ACE Mahuranath
ACE Mahuranath
ACE Mahuranath
Article abstract—Objectives: To validate a simple bedside test battery designed to detect mild dementia and differentiate
AD from frontotemporal dementia (FTD). Methods: Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) is a 100-point test
battery that assesses six cognitive domains. Of 210 new patients attending a memory clinic, 139 fulfilled inclusion criteria
and comprised dementia (n ! 115) and nondementia (n ! 24) groups. The composite and the component scores on the ACE
for the two groups were compared with those of 127 age- and education-matched controls. Norms and the probability
of diagnosing dementia at different prevalence rates were calculated. To evaluate the ACE’s ability to differentiate early
AD from FTD, scores of the cases diagnosed with dementia with a Clinical Dementia Rating ! 1 (AD ! 56, FTD ! 24,
others ! 20) were compared. Results: Two cut-off values for the ACE composite score (88 and 83) were of optimal utility
depending on the target population. The ACE had high reliability, construct validity, and sensitivity (93%, using 88 as
cut-off). Using the lower cut-off of 83, the ACE had a higher sensitivity (82%) and predictive value than the Mini-Mental
State Examination for a wide range of dementia prevalence. The ACE differentiated AD from FTD, and the VLOM ratio
(derived using component scores: [verbal fluency " language]/[orientation " memory]) of #2.2 for FTD and $3.2 for AD
was highly discriminating. Conclusion: The ACE is a brief and reliable bedside instrument for early detection of dementia,
and offers a simple objective index to differentiate AD and FTD in mildly demented patients.
NEUROLOGY 2000;55:1613–1620
The need for screening and diagnostic tests to pro- early and accurate diagnosis, preferably in the pre-
vide an objective measure of cognitive function has dementia stage of the disease3; and 3) the enormous
increased over the last two decades largely as a re- increase in memory clinics, reflecting a growing con-
sult of three developments: 1) the realization that a cern in the general community about failing memory
substantial proportion of patients previously diag- in late life.4
nosed with AD have other pathologies, notably de- Several screening and diagnostic tests for demen-
mentia with Lewy bodies1 and frontotemporal tia have been developed,5,6 but no single test is the
dementia (FTD)2; 2) the advent of disease-modifying established standard.3 More comprehensive cognitive
treatments for AD, making it vital to establish an batteries such as the cognitive section (CAMCOG) of
the Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of
the Elderly (CAMDEX)7 and the Dementia Rating
Scale (DRS)8 require specialized test equipment or
Additional material related to this article can be found on the Neurology trained personnel to administer, and are beyond the
Web site. Go to www.neurology.org and scroll down the Table of Con-
tents for the December 12 issue to find the title link for this article.
scope of routine bedside cognitive evaluation. The
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)5 is one of
From the University of Cambridge Neurology Unit (Drs. Mathuranath, Nestor, and Hodges, and W. Rakowicz) and Department of Psychiatry (Dr. Berrios),
University of Cambridge, Addenbrooke’s Hospital; and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit (Dr. Hodges), Cambridge, UK.
Received December 27, 1999. Accepted in final form September 21, 2000.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Professor John R. Hodges, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 2EF,
UK; e-mail: john.hodges@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk
Demographics
Male sex 61 19 63
Age, y* 66.6 (8.9) 63.8 (7.0) 64.4 (9.3)
Education, y* 11.1 (2.6) 12.7 (3.6) 11.3 (2.6)
ACE (maximum possible score)
Orientation score (10) 7.3 (2.6) 9.8 (0.3) 9.8 (0.3) † †
Attention score (8) 6.0 (2.2) 7.7 (0.5) 7.8 (0.4) † †
Total memory score (35) 17.8 (9.1) 30.3 (3.4) 32.6 (1.9) † †
Name and address learning (21) 13.9 (6.1) 19.4 (1.9) 20.3 (1.0) † †
Name and address delayed recall (7) 1.6 (2.1) 5.5 (1.5) 6.2 (1.0) † †
Verbal fluency (14) 6.3 (3.0) 8.6 (3.2) 11.0 (2.2) † †
Letter P (7) 3.6 (1.9) 4.2 (1.8) 5.4 (1.4) 0.12 †
Category (7) 2.7 (1.6) 4.4 (1.6) 5.5 (1.2) † †
Language (includes naming) (28) 24.1 (4.8) 26.8 (2.5) 27.8 (0.5) ‡ †
Visuospatial (5) 3.1 (1.7) 4.8 (0.4) 4.6 (0.5) † †
Mini-Mental State Examination (30) 21.8 (6.1) 29.8 (5.8) 29.1 (0.8) † †
ACE (composite score) (100) 64.8 (18.9) 88.0 (8.2) 93.8 (3.5) † †
Controls. The control group consisted of 127 age- and nificantly lower scores than the control group on all compo-
education-matched individuals who were either attendees nents and the nondementia group on all except letter
at an orthopaedic (n ! 36) or gynecologic (n ! 28) clinic, fluency.
