Pragmatics Checklist
Pragmatics Checklist
(Gestures - Preverbal)
Uses NO Words
Uses Complex
Not Present
Child’s Name Date .Completed by .
Language
Parent: These social communication skills develop over time. Read the behaviors below and place an X
in the appropriate column that describes how your child uses words/language, no words (gestures –
preverbal) or does not yet show a behavior.
Pragmatic Objective
INSTRUMENTAL – States needs (I want….)
1. Makes polite requests
2. Makes choices
3. Gives description of an object wanted
4. Expresses a specific personal need
5. Requests help
REGULATORY - Gives commands (Do as I tell you…)
6. Gives directions to play a game
7. Gives directions to make something
8. Changes the style of commands or requests depending on who the child is speaking to and what the
child wants.
PERSONAL – Expresses feelings
9. Identifies feelings (I’m happy.)
10. Explains feelings (I’m happy because it’s my birthday
11. Provides excuses or reasons
12. Offers an opinion with support
13. Complains
14. Blames others
15. Provides pertinent information on request (2 or 3 of the following: name, address, phone, birthdate)
INTERACTIONAL - Me and You…
16. Interact with others in a polite manner
17. Uses appropriate social rules such as greetings, farewells, thank you, getting attention
18. Attends to the speaker
19. Revises/repairs an incomplete message
20. Initiates a topic of conversation (doesn’t just start talking in the middle of a topic)
21. Maintains a conversation (able to keep it going)
22. Ends a conversation (doesn’t just walk away)
23. Interjects appropriately into an already established conversation with others
24. Makes apologies or gives explanations of behavior
25. Requests clarification
26. States a problem
27. Criticizes others
28. Disagrees with others
29. Compliments others
10. Makes promises
WANTS EXPLANATIONS - Tell me Why…
31. Asks questions to get more information
32. Asks questions to systematically gather information as in “Twenty Questions”
33. Asks questions because of curiosity
34. Asks questions to problem solve (What should I do? How do I know?)
35. Asks questions to make predictions (What will happen if…?)
SHARES KNOWLEDGE & IMAGINATIONS - I’ve got something to tell you…
36. Role play as/with different characters
37. Role plays with props (i.e., banana as phone)
38. Provides a description of a situation which describes the main events
39. Correctly re-tells a story which has been told to them
40. Relates the content of a 4-6 frame picture story using correct events for each frame
41. Creates an original story with a beginning, several logical events, and an end
42. Explains the relationship between two objects, actions or situations
43. Compares and contrasts qualities of two objects, actions or situations
44. Tells a lie
45. Expresses humor/sarcasm
TOTAL FOR EACH COLUMN
AUTHOR OF CHECKLIST: Goberis, D. (1999) Pragmatics Checklist (adapted from Simon, C.S., 1984).
Goberis, Beams, Dalpes, Abrisch, Baca, Yoshinaga-Itano (2012). The missing link in language development of deaf and hard of hearing children: Pragmatic Language Development. Semin
Speech Lang, 33(04), 297-309 https://www.thieme-connect.de/ejournals/pdf/10.1055/s-0032-1326916.pdf
The format of this information was designed by Karen L. Anderson, PhD, 2013, Supporting Success for Children with Hearing Loss https://successforkidswithhearingloss.com
PRAGMATICS CHECKLIST INTERPRETATION
Total the checked items in the Complex Language column. Compare to expectations below.
Emergence of
Emergence of
Age 3: 20 of 45 items; Age 4: 43 of 45 items; Age 5: 44 of 45 items; Age 6: 45 items
In months
In months
in months
Language
Using 1-3
Complex
Mastery
Words
Age of
Consider the items marked in columns other than Complex Language and compare them to
the typical performance at the ages identified. Children with hearing loss tend to be
delayed in their mastery of pragmatic language skills that typically hearing children mostly
master by age 4. Select goals for the items students demonstrate delayed performance.
24-30 months 36-42 months 42-48 months 48-54 months 54-60 months
INSTRUMENTAL – States needs (I want….)
