Explicit Grammatical Intervent PDF
Explicit Grammatical Intervent PDF
Explicit Grammatical Intervent PDF
Clinical Focus
Purpose: This article summarizes the shared principles effective practices and support more widespread adoption
and evidence underpinning methods employed in the of syntactic interventions with school-age children.
three sentence-level (syntactic) grammatical intervention Conclusion: In each approach to syntactic intervention,
approaches developed by the authors. We discuss careful and detailed analysis of grammatical knowledge is
associated clinical resources and map a way forward for used to support target selection. Intervention targets are
clinically useful research in this area. explicitly described and presented systematically using
Method: We provide an overview of the principles and multimodal representations within engaging and functional
perspectives that are common across our three syntactic activities. Treatment stimuli are varied within a target
intervention approaches: MetaTaal (Zwitserlood, 2015; pattern in order to maximize learning. Similar intervention
Zwitserlood, Wijnen, et al., 2015), the SHAPE CODING™ intervals and intensities have been studied and proven
system (Ebbels, 2007; Ebbels et al., 2014, 2007), and clinically feasible and have produced measurable effects.
Complex Sentence Intervention (Balthazar & Scott, 2017, We identify a need for more research evidence to maximize
2018). A description of each approach provides examples the effectiveness of our grammatical interventions,
and summarizes current evidence supporting effectiveness encompassing languages other than English, as well as
for children with developmental language disorder ranging practical clinical tools to guide target selection, measurement
in age from 5 to 16 years. We suggest promising directions of outcomes, and decisions about how to tailor interventions
for future research that will advance our understanding of to individual needs.
A
n estimated 7% of the world’s children are affected in early childhood, but associated language problems persist
by developmental language disorder (DLD; into early adolescence and even adulthood (Conti-Ramsden,
Bishop et al., 2017; Norbury et al., 2016; Tomblin 2008; e.g., Law et al., 2009; Nippold et al., 2009), affecting
et al., 1997, 2003). While language difficulties vary by age, psychosocial (Clegg et al., 2005) and educational (Conti-
language, and functional context, problems with morphol- Ramsden, 2008) outcomes as well as employment prospects
ogy and syntax—the “grammatical” aspects of language— (Conti-Ramsden & Durkin, 2012; Conti-Ramsden et al.,
affect a large majority of these children (Fey et al., 2004; 2018; Law et al., 2009). With significant impact on func-
Leonard, 2014; Nippold et al., 2009). DLD may be diagnosed tioning documented across the life span, interventions for
children with DLD must address their needs at a variety of
ages and support language in academic as well as voca-
a tional and social realms.
College of Health and Human Services, Governors State University,
University Park, IL
The amount of evidence supporting effective gram-
b
Moor House Research and Training Institute, Moor House School & matical intervention procedures varies considerably depend-
College, United Kingdom ing upon language, age, and targets. The vast majority of
c
Language and Cognition, University College London, United Kingdom treatment studies are focused on English-speaking children
d
HU University of Applied Sciences, Utrecht, the Netherlands aged 7 years and under (Cirrin & Gillam, 2008; Ebbels,
Correspondence to Catherine H. Balthazar: CBalthazar@govst.edu 2014) and, as a consequence, primarily address English
Editor-in-Chief: Holly L. Storkel morphological features that are usually acquired in early
Editor: Amanda J. Owen Van Horne childhood, with less focus on the developmentally advanced
Received May 13, 2019
Revision received June 13, 2019
Disclosure: The intervention approaches described in this article were developed
Accepted November 7, 2019
and investigated by the authors. The authors do not have any financial interests
https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-19-00046 related to the intervention approaches. SHAPE CODING is a U.K.-registered
Publisher Note: This article is part of the Forum: Morphosyntax trademark to Moor House School & College, who is an employer of the second
Assessment and Intervention for Children. author.
226 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020 • Copyright © 2020 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Table 1. Studies targeting grammar in children and adolescents with language disorders (Ebbels, 2014).