spouses of the attendees at the above clinics (n ! 26), or Normative data and validity. Two methods were used
members of the Medical Research Council subject panel to calculate the norms for the composite score on the ACE.
(n ! 37). Subjects with a history of head injury, drug The first cut-off score of 88 represented a value two stan-
abuse, alcoholism, and neurologic or cognitive complaints dard deviations below the mean composite score for the
were excluded. control group. The second cut-off was obtained by estimat-
ing the probability of diagnosing dementia (in terms of
Results. Table 1 summarizes the demographic charac- positive [PPV] and negative [NPV] predictive values) in
teristics and mean (SD) individual component and compos- the 139 subjects in the clinic group. We estimated the
ite scores on the ACE for the dementia, nondementia, and sensitivity, specificity, and predictive values for diagnosing
control groups. Age and education were not significantly dementia at different prevalence rates (5, 10, 20, and 40%)
different between the groups. The dementia group had sig- for each ACE cut-off score from 80 to 90. A broad range of
Table 2 Sensitivity and specificity of different ACE (and MMSE) cut-off scores for diagnosing dementia, with corresponding
probabilities of dementia (PPV) at different rates of prevalence
ACE
88 0.93 0.71 0.14 (0.99) 0.26 0.44 0.68 (0.94)
83 0.82 0.96 0.51 (0.99) 0.69 0.83 0.93 (0.89)
MMSE
27 0.74 0.96 0.48 (0.99) 0.66 0.82 0.92 (0.85)
24 0.52 0.96 0.40 (0.97) 0.58 0.76 0.89 (0.75)
Table 3 Numbers (%) of patients correctly diagnosed with dementia on the ACE and the MMSE distributed by their type and severity
ACE ! Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination; MMSE ! Mini-Mental State Examination; CDR ! Clinical Dementia Rating.
1616 NEUROLOGY 55 December (1 of 2) 2000
Downloaded from www.neurology.org by Kathy Pieper on October 26, 2011
Table 4 Comparison of mean scores on components of the
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination: AD with non-AD and
frontotemporal dementia (FTD) with non-FTD
* Mann–Whitney U-test two-tailed significance. 24) but detected by the ACE (cut-off 83) had a CDR ! 1.
† Higher mean score where p # 0.05. The ACE thus maintained a good sensitivity for diagnosing
dementia in subgroups with differing dementia type and
severity.
Differentiating AD and FTD. Of the 139 patients in
the clinic group, the ACE (cut-off score 88) identified 114
as having dementia, of whom 100 (56 AD, 24 FTD, 10 VaD,
and 10 others) had a CDR ! 1. Comparison of the AD and
FTD subgroups on different components of the ACE re-
vealed significant differences on orientation, attention, and
memory, which were worse in AD, and letter fluency, lan-
guage, and naming, which were worse in FTD (table 4).
Figure 2 contrasts the performance of patients with AD
and FTD on the most discriminating subtests. In order to
translate this pattern of scoring into an index useful in
differentiating AD and FTD subgroups, we calculated the
ratio of scores on verbal fluency plus language to orienta-
tion plus name and address delayed recall memory. This
value, (V"L)/(O"M)—the VLOM ratio—tended to be
higher in patients with AD and lower in those with FTD.
