinez (1999) has put it:
The human mind is not a video camera. We do not process and store count-
less sensory bits; rather, we construct our inner and outer worlds according
to the organizing principle of meaning. The fact that knowledge can be rep-
resented in different ways implies that knowledge is not a sensory transcrip-
tion of the external world into the inner world of the mind. (p. 21)
mously important. It has been documented for animal and human brains
that the synaptic densitythe number of synapses per neuron or per unit
volume of cortical tissuechanges over life spans and defines the limits of
the processing capacity (Huttenlocher, 1979, 1984). In human beings, syn-
aptogenesis takes place prenatally and in early infancy. By the age of 1 year
it reaches a plateau stage, followed by a progressive synapse decline, which
happens most rapidly during preschool years (Huttenlocher, 1984). How-
ever, there are regional differences in synaptogenesis in human brains. Hut-
tenlocher (Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997) compared the development in
two cortical areas: the auditory and prefrontal cortex. He found that syn-
aptic density increases more rapidly in the auditory cortex (maximum at age
3 months) than in middle frontal gyrus (peak after age 15 months). Here
synaptic growth occurs concurrently with growth of dendrites and axons
and with myelination of the subcortical white matter. The following phase
of synapse elimination also starts earlier in the auditory cortex, where it
reaches a mature level by age 12 years, than in the prefrontal cortex (Hut-
tenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997). The exuberant overproduction of neuronal
connections during infancy may be seen as an anatomical substrate for neu-
ral plasticity (Huttenlocher, 1990) that has a tremendous impact on the
unique structure of early learning. The discovery of an inverted u-shaped
structure in brain development is confirmed by the development of glucose
metabolism. Cerebral glucose consumption rises from birth until about 4
years of age, maintains from 4 to about 910 years, and then gradually
declines (Chugani, 1998). These findings have important implications for
our understanding of brain plasticity and critical periods for learning.
Neuroscientists at Geneva University (Muller, Toni, & Buchs, 2000) have
investigated the chemical changes that influence synaptic strength. The re-
searchers stimulated rat brain slices to produce long-term potentiation (LTP).
If a receiving neuron has been activated, the incoming neurotransmitter in-
duces LTP by flooding calcium ions into the spine. An hour after treatment,
20% of the synapses had developed double spines, forming a second spine
adjacent to the active one. Muller concludes that LTP triggers a duplication
of the active synapse (Barinaga, 1999, p. 1661). Presumably this causes an
increase in synaptic strength.
The neurobiological foundations for learning are derived from studies
that suggest that experience or learning induces changes in the brain that
relate to cortical thickness (Diamond et al., 1964), the size of cell bodies
(Diamond, 1967), the size of synaptic contact areas (West & Greenough,
1972), an increase in dendritic spines (Globus et al., 1973), a parallel in-
crease in the number of synapses per neuron (Turner & Greenough, 1985),
the thickness of the corpus callosum (Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, Stalger, &
Steinmetz, 1995), an increase in hippocampal neurons (Kempermann et al.,
1997), the size of the left planum temporale (Pantev et al., 1998), and the
doubling of spines through LTP (Muller et al., 2000). These research findings
relate either to the growth of number or size of new synaptic connections
by formal training or enriched environmental experience or to the growth
NEUROBIOLOGY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND LEARNING 47
Overview
A comprehensive review of brain development is beyond the scope of this
chapter. However, M. H. Johnson (1998) has identified four factors that we
believe are important to any understanding of the neurobiology of music
learning and cognition.
First, there are neural structures in the brain that are common to both
humans and other mammals, both primate and nonprimate. Differences be-
tween humans and other animals primarily concern the extent of the cerebral
cortex. Subcortical structures, such as the hippocampus and cerebellum, are
structurally similar across mammalian species.
Second, the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum continue to
develop throughout childhood. Although the vast majority of neurons are
present at birth (Rakic, 1995), synapses, dendrites, and fiber bundles con-
tinue to develop postnatally, perhaps as a function of experience. Myelin,
the fatty sheath that surrounds neuronal pathways (and is thought to in-
crease the efficiency of information transmission), also increases dramatically
after birth. The immaturity of the human brain at birth may explain some
of the limitations on learning and cognition present in infants and children.
Similarly, the dynamic postnatal development of the cortex allows more in-
tentional, purposeful behavior.
Third, different areas of the cerebral cortex develop at different rates. For
example, Conels (1939/1967) study of cortical development in the human
infant led him to conclude that the cortex develops in an inside-out fash-
ion, with outer brain layers developing in advance of inner layers. Differ-
ential development between cortical regions (i.e., visual cortex and frontal
cortex) has also been documented (Huttenlocher, 1990). These patterns of
development may influence information processing.
Finally, studies on cortical plasticity suggest that cortical specialization is
heavily influenced by experience. Although primary cortices are genetically
predetermined, there is a high degree of modulation with respect to the
extension and connectivity of functional brain areas according to experience
and learning. Therefore, brain plasticity can be seen as fundamental for the
development of mental representations.
Empirical Methods
The recent explosion of knowledge of brain development makes the task of
relating it to cognitive changes considerably more viable. Consequently, ef-
50 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Transfer Effects
signed to replicate the Mozart effect through use of the same musical com-
position as well as similar control conditions and dependent measures, Steele
and his colleagues achieved negative results (e.g., Steele et al., 1999).
There is also a growing body of evidence implicating the role of arousal
in the Mozart effect. Schellenberg and his colleagues found that changes in
arousal, induced by listening to music, mediated performance on a spatial-
temporal task (Nantais & Schellenberg, 1999: Thompson, Schellenberg, &
Husain, 2001: Husain, Thompson, & Schellenberg, 2002). This research
suggests that the Mozart effect may be due to differences in arousal, rather
than to Mozart in particular or music in general.
McKelvie and Low (2002) were the first to investigate the Mozart effect
in children. Twelve-year-old children listened to either a Mozart sonata or
to popular music by the band Aqua. There were no differences in the chil-
drens spatial scores as a function of the music condition. A study with 10
11-year-old children, however, did find a Mozart effect (Ivanov & Geake,
2003). Children who listened to either Mozart or Bach scored significantly
higher on a spatial-temporal task than children who sat in silence. The high-
est scores were achieved by the children who had studied a musical instru-
ment.
A second meta-analytic review, also undertaken by Hetland (2000a), ex-
amined studies on the effects of instrumental instruction on childrens spatial
abilities (see, for example, Rauscher et al., 1997). Figure 2.1 portrays the
effect for kindergarten children. The results of this analysis revealed an over-
all effect size of r .37, an effect that was deemed remarkably consistent
and could be generalized to similar populations of preschool- and
elementary-school-aged children, while they are engaged in similar kinds of
active music programs, with or without keyboard instruments, taught in
groups or individual lessons. The effect cannot be explained away by a Haw-
thorne effect, nonequivalence of experimental groups, experimenter bias, or
study quality. It is a solid finding (Hetland, 2000a, pp. 4142).
We will not attempt to undertake a critical analysis of these studies, but
instead, as per the focus of this chapter, we will comment on their possible
neurophysiological implications. The motivation for research on the transfer
of music listening or instruction to spatial task performance came from a
neural connectionist model of the mammalian brain proposed by Xiaodan
Leng and Gordon Shaw (Leng & Shaw, 1991). Based on Mountcastles co-
lumnar principle of cortex, the model proposed that families of neural net-
works respond to and compare spatial features of objects. By mathematically
deriving their firing probabilities the researchers determined that the net-
works evolved according to symmetries modified by Hebb learning rules.
These neural network patterns (lasting tens of seconds over large cortical
areas) corresponded to spatial-temporal task performance (requiring the
transformation of mental images over time). Leng and Shaw therefore pre-
dicted that certain musical forms and instruction might stimulate these firing
patterns, thereby enhancing spatial-temporal performance.
Although it is impossible to prove a neural model of brain function,
Leng and Shaws (1991) hypothesis is supported by data. For example, Al-
zheimer patients who listened to the Mozart sonata or silence demonstrated
improved spatial-temporal performance following Mozart (J. K. Johnson,
Cotman, Tasaki, & Shaw, 1998; J. K. Johnson, Shaw, Vuong, Vuong, &
Cotman, 1999). Neuroscientists have investigated the effect through the use
of EEG and fMRI. EEGs of subjects who performed a spatial-temporal task
after listening to the Mozart sonata revealed a carryover effect in parietal
and frontal cortex; no carryover was found when reading a story was sub-
stituted for the task (Sarnthein et al., 1997). EEGs of epilepsy patients, some
comatose, showed decreased seizure activity during exposure to the sonata
rather than silence or control music (Hughes, Daaboul, Fino, & Shaw, 1998;
Hughes, Fino, & Melyn, 1999). The compositions differentially activated
the prefrontal, occipital, and cerebellar regionsall regions associated with
spatial-temporal reasoning. These findings, although specific to music listen-
ing, may also have neurophysiological implications for the effects of music
instruction on certain spatial abilities.
An additional study utilized DC potential and ERP (Trimmel, Goger, &
Geiss-Granadia, 2003). A measure of electrical activity, DC potential is con-
sidered to reflect ongoing cortical activation at the level of the neuron. ERP,
on the other hand, measures electrical activity as a function of a stimulus
event (such as performing a spatial-temporal task). It is considered to be a
measure of cognitive activity. Subjects listened to either a composition by
Mozart, Albinoni, Schubert, or brown noise. After each condition they per-
NEUROBIOLOGY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND LEARNING 57
spatial imagery in his own thinking. Involuntary dream images were the
source of many of his most creative and influential contributions, including
the idea for his research on mental rotation, the first method of nonmetric
multidimensional scaling, and the computer algorithm that underlies addi-
tive nonhierarchical cluster analysis. Given the prominent role of spatial
abilities both in models of human abilities and in models of cognition, stud-
ies that demonstrate that music instruction may influence spatial reasoning
have important educational implications. However, due to the fact that there
is as yet no commonly accepted theoretical approach that adequately ac-
counts for these transfer effects, we recommend discretion in both the in-
terpretation of research results and their application.
velocity adaptations did not fit with an exact synchronization of steps and
metric impulses (Thaut, Miltner, et al., 1999). Effects are also reported from
patients with epilepsy where brain-wave abnormalities occur during music-
induced seizures (Critchley, 1977). Just recently, fewer clinical seizures and
fewer generalized bilateral spike and wave complexes were reported from a
patient with Lennox-Gastaut syndrome after regular exposure to Mozarts
piano sonata K. 448 (Hughes et al., 1999).
During the last two decades, advanced technology has been applied to the
treatment of deaf (or severely hearing-impaired) children who still have an
active hearing nerve. An artificial cochlea can be implanted to stimulate the
hair cells of the cochlea by electrodes that are activated through electric
impulses from an outside microphone. Cochlear implant (CI) surgeries have
become routine in Europe, Australia, and the United States since the 1980s,
and younger and younger prelingually deaf children can be treated. The most
appropriate time window for an efficient surgery is between age 2 and 4,
that is, before the development of prosody has already been stabilized. Al-
though there is only limited access to music transmitted by the available
technology of today (Fujita & Ito, 1999), new generations of speech pro-
cessors (such as Nucleus, Clarion, and Med-el) have expanded the range of
formats by developing different strategies through use of either time reso-
lution (CIS continuous interleaved sampling strategy), spectral peak res-
olution (SPEAK strategy), or a combined strategy (ACE advanced com-
bined encoder).
In recent times, increasing research efforts have been directed towards the
processing of nonspeech sounds, namely pitch, rhythm, melodies, and tim-
bres in CI users (Gfeller et al. 1997; Pijl, 1997; Fujita & Ito, 1999; Abdi et
al. 2001; Gfeller et al., 2002; Stordahl, 2002; Koelsch et al., 2004). The
general findings are that rhythm is more easily and more successfully per-
ceived than melodies, especially if they are presented without rhythm and
verbal cues. Pitch perception, however, can be improved by training and the
application of new processors that use spatial and temporal patterns of stim-
ulation. In general, the appraisal of music tends to be much lower than in
normal listeners (McDermott, 2004).
Up to now, little has been known about the cortical development of CI
patients after they recover auditory cortex areas. These areas, like the visual
cortices of children born blind, are underdeveloped and utilized by other
sensory representations. Perception of different sounds evoked through elec-
trical stimulation must be learned with respect to sound discrimination and
the gradual attribution of meaning to discern ible sounds. A clear under-
standing of the neurobiological development of mental representation can
facilitate this learning process, the goal of which relates to speech acquisi-
NEUROBIOLOGY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND LEARNING 61
Results from brain research and neurobiological findings alone can hardly
lead to immediate applications and recommendations for music education.
These data cannot be directly transferred to educational practice because
scientific descriptions are essentially different from educational prescriptions.
Empirical data are based upon objective facts and verifiable procedures; sci-
entific research is committed to objectivity, reliability, and validity. Judg-
ments in education, however, are value judgments to a large degree. Nor-
mative decisions on values can never be deduced objectively from empirical
descriptions. As Gardner (1999) puts it: We could know what every neuron
does and we would not be one step closer to knowing how to educate our
children, because the chasm between is and ought is unbridgeable
(pp. 60, 79).
Mental representation has become the key notion of the cognitive revo-
lution during the decade of the brain (Gardner, 1999). Therefore, one pos-
sible application to music education may involve the fostering of mental
representations (see chap. 7 by Andreas C. Lehmann and Jane W. Davidson
in this volume). As already mentioned, education is based on decisions that
are grounded in value judgments that deal with the what and why of
teaching, but findings in neurobiology may indicate new ways of how to
teach. Teaching interacts with the disposition and potential of each individ-
ual. Although neurobiological findings cannot tell us why to teach music of
a particular culture and what to select from the broad variety of musical
traditions, empirical findings can advise us on how and when to teach so
that mind, memory, perception, and cognition can be developed most effec-
tively. This has established the new discipline of neurodidactics (Preisser,
1998) which provides a new foundation for viewing the learning processes
in accordance with the state of brain development. If we know about electro-
chemical and hormonal processes that enhance synaptic strength and facil-
itate long-term representation that can be re-activated at any time, methods
62 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
STUDY QUESTIONS
1. How do motor and aural activities interact in music learning pro-
cesses?
2. What role does memory play in formal representation?
3. What are the neuronal correlates of different types of representation
(figural versus formal)?
4. What are the effects of declarative and procedural strategies on the
neuroplasticity evoked by long-term training?
5. What kind of subcortical activations are involved in music processing?
6. How do cortical and subcortical representations interact in music
learning?
7. Do culture-specific types of brain processing exist and consequently
are there different strategies for the development of mental representations?
Then, what would be the best ways of teaching music within and between
cultures?
8. What are the near effects of music making on cognitive development
and achievement?
9. What are the effects of listening to music on children and adults cog-
nition? Do the data justify educational applications?
10. What are the effects of music instruction on cognition? What mech-
anisms might account for these transfer effects?
REFERENCES
Abdi, S., Khalessi, M. H., Khorsandi, M., & Gholami, B. (2001). Introducing
music as a means of habilitation for children with cochlear implants. Inter-
national Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 59(2), 105113.
Aebli, H. (1980/81). Denkendas Ordnen des Tuns, 2 vols. Stuttgart, Germany:
Klett-Cotta.
Aldridge, D. (1994). Alzheimers disease: Rhythm, timing and music as therapy.
Biomedicine and Pharmacotherapy, 48(7), 275281.
Altenmuller, E., & Gruhn, W. (1997). Music, the brain, and music learning.
Mental representation and changing activation patterns through learning
(GIML series vol. 2). Chicago: G.I.A.
Altenmuller, E., Gruhn, W., Parlitz, D., & Liebert, G. (2000). The impact of
music education on brain networks. Evidence from EEG studies. Interna-
tional Journal for Music Education, 35, 4753.
Avanzini, G., Faienza, C., Minciacchi, D., Lopez, L., & Majno, M. (Eds.) (2003).
The Neurosciences and Music. Annals of The New York Academy of Sci-
ences, vol. 999.
64 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Baldwin, J. M. (1968). The development of the child and of the race. New York:
Augustus M. Kelly. (Original work published 1894).
Bamberger, J. (1991). The mind behind the musical ear: How children develop
musical intelligence. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bangert, M. & Altenmuller, E. (2003). Mapping perception to action in
piano practice: A longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neuroscience 4(1),
S. 26.
Barinaga, M. (1999). Learning visualized, on the double. Science 286, 1661.
Bastian, H. G. (2000). Musik(erziehung) und ihre Wirkung: Eine Langzeitstudie
an Berliner Grundschulen [Music education and its effects]. Mainz, Germany:
Schott.
Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (1991). Connectionism and the mind: An intro-
duction to parallel processing networks. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., & Wang, P. (1997). Language, communication and neural
systems in Williams syndrome [Special issue: Communication processes in
children with developmental disabilities]. Mental Retardation and Develop-
mental Disabilities Research Review, 3, 334342.
Black, J. E., & Greenough, W. T. (1998). Developmental approaches to the mem-
ory process. In J. Martinez & R. Kesner (Eds.), Neurobiology of learning and
memory (pp. 5588). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Breitling, D., Guenther, W., & Rondot, P. (1987). Auditory perception of music
measured by brain electrical activity mapping. Neurophysiologia, 25, 765
774.
Bruer, J. T. (1999). The myth of the first three years. New York: Free Press.
Brust, J. C. (1980). Music and language: Musical alexia and agraphia. Brain,
103(2), 357392.
Butzlaff, R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of Aesthetic
Education, 34(34), 167178.
Calvin, W. H. (1996). How brains think. New York: Basic Books.
Case, R. (1972). Learning and development: A neo-Piagetian interpretation. Hu-
man Development, 15, 339358.
Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., & Cheung, M. C. (1998). Music training improves verbal
memory. Nature, 396, 128.
Chugani, H. T. (1998). A critical period of brain development: Studies of
cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Preventive Medicine, 27(2), 184
188.
Conel, J. L. (1967). The postnatal development of the human cerebral cortex
(Vols. 18). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work pub-
lished 1939.)
Critchley, M. (1977). Musicogenic epilepsy. In M. Critchley & R. Hensen (Eds.),
Music and the brain (pp. 344353). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Crystal, H. A., Grober, E., & Masur, D. (1989). Preservation of musical memory
in Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
52(12), 14151416.
Deary, I. J. (2001). Intelligence: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Diamond, M. C. (1967). Extensive cortical depth measurements and neuron size
increases in the cortex of environmentally enriched rats. Journal of Compar-
ative Neurology, 131, 357364.
Diamond, M. C., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1964). The effects of an
NEUROBIOLOGY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND LEARNING 65
methods that are most appropriate for studying relevant cognitive processes
and mechanisms.
Perhaps the most contentious issue in the psychology of music concerns
the relative role of innate structures and learning in musical experience.
Some educators and researchers take a dim view of nativist constructs such
as musical talent (Howe, Davidson, & Sloboda, 1998), and assume a prom-
inent role of learning, education, and enculturation. To support their posi-
tion, they point to patterns of within-group similarities and between-group
dissimilarities across different musical cultures and historic periods (e.g.,
Walker, 1996). By contrast, others argue that although such instances of
learning are conspicuous, they are nevertheless constrained by under-
lying cognitive principles. Three classes of evidence point to the importance
of innate cognitive endowments. First, competence in many domains is
achieved early and rapidly, despite large differences in experience. Second,
competence is often domain specific, implicating specialized cognitive mod-
ules (Fodor, 1983). Third, there is often a marked disparity between models
of environmental influences and the mental representations acquired by
learners, which implies that information processing is constrained by cog-
nitive biases and perceptual predispositions, and assimilated into pre-existing
knowledge structures.
A second issue concerns the most appropriate level at which to investigate
and explain psychological phenomena related to music. Are listening, per-
forming, and composing music best understood in terms of neurons and
networks, or in terms of mental schemata and prototypes? Studies directed
at different analytic levels often pose different questions, use different meth-
ods, and promote different theories. Reductionism holds that explanations
based on psychological constructs such as schemata and prototypes are re-
duciblewith the help of yet-to-be-discovered bridging lawsto neurolog-
ical events in the brain. In this view, theories of cognition should be reducible
to neural models. The opposing position, argued by Fodor (1983), is that
psychological explanations are not always reducible to neural states, or that
they may be realized in multiple configurations. It would therefore be im-
possible to identify systematic bridging laws that connect psychological
states (e.g., beliefs, desires) to specific brain states. Rather, psychology would
need to construct its own autonomous generalizations using a specialized
language of thought. In this view, theories of music cognition can be de-
veloped somewhat independently of advances in neuroscience. One danger
with this strategy is that when psychological principles are not linked to
plausible physiological mechanisms, it can be difficult to evaluate their va-
lidity.
A third area of disagreement involves balancing concerns for experimen-
tal control with concerns for ecological validity. Recent collaborations be-
tween a well-known composer and a group of empirical psychologists illus-
trate the potential relevance of perceptual and cognitive data for
composition (McAdams, 2004a, 2004b; Reynolds, 2004), but it is not ob-
vious that all psychological studies involving musical materials are highly
74 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Studies of pitch have dominated the field of music cognition over the past
20 years, paralleling a similar emphasis in music theory and education. Pitch
studies have been a fruitful area of investigation because people are remark-
ably accurate at discriminating between pitches (Burns, 1999). Moreover,
mental representations of pitch are richly structured. This structure partly
reflects culture-specific conventions in music. Through passive or active ex-
posure, listeners internalize regularities in the music of their own culture,
forming long-term knowledge schemata into which novel music stimuli are
assimilated. Nonetheless, processing limits and biases also constrain mental
representations of pitch. For example, the limits of working memory con-
strain our ability to encode brief but unfamiliar melodies with accurate de-
tail. Because of such limits, unfamiliar melodies are represented largely in
terms of pitch contour (Dowling & Harwood, 1986).
its own frequency, amplitude, and phase. Fourier analysis is the mathemat-
ical technique that allows us to analyse a complex sound into its pure-tone
components. We do not normally perceive the individual partials of a com-
plex sound because cognitive mechanisms operate to fuse them together,
leading us to experience a unitary sound. According to Ohms acoustical
law, however, under certain listening conditions we have a limited ability to
hear some of the individual partials of a complex sound.