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
Age (years;months)
and number
Source and design of participants Language target Intervention Outcomes and magnitude of effectsa
Ebbels et al. (2007) 11;0–16;1 Production of verb SHAPE CODING system or a SHAPE CODING: Both intervention groups improved
RCT N = 27 argument semantic intervention significantly more than controls on verb argument
structure Direct 1:1, 1 × 30 min per week structure. (for the SHAPE CODING group vs.
for 9 weeks controls: d = 1.3, large effect size). Maintained
for 3 months. Generalized to control verbs.
Ebbels et al. (2014) 11;3–16;1 Comprehension of SHAPE CODING system The SHAPE CODING group showed significantly
RCT N = 14 coordinating Direct 1:1, 1 × 30 min per week more progress than waiting controls on targeted
conjunctions for 8 weeks conjunctions (d = 1.6, large effect size). Waiting
controls also made significant progress when
they too received intervention (d = 2.1, large
effect size). Generalized to TROG-2 assessment,
(d = 1.4, large effect size), but not to passives.
Maintained for 4 months.
Ebbels & van der Lely 11–14 years Comprehension SHAPE CODING system Three of four participants showed significant
(2001) N=4 and production Direct 1:1, 2 × 30 min per progress with comprehension and expression
Multiple baseline of wh-questions week (10 weeks for of passives. Two participants focused on
and passive passives, 20 weeks for comprehension of wh-questions and made
formation wh-questions) significant progress. All four focussed on
production of wh-questions and made significant
progress with subject questions. Two of four
made significant progress with object questions.
After 30 weeks, progress maintained for 2/3 on
passive comprehension and expression, 1/4 for
production of subject wh-questions, and 0/4 for
production of object questions.
Ebbels (2007) 11;8–12;9 Comprehension SHAPE CODING system Two of three children showed significant progress
Multiple baseline N=3 of dative and Direct 1:1, 2 × 30 min per week with dative; 2/2 showed significant progress with
comparative for 10 weeks comparative wh-questions.
wh-questions
Kulkarni et al. (2014) 8;11 and 8;10 Use of past tense SHAPE CODING system Participant A: stable baseline, then significant
Multiple baseline N=2 morphology Phase 1: 1 × 30 min per progress on sentence completion for treated
week with SLT for 10 weeks, and untreated verbs after Phase 1, and progress
plus 3.5 hr with TA for on conversation task only after Phase 2. No
Participant A and 0.5 hr change in control structure. Participant B:
with TA for Participant B. stable baseline, then significant progress with
Phase 2: SLT carried conversation after Phase 1, and progress in
out four sessions in class, sentence completion task only significant after
met with parents, and Phase 2. No change in control structure. Progress
carried out session at maintained for 6 weeks. For Participant B,
participants’ homes. generalization occurred to conversation during
Phase 1. Participant A needed generalization
therapy (Phase 2) for progress to generalize to
conversation.
(table continues)
Age (years;months)
and number
Source and design of participants Language target Intervention Outcomes and magnitude of effectsa
SHAPE CODING system plus 2/3 made significant gains in targeted expressive
Calder et al. (2018) 6;2, 6;6, and 7;0 Use of past tense
systematic cueing hierarchy morphosyntax (–ed) After 5 weeks, some
Multiple baseline N=3 morphology
Direct 1:1 with SLT, 2 × 25 min decreased for 1/3 and further increased for
for 5 weeks 1/3. Generalized to TROG (2/3) and TEGI (3/3)
Calder et al. (2020) 5;10–6;8 Use of past tense SHAPE CODING system plus Most children showed significant improvement
Multiple baseline N=9 morphology systematic cueing hierarchy on trained verbs within (8/9, d = 0.9, large
Direct 1:1 with SLT, 2 × effect size) and between (7/9, d = 0.9, large
20–30 min per week for effect size) sessions and untrained verbs (7/9,
10 weeks, 50 trials per d = 0.8, large effect size). Progress maintained
session (1,000 in total) for 5 weeks.
