Educational Psychology - Module 3 PDF
Educational Psychology - Module 3 PDF
Educational Psychology - Module 3 PDF
Five Personality 2
Six Motivation 20
1
Personality
Defining Personality
1. Psychodynamic Approach
The term psychoanalysis is used to refer to many aspects of Freud‟s work and
research, including Freudian therapy and the research methodology he used
to develop his theories. Freud relied heavily upon his observations and case
studies of his patients when he formed his theory of personality development.
According to Freud, the mind can be divided into two main parts:
1. The conscious mind includes everything that we are aware of. This is
the aspect of our mental processing that we can think and talk about
rationally. A part of this includes our memory, which is not always part
of consciousness but can be retrieved easily at any time and brought into
our awareness. Freud called this ordinary memory the preconscious.
2. The unconscious mind is a reservoir of feelings, thoughts, urges, and
memories that outside of our conscious awareness. Most of the contents of
the unconscious are unacceptable or unpleasant, such as feelings of pain,
anxiety, or conflict. According to Freud, the unconscious continues to
influence our behavior and experience, even though we are unaware of
these underlying influences.
2
personality--known as the id, the ego and the superego--work together to
create complex human behaviors.
The Id
The id is the only component of personality that is present from birth. This
aspect of personality is entirely unconscious and includes of the instinctive
and primitive behaviors. According to Freud, the id is the source of all psychic
energy, making it the primary component of personality.
The Ego
The ego is the component of personality that is responsible for dealing with
reality. According to Freud, the ego develops from the id and ensures that the
impulses of the id can be expressed in a manner acceptable in the real world.
The ego functions in both the conscious, preconscious, and unconscious mind.
The ego operates based on the reality principle, which strives to satisfy the
id's desires in realistic and socially appropriate ways. The reality principle
weighs the costs and benefits of an action before deciding to act upon or
abandon impulses. In many cases, the id's impulses can be satisfied through
a process of delayed gratification--the ego will eventually allow the behavior,
but only in the appropriate time and place.
The ego also discharges tension created by unmet impulses through the
secondary process, in which the ego tries to find an object in the real world
that matches the mental image created by the id's primary process.
3
The Superego
1. The ego ideal includes the rules and standards for good behaviors.
These behaviors include those which are approved of by parental and
other authority figures. Obeying these rules leads to feelings of pride,
value, and accomplishment.
2. The conscience includes information about things that are viewed as
bad by parents and society. These behaviors are often forbidden and lead
to bad consequences, punishments, or feelings of guilt and remorse.
The superego acts to perfect and civilize our behavior. It works to suppress all
unacceptable urges of the id and struggles to make the ego act upon idealistic
standards rather that upon realistic principles. The superego is present in
the conscious, preconscious, and unconscious.
With so many competing forces, it is easy to see how conflict might arise
between the id, ego, and superego. Freud used the term ego strength to refer
to the ego's ability to function despite these dueling forces. A person with
good ego strength is able to effectively manage these pressures, while those
with too much or too little ego strength can become too unyielding or too
disrupting.
Freud‟s theory of drives evolved throughout his life and work. He initially
described a class of drives known as the life instincts and believed that these
drives were responsible for much of behavior. Eventually, he came to believe
that these life instincts alone could not explain all human behavior. Freud
determined that all instincts fall into one of two major classes: the life
instincts or the death instincts.
4
Life Instincts (Eros)
Sometimes referred to as sexual instincts, the life instincts are those that
deal with basic survival, pleasure, and reproduction. These instincts are
important for sustaining the life of the individual as well as the continuation
of the species. While they are often called sexual instincts, these drives also
include such things as thirst, hunger, and pain avoidance. The energy created
by the life instincts is known as libido.
Initially described in his book Beyond the Pleasure Principle, Freud proposed
that “the goal of all life is death” (1920). He noted that after people
experience a traumatic event (such as war), they often reenact the
experience. He concluded that people hold an unconscious desire to die, but
that this wish is largely tempered by the life instincts.
