Baldwin Student Assessment Project
Baldwin Student Assessment Project
Baldwin Student Assessment Project
Theresa Baldwin
EDU 325
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 2
Introduction
Cody is a first grade student at a public elementary school in the Midwest United States.
He is six years old, lives in an urban setting, and has one younger sibling in kindergarten who
also attends Pugliese West. According to his homeroom teacher, he is on grade level in reading,
spelling, and writing. He is advanced in math. Cody is an eager and focused student who
always follows directions. He is a hard worker and is very kind to his classmates and to
everyone he meets. He gets along well with his peers and easily adapts in different social
settings. His organization and decision making skills are appropriate for a first grader. His
teacher does not need to use motivators or reinforcers because he is eager to please and he never
needs to be pushed.
education plan (IEP). However, he is considered at-risk and he does receive additional reading
instruction outside of the classroom four days a week. This tutoring is focused on direct,
individualized instruction in phonemic awareness, letter skills, word reading, spelling, and sight
words. I have gotten to know Cody because I tutor him two days a week. I agree with
everything his teacher said about him; he is very focused during our sessions and is a quick
learner. We have spent many sessions learning new phonemes, working on word attack skills,
memorizing sight words, and working on spelling and reading fluency. He still has a long way
to go in becoming a fluent reader, but I have seen a lot of progress in all of these areas in the past
two months.
Procedures
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 3
Since I tutor Cody with Success for All (SFA), I asked for permission from his teacher to
administer the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS) assessment during
one of our tutoring sessions. Once receiving permission, I emailed the teacher and asked if she
would fill out the student background information. She asked me to bring her a hard copy of the
document, so I did. I picked it up from her the following week. She was concerned with privacy
issues and for that reason she was very brief in her responses.
In preparation for performing the assessment efficiently, I practiced the assessment with a
peer and reviewed the probes. On the day of the assessment, October 15, I arrived at the school,
signed in, and went upstairs. I first tutored another student and then picked Cody up from his
classroom and brought him to a table in the hallway outside of the classroom. We sat down
beside each other and I told him we would be doing something different than usual. I made sure
he understood that there might be things he did not know but that he should try his very best. I
asked Cody if he was ready to begin, and he said he was. I assessed him using the DIBELS Next
measures and probes. The assessment went very smoothly. I delivered it exactly how the
manual suggested. Cody was focused and did exactly what he was told. There were some
students passing by and talking in the hallway throughout the assessment, but they did not appear
to distract Cody. We got through all the probes quickly and still had time to get in some
tutoring. I thanked him for helping me out and cooperating so well and then I brought him back
to his classroom. When I returned home, I calculated his scores and recorded them on the front
of the assessment booklet. I compared them to benchmarks and found that, although Cody’s
teacher says he is on grade level for reading, his scores indicate he is below or well-below
benchmark for all assessments except whole words read (WWR) in the Nonsense Word Fluency
probe. In response to this, I chose to target his weakest areas, Phoneme Segmentation Fluency
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 4
and Oral Reading Fluency. I then selected an evidence-based strategy for each, Elkonin boxes
for phoneme segmentation fluency and Readers Theater for oral reading fluency.
Assessments Given
The DIBELS assessment for first grade assesses four measures of early literacy skills.
The first subtest tests Letter Naming Fluency, the second tests Phoneme Segmentation Fluency,
the third tests Nonsense Word Fluency, and the fourth tests Oral Reading Fluency. The purpose
of these assessments is to identify students who may be at risk for reading difficulties, to help
teachers guide their instruction, to progress monitor at-risk students, and to examine the
effectiveness of the school’s instructional supports. All subtests are administered in one-minute
saying their names (Good & Kaminski, 2011). The assessor gives the student a page of a mix of
uppercase and lowercase letters in a random order and asks the student to start on the first line
and name the letters when the timer starts. The student begins to name the letters and the
assessor marks any letters that are named incorrectly or skipped. This probe is scored by
counting the number of letters that the student named correctly in one minute. Letter naming
fluency is a strong predictor of later reading achievement, but it is not essential for achieving
reading outcomes (Good & Kaminski, 2011). For this reason, it is not a basic early literacy skill
and thus a benchmark goal is not provided. Instruction should not be focused on letter names,
but rather on developing phonological awareness and knowledge of the alphabetic principle
monosyllabic word into the correct amount of phonemes (Haughbrook, Hart, Schatschneider, &
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 5
Taylor, 2017). The assessor explains how the assessment works to the student. He/she tells the
student that they are going to say all the sounds they hear in words. The assessor gives several
examples and then has the student practice one before beginning the assessment (Good &
Kaminski, 2011). An example of correct phoneme segmentation of the word ‘bat’ is orally
sounding out the three separate phonemes as /b/ /a/ /t/. The assessor then begins the timer and
keeps track of how many phonemes the student correctly identifies. The probe is scored by
counting the number of phonemes that the student named correctly in one minute (Haughbrook
et al, 2017).
