0% found this document useful (0 votes)
100 views27 pages

Ei08 Raymond 7116510 en

This document discusses student incentive programs in charter schools and analyzes their impact on academic achievement. It begins by providing context on reward programs in K-12 schools and introduces the concept of studying these programs in charter schools. The document then develops a taxonomy to classify different incentive programs based on the time between rewarded behavior and reward receipt, and the social distance between student and reward administrator. The rest of the document describes the survey and analysis conducted, which found that incentive programs had a consistent positive impact on reading achievement across grades but no clear impact on math achievement.

Uploaded by

ASADDirvi
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
100 views27 pages

Ei08 Raymond 7116510 en

This document discusses student incentive programs in charter schools and analyzes their impact on academic achievement. It begins by providing context on reward programs in K-12 schools and introduces the concept of studying these programs in charter schools. The document then develops a taxonomy to classify different incentive programs based on the time between rewarded behavior and reward receipt, and the social distance between student and reward administrator. The rest of the document describes the survey and analysis conducted, which found that incentive programs had a consistent positive impact on reading achievement across grades but no clear impact on math achievement.

Uploaded by

ASADDirvi
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 27

Paying for A’s: An Early Exploration

of Student Reward and Incentive Programs


in Charter Schools

Margaret Raymond
CESifo GmbH Phone: +49 (0) 89 9224-1410
Poschingerstr. 5 Fax: +49 (0) 89 9224-1409
81679 Munich E-mail: office@cesifo.de
Germany Web: www.cesifo.de
Paying for A’s:
An Early Exploration of
Student Reward and Incentive Programs in Charter Schools

Margaret Raymond
CREDO
Stanford University

April 2008
Introduction
Recent public attention has spotlighted the use of reward programs for K-12 public
school students. In districts such as Baltimore, Fulton County, Georgia, or New York
City, the efforts of educators to structure reward systems are being met with support,
skepticism and indignation. The programs use a variety of incentives such as cash or
mp3 players as incentives to shape individual student behavior including, among other
things, high school attendance, continuing enrollment or academic performance.
Districts and schools also use rewards such as social events or concerts to reward school
wide attainment of desirable outcomes, such as improved school-wide safety or
attendance. The natural laboratory of charter schools offers a unique chance to
investigate the performance implications of such programs.

Student incentive programs are an innovation in education that already has widespread
practice in charter schools across the country. Where charter schools are permitted, the
state authorizing legislation gives charter schools latitude to adopt new approaches to K-
12 education, and many have elected to incorporate incentive schemes into their
education programs. Until now, the outlines of those schemes and the degree to which
they improve student academic achievement have not been examined.

Growing interest in rewards or incentives for students occurs against the policy backdrop
of school accountability policies, which themselves employ incentives and sanctions
(negative rewards). Underperforming schools and districts face shortening timelines for
improvement. Reward systems for students are one among many innovations being tried
across the country to jumpstart an increase in student motivation and results.

The research aim of this study was to determine if the presence of such incentives and
rewards in charter schools significantly affects the academic outcomes of students. This
study examines a non-random sample of charter schools and their decisions to use or
forego an incentive program in their school to see if the systems enhance academic
achievement gains. The findings show that the program designs exist in all grade levels
but cover much of the same behavior and outcomes. They do vary in the intensity with
which students are scrutinized with longer periods of review in higher grade levels.

The analysis shows consistent impact of the programs across grades and designs in terms
of student achievement gains on state achievement tests in reading. At the same time,
there is no evident impact in math.

In the next section, I develop a taxonomy for incentive programs to classify the reward
programs used today in charter schools. The third section of this paper describes the
schemes as they operate today, based on self-reported survey results. In the fourth
section, the analytic methods used to study the impact of these systems are reviewed.
The results are presented in the fifth. A discussion of further directions for this work
concludes the paper.

A Taxonomy of Student Incentives

1
The programs studied in this analysis build on more traditional programs of incentives
such as college scholarships and “bridge” programs. For the typical types of scholarship
incentives there is a robust literature of their effectiveness focused on comparative
college attendance rates. 1 In these studies, scholarship incentives create larger demand
for higher education, but absent specific targeting do little to address the underlying
structural inequalities across the economic spectrum. 2 At the secondary level, policies
that reward students or their teachers for passing rigorous examinations such as
Advanced Placement exams or national exams in other countries create positive results,
though the results are shown to weaken when teachers are excluded or are not supportive
of the idea. 3 In both types of incentive programs, the payoff (i.e., receipt of a
scholarship) is realized well after the behavior that is targeted for reinforcement (i.e., high
school completion or specific levels of achievement), and often awarded by individuals
with no relationship to the recipients. On a stand-alone basis, then, these approaches are
weak in terms of their direct influence on behavior. They also assume students possess
long time horizons, appropriately value the reward, and calculate the inter-temporal
tradeoffs in some predictable way -- clearly assumptions that require investigation. 4

Both to create incentives for college attendance and to address some of the cultural and
socio-economic barriers previously referenced, a number of “college-bound” programs
modify the college scholarship concept in two ways. First, they reduce the payoff time
and increase the likelihood of reward by making investments in college savings accounts
based on academic results each grading period. Second, they incorporate out-of-school
social and education supports while students are still in high school both to shape
behavior in real time and to motivate them to pursue longer term education goals. In
some cases continued program enrollment is the incentive for full participation in
tutoring, mentoring, community service or career exploration activities. In other cases,
small rewards are tied to these non-academic aspects of the program. A few studies have
examined the effect of a promise or probability of future scholarships on secondary
behavior and achievement, but this work is plagued by small, localized samples and few
control observations. 5