Any sound with a discernable pitch has a periodic waveform, in that the
waveform continuously repeats itself over time. The period is the time taken
for one complete cycle of the waveform, and is the reciprocal of the repe-
tition rate. The repetition rate usually determines the perceived pitch (ex-
ceptions include circular tones, discussed below), and is measured in cycles
per second, or hertz (1 Hz 1 cycle / sec). In general, when the repetition
rate of a periodic sound is increased, the perceived pitch rises. The partials
of any periodic waveform fall along the harmonic series, and are called
harmonics. That is, if the lowest frequency component of the sound is n
then the other harmonicscalled overtonesare members of the set 2n, 3n,
4n, 5n, and so on. That is, each overtone has a frequency that is an integer
multiple of the lowest or fundamental frequency of the complex. An impor-
tant property of the harmonic series is that additional overtones do not alter
the overall repetition rate of the waveform (which is determined by the fun-
damental frequency) and therefore do not change the perceived pitch of the
complex.
Helmholtz (1863/1954) observed that several aspects of music, such as
scale structure and harmony, have compelling parallels in physical acoustics.
He noted that music from several cultures involves important scale notes
that map onto the harmonics of complex tones. In the major scale, the fifth
scale degree (sol) is equivalent (i.e., in note name, or tone chroma) to the
third harmonic of a complex periodic tone built on the first note of the scale
(do), and the third scale degree (mi) is equivalent to the fifth harmonic. The
most important musical intervals used in Western music are also found in
the harmonic series. The first and second harmonics are separated by an
octave, the second and third harmonics are separated by a perfect fifth, the
third and fourth harmonics are separated by a perfect fourth, the fourth and
fifth harmonics are separated by a major third, and the fifth and six har-
monics are separated by a minor third.
Harmonic overtones are not perceived as individual pitches. When pre-
sented with a periodic complex tone, only one pitch is usually perceived.
Other mechanisms, however, are sensitive to harmonic spectra. For example,
complex tones with different spectral contents are perceived as having dif-
ferent sound qualities, tone colors, or timbres. Harmonic overtones also af-
fects the degree of sensory consonance and dissonance of tone combinations
presented simultaneously. Tone combinations with fundamental frequencies
that are related to each other by small-integer ratios, such as the octave (2:1)
and the perfect fifth (3:2), have several harmonics in common and lead to
sensory consonance. In contrast, tone combinations with fundamental fre-
LISTENING TO MUSIC 77
quencies that are not related to each other by small-integer ratios, such as
the minor second (16:15), lead to sensory dissonance. Such combinations
contain harmonic frequencies that are not identical but that fall within a
critical band (a range of frequencies within which sensory interactions oc-
cur), resulting in rapid amplitude fluctuations that give rise to perceived
roughness and beating. Sensitivity to sensory consonance and dissonance is
thought to be independent of knowledge and enculturation. Long-term
knowledge of music also affects judgments of consonance, and this aspect
of music experience is referred to as musical consonance.
If sensitivity to sensory consonance and dissonance is independent of
knowledge and enculturation, then young infants might be expected to ex-
hibit a natural preference for consonance. Researchers have examined in-
fants preference for one type of music over another type by letting them
choose which type of music they hear. Such experiments typically place
an infant between two loudspeakers. When the infant looks toward the
speaker on her left, she hears one type of music, but she hears a different
type of music when she looks toward the speaker on her right. Hence, the
infant is controlling what she hears by the direction of her gaze. Because
infants tend to have a bias to look rightward, it is important to counter-
balance the stimuli presentations. That is, half of the infants hear Piece A
from the speaker on their left and Piece B from the speaker on their right,
whereas the other half hear Piece B from the left and Piece A from the right.
Results based on this infant preference method reveal that infants look
longer toward a speaker playing a consonant version of a musical piece than
they do toward a speaker playing a dissonant version of the same piece.
These findings suggest that listeners have an innate preference for sensory
consonance and/or an innate dislike of sensory dissonance. As infants de-
velop, these basic preferences are overlaid with effects of learning and en-
culturation, which contribute to the experience of musical consonance and
dissonance. These effects of learning may run counter to initial predisposi-
tions: indeed, in so far as the aesthetic quality of chords can be judged
outside of a musical context, the most beautiful may involve considerable
dissonance.
Zentner and Kagan (1996) presented 4-month-old infants with a melody
accompanied by a single harmony line. The melody and accompaniment
were separated by minor seconds in the dissonant condition, but by major
and minor thirds in the consonant condition. Infants preferred the consonant
versions. Trainor and Heinmuller (1998) extended these findings by consid-
ering the influence of harmonics in pairs of complex tones, which can inter-
act with each other and give rise to sensory dissonance. They found that 6-
month-old infants preferred to listen to perfect fifths and octaves compared
to tritones and minor ninths (which give rise to greater sensory interference
between harmonics). Trainor, Tsang, & Cheung (2002) tested the preferences
of 2- and 4-month-old infants for consonant versus dissonant two-tone in-
tervals. Using a looking-time preference procedure, they found that infants
of both ages preferred to listen to consonant over dissonant intervals. The
78 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
findings illustrate that infants are sensitive to sensory consonance and dis-
sonance, and exhibit a preference for consonant over dissonant tone com-
binations.
Studies of discrimination provide further evidence that sensitivity to sen-
sory consonance and dissonance is innate. Specifically, it comes naturally for
listeners to discriminate combinations of tones on the basis of their conso-
nance or dissonance. Schellenberg and Trainor (1996) presented 7-month-
old infants and adults with a background pattern of simultaneous fifths (7
semitones) presented at varying pitch levels. Listeners were tested on their
ability to discriminate the intervals in the background pattern from a new
interval, which was either a tritone (6 semitones) or a fourth (5 semitones).
Fifths and fourths are relatively consonant intervals, whereas tritones are
relatively dissonant. Both age groups used the consonance and dissonance
to discriminate these intervals. Although the fifth and fourth differ more
from each other in terms of interval size, the fifth and tritone were better
discriminated. Presumably, the dissonance of the tritone made it stand out
from the perfect fifth, whereas the relative consonance of the perfect fourth
made it sound similar to the fifth. In short, sensitivity to sensory consonance
and dissonance is evident very early in development. Listeners can learn to
appreciate dissonance, but they begin life with an initial preference for con-
sonance.
If sensitivity to sensory consonance is a basic property of the auditory
system, one might ask how this property affects scale structures and tuning
systems. In most scales from around the world, consonant intervals (e.g.,
octaves, perfect fifths and fourth) are structurally important. For example,
complex tones separated by octaves are considered to be similar in virtually
all cultures. In North Indian scales, tones separated by a fifth (the sa and
pa) are structurally important and are typically sounded continuously
throughout a piece. The most common pentatonic scale (exemplified by the
black notes on the piano), which is found in Chinese and Celtic music, can
be formed by choosing any pitch as an arbitrary starting tone, adding a
second tone a fifth higher, another tone a fifth higher than the second tone,
and so on, until a collection of five pitches is obtained. The scale is formed
by octave-transposing the collection of tones so that they fall within a single
octave.
In Western music, a number of tuning systems have been used historically
(for a review see Burns, 1999). One, called Pythagorean tuning, extends the
pentatonic scale described above with additional tones that continue the
cycle of fifths. In another tuning system, called just intonation, the scale
is formed by tuning notes so that their fundamental frequencies form small-
integer ratios with the fundamental frequency of the first note of the scale
(do). Both of these scales limit the possibility of transpositions between keys,
because some instances of particular intervals (e.g., the perfect fifth between
C and E) are tuned differently than other instances (e.g., C# and F).
Equal temperament represents a compromise solution. It guarantees that
all intervals (i.e., all perfect fifths, or all major thirds) are tuned identically,
LISTENING TO MUSIC 79
and that important intervals do not deviate greatly from small-integer fre-
quency ratios (fifths and fourths deviate from exact small-integer ratios by
2% of a semitone; major and minor thirds are slightly more mistuned).
These minor deviations, although discriminable in some cases, are no greater
than the typical tuning deviations observed in the performances of singers
or stringed-instrument players. Moreover, such small departures from exact
small-integer ratios have little effect on the perceived consonance of these
intervals, which may explain why equal temperament has endured for many
years.
sical training, who perceive similarities between tones that are separated by
an octave (Allen, 1967; Kallman, 1982).
If pitch chroma and pitch height are basic dimensions of pitch perception,
one should expect to find similar evidence among nave listeners. When
musically-untrained adults or children judge the similarity between pure
tones, however, they tend to focus exclusively on the dimension of pitch
height (Allen, 1967; Kallman, 1982; Sergeant, 1983). For example, C4 and
C#4 (notes separated by a semitone) are perceived to be highly similar, but
C4 and C5 (notes separated by an octave) are perceived to be no more similar
than C4 and B4 (notes separated by a major seventh). These findings do not
rule out the possibility that sensitivity to the chroma dimension could be
uncovered with tasks that measure implicit rather than explicit knowledge
of musical associations. For example, Demany & Armand (1984) found that
three-month-old infants are less surprised when one note of a melody is
replaced by a note that is an octave apart than when it is replaced by a note
that is a seventh or ninth apart. As another example, Humphreys (1939)
showed that a conditioned galvanic skin response to one particular pitch
generalises to pitches an octave apart. Nonetheless, the findings make it clear
that music educators should not assume that octave equivalence is explicitly
understood by nave listeners.
An early psychological model of pitch perception incorporated the two
dimensions of pitch height and pitch chroma. As shown in Figure 3.1, these
two dimensions are depicted as orthogonal dimensions of a geometrically
regular helixa monotonic dimension of pitch height and a circular dimen-
sion of pitch chroma. Shepard (1964) reported evidence that these dimen-
sions are psychologically relevant and orthogonal. He created tones with
well-defined chroma but ambiguous height. Such circular tones were
constructed by combining 10 pure-tone components spaced at octave inter-
vals, and imposing a fixed amplitude envelope over the frequency range such
that components at the low and high ends of the range approach hearing
threshold.
Although the overall pitch height of any circular tone is somewhat in-
determinate, listeners experience certain circular tones as higher or
lower than others. In particular, listeners tend to perceive the relative
height of these tones so as to maximize their pitch proximity. For example,
when presented the circular tones C followed by D, one can perceive the
second tone as either higher or lower than the first tone (C up to D,
or C down to D). Both interpretations are possible because their individual
pitch heights are ambiguous. Nonetheless, listeners typically perceive the
second tone (D) as higher than the first tone (C), because this interpretation
implicates a pitch distance of only two semitones. Listeners almost never
perceive the second tone as lower than the first tone, because that interpre-
tation would implicate a pitch distance of 10 semitones.
Shepard created fascinating patterns of circular tones in which chroma
varied continuously around a chroma circle (see figure 3.1). For ascending
patterns, he shifted all of the octave-spaced components up in frequency
LISTENING TO MUSIC 81
Figure 3.1. The helical model of pitch and the chroma circle.
The helical model combines two dimensions of pitch: height
and chroma.
(adding new components at the low end of the amplitude envelope) until
the complex returned to the initial configuation. When chroma was shifted
continuously in a clockwise direction around the chroma circle (C, C#, D,
D#, etc.), the pattern was perceived as ascending endlessly. When chroma
was shifted continuously in a counter-clockwise direction (C, B, A#, A, etc.),
the pattern was perceived as descended endlessly. That is, listeners perceived
changes in pitch chroma but not in overall pitch height. Most importantly,
these effects were perceived by musically trained and untrained listeners
alike. By making pitch height indeterminate, Shepard demonstrated that
82 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
pitch chroma has perceptual significance, even for untrained listeners. Thus,
although untrained listeners may lack explicit knowledge of the similarity
between tones with the same chroma, they nonetheless demonstrate sensi-
tivity to chroma.
If pitch class and pitch height are orthogonal, then the relative height of
circular tones that are directly opposite on the circle, such as C and F#,
should be ambiguous or indeterminate. Deutsch addressed this prediction in
a series of experiments (for a review, see Deutsch, 1999b). She reported that,
for a given individual, certain circular tones (i.e., chromas) are reliably and
consistently judged as being higher than the circular tone that is opposite
on the circle, suggesting that the perceived height of a tone is systematically
related to its pitch chroma. For example, some listeners reliably perceive a
successive presentation of two particular circular tones separated by a tritone
(e.g., C and F#) as upward pitch movement, whereas others perceive the
same two tones as downward pitch movement. This so-called tritone para-
dox, and related findings, implies that pitch chroma and pitch height are not
entirely independent.
Shepard (1982, 1999) and others have proposed more elaborate models
of pitch perception with additional dimensions besides those based on height
and chroma. These models account for affinities between tones separated by
perfect fifths and fourths, which are said to be relevant for musically trained
listeners tested with harmonically rich tones presented in musical contexts.
Even more elaborate models have been proposed to account for affinities
between tones separated by thirds. As described later, the establishment of
a musical key also shapes pitch judgments (for a review, see Krumhansl,
1990). For example, in the context of the key of C major, C and G are
perceived to be more strongly associated with each other than E and B, even
though both tone pairs represent a perfect fifth interval (Krumhansl, 1979).
Absolute Pitch
It is often asserted that most listeners are sensitive to relative pitch but not
to absolute pitch. Relative pitch refers to the ability to produce, recognize,
or identify pitch relations, such as those that define a musical interval or a
melody. To illustrate, Twinkle Twinkle Little Star can be sung in a high or
a low voice, or performed on a tuba or a piccolo. As long as the pitch
relations conform to those of the melody, it is usually recognizable. Abso-
lute pitch refers to the ability to produce, recognize, or identify an individual
pitch (e.g., middle C) without reference to any other pitch. Whereas relative
pitch is the norm among trained and untrained listeners, absolute pitch is
rare, occurring in about 1 in 10,000 people (Levitin & Rogers, 2005; Tak-
euchi & Hulse, 1993). Absolute pitch can be a valuable skill for musicians,
but it can also interfere with the ability to perceive pitch relations (Miyazaki,
1993; 2004). Because melodies are defined by pitch and duration relations
rather than with reference to any absolute pitch, relative pitch is arguably a
more musical mode of pitch processing.
LISTENING TO MUSIC 83
Melody
Melodic Intervals
A melodic interval is created when two tones are sounded in sequence. They
are experienced as large or small, consonant or dissonant, and for musically
trained listeners, they are associated with category labels such as the major
third, the minor sixth, and the octave. The ability to recognize and appre-
ciate melodic intervals is one consequence of the more general skill known
as relative pitch. Relative pitch allows us to perceive, appreciate, and re-
member melodies. Large melodic intervals or leaps form the basis for gap-
fill melodies (Meyer, 1973) and are experienced as a point of accent (Boltz
& Jones, 1986; Drake, Dowling & Palmer, 1991; Jones, 1987). Conversely,
melodies sound more coherent or cohesive when they consist of a sequence
of small intervals (Huron, 2001; Russo, 2002). Interval size may also influ-
ence melodic expectancy (Larson, 2004; Narmour, 1990) and grouping (De-
liege, 1987; Lerdahl, 1989; Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983).
Interval size is normally defined according to the number of semitones
that separate the lower and upper pitch of the interval, consistent with a
logarithmic relation between frequency and pitch. Recent evidence by Russo
& Thompson (in press; 2005), however, indicates that judgments of interval
size are influenced by a large number of factors. Participants in these studies
were presented with sequences of two pitches and judged the size of the
melodic interval formed by them. Estimates were larger for intervals in the
high pitch register than in the low pitch register, and for descending intervals
than for ascending intervals. Ascending intervals were perceived as larger
than descending intervals when presented in a high pitch register, but de-
scending intervals were perceived as larger than ascending intervals when
presented in a low pitch register (Russo & Thompson, in press). Moreover,
if the upper pitch of an interval had a brighter timbre (i.e., higher spectral
centroid) than the lower pitch, the interval was judged to be significantly
larger than if the timbral properties of the two pitches were reversed (Russo
& Thompson, in press b). The latter effect was so powerful that for some
conditions participants judged a seven-tone pitch interval to be larger than
a six-tone interval. Other evidence suggests that listeners even consider the
facial expressions of performers when estimating the size of melodic intervals
LISTENING TO MUSIC 85
Figure 3.2. Stimuli from Schellenberg and Trehub (1996b). Sequences consisted of 2
alternating tones, transposed after the two tones were presented four times. The alter-
nating tones were separated by a perfect fifth (P5, top panel), a tritone (TT, middle
panel), or a perfect fourth (P4, lower panel). Infants 6 months of age were tested on
their ability to detect displacements of the top tone during change trials.
LISTENING TO MUSIC 87
processing advantage for consonant intervals (see also Schellenberg & Tre-
hub, 1994a, 1994b).
Melodic Contour
The contour of a melody refers to its pattern of upward and downward
changes in pitch over time, irrespective of the absolute pitches involved, or
the specific size of the intervals between pairs of adjacent tones. Figures 3.3a
and 3.3b display melodies with a different contour, a different interval struc-
ture, and different absolute pitches. By contrast, figures 3.3a and 3.3c illus-
trate melodies with the same contour, but different intervals and absolute
pitches. figures 3.3a and 3.3d display melodies with the same interval struc-
ture, yet different absolute pitches (one is an exact transposition of the
other). Finally, figures 3.3a and 3.3e illustrate melodies that are identical in
all respects: absolute pitch, intervallic structure, and contour.
How would these melodies be retained, or mentally represented, in
memory? One possibility is that our mental representations are more-or-less
veridical. As such, they would include absolute pitches, precise intervals be-
tween tones, and melodic contour. On this view, only the melodies dis-
played in figures 3.3a and 3.3e would be represented equivalently in
memory. Although absolute pitches play a role in musical experience, as
noted above, explicit memory for melodies is more often based on pitch
relations.
A second possibility it that listeners mental representations are deter-
mined by abstracting a sequence of intervals and discarding information
about absolute pitch. If so, then the melodies shown in figures 3.3a, 3.3d,
and 3.3e would be represented equivalently in memory, while the other two
melodies would have different representations. For novel melodies, however,
listeners often have difficulty retaining the exact pattern of intervals, which
suggests that unfamiliar melodies are seldom represented with this degree of
precision.
A third possibility is that listeners mental representations are relatively
crude, determined simply by the direction of the upward and downward
shifts in pitch (i.e., melodic contour). In other words, fine-grained informa-
tion about intervals and absolute pitch would typically not be perceived and
encoded accurately during listening. The melody in figure 3.3a would be
represented merely by its contour of up-down-up, and the mental represen-
tations of four of the five melodies would be equivalent (a, c, d, e).
Note that these melodic features (contour, intervals, and absolute pitch)
are embedded hierarchically. If a listener remembers the absolute pitch of
each of the tones in a melody, she can reconstruct the intervals between
tones, and the melodic contour. If the listener remembers the sequence of
intervals (but not the absolute pitches), then she can reconstruct the melodic
contour. Conversely, melodies that differ in contour necessarily differ in in-
terval size and absolute pitch. Melodies that differ in one or more intervals
Figure 3.3. The standard melody in panel A has an up-
down-up contour. The melody in panel B has a different con-
tour, different intervals, and different pitches. The melody in
panel C has the same contour as the standard but different
intervals and pitches. The melody in panel D has the same
contour and intervals as the standard, but different pitches; it
is an exact transposition. Finally, the melody in panel E has
the same contour, intervals, and pitches as those in the stan-
dard.
88
LISTENING TO MUSIC 89
may or may not differ in contour, but at least one tone must differ in ab-
solute pitch.
In general, research indicates that listeners mental representations of
novel melodies contain contour information but relatively little information
about absolute pitch or exact interval size. Memory for the absolute pitches
of novel melodies tends to be poor, and memory for the exact intervals
between notes also tends to be poorer than memory for contour. Findings
from studies of infants are particularly compelling in this regard (for a re-
view, see Trehub, Schellenberg, & Hill, 1997). Moreover, the findings for
adults memories for novel melodies converge nicely with those from infants
(for a review, see Dowling, 1994).
Studies with adults often adopt the same-different experimental method.
On each experimental trial, listeners hear a standard (original) and com-
parison melody and judge whether they are the same or different. By sys-
tematically varying the ways in which the comparison differs from the stan-
dard (as in figure 3.3) and assessing the effects of such changes on judgment
accuracy, one can determine which features are represented in memory. Lis-
teners make errors about the interval and absolute-pitch information of
novel melodies relatively soon after they are presented. By contrast, they
retain contour information for longer periods of time.
The experimental method is necessarily altered for studies with infants.
Infant listeners are trained to turn their head toward a loudspeaker when
they hear a change in an auditory stimulus presented repeatedly (e.g., a
melody), a process that occurs naturally anyway. By reinforcing this response
with illuminated and activated toys, infants maintain their interest in the
task. If they reliably turn their head toward the speaker for certain melodic
changes but not for others, we can conclude that the former changes are
relatively detectable for infants, whereas the latter changes are relatively
undetectable. Several studies have confirmed that contour changes are very
noticeable for infants, whereas changes that maintain contour but alter ab-
solute pitch or interval size are far less noticeable (Trehub et al., 1997). In
short, infants perceive and remember the contour of melodies.