Generalized to grammaticality judgment (d = 0.26,
small effect size) and standardized measures
of expressive grammar, SPELT-3 (8/9), but not
receptive grammar (1/9). Gains on control
measures not significant when participants
combined: 3s (1/9, d = −0.05, no effect); ’s
(2/9, d = 0.1, no effect).
Tobin & Ebbels (2019) 10–14 years with Singular vs. plural SHAPE CODING system Significantly more progress with intervention than
Balthazar et al.: Explicit Grammatical Intervention
Single-baseline design complex needs auxiliary or copula Direct 1:2 or 1:3, 2 × 20 min during baseline (d = 0.92, large effect size).
(including six with per week for 4 weeks Generalized from auxiliary to copula and
Down syndrome) vice versa.
N = 11
Ebbels (2007) 11–13 years Use of past tense SHAPE CODING system As a group, postintervention scores did not differ
Group study N=9 morphology in Direct group for 1:9, then 1:2 from preintervention scores, despite a large
comparing pre- writing for two children, 1 hr per effect size (d = 1.7). Six of nine children
and posttest week for 16 weeks + improved; two improved only after an additional
scores approximately 4 hr for paired therapy. Generalized to spontaneous
two children written work in class.
(table continues)
229
Table 1. (Continued).
Age (years;months)
and number
Source and design of participants Language target Intervention Outcomes and magnitude of effectsa
Balthazar & Scott 10;10–14;11 Production of Complex Sentence Intervention Written sentence combining pre–post improved
(2018) N = 30 complex Direct 1:1, 9 or 18 × 40–60 min significantly (η2 = .816; medium effect size); effects
Multiple-baseline sentences once or twice a week for differed for each sentence, with significant gains
design (adverbial, object 9 weeks on average for adverbial (SMDp = .91; large effect
complement, and size) and relative (SMDp = 1.00; large effect
relative clauses) size) clauses. Twenty-four participants (80%)
demonstrated a medium or large effect size on
at least one of the sentence types. Seven (23%)
of the participants achieved a medium or large
effect size on two sentence types; eight
participants (27%) achieved it on all three.
Similar gains in both the low- and high-dosage
groups. No generalization to written narratives.
Zwitserlood, Wijnen, et al. 9;3–12;8 Comprehension and MetaTaal Production of relative clauses via (written) sentence
(2015) N = 12 production of Direct 1:1, 10 × 30 min combining improved significantly with intervention
Single-baseline relative clauses twice a week for 5 weeks (r ranged between −.53 and −.60; medium
design effect size). Elicited production (without written
prompts) and comprehension of relative clauses
did not improve. Maintained for 3 months.
Zwitserlood (2015) 9;7–15;11 Comprehension MetaTaal Production of relative clauses via (written) sentence
Single-baseline N = 18 and production of Direct 1:1, 10 × 30 min combining improved significantly with intervention
design relative clauses twice a week for 5 weeks (r ranged between .40 and .50; medium size
effects). Elicited production (without written
prompts) and comprehension of relative clauses
did not improve. Maintained for 3 months.
Note. RCT = randomized controlled trial; TROG-2 = Test for Reception of Grammar (Bishop, 2003); SLT = speech-language therapist; TA = teacher aide; TEGI = Rice/Wexler Test of
Early Grammatical Impairment (Rice & Wexler, 2001); SPELT = Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test 3 (Dawson et al., 2003).
a
Effect sizes in each of these studies were reported for different language targets and outcome measures and using a number of different effect size metrics. While these values are
not directly comparable across studies, for illustrative purposes, we have provided effect size values and magnitude of effects where possible. Please refer to the original study
reports for technical information regarding effect size.
232 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
colors are not used, or the internal structure, as the aim is to a tensed verb) and this has to match the time arrows on
show the overall meaning of the sentence. However, colors any “when” triangle.
and additional internal shapes could be included, if desired.