The incentive and reward programs in this study represent further evolution of the
“college bound” design. These programs are operating in a wider range of grade levels
than the programs previously discussed. The behavior is more granular and short-term;
in addition to grades and achievement test performance, most incentive systems in this
study also cover basic learning behaviors such as sitting quietly, tracking the teacher,
completing class work and general decorum, which have been shown to positively
correlate with academic outcomes. 6 The lag between desired result and payoff is shorter,
creating strong reinforcement of targeted behavior. The outlines of operant conditioning
in token economies are apparent. One note of particular interest from prior work is the
tendency for a significant share of individuals not to respond to the incentive program at
all. In addition, closer proximity between the student and those who evaluate behavior
and dole out rewards provides a secondary source of reinforcement between students and
educator role models. 7

2
Incentive systems differ from each other in other ways. One way is in the interpersonal
proximity of subject and the individual administering the reward. The same reward can
have stronger or weaker effect depending on the degree of closeness in the interpersonal
dyad. Clearly, family members would be the strongest actors in shaping behavior, but
teachers and other adults that a child regularly encounters also can have a strong
influence on the sustainability of reinforcement. 8 As one move further away from the
center of an individual’s world, their implicit endorsement of the reward diminishes in
magnitude. Reward systems can materially affect their results if the are delivered by
individuals that are closely related to the subjects they aim to influence.

Little empirical attention has been paid to the impact of the more recent conceptions of
incentives and rewards. In two studies with hyperactive students, token economies
influenced behavior but did little to improve academic performance. Eric Bettinger has
analyzed a randomized experiment in Coshocton Ohio in which students were given cash
rewards if they performed well on state achievement tests; he found a small significant
increase in Math performance but not in reading. 9 The evidence is modestly positive
and contradicts the opposing view that rewards impair the intrinsic motivation of
students. 10

Figure 1 presents a spatial taxonomy that distinguishes the types of programs described
above along two dimensions. The first is the time lag between the behavior that is
identified as worthy of reward and the receipt of the reward itself. The longest lags occur
with college scholarships that have an uncertain payoff many years in the future. The
second dimension is the degree of social distance between student and the individual(s)
empowered to assess the student behavior and award points, credits or direct rewards. In
the case of college scholarships, the individuals deciding the worthiness of a student are
typically anonymous. The programs included in this study are located closer to the
origin than the traditional incentive programs models. They have the shortest time
between behavior and rewards, ranging from a few days to several months, and they are
directly familiar with the individuals making the decisions about their behavior. This
taxonomy provided a useful way to translate survey responses into an intensity measure
for the modeling described below.

Study Approach
Data. In October 2007, an electronic survey was sent to 250 charter schools in seventeen
states that had previously agreed to participate in a longitudinal study of overall charter
school effectiveness. The survey was sent to school principals asking for information
about the use of student incentives or rewards systems in their schools. If a principal
reported that the school did not use such a system, the survey ended and the remaining
questions were unanswered. If the response was affirmative, additional information was
gathered about the system, including the frequency of assessment, the frequency of
resetting the system, the areas of student life that were covered by the system including
individual behavior or academic performance, citizenship or school-wide behavior or
academic performance. Principals were asked to identify who in the school was
empowered to assess student behavior and in what areas. The nature of rewards was also
explored. Finally the principal was asked to rate separately the degree to which they, the

3
teachers and other staff viewed the reward system as fair and effective; these data were
used to create measures of cohesion in perceived effectiveness and fairness in each
school.

Completed surveys were received from 186 schools, a 74 percent response rate. The full
sample is used in the following section to describe the prevalence of student reward
systems and outline the dimensions of those that sample schools currently operate. The
unit of analysis in the descriptions is the school.

The survey information was used to create three different variables to represent the
incentive/reward systems. First, a simple binary variable represents the presence or
absence of such a system in each school. Second, separate binaries were constructed to
reflect if the system focused on academics (such as grades of state test performance) or
behavior (classroom behavior, citizenship or social interactions). Finally, a continuous
variable was developed to reflect the intensity of attention on the student in each
program. 1

To assess the effect of these programs, a subset of the responding schools was studied.
To be included in the subset, schools had to have been open for two or more years and
have students enrolled in 4th grade or higher in 2007. Demographic data and state
achievement test scale score data for the academic years 2004/05, 2005/06 and 2006/07
were compiled from the Core of Common Data and state education departments. Within
each school, achievement test scale scores for each grade and subject were standardized
using grade-level means and standard deviations for each test year that were obtained
from the state education agencies. Additional information such as the year the school was
opened, the specific location of the school, its membership in a larger network of schools
and the grades they served in each year of study were obtained from the schools.

A panel dataset was created whose observations consist of a cohort in a school for two of
the three academic years, i.e. 2004/05 to 2005/06 and 2005/06 to 2006/07. Standardized
cohort gain scores were created for each grade and schooland used as the dependent
variable in the impact estimation.

Analysis. The analysis consists of two parts. In the first, simple statistics are used to
describe the prevalence of rewards systems and the attributes of the ones that were
reported.