Why is melodic contour so easy to remember? Many have speculated that
our sensitivity to contour in music stems from the adaptive importance of
contour in speech. Studies of speech perception in infancy reveal that infant-
directed speech (i.e., motherese) differs from adult-directed speech in a num-
ber of systematic ways (Fernald, 1991), and that infants prefer to listen to
infant-directed over adult-directed speech (Cooper & Aslin, 1990; Fernald,
1985). One of the distinctive aspects of infant-directed speech is its exag-
gerated use of pitch contour. Different pitch contours are used to express
different messages to infants (e.g., approval, arousal, etc.), and the altera-
tions that adults make in their speech to infants are remarkably similar
across cultures (Fernald et al., 1989). Hence, sensitivity to contour patterns
in speech could facilitate bonding between infants and their caregivers, and
the language-acquisition process. Such sensitivity has obvious adaptive
90 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
value, and could account for infants sensitivity to contour in music. Re-
searchers have identified a number of implications of this heightened sensi-
tivity to melodic contour. First, melodies with fewer changes in contour are
perceived as simpler than melodies with more contour changes (Boltz &
Jones, 1986). Second, listeners attend more to notes at points of contour
change than they do to notes that are embedded within an ongoing contour
(Dyson & Watkins, 1984). For example, when presented with a 5-tone se-
quence with an up-up-down-down contour (see figure 3.4), listeners atten-
tion is drawn more to the middle (third) tonethe point of contour
changethan it is to the second or fourth tones. In other words, tones at
contour points (at contour changes or at the beginning or end of melodies)
are more likely to be represented in memory than are other tones. It follows,
then, that melodies with a relative abundance of contour points contain a
relative abundance of salient bits of information, and should be relatively
challenging to process and remember.
A number of recent studies have examined the possible relation between
contour sensitivity in melody and sensitivity to speech intonation. In partic-
ular, the ability to make sense of pitch changes in these two domains could
be associated with shared neural resources. Patel, Foxton & Griffiths (in
press) found, for example, that musically tone-deaf individuals have diffi-
culty discriminating intonation contours extracted from speech. However,
Peretz & Hyde (2003) showed that such individuals could sometimes dis-
criminate intonation contours in speech at normal performance levels, as
long as the discrimination required attention to coarse-grained aspects of
pitch. Peretz & Hyde concluded that although sensitivity to pitch changes
in music and speech may be associated with shared neural resources, music
often involves fine grained changes in pitch such that correlated performance
levels are not always expected.
The potential neural overlap between music and speech is not restricted
to pitch contour. The ability to decode speech prosody might engage a range
of processes that are involved in music listening, including sensitivity to con-
tour, loudness, tempo, and rhythm (Ilie & Thompson, in press). In support
of this idea, Thompson, Schellenberg & Husain (2004) reported that adults
with extensive training in music were significantly better at decoding the
emotional connotation of speech in an unfamiliar language. Because partic-
ipants could not understand the verbal content of the speech, they relied on
Figure 3.5. Schematic diagram of the scales used by Trehub, Schellenberg, and
Kamenetsky (1999). Each scale had seven tones per octave. One was the familiar
major scale (left). The other were completely artificial. The unequal-step scale
(middle) was formed by dividing the octave into 11 equal steps and selecting a
subset of 7 tones. The equal-step scale (right) was formed by dividing the octave
into 7 equal steps.
structurally similar in any ways to linguistic utterances? Can infants use cues
in the music to segment music properly?
Krumhansl and Jusczyk (1990) tested infants preferences for Mozart
pieces that were correctly or incorrectly segmented. Correctly seg-
mented pieces had pauses inserted between phrases. Incorrectly segmented
pieces had pauses inserted within phrases. To an adult listener, both versions
sound somewhat strange, although the former version seems more musical
because the pauses occur at natural breaks in the music. Looking times
indicated that infants preferred the correctly segmented pieces, in which seg-
ments tended to end with relatively long notes and downward pitch con-
tours. Interestingly, spoken utterances also tend to end with words of rela-
tively long duration and downward pitch contour. This correspondence
between spoken utterances and musical phrases leads to two related inter-
pretations of the results: (1) infants might prefer the correctly segmented
pieces because they bear structural similarities to spoken utterances, which
infants have learned from being exposed to speech, or (2) downward con-
tours and extended durations naturally mark the end of all auditory signals.
Either way, the findings indicate that listeners implicitly understand at a very
early age that melodic phrases tend to end with downward pitch motion
and notes of relatively long duration. Such cues allow listeners to segment
melodies into melodic groups such as phrases and motifs, and to identify
boundaries between larger forms such as movements and sections.
In addition to phrase boundaries, the perception of melodic groups is
influenced by other auditory cues such as pitch proximity and timbral sim-
ilarity of notes (for a review, see Deutsch, 1999a). Such cues not only influ-
ence perceived connections between adjacent notes in a melody, they can
also induce perceived connections between salient or significant notes across
several melodic groups, leading to the perception of higher-level melodic
structures. That is, melodic groups are perceived at multiple hierarchic levels,
and excerpts with the same higher-level structure are perceived as similar
despite differences in surface structure (Serafine, Glassman, & Overbeeke,
1989).
Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983) proposed a comprehensive and influential
theory of melody that emphasized its hierarchic structure. They first noted
that Western tonal melodies can be analyzed into essential notes and orna-
mental notes. Ornamental notes are notes in the melody that can be removed
without altering the essential character of the melody. After ornamental
notes are removed, what is left is a simplified version of that melody. With
this idea as a starting point, Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983) used the term
reductional structure to describe how large-scale melodic structures can
be analyzed into simpler and simpler skeletons. They proposed two ways in
which melodies can be simplified.
In time-span reduction, certain beats and groups in a melody are desig-
nated as ornaments on other groups and beats. This simplification process
is continued in an interactive manner, resulting in a tree diagram that spec-
ifies the relative dominance of each event. In prolongation reduction, pri-
LISTENING TO MUSIC 95
Melodic Expectancies
As a listener hears a melody, she continuously forms expectancies about
upcoming notes she will hear. Sometimes we expect a particular note, but
usually our expectations are not so specific. Rather, some notes seem rela-
tively likely, whereas other notes seem less likely. Learning and exposure to
musical styles exert a large influence on such expectancies. Is there a role
for basic cognitive processes in the formation of melodic expectancies?
According to Narmour (1990), the answer is yes. His theory of melodic
expectancy, called the Implication-Realization Model, posits that listeners
expectancies are formed from a combination of bottom-up and top-down
factors (see Thompson, 1996, for a reivew). In Narmours use of these terms,
bottom-up refers to innate or hardwired cognitive and perceptual tenden-
cies; top-down refers to expectancies that result from experience, either
with music in general, or with a particular musical piece. As examples, pitch
proximity (expecting notes that are proximal in pitch) is considered to be
an innate influence on melodic expectancy, whereas tonality (expecting notes
in proportion to their importance in the scale) is thought to be based on
long-term knowledge of Western music.
Two experimental methods have been used to test expectancies. In one
method, listeners hear a fragment of a melody followed by a test tone. Their
task is to rate how well the test tone continues the fragment. The assumption
is that tones conforming to listeners expectancies will be rated as relatively
good continuations. The other method is a production task, in which par-
ticipants play or sing a continuation to a stimulus provided by the experi-
menter. The main finding from all of the relevant studies is that proximity
explains the lions share of the variance in listeners responses, regardless of
experimental method, age, musical style, and participants music training or
cultural background (e.g., Cuddy & Lunney, 1995; Schellenberg, 1996,
1997; Schellenberg, Adachi, Purdy, & McKinnon, 2002; Thompson, Cuddy,
& Plaus, 1997). In general, tones that are proximate in pitch to the last tone
heard are the most expected (see von Hippel, 2000). This association is
more-or-less linear, such that tones farther and farther away from the last
tone heard receive lower and lower ratings, or are less likely to be sung or
played.
Narmour (1990) proposed several bottom-up principles of melodic ex-
pectancy. In an examination of Narmours theory, however, Schellenberg
96 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Western Harmony
The term harmony refers to rules and conventions governing the simul-
taneous combination of tones into chords and chord progressions (sequences
of chords). Although music from other cultures allows for more than one
tone to be presented simultaneously, Western music is unique in its regular
use of chords and rules governing chord progressions (i.e., monophonic mu-
sic and solo singing are relatively rare). Because Western harmony is specific
to Western music, its rules must be learned, either by regular music listening
or by formal training in music. Accordingly, one might ask when listeners
have acquired knowledge of the rules of Western harmony as a function of
age and/or exposure to music.
In Western harmony, a sense of finality (i.e., closure) is expressed typically
by having the tonic chord (the chord based on do) as the final event in a
phrase or piece. Listeners with musical training are aware of this convention
and they can identify the tonic chord when listening to a musical piece. By
contrast, untrained listeners have no knowledge of tonic chords or harmonic
rules, and they cannot articulate explicitly what they know about harmony.
Nonetheless, they may have acquired extensive but implicit knowledge of
harmony through years of exposure to Western music. In order to examine
listeners implicit knowledge of harmony, we can ask them to make an un-
related judgment about the final chord of a musical piece, such as whether
it is sung with one of two vowels (e.g., /i/ or /u/), or played on one of two
instruments (e.g., piano or trumpet). The listener is unaware that the har-
monic function of the final chord is also being manipulated (i.e., either a
tonic chord or a less stable chord in the key of the piece). If untrained
listeners have implicit knowledge of harmony, they should expect a piece to
end on the tonic. Accordingly, accuracy and response speed on the task they
are asked to perform may improve when the final chord is consistent with
their implicitly formed expectation.
Across a wide variety of implicit tasks, untrained listeners show a per-
formance advantage when the final chord is the tonicin accuracy, response
speed, or both accuracy and speed (for a review see Bigand & Poulin-
Charronnat, 2005). Musically trained and untrained participants show
similar advantages for tonic chords, which indicates that the acquisition of
implicit knowledge of Western harmony does not require formal training in
music. In a developmental study of children who varied both in age and in
amount of music lessons (Schellenberg, Bigand, Poulin, Garnier, & Stevens,
2005), 6-year-olds with no music lessons showed a performance advantage
for tonic chords that was similar in magnitude to that of 11-year-olds who
were studying music at a conservatory. These findings suggest that acquisi-
tion of implicit knowledge for Western harmony is rapid and effortless,
LISTENING TO MUSIC 99
such that some aspects of this knowledge are relatively mature by 6 years
of age.
Implied Harmony
Because melodies are so often heard with a harmonic accompaniment, lis-
teners gradually learn to associate isolated melodies with plausible harmonic
accompaniments, even when none is present. Consider the melody illustrated
in figure 3.7 (from Trainor & Trehub, 1992, 1994). The melody implies a
shift from a tonic harmony in the first measure to a dominant harmony in
the second measure. Musically trained listeners are highly sensitive to this
implied harmony, and consistently identify the implied harmonic change. Are
untrained listeners also sensitive to these shifts in implied harmony? If so,
how does sensitivity to implied harmony change over development?
In one experiment (Trainor & Trehub, 1992), adults and 8-month-olds
were tested on their ability to discriminate alterations to the melody shown
in figure 3.7, which was presented repeatedly in varying transpositions. In
each case, the alteration consisted of an upward displacement to the sixth
tone in the melody. For some listeners, the displacement was a shift upward
by 1 semitone (e.g., from G to G#), which is a small alteration in terms of
interval size but inconsistent with the implied harmony. For other listeners,
the displacement was a shift upward by 4 semitones (e.g., from G to B),
which is a much larger change in interval size but consistent with the implied
dominant harmony. Adult listeners found the former changewhich vio-
lated the implied harmonyeasier to detect than the latter changewhich
was consistent with the implied harmony. Infant listeners performed equally
well in both conditions. Interestingly, infant listeners actually outperformed
adults at detecting the larger but harmonically consistent shift in pitch. In
other words, adult listeners appear to have a very well developed sense of
implied harmony that they acquire through years of exposure to music, such
that changes in a melody that are consistent with the implied harmony are
relatively unnoticeable. By contrast, infants appear to be less sensitive to
implied harmony.
When does sensitivity to implied harmony develop? A follow-up experi-
ment (Trainor & Trehub, 1994) addressed this question by testing listeners
5 and 7 years of age. The children were asked to detect the 1-semitone
harmony-violating change, the 4-semitone harmony-consistent change, and
an intermediate 2-semitone shift (e.g., from G to A) which was consistent
with the underlying key signature (C major) but violated the implied har-
mony. The 5-year-olds found the 1-semitone change easier to detect than the
other changes. In other words, the shift that violated both the implied har-
mony and the underlying key signature was more noticeable than the other
shifts. For the 7-year-olds, the 2-semitone shift that violated the implied
harmony but not the key was easier to detect than the 4-semitone harmon-
ically consistent shift. These data, considered in conjunction with those from
the previous study, indicate a systematic developmental progression. Infant
listeners have a relatively poor sense of key. By 5 years of age, children are
sensitive to key membership but not to implied harmony. By 7 years of age,
children are sensitive to implied harmony.
Tonality
Tonality is a basic organizing principle in Western tonal music. It refers to
the organization of the twelve pitches in the chromatic pitch set around a
reference pitch, called the tonic. The establishment of tonality, also called
key, involves inducing a hierarchical organization of stability or importance
on the chromatic pitch set. In this hierarchy, the tonic pitch (do) is most
stable, followed by other pitches of the tonic triad (mi and sol), followed by
other pitches in the scale (diatonic tones). Pitches that are not in the scale
(nondiatonic tones) are perceived to be unstable.
A number of landmark studies have been conducted to investigate the
effects of tonality on judgments of pitch (for a review, see Krumhansl, 1990).
Krumhansl and her colleagues used the probe-tone technique to investi-
gate the psychological implications of tonality (Krumhansl & Kessler, 1982;
Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979). The method involves presentation of a mu-
sical stimulus that clearly defines a musical key (e.g., scale, cadence, chord
sequence) followed by a probe tone. Listeners rate how well each probe
tone in the chromatic scale fits with the established key. When musically
trained listeners are tested with this method, probe-tone ratings are quite
consistent across listeners and reflect a hierarchical organization that has
become known as the tonal hierarchy.
The tonal hierarchy is more easily uncovered with musically trained than
LISTENING TO MUSIC 101
Rhythm
to adjust their phase and period to external periodicities, units with natural
resonances closest to existing periodicities lock on and track the tempo (see
also McAuley & Jones, 2003). Related to this issue is the question of how
simple patterns (i.e., small-integer ratios of duration) are represented in
memory when the durations and periodicities of actual performances are
highly variable. In order to account for this quantization problem, Desains
(1992) connectionist model assumes that rhythmic experiences are biased
towards temporal patterns defined by small integer ratios.
Some researchers have examined the extent to which pitch and rhythm
interact in perception and memory. This question may be relevant to music
educators because pitch and rhythmic skills are often assessed separately in
traditional pedagogical exercises, implying distinct cognitive mechanisms for
processing these dimensions. Music theory also suggests a separation be-
tween rhythm and pitch. For example, the Schenkerian reduction technique
for analyzing tonal works assumes that surface temporal relations operate
independently of pitch structure.
Results on this issue suggest that pitch and rhythm interact at some levels
of processing, but operate independently at other levels. Jones, Boltz and
Kidd (1982) reported that rhythmically accented tones in a melody are better
recognized than unaccented tones. Similarly, if pitch and rhythm patterns
imply different metrical groupings, memory for melodic sequences is gen-
erally poor (Boltz & Jones, 1986; Deutsch, 1980). Such findings suggest that
pitch and rhythm are integrated in memory for music. Additional evidence
for non-independence of pitch and rhythm processing comes from a study
that required listeners to rate the emotionality (i.e., happiness, sadness, scar-
iness) of melodies (Schellenberg, Krysciak, & Campbell, 2001). The melo-
dies pitch and rhythmic properties proved to be interactive in their influence
on listeners ratings.
Other researchers contend, however, that pitch and rhythm are processed
independently. In some experiments, pitch and rhythm have made statisti-
cally independent contributions to judgments of melodic similarity (Mona-
han & Carterette, 1985), and to ratings of phrase completion (Palmer &
Krumhansl, 1987a, 1987b). Moreover, memory for the pitch and rhythm
patterns of a short melody is far better than memory for how those features
are combined. Thompson (1994) presented listeners with two test melodies
followed by two comparison melodies. The task was to indicate if the two
comparison melodies were identical to the two test melodies, disregarding
the order in which the melodies occurred. In one condition, participants
performed a distractor task as the melodies were presented. In another con-
dition, participants listened attentively to the melodies. When the compari-
son melodies involved a novel pitch pattern or a rhythm that was not present
in the test melodies, performance was highly accurate for participants in
both conditions. When the comparison melodies were constructed by com-
bining the pitch pattern of one test melody with the rhythm of the other test
melody (or vice versa), however, distracted participants performed poorly.
LISTENING TO MUSIC 109
Timbre
Figure 3.8. Transient attributes of sound. Frequency is displayed as a function of time and
intensity.
mation. Note that the excerpts were too brief to convey any relational
information (re: pitch and duration), and that the absolute pitch of the ex-
cerpts would be identical whether they were played backward or forward.
Thus, successful performance presumably stemmed from accurate and de-
tailed memory of the recordings timbres, which consisted of many complex
tones from many different instruments.
By definition, timbre and pitch are different. Nonetheless, some studies
suggest that timbre and pitch are not perceived independently. For example,
it is easier to judge whether two tones (Crowder, 1989), two chords (Beal,
1985), or two melodies (Radvansky, Fleming, & Simmons, 1995) are the
same when standard and comparison stimuli are played on identical rather
than different musical instruments. This finding suggests thatdespite our
experience of pitch and timbre as distinct musical qualitiesthey interact
with each other in music processing.
Krumhansl and Iverson (1993) used another approach to study interac-
tions between pitch and timbre. Participants were asked to classify stimulus
items into two groups as quickly as possible based on a specified target
attribute (e.g., pitch). Not only did the target attribute vary; an irrelevant
attribute (e.g., timbre) also varied from trial to trial. Performance on the
categorization task was relatively good when variation in the irrelevant at-
tribute was correlated with variation in the target attribute, but relatively
poor when variation in the irrelevant attribute was uncorrelated with vari-
ation in the target attribute. That is, subjects were unable to attend to the
pitch of a tone without being affected by its timbre, or vice versa. This
finding provides converging evidence that pitch and timbre are not percep-
tually separable from each other.
Although pitch and timbre may be integrated successfully at some points
in the information-processing system, listeners can also become confused
about the ways in which pitches and timbres are combined. In one experi-
ment, participants listened for particular combinations of pitch and timbre
in arrays of tones that were presented simultaneously but emanated from
different locations in the auditory field (Hall, Pastore, Acker, & Huang,
2000). An examination of errors indicated that participants often perceived
an illusory conjunction of pitch and timbre. That is, participants often heard
the timbre of one tone combined with the pitch of another tone. Estimates
of illusory conjunction rate ranged from 23% to 40%. The findings provide
evidence that after an initial stage in which individual features of musical
tones are registered (e.g., pitch, timbre), there is a stage in which these sep-
arately registered features are integrated (feature integration). Illusory con-
junctions arise when errors occur at the feature-integration stage of proc-
essing.
What is the relation between timbre and harmony? The principle of har-
monicity, outlined by Bregman (1990), suggests that partials that fall along
the harmonic series of a single note are fused together, such that listeners
perceive the distinct timbral qualities of each note holistically. When two
instruments play the same note, or two different notes with many harmonics
114 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
in common, there may be some confusion in this process. Such fusion effects
actually contribute to our perception of harmony. For example, if one plays
two piano notes separated by a fifth, those two notes have several partials
in common. Perceptual mechanisms will tend to fuse or con-fuse some of
the components of the two different notes, giving rise to emergent timbral
effects. Compositions by Debussy exemplify the varied timbral effects of
combining tones.
Although pitch and rhythmic relations are usually considered to define
musical structure, some contemporary composers and theorists (e.g., Ler-
dahl, 1987; Slawson, 1985) have considered the possibility that timbre might
be used compositionally in a way that is analogous to the use of pitch. One
challenge with this compositional approach is that timbre has a powerful
influence on auditory stream segregation. Thus, any large shift in timbre is
likely to signal a separate auditory stream, which would conflict with the
goal of creating a coherent sequence analogous to a melody. Nonetheless,
changes in timbre that correspond to reductions in sensory dissonance (i.e.,
fewer harmonics falling within a critical band)and increases in fusion
may be perceived as a change from tension to release, which commonly
occurs at points of musical closure (Pressnitzer, McAdams, Winsberg, &
Fineberg, 2000). In other words, changes from relatively dissonant to rela-
tively consonant timbres have some of the resolving properties of a perfect
cadence. Moreover, it is clear that timbre has a profound influence on our
appreciation of music. For example, imagine a piano piece composed by
Chopin performed by a horn section, or a song by U2 played on an accor-
dion. Such changes in instrumentation would undoubtedly affect our expe-
rience of the music.
Conclusion
STUDY QUESTIONS
1. Debates in music cognition tend to center on a small number of con-
tentious issues. Discuss two of these issues.
2. Helmholtz observed that certain aspects of music have parallels in
physical acoustics. Provide a critical review of his observations.