1. Makes polite requests 36-42 (90%) 24-30 (79%) 24-30 (21%)
2. Makes choices 36-42 (85%) 24-30 (58%) 24-30 (35%)
3. Gives description of an object wanted 36-42 (83%) 24-30 (54%) 24-30 (21%)
4. Expresses a specific personal need 36-42 (89%) 24-30 (58%) 24-30 (29%)
5. Requests help 36-42 (84%) 24-30 (58%) 24-30 (29%)
REGULATORY - Gives commands (Do as I tell you…)
6. Gives directions to play a game 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (22%) 24-30 (2%)
7. Gives directions to make something 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (35%) 24-30 (6%)
8. Changes the style of commands or requests depending on who the child is speaking to 36-42 (84%) 24-30 (58%) 24-30 (7%)
and what the child wants.
PERSONAL – Expresses feelings
9. Identifies feelings (I’m happy.) 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (57%) 24-30 (6%)
10. Explains feelings (I’m happy because it’s my birthday 36-42 (80%) Emerging 30-36
11. Provides excuses or reasons 36-42 (80%) Emerging 30-36
12. Offers an opinion with support 36-42 (74%) Emerging 30-36
13. Complains 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (44%) 24-30 (15%)
14. Blames others 36-42 (75%) Emerging 30-36
15. Provides pertinent information on request (2 or 3 of the following: name, address, phone, birthdate) 36-42 (75%) 36-42 (42%) 36-42 (30%)
INTERACTIONAL - Me and You…
16. Interact with others in a polite manner 36-42 (89%) 24-30 (65%) 24-30 (54%)
17. Uses appropriate social rules such as greetings, farewells, thank you, getting attention 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (72%) 24-30 (54%)
18. Revises/repairs an incomplete message 48-54 (80%) 24-30 (29%) 24-30 (54%)
19. Attends to the speaker 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (50%) 24-30 (54%)
20. Initiates a topic of conversation (doesn’t just start talking in the middle of a topic) 42-48 (85%) 24-30 (36%) 24-30 (54%)
21. Maintains a conversation (able to keep it going) 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (36%) 24-30 (54%)
22. Ends a conversation (doesn’t just walk away) 48-54 (90%) 24-30 (28%) 24-30 (54%)
23. Interjects appropriately into an already established conversation with others 48-54 (85%) 24-30 (26%) 24-30 (54%)
24. Makes apologies or gives explanations of behavior 48-54 (86%) 24-30 (36%)
25. Requests clarification 48-54 (90%) Emerging 30-36
26. States a problem 36-42 (74%) 24-30 (58%) 24-30 (54%)
27. Criticizes others 48-54 (81%) 36-42 (25%) 24-30 (54%)
28. Disagrees with others 36-42 (74%) 24-30 (36%) 24-30 (54%)
29. Compliments others 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (28%) 24-30 (54%)
30. Makes promises 54-60 (82%) 36-42 (5%) 48-54 (71%)
WANTS EXPLANATIONS - Tell me Why…
31. Asks questions to get more information 36-42 (79%) 24-30 (29%) 24-30 (15%)
32. Asks questions to systematically gather information as in “Twenty Questions” 48-54 (77%) 36-42 (26%) 36-42 (37%)
33. Asks questions because of curiosity 36-42 (90%) 24-30 (21%) 24-30 (15%)
34. Asks questions to problem solve (What should I do? How do I know?) 48-54 (90%) 36-42 (21%) 36-42 (52%)
35. Asks questions to make predictions (What will happen if…?) 48-54 (78%) 36-42 (27%) 36-42 (47%)
SHARES KNOWLEDGE & IMAGINATIONS - I’ve got something to tell you…
36. Role play as/with different characters 36-42 (74%) 24-30 (36%) 24-30 (14%)
37. Role plays with props (i.e., banana as phone) 36-42 (90%) 24-30 (50%) 24-30 (21%)
38. Provides a description of a situation which describes the main events 48-54 (95%) 24-30 (21%) 36-42 (69%)
39. Relates the content of a 4-6 frame picture story using correct events for each frame 48-54 (90%) 24-30 (28%) 36-42 (72%)
40. Creates an original story with a beginning, several logical events, and an end 48-54 (81%) 36-42 (27%) 36-42 (59%)
41. Explains the relationship between two objects, actions or situations 48-54 (80%) 24-30 (42%) 36-42 (69%)
42. Compares and contrasts qualities of two objects, actions or situations 48-54 (100%) 24-30 (20%) 36-42 (64%)
43. Correctly re-tells a story which has been told to them 48-54 (81%) 24-30 (21%) 36-42 (68%)
44. Tells a lie 48-54 (86%) Emerging 30-36 36-42 (52%)
45. Expresses humor/sarcasm 48-54 (91%) 24-30 (43%) 36-42 (58%)
AUTHOR OF CHECKLIST: Goberis, D. (1999) Pragmatics Checklist (adapted from Simon, C.S., 1984). Percentages specified above are estimates only from graphs of research results.