Defense Mechanisms
5
Projection: attributing uncomfortable feelings to others.
Rationalization: creating false but credible justifications.
Reaction Formation: overacting in the opposite way to the fear.
Regression: going back to acting as a child.
Repression: pushing uncomfortable thoughts into the subconscious.
Sublimation: redirecting 'wrong' urges into socially acceptable actions.
During the oral stage, the infant's primary source of interaction occurs
through the mouth, so the rooting and sucking reflex is especially important.
The mouth is vital for eating, and the infant derives pleasure from oral
stimulation through gratifying activities such as tasting and sucking.
Because the infant is entirely dependent upon caretakers (who are
responsible for feeding the child), the infant also develops a sense of trust and
comfort through this oral stimulation.
The primary conflict at this stage is the weaning process--the child must
become less dependent upon caretakers. If fixation occurs at this stage, Freud
believed the individual would have issues with dependency or aggression.
Oral fixation can result in problems with drinking, eating, smoking, or nail
biting.
6
The Anal Stage
During the anal stage, Freud believed that the primary focus of the libido
was on controlling bladder and bowel movements. The major conflict at this
stage is toilet training--the child has to learn to control his or her bodily
needs. Developing this control leads to a sense of accomplishment and
independence.
According to Freud, success at this stage is dependent upon the way in which
parents approach toilet training. Parents who utilize praise and rewards for
using the toilet at the appropriate time encourage positive outcomes and help
children feel capable and productive. Freud believed that positive experiences
during this stage served as the basis for people to become competent,
productive, and creative adults.
However, not all parents provide the support and encouragement that
children need during this stage. Some parents' instead punish, ridicule, or
shame a child for accidents. According to Freud, inappropriate parental
responses can result in negative outcomes. If parents take an approach that
is too lenient, Freud suggested that an anal-expulsive personality could
develop in which the individual has a messy, wasteful, or destructive
personality. If parents are too strict or begin toilet training too early, Freud
believed that an anal-retentive personality develops in which the individual
is stringent, orderly, rigid, and obsessive.
During the phallic stage, the primary focus of the libido is on the genitals.
Children also discover the differences between males and females. Freud also
believed that boys begin to view their fathers as a rival for the mother‟s
affections. The Oedipus complex describes these feelings of wanting to
possess the mother and the desire to replace the father. However, the child
also fears that he will be punished by the father for these feelings, a fear
Freud termed castration anxiety.
The term Electra complex has been used to described a similar set of feelings
experienced by young girls. Freud, however, believed that girls instead
experience penis envy.
Eventually, the child realizes begins to identify with the same-sex parent as a
means of vicariously possessing the other parent. For girls, however, Freud
believed that penis envy was never fully resolved and that all women remain
somewhat fixated on this stage. Psychologists such as Karen Horney disputed
this theory, calling it both inaccurate and demeaning to women. Instead,
Horney proposed that men experience feelings of inferiority because they
cannot give birth to children.
7
The Latent Period
During the latent period, the libido interests are suppressed. The
development of the ego and superego contribute to this period of calm. The
stage begins around the time that children enter into school and become more
concerned with peer relationships, hobbies, and other interests.
The latent period is a time of exploration in which the sexual energy is still
present, but it is directed into other areas such as intellectual pursuits and
social interactions. This stage is important in the development of social and
communication skills and self-confidence
8
The trait approach to personality is one of the major theoretical areas in the
study of personality. The trait theory suggests that individual personalities
are composed broad dispositions. Consider how you would describe the
personality of a close friend. Chances are that you would list a number of
traits, such as outgoing, kind and even-tempered. A trait can be thought of as
a relatively stable characteristic that causes individuals to behave in certain
ways.