3. Summarize and evaluate an example of empirical evidence that
sensitivity to sensory consonance and dissonance is innate.
4. Describe the research that led to the Mel scale, and discuss its signif-
icance.
5. Describe recent research suggesting that the skill of absolute pitch
may be more prevalent than previously believed.
6. Why is melodic contour so easy to remember?
7. Describe similarities and differences between music and speech pros-
ody, and discuss the implications of this comparison.
8. Describe the two ways that melodies are simplified according to Ler-
dahl and Jackendoffs theory of reductional structure.
9. When does sensitivity to implied harmony develop? What evidence
was used to reach this conclusion?
10. Discuss how general principles of auditory scene analysis might play
a critical role in voice-leading practices.
11. Discuss evidence for and against the view that pitch and rhythm are
processed independently.
12. Outline the major acoustic properties that influence the perception
of timbre.
REFERENCES
Allen, D. (1967). Octave discriminability of musical and non-musical subjects.
Psychonomic Science, 7, 421422.
Auhagen, W. & Vos, P. G. (2000). Experimental methods in tonality induction
research: A review. Music Perception, 17, 417436.
Balkwill, L. L. & Thompson, W. F. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of the
perception of emotion in music: Psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Per-
ception, 17(1), 4364.
Balkwill, L-L & Thompson, W. F. & Matsunaga, R. (2004). Recognition of emo-
tion in Japanese, Western, and Hindustani music by Japanese listeners. Jap-
anese Psychological Research, 46(4), 337349.
LISTENING TO MUSIC 117
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality
or myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399442.
Humphreys, L. F. (1939). Generalization as a function of method of reinforce-
ment. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 361372.
Husain, G., Thompson, W. F. & Schellenberg, E. G. (2002). Effects of musical
tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities: Re-examination of
the Mozart effect. Music Perception, 20(20), 151171.
Huron, D. (2001). Tone and voice: A derivation of the rules of voice leading
from perceptual principles. Music Perception, 19(1), 164.
Huron, D. (1991a). The avoidance of part-crossing in polyphonic music: Per-
ceptual evidence and musical practice. Music Perception, 9, 93104.
Huron, D. (1991b). Tonal consonance versus tonal fusion in polyphonic sonor-
ities. Music Perception, 9, 135154.
Ilie, G. & Thompson, W. F. (in press). A comparison of acoustic cues in music
and speech for three dimensions of affect, Music Perception.
Iverson, P. (1995). Auditory stream segregation by musical timbre: Effects of
static and dynamic acoustic attributes. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 21, 751763.
Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attention and responses to time.
Psychological Review, 96, 459491.
Kallman, H. J. (1982). Octave equivalence as measured by similarity ratings.
Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 3749.
Kronman, U., & Sundberg, J. (1987). Is the musical ritard an allusion to physical
motion? In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and Perception in Rhythm and Music
(pp. 5768). Stockholm: Publications issued by the Royal Swedish Academy
of Music, No. 55.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1995). Music psychology and music theory: Problems and
prospects. Music Theory Spectrum, 17, 5380.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1979). The psychological representation of musical pitch in
a tonal context. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 346374.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford: Ox-
ford University Press.
Krumhansl, C. L. (2000a). Rhythm and pitch in music cognition. Psychological
Bulletin, 126, 159179.
Krumhansl, C. L. (2000b). Tonality induction: A statistical approach applied
cross-culturally. Music Perception, 17, 461480.
Krumhansl, C. L., & Iverson, P. (1992). Perceptual interactions between musical
pitch and timbre. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 18, 739751.
Krumhansl, C. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1990). Infants perception of phrase struc-
ture in music. Psychological Science, 1, 7073.
Krumhansl, C. L., & Keil, F. C. (1982). Acquisition of the hierarchy of tonal
functions in music. Memory & Cognition, 10, 243251.
Krumhansl, C. L., & Kessler, E. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in per-
ceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psycho-
logical Review, 89, 334368.
Krumhansl, C. L., & Shepard, R. N. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of
tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 579594.
120 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track
time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119159.
Leahey, T. H. (1997). A history of psychology (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Leman, M. (2000). An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in
probe-tone ratings. Music Perception, 17, 481510.
Lerdahl, F. (1987). Timbral hierarchies. Contemporary Music Review, 2, 135
160.
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Levitin, D. J. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the
production of learned melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 414423.
Levitin, D. J. (1999). Memory for musical attributes. In P. R. Cook (Ed.) Music,
cognition, and computerized sound (pp. 209227). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Levitin, D. J., & Cook, P. R. (1996). Memory for musical tempo: Additional
evidence that auditory memory is absolute. Perception & Psychophysics, 58,
927935.
Levitin, D. J. & Rogers, S. E. (2005). Absolute pitch: perception, coding, and
controversies. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(1), 2633.
McAdams, S. (2004a). Problem-solving strategies in music composition: A case
study. Music Perception, 21, 391429.
McAdams, S. (2004b). Prolog. In D. J. Levitin (Ed.) The Angel of Death Project.
Music Perception, 22, 171172.
McAuley, J. D., & Jones, M. R. (2003). Modeling Effects of Rhythmic Context
on Perceived Duration: A Comparison of Interval and Entrainment Ap-
proaches to Short-Interval Timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Hu-
man Perception and Performance, 29, 11021125.
Meyer, L. (1973). Explaining music: Essays and explorations. Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits
on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.
Miyazaki, K. (1993). Absolute pitch as an inability: Identification of musical
intervals in a tonal context. Music Perception, 11, 5572.
Morton, D. (1976). The traditional music of Thailand. Los Angeles: University
of California Press.
Narmour, E. (1990). The analysis and cognition of basic melodic structures.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Narmour, E. (1992). The analysis and cognition of melodic complexity. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Oram, N., & Cuddy, L. L. (1995). Responsiveness of Western adults to pitch-
distributional information in melodic sequences. Psychological Research, 57,
103118.
Palmer, C. & Krumhansl, C. L. (1987a). Independent temporal and pitch struc-
tures in determination of musical phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: Human Perception & Performance, 13, 116126.
Palmer, C. & Krumhansl, C. L. (1987b). Pitch and temporal contributions to
musical phrase perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 116126.
Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in
musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409464.
LISTENING TO MUSIC 121
Areas of Research
Neurobiological Research
In neurobiological research, the development of visual image-mapping pro-
cedures (e.g., magnetic resonance imaging [MRI], positron emission tomog-
raphy [PET]) provided new evidence for neurobiological correlates of mu-
sical activities (see also chap. 2, by Gruhn and Rauscher, in this volume).
Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, Staiger, and Steinmetz (1995) showed that the
planum temporale, a brain area in the left temple region, is anatomically
more developed in musicians, especially those with absolute pitch, than in
nonmusicians. It has also been shown that the corpus callosum, which con-
nects the two hemispheres of the brain, is morphologically more highly de-
veloped in musicians than nonmusicians. Another study group found that
the specific area of the motor cortex that coordinates the movements of the
left hand is more anatomically extended in string players than in nonmusi-
cians (Elbert, Pantev, Wienbruch, Rockstroh, & Taub, 1995). This enlarge-
ment, correlated with the age at which the persons started to play their
instruments, suggests that the anatomical or neurological/neurobiological
differences between musicians and nonmusicians are a result of musical
training and development and that these differences may, in turn, be a factor
in any variations in musical development (Schlaug, 2001; Pascual-Leone,
2001).
126 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Expertise Research
Expertise research focuses on the crucial role of deliberate practice for the
development of musical abilities (see chap. 7, by Andreas C. Lehmann and
Jane W. Davidson, in this volume). From this point of view, expert perfor-
mance can be seen as a result of the accumulated hours of training and
deliberate practice over a lifetime. After the acquisition of expert perfor-
mance in a number of areas like sports and chess was addressed, the exper-
tise approach was successfully adapted to the domain of music (e.g., Erics-
son, Krampe, & Tesch-Romer, 1993; Krampe, 1994; Jrgensen & Lehmann,
1997). Recently the expertise paradigm has also become a helpful approach
to study other levels of instrumental performance (Sloboda & Davidson,
1996; Sloboda, Davidson, Moore, & Howe, 1994). On the other hand, there
are clear limitations of the expertise approach. For instance, the motivation
for practice or interindividual differences in the effects of practice remain
open to question. Furthermore, the acquisition of expertise in composing
and in musical creativity seems to differ from that in playing an instrument.
While highly trained processes and automated routines are fundamental for
high performance on piano, violin or other instruments, it is exactly these
elements that are counterproductive to creativity (Simonton 2000). Expertise
research, with its focus on environmental variables, and behavioral genetics
and neurobiological research, with its emphasis on innate capacities, to-
gether have stimulated the old naturenurture debate.
General Assumptions
Individual Differences
It is well documented that large developmental differences in music abilities
already exist at an early age and in adolescence. They are caused by differ-
ences in giftedness and talent and the sociocultural background as well as
by lessons, motivation, and practice (e.g., Kelly & Sutton-Smith, 1987). Dur-
ing adulthood, these individual differences, for example, among profession-
als, amateurs, and people untrained in music, become even more pronounced
due to commitment and practice in the case of professionals and lack of use
in nonmusicians. Therefore, research on musical development, especially de-
velopment in adulthood, requires a strong differential perspective. One ap-
proach distinguishes between normative and specialist development
(Hargreaves, 1996, p. 150). Normative development is that which naturally
happens to children as they grow up in a given culture regardless of any
specialized attention or guidance, while specialist development and spe-
cialist education is consciously devoted to the development of high levels
of musical skill or expertise (Hargreaves, 1996, p. 150). Even though Har-
greaves advises against the strict separation of these two aspects, further
distinctions seem to be necessary. Different aspects also become obvious if
the careers and the development of musicians in jazz, rock, and popular
music are compared with those in classical music. In such nontraditional
domains there is a lack of theoretical concepts to describe the musical de-
velopment.
Cross-Generational Differences
Since musical development is culturally and historically embedded, it is af-
fected by the changing historical and cultural surroundings, especially the
changes in musical culture. Childrens musical development depends much
on the current musical culture, which can differ largely from their parents
culture. This becomes obvious when one compares the musical develop-
ment of the generation that grew up with rock and popular music as a
main influence to that of the generation before. Hence, contemporary his-
tory can affect musical development (Gembris, 1997). The rise of new me-
dia technology (e.g., computers and the internet) and the emergence of new
musical styles contribute to an increasing variety of musical development in
the fields of composition, performance, listening, and preferences.
Therefore, parents and teachers should be aware that the childrens and stu-
dents musical development may differ considerably from their own. Cor-
respondingly, research on musical development should broaden its focus on
traditional Western art music to include all styles (e.g., Hemming & Klei-
nen, 1999).
130 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
The basic musical abilities that are required for participation in ones own
musical culture are developed in the first 10 years of life. This process of
enculturation takes place automatically and subconsciously, without the
need of formal instruction. Musical stimulations that arise from the individ-
ual social and cultural environment seem to be most important. A musically
rich social environment can lead to certain musical abilities being acquired
earlier than in a social environment with less musical activity (Kelly &
Sutton-Smith, 1987). But the development of basic musical abilities can also
be positively influenced through teaching. Morrongiello (1992) observed
that musical practice leads to more precise perception of melodies, better
musical memory, and an earlier sensitivity for keys. Other studies confirm
that the acquisition of certain musical abilities can be accelerated through
teaching; however, students without lessons do catch up after a while (e.g.,
Behne, 1974). The possibility to accelerate the development of basic musical
abilities through lessons may be constrained by their connection with general
mental, emotional, and sensorimotor developmental processes (Gembris &
Davidson, 2002). Note that since large individual differences can occur in
both general and musical development, the age-related details that follow
are points of reference, and divergent ages cannot easily be interpreted as
indicating unusual talent or developmental retardation.
Prenatal Development
The onset of musical learning and musical memory occurs before birth. The
nerve cells of the inner ear start to work in the fifth or sixth month of
pregnancy and respond to midrange frequencies. Approximately in the sev-
enth month of gestation, the fetus perceives extrauterine acoustic stimuli.
When children are born, they have already had some acoustic experiences.
An experiment by Feijoo (1981) showed that babies stopped crying when
they listened to short melodies that had been presented to them over a period
of 4 weeks during the sixth to eighth month of pregnancy. Satt (1984) pro-
vided evidence for the ability of 16 newborns to recognize a lullaby that
they had heard every day during the last 2 months of pregnancy. Hepper
(1991) demonstrated the ability of infants just a few days old to recognize
the main tune of a television series that they already heard before birth.
Another study showed that newborns seemed to remember single words
from a story read out loud by their mothers during the last 6 weeks of
pregnancy (DeCaspar & Spence, 1986). These results demonstrate that re-
actions to music must already occur during prenatal development. However,
the methods used in these experiments still offer no reliable distinction be-
tween direct reactions of the child and indirect reactions influenced by the
mothers reaction.
THE DEVELOPMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES 131
The fact that unborn children can react to music has been taken up by
the music industry, which offers a number of CDs especially designed for
pregnant women. These products supposedly have a positive influence on
the health of the mother and the unborn child. Experiments with animals
have demonstrated that prenatal auditory deprivation leads to a retarded
development, while careful acoustic enrichment seemed to improve the struc-
ture and function of auditory tracts in the fetus (Lecanuet, 1996, p. 18).
Melody Perception. Growing research over the last 20 years supports the
idea that infants sensations are not just vague and mellow; instead, their
reactions can be interpreted as recognizing behavior (see Fassbender, 1996,
for a review). By the age of 6 months, infants are capable of distinguishing
several short melodies with the melodic contour as the most important dis-
tinguishing feature. At this age they cannot yet recognize absolute pitch or
specific intervals (Dowling, 1988, 1999). Most of the present studies indicate
that melodic perception in its early stages aligns itself with the melodic con-
tour and moves on to more specific details with increasing age (Dowling,
1999). Lamonts (1998) experiments demonstrate that informal musical ex-
132 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Early studies in the 1960s already demonstrated that newborns are able to
distinguish between regular and irregular heartbeat or click sounds (Spiegler,
THE DEVELOPMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES 133
Harmony and Tonality. Even though findings about the development of to-
nal sensitivity are not consistent throughout, a clear tendency can be ob-
served: More recent studies record younger ages than did previous studies
for harmonic perception and the preference for consonance over dissonance.
Based on experiments from 1913, Valentine (1962) found that children were
able to distinguish between major and minor at the age of 9 and those with
intensive musical training between the ages of 5 and 7. Imberty (1969) first
noticed this ability in children at the age of 7, while it can be observed at
the age of 5 and younger today. Again, the increased influence of the mass
media could account for the fact that children become accustomed to the
Western major-minor tonal system at an earlier age, which in turn may lead
to an acceleration in the identification of harmonies (Zenatti, 1993, p. 192).
With the use of refined research methods, these effects could possibly be
observed at even younger ages.
In a number of experiments with 5- to 10-year-olds, Zenatti (1993) ex-
amined the musical preference for tonality versus atonality and consonance
versus dissonance. The children listened to pairs of musical stimuli (e.g., a
tonal and an atonal melody) presented from tape and were asked which
melody, rhythm, or chords of the two versions they preferred. Children
younger than 5 were still at chance level; that is, they had no clear prefer-
ence. However, at the age of 6 the preference for consonant harmonization
increased as rapidly as the dislike for atonal melodies and dissonance. By
the age of 9 or 10 the preference for tonality and consonance was between
90 and 95% (cf. Zenatti, 1993, p. 182; also Minkenberg, 1991, for sup-
porting results). This suggests that enculturation into the Western tonal sys-
tem is virtually concluded at that age. As a result, the openness to contem-
porary music or music from non-Western cultures constantly decreases after
the age of 5 or 6. An educational countermeasure could be presenting more
nontonal music to preschool children and indicating to them that it can be
enjoyed just the same as familiar music.
sad. Similar effects can be found through use of prosody in language (Tre-
hub, Trainor, & Unyk, 1993) or the nonverbal interactions between infants
and their parents (see earlier). It can be concluded that there must exist some
prototypical gestures or movements of acoustic expression that are indepen-
dent from tonality and common to both language and music. Interestingly,
the Italian composer Claudio Monteverdi (15671643) used the prosody of
the language as a basis for his stile concitato compositions to increase the
affective power of his music.
rhythmic, melodic, or tonal changes had been made (Pflederer, 1964; Pfle-
derer & Sechrest, 1968). This ability to coordinate different aspects of music,
that is, to conserve, increases with age, although, as Nelson (1987, p. 26)
found, it does not necessarily develop at the same time in different domains.
Other studies provided evidence for an intermediate stage, where children
can perceive but not coordinate more than one dimension at a time (Flam-
mer, 1996, p. 129). For example, children at this age are mostly able to
identify transposed melodies, but they have difficulties describing pitch re-
lations with the right words, like high and low (Flowers & Costa-Giomi,
1991; also Shuter-Dyson, 1982, p. 75). As a consequence, these abstract
notions should not be used for pre-school-age music education and rather
be replaced by metaphorical pictures.
ticulation of syllables with vowels that are slightly prolonged and appear on
stable pitches. The author also observed a sequential organization of the
song, referring to more or less stabilized tonal patterns. According to Dowl-
ing, the first actual singing can be observed between the 6th and the 18th
month of life. At first, these glissando-like improvisations on single syllables
occupy only a narrow pitch range. Later they turn into recognizable songs,
often with a sequential organization of the sounds. Thus a typical song of
an 18-month-old-child consists of an often-repeated phrase with a steady
melodic contour at a continuously changing level of pitch. The song is quite
often interrupted by breathing; however, the rhythmical contour remains
within the phrase and sometimes even stretches over several phrases. These
songs are often derived from the rhythm of language.
In their second year of life, children are able to sing single short phrases
of a song, frequently turning them into spontaneous improvisations and re-
peating them quite often. Microtonal figures in spontaneous singing slowly
make way for more accurate intervals, resulting in an overall impression
that is clearly related to the diatonic system (cf. Moog, 1976). Between the
ages of 3 and 4, children combine different songs and song fragments into
something like a medley. They can repeat songs they hear and increase
phrase contour of the presented song by trial and error. Other researchers
observed that children could reproduce all of the lyrics, the main rhythms,
and also the formal segments of a song starting at the age of 4 (cf. Shuter-
Dyson & Gabriel, 1981, p. 117).
Children will have acquired the singing range of an octave with all its
steps once they are 6 or 7 years old (Davidson, 1994; Minkenberg, 1991).
Although they still might miss certain pitches, this does not mean they are
unable to recognize the pitches (Goetze, Cooper, & Brown, 1990). The de-
velopment of the ability to sing comes to an end around the age of 8 years.
Generally, by this time children are able to sing a song correctly. This ability
remains at this level unless music instruction and practice follow. As always,
however, a broad range of interindividual differences are observable. The
singing abilities of untrained adults are not much different from those of 8-
to 10-year-old children (see Davidson, 1994; Davidson & Scripp, 1990,
p. 66; Minkenberg, 1991; Stadler Elmer, 2000; 2002, for further details).
erties of the harmonic series (e.g., Metzler, 1962; Werner, 1917). The third
group could be labeled contour theories. Proponents of this approach ad-
vance the notion that the learning process begins with the melodic contour
and that pitch and tonality follow. Learning to sing is thus assumed to pro-
ceed from global to more local features. According to Davidsons (1994)
theory of contour schemata, the development of a contour schema starts
with a falling third into which the other intervals are placed. The contour
schema will then expand with the childs age: at first to a fourth and up to
a sixth, which is mastered by the age of around 6 or 7. One author, however,
raises a number of critical arguments against all of these theories and sug-
gests the following course of development (Stadler Elmer, 2000):
class, peers, and mass media (see Zillmann & Gan, 1997, for an overview).
It is difficult to determine the degree of relevance of each of the factors, since
their importance changes over the lifetime and may even vary depending on
the context. In general, the influence of parents and school decreases during
adolescence, at the same time when the importance of peers and media in-
creases. Furthermore, the degree of certain influences is determined by a
persons social class.
We should also realize that adolescents not only are passively influenced
by their sociocultural environment in the development of their musical pref-
erences but also are rather actively choosing and shaping their own envi-
ronment. This view of self-socialization is a relatively recent one and has
been adapted to music by Muller (1999). Musical self-socialization takes
place in an unhampered fashion far away from the influences of formal
education, preferably within the youth culture.
attitudes dramatically change between the 10th and 20th year of life. Around
this time, musical preferences separate from the taste of parents and teachers.
The close connections to the adult musical world that exist during childhood
are replaced by the desire to establish ones own musical taste and world.
In this context, the taste of peers plays an important role and also the music
industrys current products and the identification with specific musical styles,
groups, idols, and stars.
It is typical for teenagers to change their preferences quickly and to ex-
perience music with strong accompanying emotions (Behne, 1986). This be-
comes manifest in the strong enthusiasm for their currently preferred music
and a correspondingly strong rejection of other musical styles. Because their
preferences mirror the strong emotional impact of the music, adolescents are
often quite adamant when it comes to defending their current preferences
(Behne, 1997, p. 149). This close emotional relationship does not exist dur-
ing childhood or adulthood to nearly the same degree.
As adolescence is relatively short compared to the entire life span, it is
the time in which the greatest changes of musical preferences occur. This
period is most important for the musical development not only because of
the dramatic changes in preferences but also because the behavioral patterns
of listening acquired by the end of adolescence will remain prevalent for
adulthood (Behne, 1986, p. 56; Dollase, Rusenberg, & Stollenwerk, 1986,
p. 183; Holbrook & Schindler, 1989; Lehmann, 1994). A distinctive pref-
erence for the music of ones youth can be quite frequently observed at a
much older age (e.g., Jonas, 1991). A study of Holbrook and Schindler
(1989) demonstrated that preferences of adults from different generational
cohorts were clearly related to the music that was popular when they were
about 24 years old.