SOURCE OF DATA FOR TYPICALLY DEVELOPING CHILDREN: Goberis, Beams, Dalpes, Abrisch, Baca, Yoshinaga-Itano (2012). The missing link in language development of deaf and hard of
hearing children: Pragmatic Language Development. Semin Speech Lang, 33(04), 297-309 https://www.thieme-connect.de/ejournals/pdf/10.1055/s-0032-1326916.pdf
The format of this information was designed by Karen L. Anderson, PhD, 2013, Supporting Success for Children with Hearing Loss https://successforkidswithhearingloss.com
The Missing Link in Language Development of Deaf and Hard of
Hearing Children: Pragmatic Language Development
Dianne Goberis, Dinah Beams, Molly Dalpes, Amanda Abrisch, Rosalinda Baca, Christine Yoshinaga-Itano (2012). The missing link in language development of deaf and hard of hearing
children: Pragmatic Language Development. Semin Speech Lang, 33(04), 297-309 https://www.thieme-connect.de/ejournals/pdf/10.1055/s-0032-1326916.pdf
◦ Parents completed 45-item Pragmatics Checklist ◦ Children with hearing loss were between 3-7 years
◦ 109 children with normal hearing age 2-7 years ◦ Normal hearing: 43% male; 56% female Hearing loss:
◦ 126 children with hearing loss of all degrees (19.8% 51% male; 49% female
mild; 24.2% moderate/moderate-severe; 32.9% severe; 23.1% ◦ All children cognitively normal, English-speaking
profound)
Percentage of Items Mastered by Age for Normal Hearing and Hearing Loss Groups
Items Not Mastered by Children with Hearing
Loss by Age 7 Years
Provides information on request
Repairs incomplete sentences
Ends conversations
Interjects
Apologies
Request clarification
Makes promises
Ask questions to problem solve
Asks questions to make predictions
Retells a story
Tells 4-6 picture story in right order
Creates original story
Proportion Achieving 50% or more of the items with complex language Explains relationships between objects-
action-situations
Compares and contrasts
Summary
Pragmatic language skills are the most abstract and
complex of all language skills. Even when the child
has age-appropriate vocabulary and syntax skills,
she or he may not yet have learned how to use
these skills in a socially appropriate manner for
specific social purposes. Young children with NH
acquire these skills rapidly between 3 and 4 years of
age and are able to use these pragmatic language
skills using complex language. Children who are
DHH acquire these skills much more slowly even
with targeted intervention strategies. Without
Intervention Strategies mastery of these skills, children will encounter
Parents often wonder what language skills they can work on in the home. This significant challenges with literacy, written
questionnaire helps parents target specific language skills to model within the communication, and abstract conversational
home providing natural opportunities for the child to practice them. If the communication.
child is in an integrated environment, the teacher questionnaire helps identify some of the strengths and weaknesses of the child's
language abilities and the teacher may then optimize language learning in classroom situations where these strategies could be
naturally practiced. Teachers frequently judge language skills based upon the speech intelligibility of the child. However, pragmatic
language skills are often so subtle that the teacher does not identify specific areas of delay or difficulty. In addition to the analysis of
situations in which language becomes difficult, it is also possible to determine whether the student has sufficient vocabulary and
grammatical capabilities to communicate his or her needs and wants. If the student lacks specific content knowledge in vocabulary
or syntax, these skills can be taught through individualized intervention.