The impact analysis in the second part is conducted separately for reading and
mathematics performance. An initial set of basic models test the premise that use of a
reward system makes a significant contribution to student learning. Initial models use

1
The reported frequency of assessment for the programs was transformed into a proportion ranging from .01
(assessment occurs once each semester) to 1.0 (assessment occurs in each period of the school day or continuously),
with intervening values of 0.2 (once a week) 0.1 (once a fortnight).0 .05 (once a month) and so on. This factor was
then multiplied by the average rating of the system’s effectiveness for the school. This manipulation resulted in a
variable that ranges from 0 (no system) to 1.0 (a system with continuous assessment and total belief in its effectiveness)
with 46 intervening values.

4
OLS regression to estimate the impact of incentive/reward systems on student
achievement gains, while controlling for differences in student population, level of
poverty as reflected in Free and Reduced Price Lunch program eligibility, and special
populations such as English Language Learners or students identified as needing special
education. An additional independent variable is the baseline year performance; it is
included to reflect the diminishing effects of prior learning over time. Variations of the
OLS models included interactions of reward systems with grade span (elementary,
middle school or high school) and calendar year.

A second set of models test variations on the use of an incentive/reward system variable.
Models are developed to test the importance of strong belief in the effectiveness of the
system, as captured in the average rating of effectiveness across principals, teachers and
staff; values ranged from 0 (no confidence in system effectiveness) to 1.0 (complete
confidence). One model tests the combined effects of having a system and the average
ratings of effectiveness while a related model uses only the average ratings of
effectiveness. In the same vein, a dummy variable is added to the basic model to reflect
schools in which the adults have high cohesion in their assessments of the benefits of a
reward system, as measured by an average effectiveness rating above .8. The cohesion
variable is also tested as a replacement for the broader dummy of simple presence of a
reward system. Finally, the composite variable developed to capture the intensity of the
system is added to the basic model and subsequently tested as a substitute for the dummy
variable for presence of a reward system.

The third set of estimates focus on the role of being part of part of a charter network.
Specifically, some charter schools are stand alone schools, while others are parts of larger
organization such as Leadership Public Schools or Uncommon Schools. These networks
offer support and structure to their schools and this set of models investigates whether the
incentive systems are proxies for larger network effects. The estimation builds on
previous estimates by adding network membership. Thus, the effects of more refined
characterizations of the reward systems were tested in models that also included a factor
for charter network membership.

The final phase of the analysis extended the model beyond simple ordinary least squares
regression. The concern is omitted variables bias in OLS, arising if there is an
unobserved factor that is strongly associated with adoption and with performance.
The correction for this effect -- two-stage least squares using an instrumental variable –
requires an exogenous variable that can provide predictive power to the independent
variable of concern but is not correlated with the outcome. We need exogenous variation
in the use of incentive schemes. Selection of suitable instruments is always a challenge,
as it was here. Here, we consider variation in the educational attainment of the
communities surrounding the charter school. Specifically, if school leaders use
neighborhood characteristics such as low education levels to decide on whether to
introduce incentive programs – and these do not separately affect achievement – we can
instrument incentive systems with this measure. Using the location of the charter school,
the education profile for adults aged 18 – 64 residing in the zip code surrounding the
school was obtained from the US Census Bureau website. From these, the proportion of

5
the adult population with a high-school education or less was calculated. This approach
to the estimation was explored in the fourth set of models.

Incentive/Reward Systems
Since information on the design and function of incentive/reward systems is scarce, this
section presents a brief overview of the plans. Fifty-seven percent of the charter school
respondents (106 of 186) report using an incentive/reward system with their students.
Where a system is used, it applies to all the grades in a school. About 40 percent use a
scheme of points that accumulate over time that are then redeemed for rewards, while
about a third use negative incentives such as subtracting points from a weekly starting
total. The remaining schools use a combination or devise occasional short-term efforts
such as before formative assessments.

Among the school that use an incentive/reward system, wide variation emerges in which
school personnel participates in reviewing students, how often the students are assessed
and how frequently the tally for each student resets.

Of the 106 schools that use an incentives/reward system, 99 (or 93 percent) cover both
academic effort and behavior in their rubrics. Three schools focus only on behavior and
four schools only on academics. A more detailed breakdown of what areas of student life
these systems cover appears in the table below.

Percent of All Schools


Using Reward Systems
Academic Concerns (103 schools) That Cover this Area
Completion of school work 89
Attendance 70
Grades 42
School- or grade-wide academic
performance 33

Behavior Concerns (102 schools)


Classroom behavior 93
Student-adult conduct 86
Citizenship 80
Peer-to-peer conduct 77
Extra effort 58
School- or grade-wide behavior 36

Most often, schools that use a system focus on classroom behavior, followed by
completion of assignments (89 percent) and student-adult interactions, such as speaking
respectfully or accepting direction (86 percent).

Teachers are relied on most heavily to implement the reward systems in the schools that
use them as displayed in the table below. This makes sense considering the closer
proximity of the teacher to students during the day. But the proportions also illuminate

6
that the role of principals in these schemes skews more to the disciplinarian rather than
the academic leader: Larger shares of schools report the principal’s involvement in
behavioral areas than in academic ones.

Who Assesses Students?*

Number of
Academic Concerns Schools Principals Teachers Staff
Completion of school work 94 17.0% 97.9% 16.0%
Attendance 74 44.6% 79.7% 58.1%
Grades 45 55.6% 91.1% 24.4%
School- or grade-wide
academic performance 33 97.0% 93.9% 42.4%

Behavior Concerns
Classroom behavior 99 37.4% 98.0% 26.3%
Peer-to-peer conduct 82 68.3% 100.0% 59.8%
Student-adult conduct 91 71.4% 97.8% 65.9%
Citizenship 85 65.9% 96.5% 63.5%
Extra effort 62 54.8% 96.8% 48.4%
School- or grade-wide behavior 38 89.5% 97.4% 47.4%

* Values are the percentages of schools using Principals, Teachers or Staff to rate each area.