Nonsense Word Fluency measures a student’s ability to decode nonsense words, which
are letter sequences that follow regular phonetic rules but have no meaning, for example, lif or
yom or mib (Marshall, 2018). The measure of Nonsense Word Fluency assesses students’
read and sound out each separate phoneme in a word (Cummings, 2011). These separate
phonemes in a word are known as letter sounds, and for this probe, the student is scored by how
many correct letter sounds (CLS) he/she pronounces. According to research, there are distinct
connections between a student’s ability to pronounce correct letter sounds and later reading
Oral Reading Fluency tests a student’s reading accuracy and fluency while he/she reads
connected text (Haughbrook et al, 2017). The score is the number of words from a passage that
the student reads correctly in one minute (Cummings, 2013). Words that are not read correctly
and thus not scored are omitted words, substituted words, and words that were not said within
three seconds (Haughbrook et al, 2017). The probe also measures reading comprehension, as the
student is asked to retell what he/she read. By examining a student’s accuracy of reading a
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 6
passage, Oral Reading Fluency measures word attack skills and advanced phonics skills (Good &
Kaminski, 2011). Verbal reading proficiency is one of the most common, effective, and reliable
indicators of reading comprehension (Rasplica & Cummings, 2013). This is because fluent
readers are able to use their brain for comprehending texts rather than for decoding words.
Cody’s probe results were compared to the DIBELS Next Benchmark Goals. The
DIBELS assessment is typically given three times a year, at the beginning of the year, in the
middle of the year, and at the end of the year. There are different benchmark scores for each of
these three times of year. Cody’s scores were compared against the beginning-of-the-year
benchmarks, since the assessment was given in October. The only measures that were compared
to middle-of-the-year benchmarks are the Oral Reading Fluency words correct and accuracy,
since these are not typically assessed in the beginning of first grade.
Cody’s Letter Naming Fluency score was 41, which means that he was able to correctly
name 41 letters during the one minute probe. There is no benchmark provided for Letter Naming
Fluency, so this score was not compared to benchmark. In the Phoneme Segmentation Fluency
measure, Cody scored 24, which means that he was able to pronounce 24 separated phonemes in
one minute. His score is well below benchmark because a benchmark score is between 40 and
46. This indicates that he is likely to need intensive support in this area. In the Nonsense Word
Fluency measure, Cody got 25 letter sounds correct and read four whole words without orally
sounding them out first. While the four Whole Words Read is actually above benchmark for the
beginning of the year, the score of 25 Correct Letter Sounds is below benchmark. This signifies
that he is likely to need strategic support in this area. The Oral Reading Fluency probe was
given three times with three different passages. Cody read an average of ten words correctly in
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 7
one minute, which is a score of 10. This score is well below benchmark (23-33). However, this
is a Middle-of-the-Year benchmark since there is not one provided for the beginning of the year,
so he may be able to perform a little better by the middle of the year. His Oral Reading Fluency
accuracy, which is the percent of all the read words that were read correctly, was 70%, which is
considered below benchmark, indicating a need for strategic support. Cody’s composite score of
90 indicates that he is well below benchmark and is therefore likely to need intensive support.
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
PSF NWF-CLS ORF Words Correct* ORF Accuracy (%)*
A common pattern in the Phoneme Segmentation Fluency assessment was that, for
almost all of the words, Cody segmented the word into its onset and rime, rather than into its
individual phonemes. For instance, for the word song, he said /s/ /ong/. This pattern continued
for eight of the twelve words he read during the minute-long probe. For the Nonsense Word
Fluency assessment, Cody did fairly well in saying the words correctly after sounding it out.
One issue, which I know is an area of difficulty for him, is that he often mixes up the letters b
The Oral Reading Fluency assessment was clearly a struggle for Cody. He was only able
to read an average of ten words in a minute. He was not distracted, but would stare at a word for
a few seconds before orally attempting to sound it out. Once he did sound it out, he still only got
the word correct 70% of the time. When I asked him to retell what he had read, he just started
listing some of the words that were in the passage in no particular order and with no meaning.