Lines for singular and plural. Noun–verb agreement Practicalities of Introducing and Using the SHAPE
is shown in the SHAPE CODING system by using double CODING System
lines under plural nouns and verbs and single lines under The SHAPE CODING system is a flexible tool that
singular nouns and verbs (see Figure 3). This helps avoid can be used to teach whichever aspect of morphosyntax is
errors such as “the boys is running,” as the noun and verb the current target for the child. Thus, the first step is for
must match in terms of single or double lines. A double the SLP to decide which area to focus on, and this will
red line in the oval noun phrase requires a double blue line depend on which specific areas the child is struggling to
in the diamond auxiliary or copula (which goes with “are” understand or produce and which of these is most func-
or “were”). If there are two entities in the oval (and hence tional and important in the child’s current circumstances,
two red lines in total, e.g., one under “boy” and one under for example, which will help the child communicate or access
“dog”), a double line is required in the diamond. This can and participate in lessons better. These decisions are sepa-
help prevent errors such as “the boy and his dog is run- rate from decisions about which methods to use. Once the
ning” as the single line under “is” does not match the two SLP has chosen a target, the SHAPE CODING system
red lines in the oval. may help them show the child how the rules for that struc-
Arrows. Verb morphology is indicated by underlining ture work. It is likely that only some aspects of the SHAPE
verbs (in blue) and using arrows (see Figure 4). Tensed CODING system will be required, depending on which
verbs have vertical arrows, with a vertical arrow in the middle structure is targeted, and only those aspects need to be
of the line for present tense and at the left-hand end for taught. Others can be added in later as other targets are
past tense. The progressive participle has a zigzag under introduced.
“–ing” to represent the continuous aspect. Children are The SHAPE CODING system is introduced to chil-
taught that every sentence must have “a down arrow” (i.e., dren using complete sentences, usually using structures they
Oval Who/what? External argument noun phrase (subject in active sentences) Red (often also pink)
Rectangle Who/what? Internal argument noun phrase (belongs inside other shapes) Red (often also pink)
Cloud What like/how feel? Adjective phrase Green
Semicircle Where? Usually prepositional phrase (preposition + internal argument NP) Often yellow
Hexagon What doing? Verb phrase Blue
Diamond Auxiliary, copula, modal verbs Blue
Triangle When?
Flag How?
Red Nouns and pronouns dog, chair, he, him Ovals and rectangles
Pink Determiners and possessive pronouns the, a, his Often required with “red words” in ovals and rectangles
Blue Verbs sleep, snore Hexagons and diamonds
Green Adjectives small, sad Clouds
Yellow Prepositions on, under, beside Semicircles
Brown Adverbs slowly, fast Flags
Purple Coordinating conjunctions and, but, or
Orange Subordinating conjunctions when, if, because Often triangles
can already produce and understand. The starting point a question (e.g., “who”) and its answer (e.g., “the man,”
will depend on what structures are targeted, but given that “the dog,” or “Sam”). It is also important that the children
many grammatical targets for children with DLD involve do not think that each shape contains a single word, so a
verbs, the most common starting point is probably a simple next step may be to introduce subject–verb–object sentences
subject–verb sentence, for example, “the man is laughing.” (e.g., “the dog is chewing a bone”) and show that the answer
This would normally be introduced in the present progressive to the question “what is the dog doing” is “chewing a
(even if the child usually omits the auxiliary, this would bone,” thus more than one word. The rectangle may or
still be shown but would not be a focus in the initial stages). may not be put around “a bone” depending on whether the
The initial focus is for the child to understand that the SLP wants to focus on this (e.g., this may be desirable if
person or thing doing the action (“the man”) goes in the the focus will be on verb argument structure or increasing
oval and answers the question “who/what?”, while the ac- sentence length but may not be necessary if the focus will
tion (“laughing”) goes in the hexagon and answers the be on tense or agreement).