Even if systems focus on the same things, they can differ in their periodicity. System-
using schools reported how frequently the systems are designed to assess student
behavior. For schools that use different periods for different elements of their plan, the
most frequent interval was recorded 2 . The table below illustrates that slightly more than
half the schools are structured to monitor students throughout the school day, either for
the time a student is in classes or continuously from beginning to end of a school day.
The remaining schools use a less frequent approach, including some that only conduct
their reviews at the end of grading periods (every 5 weeks or quarterly), semesters or
academic years.

Frequency
of Review Number of Schools Percentage

Once/Semester 2 0.67%
Once/Quarter 2 0.67%
Once/5 weeks 2 0.67%
Monthly 61 20.40%

2
For example, a school may review grades at the end of a grading period for achievement but also monitor
students weekly for completion of assignments; in such cases, the frequency of review was coded as
weekly.

7
Biweekly 2 0.67%
Weekly 62 20.74%
During Classes 36 12.04%
Constantly 132 44.15%

The range of available rewards is slanted to those with an immediate benefit or perceived
value to students. Brightly colored pens or notebooks at the school store have both utility
and notability. Earning the right to wear the specially designated colored polo shirt is
less prevalent only because many schools are sensitive to family budget concerns or do
not have the resources to provide the shirts themselves. Resource constraints also limit
the use of cash or college fund investments as rewards in most of the schools.

Reward Number of Schools Percentage

Access to select activities 87 82.1%


Certificate of merit 67 63.2%
Purchase items at school
store 57 53.8%
Special uniform 45 42.5%
Cash 9 8.5%
College fund contribution 4 3.8%
Other rewards 14 13.2%

Charter schools have all been created relatively recently and are typically dynamic in
their early years in size and structure. Since charter schools use different approaches to
build their enrollment, they are difficult to classify using typical grade spans of
elementary, middle and high school ranges. For example, a school may ultimately aim to
serve grades K – 8 but start by enrolling grades 1 and 6 and fill in grades year by year.
Accordingly, the descriptive data that follows applies to grades, not schools.

The chart below shows the proportion of schools that serving each grade that make use of
an incentive reward system. The prevalence of reward systems rises in upper elementary
to include all the schools with 9th grade before tapering off in the senior high grades.

8
Percentage of Schools Serving Each Grade that Use a Reward
System

100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
Proportion of
50%
Grades Uses System
40%
Does Not Use System
30%
20%
10%
0%
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Grade

Despite higher prevalence of reward systems in higher grades, ratings by principals of the
effectiveness the system decline as the grade level increases. Principals were asked to
rate their sense of how effective the system is on a 1 (Low) to 10 (High) scale. When the
ratings are examined by grade show as in the table below, the systems are considered
more effective in early grades, and the rating declines over successive grades. The
increased rating in 10th grade is partially explained by the fact that one school serves high
school dropouts and features the reward system as a central element of their model; were
that school excluded, the mean would be 5.33.

Average
Principal
Rating of
System Standard
Effectiveness Deviation
Grade
2 8.55 1.34
3 8.72 1.27
4 8.71 1.49
5 8.58 1.41
6 8.52 1.4
7 8.17 1.6
8 7.98 1.61
9 6.5 2.12
10 6.55 1.99

9
Analysis of Impact

The basic models for both reading and mathematics regress one-year gains in
achievement on baseline performance, demographics, poverty status, language
proficiency, special education status and a binary variable to indicate whether a school
employs a reward/incentive system. A description of the variables used in the analysis is
presented in Appendix A. The basic model is then expanded with additional refinements
to measurement of reward system.

Reward System Impact on Reading Performance.

Basic Models The results for the basic Reading models are found in Table 1. The basic
model reflects the expected relationships of baseline scores, ethnicity and special
education status. In the basic model, the simple presence of an incentive system is
positive and significant (Model 1). The effect, when represented by a binary variable, is
sensitive to model specification, though the coefficient remains positive through all the
model variations. Its significance does not change with the addition of calendar year
(Model 2), which was added to consider the increased urgency of performance under
their charter and the No Child Left Behind accountability requirements. 3 As seen in
Model 2, the time variable is insignificant. The addition of interaction variables to test
whether there was a difference in effect by grade span (Model 3) revealed an unexpected
negative and significant coefficient for the elementary grades interaction. Taken on its
face, the coefficient would suggest that all else being equal, elementary schools that use
reward systems produce lower learning gains compared to those that do not use them.

Variations to Basic Model As discussed earlier, we wanted to explore the impact of


reward systems more deeply to see if they had common attributes that systematically
associated with greater learning results. Three variables were developed: a measure of
the perceived effectiveness of the system by the adults using it (Average Ratings of
Effectiveness), a binary that subsets those schools that use a system that also have strong
alignment among adults about the usefulness of a reward system (Cohesion), and the
intensity of intervention that the reward system creates through its structure and function
(Intensity). These system qualities were studied to see if more nuanced relationships
might better explain the effect of these reward systems on student academic gains. The
results of these estimations are presented in Table 2.