words. His average retell score was 3.6, which means he was able to provide 3.6 words that were
relevant to the passage that he read. Although Cody performed below benchmark on most of the
assessments, he gave his full attention to the test and certainly gave his best effort, even when he
Based on the results of the DIBELS assessment, two areas were targeted for further
improvement: phoneme segmentation and reading fluency. Proficient phonemic awareness, the
ability to identify and manipulate individual sounds in spoken words, by first grade is a strong
indicator of later reading success, and phoneme segmentation is a significant aspect of phonemic
awareness (Keesey, Konrad, & Joseph, 2015). That is why it is critical for a first grader to be
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 9
proficient in phoneme segmentation. A strategy that has been shown to improve phoneme
segmentation fluency is Elkonin sound boxes (Elkonin, 1963; Keesey et al, 2015). This is a
strategy in which words are orally broken into their phonemes by pointing at or pushing tokens
into boxes. For the word sheep, for example, a student would push a token into each of the three
provided boxes as he/she verbally segments the phonemes /sh/ /ē/ and /p/ (McCarthy, 2008). The
strategy is done orally and nothing is written, as the goal is not to practice spelling or writing but
to increase phonemic awareness. Keesey, Konrad, and Joseph (2015) conducted a study to
examine the effects of sound boxes on phonological, reading, and spelling skills of three at-risk
kindergarteners. They found that sound box instruction increased the students’ segmenting skills
and letter-sound correspondences, which in turn improved their reading and spelling skills. In
addition, the students maintained these skills after intervention. If this strategy were to be used
with Cody, progress could be monitored by administering the Phoneme Segmentation Fluency
The second target area for Cody is Oral Reading Fluency, because he demonstrated a
need for intensive intervention in this area. A strategy that has been shown to significantly
improve reading fluency is Readers Theater (Martinez, Roser, & Strecker, 1998/1999). In
Readers Theater, students perform a written script. There are no costumes, props, or scenery
involved, so the students must rely on voices to convey meaning (Young & Rasinski, 2009).
This requires them to interpret the author’s intended meaning of the text and deliver that
meaning to an audience by oral interpretative reading. This strategy provides struggling readers
al, 2013). The strategy incorporates repeated and assisted reading until the student reaches a
high level of fluency before he/she performs (Mraz et al, 2013; Young & Rasinski, 2009).
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 10
Students are provided with guidance and feedback from their teacher and peers which helps them
improve further. There are many benefits to this strategy. It improves reading performance,
prosody, accuracy and automaticity in word recognition, oral reading fluency, interpretive skills,
and comprehension. It is also a highly engaging activity that can motivate students to read (Mraz
et al, 2013; Young & Rasinski, 2009). If this strategy were to be used with Cody, progress could
be monitored by administering the Oral Reading Fluency probe continually throughout and after
the intervention.
Conclusion
The information and data gathered from this assessment were very beneficial and useful
signifies where growth is needed and helps teachers plan instruction around their students’ needs.
Cody is below benchmark in most areas of reading, and so his teacher can make instructional
decisions and offer him the support that he needs to be a successful reader and a successful
student. Cody is an intelligent boy and a quick learner, and if he is given additional support, I
think that he will be able to reach benchmarks by the end of the year.
(CBM) which was a very valuable experience. I learned how important it is to be sure that
students are receiving the instruction that they need, and the only way to know what they need is
through assessments such as DIBELS. I also got practice using assessment data to make
hypothetical instructional decisions and justify the rationale behind why I chose the specific
strategies. Overall, this project taught me a lot and helped me to feel more prepared to be a
teacher.
STUDENT ASSESSMENT PROJECT 11
Bibliography
Cummings, K. D., Dewey, E. N., Latimer, R. J., & Good, R. H. (2011). Pathways to word
reading and decoding: The roles of automaticity and accuracy. School Psychology
Cummings, K. D., Park, Y., Schaper, H. A. B. (2013). Form effects on DIBELS Next oral
reading fluency progress- monitoring passages. Assessment for Effective Instruction, 38(2),
91-104.
Elkonin, D. B. (1963). The psychology of mastering the elements of reading. In B. Simon & J.
Simon (Eds.), Educational psychology in the U.S.S.R. (pp. 165–179). London, England:
Good, R.H., & Kaminski, R. A. (2011). DIBELS Next assessment manual. Eugene, OR: Dynamic
Measurement Group.
Haughbrook, R., Hart, S. A., Schatschneider, C., & Taylor, J. (2017). Genetic and environmental
influences on early literacy skills across school grade contexts. Developmental Science,
20(5), 1-12.
Keesey, S., Konrad, M., & Joseph, L. M. (2015). Word boxes improve phonemic awareness,
Marshall, A. (2018, March 19). The nonsense of teaching nonsense words. Retrieved from
https://blog.dyslexia.com/nonsense-teaching-words/
Martinez, M., Roser, N.L., & Strecker, S. (1998/1999). “I never thought I could be a star”: A
McCarthy, P. A. (2008). Using sound boxes systematically to develop phonemic awareness. The
Mraz, M., Nichols, W., Caldwell, S., Beisley, R., Sargent, S., & Rupley, W. (2013). Improving
oral reading fluency through Readers Theatre. Reading Horizons Journal, 52(2), 163-
180.
Rasplica, C., & Cummings, K. D. (2013). Oral Reading Fluency. Retrieved from https://council-
for-learning-disabilities.org/what-is-oral-reading-fluency-verbal-reading-proficiency
Young, C., & Rasinski, T. (2009). Implementing Readers Theatre as an approach to classroom