9
Cattell‟s Sixteen Source Traits
1. Introversion/Extraversion:
Introversion involves directing attention on inner experiences,
while extraversion relates to focusing attention outward on other
people and the environment. So, a person high in introversion
might be quiet and reserved, while an individual high in
extraversion might be sociable and outgoing.
2. Neuroticism/Emotional Stability:
This dimension of Eysenck‟s trait theory is related to moodiness
versus even-temperedness. Neuroticism refers to an individual‟s
tendency to become upset or emotional, while stability refers to
the tendency to remain emotionally constant.
3. Psychoticism:
Later, after studying individuals suffering from mental illness,
Eysenck added a personality dimension he called psychoticism to
his trait theory. Individuals who are high on this trait tend to
have difficulty dealing with reality and may be antisocial, hostile,
non-empathetic and manipulative.4
10
The Five-Factor Theory of Personality
Both Cattell‟s and Eysenck‟s theory have been the subject of considerable
research, which has led some theorists to believe that Cattell focused on too
many traits, while Eysenck focused on too few. As a result, a new trait theory
often referred to as the "Big Five" theory emerged. This five-factor model of
personality represents five core traits that interact to form human
personality.5 While researchers often disagree about the exact labels for each
dimension, the following are described most commonly:
1. Extraversion
2. Agreeableness
3. Conscientiousness
4. Neuroticism
5. Openness
While most agree that people can be described based upon their personality
traits, theorists continue to debate the number of basic traits that make up
human personality. While trait theory has objectivity that some personality
theories lack (such as Freud‟s psychoanalytic theory), it also has weaknesses.
Some of the most common criticisms of trait theory center on the fact that
traits are often poor predictors of behavior. While an individual may score
high on assessments of a specific trait, he or she may not always behave that
way in every situation. Another problem is that trait theories do not address
how or why individual differences in personality develop or emerge.
11
stimulus-response psychology, which were dominant in the first half of
this century. Like the psychoanalytic approach, social learning approach
to personality is very deterministic. In contrast to the psychoanalytic
approach, however, it pays very little attention to biological determinants.
Like its parent, behaviorism, the social learning approach has been
strongly influenced by the ideas of Darwin. The processes of learning
shape the individual‟s behavioral repertoire to be adaptive to his or her
environment. Through its emphasis on specifying the environmental
variables that evoke specific behaviors, social learning theory has made a
major contribution to both clinical psychology and personality theory. It
has led us to see human actions as reactions to specific environments, and
it has helped us to focus on the way in which environments control our
behavior and how they can be changed to modify behavior. The careful
application of learning principles has proved successful in changing
maladaptive behavior. S.C. theorists have also challenged the notion that
individuals are cross-situanionally consistent, forcing other personality
theorists to reexamine the fundamental assumptions of their approaches.
Social learning theorists have been criticized for overemphasizing the
importance of situational influences on behavior and thus losing the
person in personality psychology. Many personality theorists are
unwilling to concede that personality has as little cross-situational
consistency as social learning implies.
Of the hundreds of studies Bandura was responsible for, one group stands out
above the others -- the bobo doll studies. He made of film of one of his
students, a young woman, essentially beating up a bobo doll. A bobo doll is
an inflatable, egg-shape balloon creature with a weight in the bottom that
makes it bob back up when you knock him down.
The woman punched the clown, shouting “sockeroo!” She kicked it, sat on it,
hit with a little hammer, and so on, shouting various aggressive phrases.
Bandura showed his film to groups of kindergartners who, as you might
predict, liked it a lot. They then were let out to play. In the play room, of
12
course, were several observers with pens and clipboards in hand, a brand new
bobo doll, and a few little hammers.
And you might predict as well what the observers recorded: A lot of little
kids beating the daylights out of the bobo doll. They punched it and shouted
“sockeroo,” kicked it, sat on it, hit it with the little hammers, and so on. In
other words, they imitated the young lady in the film, and quite precisely at
that.