For data on the sensitivity, specificity, and predictive val-
ues (at prevalence rates of 10, 20, 30, and 40%) of various
VLOM ratios for differentiating AD from non-AD (and
FTD from non-FTD), please access www.neurology.org. It
is clear that a VLOM ratio of $ 3.2 optimally differentiates
AD from non-AD (sensitivity 75% and specificity 84%), and
a ratio of # 2.2 optimally differentiates FTD from non-FTD
(sensitivity 58% and specificity 97%). Figure 3 shows that
in the patient subgroup with an ACE score #88 and CDR
! 1 the majority with a VLOM ratio below 2.2 had FTD,
whereas above a ratio of 3.2 most had AD. Between 2.2
and 3.2, patients could have FTD, AD, or one of the other
dementias.
The demographic variables of age, years of education,
and gender were added to a logistic regression model as
covariates to determine their effect on the predictive out-
come (dementia or nondementia) obtained using each of
Figure 2. Box plots comparing AD with frontotemporal the instruments—ACE (cut-off scores 88 and 83) and
dementia (FTD) on subcomponents of the Addenbrooke’s MMSE (cut-off scores 24 and 27). Whereas none of these
Cognitive Examination (ACE): (A) verbal (letter) fluency demographic variables was significant at the 0.05 level for
and language; (B) orientation and delayed recall memory. either of the ACE cut-off scores, age (p ! 0.05) and gender
December (1 of 2) 2000 NEUROLOGY 55 1617
Downloaded from www.neurology.org by Kathy Pieper on October 26, 2011
(p ! 0.02) were found to be significant for both the MMSE dementia (CDR ! 1) when they received the ACE.
cut-off scores. When the lower cut-off score of 83 was used, the ACE
detected dementia in 79% of all patients with demen-
Discussion. Extensive experience with the ACE tia, a third more than that achieved using the con-
suggests that it is a reliable instrument for the early ventional cut-off score of 24 on the MMSE. It
detection of dementia and meets most of the stan- appeared particularly sensitive in FTD where it
dards required by such an instrument.3 nearly doubled the detection rate when compared to
How the ACE differs from the MMSE. Since the the MMSE. In a third of all patients with dementia
introduction of the MMSE over two decades ago5 in this study a clinical consensus diagnosis based
there has been an enormous increase in our knowl- upon data from extensive assessments could only be
edge of the dementias and of normal cognition. The reached after a further follow-up of 6 to 12 months.
major differences between the ACE and the MMSE These facts highlight the utility of the ACE in the
lie in the evaluation of memory, fluency, and lan- early diagnosis of dementia.
guage. The memory component of the ACE was ex- Differentiating AD and FTD at an early stage with
panded to reflect the importance of episodic memory the ACE. Predicting the type of dementia that pa-
impairment in the early detection of AD.11,37,38 The tients will eventually develop is often difficult early
name and address delayed recall test appears to be a in the course of the disease. We are unaware of any
particularly sensitive measure in AD. The ACE also other bedside instrument capable of differentiating
incorporates verbal fluency, which relies upon fron- patients at an early stage of dementia. On the ACE,
tal executive function.39 Additionally, letter-based the patients with FTD performed better on tests of
fluency depends upon phonologic processing and orientation and episodic memory, whereas the pa-
category-based fluency on semantic memory.40 tients with AD did relatively better on verbal fluency
Whereas letter-based fluency is particularly sensi- and language, consistent with the known neuropsy-
tive to FTD, category fluency is a marker of semantic chological profiles of the two disorders.12,13,17,47 Based
memory breakdown, including that in AD.41,42 upon this pattern of performance, we devised the
The naming task in the ACE was made more dif- VLOM ratio to differentiate AD and FTD. To illus-
ficult by having 12 line drawings of medium or low trate this point, consider the example of a patient
frequency concept, based on the finding that naming presenting to a typical memory clinic in which the
is highly dependent upon the concept frequency and prevalence rate of AD is around 40% and FTD is
age of acquisition.43 In this study, the AD patients lower, at 10%. If the patient has a VLOM ratio $3.2,
performed well on naming tasks, reflecting their then the probability of AD is 76% and if the ratio is
mild disease. The changes to the nonsemantic as- ! 3.2, then the probability of not having AD is about
pects of language evaluation in the ACE are in- 83%. If the patient’s VLOM ratio is #2.2, then the
tended for early detection of FTD, particularly the probability of FTD is 71% and conversely, if the ratio
progressive aphasic variants.2,13 Changes to the is " 2.2, then FTD can be excluded with 95% cer-
visuospatial section include adding a three- tainty. It is evident that the confidence with which a
dimensional wire cube copying and a clock face diagnosis of AD (or FTD) can be made, or excluded,
drawing test, which have greater scope for detecting depends on the prevalence rate of AD (or FTD) in the
impaired constructional abilities than copying a two- population in question. The predictive values of dif-
dimensional figure.44,45 Both the AD and FTD groups ferent cut-offs for different prevalence rates can be
performed well on this component, reflecting the found at www.neurology.org. Although not infallible,
very mild nature of the dementia in this cohort. an objective and simply calculated index such as the
Psychometric properties of the ACE. The reliabil- VLOM ratio will be helpful in prognosticating and
ity of the ACE is evident in its high internal consis- planning future management of patients.