Behne, 1997, and Finnas, 1989). Thus it is entirely possible for a student to
generally dislike a genre like opera but, when confronted with a recording
of, say, Che gelida manina from Puccinis La Boheme, be quite touched.
K.-E. Behne (1986) carried out an investigation into the verbal and lis-
tening (behavioral) preferences of over 1,000 students aged 10 to 22. He
observed that different classical music styles, when presented verbally (word
labels), were viewed as a homogenous complex by the majority of the ad-
olescents. However, when actual examples of classical music styles were pre-
sented (listening preferences), classical music was not rejected as a whole.
Among the various youth subcultures there are those in which classical
music is rated highest on preference scale (e.g., Bastian, 1989, p. 236). Nat-
urally, this group represents a minority. Still, some music-listening studies
have demonstrated a surprising tolerance for different classical music genres
among younger and older people as well as openness to the oldies but
goldies of popular music among older people (Gembris, 1995).
Gender Differences
In general, there seem to exist hardly any differences between girls and boys
regarding musical instruments or musical styles during childhood (Maidlow
THE DEVELOPMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES 145
& Bruce, 1999). However, the increase in preference for popular music dur-
ing adolescence seems to be more pronounced for boys than for girls; that
is, girls dislikes of nonpopular music are less strong than those of the boys.
Also, girls seem to like a broader range of classical music (Hargreaves,
Comber, & Colley, 1996). The authors explain this with the fact that girls
generally take more instrumental lessons and are thus ahead in music edu-
cation, resulting in a wider acceptance of different musical styles. Some re-
searchers do find gender differences. Martin, Clarke, and Pearce (1993), for
instance, found that boys prefer rock and heavy metal music while girls had
a preference for popular music. These differences are obviously related to
the gender stereotyping of certain musical styles (see Green, 1997; and Maid-
low & Bruce, 1999, for detail on gender differences with regard to prefer-
ences).
Results show surprisingly clearly how strongly individual listening styles are
connected with the experience of individual problems, depression appearing
to be the most important experience in this context. In most cases, plausible
explanations for the interrelations between problem experience and listening
style are possible. (p. 157)
abilities to cope with these changed desires in instrumental music lessons are
quite limited.
Parents and teachers know very well that puberty is a critical stage for
musical development. It is surprising that research has not yet adequately
addressed the influences of changes in personality, motivation, interests, and
sociopsychological conditions typical for this developmental stage on music
instruction.
ativity if one does not only want to consider the number of compositions
but also quality and other aspects (cf. Gembris, 2002, p. 375f).
More recently, a barrage of studies by Simonton have investigated the
possible influences of culture, society, history, and biographical situation on
creative productivity (e.g., Simonton, 1997, for a review). Based on his work,
the author put forth a mathematical model that suggests that the lifetime
trajectory of musical productivity (composition) could be represented by a
tilted J-shaped function. Hence, the creative process rises quickly up to a
peak that falls around the age of 40 (give or take a few years depending on
the musical genre), after which there is a steady decrease. Of course, these
kinds of statistical and graphic representations of creative productivity over
the life span are idealized and based on averages. Given that individual
differences of adults increase with age (see earlier), actual composers can
differ from one another to a large extent.
Langner, in prep. (2005a, b); Mills & Smith, in prep (2005), and the con-
sequences for the musicians personal and professional identity as well as for
the development of musical abilities have yet to be explored.
First, Rubinstein said that he played fewer pieces (selection); second, he in-
dicated that he now practices these pieces more often (optimization); and
third, he said that to counteract his loss in mechanical speed he now used a
kind of impression management such as introducing slower play before fast
segments, so to make the latter appear faster (compensation). (p. 1055)
However, it is likely that only pianists and conductors can maintain these
kinds of outstanding results at old age, while it might be impossible for
woodwind, brass, or string players. Those will be affected earlier by the
decrease of sensorimotor functions as a result of aging. Additional problems
for violin players that would lead to weaknesses in intonation and bowing
could be the decline of the sense of touch along with the reduction of the
skins sensibility and reduced agility and speed.
Even though biological changes of the voice are also unavoidable, singers
can work against them to a certain degree. Sataloff (1992, p. 20) suggests
that in order to maintain a good physical condition it is necessary to exercise.
Also, regular technical practice eliminates undesirable tremolo and improves
the smoothness, exactness, and stamina of the voice for old as well as for
young singers. According to Sataloff, many functions of the voice could be
maintained at higher levels much longer than it is generally believed. Thus
a singing career could possibly be extended beyond the age of 60 and into
the seventh decade of life.
THE DEVELOPMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES 151
Conclusion
STUDY QUESTIONS
NOTE
I am grateful to Mirjam Schlemmer and Jan Hemming for their indefatigable
help with the English manuscript and the reviewers for their insightful com-
ments.
REFERENCES
Baacke, D. (1993). Jugendkulturen und Musik. In H. Bruhn, R. Oerter, & H.
Rosing (Eds.), Musikpsychologie: Ein Handbuch (pp. 228237). Reinbek,
Germany: Rororo.
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1989). Optimierung durch Selektion und Kom-
pensation: Ein psychologisches Modell erfolgreichen Alterns. Zeitschrift fur
Padagogik, 35, 85105.
Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity:
Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psy-
chology, 28, 121125.
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-span theory in
developmental psychology. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook
of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th
ed.) (pp. 10291143). New York: Wiley.
Bamberger, J. (1991). The mind behind the musical ear: How children develop
musical intelligence. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bartlett, J. C., & Snelus, P. (1980). Lifespan memory for popular songs. Amer-
ican Journal of Psychology, 93, 551560.
Bastian, H. G. (1989). Leben fur Musik. Eine Biographie-Studie uber musikal-
ische (Hoch-) Begabungen. [A life for music: A biographical study on high
ability in music]. Mainz, Genmany: Schott.
Behne, K.-E. (1974). Psychologische Aspekte der Musikalitat. In Forschung in
der Musikerziehung (pp. 7494) Mainz, Germany: Schott.
Behne, K.-E. (1986). Horertypologien: Zur Psychologie des jugendlichen Musik-
geschmacks. Regensburg, Germany: Bosse.
Behne, K.-E. (1997). The development of Musikerleben in adolescence: How
and why young people listen to music. In I. Deliege & J. Sloboda (Eds.),
Perception and cognition of music (pp. 143159). Hove, UK: Psychology
Press.
Behrens, G. A., & Green, S. B. (1993). The ability to identify emotional content
of solo improvisations performed vocally and on three different instruments.
Psychology of Music, 21, 2033.
Birren, J. E., & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.). (1996). Handbook of the psychology of
aging (4th ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Brosgole, L., Kurucz, J., Plahovinsak, T. J., Boettcher, P., Sprotte, C., & Havel-
iwala, Y. A. (1983). Facial-affect recognition in normal pre-school children
and in elderly persons. International Journal of Neuroscience, 20(12), 91
102.
Brosgole, L., Kurucz, J., Plahovinsak, T. J., Sprotte, C., & Haveliwala, Y. A.
156 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
bruce torff
A quick look around the world yields abundant examples of impressive hu-
man accomplishments. People are able to set new records in athletics, pub-
lish groundbreaking scientific works, and produce beautiful and challenging
new music and art. Examples such as these raise old and vexing questions
about the human abilities involved in complex performances like those in
athletics, science, music, and art. To what extent do complex performances
require abilities unique to particular domains and disciplines (e.g., language,
mathematics)? To what extent are complex performances dependent upon
general abilities that cut across domains and disciplines? How and why did
abilities take this form, over the aeons? How do abilities change with age
and experience? How can they be fostered?
These questions have been tackled over the centuries by a diverse group
of psychologists, philosophers, sociologists, anthropologists, educators, and
others focused on cognition, learning, development, and education. Histor-
ically, complex performances have posed a daunting challenge for these re-
searchers. Accordingly, psychological work in the last century has made
more headway with simpler performances that are easily measured, espe-
cially ones in laboratory settings that allow researchers to control the context
in which tasks and instruments are implemented. Complex performances are
far harder to study, and the psychological literature reflects this fact. As a
result, practitioners and educators in many disciplines find the psychological
literature too remote to be of much value (Egan, 1992).
But in recent decades, new theory and research have offered fresh per-
spectives on the development and nurturance of human abilities. Since the
cognitive revolution that began in the late 1950s, attention has focused
165
166 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
No discipline has a longer and more contentious history than studies of the
human mind. Beginning perhaps with Platos Meno, two millennia of schol-
arly attention have focused on the minds structure, development, and nur-
turance. For most of this history, the workings of the mind have been viewed
as issues of interest largely in the discipline of philosophy. But the industrial
revolution brought forth a great flurry of activity in the sciences, and schol-
ars interested in the mind sought scientific methods for studying their
quarry. Psychology diverged from philosophy in the late 19th century, armed
with newly crafted research methods modeled on the hard sciences. The new
science of the mind put forward three theoretical models of human abilities.
First, the new focus on instrumentation and research methods led to a
particular theoretical/methodological perspective of the mind, one that fo-
A COMPARATIVE REVIEW OF HUMAN ABILITY THEORY 167
must take into account the culture in which abilities are created and given
meaning.
The emergence of contextualism has coincided with a surge of interest in
a sociocultural theory of cognitive development put forth by Vygotsky and
independently by Mead in the 1930s (Mead, 1934/1956; Vygotsky, 1978;
see also Cole, 1996; Wertsch, 1985). The essence of sociocultural theory is
the claim that the mind is socially formedthat is, the structure and function
of cognitive abilities are constituted by culture as the individual interacts
with the sociocultural environment. The individuals performance is sup-
ported by a variety of culturally created mediators, which include physical
tools, social conventions, and symbolic media. Learning (internalization) oc-
curs as individuals construct mental representations and habituate actions
as guided by mediational elements. According to sociocultural theory, cul-
tural concepts form the foundation of the way individuals make sense of the
world, and the individuals thought processes are thus imprinted through
interaction with the cultural environment. In recent decades, four new lines
of sociocultural theory and research have appeared, under the headings
Everyday Cognition, Socially Shared Cognition, Distributed Cogni-
tion, and Situated Cognition.
Everyday cognition: With the rise of sociocultural theory came a spate of
studies of thinking and learning in nonacademic contexts, much of it under
the banner of everyday cognition (Lave, 1988; Lave & Wenger, 1993; Rogoff
& Lave, 1984). Researchers who looked outside the classroom at instances
of everyday activityon the job and at homefound examples of ingenious
strategies that people devised to exploit environmental affordances and over-
come situational constraints. For example, truck drivers were found to stack
milk crates through use of a context-embedded method of counting that is
remarkably effective, if remote from the school-oriented approach of making
a formal count (Scribner, 1984). Studies of everyday cognition reveal how
seldom the strategies people use in life and on the job resemble the formal
knowledge taught in schools. Sociocultural theory points up the learning
inherent in everyday settingsvisiting a restaurant, completing a tax form,
programming a VCR. Focusing on the ubiquity of mediators in the world
around us, sociocultural theory underscores that learning occurs everywhere,
all the timeeven when there is no intent to teach or learn. From this per-
spective, the terms education, socialization, and enculturation are closely
related notions, if not outright synonyms.
Socially shared cognition: Sociocultural theory pays particular attention
to one form of cultural mediationthe efforts made and encouragement
given by other people. The growing interest in socially shared cognition
refers to the study of how people come to engage in shared belief (Resnick,
Levine, & Teasley, 1991). Apart from knowledge people hold through direct
observation, all knowledge is the result of entering into a shared belief with
a group of like-minded others called a community of practice. According to
researchers focused on socially shared cognition, learning occurs when the
learner comes to agreement with other people with whom the learner inter-
170 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
acts in a community of practice. People in the United States share the belief
that John Wilkes Booth assassinated Lincoln because we participate in a
community of practicethe one that concerns the historical beliefs of Amer-
ican culture as taught in secondary schools. Of course, such beliefs may later
be rethought and changed; as with any other cultural product, beliefs about
history are dynamic and evolve as the culture does.
Bruner (1990, 1996) uses the term intersubjectivity to describe interac-
tional processes through which individuals come to share beliefs with others.
Through intersubjective exchange, Bruner suggests, people fail to arrive at
the exact same construal of events but come to enough of a shared under-
standing to make sense of whats going on and continue the interaction.
Intersubjectivity is at the heart of sociocultural theory, because it is shared
belief in communities of practice that orients the individual and gives the
world meaning.
Distributed cognition: If cognitive activities are shared between person
and context, it follows that part of the resources (the intelligence) required
to get something done is handled by the environment, like a sort of pros-
thetic (Perkins, 1995). In communities of practice, people are assisted by the
intelligences of the cultural environmentthe physical, social, and symbolic
elements that do part of the job. Abilities are thus said to be distributed
spread between person and environmental elements. For example, lawyers
have at their disposal thick books that detail laws and precedents; we expect
a lawyer not to memorize everything but to also know how to look things
up. From the perspective of distributed-cognition researchers, the abilities
needed in law are in part in the practitioners heads but in part distributed
among the various intelligences of the cultural environment.
Situated cognition: A strong form of sociocultural theory has come for-
ward under the headings situated and situative cognition (Greeno,
1998; Lave & Wenger, 1993; Seely Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989). From
this perspective, individuals and situations cocreate activity and thus cannot
be studied separately. Abilities are seen as outgrowths of particular situa-
tional affordances and constraints, and only in context do they make sense.
People develop strategies for doing things that depend on a certain context,
and only in this context are they able to work without difficulty.
Dependence on situational affordances and constraints means that it is
very difficult for individuals to transfer knowledge across contexts, even
isomorphic ones. (Every teacher has had the experience of teaching some-
thing only to find that the lesson did not transfer as needed to a nearby
context.) According to situated-cognition researchers, transfer occurs
through generalization of knowledge, but this process can be fraught with
difficulty. As a result, people often find it hard to work unless the environ-
mental support is just right. Consider, for example, that there are places to
which you are able to drive but to which you are unable to give another
person adequate directions. You know the route, but only well enough to
get yourself there, as if by feel. Embedded in the environment are memory
cuesenvironmental objects that, when presented to your senses and inter-
A COMPARATIVE REVIEW OF HUMAN ABILITY THEORY 171
mingled with your memories of the terrain, help you to make all the nec-
essary turns. Memory and learning, it turns out, are not simply mental
achievementsthey are collaborations between the individual and a
particular set of environmental circumstances.
The situative perspective underscores that all learning is a product not
only of the persons intellectual efforts but also of whatever situational ele-
ments are present when the learning occurs. All learning is thus linked to its
situation of origin, and it is of utmost importance for educators and re-
searchers to explore how situational affordances and constraints influence
learning.
what learning is, and how psychological research ought to proceed. This
work rebuts the assumption that abilities are context-independent talents
undergirded by mental structures that can be teased out for isolated analysis.
Sociocultural theory emphasizes that abilities are stitched together by situ-
ational elementsproducts of culture. The view of abilities as situated ac-
tions calls into question the assertion that psychological work ought to focus
on the functioning of the individual person. Analysis of the personinsocial
context is a significant methodological complication but one that promises
to provide psychological studies that seem more meaningful to practitioners
interested in the abilities used in complex performances.
early-developing modules (S. Carey & Gelman, 1991; Gelman & Au, 1996).
These innately specified structures are seen as guiding frameworks for spe-
cific types of cognitive activity, and they need not be called into conscious
awareness in order to work (Torff & Sternberg, 2001b).
From this perspective, human cognition is to a significant extent domain-
specific rather than domain-general (Gelman & Au, 1996; Hirschfeld &
Gelman, 1994). Domains are thought to be modular components of an in-
nately specified cognitive system and are thus not equivalent to culturally
specified disciplines described by socioculturists and evolutionists, among
others. Detailed studies of human performances rooted in innately specified
structures have been reported in several domains, including language (Pinker,
1994, 1998); psychology or theory of mind (Astington, 1993; Wellman,
1990); quantitative reasoning (Gelman & Brenneman, 1994); spatial cog-
nition (Kellman, 1996; Spelke, 1994; Spelke & Hermer, 1996); and biology
(Atran, 1994; C. Carey, 1985; S. Carey & Smith, 1993; Keil, 1989, 1994).
The extent to which these intuitive constructs develop in any mean-
ingful sense of the word is a contentious issue among psychologists. Some
see these innately specified constructs as guiding all subsequent cognition,
throughout the life span, without evincing significant developmental changes
(Spelke, 1994; Spelke & Hermer, 1996). This is Mother Natures program-
ming for thought, and it grows but does not significantly change, much as
infants have 10 fingers that grow larger and more dexterous but not fun-
damentally different in structure.
Other psychologists argue that profound developmental changes are ev-
ident in innately specified structures. Perhaps the leading proponent of the
development of cognitive abilities is Susan Carey, whose conceptual
change model has proven influential (S. Carey, 1985; S. Carey & Smith,
1993). Conceptual judgments change significantly as the child develops, the
argument goes, as a new set of beliefs replaces an old set with which the
new one is qualitatively different and inconsistent (in Careys term, incom-
mensurable). Educational interventions, Carey argues, have the power to
usher along developmental changes that individuals need to make if they are
to develop accurate representations of the physical, mathematical, psycho-
logical, and biological worlds, and perhaps others as well.
The adevelopmental view also has been criticized by Karmiloff-Smith
(1993), who puts forth a general theory of developmental changes in psy-
chological structure and function called representational redescription.
Karmiloff-Smith describes cognitive development in terms of three phases of
representational character of knowledge in a domain: (1) implicit (unavail-
able to conscious awareness; (2) explicit level 1 (conscious but unavailable
to verbal report), and (3) explicit level 2 (available to verbal report). More-
over, as this developmental progression occurs, knowledge from one domain
becomes increasingly available to other modules, and thus knowledge that
begins as domain-specific becomes increasingly generalized. Developmental
change therefore involves a process of increasing connections (mapping)
A COMPARATIVE REVIEW OF HUMAN ABILITY THEORY 175
Expertise. The final set of pluralistic views of human abilities is put forth
by researchers interested in expertise (see Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1993).
Here the emphasis is on the development of discipline-specific expertise
the capacity to engage successfully in the tasks required in a real-world
discipline (e.g., chess, auto mechanics). In this view, what is important about
abilities is structured not by underlying modules of cognitive functioning (a
domain-specific view) but by the external world of disciplines and canons
of knowledge and skill they contain (a discipline-specific view). Researchers
have investigated the discipline-specific abilities in disciplines as diverse as
chess (Chase & Simon, 1973), physics (Chi, Glaser, & Farr, 1988), and
teaching (Shulman, 1990). The functionalist tack taken by expertise re-
searchers concentrates on the disciplinary organization of thought; placing
less emphasis on underlying structural changes such participation might
cause. From this perspective, abilities are a direct response to the require-
ments of the task, discipline, and culture at hand. Expertise researchers see
abilities as forms of developing expertise, and this development is structured
by the way the discipline does things. The literature on expertise is akin to
the sociocultural theory in that it examines culturally crafted knowledge;
however, only the socioculturalists focus on changes in cognitive structure
that result from real-world activities.
viduals gain age and expertise. The traditional models of human abilities
behaviorism, Piaget, and general intelligenceshare an optimistic view of
the trajectory of human development. Behaviorists assume that abilities
gradually accrue as reinforcement is administered. Piagetians assume that
stage changes in cognitive functioning occur as the individual is afforded
appropriate opportunities to act on the environment. General-intelligence
researchers assume that people gain knowledge and skill given individual
differences in general processing capacity.
These optimistic stances are called into question by a burgeoning body
of theory and research in the psychology of human abilities. The assumption
that natural human development is a smooth and unfettered process has
been undermined by evidence that development can be hindered when the
abilities needed in a domain or discipline are not integrated.
Most teachers have had the experience of teaching something to a group
of students, only to find that the students previously existing ideas have
interfered with the lesson. These experiences have been corroborated by
some troubling research. For example, studies in the domain of physics show
that students hold fast to misconceived notions about force and agency (no-
tions derivative of Aristotelian dynamics), even after successfully passing
a course that features the prevailing scientific view of the physical world
(Newtonian dynamics) (diSessa, 1993; Larkin, 1983; McCloskey, Camar-
azza, & Green, 1980). Even students who perform in an exemplary manner
in the course revert to intuitive but inaccurate ideas about physics when
tested outside the classroom.
In this example, educational outcomes are influenced by intuitive concep-
tionsknowledge or knowledge structures that need not be available to
conscious reflection but act to facilitate or constrain task performance (see
Torff & Sternberg, 2001a, 2001b). The domain of physics has provided the
smoking gun that shows that learning can be impeded, sometimes severely,
by intuitive conceptions. Similar phenomena have been noted in a variety of
other domains, which include biology (e.g., Keil, 1989, 1994), numerical
reasoning (e.g., Gelman, 1991; Gelman & Brenneman, 1994), and psychol-
ogy (or theory of mind) (e.g., Astington, 1993; Leslie, 1987; Wellman,
1990).
A set of three themes emerges from these bodies of work. First, as illus-
trated in the physics example, individuals employ a variety of intuitive con-
ceptions that exert a powerful force on the kind of thinking they do in all
sorts of situations, inside and outside the classroom. Seldom brought to
conscious awareness, intuitive conceptions are typically hidden from view
and often take the form of assumptions on which patterns of thought and
action are predicated. One need not, for example, have conscious awareness
of ones thinking to successfully recognize faces or add small quantities.