This might seem like a real nothing of an experiment at first, but consider:
These children changed their behavior without first being rewarded for
approximations to that behavior! And while that may not seem
extraordinary to the average parent, teacher, or casual observer of children,
it didn‟t fit so well with standard behavioristic learning theory. He called the
phenomenon observational learning or modeling, and his theory is usually
called social learning theory.
Bandura did a large number of variations on the study: The model was
rewarded or punished in a variety of ways, the kids were rewarded for their
imitations, the model was changed to be less attractive or less prestigious,
and so on. Responding to criticism that bobo dolls were supposed to be hit, he
even did a film of the young woman beating up a live clown. When the
children went into the other room, what should they find there but -- the live
clown! They proceeded to punch him, kick him, hit him with little hammers,
and so on.
All these variations allowed Bandura to establish that there were certain
steps involved in the modeling process:
13
3. Reproduction. At this point, you‟re just sitting there daydreaming. You
have to translate the images or descriptions into actual behavior. So you
have to have the ability to reproduce the behavior in the first place. I can
watch Olympic ice skaters all day long, yet not be able to reproduce their
jumps, because I can‟t ice skate at all! On the other hand, if I could skate, my
performance would in fact improve if I watch skaters who are better than I
am.
4. Motivation. And yet, with all this, you‟re still not going to do anything
unless you are motivated to imitate, i.e. until you have some reason for doing
it. Bandura mentions a number of motives:
a. past reinforcement,
b. promised reinforcements (incentives) that we can imagine.
c. vicarious reinforcement -- seeing and recalling the model being reinforced.
Notice that these are, traditionally, considered to be the things that “cause”
learning. Bandura is saying that they don‟t so much cause learning as cause
us to demonstrate what we have learned. That is, he sees them as motives.
Of course, the negative motivations are there as well, giving you reasons not
to imitate someone:
d. past punishment.
e. promised punishment (threats).
d. vicarious punishment.
Self-regulation
A very important concept in psychology that can be understood well with self-
regulation is self-concept (better known as self-esteem). If, over the years,
you find yourself meeting your standards and life loaded with self-praise and
self-reward, you will have a pleasant self-concept (high self-esteem). If, on
the other hand, you find yourself forever failing to meet your standards and
punishing yourself, you will have a poor self-concept (low self-esteem).
2. Regarding standards -- make sure your standards aren‟t set too high.
Don‟t set yourself up for failure! Standards that are too low, on the other
hand, are meaningless.
The idea that we are responsible for our own lives, embodied in
existentialism, is exemplified in the work of Carl Rogers. However Rogers
approach was extremely OPTIMISTIC. Rogers believed that “The organism
has one basic tendency and striving- to actualize, maintain, and enhance the
experiencing organism” (1951, p. 487).
Rogers used the term Fully Functioning Person for someone who is self-
actualizing. These people are OPEN TO EXPERIENCING THEIR
FEELINGS, don‟t feel threatened by those feelings no matter what they are.
They trust their own feelings. They are open to the experiences of the world.
They live lives full of meaning, challenge and fulfillment.
One way of looking at the self is to look at the ideal self and the actual self:
The actual self is what you are now or even what you THINK you are because
remember from this perspective it‟s all about subjective perceptions.
There‟s a second kind of congruence and that is between the actual self and
experience. That is the experiences in life should fit with the type of person
you think you are. So there will be incongruity if you think you‟re generous
but find yourself being mean to someone or if you think your ruthless and you
find yourself being soft and mushy. If you think you‟re clever and do badly in
a test there will be incongruence.
17
partner but tell yourself that it doesn‟t matter because your partner won‟t
mind.
Hierarchy of Needs
4. ESTEEM NEEDS: needs for a sense of mastery and power. Need for
appreciation from others.