tency, which indicates that all its component scores Applications of the ACE. When screening an at-
contribute to the measurement of cognitive functions risk population for dementia, a test instrument
and correlate well with the composite score, which in should have a high sensitivity for mild dementia as a
turn determines the presence or absence of demen- missed diagnosis can delay implementation of useful
tia. Among its various components, construct valid- social and pharmacologic interventions. At a specific-
ity was best for memory and verbal fluency showing ity of 71%, a cut-off score of 88 on the ACE offered a
good concordance with standard neuropsychological high sensitivity of 93%. At a dementia prevalence
tests. The ACE also showed good construct validity rate of 40% (such as in a memory clinic) a very high
for the diagnosis of dementia and had a high sensi- proportion (68%) of those who screen positive will
tivity, even with the lower cut-off score of 83. have dementia and 94% of those who score above the
Early diagnosis with the ACE. In the evaluation cut-off will not. The lower cut-off score of 83 might be
of any new instrument, an important question to be more appropriate for research studies or therapeutic
answered is how advanced dementia needs to be be- trials, which require a high specificity, or when
fore the instrument can detect it. To address this screening populations with a low base rate of demen-
issue we used the CDR to grade the severity of de- tia. At a sensitivity of 82%, a cut-off score of 83 had a
mentia in functional terms.46 The majority of pa- high specificity of 96%. In a population with a 10%
tients in this study had only mild or lesser grades of dementia prevalence rate, 69% of those who fail the
1618 NEUROLOGY 55 December (1 of 2) 2000
Downloaded from www.neurology.org by Kathy Pieper on October 26, 2011
test will eventually meet diagnostic criteria for de- diagnostic criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type. Neu-
rology 1994;44:872– 877.
mentia, and only 2% of those with dementia will 2. Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. Frontotemporal
escape detection. lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria.
The influence of age, gender, education, and cul- Neurology 1998;51:1546 –1554.
tural background on the ACE and the MMSE needs 3. Gifford DR, Cummings JL. Evaluating dementia screening
tests: methodologic standards to rate their performance [edito-
further evaluation. In our patient sample, age, num- rial; comment]. Neurology 1999;52:224 –227.
ber of years of education, or gender did not influence 4. Verhey FR, Jolles J, Ponds RW, et al. Diagnosing dementia: a
the predictive outcome using the ACE, but both age comparison between a monodisciplinary and a multidisciplinary
and gender influenced the predictive outcome using approach. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci 1993;5:78 – 85.
5. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state.” A
the MMSE. practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for
Alterations in personality and behavior are well- the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189 –198.
recognized features of early FTD.48 There are, how- 6. Buschke H, Kuslansky G, Katz M, et al. Screening for demen-
tia with the memory impairment screen [see comments]. Neu-
ever, no reliable patient-based measures of behavior. rology 1999;52:231–238.
We deliberately avoided carer-based measures in our 7. Huppert FA, Brayne C, Gill C, Paykel ES, Beardsall L.
instrument with the intention of designing a brief CAMCOG–a concise neuropsychological test to assist demen-
objective test. Although sensitive to early dementia tia diagnosis: socio-demographic determinants in an elderly
population sample. Br J Clin Psychol 1995;34:529 –541.
on its own, the ACE may prove even more useful 8. Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological
when used in combination with carer-based instru- Assessment Resources, Inc., 1988.
ments for behavior, such as the Neuropsychiatric 9. Tombaugh TN, McIntyre NJ. The Mini-Mental State Exami-
nation: a comprehensive review [see comments]. J Am Geriatr
Inventory.19 Soc 1992;40:922–935.