Abilities, in this view, are constituted in part by knowledge and skill of which
people have conscious access and also by intuitive forms of knowledge and
skill.
Second, powerful as intuitive conceptions are for making commonsense
178 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
modern humans should evince ways in which their behavior seems ill suited
to contemporary challenges, intellectual and otherwise. Evolutionary psy-
chologists suggest that not always does the adaptive legacy we have from
our forebears support our performance in the modern world.
A similar view can be seen among cognitive-developmental psychologists.
Part of Piagets legacy was his demonstration that young children often ex-
hibit distinctive conceptions of the world. More recent theory attributes
these conceptions to a combination of innate constraints on learning and
early experience (S. Carey & Gelman, 1991). Together with the evolution-
ists, cognitive-developmentalists press the case that the human genetic en-
dowment is a root cause of the challenges of integration of abilities. But
cognitive-developmentalists place additional emphasis on ontogenetic
changes in the development of abilities. At the heart of these disjunctures lie
the problems inherent in the developmental interplay of endogenous factors
(nature) and exogenous ones (nurture).
Sociocultural theorists have little quarrel with evolutionist and cognitive-
developmental theory, but they locate the primary causes for disjunctures
elsewhere, at least in part: in interactions between the individuals and the
ambient culture. Consider, for example, the concept of folk psychologya
set of socially shared ideas about how the mind works and what learning
and knowledge are (Bruner, 1990, 1996; Olson & Bruner, 1996). A cultures
folk psychology is a shared conception of how people think, how they
should act, and how they learn, among other things. But not always do the
precepts of a cultures folk psychology prove to be a boon to educators.
Rather, folk beliefs can come into conflict with the formal concepts created
and taught by experts in a discipline, leading to the disjunctures between
abilities noted earlier.
Beliefs about teaching and learning called folk pedagogy help to make
the point. In Western culture, especially in the United States, a popular folk
belief holds that learning occurs when students absorb information from the
environment and thus the best teaching occurs when the environment is
made rich with information transmitted to students from teachers, books,
and other sources. Studies of teachers beliefs show that prospective teachers
often hold fast to a transmission model of teaching, despite the construc-
tivist view (taught in most teacher-education programs) that knowledge is
constructed individually by each learner, based on environmental input and
individual reflection (e.g., Brookhart & Freeman, 1992; Bruner, 1996; Doyle
& Carter, 1996; Hollingsworth, 1989; Kagan, 1992; McLaughlin, 1991;
Morine-Dershimer, 1993; Shulman, 1990; Strauss, 1993; 1996; Strauss &
Shilony, 1994; Torff, 1999b; Torff & Sternberg, 2001a, 2001b); Woolfolk
Hoy, 1996; Zeichner & Gore, 1990). Of course, the constructivist pedagogy
might ultimately be found wanting. But it seems clear that people who are
trained in education hold powerful intuitive conceptions about teaching and
learning and these intuitive conceptions exert a great deal of influence on
the way people think and act in classroom settings.
Folk pedagogy predisposes individuals to think and teach in particular
180 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
ways, some of which are inconsistent with the concepts and practices char-
acteristic of expert teaching. As with intuitive conceptions in other domains,
folk pedagogy may tend to persist despite successful participation in pre-
service training programs. Becoming an expert teacher, then, is not simply a
matter of gaining new knowledge or of replacing inadequate preconceptions
in a straightforward manner. Rather, explicit efforts by teachers of educa-
tional psychology are needed to counter these uncritically held beliefs, prin-
cipally by encouraging prospective teachers to engage in activities that fa-
cilitate relevant forms of cognitive change.
This example makes clear that intuitive conceptions held by teachers
as well as those held by studentsinfluence educational outcomes, and not
always for the better. The example also illustrates how sociocultural theory,
as well as the cognitive-developmental and evolutionary theory, accounts for
disjunctures between abilities. The shared beliefs in a community of practice
may ill fit the time-honored procedures and practices created by disciplinary
experts, resulting in disjunctures between abilities.
Educational Implications
Education in Context
If knowledge and skill are shaped through context and only in context do
tasks make sense, then any pedagogy begins with a detailed analysis of the
abilities the domain or discipline requires, with the goal of crafting educa-
tional practices to foster the needed abilities. Chess masters, for example,
A COMPARATIVE REVIEW OF HUMAN ABILITY THEORY 181
Integration of Abilities
The research reviewed earlier supports the conclusion that not always is the
path to expertise in a domain or discipline a smooth one. The specter of
conflicts between abilities calls for diagnostics that identify the difficulties
that obtain in a particular domain or discipline. This is seldom done, as in
the physics example discussed earlier. The finding about intuitive physics
came as a surprise to many in the world of science teaching. Similar surprises
may await educators in other disciplines.
Psychologists and educators are working to counter these difficulties, and
the emerging consensus among them is that learners must confront naive
views, to think about them and question them, not work around them.
Learning about Newton did not dislodge the flawed intuitive physics. Whats
needed is pedagogy that requires students to critically analyze their intuitive
responses and to consider ways in which they might be improved. In what
is in essence a constructivist response to the problem, the focus falls on
teaching for integration between abilities, with the aim of strengthening the
relationships among abilities required in a discipline.
Promising initiatives that aim to enhance this integration fall under the
various headings reflective thinking (Gardner, 1991; Paris & Ayres, 1994),
reflective teaching (Liston & Zeichner, 1996), and teaching for understand-
A COMPARATIVE REVIEW OF HUMAN ABILITY THEORY 183
Conclusion
STUDY QUESTIONS
1. The psychology of human abilities is said to be preparadigmatic, with
three historically important theoretical frameworks vying to explain the vast
184 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
array of human capacities and talents. What are these theoretical frame-
works, how do they compare and contrast, and what are their shortcomings
(according to contemporary psychologists)?
2. In the last two decades, the contextualization of theory and research
in the psychology of human abilities has been emphasized. How does this
contextual psychology differ from earlier theoretical frameworks, what
theoretical notions have grown out of this approach, and what are its im-
plications for methodology in psychological research?
3. Historically, psychologists have sought a single mechanism or process
to explain the full range of human abilities, but in recent years psychologists
have suggested that individuals benefit from multiple, theoretically-distinct
abilities (i.e., modules) that combine as individuals perform tasks. What
are these pluralistic models of human abilities, what sets of mental modules
have been posited, and how do these pluralistic approaches differ in ontog-
eny and phylogeny of abilities?
4. Human development has traditionally been seen as smooth process of
acquiring knowledge and skill, but recent theories suggest that various hu-
man abilities may fail to integrate as needed, or come into outright conflict.
What causes such difficulties, according to these theorists?
5. As diverse as contemporary theoretical perspectives on human abilities
are, they suggest a set of educational implications. What are these implica-
tions?
REFERENCES
Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cognitive theory. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Astington, J. (1993). The childs discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Har-
vard University Press.
Atran, S. (1994). Core domains versus scientific theories. In L. Hirschfeld & S.
Gelman, (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in cognition and cul-
ture (pp. 316340). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas.
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). (1992). The adaptive mind: Evo-
lutionary psychology and generation of culture. New York: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1993). Surpassing ourselves: An inquiry into
the nature and implications of expertise. New York: Open Court.
Brookhart, S., & Freeman, D. (1992). Characteristics of entering teacher can-
didates. Review of Educational Research, 62, 3760.
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Cahan, E., & White, S. (1992). Proposals for a second psychology. American
Psychologist, 47(2), 224235.
Carey, C. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge: Bradford/MIT.
A COMPARATIVE REVIEW OF HUMAN ABILITY THEORY 185
Carey, S., & Gelman, R. (Eds.). (1991). The epigenesis of mind. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Carey, S., & Smith, C. (1993). On understanding the nature of scientific knowl-
edge. Educational Psychologist, 28(3), 235251.
Chase, W., & Simon, H. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4,
5581.
Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Eds.). (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanov-
ich.
Cole, M. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic
Books.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Collins, A., Brown, J., & Newman, S. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teach-
ing the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In C. Resnick (Ed.),
Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser
(pp. 453494). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary
psychology as the missing link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The latest and the best:
Essays on evolution and optimality (pp. 227306). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
diSessa, A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruc-
tion, 10, 105225.
Doyle, W., & Carter, K. (1996). Educational psychology and the education of
teachers: A reaction. Educational Psychologist, 31(1) 5162.
Egan, K. (1992). Review of The Unschooled Mind (by Howard Gardner).
Teachers College Record, 94, 2, 397406.
Eysenk, H. (1986). The theory of intelligence and the psychopsysiology of cog-
nition. In R. J. Sternberg & D. R. Detterman (Eds.), What is intelligence:
Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition (pp. 134). Norwood,
NJ: Ablex.
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gardner, H. (1991). The unschooled mind. New York: Basic Books.
Gardner, H. (1993). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New
York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1983).
Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed. New York: Basic Books.
Gardner, H., Torff, B., & Hatch, T. (1996). The age of innocence reconsidered:
Preserving the best of the progressive tradition in psychology and education.
In D. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Handbook of psychology in education
(pp. 2855). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Gelman, R. (1991). First principles organize attention to and learning about
relevant data: Number and animateinanimate distinction as examples. Cog-
nitive Science, 14, 79106.
Gelman, R., & Au, T. (Eds.). (1996). Perceptual and cognitive development.
New York: Academic Press.
Gelman, R., & Brenneman, K. (1994). First principles can support both univer-
sal and culture-specific learning about number and music. In E. Hirschfeld
& S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in cognition and
culture (pp. 369390). New York: Cambridge University Press.
186 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Torff, B. (1997). Into the wordless world: Implicit learning and instructor mod-
eling in music. In V. Brummet (Ed.), Music as intelligence (pp. 7792). Ithaca,
NY: Ithaca College Press.
Torff, B. (1999a). Beyond information processing: Cultural influences on cog-
nition and learning. In S. Ulibarri (Ed.), Maria Montessori explicit and im-
plicit in the 20th century (pp. 839). Mexico City: Association Montessori
Internationale.
Torff, B. (1999b). Tacit knowledge in teaching: Folk pedagogy and teacher ed-
ucation. In R. Sternberg & J. Horvath (Eds.), Tacit knowledge in professional
practice (pp. 195214). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Torff, B., & Gardner, H. (1999). The vertical mind: The case for multiple intel-
ligences. In M. Anderson (Ed.), The development of intelligence (pp. 139
160). London: University College Press.
Torff, B., & Sternberg, R. (2001a). Intuitive conceptions among learners and
teachers. In B. Torff & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Understanding and teaching the
intuitive mind: Student and teacher learning (pp. 326). Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
baum.
Torff, B., & Sternberg, R. (Eds.). (2001b). Understanding and teaching the in-
tuitive mind: Student and teacher learning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Torff, B., & Warburton, E. (2003). Old and new models of intelligence: The
assessment conundrum. In M. Pearn (Ed.), Individual deveopment in organ-
isations. London: Wiley.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Watson, J. (1924). Behaviorism. New York: Norton.
Wellman, H. (1990). The childs theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/
MIT.
Wertsch, J. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Wiske, M. (Ed.). (1993). Teaching for understanding. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Woolfolk Hoy, A. (1996). Teaching educational psychology: Texts in context.
Educational Psychologist, 31(1), 3540.
Zeichner, K., & Gore, J. (1990). Teacher socialization. In W. Houston (Ed.),
Handbook of research on teacher education (pp. 329349). New York: Mac-
millian.
Making Music and Making Sense
Through Music
6
Expressive Performance and
Communication
reinhard kopiez
What, then, from the feelings, can music present if not their content? Only
that same dynamic mentioned above. It can reproduce the motion of a phys-
ical process according to the prevailing momentum: fast, slow, strong, weak,
rising, falling. Motion is just one attribute, however, one moment of feeling,
not feeling itself. (p. 11)
A secondary source dates from the late 1950s, a time when psychology
was largely dominated by cognitive psychology, which had deliberately ex-
cluded emotions and feelings from their discussions (see, e.g., Gardner,
1985). The influence of this tendency upon music psychology can be seen in
the second edition of Deutschs Psychology of Music (1999), with its sur-
prising lack of a chapter on music and emotion. Fortunately, this trend is
changing and emotion is now viewed as a very important topic in both
psychology and music psychology.
Answer 3. The meaning of music is the expression of emotion. The ex-
treme opposite of purely formal aesthetics is Hauseggers (1885) aesthetics
of expression (Ausdrucksasthetik). In his evolutionary approach, influenced
by Darwins 1872 treatise The Expression of the Emotions in Animal and
Man (Darwin, 1872/1965), Hausegger developed a theory of listening to
and understanding music based on an intuitive and universal understanding
192 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
2000, 2002; Langner, Kopiez, & Feiten, 1998). In a recent approach, Chaffin
et al. (2002) revealed that the existence of a global musical concept (the so-
called big picture) at the beginning of the practice of a new piece is a
characteristic feature of professional musicians.
number of factors. In addition, the timing clusters for the first melodic ges-
ture did not correspond to the three main factors found in the principal
component analysis of the first eight measures. Also, the perceptual experi-
ence when listening to the recordings does not correspond to such an un-
differentiated statistical grouping. Where does this mismatch arise? We could
assume that such a method of analyzing timing (or other variables) on a
note-to-note level offers only a very limited insight into the nature of musical
interpretation and the ideas communicated by the player. Consequently, we
suggest that future methods of timing analysis include the possibility of si-
multaneous timing analysis of layers with different time-frame sizes (on a
bar-to-bar level or a phrase-to-phrase level) of the same performance. Only
such an approach would be commensurate with the complexity of musical
thought observed in outstanding performers.
The considerations outlined earlier raise a further question: Is the per-
formers ability to shape time and intensity limited by the listeners expec-
tancies? Inspired by research into the attractiveness of human faces, Repp
(1997a) carried out a so-called average performance experiment, in which
listeners were asked to rate 11 performances of Traumerei in terms of
quality and originality. One of the listening examples had been generated
from the average timing and intensity data of the 10 other MIDI recordings.
The pedaling sequence remained constant throughout all 11 versions. Repp
found that the average version was ranked second-highest in terms of
performance quality but second-lowest in terms of individuality. A plausible
interpretation of the results offered by the author assumes that listeners de-
velop a performance prototype that guides their expectancies; the average
version could fulfill this expectancy and provide an ideal interpretation due
to the compensation of extremes through the averaging. Despite the absence
of a convincing theoretical explanation for this effect, the same preferential
tendency for averaged versions can be observed in the evaluation of rhyth-
mic performances. In a cross-cultural study on the perception of rhythm,
Kopiez, Langner, and Steinhagen (1999) found a consistently high evaluation
of the average performance of rhythm samples.
weights for the melodic, metric, and harmonic structure explained between
65 and 85% of the microstructure timing of a performance. This finding
strongly supports the assumption that a close relationship exists between
expressive intentions and compositional features, where the first can be ex-
plained with reference to the second. Such an approach goes far beyond the
surface analysis of a small section by Repp (1992a) and gives detailed insight
into the complexity of musical thought. Although Beran and Mazzola (2000)
were able to confirm the fundamental structure of Repps principal com-
ponent analysis, large individual differences were shown to exist in relation
to the relative importance of single structural parameters (harmony, melody,
metric). In the case of Horowitzs three performances of Traumerei, the
performers view of the piece obviously changed over time: After his first
recording of the piece, in 1947which was dominated by a very localized
melodic and metrical thinking (a kind of performance near-sight)Ho-
rowitzs interpretation became more coherent in his 1963 and 1965 record-
ings. The different time perspectives in the studies by Repp (1992a) and
Beran and Mazzola (2000) also go some way toward explaining the seem-
ingly contradictory results achieved. As a result, it could be argued that
although the timing of the first eight bars and the single melodic gestures
remain unchanged over decades, more global timing concepts reveal changes
when the piece is analyzed as a whole.
In light of these considerations, it is evident that inverse performance
theory is also of great practical importance since it offers the transformation
of the metaphorical language of musical criticism into an exactly defined
parametric space. At the same time, however, it must be pointed out that
research that concerns the relationship between performance features and
the structural properties of the score is still new and expanding.
there was no single-feature mechanism for cue detection. Since the reliability
of identification improved with the increasing number of expressive para-
meters involved in cross-synthesis, it is possible to conclude that the inter-
action between expressive parameters plays a decisive role in the correct
identification of emotional intentions in performance.
sense, the term character has two connotations: It is connected first to the
specific expressive individuality of a piece and second to the unmistakable
emotions presented in a performance.
The idea that music might be a universal language that can be understood
without prerequisites and independently from the individuals cultural back-
ground is a widely held conviction. Although this belief is asserted with great
persistence in both politics and everyday life, this issue is not simple, judged
from the standpoint of music psychology. It is already clear that the study
of musical universals and the biological foundations of music have contrib-
uted a new perspective characterized by comparative research methodologies
(see Carterette & Kendall, 1999). While this has resulted in the creation of
new research disciplines such as biomusicology (Wallin, Merker, & Brown,
2000), aimed at providing us with a fascinating insight into the auditory
perception among animals (see Fay, 1988, for an extensive data collection),
it is still very difficult to draw conclusions from the literature relevant to the
human music perception. Very often, the studies of comparative music psy-
chology, based on studies of animal behavior, are obtained with simple stim-
uli that have little resemblance to real music. Although interesting in them-
selves, the results that concern virtual pitch in cats (Hefner & Whitfield,
1976), octave stretching (Carterette & Kendall, 1999, p. 739), or octave
identity (surely not a universal; see Hulse & Page, 1988; Hulse, Takeuchi,
& Braaten, 1992) have little immediate bearing on the human listening ex-
perience.
Having said this, it is important to recognize the contribution of research-
ers who investigate auditory perception in animals and newborn infants,
which allow great insight into the importance of the basic processes in music
listening and have shown that such processes appear on a hardware basis
in all vertebrates regardless of cultural influences. For instance, studies by
Porter and Neuringer (1984) revealed that pigeons can first learn to distin-
guish between musical styles (e.g., Bach and Stravinsky) and then success-
fully transfer their acquired stylistic knowledge to unfamiliar examples in a
discrimination test. Hulse and Cynx (1986) found that auditory stream seg-
regation already works in starlings, while Fassbenders (1996) study of au-
ditory capacity in early infancy concluded that most musically relevant per-
ceptual mechanisms, such as contour perception, are probably developed at
a very early age. It is essential here to realize that music is not considered a
spoken language and, as Sloboda (1985, p. 12) points out, it would indeed
210 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
If, for example, a habitual smoker wants a cigarette and, reaching into his
pocket, finds one, there will be no affective response. . . . If, however, the man
finds no cigarette . . . and then remembers that the stores are closed . . . he
will very likely begin to respond in an emotional way. (p. 13)
Meyer (p. 5) regards the idea of universalism as one error of music psy-
chology, besides hedonism and atomism, and claims that music in a style
with which we are totally unfamiliar is meaningless (p. 35). However, I
propose that the general tendency to build up expectancies while listening
to music (regardless of the specific cultural experience) is worthy of consid-
eration as a potential musical universal. In Narmours theory of expectancy,
his implication-realization model (which is similar to Meyers ideas yet
more formalized), we find a universally applicable perceptual model for mel-
MAKING MUSIC AND MAKING SENSE THROUGH MUSIC 213
This chapter set out to show that the process of musical communication was
dependent on the existence of relevant knowledge and experience on the
MAKING MUSIC AND MAKING SENSE THROUGH MUSIC 215
part of the listener. As I have argued elsewhere (Kopiez, 2002), there is some
convincing evidence that suggests that listeners always listen to music with
their own ears and that music appreciation is based on culturally specific
knowledge and education. The multicultural makeup of todays classroom,
with its accompanying disparity of cultural knowledge and experience, does
not allow for a simple, unified way to nurture communication skills. One
suggestion seems tenable, however: The listening (and music-making) ex-
perience should take precedence over normative rules; this means that teach-
ers should avoid oversimplified labeling of musical expression to explain
the contents of music. Rodriguez (1998, p. 57) claims that learning expe-
riences in the music classroom should be designed to nurture sensitivity to
musical expression, but how can this be realized under such difficult con-
ditions? We would like to propose a few simple methods as starting points
for the teacher who is trying to impart to his or her students the peculiarities
of musical communication:
Spoken language. It is relatively easy to read and record a short poem with
and without different states of expression. Listeners attention can be fo-
cused on parameters such as accents, accelerations, and dynamic shaping.
With more technically advanced means, it would be interesting to extract
parameters such as speech melody as cues for specific emotional states in
language by using appropriate software (e.g., Boersma, 2004). Students can
also be confronted with the impact of different speech melodies that cor-
respond to different intended expressions.
Mechanical versus expressive performance in music. As Burnsed (1998) has
shown, a significant preference for folk songs played with expressive dy-
namics over those played without expression already exists in elementary
school students from Grades 1 to 5, and even in musically less educated
students expressive shaping increases aesthetic attractiveness. The basic ex-
perience of expression in music can be imparted on any instrument and by
using any simple melody with the following setting. The required technical
equipment amounts only to a simple MIDI sequencer for the generation of
the unexpressive version and editing of the expressive deviations in the
editor window. In order to demonstrate the difficulty of consistent expres-
sive shaping, nominal note values can also be manipulated by the use of a
graphic MIDI editor.