#1
Giving Directions
Most early intervention specialists and speech-language pathologists (SLPs) work on simple commands such as understanding: go
get your shoes, put your shoes on, open the door, get in the car. However, teachers and SLPs sometimes overlook having the child
give directions to others. There are some pragmatic language skills that are important for giving directions, such as to play a game or
to make something. To teach these skills, the SLP/teacher needs to carefully analyze all of the cognitive linguistic steps that must be
understood to provide directions to another about playing a game or making something. The child needs to understand what is in
the mind of the other individual who must learn a game or learn how to make something (i.e., theory of mind). Can the child explain
the steps well enough so that another individual can successfully make something: a picture, a snowman, a peanut butter and jelly
sandwich, or an arts and crafts activity?
#
Making Something
Children also need to learn the sequence of information that they need to tell someone else about how to make something. Even
when the child understands the sequence and is capable of making the desired food item, arts and crafts project, or object, the child
may be unable to explain to another how to do it. Children do not always understand what basic information is important for
another person to know. Remember to teach sequence. For a peanut butter and jelly sandwich, if the child says “peanut butter” and
omits specific instructions in the sequence, the teacher/SLP can respond with absurdities such as, “Do I put it in my hand? Where's
the bread?” The teacher/SLP needs to identify each of the steps in the sequence. It is often helpful to have pictures that are
associated with each step in the sequence. Using a chart with pictures that depict each step in the process, children will cross out
each step as they either do it, indicating comprehension, or as they tell another to do it, demonstrating the pragmatic expressive
language skill. Then the teacher/SLP puts the items back in the right sequence to review. The order may also be jumbled to
demonstrate that the end result will not be correct. The teacher could teach the child the correct sequence with picture cues and
written language: (1) get a banana, chocolate, and nuts, (2) peel the banana before cutting, (3) cut the banana, (4) dip the banana
into the chocolate, (5) roll the chocolate-covered banana in nuts, (6) freeze the banana. Teaching requires redundancy and
repetition. Make sure to let the child make mistakes. If it isn't in order, can the task be completed? Children need to learn that
making things typically requires a specific order or sequence of events to accomplish the goal.
Playing 20 Questions
Although children with NH learn how to play the game 20 questions without having to learn the steps, children who are DHH often
need specific instructions. How do you select the question to be asked? Which type of question is better, “Is it a dog? Is it alive? Is it
an animal?” When the question is answered, what should the child do with the answer? Does the child eliminate any possibilities? It
is easiest to learn how to play the game if the options are limited? With pictures of the possible answers, after the question is
answered, it is possible to eliminate some of the pictures. Can the child explain why these answers would be eliminated? The child
should cross out the pictures of answers that cannot be the chosen one because of the answer given to the previous question. Then
the child needs to look at the remaining pictures and formulate a question that will either provide the information about which one
is the chosen answer or eliminate more of the potential answers. The SLP may need to teach the child what questions might yield
the best ability to eliminate answers. These questions are frequently category questions, such as “Is it alive?” or “Is it an animal?”
The child must learn how to keep information in his or her head to use for formulating the next question. The child must understand
how to categorize things. Categorization often begins with visual obvious characteristics, such as the color, “Is it white?” Or the size,
“Is it big?” Or, “Is it alive?” Or, “How do animals move? Do they fly? Swim? Walk?”
Recognizing a Falsehood
To survive in this society, it is important that children understand the difference between truth and a lie. Children need to learn
when they are being “tricked.” They need to understand what it means to be gullible, so that social victimization can be prevented.