5. SELF ACTUALIZATION: similar use of the term to the way Rogers used
it. “The tendency to become whatever you‟re capable of becoming”: The
highest of human motives. In trying to describe the process of self-
actualization, Maslow focused on moments when self actualization was
clearly occurring. Maslow used the term “peak experiences” to refer to
moments of intense self-actualization. At these moments people feel
connected to their surroundings and aware of all the sounds and colours
around them. There‟s a loss of a sense of time as the experience flows around
you. You may feel awe, wonder or even ecstasy. This is similar to what
Csikszentmihalyi (chick-sent-me-high) calls “flow” but he sees it not so much
as joy or ecstasy but rather as a period of intense concentration, with a
slightly elevated mood when time flows by very quickly.
18
Motives WEAKEN as go from the more primitive to the higher needs (up the
pyramid). In general you need to deal with lower level needs before you can
move onto other needs.
19
Motivation
The word motivation is coined from the Latin word "movere", which means to
move. Motivation is defined as an internal drive that activates behavior and
gives it direction. The term motivation theory is concerned with the processes
that describe why and how human behavior is activated and directed. It is
regarded as one of the most important areas of study in the field of
organizational behavior. There are two different categories of motivation
theories such as content theories, and process theories.
Achievement motivation: This is involved where students learn 'in the hope of
success'. Ausubel suggests that there are three elements in motivation of this
type:
Intrinsic motivation: In this case there are no external rewards; the task is
undertaken for the pleasure ind satisfaction it brings to the student. It seems
to be central to 'high quality involvement' in a task and to be self-maintaining
and self-terminating. Curiosity and a desire to meet challenges may
characterize the learning of students motivated in this style.
Theories of Motivation
1. Physiological needs, e.g. hunger, thirst, leading to a desire tor food and
water.
These needs are hierarchical; high-level needs will be attended to only after
low-level needs are satisfied. Maslow's basic needs (physiological, safety and
belonging) are termed deficiency because they motivate (lead to behavior)
when the organism has a deficiency with respect to a need (for example, lacks
21
food or water). The metaneeds (esteem and self-actualisation) are termed
growth needs because they motivate behaviors that do not result from
deficiencies but from a natural human tendency toward growth. The growth
needs will be attended to only after the basic needs are reasonably satisfied.
The ultimate need is that of self-actualisation. Self-actualization is a difficult
concept to explain. Leclerc et al (1998) in their survey of 26 "experts"
indicates a general consensus that self-actualization is a process rather than
a state. It is a process of growth—of becoming—evident in the unfolding and
fulfillment of self. In a sense it is trying to become the best we can, as
individuals, become.
Application to education:
The essential point is that from Maslow's persepctive, no learning will take
place unless the students basic needs are met. Thus they need to have their
physiological needs met along with feelings of safety and also experience a
sense of belonging. This makes intutive sense (has face validity) because a
student who is hungry will never work well, nor will a student who is being
bullied (lack of safety) and neither will the student who feels that they are an
'outsider' and that they have no friends. If these basic needs are met then
motivation to learn should be present, especially if the student gains some
esteem from their efforts. Indeed, for Malsow, the best motivation for
learning will occur when we are attempting to self-actualise, for this is when
we will be truly experiencing growth and development. Ultimately any factor
which prevents us from self-actualising will be a hinderence to our
motivation to learn.
Bandura informs us that self-efficacy has to do with our own estimates of our
personal effectiveness. "Perceived self-efficacy," he writes, "refers to beliefs in
one's capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to
produce given attainments" (1997, p. 3). The most efficacious people are those
who are most competent. Accordingly, self-efficacy has two related
components: The first has to do with the skills—the actual competencies—
required for successful performance; the second concerns the individual's
personal estimates of competence.
Positive and negative reinforcement are used in virtually all classrooms; teachers
praise and admonish students, they give high and low grades, they smile and frown.