3
For all public schools, The No Child Left Behind Act imposes performance expectations by requiring
each school to demonstrate that all students attain Adequate Yearly Progress on state standardized
achievement tests or face increasingly onerous consequences. Under the law, after six years of failing to
meet the targeted performance levels, the legislation called for a school to be significantly reconstituted, or
taken over by the state. Charter schools face an additional layer of accountability with the agency that
grants their charter, and in most cases a shorter time frame to prove in. So time would be expected to have
more impact on charter schools than on traditional public schools.

10
One possibility is that the perceptions among those who administer the system might
make a difference in how well the systems operate. Lagging confidence in the approach,
for example, might lead to lesser impacts. As shown in Model 4, adding the variable
‘Average Rating of Effectiveness’ whose values range from 0 (has no system) to 1.0 (all
adults give the higest possible rating) does not significantly contribute to student gains
when added to the Basic Model. The effectiveness variable is used as a substitute for the
binary factor in the model in Model 5, and the coefficient of 0.15 is significant to roughly
the same degree as the binary.

A related refinement examined the impact in schools where the average rating of
effectiveness was an 0.8 or higher, indicating that there was strong alignment and support
for the system among principal, teachers and staff. Using this specification limits
consideration to those schools that create a unified front to students and hold the
expectation that the systems work. Added to the basic model, as appears in Model 6, the
binary for strong cohesion is not significant when added to the basic model, but when
substituted for the more widespread system use variable in Model 7, produces one of the
strongest effects, a coefficient of 0.156, an even greater magnitude than the coefficient on
the simple measure.

To cover the possibility that the success of these systems is a function of the “contact”
with students, a variable was created that combines two separate dimensions: how
frequently the system design rates student conduct and the average ratings of
effectiveness among school personnel– the intensity variable. The Intensity variable
with continuous values from 0 to 1.0 might more accurately portray the differences across
schools in cohesion or granularity of review of student behavior. The initial estimation of
this relationship included Intensity as an additional exogenous factor to explain student
academic gains; the results shown in Model 8 indicate that use of an incentive/reward
system loses its significance when an additional factor for intensity is incorporated into
the model.

However, the fact that the intensity variable is continuous serves two functions. First, it
provides a way of distinguishing among the schools using a reward system, which is
expected to translate to more precise estimations in the models than a simple binary.
Second, it consolidates two strongly correlated factors into a single metric in a way that
preserves the explanatory power of each. If the intensity variable has a value greater than
zero, it means that there is a system operating. If the value is high, it means that there is
both a shared view that the system is effective and that it is comprehensively applied. As
one respondent noted, “We’re all over the students all the time.” Middling values arise if
the school personnel are less convinced of the value of the approach or less unified in
their viewpoints which could led to inconsistency or when the review of student conduct
is sporadic, which could lead to inconstancy. So instead of including both frequency and
cohesion into the model with their correlation of r = .67, the consolidated variable offers
an alternative to the binary factor of “has/does not have a reward system”. When the
intensity variable substitutes for the binary use factor, as seen in Model 9, it produces the
largest coefficient of all the models, 0.18, which is statistically significant. Thus, a

11
change from no program one that is both continuous and strongly supported by school
staff produces a .18 standard deviation increase in reading gains.

Best Models The incremental modifications in the preceding phase of analysis helped
form an understanding of the attributes of reward systems that materially affect their
utility as a driver of student learning gains. The analysis then turned to the task of using
the available data to identify the conditions of maximum impact for reward systems. A
large number of models were explored in the process; the best resulting models appear in
Table 3.

Many of the schools that participated in this study are members of a charter school
network or Charter Management Organization (CMO). Unlike single charter schools,
network charter schools are thought to derive benefits from the larger organizational
structure, ranging from shared services such as human resources or information
technologies. Another possible advantage stems from the notion that networks typically
grow from successful original schools and therefore transfer a proven education model to
the new environment, lessening the chance that the school is unsuccessful. Some charter
networks include reward systems in their education model and some don’t, so it is
important to consider the effects of network participation separately from the effects of
reward systems.

When the school’s membership in a charter network is considered (Model 10), the reward
system variable loses its significance by a small margin but is similar in magnitude to the
simple cases. Importantly, the fact that a school is part of a network produces a shift of
0.13 standard deviations in student achievement gains.

The greatest impact for reward systems was found to occur in conjunction with school
participation in charter networks. Models 11 and 12 show that similar results are
obtained when reward systems are strongly supported by the adults who administer them
or when the system has a high degree of intensity. Since these models consider the
effects of network participation and reward system operations separately, they provide
the largest degree of insight into the value of reward systems for creating student gains in
reading. They show that the effects of charter network membership and use of a reward
system are cumulative.

Extensions to the Model The models presented above assume a straightforward


relationship between schools’ use of a reward system and the gains that result in the
students in those schools. However, there remains the concern that schools that adopt
incentive and reward systems do so in systematic ways that are not reflected in the model.
If schools that adopt a reward system have other characteristics not captured in the
included exogenous variables, then importance of reward systems may be biased.
Indeed, the surprising finding of a negative coefficient on the elementary grade – reward
system interaction suggests the possibility of omitted variables. To address this question,
a two-staged least squares model was developed. The results appear in Table 4.

12
Preliminary investigation of the instrumental variable shows that it is highly correlated
with the use of reward systems and insignificant in creating achievement gains. The first
column of Table 4 shows the simple regression of low education attainment on use of
reward systems.