Synthesis of emotions. Another approach, which would allow pupils to gain
more insight into the communication of musical meaning, involves the use
of software such as Director Musices (see Friberg, 1999) or the simple
program referred to by Rodriguez (1998) for the synthesis of emotions in
a given piece of music. These files or other synthesized versions of MIDI
files can be evaluated by means of a semantic differential (scale with polar
adjectives) regarding the intended emotion. Moreover, to demonstrate the
importance of different expressive parameters in music, the expressive de-
viations in melodies can be deconstructed by setting the timing and in-
tensity deviations to their nominal value.
Improving expressive communication. As a result of the insight that ex-
pressive performance is based on a rule system that has to be learned by
216 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
player and listener, Woody (1999) showed that successful imitation of ex-
pressive dynamics in a short piano composition by a student correlated with
the correct verbal identification of the expressive features used in the aural
model before attempting an imitative performance. To improve communi-
cation between teacher and student on the one hand and performer and
listener on the other, the author suggests that verbal instructions about the
intended aural model are given to the student before trying an imitative
performance. Finally, only a conscious and clear performance plan can
guarantee easy perception of expressive intentions by the listener. Such an
imitative exercise (accompanied by verbal reports of the perceived expres-
sive intentions) could easily be realized through the use of simple melodies
played with different expressive features. In a recent investigation by Juslin
and Laukka (2000), performers were instructed to communicate different
emotions to the subjects. After cognitive feedback about intended and per-
ceived acoustical cues was given, accuracy of expressive communication
increased by about 50%. These findings suggest that cognitive feedback is
a useful tool in improving communication between player and audience.
Actually, both of these approaches might be adapted to group situations.
Washing dishes is just like language. Both the water and the tea towels are
dirty, but we can still succeed in cleaning the plates and glasses. Similarly,
language involves unclear concepts and a logic that is limitedin ways that
we do not directly understandto specific areas of application. In spite of
this, we can develop a clear understanding of the natural world. (cited in
Heisenberg, 1986, p. 190)
STUDY QUESTIONS
3. Where in everyday life can you find evidence that supports the idea of
music as a universal language that can be understood without prerequi-
sites?
4. Covering is a means of interpretation in popular music. Listen to fa-
mous cover versions (e.g. My way originally performed by Frank Sinatra
and covered by Sid Vicious from the Sex Pistols or With a little help from
my friends originally performed by the Beatles and covered by Joe
Cocker). Try to identify the intended musical message and the musical
means used (e.g. instrumentation, tempo). How does the message change
in the covered version?
NOTES
I would like to thank the following people for stimulating discussions and
invaluable comments on earlier drafts of this chapter: Alf Gabrielsson, Patrik
Juslin, Gunter Kreutz, Jorg Langner, Guerino Mazzola, Richard Parncutt,
Ulrich Pothast, and four anonymous reviewers.
1. For the software Rubato see Mazzola (2002), for Melodia and Direc-
tor Musices see http://www.speech.kth.se/music/performance/download
2. Although there are certain similarities between the ideas of Kurth and
those of the German pianist Truslit (1938; partially translated by Repp
[1993]), there is no evidence of any personal contact. The writings of Kurth
have partially been translated into English (see Rothfarb, 1988, 1989; and
Rehding, 1995).
REFERENCES
Allesch, C. G. (1987). Geschichte der psychologischen Asthetik [History of psy-
chological aesthetics]. Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Balkwill, L.-L., & Thompson, W. F. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of the
perception of emotion in music: Psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Per-
ception, 17(1), 4364.
Behne, K.-E. (1982). MusikKommunikation oder Geste? [MusicCommuni-
cation or gesture?] Musikpadagogische Forschung, 3, 125143.
Beldoch, M. (1961). The ability to identify expressions of feelings in vocal,
graphic, and musical communication. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Co-
lumbia University, New York.
Beran, J., & Mazzola, G. (2000). Timing microstructure in Schumanns Trau-
merei as an expression of harmony, rhythm, and motivic structure. Com-
puters and Mathematics with Applications, 39, 99130.
Bhatti, S., & Gregory, A. (2000). Cross-cultural study of affective responses to
Qawwali. In C. Woods, G. Luck, R. Brochard, F. Seddon, & J. A. Sloboda
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Music Perception
and Cognition, (pp. 13211328). Keele, UK: Keele University.
Boersma, P. (2004). Praat [Computer software] Retrieved October 22, from
http://www.praat.org.
218 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Hausegger, F. von (1885). Die Musik als Ausdruck [Music as expression]. Vi-
enna: Konegen.
Hefner, H., & Whitfield, I. C. (1976). Perception of the missing fundamental in
cats. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59, 915919.
Heisenberg, W. (1986). Der Teil und das Ganze: Gesprache im Umkreis der
Atomphysik [Physics and beyond: Encounters and conversations] (6th ed.).
Munich, Germany: Piper.
Henderson, M. T. (1936). Rhythmic organization in artistic piano performance.
In C. E. Seashore, (Ed.), Studies in the psychology of music: Vol. 4. Objective
analysis of musical performance (pp. 281305). Iowa City, IA: University of
Iowa Press.
Hulse, S. H., & Cynx, J. (1986). Interval and contour in serial pitch perception
by a passerine bird, the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Journal of Com-
parative Psychology, 100, 215228.
Hulse, S. H., & Page, S. C. (1988). Toward a comparative psychology of music
perception. Music Perception, 5(4), 427452.
Hulse, S. H., Takeuchi, A. H., & Braaten, R. F. (1992). Perceptual invariances
in the comparative psychology of music. Music Perception, 10(2), 151184.
Huron, D. (1991). The ramp archetype: A score-based study of musical dynam-
ics in 14 piano composers. Psychology of Music, 19(1), 3345.
Jaques-Dalcroze, E. (1921). Rhythm, music, and education (H. Rubinstein,
Trans.). New York: Putnam.
Juslin, P. N. (1997). Perceived emotional expression in synthesized performances
of a short melody: Capturing the listeners judgment policy. Musicae Scientiae,
2(1), 225256.
Juslin, P. N. (2000). Cue utilization in communication of emotion in music per-
formance: Relating performance to perception. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 17971813.
Juslin, P. N. (2001). Communicating emotion in music performance: A review
and a theoretical framework. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music
and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 309337). New York: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2000). Improving emotional communication in mu-
sic performance through cognitive feedback. Musicae Scientiae, 4(4), 151
183.
Juslin, P. N., & Madison, G. (1999). The role of timing patterns in recognition
of emotional expression from musical performance. Music Perception, 17(2),
197221.
Kamenetzky, S. B., Hill, D. S., & Trehub, S. E. (1997). Effect of tempo and dy-
namics on the perception of emotion in music. Psychology of Music, 25(2),
149160.
Kant, I. (1987). Critique of judgment (W. S. Pluhar, Trans. and Ed.). Indianap-
olis: Hackett. (Original work published 1790 as Kritik der Urteilskraft)
Karno, M., & Konecni, V. J. (1992). The effects of structural interventions in
the first movement of Mozarts symphony in G-Minor, K. 550, on aesthetic
preference. Music Perception, 10(1), 6372.
Kendall, R. A., & Carterette, E. C. (1990). The communication of musical ex-
pression. Music Perception, 8(2), 129164.
Kirby, F. E. (1970). Beethovens Pastoral Symphony as a sinfonia caracteris-
tica. Musical Quarterly, 56, 605623.
MAKING MUSIC AND MAKING SENSE THROUGH MUSIC 221
Psychologists seem to agree that musical skills share many common features
with skill-related phenomena in other areas outside of music, such as lan-
guage, games, sports, science, and other domains. Music making entails per-
ceptual skills (e.g., apprehending structural information as well as social
information, including nonverbal cues exchanged between performer and
audience), cognitive skills (e.g., memory, decision making, pattern recogni-
tion), and of course motor skills. These skills function, interact, and evolve
in complex ways that we are slowly starting to understand. However, since
each performer, be it the professional orchestra musician who earns his or
her living playing an instrument or the amateur who plays solely for enjoy-
ment, is a unique individual with his or her individual biography, there is
no such thing as the musical skill. Instead, the ways in which the skill
was developed as well as the final skill structure will necessarily differ from
person to person as a result of individual learning histories.
Each performer has artistic intentions, that is, a message to be conveyed.
Some of these intentions are related to communicating information about
the musical structure and expressive embellishments to it for aesthetically
driven goals. For instance, slowing at a cadence point has become recognized
generally in Western music as sounding better than not slowing. Other
intentions are more emotionally directed, such as playing a piece sorrow-
fully or happily. Of course, music making does not happen in a vacuum;
it most always involves other people, be they coperformers or members of
the audience with whom the performer communicates.
As a socially and culturally embedded skill, music making is one of the
most important activities in all cultures. Attempting to understand musical
skill is one way of paying tribute to the tremendous achievements of past
225
226 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
and present musicians. At the same time, we may be able ultimately to derive
suggestions for the improvement of music teaching and learning.
The integrative metaphor for this chapterwhich will connect the dif-
ferent aspects that regard musical skills and their acquisitionis the image
of a Western classical performer who has just stepped onto the stage to
perform in front of an audience. Most of what we say would also apply to
musicians in other musical traditions and cultures. We will first consider the
performers current skills that are about to be displayed in some form; we
will then discuss the hidden side of these skills, namely, their acquisition
in the course of the musicians biography; and finally, we will address issues
that surround the display of skills, more specifically the interaction between
audience and performer and the interaction among performers. This section
addresses issues that pertain to socially situated cognition and sociocultural
influences on cognitive processing. A brief concluding section will address
educational consequences of research discussed in this chapter. Throughout
the chapter, we will address the qualitative and quantitative aspects that may
distinguish different levels of performance and comment on research meth-
odologies that are employed to assess these issues. We will attempt to answer
the following questions: What does the performer bring to the task of per-
forming; what does skill acquisition entail; and finally, what happens during
performance? When we talk about performers, we do not refer to expert or
elite performers only but rather to any person who steps out on stage with
the intention to perform in front of other people.
(e.g., reaching motions, tapping, solving simple logic problems), other re-
searchers try to tackle more complex skills in realistic settings inside and
outside of the laboratory (e.g., memorizing a song, acquiring a new piece of
music, judging music performance). The sophistication or expertise of the
participant is also a factor to consider: We can expose participants to tasks
they have never seen before and track their learning curves, or we can study
established experts who are performing tasks that are highly familiar to
them. Furthermore, tasks can be performed in different types of environ-
ments, which require various degrees of flexibility or transfer. Some so-called
closed environments offer stable conditions under which to perform the task
(e.g., chess, swimming), while other environments are open, forcing the par-
ticipant to operate under unpredictable conditions (e.g., mountain climbing).
In music, we could maybe classify the performance of rehearsed solo rep-
ertoire as a closed skill while improvisation (especially in an ensemble) could
be classified as an open one.
What emerges as a finding from the literature is that a skill is a skill is
a skill, meaning that the findings are consistent across domains regardless
of the stimulus onto which we direct our perception, cognition, or action.
Skill researchers assume that human beings by and large cope with the prob-
lems they are facing in any domain by applying roughly similar perceptual,
cognitive, and psychomotor solutions. Thus by studying one area of skill,
such as musical skill acquisition and expertise, we learn some things about
other domains and vice versa. The ultimate goal is to apply the results to
education and benefit future generations of learners and their teachers.
Obviously, acquiring one skill does not allow us to perform all skills but
only those with similar task characteristics; in other words, the transfer of
skills from one domain to another is rather limited (see Tunks, 1992, for a
more detailed discussion of transfer of learning). For example, the transfer
of knowledge and skills (if we want to make this distinction and not assume
that knowledge is an essential part of the skill) from one musical instrument
to another depends on how similar the two instruments are. Pianists can
easily adapt to playing on a harpsichord and saxophone players may ex-
perience few difficulties learning the flute, but switching to a string instru-
ment would be difficult for them both. As obvious as it seems, this fact is
not trivial, because it demonstrates a touchstone in skill research: namely,
the domain-specificity of skills. This finding suggests that even high-level
experts may experience problems if the stimulus material does not com-
pletely match typical requirements. For example, if an outstanding trumpet
player with extensive experience in playing band music attempts to sight-
read or memorize atonal music, he or she might be confronted with unex-
pected limitations. In the worst case, our brave trumpeter may not out-
perform a good band student. This finding is sometimes called the
structure-by-skill (or expertise) interaction, because it means that skilled in-
dividuals only perform at superior levels when the stimuli are representative
of the structural properties for which the cognitive mechanisms were devel-
oped. Furthermore, transfer may vary from one skill level to the next as
228 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Palmer and Meyer (2000) showed. In their experiment, beginning and ad-
vanced pianists learned melodies and later were asked to perform them with
a different hand, a different clef, or different notes (retaining the original
fingering). The results showed virtually no transfer for the novices while the
experts demonstrated reasonable transfer in expected ways, and the authors
suggest that as skill increases, mental representations for performance be-
come dissociated from the movements required to produce a musical se-
quence (pp. 6667).
The issue of specificity leads us to a related characteristic of skilled or
expert performance, namely, that skilled performers become maximally
adapted to the task demands under which they usually operate. For this,
performers undergo extensive adaptations with regard to physiological, psy-
chomotor, and cognitive factors that allow them to be suited optimally for
the task at hand. For example, athletes show a number of physiological
adaptations, such as enlarged hearts or altered angles of rotation of their
limbs or even neurophysiological changes that distinguish them from non-
experts. On musicians, the angle of forearm rotation of pianists is altered in
comparison to violinists, and both differ from the normal population (Wag-
ner, 1988). These changes reflect habitual usage and movement demands of
different instrumentalists. Also, the coordination of sensory input and motor
responses is enhanced: Tennis players, for instance, can respond to advance
visual cues of their opponents in lieu of having to react merely to the quickly
approaching ball, which would leave no time to select a response and per-
form the corresponding movement. In music sight-reading, better readers
perform very different eye movements from less skilled readers, allowing the
better readers to make intelligent use of the limited viewing time by look-
ing ahead (Goolsby, 1994). Probably the most pronounced changes occur at
the cognitive level that involves problem-solving and memory skills that are
tailored to the experts needs. Over time, experts have built up what are
referred to as retrieval structures, which allow them to encode quickly and
meaningfully what they see or hear and manipulate the information in de-
sirable ways (Ericsson & Kintsch, 1995). This enables fast access to infor-
mation stored in long-term memory, whereas novices often have problems
storing and subsequently retrieving the information. For example, chess ex-
perts have stored large numbers of chess positions in long-term memory and
are able to plan several moves in advance. Concert pianists have to memorize
copious amounts of material and do so by practicing cues or prompt points
at which they could continue should they have memory lapses (Chaffin &
Imreh, 1997, Chaffin, Imreh & Crawford, 2002), and actors work hard at
understanding the character and his or her motives they are portraying (No-
ice & Noice, 1997).
that is, beginners or amateurs. But why bother talking about experts when
we may be interested in studying how school-age children learn to perform
music? There are several answers to this question:
1. Experts already show those cognitive and psychomotor adaptations
that nonexperts are still trying to acquire, and therefore looking at experts
might lead us to better understand where the novices development is head-
ing.
2. Experts are usually more able to verbalize and explain what happened
in the course of performance, while novices can be quite surprised that they
could do a given task at all; often they cannot report anything about their
performance (e.g., Hargreaves, Cork, & Setton 1991). This is possibly the
reason why many musicians like reading biographies of and books by fa-
mous musicians: These books usually contain deep and informative (and
sometimes controversial) insights into musical processes not readily available
to the lesser expert.
3. The performance of experts has been demonstrated to be less variable
and less affected by extraneous conditions than that of novices, which is an
important methodological consideration for laboratory studies (Ericsson &
Smith, 1991). Several researchers have demonstrated that pianists can re-
produce their own performance with extreme accuracy, which allows us to
study reliable timing mechanisms in performance (see Kopiez, in this vol-
ume). Conversely, trying to study the same timing mechanisms in a group
of band students would be more difficult since it is known that mood, mo-
tivation, and variability in skill can create large amounts of variance noise
in the performance data (Proctor & Dutta, 1995).
Therefore, while studying experts has its merits, it also has its drawbacks,
in that we do not know exactly how well our results generalize from one
population to the other. It is necessary to undertake studies that involve
various levels of performance (e.g., Drake & Palmer, 2000; Palmer & Meyer,
2000) and even study people who drop out of music studies (e.g., Sloboda,
Davidson, Howe, & Moore, 1996) to ensure the validity and applicability
of research findings.
group could be caused by large skill differences within the group. Or it may
be that better performers are more likely to seek out improvisational activ-
ities and try to learn to improvise. Why are musical skills related at all? Two
alternative accounts are possible: It is possible that some skills transfer such
that learning to play an instrument might foster musical reading as well as
memory and musical imagery as a by-product, or there is indeed some un-
derlying common factor or mechanism consistent with the idea of a musical
intelligence (Gardner, 1983; see also Kopiez and Gembris, chaps. 6, 4 this
volume). The decision for or against one account (or a combined version)
remains speculative until further empirical evidence is available.
However, the educational consequences of the view that musical skills are
rather specific is clear, namely, that each requires its own specialized training.
For example, if you want your students to be able to improvise, you have
to offer activities that would promote this skill, such as creating melodies,
copying improvisations by ear, and listening to other people improvise. Un-
fortunately, at this point we do not know much about expressive skills and
their acquisition, but it is likely that also here imitation of idiomatic patterns
and listening experience plays an important role (Woody, 2003).
Figure 7.1. Davidson and Scripps (1992) Matrix of Cognitive Skills in Music: Two conditions
of knowledge crossed with three ways of knowing.
goal to promote not only individual skills but also the interaction and in-
terconnectedness among them.
The model itself does not make any claims about underlying processes
and the exact nature of the representations involved. The authors clearly
state that the different ways of knowing are learned and trained and that
they can be practiced through use of cognitive tools (p. 404), better
known to educators as teaching methods, such as modeling and the utili-
zation of metaphors. Accordingly, we can analyze teaching situations, as do
L. Davidson and Scripp (pp. 404406), with regard to the types of repre-
sentations they are likely to produce. The idea that teaching results in rep-
resentation is shared with the next model to be described, in which certain
representations are seen as central to music making and hence constitute the
ultimate goal of teaching and practice.
Triangular Model of Mental Representation in Music Performance. The claim
for the model described in the following is that instruction could be tailored
to result in the postulated representations (Ericsson, 1997; Lehmann & Er-
icsson, 1997). Whereas the Davidson and Scripp model was rather descrip-
tive in nature, applying a general psychological model of skills to musical
skills in declarative and procedural instances, the following model aims at
providing musicians with a framework for their daily work. The triangular
model of musical performance skills assumes that musicians need at least
three different types of mental representations. Those representations cor-
respond to (1) a goal representation, (2) a production representation, and
(3) a representation of the current performance. (Woody, 1999a renames
those representations to goal imaging, motor production, and self-
monitoring.) It is appropriate to conceive of these representations as answers
to questions that teacher and student can ask, namely, what should the
music sound like? how is the sound achieved? and what is coming out
right now? Consequently, the goal representation captures whatever the
performer does internally to represent the desired auditory outcome (includ-
ing decisions about how to interpret the piece); the production representa-
tion entails the procedural knowledge of the performer of how to execute
the music on the instrument; the representation of the current performance
is the internal description of what is being produced at the moment (see
figure 7.2).
There is empirical and everyday evidence for the existence of the proposed
mental representations. Although they are not directly observable, indicators
for their existence and functioning can be found in task performance and
through the use of verbal reports (think-aloud protocols). Task performance
under changed or stressful conditions often reveals flaws in the functioning
of our representations and deficient interactions among the three compo-
nents. The following are examples of flawed representations:
The player knows that a crescendo is coming up and thinks he or she is
playing it, but the teacher or audience does not hear anything. The singer
236 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
is out of tune with the piano but is satisfied with the performance (problem
with motor and current performance representation but correct goal im-
aging).
A player is not sure how a piece continues, yet the right notes still come
out of his or her musical instrument (motor representation and memory
function, but the goal representation is deficient).
The performer knows how it should sound but realizes that what is being
produced is not correct (problem with establishing a motor representation
but correct goal imaging and monitoring of ongoing performance).
It is fair to say that nobody is born with the skills necessary to go onstage
to perform and that skills do not accrue overnight but take a long time to
develop. Granted, there are individual differences in personality, speed of
development, and motivation, but even the most gifted individuals require
large amounts of training and practice to develop noteworthy levels of per-
formance. This development or skill acquisition phase is especially of interest
once the level of performance exceeds the average adult level prevalent in
the population, a level that is almost passively acquired through accultura-
238 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
tion. For example, most people can sing the national anthem or whistle
popular tunes; however, in Western classical music, we are concerned with
levels of performance that come about as a result of deliberate and time-
consuming instruction and training (which could include expert singers and
whistlers). In this part of the chapter, we will discuss the development of
expertise in music, introduce the concept of deliberate practice, and address
issues that surround practice, such as the preparation of a specific piece and
practice efficiency (see J. W. Davidson, Howe, & Sloboda, 1997; Lehmann,
1997b; Jrgensen, 2004, for reviews).
individuals biography. This biography also entails a cultural and even his-
torical dimension. For example, unlike today piano teaching and training in
the 19th century not only promoted the playing of rehearsed material but
also fostered the acquisition of improvisational skills (Gellrich & Sundin,
1993). Furthermore, teaching methods and materials became available and
evolved over time, as was the case for jazz music. But also at the level of
the individual, skill acquisition is a unique experience that results in a unique
set of skills. Take, for example, an average music student who starts playing
a musical instrument in school and practices very little at home and contrast
this situation with that of a musical prodigy who has been privately tutored
since early childhood. This ensures not only unique musical biographies (and
hence skill trajectories) but also a distinctive artistic voice. Given that prac-
tice is the most common activity musicians engage in during skill acquisition,
we will look at it more closely now (see Hallam, 1997; Jrgensen, 2004 for
references regarding practice).
resources, that is, mental effort, and it can therefore only be sustained over
many months and years without symptoms of physical and psychological
burnout when applied in moderation. Around 4 or 5 hours a day seems to
be the optimal amount for adultsless, of course, for children. Finally, the
person needs to be motivated. Although Ericsson et al. make no assumption
about how this motivation comes about or how it is maintained, other au-
thors view motivation as a direct result of talent (e.g., Winner, 1996, espe-
cially for prodigious children) or as a by-product of expert teaching (e.g.,
Gholson, 1998, for a detailed analysis of Dorothy DeLays teaching). Mo-
tivational differences, which may be personality-related, seem to play a de-
cisive role in being successful at the beginning of instruction (Duke, 1999;
ONeill & Sloboda, 1997).