question “what doing?”. This can be done by turning shapes When teaching the child a new grammatical structure
around that have the question on the back and the phrase or rule, the following sequence may be used.
on the front, so that they see the direct connection between
1. The SLP introduces the target structure and gram-
the question and the answer. Laminated shapes work well
matical rule using the SHAPE CODING system and
for this, and the SHAPE CODING app also aims to rec-
demonstrates its meaning (often using small figures
reate this by flipping the shape following a long hold to
that can be placed on the relevant shapes and also
reveal the question. Omissions of determiners “the,” pos-
act out the meaning).
sessive pronouns “my,” or progressive endings “–ing”
are less important at this stage and can be a focus of later 2. Templates for that structure may be created using
intervention. the shapes, colors, arrows, and/or lines as appropri-
A key early step when using the SHAPE CODING ate to the target. These may be on paper or may be
system is for the children to understand the link between separate laminated shapes that can be written on or
234 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
236 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
begins the same way as sentence identification, but the Preference production. The preference production
main and subordinate clauses are pulled apart. These sen- task highlights meaning contrasts between pairs of sen-
tences can be presented on paper or on a word processing tences that are similar in structure. It requires the student
program or app (see Figure 7). Students either write out to integrate what they know about the world with the con-
the main clause and the subordinate clause on paper or cepts encoded by the various types of complex sentence
highlight and move these clauses into the appropriate slots clauses. The clinician presents two sentences that are almost
on a computer or tablet device. Deconstruction is particu- the same, except for one key word or phrase. The clinician
larly helpful for illustrating how relative clauses, which and student read the sentences aloud together and then
involve both pronominal replacement and often a change find and highlight the subordinate clause in each one. Then,
in word order, are constructed. the clinician will ask the student to think about what each
Sentence combining. Sentence combining also illus- sentence means and decide which one he or she agrees
trates the relationship and transformation involved in cre- with more, or which one is true, or which one makes more
ating a complex sentence. Two clauses are presented in a sense. Some clinician assistance is usually required to make
form that facilitates a student’s ability to easily rearrange the inferences necessary to decide which sentence is the
words to create their responses, such as movable strips of most sensible. It may also be necessary to explain vocabulary
paper or electronic media as with sentence deconstruction. words with which the student is unfamiliar; usually, a
The clinician helps the student start combining by building simple explanation or synonym will suffice.
the ideas behind the sentence and gradually guiding the Cloze production. A more advanced discourse-level
student into the desired sentence form. Grammatical, spell- production activity, which is more challenging because it
ing, and other errors are not viewed as incorrect for the taxes memory, conceptual, linguistic, and metalinguistic
purposes of this task. Only clause structure errors are di- resources simultaneously, is a cloze production passage. In
rectly corrected and discussed. All other errors are simply cloze production, the clinician prepares a short reading
corrected through modeling or recasting. passage that contains the target complex sentence type(s).
Sentence generation. Sentence generation can be ac- One copy is complete, and a second copy is modified to
complished in a number of ways. In the CSI protocol, we remove the target subordinate clause(s) and replace them
provided a brief, two- to three-sentence “story” and a main with blanks. This is easily done on a computer within a
clause followed by a blank, in order to elicit the subordinate word-processing program. Initially, the clinician and student
clause (see Figure 8). This process works well for all subor- read the first passage aloud together. Then, the complete
dinate clause types but is particularly useful for teaching version is removed, and the passage is again read aloud
object complement clauses. together, stopping where the blanks are. The student is
Figure 6. Example of a completed student worksheet used for the sentence identification task. The student’s
response is to highlight the target subordinate clause, in this case, a relative clause.
238 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
MetaTaal
The MetaTaal approach derives from the program
“Grammar in Form and Color” developed in Denmark in
the early ’70s for children with severe hearing problems
(K. Thyme, personal communication, December 8, 2010).
This intervention program targeted morphosyntactic struc-
tures by using a metalinguistic and multimodal approach.