Role-play can consist of pretending to be a trickster, such as “coyote” in the southwest of America. Should I believe the trickster?
Are there any clues provided about tricksters? What strategies can be used to determine whether something is true or something is
a lie? How does the child know when to believe someone? A child cannot understand truth without understanding a lie.
Persuasion
Why is it important for children to learn how to be persuasive? What are some real life examples when the child may find it
important to persuade someone? A child may want to know how to persuade his or her parents to allow them to do something or
get something; permission to do a particular activity, go to a party, go to get ice cream, etc. A child may also want to persuade other
children to let him or her play with them. What are some strategies that can be used? How can the child assess whether or not the
strategy is successful?
3
Revision of Unclear Message
First the child must be able to identify when the message was unclear to another person. If they can identify miscommunication,
then they can supply options. The teacher/SLP should model for the child. If the child says, “boat,” the SLP/teacher could question,
“Do I want the boat? Do I want to eat the boat? Do you want to get on the boat? Is it a blue boat?”
Acknowledgments
Data collection for children with NH and typical development and data analysis was made possible through a grant from Centers for
Disease Control, Association of University Centers on Disability, CDC-AUCD 433, 470. Data collection was supported by the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention (grant/cooperative agreement number UR3/CCU824219), National Institutes of Health (contract
number N01-DC-4-2141), Maternal and Child Health, the Colorado Department of Education (contract number H325D030031A,
H32C030074), the University of Colorado at Boulder, the Colorado Home Intervention Program, and the Colorado Department of
Public Health and Environment. We wish to acknowledge the contributions of the following individuals to this project: student
coders, CHIP facilitators and the participating families.
References
1 Yoshinaga-Itano C, Baca RL, Sedey AL. Describing the trajectory of language development in the presence of severe-to-profound
hearing loss: a closer look at children with cochlear implants versus hearing aids. Otol Neurotol 2010; 31: 1268-1274
2 Conti-Ramsden G, Botting N. Social difficulties and victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. J Speech Lang Hear Res
2004; 47: 145-161
3 Farmer M. Language and social cognition in children with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41: 627-
636
4 Ketelaars MP, Cuperus JM, van Daal J, Jansonius K, Verhoeven L. Screening for pragmatic language impairment: the potential of
the children's communication checklist. Res Dev Disabil 2009; 30: 952-960
5 Day PS. Deaf children's expression of communicative intentions. J Commun Disord 1986; 19: 367-385
6 Nicholas JG. Age differences in the use of informative/heuristic communicative functions in young children with and without
hearing loss who are learning spoken language. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2000; 43: 380-394
7 Nicholas JG, Geers AE. Communication of oral deaf and normally hearing children at 36 months of age. J Speech Lang Hear Res
1997; 40: 1314-1327
8 Most T, Shina-August E, Meilijson S. Pragmatics abilities of children with hearing loss using cochlear implants or hearing aids
compared to hearing children. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2010; 15: 423-437
9 Prutting CA, Kirchner DM. A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. J Speech Hear Disord 1987; 52: 105-119
10 Yoshinaga-Itano C. Assessment and intervention with preschool children who are deaf and hard-of-hearing. In: Alpiner J,
McCarthy P, , eds. Rehabilitative Audiology Children and Adults. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 1999: 140-177
11 Goberis D Pragmatics Checklist (adapted from work done by Simon CS, 1984):1984
12 Simon CS. Functional-pragmatic evaluation of communication skills in school-aged children. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 1984; 15:
83-97
13 Yoshinaga-Itano C, Gilkerson J. Paradigm shifting: automatic assessment of natural environments. Paper presented at: AG Bell
2010 Biennial Convention; June 25, 2010; Orlando, FL
14 Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Coulter DK, Mehl AL. Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics
1998; 102: 1161-1171
The format of this information was designed by Karen L. Anderson, PhD, 2013, Supporting Success for Children with Hearing Loss https://successforkidswithhearingloss.com