Limitations and future directions. The problem 10. Feher EP, Mahurin RK, Doody RS, Cooke N, Sims J, Pirozzolo
of false positive diagnosis with the higher cut-off FJ. Establishing the limits of the Mini-Mental State: exami-
score on the ACE is likely to be more apparent than nation of subtests. Arch Neurol 1992;49:87–92.
11. Greene JD, Baddeley AD, Hodges JR. Analysis of the episodic
real in this study. Early AD patients are typically memory deficit in early Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from the
only amnestic11 and do not fulfill the DSM-IV criteria doors and people test. Neuropsychologia 1996;34:537–551.
for dementia. However, they often fail on the ACE 12. Gregory CA, Orrell M, Sahakian B, Hodges JR. Can fronto-
because of the deliberately large weighting (35 of the temporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease be differentiated
using a brief battery of tests? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997;
100 points) toward memory. Patients with other iso- 12:375–383.
lated cognitive deficits, such as aphasia, which occur 13. Hodges JR, Patterson K, Ward R, et al. The differentiation of
in early FTD, are also likely to appear as false posi- semantic dementia and frontal lobe dementia (temporal and
frontal variants of frontotemporal dementia) from early Alz-
tives on the ACE. In our sample, three of the seven heimer’s disease: a comparative neuropsychological study.
patients receiving a false positive diagnosis on the Neuropsychology 1999;13:31– 40.
ACE had FTD and all three had progressive nonflu- 14. Naugle RI, Kawczak K. Limitations of the Mini-Mental State
ent aphasia. Examination. Cleve Clin J Med 1989;56:277–281.
15. Teng EI, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS)
The ACE requires more time than the MMSE to Examination. J Clin Psychiatry 1987;48:314 –318.
complete but rarely takes in excess of 15 to 20 min- 16. Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, Funnell E. Semantic de-
utes, except in very demented subjects. It is most mentia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atro-
phy. Brain 1992;115:1783–1806.
useful, however, in patients with early and mild de- 17. Pachana NA, Boone KB, Miller BL, Cummings JL, Berman N.
mentia. The loss in brevity is offset by a gain in the Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer’s
sensitivity for early dementia and being able to pre- disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc
dict the type of dementia. Although the ACE is supe- 1996;2:505–510.
18. Mendez MF, Doss RC, Cherrier MM. Use of the cognitive estima-
rior to the MMSE, a comparison with independent tions test to discriminate frontotemporal dementia from Alzhei-
batteries such as the AD Assessment Scale–Cog49 mer’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1998;11:2– 6.
and the Dementia Rating Scale8 will be valuable. 19. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg–Thompson S, Ca-
Our results apply to a clinic-based patient popula- rusi DA, Gornbein J. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory. Com-
prehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia.
tion, age 44 to 88 years, with predominantly degen- Neurology 1994;44:2308 –2314.
erative dementia. The applicability of the ACE in the 20. Wechsler D. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised.
community requires investigation. The inter-rater New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1981.
21. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised manual. San
and intrarater reliability of the ACE could not be Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1987.
evaluated in this retrospective analysis, but as the 22. McKenna P, Warrington EK. Graded Naming Test. Windsor,
ACE assesses cognitive functions in an objective UK: NFER-Nelson, 1983.
manner the rater related bias is likely to be low. 23. Osterrieth PA. [A test of copying a complex figure: a contribu-
tion to the study of perception and memory.] Archives de
The index for differentiating AD from FTD Psychologie (Geneva) 1944;30:205–220.
(VLOM ratio) is derived using a sample population 24. Miller E. Verbal fluency as a function of a measure of verbal
that is relatively large (n ! 100). Nevertheless, these intelligence and in relation to different types of pathology.
Br J Clin Psychol 1984;23:53–57.
findings clearly need to be replicated in an indepen- 25. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A
dent and prospectively studied sample. new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry
1982;140:566 –572.
26. Galton CJ, Patterson K, Xuereb JH, Hodges JR. Atypical and
References typical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical, neuro-
1. McKeith IG, Fairbairn AF, Bothwell RA, et al. An evaluation psychological, neuroimaging and pathological study of 13
of the predictive validity and inter-rater reliability of clinical cases. Brain 2000;123:484 – 498.