The program used LEGO bricks of various shapes and
colors to depict word classes and grammatical functions.
Building sentences with these LEGO bricks helped children
to understand and produce grammatical sentence struc-
tures. Thyme based her approach on Freunthaller (1937),
who developed a program that visualized grammar in
order to teach German and foreign languages to children
with severe hearing problems. Freunthaller’s basic idea was
that children with grammatical problems could learn mor-
phosyntactic structures with visual support and “pattern
practice.” Thyme’s program was never officially published,
but Van Geel (1973) translated and adapted the interven-
tion for Dutch children with DLD. The program became
quite popular in the Netherlands, and LEGO provided
special packages containing all the LEGO bricks needed to
work with the program. In later years, the interest in the
approach gradually waned, and the program was not devel-
oped further. The original versions of Grammar in Form
and Color only contained material to construct simple sen-
tences and coordinated sentences. Zwitserlood, Wijnen,
et al. (2015) further adapted the Dutch Grammar in Form
and Color program with LEGO bridges functioning as
connectors in order to build more complex sentences. This
240 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
and retention period, the children did not receive therapy. executed, except for a small pilot study investigating the
Results showed that the children made significant progress feasibility of using MetaTaal to teach English as a foreign
on two out of three relative clause production tasks, but language to Dutch adolescents with DLD (Develing, 2018).
not on the comprehension task. Subsequently, this inter- Most SLPs working with MetaTaal are members of the
vention study was replicated with a group of 18 children MetaTaal Facebook and Google discussion groups. Mem-
with DLD, with a mean age of 12;9 (Zwitserlood, 2015). bers often report that children are motivated to work with
These children visited four different special schools for chil- the program and make good progress during treatment
dren with DLD in the Netherlands and received additional and also on standardized language measures.
special care because of behavioral problems. Four different
SLPs treated the children with the same protocolled inter-
vention program. They were also blind to the results of all Summary and Directions for Future Research
measurements until the study was finished. Results from
Evidence for use of the syntactic intervention
this replication study were similar to the first study, with
approaches discussed here, with a range of people with
significant gains on the production tasks and no gains on
DLD and for a range of grammatical structures, is slowly
the comprehension task. It is noteworthy that this somewhat
growing, but many areas remain to be investigated. All
older group of children scored higher during baseline
measurements and also obtained higher scores during three of our approaches could also be described as treat-
the posttherapy and retention measurements. Notably, ment “packages,” interventions that use a variety of tools
it was observed in both studies that the relative clause sen- and methods together (i.e., metalinguistic instruction, stim-
tence repetition task yielded no effect immediately post- ulus organization, multiple modalities, target selection,
therapy, but the children did improve between the last intervention schedule). These separate components have not
baseline measurement and the retention measurement. been teased apart, and we therefore do not know to what
This interesting finding suggests that learning of complex extent they contribute differentially to treatment effects;
grammatical structures may be protracted and that admin- however, it is not clear whether separating elements is fea-
istering retention measurements is important. sible or necessary in order to make progress in maximizing
Based on the promising results of both MetaTaal treatment effectiveness. Generally, the greatest need for
intervention studies, the MetaTaal program was expanded syntactic intervention research involves practical variables
with other subordinate clause types, in particular, adverbial that could be leveraged to promote greater success of chil-
clauses with conjunctions “omdat” (because) and “als…dan” dren with DLD and greater adoption of evidence-based
(if/when…then). With this addition, MetaTaal covers the practices by SLPs internationally.
most frequently used subordinate clause types in spoken
Dutch child language. Currently, the program is used widely Treatment Delivery
by SLPs in special education and private practices. To With a few exceptions, these interventions have been
date, no further MetaTaal intervention studies have been delivered using individualized one-on-one child/clinician
Figure 10. Coordinated sentence with reduction built with LEGO bricks.
Figure 12. Sentence built with LEGO bricks containing a center-embedded subject-subject-type relative clause.
242 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
244 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020
246 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 226–246 • April 2020