In Ericsson et al.s original studies, three groups of adult violinists were
compared with regard to the amount of time they had spent practicing over
their life span. Two groups were made up of the best and good students
as rated by their academy teachers, and the last group consisted of aspiring
music teachers. To ascertain whether the best students were comparable
to current professionals, the authors also surveyed members of professional
orchestras. Results showed that experts had practiced more, even at younger
ages (see figure 7.3a). Adversely, even at advanced ages, amateurs only ac-
cumulated a fraction of the time that full-timers accrued. The results are
generally consistent with a later study by Sloboda, Davidson, Howe, and
Moore (1996), who investigated a large sample of teenage music students
at five different levels of achievement: from the highest level of achievement
(Group One in figure 7.3b) to students who had abandoned playing alto-
gether (reported as Group Five in the figure). The better students had prac-
ticed more, even in the beginning stages of learning.
Since correlations do not necessarily imply causation, it cannot be ruled
out that other variables might have caused better performers to practice
more. For example, one could argue that more talented people tend to be
more motivated and therefore practice more and thus the difference between
the groups was really due to differences in innate dispositions (for an exten-
sive coverage of this and other aspects of the naturenurture debate see
Howe, Davidson & Sloboda, 1998, and some 30 commentaries published
along with the target article).
Assessing deliberate practice is usually done by interviewing the subjects
and obtaining retrospective estimates of practice for every year since the start
of practice. Critics have suggested that these estimates may be unreliable and
that time practicing alone may not be a good indicator for deliberate prac-
tice. Although this criticism is viable, there are also arguments in favor of
this method of assessing practice. First, errors in estimates will likely affect
all subjects of a given study similarly and therefore not influence the pattern
of results, only the absolute magnitude of the estimates. This magnitude
varies considerably among instruments anyway, with string and keyboard
players topping the list of instrumentalists (Jrgensen, 1997). Second, if we
Figure 7.3. Relation between accumulated life-time deliberate/formal practice
and attainment in instrumental music performance. Figure 7.3a: Study with ex-
perts by Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Romer, 1993. Figure 7.3b: Study with nov-
ices by Sloboda, Davidson, Howe, & Moore, 1996.
241
242 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
find significant effects such as those described earlier, we are probably un-
derestimating the real effect size, since the estimates will include amounts of
time with nonoptimized practice. Third, the type of training activity assessed
with estimates depends on the goal of the study: Time at the instrument may
be a suitable indicator for classical music training but not so for skill ac-
quisition in jazz (although good jazz musicians practice by themselves, too).
To use the retrospective estimates method in chess you might need to assess
the time spent studying published games (Charness, Krampe, & Mayr,
1996), in sight-reading you might need to assess time spent accompanying
(Lehmann & Ericsson, 1996), and in wrestling, time on the mat (Starkes et
al., 1996).
What we learn from these studies is that the amount of optimized practice
is related to the attained performance. Contrary to some criticisms of the
expertise view, this approach does not rule out possible individual differences
in aptitude; it merely focuses our view on those aspects of skill acquisition
that we can influence as educators. The criticism that this approach neglects
the quality of practice has already been mentioned and will be discussed in
the next section.
Practicing as a Skill. In light of the many factors that play a role in making
practice efficient, it comes as no surprise that experts and novices differ in
the way they practice. What emerges as a consistent finding from studies
that looked at the preparation of a specific piece or performance (Chaffin
& Imreh, 2000; Gruson, 1988; Hallam, 1995; Jrgensen, 1997; Lehmann
& Ericsson, 1998; Miklaszewski, 1989; Nielsen, 1997; Williamon & Val-
TAKING AN ACQUIRED SKILLS PERSPECTIVE 243
instance), there were also some striking differences, with the first rendition
beginning much louder than the second. A structural analysis of this Chopin
prelude reveals that it can be viewed either in a binary AA' structure or in
a more unified manner. The researchers concluded that the pianist varied the
two interpretations by highlighting each of the two possible structural frame-
works. The pianist offered little precise knowledge of what he did to create
differences between the two performances, other than saying that in the first
interpretation he was preoccupied with knowing whether or not one of the
researchers had children and so saw a scene with children and claiming that
in the second interpretation he imagined the piece within a rugged moorland
landscape. This use of visual imagery is commonly reported by many creative
and performing artists as a means of focusing their expressive intentions.
Experts often lack declarative knowledge about how they achieve certain
moment-by-moment changes, and our example illustrates that performers
often do not have a full awareness of what they do during a performance.
Instead, they monitor and influence the lower level processes from higher
and more abstract levelssuch as a guiding idea or atmosphere.
From the general psychology literature it is known that arousal, with all
its associated physiological traits (increased heart rate and thus heightened
oxygen supply), can facilitate mental operations and can often result in per-
formance task improvements in many domains (Gleitman, 1991). Musical
performance is no different, for when circumstances produce optimal
arousal, highly focused, energized, and inspired performances are often re-
ported (Evans, 1994). Cziksentmihalyi (1990) has referred to the intense
experience of optimal performance as flow. Given the overwhelmingly
positive reports of the enhanced quality of flow-driven or inspired-
feeling performances, it seems important that the performer should aim to
optimize his or her arousal level to become focused on the task. This implies
arousal management, which, in turn, is also a characteristic of high-level
performers (Wilson, 1997). The key point is, of course, that one cannot
produce an inspired performance without having extensive and easily op-
erationalized knowledge. The relevant task-related physical and mental skills
are achieved in often arduous and time-consuming rehearsal and practice
sessions.
Obviously, excessive arousal is undesirable, as it leads to performance
anxiety or stage fright (see Wilson, 1997, for a concise coverage), and thus
reducing the potential for being creative moment by moment. If, for exam-
ple, the anxiety leads to an increase in heart rate that causes palpitations,
the performance may be impaired. Indeed, violinists often report the dire
consequences of palpitations on their ability to control their bowing arm,
with the racing heart leading to an arm tremor.
So far, we have looked at individual performance. But musical perfor-
mance is a social act, and whether it is real or virtual (in the recording studio,
for example), the audience is critical in shaping the actual performance
event. Also, coperformers influence how the performance is negotiated and
presented to an audience (J. W. Davidson, 1997).
TAKING AN ACQUIRED SKILLS PERSPECTIVE 245
Nonverbal Communication
It is well known that vision is the dominant perceptual sense, with at least
75% of all information being communicated through this channel; hearing
only covers 13% and touch 6% (Long, 1997). It comes as no surprise,
therefore, that an increasing amount of research reports the critical role that
body movements have in the communication of musical performance infor-
mation. For example, J. W. Davidson (1993, 1994) found that audiences
detect finely grained information about musical expression and intention
from musicians body movements as well as their sounds. This is not sur-
prising, of course, since the body produces the musical sounds and is driven
by and itself cues the mental representations of the music. But in a climate
where different and contradictory teaching philosophies about how to train
musicians to play their instruments abound, it is worth critically evaluating
the role of nonverbal aspects of performance communication in detail.
The first major point is that nonmusicians tend to be more reliant on the
visual cues than the musical cues to discern whether a performer is playing
with or without expression (J. W. Davidson, 1995). The visual cues that
differentiate between performances range from varying quantities of move-
ment (varying from stillness to rapid actions like cyclical body swaying) to
the use of specific gestures that seem to form a movement repertoire (Da-
vidson, 2002). In terms of musical expression, it has been reported that the
varying degrees of movement and the employment of gestural cues are in-
evitable and largely desirable performance actions. Indeed, J. W. Davidson
and Dawson (1995) showed that when performers were only able to make
restricted movements in the music-learning process both sound and move-
ment aspects of their final performances were much more constrained and
less aesthetically appealing than when musicians were encouraged to use
their bodies in a natural manner. The more obviously culturally determined
gestures, such as an illustrative lifting of the saxophone bell to highlight the
effort involved in playing while also creating an impression of playing with
more intensity, are also reported as being desirable but need to be presented
at an optimal level: Too many and the performance seems overly exagger-
ated; too few and the performance seems stilted (Clarke & Davidson, 1998).
The second point of emphasis is that many performance movements have
clear roles:
Coordination
Performers in ensembles have been found to share characteristics in timing,
which indicate that the entire group has to be considered as one body with
different parts to it and with some sort of unifying performance plan (Ga-
brielsson, 1999). For example, in Baroque music performance the melody
instruments lead, while in jazz music the rhythm sections lead. By leading,
we mean that the onsets of new notes or harmonic changes came earlier for
some instruments than for others. Whether instrumentalists are aware of
this or not at any given time is a secondary concern. More important is the
notion that asynchronies must be coordinated by having specific ideas about
how the end result should sound and by being able to time individual per-
formance within the time frame of the other musicians. If it were the case
that the melody instruments tried to get ahead all the time, we should ob-
serve a steady increase in tempo (which we do not, at least not in profes-
sional performances). Thus performance in an ensemble requires a common
idea of what the piece should sound like. Unfortunately, in most classrooms
248 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
only one person, namely, the band or choral director, knows what the result
should be. Therefore, successful ensemble performance has to rely partly on
a shared mental representation, which has to be communicated to one an-
other either during performance via the conductor or through congruent
body movements of the different members of the ensemble. This is demon-
strated in figure 7.4.
Williamon and Davidson (2002) suggest that the balance between indi-
vidual and shared representation emerges during rehearsal and by the time
of performance coperformers are far more focused on shared rather than
individual musical goals. For instance, in a case study (which obviously has
Figure 7.4. Two panels showing at the top a group of musicians with individual
mental representations (desired performance or goal images) and at the bottom a
group with a shared mental representation.
TAKING AN ACQUIRED SKILLS PERSPECTIVE 249
only limited generalizability) the researchers traced two pianists who worked
on and then presented piano duo and duet repertoire in a concert. Initially,
issues such as when to place a chord and who should lead at a certain point
were central to their preparation concerns, but as a joint plan emerged, two
interesting phenomena occurred: (1) both began to move in a similar manner
(a rhythmic swaying that demonstrated some link to the musical pulse) to
facilitate the coordination of the music; (2) by the performance, they were
looking at each other constantly to negotiate subtle and more creative timing
variations than those structured in the rehearsals. Thus there was a critical
shift to a joint plan and action. Given the limited scope of the study, it is
unclear at this point how these coordinative and communicative skills
emerge and what types of training would facilitate their development, but
it is evident that teachers should note these kinds of changes in behavior
and attempt to find strategies to promote their development.
Appeal and Gender. As noted when considering the critical role of nonver-
bal behavior in music performances, audiences are generally sensitive to mu-
sical cues of various types; however, there are all kinds of factors that influ-
ence observer judgment. For instance, Wapnick, Darrow, Kovacs, and
Dalrymple (1997) carried out a brief study of university entrance auditions
for singers and discovered that those who were more animated, smiled more
often, and established more eye contact with their audience were rated more
highly and thought to be more attractive. In addition, J. W. Davidson and
Coimbra (2001) found that musical quality was only one of a number of
audience concerns with singers. Critical additional factors included whether
or not the performer looked tense and how verbal introductions were deliv-
ered. Indeed, the display that surrounded the music was of equal salience.
Like Wapnick et al., Davidson and Coimbra discovered that first written
comments by the audience tended to be about attractiveness, with phrases
like odd-looking chap and pretty girl, pretty voice.
In a series of experiments that used video recordings of female or male
performers playing either a jazz or a classical piece of music, subjects were
asked to rate the performances and the performers (experiments reported in
Behne, 1994). The subjects were not aware that the sound tracks for a given
piece were always identical, only the performers had been substituted. The
results revealed a strong influence of the visual attributes on the evaluation
of the performance, and gender stereotypes, such as female: expressive/dra-
matic and male: technical/precise, emerged. Consistent with results by J. W.
Davidson (see earlier), musicians tended to extract more information from
the visual display about the underlying musical interpretation than did non-
musicians.
Gender stereotypes also exist at the level of choice of instrument. For
250 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
example, ONeill and Boulton (1996) reported peer pressure for children to
learn only instruments appropriate to their gender: Flutes were deemed most
appropriate for girls, drums for boys! An interview by Johnsen (1992) with
Rebecca Bower (an American trombonist and one of the first successful fe-
male section leaders in a professional orchestra in the United States) showed
that gender biases are rife in music. Bower said that at auditions she was
refused jobs on grounds of being a woman in a mans world (trombonists
are male, after all!) and that audiences tended to be shocked to see that
someone so frail and feminine could play such a loud male instrument.
Not surprisingly, the switch to blind auditions, that is, auditions that use
a screen, has led to a considerable increase of women in professional or-
chestras since the 1970s (Goldin & Rouse, 1997).
Race and Gender. Of course, men and women behave and dress differently,
and so audiences react to performers differently, too. Indeed, anyone who
has attended various religious services will attest that African-American gos-
pel singers and their audiences are far less inhibited than their Anglo-Saxon
counterparts. Racial issues, like those having to do with gender or any other
kind of social bias, are complicated and mostly undesirable. In Europe at
least, there is not an established tradition of black classical musicians. When
J. W. Davidson (2003) asked audiences to evaluate men and women musi-
cians of black and white origin, they discovered that both black and white
male and female audience members preferred the performances by black
women. Objectively, the women were no better than the other performers
(indeed, the video sound track they were apparently performing was made
by another performer and was the same as that used for the other perform-
ers). The researchers interpreted this result as an illustration of positive dis-
crimination in favor of black women, who are not usually associated with
classical music. Elliott (1995), however, found many more typical and neg-
ative race and gender biases when black and white flute and trumpet stu-
dents were viewed and evaluated: Whites were rated higher than blacks, and
black women were rated much worse than black men on their abilities to
play a stereotypical male instrumentthe trumpet.
To summarize, the display of skill takes place in a social setting and is
therefore influenced (biased) in much the same way by social perceptions as
are all nonmusical social situations. More important, musicians have to ac-
quire nonverbal means of underlining their musical messages, and they
have to learn how to coordinate their performances with other musicians
musically and physically.
of her review that when genetic factors are strong, this does not mean that
environmental ones, including parenting, must be weak. The relation be-
tween the two is not a zero-sum game, and the additive assumption [i.e.,
that heritability indices are subtracted from 100 to yield the variance ex-
plained by the environment] is untenable (p. 22). What applies to parenting
could easily be said for other social learning situations, such as those pro-
vided by music education.
Many research methodologies employed in skill researchwhich by def-
inition is research in teaching, learning, and performancecould be turned
into educational tools by clever practitioners. Take, for instance, the verbal
protocol methodology mentioned earlier. If researchers can ask their subjects
to think aloud in order to find out what cognitive processes play a role
during performance, why should teachers not use similar strategies to get
into their students heads (Woody, 1999a)? Such attempts would also serve
to bridge the gap between research and teaching, which is sometimes re-
grettably large.
The most important characteristic of research in skill acquisition and hu-
man performance is that this research opens up an optimistic view for ed-
ucators because it does not focus on innate abilities (see Howe, 1999, for
supporting arguments). A common misconception is that skill researchers
deny or ignore individual differences that arise from factors beyond training
and practice; this is certainly not the case. But if we can explain some in-
dividual differences in performance by taking recourse to acquired cognitive
mechanisms, such as mental representations or aspects of practice, rather
than by attributing individual differences to (almost) immutable genetic or
dispositional factors, educators have a chance to consider those explanations
in designing future education. High performance is certainly not the be-all
and end-all of music education, but the need to achieve satisfactory levels
of performance (however one defines them) will always be part of successful
music teaching and learning.
STUDY QUESTIONS
1. (a) What are the general characteristics of skills and why are scientists
interested in skilled or expert performance? (b) Explain the nature and role
of mental representations.
2. Discuss the relation between practice and performance, taking into
account quality and quantity of practice. When is quantity of practice a good
predictor of outcome and when not?
3. The assessment of music performance is supposed to be objective and
unbiased. Is this true, and if not, what influences the audiences judgment?
(Include examples from your own experience).
TAKING AN ACQUIRED SKILLS PERSPECTIVE 253
REFERENCES
Barry, N., & Hallam, S. (2001). Practice. In R. Parncutt & G. McPherson (Eds.),
Science and Psychology of Music Performance (pp. 151166). Oxford: Ox-
ford University Press.
Barry, N. H., & McArthur, V. H. (1994). Teaching practice strategies in the mu-
sic studio: A survey of applied music teachers. Psychology of Music, 22, 44
55.
Behne, K. E. (1994). Gehort, gedacht, gesehen: Zehn Aufsatze zum visuellen,
kreativen und theoretischen Umgang mit Musik [Ten papers on the visual,
creative and theoretical approach to the musical experience]. Regensburg,
Germany: Con Brio.
Bloom, B. S. (Ed.). (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York: Bal-
lantine.
Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (1997). Pulling teeth and torture: Musical memory
and problem solving. Thinking and Reasoning, 3, 315336.
Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., & Crawford, M. (2002). Practicing Perfection: Memory
and Piano Performance. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Charness, N., Krampe, R. T., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coach-
ing in entrepreneurial skill domains. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to
excellence (pp. 5180). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clarke, E. F., & Davidson, J. W. (1998). The body in performance. In W. Tho-
mas (Ed.), Composition-performance-reception (pp. 7492). Aldershot, UK:
Ashgate.
Clynes, M., & Walker, J. (1982). Neurobiological functions of rhythm, time and
pulse in music. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, mind, and brain (pp. 171216).
New York: Plenum.
Cook, N. (1998) Analysing musical multimedia. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Cottrell, N. B. (1972). Social facilitation. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experi-
mental social psychology (pp. 131169). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Win-
ston.
Cziksentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New
York: Harper & Row.
Davidson, J. W. (1993). Visual perception of performance manner in the move-
ments of solo musicians. Psychology of Music, 21, 103113.
Davidson, J. W. (1994). What type of information is conveyed in the body move-
ments of solo musician performers? Journal of Human Movement Studies, 6,
279301.
Davidson, J. W. (1995). What does the visual information contained in music
performances offer the observer? Some preliminary thoughts. In R. Steinberg
(Ed.), The music machine: Psychophysiology and psychopathology of the
sense of music (pp. 105113). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
Davidson, J. W. (1997). The social in music performance. In D. Hargreaves &
A. North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 209248). Oxford: Ox-
ford University Press.
Davidson, J. W. (2002). Understanding the expressive movements of a solo pi-
anist. Musikpsychologie, 16, 729.
Davidson, J. W., & Coimbra, D. C. (2001). Investigating performance evaluation
by assessors of singers in a music college setting. Musicae Scientiae, 5, 3354.
254 MUSICAL COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT
Sundberg, J., Fryden, L., & Friberg, A. (1995). Expressive aspects of instrumen-
tal and vocal performance. In R. Steinberg (Ed.), Music and the mind ma-
chine: The psychophysiology and psychopathology of the sense of music
(pp. 4962). New York: Springer.
Todd, N. P. M. (1985). A model of expressive timing in tonal music. Music Per-
ception, 3, 3358.
Trusheim, W. H. (1991). Audiation and mental imagery: Implications for artistic
performance. Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 2, 138
147.
Tunks, T. W. (1992). The transfer of music learning. In R. Colwell (Ed.), Hand-
book of research on music teaching and learning (pp. 437447). New York:
Schirmer Books.
Wagner, C. (1988). The pianists hand: Anthropometry and biomechanics. Er-
gonomics, 31, 97131.
Wapnick, J., Darrow, A. A., Kovacs, J., & Dalrymple, L. (1997). Effects of phys-
ical attractiveness on evaluation of vocal performance. Journal of Research
in Music Education, 45, 470479.
Williamon, R.A. & Davidson, J.W. (2002) Exploring co-performer communi-
cation. Musicae Scientiae, 6, 117.
Williamon, A., & Valentine, E. (2000). Quantity and quality of musical practice
as predictors of performance quality. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 353
376.
Wilson, G. D. (1997). Performance anxiety. In D. Hargreaves & A. North (Eds.),
The social psychology of music (pp. 229248). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Winner, E. (1996). The rage to master: The decisive role of talent in the visual
arts. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence (pp. 271302). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Woody, R. H. (1999a). Getting into their heads. American Music Teacher, 49(3),
2427.
Woody, R. H. (1999b). The relationship between advanced musicians explicit
planning and their expressive performance of dynamic variations in an aural
modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 331342.
Woody, R. H. (2003). Explaining expressive performance: Component cognitive
skills in an aural modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education,
51(1), 5163.
Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269274.
Zdzinski, S. F. (1996). Parental involvement, selected student attributes, and
learning outcomes in instrumental music. Journal of Research in Music Ed-
ucation, 44, 3448.
Index