December (1 of 2) 2000 NEUROLOGY 55 1619
Downloaded from www.neurology.org by Kathy Pieper on October 26, 2011
27. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical ease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Arch Neurol
manual of mental disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV). Wash- 1991;48:278 –281.
ington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994;143–147. 39. Elfgren CI, Ryding E, Passant U. Performance on neuropsy-
28. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, chological tests related to single photon emission computer-
Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report ised tomography findings in frontotemporal dementia. Br J
of the NINCDS–ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Psychiatry 1996;169:416 – 422.
Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on 40. Martin A, Wiggs CL, Lalonde F, Mack C. Word retrieval to
Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939 –944. letter and semantic cues: a double dissociation in normal sub-
29. Roman GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Vascular de- jects using interference tasks. Neuropsychologia 1994;32:
mentia: diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the 1487–1494.
NINDS–AIREN International Workshop [see comments]. 41. Hodges JR, Patterson K. Is semantic memory consistently
Neurology 1993;43:250 –260. impaired early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease? Neuro-
30. Rahman S, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Rogers RD, Robbins TW. anatomical and diagnostic implications. Neuropsychologia
Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotempo- 1995;33:441– 459.
ral dementia. Brain 1999;122:1469 –1493. 42. Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, Salmon DP, Katzman R,
31. Holroyd S, Duryee JJ. Characteristics of persons utilizing a Thal LJ. Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection
geriatric psychiatry outpatient clinic. J Geriatr Psychiatry of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 1992;49:1253–
Neurol 1997;10:136 –141. 1258.
32. Hogan DB, Thierer DE, Ebly EM, Parhad IM. Progression and
43. Lambon Ralph MA, Graham KS, Ellis AW, Hodges JR. Nam-
outcome of patients in a Canadian dementia clinic. Can
ing in semantic dementia–what matters? Neuropsychologia
J Neurol Sci 1994;21:331–338.
1998; 36:775–784.
33. Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, et al. Prevalence and
44. Griffiths KM, Cook ML, Newcombe RL. Cube copying after
severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia
in an elderly population. Lancet 1997;349:1793–1796. cerebral damage. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1988;10:800 – 812.
34. Boersma F, Eefsting JA, van den Brink W, Koeter M, van 45. Brodaty H, Moore CM. The Clock Drawing Test for dementia
Tilburg W. Prevalence of dementia in a rural Netherlands of the Alzheimer’s type: a comparison of three scoring meth-
population and the influence of DSM-III-R and CAMDEX cri- ods in a memory disorders clinic. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
teria for the prevalence of mild and more severe forms. J Clin 1997;12:619 – 627.
Epidemiol 1998;51:189 –197. 46. Morris JC. Clinical dementia rating: a reliable and valid diag-
35. Sox HC, Blatt MA, Higgins MC, Marton KI, eds. Medical deci- nostic and staging measure for dementia of the Alzheimer
sion making. Stoneham, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann. 1988. type. Int Psychogeriatr 1997;9:173–178.
36. McDowell I, Newel C. The theoritical and technical founda- 47. Varma AR, Snowden JS, Lloyd JJ, Talbot PR, Mann DM,
tions of health measurement. In: McDowell I, Newel C, eds. Neary D. Evaluation of the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria in the
Measuring health: a guide to rating scales and questionnaires. differentiation of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal de-
New York: Oxford University Press, 1996:10 – 46. mentia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;66:184 –188.
37. Herlitz A, Small BJ, Fratiglioni L, Almkvist O, Viitanen M, 48. Miller BL, Ikonte C, Ponton M, et al. A study of the Lund-
Backman L. Detection of mild dementia in community sur- Manchester research criteria for frontotemporal dementia:
veys. Is it possible to increase the accuracy of our diagnostic clinical and single-photon emission CT correlations. Neurol-
instruments? Arch Neurol 1997;54:319 –324. ogy 1997;48:937–942.
38. Welsh K, Butters N, Hughes J, Mohs R, Heyman A. Detection 49. Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzhei-
of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s dis- mer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 1984;141:1356 –1364.
Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://www.neurology.org/content/55/11/1613.1.full.html
Neurology ® is the official journal of the American Academy of Neurology. Published continuously since
1951, it is now a weekly with 48 issues per year. Copyright © 2013 American Academy of Neurology. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0028-3878. Online ISSN: 1526-632X.