In the IV model, the coefficient on the instrumental variable construction of the reward
system factor was not significant. The loss of significance suggests that the choice to
implement a reward system is influenced by the lack of strong support for educational
achievement in the communities where the schools locate, and that after controlling for
that effect, the systems do not add much to student learning. There are, however,
reasons to be concerned about these estimates. The samples are limited and the choice
of instrument raises questions about persistent omitted relationships between the use of
reward systems and determinants of achievement gains.

Reward System Impact on Mathematics Performance. The same array of models


developed for the assessing the impact of incentive/reward systems on reading was tested
on mathematics gains. The results appear in Appendix B. Unlike the models for Reading
which showed a consistent positive effect from the adoption of a reward system, the
performance of these systems on mathematics learning gains was found to be uniformly
poor. Regardless of how the presence of a reward system is represented, the effect was
insignificant. Moreover the rest of the variables in the models did little to explain the
observed results in Math learning either. Contrary to the results obtained by Bettinger,
this analysis does not support use of an incentives or reward system to foster gains in
student learning in Mathematics.

13
Discussion

This exploratory research examined the role that incentive/reward programs in K-12
charter schools have on the academic learning of students. It is novel in a number of
ways. It is one of the first times that charter schools as a group provide insight on the
efficacy of one of their innovations; the maturity of the sector has reached a point where
their potential as R&D organizations for public education can be realized. Further, since
similar programs are growing in both number and complexity around the country, the
lessons gleaned from this analysis provide immediately useful input for decisions
regarding their use. As well, the results offer current users of reward systems insight
into how to improve the impact of their designs.

The role for reward systems in accelerating mathematics learning is not informed by this
analysis. Contrary to other work that found positive effects of a reward system on
mathematics learning (but not reading), this research was unable to identify similar
effects. Differences in study design may account for the differences in results; Bettinger
used a random assignment design in one community, whereas this work includes a broad
cross-section of schools and is not randomized. Unfortunately, these results are
consistent with recent national findings that the parameters of sound instruction in
mathematics remain obscure. 11

Across a variety of model specifications, reward systems are found to have stable and
consistent positive impacts for student learning in reading. The effect holds across grades
and across network and non-network charter schools. Moreover, the success of
incentive/reward systems appears to hinge on a number of factors that are under the
control of the school.

Schools whose personnel are strongly aligned in the view that reward systems are
effective fare better than schools where the support is weak or where the adults are less
aligned. Where the adults align, several important conditions arise. The rules of the
reward system are more consistently applied, which in turn leads to a more uniform
signal of expectations for students both for behavior and for learning, which are well
documented antecedents to achievement. 12 At the same time, having strong confidence
in the effectiveness of a reward system is likely also to improve its impacts by reinforcing
the expectations of the adults who employ it with students.

Additionally, schools in which there is continuous or near-continuous assessment of


student conduct produce larger gains in reading than schools that have reward systems
but tally up less frequently. This finding is consistent with established wisdom about
continuity in reinforcement and reward: the more constantly behavior is reinforced, the
faster and more enduring the learning. So schools might benefit from a redesign of their
systems to provide their students more consistent and continuous feedback.

It is difficult to interpret the meaning of the instrumental variable model. One possibility
is that the concern for endogeneity is unfounded and that the simple regression models
are sufficient to explain the relationship between reward systems and student learning.

14
Another is that there is endogeneity but the limitations of either data or the choice of
instrument fail to reveal the underlying relationship. The strong correlation between the
instrumented reward system variable and student poverty hints at the complexity of the
underlying dynamics. Other approaches to the instrument may do a better job of teasing
out these effects. Adding another year of data and expanding the number of schools in
the sample will also provide a more solid empirical base on which to test these
relationships.

Recognizing that this analysis is preliminary, the findings are still promising. The use of
a reward system was examined in a variety of ways and was found to be a significant
predictor of student learning gains in reading. Further, the best model results reveal an
important insight: participation in a network produces an independent effect on student
learning that can be enhanced if the network also has a system that all the adults can
strongly get behind. Likewise, the bump in performance that comes from being part of a
network can be enhanced by increasing the intensity of the system that is in place.

Viewed in the larger context of school performance and improvement, this analysis
illuminates the value of incentive/reward systems when structured and used
appropriately. The average effect size on student learning gains associated with the
adoption of a reward system across all the systems studied was 0.11 standard deviations.
When systems operate under conditions of cohesive and strong support among school
personnel and have continuous feedback built into their design, the effect size increases
to 0.16. These compare favorably to other school attributes that are considered indicators
of better quality; these models revealed a consistent effect size for a charter school being
part of a larger network of .16 standard deviations. This comparison suggests that when
done right, reward systems can produce a jump in learning gains for significantly less
resources.

Areas of Further Work

As this is a first look at the structure and impact of incentive/reward systems, much work
remains to be done. Data refinements to examine more schools and to examine the
effectiveness of systems using student-level data are two obvious directions for future
research. As well, additional development of the instrumental variable models may shed
additional light on the conditions for which system adoption holds the highest promise.
One option is to consider whether market strength of charter schools in a community
affects the possibility that schools choose these programs as a competitive differentiator.
And finally, inclusion of new data on the longer term impacts of such systems on students
would further an understanding of the durability of the benefits these systems are found
to deliver.