These and a thousand other indicators of approval or disapproval are examples of
reinforcement. When reinforcement is used judiciously and systematically, it can have
profound effects on behavior. However we are not simply hungry rats in a Skinner box. If
we look into a classroom, we will see that behaviour is not simply driven by external
rewards like chocolate bars or gold stars or high marks. Rather behaviour is driven by
cognitions and by emotions. It is not surprising that current applications of behavioursit
principles to the classroom take thinking into account. As Stipek (1988) notes, the most
powerful reinforcers for students are stimuli such as praise, and that given that the
effectiveness of these stimuli clearly depend on a student's interpretations of the teacher's
behavior, it is apparant that cognition is central to understanding how reinforcement
works as a motvator. Thus a simplistic stimulus-response reading of behaviouristic
principles will not offer an adequate understanding of the use of reinforcement in the
classroom.
23
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Ames, R., and Ames, C. "Motivation and Effective Teaching." In B. F. Jones and L. Idol (eds.),
Dimensions of Thinking and Cognitive Instruction. Hillsdale, N. J.: ErIbaum, 1990.
Angelo, T. A. "Ten Easy Pieces: Assessing Higher Learning in Four Dimensions." In T. A. Angelo
(ed.), Classroom Research: Early Lessons from Success. New Directions for Teaching and
Learning, no. 46. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1991.
Belmont, M., & Marolla, F.A. (1973). "Birth order, family size, and intelligence". Science 182: 1096–
1101.
Binet. A., & Simon, T. (1916). The development of intelligence in children. Baltimore, Williams &
Wilkins. (Reprinted 1973, New York: Arno Press; 1983, Salem, NH: Ayer Company).
Bligh, D. A. What's the Use of Lecturing? Devon, England: Teaching Services Centre, University of
Exeter, 1971.
Brock, S. C. Practitioners' Views on Teaching the Large Introductory College Course. Manhattan:
Center for Faculty Evaluation and Development, Kansas State University, 1976.
Cashin, W. E. "Motivating Students." Idea Paper, no. 1. Manhattan: Center for Faculty Evaluation
and Development in Higher Education, Kansas State University, 1979.
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. A., & Sharp, D. W. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking.
New York: Basic Books.
Erickson, B. L., and Strommer, D. W. Teaching College Freshmen. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass,
1991.
Forsyth, D. R., and McMillan, J. H. "Practical Proposals for Motivating Students." In R. J. Menges
and M. D. Svinicki (eds.), College Teaching: From Theory to Practice. New Directions in Teaching
and Learning, no. 45. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1991.
24
Gottfredson, L. S. (Ed.) (1997). Intelligence and social policy. Intelligence, 24(1). (Special issue)
[1]PDF (798 KiB)
Gottfredson, L. S. (1998). The general intelligence factor. Scientific American Presents, 9(4):24-29.
[2]PDF (319 KiB)
Haier, R. J., Chueh, D., Touchette, P., Lott, I., Buchsbaum, M., Macmillan, D., et al. (1995). "Brain
size and cerebral glucose metabolic rate in nonspecific mental retardation and Down syndrome".
Intelligence 20: 191–210.
Hawkings, Jeff (2005). On intelligence, Times Books, Henry Holt and Co. ISBN 0-8050-7456-2
McMillan, J. H., and Forsyth, D. R. "What Theories of Motivation Say About Why Learners Learn."
In R. J. Menges and M. D. Svinicki (eds.), College Teaching: From Theory to Practice. New
Directions for Teaching and Learning, no. 45. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1991.
Nagoshi, C. T. & Johnson, R. C. (1986). "The ubiquity of g". Personality and Individual Differences
7: 201–207.
Noguera, P.A. (2001). Racial politics and the elusive quest for excellence and equity in education.
In Motion Magazine article
Rlomin, J. C. DeFries, G. E. McClearn, M. Rutter, Behavioral Genetics (Freeman, New York, ed. 3,
1997).
Stocking, G. (1968). Race, culture, and evolution. New York: Free Press.
25