15
Figure 1
Taxonomy of Academic Incentive Programs

State
Tuition College
Promise Scholarships
Programs
Social Distance Between
Student and Judge

Adopt-a-
School
College
College- Funds
Bound
Results- Programs
Based
Incentive
Systems

School
Token
Economies

Time Lag to Reward

16
Table 1
Incentive System Impact Analysis- Reading

Baseline Models

1 2 3
Reading
Dependent Variable: with Model Model with
Standardized Reading Gains Baseline with Time Grade Span

Base Year Performance -0.325 -0.323 -0.326


(0.055)** (0.056)** (0.052)**

Uses Reward System (Binary) 0.116 0.11 0.176


(0.054)* (0.053)* (0134)

Percent Free/Reduced Lunch 0.07 0.048 0.011


(0.138) (0.138) (0.131 )

Percent English Learners -0.516 -0.496 -0.410


(0.137)** (0.134)** (0.132)**

Percent Special Education -1.335 -1.34 -1.297


(0.374)** (0.380)** (0.378)**

Percent Black -0.285 -0.258 -0.23


(0.143)* (0.144) (0.135)

Percent Hispanic -0.055 -0.042 -0.066


(0.131) (0.130) (0.129)

Year -0.096
-0.057

Elementary School Grade* Uses System -0.378


(0.169)*

Middle School Grade * Uses System -0.024


(0.130)

Constant 0.244 192.935 0.253


(0.052)** (114.257) (0.051)**

Observations 119 119 119


R-squared 0.32 0.35 0.37
Robust standard errors in parentheses
* significant at 5%; ** significant at 1%

17
Table 2
Incentive System Impact Analysis- Reading

Model Variations

4 5 6 7 8 9
Dependent Variable: Effectiveness/Use Effectiveness Cohesion/Use Cohesion Intensity/Use Intensity
Standardized Reading Gains Model Only Model Model Only Model Model Only Model

Base Year Performance -0.340 -0.336 -0.354 -0.356 -0.329 -0.327


(0.053)** (0.055)** (0.055)** (0.056)** (0.054)** (0.054)**

Uses Reward System (Binary) -0.059 0.027 0.050


(0.231) (0.069) (0.085)

Average Effectiveness Rating 0.210 0.150


(0.027) (0.006)*

Cohesion 0.137 0.156


(0.070) (0.054)**

Intensity 0.130 0.180


(0.011) (0.007)*

Percent Free/Reduced Lunch 0.092 0.082 0.079 0.092 0.040 0.048


(0.136) (0.138) (0.136) (0.134) (0.134) (0.132)

Percent English Learners -0.536 -0.528 -0.558 -0.570 -0.440 -0.418


(0.136)** (0.136)** (0.129)** (0.129)** (0.141)** (0.137)**

Percent Special Education -1.439 -1.412 -1.526 -1.531 -1.372 -1.350


(0.374)** (0.376)** (0.370)** (0.372)** (0.389)** (0.396)**

18
Percent Black -0.310 -0.301 -0.297 -0.303 -0.247 -0.241
(0.142)* (0.143)* (0.139)* (0.140)* (0.139) (0.138)

Percent Hispanic -0.065 -0.062 -0.040 -0.037 -0.057 -0.056


(0.130) (0.131) (0.132) (0.131) (0.131) (0.131)

Constant 0.251 0.248 0.253 0.255 0.239 0.238


(0.053)** (0.053)** (0.052)** (0.053)** (0.053)** (0.053)**

Observations 119.000 119.000 119.000 119.000 119.000 119.000


R-squared 0.330 0.330 0.350 0.350 0.330 0.330
Robust standard errors in
parentheses
* significant at 5%; ** significant at
1%

19
Table 3
Incentive System Impact Analysis - Reading

Best Model Solutions

10 11 12
Dependent Variable: Network Best Best
Standardized Reading Gains Model Model 1 Model 2

Base Year Performance -0.342 -0.383 -0.348


(0.055)** (0.054)** (0.054)**

Uses Reward System (Binary) 0.095


(0.053)

Member of charter network 0.133 0.171 0.147


(0.067)* (0.068)* (0.069)*

Average Effectiveness Rating

Cohesion 0.163
(0.052)**

Intensity 0.016
(0.007)*

Percent Free/Reduced Lunch -0.064 -0.106 -0.111


(0.152) (0.142) (0.146)

Percent English Learners -0.527 -0.574 -0.432


(0.135)** (0.123)** (0.134)**

Percent Special Education -1.263 -1.497 -1.292


(0.358)** (0.352)** (0.368)**

Percent Black -0.306 -0.318 -0.261


(0.145)* (0.140)* (0.140)

Percent Hispanic -0.061 -0.047 -0.064


(0.130) (0.127) (0.130)

Constant 0.263 0.278 0.258


(0.052)** (0.053)** (0.052)**

Observations 119 119 119


R-squared 0.35 0.39 0.36
Robust standard errors in parentheses
* significant at 5%; ** significant at 1%

20
Table 4
Incentive System Impact Analysis - Reading

Instrumental Variable Model

Logistic
Prediction IV Model

Low Education 1.047


(.008)**

Base Year Performance -0.319


(0.055)**

Linear Predictor – Uses Reward System 0.069


(0.173)

Percent Free/Reduced Lunch 0.11


(0.197)

Percent English Learners -0.534


(0.177)**

Percent Special Education -1.261


(0.486)*

Percent Black -0.303


(0.141)*

Percent Hispanic -0.051


(0.137)

Constant (1.892) 0.247


(5.71)** (0.062)**

Observations 577 119


R-squared 0.32
Absolute value of z statistics in
parentheses
* significant at 5%; ** significant at 1%

21
APPENDIX A
Description of Variables

Min Max
Variable Description Value Value
Change in standardized scale score on
Reading Gain state achievement tests Y2 – Y1 -.564 1.097

Base Year Standardized score on state achievement test in Y1


Score -1.191 .980

Use Reward Binary reflecting adoption of an incentive/reward


System system in school 0 1.0

Linear Logistical prediction of likelihood of having reward


Predictor system in school (based on Low Education IV) 0 1.0

Low Percent of adults 18 – 64 in school’s zip code with


Education HS education or less 0 100

Year Calendar year in Y2 2004 2007

Binary reflecting school’s membership in charter


Network school network or Charter Management Organization 0 1.0

Elementary Binary representing cohort enrolled in elementary


Grade school grade in Y2 0 1.0

Middle School Binary representing cohort enrolled in middle school


Grade grade in Y2 1 1.0

Average Average of ratings of Principal, teachers and staff


Rating of perceptions of how effective the incentive/reward 0 1.0
System system is in their school
Effectiveness

Binary reflecting schools with a reward system and 0 1.0


Cohesion an average rating of effectiveness >.8

Index variable equal to the product of the average


Intensity rating of system effectiveness and the a measure of 0 1.0
how frequently the system design calls for review of
students (once a semester to continuously)
APPENDIX B
References
1
Ehrenberg, Ronald G., Liang Zhang and Jared M. Levin. “Crafting a Class: The Trade-
off Between Merit Scholarships and Enrolling Lower-Income Students.”, NBER
Working Paper 11437, June 2005.
2
Dynarski, Susan. "Does Aid Matter? Measuring the Effect of Student Aid on College
Attendance and Completion." American Economic Review 93.1 (March 2003): 279-288.

“From 6 New Benefactors, 425 College Dreams”, New York Times, June 21, 1986.
4
Frederick, Shane, George Lowenstein and Ted O’Donoghue. “Time Discounting and
Time Preference: A Critical Review.” Journal of Economic Literature, Vol. XL,(June,
2002), pp. 351-401. Bishop, John. Drinking from the Fountain of Knowledge: Student
Incentive to Study and Learn -- Externalities, Information Problems and Peer Pressure in
Eric A. Hanushek and Finis Welch (eds.) Handbook of the Economics of Education,
Volume 2, 2006, Elsevier B.V. pp. 909 - 944.

5
Agresta, Kate, Jeff Buenrostro, Jenna Lawrence, Abigail Rubin. “ON TRACK: Factors
Influencing Student Retention in the College Track Program”, unpublished, January
2007.

Pryor, Donald and Rebecca Sumner. “The Hillside Work-Scholarship Connection:


Charting a Course for the Future”, Rochester, NY: The Center for Governmental
Research, 2004.
6
Harris Cooper. Homework. White Plains, NY: Longman Publishers. 1989.
7
Power, Thomas J, James L. Karaustis, and Dina F. Habboushe. Homework Success for
Children with ADHD: A Family-School Intervention Program. New York: The Guilford
Press, 2001.

Reitman, David, et al. "The Influence of a Token Economy and Methylphenidate on


Attentive and Disruptive Behavior During Sports With ADHD-Diagnosed Children."
Behavior Modification 25.2 (2001): 305-323.

Students in Cash-Incentives Study Score Higher in Math, Education Week, February 27,
2008
7
Angrist, J., and V. Lavy, (2004) “The Effect of High Stakes High School Achievement
Awards: Evidence from a School-Centered Randomized Trial”, IZA Discussion Papers
Number 1146.

Jackson, C. Kirabo “A Little Now for a Lot Later: A Look at a Texas Advanced
Placement Incentive Program”, Dec 7, 2007
Kremer, Michael , Edward Miguel, Rebecca Thornton. “Incentives to Learn”, NBER
Working Paper No. 10971, December 2004.
8
Lubbers, Miranda J. Group composition and network structure in school classes: a
multilevel application of the p* model. Social Networks, Volume 25, Issue 4, October
2003, Pages 309-332.

9 Bettinger, Eric. Paying to Learn: The Effect of Financial Incentives on Elementary


School Test Scores. Paper Presented at CESifo Conference “Economic Incentives: Do
They Work in Education? Insights and Findings from Behavioral Research,” Munich, 16-
17 May 2008.
10
Corpus, J., Lepper, M., & Iyengar, S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational
orientations in the classroom: Age differences
and academic correlates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(2), pp. 184-195.

Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A. L., Humenick, N. M., Perencevich, KC., Taboada, A., &
Barbosa, P. (2006). Influences of stimulating
tasks on reading motivation and comprehension. The Journal of Educational Research,
99(4), 232-245.

Jalenko, Mary Renck. Beyond Benchmarks and Scores: Reasserting the Role of
Motivation and Interest in Children’s Academic Achievement. Childhood Education
International Issue, 2007. pp. 395-407.

11
National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Foundations for Success: The Final Report of
the National Mathematics Advisory Panel, U.S. Department of Education: Washington,
DC, 2008. page 35.
12
Bishop, John. Drinking from the Fountain of Knowledge: Student Incentive to Study
and Learn -- Externalities, Information Problems and Peer Pressure in Eric A. Hanushek
and Finis Welch (eds.) Handbook of the Economics of Education, Volume 2, 2006,
Elsevier B.V. pp. 912.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy