The Biology of Homosexuality
The Biology of Homosexuality
The Biology of Homosexuality
Editors
Gregory F. Ball, Johns Hopkins University
Jacques Balthazart, University of Liège
Randy J. Nelson, Ohio State University
J ACQUES BALTHAZART, P HD
GIGA Neurosciences
University of Liège, Belgium
1
1
Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press, Inc.,
198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY, 10016
United States of America
Oxford University Press, Inc. publishes works that further Oxford University’s objective
of excellence in research, scholarship, and education
Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or
transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press,
Inc., or as expressly permitted by law, by licence, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reproduction
rights organization. Inquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the
Rights Department, Oxford University Press, Inc., at the address above
You must not circulate this work in any other form and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
____________________________________________________
1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
This material is not intended to be, and should not be considered, a substitute for medical or other
professional advice. Treatment for the conditions described in this material is highly dependent on the
individual circumstances. And, while this material is designed to offer accurate information with respect to
the subject matter covered and to be current as of the time it was written, research and knowledge about
medical and health issues is constantly evolving and dose schedules for medications are being revised
continually, with new side effects recognized and accounted for regularly. Readers must therefore always
check the product information and clinical procedures with the most up-to-date published product
information and data sheets provided by the manufacturers and the most recent codes of conduct and safety
regulation. The publisher and the authors make no representations or warranties to readers, express or
implied, as to the accuracy or completeness of this material. Without limiting the foregoing, the publisher and
the authors make no representations or warranties as to the accuracy or efficacy of the drug dosages
mentioned in the material. The authors and the publisher do not accept, and expressly disclaim, any
responsibility for any liability, loss or risk that may be claimed or incurred as a consequence of the use and/or
application of any of the contents of this material.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
References 163
Index 179
This page intentionally left blank
INTRODUCTION: WHY THIS BOOK?
evidence, I hope this book will help make society aware that sexual orientation
is probably under the control of embryonic endocrine/genetic phenomena in
which there is little room for individual choice. Therefore society needs to
rethink its attitudes toward homosexuals and their parents.
Animal studies are of great support in understanding the controlling forces
of sexuality. These studies demonstrate the existence in animals of neuro-
biological mechanisms determining the behavior and sexual orientation that
seem also to be present, mutatis mutandis, in human studies. This experimental
work, carried out in strictly controlled conditions, provides a strong support
to interpret correlative studies conducted in humans, even if the details of the
mechanisms involved are currently not fully understood. They probably never
can be, given that it is impossible to perform most types of experiments in
humans (for obvious ethical reasons) and that there is a very long latency
(greater than 20 years) between the events that probably induce homo- or hetero-
sexuality (endocrine environment of the embryo and its genetic heritage) and
the identification of their effects (sexual orientation of the adult). Prospective
studies are thus extremely difficult and expensive if you consider, in addition,
the limited number of homosexuals who appear in the population studied.
Retrospective studies are possible, but less convincing because the selection of
subjects and their controls, even if they are well matched, can be skewed by
unidentified factors.
For more than 35 years, I have been studying the hormonal and neural
mechanisms that control the so-called instinctive behaviors (spontaneously
expressed and species-specific) in animals. Much of this work has been devoted
to the study of male sexual behavior, and led me to turn my attention to sex dif-
ferences that are associated with this behavior. In most species, males and
females indeed show different behaviors, and furthermore they mostly direct
these behaviors toward the opposite sex (i.e., they show a marked sex difference
in sexual orientation). These differences in behavior and its orientation are
clearly controlled in animals by steroid hormones produced by the gonads.
The action of these steroids on behavior takes place mostly in the brain, par-
ticularly in some of its oldest parts from an evolutionary point of view, the
hypothalamus and preoptic area. The primary goal of research in neuroendo-
crinology is to advance, at the basic level, our knowledge of how the brain func-
tions. Clearly, however, the potential application of this knowledge to the
human species is an issue that cannot be escaped. Is human sexual behavior
under the control of testicular and ovarian hormones? And do these hormones
affect the orientation of this behavior?
It is interesting that research on the neuroendocrine basis of sexual behavior
in animals and humans is a discipline that has developed more in the Anglo-
Saxon countries, mainly the United States and to a lesser extent Great Britain.
Introduction: Why This Book? xi
The bulk of the scientific literature in this area is therefore published in English.
For the same reason, there are also a few English-language books devoted to the
biological mechanisms that control animal or human sexuality in general,
including sometimes a discussion of sexual orientation (LeVay, 1993; 1996;
Strong & DeVault, 1997; Rosenzweig et al., 2004; LeVay & Valente, 2006; Agmo,
2007).
However, there is not, to my knowledge, a simple description of the biologi-
cal mechanisms that are involved in the determination of sexual orientation in
animals and also presumably in humans. By showing herein that the sexual
orientation of a subject is controlled by a set of biological and environmental
factors, I hope to provide that book. This text was originally written and pub-
lished in French in early February 2010. While its English version was being
prepared, another book written by Simon LeVay was published that also focuses
on the biological bases of human homosexuality (Gay, Straight, and the Reason
Why, Oxford University Press, 2010). Although much of the experimental evi-
dence presented in LeVay’s book is also discussed here, these two books differ
in their overall approach. LeVay starts from the notion of sexual orientation in
humans and discusses this orientation based on human studies largely, using
animal studies mainly to support conclusions derived from observations in
humans. In contrast, the present text starts from the animal work that is the
focus of my scientific research and in a second step analyzes how this animal
work potentially applies to humans. It’s quite interesting that while these two
books start from different premises, they reach the exact same conclusion, that
is, that homosexuality in humans is to a very large extent, if not exclusively,
determined by biological factors acting prenatally or soon after birth and that
the social or educational environment plays at best a subsidiary role in this
determinism. The conjunction of these two books thus strongly reinforces the
general conclusion presented in the following pages.
It must also be noted that while describing the experimental support of this
thesis, the book will focus on more than just homosexuality, as the context in
which biological factors act produces multiple results. Ignoring the nonhomo-
sexual results of the process would present an incomplete, context-free picture
of the factors at work. Thus, even readers not particularly interested in homo-
sexuality will find this book a useful guide to the biological bases of sexual ori-
entation. Understanding the roots of homosexuality requires also investigating
the bases of heterosexuality, since both forms of sexual orientation only repre-
sent the two extremes of a continuum called sexual orientation. This question
is then of interest to everyone. I will also note in advance that because male
homosexuality is more clear-cut than female homosexuality and its biological
origins are better documented, at times it will dominate the discussion. This is
merely an unfortunate result of where our current scientific understanding lies
xii Introduction: Why This Book?
and nothing more. This book presents as balanced a portrait as possible, and no
slight or judgment is intended.
Scientists reading this book will think, “We have heard all this before.”
Certainly the content of this book is not scientifically revolutionary; it has been
known for at least a decade, though over that time more evidence has continued
to pile up. But somehow that information has either not made its way into the
world outside the laboratory or has not been presented in a sufficiently defini-
tive manner to affect the general population’s views on the matter (with the
exception of the book of Simon LeVay mentioned in the preceding paragraph
[LeVay, 2010]). Scientists are thus not the only audience for which this book is
intended. This is a book also for laymen, but it contains enough science that
rather than just being told “X is true,” they are shown why it’s true in a way that
I hope will give them a genuine understanding of the subject, not just an opin-
ion. In this respect, this book can thus also be used by scientists and teachers for
their seminars or advanced courses on sexuality. However, if they wish, readers
can skip over the more hardcore science in Chapters 3–4 and still emerge with
plenty of useful knowledge. There’s a glossary at the end that will help those
readers navigate the later chapters.
Of course, presenting a mass of facts without a guiding organization would
result in a mere collection of lab reports, not a book. Some will say this book
advocates a specific position rather than letting readers draw their own conclu-
sions. I arrived at this position because the scientific facts led there. They also
led Simon LeVay to this same conclusion, and that’s why this book leads readers
to the same place.
The Biology of Homosexuality
This page intentionally left blank
1
Sexuality 101
The Basics
one of the behavioral traits most differentiated between men and women that
has been identified.
Regardless of which sex is found interesting or exciting, each human being
is also confident of belonging to one sex, male or female. This conviction is
unchangeable, and it often seems to develop during early childhood. Most
people have a sexual identity that matches their genitalia, although a small
number of people are convinced to the contrary and believe themselves “to be
born in a body that does not correspond to their gender.” They are called trans-
sexuals by sexologists. This gender identity is already apparent in early child-
hood and, contrary to what was thought until recently, it is usually difficult or
impossible to change it later (see the story of John/Joan, Chapter 2). Clinical
data suggest that the organization of sexual identity could be controlled by pre-
natal sex steroids. This idea is difficult to assess, in part because there is no
animal model for studying sexual identity. It is impossible to ask an animal,
whatever its species, to what sex it belongs. In addition to communication dif-
ficulties associated with such an undertaking, this would imply that the animal
is aware of its own body and sex, which is far from proved even though recent
research daily shows new sophisticated cognitive skills not only among pri-
mates but also in species more distant from humans, such as dolphins and even
birds such as parrots and corvids (a family including crows and jays).
The fourth and last dimension of human sexuality concerns the role played
by an individual in society. This gender role is deeply influenced by the struc-
ture of the society in which the individual lives, even if prenatal hormonal
influences also seem to play a role (see Chapter 2). Thus, housework tasks were
typically performed by women in Western Europe until the First World War,
but are now widely shared by men and women. Farm work tasks also are typi-
cally male or female depending on the human group. It would probably be futile
to search for biological bases for this type of sexual difference, even if sociobiol-
ogy suggests that selective pressures acting on our ancestors may have selec-
tively adjusted men and women for various jobs (this idea is broadly contested).
Other aspects of the gender role (play behavior during childhood, hobbies,
etc.), however, may be influenced by prenatal factors.
The four dimensions of human sexuality that I have distinguished are cor-
related and in agreement with each other in the majority of individuals. This
correlation can be expected both on the basis of coordinated learning that
affects these different features and on the basis of a prenatal hormonal envi-
ronment that plays a role in their organization. However, it is important to
distinguish them clearly, because these dimensions may be exceptionally dis-
cordant. In particular, it is critical to distinguish between orientation and gender
identity. A transsexual man who thinks he in fact is a woman may be sexually
attracted by men or by women; sexual identity does not determine sexual
6 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
book so-called primitive societies where homosexual relations are the norm
among teenagers, even encouraged, because access to girls is forbidden before
marriage (Chapter 2). In all these circumstances, it was observed that homo-
sexual behaviors regress or disappear when opposite-sex partners become
available, so there is not preferential homosexual attraction in these cases. The
homosexual activity is simply an outlet for sexual motivation without necessar-
ily being preferred to heterosexual activity.
The preferential or exclusive attraction to partners of the same sex is in con-
trast a largely human characteristic that does not occur frequently in animals
but can be induced by changes in the embryonic hormonal environment or by
injury to a specific area of the brain. Such spontaneous exclusive homosexual
attraction has been identified in certain populations of sheep (Chapter 4). This
is the preferential or exclusive attraction to individuals of the same sex that I am
talking about mainly in this book. I will show that this attraction is dependent,
at least in part, on prenatal biological phenomena that are largely beyond the
will of the concerned individual.
The open display in society of this homosexual attraction is the third and
last aspect to be considered independently. It is clear that this public revelation
can only follow prior personal acceptance of homosexual orientation. Its public
disclosure (“coming out”) is, however, a conscious act that largely depends on
the willingness of the person. This willingness interacts with the greater or
lesser tolerance of those around him or her, which will facilitate or complicate
the process. Many homosexuals, including some very famous ones (to name
a few, writer Gertrude Stein, blues singer Alberta Hunter, and tennis player
Billie Jean King among women; writer Marcel Proust, composer Peter Ilyich
Tchaikovsky, and actor Rock Hudson among men), have historically lived with
their sexual orientation in a more or less hidden condition.
The public disclosure and recognition of homosexuality are largely influ-
enced by social context and the willingness of the individual. However, homo-
sexual orientation develops in a distinctively independent manner from the
will of the individual, under the influence of biological factors that act mostly
during embryonic life. That is the point of this book.
Note, though, that I am well aware that homosexuality (and sexual orienta-
tion in general) is a complex phenomenon that is probably not the result of
a single cause. In addition, male and female homosexuality may have different
explanations, at least in part. Finally, within the same sex, it is likely that differ-
ent reasons or causes, or a combination thereof, can induce and thus explain
the homosexual orientation. I put forward in this book all the work showing
that at least a part of male and female homosexuality has a biological origin
related to endocrine or genetic factors. This does not preclude a potential
contribution of social influences and education, or conscious choices made
8 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
(NHSC) used a self-administered survey and reported that 7.1% of men and
13.6% of women feel sexual attraction for people of their own sex (Mosher
et al., 2005). But only 1.5% of men and 0.7% of women reported exclusively
homosexual attraction (see Figure 1.1). These data could be an underestimate,
but it is likely that they are closer to reality than the estimates of Kinsey and
colleagues, given the greater precautions taken to ensure the objectivity of the
responses of subjects and the representativeness of the analyzed samples.
Despite the biases that are potentially present in all such studies, reported per-
centages of homosexuality in all societies studied mostly vary between 2 and
10% (Chapter 2). While slight variations are observed, these percentages are
relatively similar in two studies conducted more than 50 years apart. The atti-
tude of society toward homosexuals has changed profoundly in this period;
based on that, we would expect an increase in their relative numbers, but the
opposite is observed in all cases for men. This stability obviously suggests ques-
tions about the mechanisms that lead to such a consistency.
The work of Kinsey and colleagues used a seven-point scale to characterize
the sexual orientation of individuals ranging from zero (totally heterosexual) to
six (exclusively homosexual). The latest NHSC research is based on a five-point
scale that seems to provide more reproducible results. As shown in Figure 1.1,
10
1. Opposite sex
% of population
0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Categories
Figure 1.1 Distribution of sexual attraction in the U.S. population of men and
women aged 18 to 44 years (according to data in Mosher et al., 2005). Note that
the vertical axis is interrupted between 10 and 80% (angled lines) in order to permit
visualization of detail in the low range of this axis.
10 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
the vast majority of individuals, for both sexes, has a preferential or exclusive
attraction to individuals of the opposite sex.
It is interesting that the curve describing the distribution of sexual orienta-
tions is different for men and women. For men, there is clearly a bimodal distri-
bution that has two peaks at the extremes (strictly heterosexual and strictly
homosexual), while among women there is instead a gradual decrease of per-
centages when moving from an exclusively heterosexual to exclusively homo-
sexual orientation. Thus, the categories homosexual and heterosexual describe
relatively separated populations in men, while this distinction is more subtle in
women.
Correspondingly, the stability of sexual orientation would be greater in men
than in women. Longitudinal studies show that bisexual women or women with
a nonexclusive preference for one sex more frequently change their sexual ori-
entation than men. Sexual orientation is, however, generally stable over time.
Only a small percentage of subjects change during their lifetime.
Finally, bisexuality, defined as the existence of any degree of attraction for
both sexes, seems to be more common than strict homosexuality. This broad
definition of bisexuality, however, includes a large number of individuals
who are attracted much more to one sex than the other. If we define a more
restrictive bisexuality as an attraction more or less equal to subjects of both
sexes, it is then observed much more rarely in men than in women. Some sex-
ologists even contest that such a bisexuality is present in men. Measures of
genital arousal (tumescence and erection) in men who were presented erotic
pictures of men and women have shown that men who call themselves bisexual
show genital arousal much more for one sex (often men) than the other (Rieger
et al., 2005) and often show excitement exclusively for photos of male subjects
(Freund, 1974) (Rieger et al., 2005). LeVay also notes that the term bisexual is
frequently used by young men who are in the process of becoming aware of and
publicly revealing their homosexual orientation (LeVay & Valente, 2006). Some
studies show that a percentage of homosexuals that can be as high as 40% iden-
tified themselves at an early stage of their young adult life as bisexual before
describing themselves later as homosexual. The frequency of true bisexuality in
men could be substantially overestimated.
Female bisexuality is, in contrast, more widely recognized. At the genital
level, most women are in fact bisexual; when shown erotic videos, their reac-
tions show genital arousal in response to men and women regardless of their
attraction to one sex or the other and their identification as homo-, bi-, or hetero-
sexual (Chivers et al., 2004). These data do not, however, exclude the existence
of exclusive homo- or heterosexuality among women. They only show that the
physiological genital arousal can be (partly) independent of verbal statements
of sexual attraction.
Sexuality 101 11
Finally, one must clearly distinguish between sexual physical attraction and
the desire for emotional intimacy. These two aspects of the human personality
are not always correlated, and persons (usually women) physically attracted by
one sex may fall in love with people of the opposite sex or of both sexes. This
dichotomy obviously complicates the definition of bisexuality. It particularly
affects women in Western cultures, but in other cultures there are also passion-
ate friendships between heterosexual men that do not include any physical
sexual element (Nardi, 1992, in LeVay & Valente, 2006, p. 228).
2
of a girl by a man) and induce a repulsion for members of the same sex as
the perpetrator of the assault: the girl would become disgusted with men and
sexually attracted by women, therefore being a lesbian. Alternatively, the early
experience could be attractive and, in this case, lead to a sexual orientation
toward members of the sex having played an active role in this experiment. One
could imagine a young boy, involved in a pedophile relationship with an adult
male, experiencing pleasure and consequently developing as homosexual and
finding sexual pleasure in the company of men (Churchill, 1967; Cameron
& Cameron, 1995). This theory is similar to the concept of impregnation
(“imprinting”) developed by ethologists, which proposes that the first experi-
ences of a young animal induce lifelong preferences (Lorenz, 1937; Lorenz,
1950).
This interpretation does not, however, take into account a number of obser-
vations. There are many societies where homosexual relations are the rule
among adolescent boys [e.g., some cultures in New Guinea, see (Diamond,
1993)]. This social structure favoring homosexuality in adolescents is imple-
mented in a more or less organized way in order to preserve the virginity of
girls before marriage. The homosexual relationship of young boys with an adult
male would also play an initiatory role. Various anthropological studies have
clearly established that in adulthood, the percentage of men in these societies
who persist in a homosexual orientation is not above the average found in other
societies that forbid juvenile homosexuality. The first sexual experiences of
these young boys do not seem to significantly affect their future sexual orienta-
tion (see Table 2.1) (Diamond, 1993).
Table 2.1: The table lists average percentage of male homosexuality in several
European, North American and Asian countries that differ widely in their atti-
tude toward homosexual orientation. The 3 countries whose name is followed
by an asterisk are very tolerant and even favor adolescent male homosexuality.
14 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
This does not seem to affect the occurrence of homosexual orientation in adult-
hood. (Diamond, 1993).
Furthermore, there is no difference in Western societies between the frequency
of homosexuality among adults who spent their youth in unisex schools (boys
and girls being educated in totally separate locations) or in mixed-sex schools,
although it is well known that voluntary homosexual relations occur more fre-
quently in a unisex education environment. If the early life experiences theory
had some support, we should expect a higher incidence of homosexuality among
adults raised in single-sex schools, which is absolutely not the case (Wellings
et al., 1994).
It should be noted that the percentage of adult gay men is remarkably con-
stant in all human societies despite significant differences in the ease with which
this orientation is openly accepted. An analysis published by Milton Diamond
in 1993 indicated percentages fairly stable between 2 and 8% in societies as
diverse as Great Britain, the Netherlands, and the United States. In addition,
Diamond did not find any obvious relationship between the attitude of the soci-
ety vis-à-vis homosexuality and its incidence (see Table 2.1) (Diamond, 1993).
The percentage of homosexual men is no higher in very tolerant societies such
as the Philippines and Thailand than in societies where it is more difficult to live
with a homosexual orientation (Europe, United States). These comparisons
across different cultures provide percentages of homosexuality comparable to
those reported in the first systematic study of its kind, published by Kinsey (less
than 5–10% of men strictly or almost exclusively homosexual in the United
States at the time) (Kinsey et al., 1948). These estimates are also confirmed by
more recent studies: Mosher and colleagues reported in 2005 that 2 to 3% of
men would be preferentially or exclusively homosexual, while the attitude of
society vis-à-vis homosexuality has become considerably more lenient (Mosher
et al., 2005). This may be anecdotal, but it is intriguing to note that this percent-
age is in the same order of magnitude as what has been reported for homosex-
ual rams (see Chapter 4). One might therefore wonder whether this percentage
might reflect a fairly deterministic mechanism that acts in both animals and
humans and is thus more likely biological in nature than environmental.
PSYCHOANALYTIC THEORIES
On the basis of his psychoanalytic practice, Sigmund Freud developed a theory
of homosexuality stating that the dynamics of child–parent relationships ulti-
mately determine the sexual orientation of adults. For example, a dominant and
possessive mother and/or a distant or absent father might foster a boy develop-
ing a homosexual orientation by disrupting the resolution of the Oedipal phase
of psychosexual development.
Sexuality and Sexual Orientation 15
These Freudian and post-Freudian theories were popular for about a half
a century, but in recent decades many studies have challenged their scientific
validity and even suggested various forms of scientific fraud (from pure lie to
partial concealment of results) from Freud himself, and the Oedipal theory has
been thoroughly discredited for over two decades now on this basis alone, not
to mention that psychoanalysis in general has been described as a nonscientific
theory because it is not falsifiable or refutable (Karl Popper sense). It has a rea-
sonable internal consistency but has systematically refused to consider obser-
vations going against the theory (Van Rillaer, 1980; Bénesteau, 2002; Dufresne,
2007; Onfray, 2010). In American and British scientific circles, it is now largely
ignored, but it must be dealt with here because of its persistence in the culture,
since the beliefs of the general public understandably lag behind science’s
advance.
While some observations seem to support psychoanalytic theory, keep in
mind that “correlation is not causation.” Thus, while retrospective studies have
shown that homosexual men tend to describe their relationship with their
mother as closer than the average and with their father as distant and even hos-
tile (Bell et al., 1981; Freund & Blanchard, 1983), it may be that parental atti-
tudes of indifference or hostility toward a child who is going to become
homosexual are induced by the character traits of the child that do not meet
the expectations of parents. It is in this context important to note that one of
the best predictors of adult homosexuality is the presence during childhood of
gender-nonconformism. Children that will become gay adults conform less
strictly to gender norms. Boys who will become gay engage in less aggressive
play, less typical boys’ activities, and enjoy interacting with girls and engage in
typically female activities. The reverse is true for girls who will later become
lesbians. It is thus quite conceivable that the feminized attitude of a pre-gay boy
is by itself responsible for the more distant relationship with the father. If the
remote or absent attitude of the father were responsible for homosexuality in
boys, one would expect an increased incidence of male homosexuality in chil-
dren raised by single mothers, a situation encountered quite frequently in
Western Europe and the United States during the last 20 to 30 years. However,
not a single study has been able to identify such an effect.
defend the idea that behavior can be entirely innate or entirely acquired. All
behaviors are partly innate and partly acquired by learning; the respective con-
tribution of these two causes varies according to the behavior in question.
Various learning theorists have suggested that sexual orientation could be
the result of conscious and unconscious learning imposed by parents, educa-
tors, and even society in general. However, homosexuality does not appear to
be a desired character trait in any Western society, so heterosexual parents and
educators would not wish to instill homosexuality in their children or students,
and probably are doing all they can to prevent its development. This learning
could only be transmitted by parents/teachers against their own will. Fur-
thermore, we now have enough data to analyze the fate of children raised by
homosexual parents who, we might imagine, might be at least more tolerant
even if they do not encourage the development of a sexual orientation identi-
cal to their own. However, the data currently available suggest that children of
gay parents are generally heterosexual (Stacey & Biblarz, 2001). Similarly, chil-
dren born to lesbian mothers who are raised by a couple of lesbians are in gen-
eral heterosexual. Thus, this behaviorist theory has not received support from
studies in the last 30 years. Conscious attempts to change the gender identity
and sexual orientation of young children also often end in failure (witness the
case of John/Joan also called Bruce/Brenda later in this chapter), and numerous
attempts to change sexual orientation of gay adults have all completely failed.
These attempts included various forms of behavioral therapy and aversive ther-
apy involving the presentation of erotic or pornographic images to homosexu-
als in association with the swallowing of an emetic (a compound inducing
vomiting) or other aversive stimulation (see LeVay, 1996 for more detail).
Medical approaches, as well, attempted to “cure” homosexuality–when it was
still considered a disease–by radical approaches such as castration, injection
of various hormones, electroconvulsive shocks, and even lobotomy (surgical
removal of the frontal lobe). American historian Jonathan Katz counted no less
than 36 methods used during a century in North America to deal with homo-
sexuality (Katz, 1976). No reproducible results could ever be achieved by all
these mutilations or behavioral therapies.
affect this behavior are largely, if not exclusively, the result of learning and social
influences. These statements sometimes claim to be based on findings of
modern neurobiology that have demonstrated a remarkable plasticity in the
structure and function of the human brain during its development and even in
adulthood. However, this is an incomplete understanding of recent results in
neurobiology. There is indeed a great plasticity of the brain during its develop-
ment. The human brain is not fully formed at birth, and only a very small per-
centage of its neurons are mature and connected. During the first few years
of life, the vast majority of the neurons that were present at birth will die; only
a small number of them (one in ten by some estimates) will survive and be
incorporated into functional circuits. The human brain is thus extremely plas-
tic, and this plasticity is guided in part by learning. The adult brain is thus
largely the result of experience gained during development. This does not mean,
however, that behavioral predispositions do not exist at birth that will resist the
potential influence of learning.
Now that the human genome has been sequenced and to some extent inter-
preted, we know that it contains only 25,000 to 30,000 genes [sequences of
deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) directing the synthesis of one or several proteins;
see Chapter 3]. The amount of information contained in these 25,000 genes is far
from sufficient to account for the complexity of the tasks that are performed by
a human subject and thus by his or her brain. For example, it is impossible to
imagine that the different concepts that we master in adulthood–multiplication
tables, to take an extreme example–can be encoded in our genetic material. They
must be learned. But learning is limited to some extent by the underlying neural
structures, and these clearly depend on our genetic heritage. Learning is limited
or even impossible in other species that have different genetic makeup (rats;
even chimpanzees) or in human beings suffering from a genetic disease that has
changed the development of the brain (e.g., some cases of Down syndrome).
Learning takes place in the presence of very specific biological constraints
whose complexity is currently being unraveled. For example, modern linguis-
tics suggests that all human languages tend to use the same sounds to describe
the mother and father (mama, papa, baba), the first two words that are often
made by young children. Some see this as a relic of linguistic structures that
were present in the original language spoken by the first humans. Alternatively,
given that one limiting factor for language development in young children is
the poor motor coordination of the muscles of the larynx, one can also think
that this community of early vocabulary stems, at least in part, from the relative
ease with which these sounds are pronounced. Language in humans is entirely
cultural and learned, but it could be limited, at least partially, by anatomical and
physiological constraints. Alternatively, one might even think that some pho-
nemes of human languages are genetically controlled, even if such a proposal
Sexuality and Sexual Orientation 19
motor disorders are corrected at the same time, clear evidence of their specific
chemical origin.
At a more complex behavioral level, it is well established that drugs that aff-
ect the transmission of information between neurons by another neurotrans-
mitter, serotonin, have reproducible effects on the mood and general state of
well-being of the patient. Several drugs are able to change the transmission
by serotonin and accordingly have major effects on mood. Also note that addic-
tive drugs (opium, cocaine, heroin, alcohol, etc.) are actually chemical com-
pounds extracted from plants or prepared by man that modify the activity of
specific neurotransmitter systems and consequently the perception of the world
and the behavior of consumers (Cooper et al., 1996).
Finally, the most accurate data that now support the notion that the brain
controls our behavior as well as our mental activity comes from sophisticated
studies performed during the last 10 to 20 years thanks to the development of
medical imaging techniques such as positron emission tomography (PET) and
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). These techniques, based on
principles of physics too complex to be explained here, allow the researcher or
the physician to identify brain areas that display, at some point in time, a high
electrical or metabolic activity (Miller, 2008; Op de Beeck et al., 2008). This
information is collected in the form of anatomical images on a computer screen.
It can be obtained from awake patients who must simply remain motionless
with their head placed in the imaging device. It is then possible ask the subject
to perform a specific task or produce a specific mental activity (e.g., imagine
a sequence of words beginning with the same letter, make mental calculations,
pretend to play a specific social role, etc.) and to identify which areas of the
brain are active at that time. By combining the progress of these imaging tech-
niques with sophisticated experimental protocols, researchers have been able to
locate nerve sites controlling subtle mental activities such as handling specific
concepts (numbers, words, etc.) or the expression of emotions (anxiety, disap-
pointment, etc.).
We will see that the brain areas that control sexual behavior of mammals
are identified and their functioning is well understood. These brain areas are
present in humans and are anatomically and functionally very similar to their
counterparts in animals. The understanding of human behavior can thus be
based, to some extent, on the results of experiments conducted on various spe-
cies of mammals and other higher vertebrates.
It should nevertheless be noted that if the brain controls behavior, behavior
can conversely have a massive impact on brain function and even brain structure.
The human brain remains plastic throughout adulthood and is influenced by the
lifestyle of an individual and the behavior he or she expresses. For example, it has
been shown that London taxi drivers have a hippocampus (a brain structure
Sexuality and Sexual Orientation 23
development and on the sexual identity and sexual orientation is slowly finding
its place in general public awareness. Medical doctors, and even sexologists,
often largely underestimate its importance, yet many clinical cases clearly high-
light the permanent effects of the embryonic hormonal milieu. A typical example
of this influence hit the headlines in the late 1990s and made a major contribu-
tion to the initiation of a change in interpretation of sexual development that
I hope will become widespread. Because of its exemplary character and the his-
toric role it played, this clinical case deserves to be exposed in detail here.
Two Canadian monozygotic twins named Bruce and Brian Reimer, born in
the early 1960s, were affected at the age of seven months by phimosis, a benign
problem in which the glans penis is stuck in the foreskin. Doctors advised the
parents to circumcise the children, but during the operation, the penis of Bruce,
one of two boys, was completely destroyed by electrocautery. Local doctors
indicated to the parents that the mutilated child would be unable in adulthood
to have a normal sex life and would suffer from social maladjustment. The par-
ents, who were obviously devastated by this prediction, consulted Professor
John Money at Johns Hopkins University to help them decide how to manage
this problem.
Because it was impossible at that time to reconstruct by surgery a penis for
the boy, Money advised that Bruce be transformed into a girl and raised as such.
Indeed, it was thought at the time that if a child were raised in an unambiguous
way as a girl, he would develop into a normal heterosexual woman in adult-
hood (Money & Ehrhardt, 1972). From that time Bruce was thus called Brenda
and was dressed and treated like a girl. At the age of two years, the sex change
was completed by a medical intervention. The child was surgically castrated
and a rudimentary vagina was constructed from the skin of the scrotum.
Brenda/Bruce and her/his brother Brian were raised as a brother and sister and
monitored periodically by Professor Money, who for years advised parents on
what to do to optimize the feminization of Brenda/Bruce.
In the years that followed, scientific reports and presentations at conferences
by Professor Money showed that the sex change was a complete success and
that Brenda (Bruce) was developing as a normal girl, apart from the fact that
she/he was slightly masculine in terms of playing activity (a feature of gender
role). According to Money, Brenda/Bruce was copying her/his activities, includ-
ing domestic activities, from her/his mother, while her/his brother Brian was
imitating his father’s activities. Brenda/Bruce chose dolls as a gift and Brian
chose cars (LeVay, 1996). This case thus supported the contention that culture
has a decisive influence on sexual identity and gender role and denied the
importance of genetics and biology on human behavior. Sexologists were con-
vinced and claimed that babies are born neutral from the sexual point of view
and if you take them early enough and you’ll make girls or boys.
Sexuality and Sexual Orientation 25
Money then lost contact with the family of Brian and Brenda/Bruce. Several
decades later (in the 1990s), biologist and sexologist Milton Diamond (University
of Hawaii) and his colleague Keith Sigmundson were able to discover what had
happened to Brenda/Bruce during late childhood and puberty (Diamond &
Sigmundson, 1997). The child was in fact never socialized as a normal girl and
he rebelled early. Brenda/Bruce was refusing to wear feminine clothing, uri-
nated standing up, and felt permanently that there was something wrong.
A “female puberty” was induced by treatment with estrogen, but Brenda/
Bruce hated her/his breasts and refused the hormone. At the age of fourteen, he/
she insisted on knowing the truth, which his/her parents reluctantly revealed.
He/she was relieved to finally understand the root of the conflicting feelings he/
she experienced. At the age of 15 years, Brenda/Bruce asked people to call him/
her David and he went through additional surgery to recover a masculine iden-
tity. He underwent a double mastectomy (surgical removal of breasts) and a
phalloplasty (surgical reconstruction of a penis). He was also treated with
testosterone. He had always been exclusively attracted to women (sexual orien-
tation in agreement with genetic and hormonal sex during embryonic develop-
ment, not with sex of education during childhood) and, in adulthood, he
married a woman with whom he had male-typical sex with his prosthesis. He
also adopted children and from that time on lived a relatively normal life as
husband and father. Unfortunately he committed suicide at the age of 38 years,
for reasons that remain unknown but probably include the breakdown of his
marriage, financial difficulties, and the premature death of his brother Brian.
His traumatic childhood experiences may have also contributed to this event.
Bruce/Brenda/David Reimer was given the pseudonym of John/Joan in the
work of John Money. Before his death in 2004, however, David wanted to make
his life experience public so as to prevent such errors from recurring in the
future. His story is told in a book written by John Colapinto, As Nature Made
Him: The Boy That Was Raised as a Girl (New York, Harper Collins, 2000). This
story illustrates the dramatic importance of how our society has dealt and still
deals with issues related to sexual identity and sexual orientation while ignor-
ing the power of hormonal influences.
This case suggests a conclusion very different from what was widely accepted
by Money and his contemporaries in the 1960s. It provides evidence that human
beings are not psycho-socially neutral at birth. In keeping with their legacy as
mammals, they have trends and predispositions to react according to a male or
female pattern. According to Diamond, sexual identity is influenced by social
factors but also by the embryonic hormonal environment. John/Joan had obvi-
ously been exposed in utero to androgen levels typical of a young boy (his penis
was normally formed at birth). In this case, the hormonal factors would have
outweighed the social influences in determining identity and gender role.
26 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
A similar case of penile ablation has been reported (Bradley et al., 1998). This
genetically male (XY) subject accidentally lost his penis at the age of two months
and was raised as female from the age of seven months on. In adulthood, con-
trary to John/Joan, he adopted a female sex identity despite the fact that during
childhood he enjoyed stereotypically masculine toys and games (partially mas-
culinized sex role). His sexual orientation was essentially bisexual. He was pre-
dominantly but not exclusively attracted by women. The role of education as
compared to embryonic hormones was thus apparently more prominent in this
case than in the patient studied by John Money, possibly because the sex of
rearing (as female) was adopted earlier.
It should also be noted that these cases of penile ablation are not isolated and
that the conclusions they lead to are reinforced by the analysis of many other
clinical conditions. This is particularly true of males suffering from cloacal
exstrophy (Chapter 9), a malformation in which external genitalia do not grow
during embryonic development. Although “corrected” surgically and raised as
girls, many of these subjects have male characteristics and nearly half of them
identified as boys and later men.
3
BEHAVIORAL ENDOCRINOLOGY
In this chapter I present a brief review of a mass of data widely accepted in the
scientific world concerning the neuroendocrinology of behavior [for a more
detailed discussion, interested readers may refer to Becker and colleagues (2002;)
and Nelson (2005)]. Although I present a dense array of technical points, I encour-
age nonscientists to concentrate on general principles rather than trying to under-
stand every single term. In this context it is less important to know exactly what
17β-estradiol and 5α-dihydrotestosterone are and more important to know what
they do.
In this section I describe what is known concerning the endocrine control of
copulatory behavior in males and females. The elements of hormonal control of
these behaviors need not be the same as those of sexual orientation, but recent
research has shown that they frequently do overlap. Since much more research
has been done concerning copulatory behavior itself, it is clearly useful to fi rst
review this huge corpus of data. I then consider what is known specifically
about the control of sexual orientation in animals.
From a reductionist point of view, most behaviors can be considered as a series
of muscle contractions triggered by a series of organized nerve impulses. To acti-
vate a behavioral pattern, a hormone should thus alter the electrical activity in
28 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
CH3
CH3 CH3
O OH
Cholesterol Pregnenolone
CH3
CH3 C=O
CH3
Progesterone
OH
CH3
CH3
Testosterone
Aromatase 5α-reductase
OH OH
CH3 CH3
CH3
HO O
H
Estradiol 5α-Dihydrotestosterone
Figure 3.1 Simplified illustration of metabolic pathways for the synthesis of sex steroids
from cholesterol.
within neurons. The mechanisms involved remain poorly understood and are
currently the subject of a lot of research, namely in our laboratory (see, for
example, McEwen & Alves, 1999; Balthazart & Ball, 2006; Cornil et al., 2006;
Taziaux et al., 2007; Balthazart et al., 2009; Vasudevan & Pfaff, 2008; Micevych
& Dominguez, 2009). I shall not discuss them in this volume, but it is neverthe-
less important to bear in mind that steroids are probably able to produce rapid
effects on behavior with latencies of about a minute or even a second.
Nucleus of the
Cortex stria terminalis
Olfactory
bulbs Optic tectum
Septum Cerebellum
Amygdala Preoptic
area Hypothalamus
Figure 3.2 Schematic representation of the binding sites of sex steroids in the brain of
vertebrates (according to Morrell & Pfaff, 1978).
The Hormonal Control of Sexual Behavior 33
TESTOSTERONE
Aromatase 5α-Reductase
Estradiol 5α-Dihydrotestosterone
Estrogen Androgen
receptors receptors
Modification of
transcription
New mRNA
New proteins
Changes in
synaptic transmission
MALE SEXUAL
BEHAVIOR
Figure 3.3 Schematic illustration of hormonal mechanisms at the cellular level that
activate the expression of male sexual behavior in vertebrates.
Non Differentiated
(6 weeks)
Gonad
Wolffian duct
Müllerian duct
Urogenital sinus
Genital tubercle
Genital folds
Testicle Ovary
Deferent Fallopian
duct tube
Uterus
Bladder
Wolffian duct
Müllerian duct (atrophied)
(atrophied) Bladder
Glans penis
Clitoris
Male Female
(8 weeks) (8 weeks)
Masculinization (Demasculinization)
(Defeminization) Feminization
6 weeks
Genital tubercle
Genital
swellings
Genital folds
Anus
Boys and girls
2nd trimester
Boys Girls
Birth
Clitoris
Glans penis
Uretral opening
Penis Outer labia
Testicles Inner labia
Scrotum Vaginal opening
Figure 3.5 Embryonic development and sexual differentiation of the external genitalia
in humans. A single genital outline is present at six weeks after conception. Under the
influence of testicular testosterone and its metabolite, 5α-dihydrotestosterone, the genital
folds fuse to form the scrotum and the genital tubercle develops in the penis. Without
these hormones, genital folds form the labia of the vulva and genital tubercle forms the
clitoris.
In a first step during the early embryonic development, the SRY gene is acti-
vated and its transient expression in the gonads results in the synthesis of TDF
protein in specific cells that are the future Sertoli cells. This TDF protein stimu-
lates, directly or indirectly, the expression of many genes (organizers or architect
genes) that lead to the differentiation of the undifferentiated gonad into testis.
There is no SRY gene on chromosome X and, therefore, in the absence of the
protein TDF, the primordial gonadal tissue will not produce a testis. In humans,
differentiation of the female ovaries, however, is a complex process that is still
not fully understood and involves additional control factors.
The Hormonal Control of Sexual Behavior 39
During the second stage, specialized cells of the differentiated testis will
begin to secrete two chemical messengers, the testicular hormones, which will
spread in the whole body to organize it into a male individual (man). The inter-
stitial cells (Leydig cells) secrete testosterone, which determines the genitals’
form [fusion of the lips (labia) of the vulva to form a scrotum and develop-
ment of the genital tubercle to form a penis (see Fig. 3.5)] and maintenance of
Wolffian ducts (which form the epididymis, the vas deferens, and seminal vesi-
cles). Testosterone is also implicated in the development and organization in a
male direction of all physical and behavioral characters that are sexually dif-
ferentiated. Another type of testicular cell (Sertoli cells) in parallel will produce
anti-Müllerian hormone, which induces the regression of the Müllerian ducts.
The establishment of the male sex phenotype is therefore under the action of
two testicular hormones, testosterone and anti-Müllerian hormone.
In females, the gonads become ovaries. The absence of testicular hormones
is responsible for the persistence of the Müllerian ducts, disappearance of the
Wolffian ducts, and development of external female genitalia (labia majora and
minora, clitoris, feminization of the genitalia). The development of the female
phenotype is for the most part spontaneous. It occurs in the absence of SRY and
hence in the absence of testosterone and anti-Müllerian hormone. The female
sex is considered the “default” sex, since it develops in the absence of a specific
genetic or hormonal influence.
In summary, sexual differentiation takes place in two stages, a first leading
from genetic sex to gonadal sex and a second during which the gonadal sex
induces, by hormonal influences, the phenotypic sex differences. At birth, the
sexual phenotype is visible in many mammals, including humans. The sexual
organs are differentiated, but these organs are not functional. It is only at puberty
that the hormones (testosterone in males and estradiol/progesterone in the
females), secreted by the gonads in increasing quantities, will take control of the
reproductive function of typical adult. These hormones then control the final
maturation of the reproductive tract, the appearance of secondary sexual char-
acteristics, and sexual behavior.
EMBRYONIC CX + T or E2
TREATMENTS (-T)
Adult female
Adult male
Pseudo-male Pseudo-female
Female behavior
After treatment with E2/P − − +++ +++
Male behavior
After treatment with T +++ +++ + +
Figure 3.6 Schematic representation of the hormonal processes during ontogenesis
that induce sexual differentiation of brain and behavior. CX: castration; T:
testosterone; E2: estradiol; P: progesterone. + + + Behavior regularly
expressed; + rare behavior; – behavior absent.
The Hormonal Control of Sexual Behavior 41
the brain to sexual differentiation is also variable from one species to another,
and, in monkeys, for example, androgens themselves seem more important
than estrogens. Extrapolation to humans of the mechanisms described on the
basis of studies in animals should thus be made with due caution and still
requires verifications that must be based on clinical or epidemiological studies.
However, the main principles of control of sexual differentiation deducted from
animal studies appear to be applicable to humans (see Chapter 6). Differences
between species lie in the details.
and Art Arnold discovered that several components of this neural circuit are
affected by a major physical sex difference (Nottebohm & Arnold, 1976). In par-
ticular, three groups of neurons of the telencephalon—HVC (formerly known as
the high vocal center), RA (the robust nucleus of the arcopallium), and area X of
the medial striatum—have a volume two to five times larger in males than in
females, depending on the species considered. The difference in size is so great
that it can be seen with the naked eye in a brain section stained by appropriate
methods.
The discovery of a large neuroanatomical sex difference in songbirds led
various researchers to reconsider the idea that such differences did not exist in
mammals. In particular, the group of Professor Roger Gorski (University of
California at Los Angeles) re-analyzed the structure of the preoptic area in male
and female rats. He discovered the existence of a sexually dimorphic nucleus
(SDN) in the preoptic area of this species (Gorski et al., 1978). This difference is
also very important: the SDN of the male rat is five times larger than the SDN
of females, and this difference may also be seen with the naked eye on a brain
section prepared for microscopic examination (see Figure 3.7).
The location of this nucleus in the middle of the preoptic area—which is,
remember, the area of the brain involved in a privileged way in the activation by
100
Volume of SDN (% of control males)
80
60
40
20
0
Ctrl CX CX Ctrl T E2 DES
(d1) +T
Males Females
Figure 3.7 In rats, the volume of the sexually dimorphic nucleus (SDN) of the preopic
area is about five times larger in males than in females—a difference induced by steroids
during the perinatal life. Castration (CX) during the first day after birth (d1) reduces
nucleus size in males, and this may be offset by treatment with testosterone (T). Perinatal
treatment of females with testosterone, estradiol (E2), or a synthetic estrogen (DES)
increases the volume of this nucleus, but these organizing effects will only be observed
later in adulthood (according to Gorski, 1984).
The Hormonal Control of Sexual Behavior 45
area of many other species of mammals, including humans (see Chapter 4),
birds, reptiles, and amphibians. These dimorphic structures of the preoptic area
are apparently not homologous in different species and are not necessarily con-
trolled by the same hormonal mechanisms. The larger size in males of the preop-
tic SDN is indeed, in some species such as adult Japanese quail, the result of the
presence of a higher concentration of testosterone in males than in females. In
technical terms, this difference is linked to a different activation in adulthood by
steroids rather than to a differential organization during early life. It seems that
in most mammals studied, the sexual dimorphism in the size of the SDN of the
preoptic area is the result of a differentiation by sex hormones during early life,
either embryonic or immediately postnatal.
Finally, following this identification of an SDN in the preoptic area in many
species, neuroanatomists have analyzed the possible existence of physical dif-
ferences between other parts of the brains of males and females. They have
identified a variety of structures that are either larger in males than in females
(nucleus of the stria terminalis in rats) or in females than in males (ventro-
medial nucleus of the hypothalamus). I return to this topic in more detail when
analyzing the physical differences between the brains of men and women
(Chapter 6).
4
easily studied in animals. The test animal can be offered a choice between
a male or female sexual partner, and the observer can record toward which
of these partners the test animal orients its sexual behavior. This type of research
is not as developed as the research on sexual behavior in the stricter sense, and
it was started more recently. However, two important principles are already
firmly established: (1) that the sexual orientation of reproductive behavior is
controlled, both in adulthood and during its development from conception
through maturation, by the same hormones that control sexual behavior, and
(2) that these hormones act in the same brain regions to activate sexual behav-
ior and determine its orientation. I summarize these data in the following
pages as they establish the theoretical context from which I propose thereafter
an explanatory model of human homosexuality based on early (embryonic)
and irreversible effects of sex steroids.
A
?
B Towards
100 male
75
Percentage of tests
75
MALES FEMALES
100 Towards
female
If we carry out a bilateral electrolytic lesion of the preoptic area at the level
of this dimorphic nucleus, males who previously spent most of their time in
the room of the three-compartment unit containing a female have a nearly
complete reversal of this choice and during the postlesional tests spend most
of their time in the chamber containing a stimulus male (Paredes & Baum,
1995). They therefore show a preference that in humans might be described as
homosexual.
This effect of lesions of the preoptic area is anatomically specific, as demon-
strated by the experimental subjects in which the lesion more or less missed
the target. Indeed, carriers of a unilateral lesion of the preoptic area (the con-
tralateral lesion missed the target) continue to show a preference for the com-
partment containing a sexually receptive female. The same is true for males in
whom the lesion has completely missed the preoptic area on a bilateral basis.
Only bilateral lesions placed exactly in the preoptic area are able to reverse the
sexual orientation of males. Similar results were observed in male rats (Paredes
et al., 1998).
Fewer data are available for female sexual preferences although one study
showed that lesion of the ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus (a struc-
ture that controls female receptivity) decreases the interest of female ferrets for
male olfactory signals (Robarts & Baum, 2007).
mate with sexually receptive females that are present and, even more tellingly,
allow the male stimulus to mount them. They are thus showing a form of sexual
preference that, in humans, would be qualified as homosexuality or at least
bisexuality. In addition, these rats have, as adults, a small SDN in the preoptic
area (see previous chapter), which is characteristic of the female (Houtsmuller
et al., 1994). It therefore appears that absence of estrogen exposure in male rats
during the perinatal life has a lasting effect on the sexual preferences of the male
in adulthood. These rats are not masculinized and tend to prefer other males to
females.
Recent studies confirm this interpretation by showing that the treatment
(during the first three weeks of life) of young female rats with estrogen (estra-
diol benzoate) has the opposite effect. This treatment increases their preference
for females, a preference that would be classified as homosexual in women
(Figure 4.2) (Henley et al., 2009) (see Henley et al., 2011 for review). These pref-
erence tests were conducted at the adult age in ovariectomized females sub-
jected to various hormone treatments (estrogen alone or estrogen plus
progesterone) to activate different aspects of sexual behavior. The preference
reversal was observed in both hormonal conditions, thus suggesting that
“homosexual” preferences induced by perinatal treatment cannot be modified
by hormones in adulthood.
Towards female
300
(female-male)
200
100
Control
0
EB EB
−100 high low
−200
Towards male
Figure 4.2 Effect of treatment with an estrogen, estradiol benzoate (EB), at high
or low dose, during the first three weeks of life on the sexual preferences of female
rats. The preference score represents the time spent by the animal in the test chamber
containing a female minus the time spent in the chamber containing a male. A negative
score indicates a preference for males (usually observed in female controls). A positive
score indicates a reversal of this choice (preference for female) following treatment
with EB at both doses used (according to Henley et al., 2009).
52 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
maternal estrogens during embryonic life, AFPKO (Bakker et al., 2006)] simi-
larly reveal a major impact of embryonic hormonal alterations on the sexual
preferences of adult subjects. Because these altered preferences cannot usually
be modified by adult endocrine treatments, it is assumed that they derive from
organizational effects of steroids that took place in early life, even if this conclu-
sion is often indirect and has not been formally demonstrated.
In a long series of experiments, Julie Bakker and collaborators analyzed the
preferences displayed by male and female ArKO or AFPKO mice when pre-
sented with the opportunity to investigate body odors or cage bedding soiled
by male or female stimuli. Studies with ArKO mice indicated that the develop-
ment of sexual preferences in influenced by estrogens. Gonadally intact male
ArKO mice (contrary to wild-type males) failed to show a preference for an
estrous female versus a sexually active male when asked to discriminate between
these stimuli based on volatile body odors (Bakker et al., 2002a). Accordingly,
AFPKO females that are not protected from maternal estrogens during embry-
onic life display defeminized sexual behavior and partner preferences. They are
unable to display lordosis behavior in adulthood (Bakker et al., 2006) and they
show, contrary to wild-type females, a robust preference for an estrous female
over an intact male when choosing between these stimuli based on volatile
body odors (Bakker & Brock, 2010; Brock & Bakker, 2011). Together these data
thus point to an important role of estrogens in the development of partner pref-
erence in mice. The relative role of estrogens (this section) vs. androgen (previ-
ous section) in the organization of these sexually differentiated characteristics
remains to be established.
Together these findings thus support the idea that sexual preferences in
mammals are controlled during ontogeny by the action of sex steroids, mainly
estrogenic metabolites of testosterone in rats, possibly androgens and estrogens
in mice. They are in agreement with clinical and correlative data suggesting the
same conclusion in humans (see Chapters 8 and 9).
Finally, it is important to emphasize that these early hormonal manipulations
seem to have absolutely irreversible effects on both the type of sexual behavior
to be made in adulthood (male or female typical) and its orientation (homo- vs.
heterosexual). So far, we have failed to find an experimental manipulation that
would reverse in adulthood these behavioral characteristics induced by the hor-
monal milieu during embryonic or immediately postnatal life. In particular, the
hormones secreted by adults are totally unable to change the type of sexual
behavior expressed by an individual (male or female) and its orientation (homo-
sexual or heterosexual). These animal studies play a critical role when we con-
sider the potential hormonal basis of human homosexuality.
Most mammalian species, and in particular all those species that have been
investigated in order to understand the endocrine controls of sexual preferences
54 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
(rats, mice, ferrets, sheep), do not form stable pair bonds. This lack of stable
bonds obviously prevents us from investigating this important aspect of sexual
preference. In contrast, the majority of avian species form intersexual bonds
that last either for one reproductive season or sometimes for the entire life.
A highly relevant body of research has been carried out on the development
of these sexual bonds in one songbird species, the zebra finch (Taeniopygia
guttata) (summarized in Adkins-Regan, 2011). Zebra finches breed colonially
but form socially monogamous heterosexual pairs at a young age that usually
last until one member of the pair dies. Multiple behaviors that can be easily
recorded attest the presence of this pair bond, such as clumping (perching in
close contact), allopreening (preening each other), and spending time in a nest
box together. These behaviors are observed almost exclusively within hetero-
sexual pairs. Homosexual pairs do not occur in the wild and do so only rarely
in captivity.
Although social context during rearing does affect partner preferences, these
behaviors also seem to be controlled to a large extent by the action of steroids
during development (organizational effect) but not in adulthood. Adult castra-
tion or treatment with exogenous androgens or estrogens will eventually mod-
ulate the expression of the behaviors indicative of pair bonding (decrease and
activation of the behaviors, respectively) but will never modify their direction
(toward a male or a female). The heterosexual direction of the sexual partner
preference in zebra finches does not depend on the action of sex steroids in
adulthood.
In contrast, if young females are treated with estrogens during the first two
weeks posthatch, they will prefer other females over males in two-choice prox-
imity tests and will pair with females in mixed-sex aviaries where they have
ample choice of partners (Adkins-Regan & Ascenzi, 1987; Mansukhani et al.,
1996; Adkins-Regan, 1999). It is quite interesting that this “homosexual” part-
ner preference only develops if the estrogen-treated females are housed in all-
female aviaries during their development. Neither all-female housing alone nor
estrogen treatment alone produce a significant modification of the sexual part-
ner preference. This finding obviously raises the question of what is actually
modified by the treatment with estrogens. It has been speculated that the steroid
masculinizes the way in which females learn (or become imprinted on) the sex
of their partner, but additional work would be need to resolve this question.
These experiments therefore suggest that, in birds also, the action of steroids
during development modify sexual preferences in a long-lasting manner.
Interpretation of the results is complicated, however, by a number problems,
including the facts (1) that the estrogen treatment that was used has been shown
to display some toxicity and (2) that different treatments with aromatase inhibi-
tor, which should decrease estrogen production and thus have an effect opposite
to that of treatments increasing estrogen concentrations, also resulted in a shift
Biological Determinism of Sexual Orientation in Animals 55
from opposite-sex to same-sex preference in females and did not affect partner
preferences in males (Adkins-Regan et al., 1996; Adkins-Regan & Wade, 2001).
Similar paradoxical results have been previously reported during the experi-
mental analysis of the sexual differentiation of singing activity and of the song
control circuits in the zebra finch brain. Mechanisms mediating the sexual
differentiation of brain and behavior in this species thus remain quite mysteri-
ous and clearly involve genetic effects that are not mediated by changes in
gonadal steroid production (the so called “direct genetic effects,” (Arnold et al.,
2004; Arnold & Chen, 2009). More work will be needed on this topic but for the
purpose of the present discussion, it should be noted that steroid action during
ontogeny is able to affect the direction of partner preference in a long-lasting
manner and the type of social bond that will be displayed by adult birds (see
Adkins-Regan, 2011 for more details).
In conclusion, in animals that were studied, sexual orientation differentiates
during ontogenesis under the influence of the same hormonal stimuli that dif-
ferentiate the expression of motor behavior patterns. The embryonic or neona-
tal hormones determine not only the type of behavior that will be present in
adulthood but also the subjects’ homosexual or heterosexual orientation.
Although social factors such as education and social environment must also
potentially be taken into account, it seems that the biological (hormonal) fac-
tors described in animals also contribute to the determinism of sexual orienta-
tion in humans (see Chapters 8 and 9).
HOMOSEXUAL SHEEP
All the studies mentioned above describe animals that have certain aspects
of what in humans is called a homosexual orientation. In all cases, however,
note that it is almost never an exclusively homosexual orientation, only a homo-
sexual preference in animals that are essentially bisexual. In addition, all animal
models described so far relate to homosexual behavior induced experimentally,
as opposed to behaviors that occur spontaneously. Over the past ten years,
a spontaneous model of exclusive homosexuality has been described in sheep
and is considered in detail here.
In studies of sexual behavior of male sheep, ethologists had early realized
that the sexual capacity of rams is very variable. When presented with sexually
receptive females, some males are capable of achieving as many as five or six
ejaculations during a period of 30 minutes, while others are much less active or
completely inactive in the presence of females (Resko et al., 1996; Pinckard
et al., 2000). This sexual ability is obviously the object of special attention from
farmers, as it makes some males much more productive breeding stock than
others. Hundreds of males are studied each year in behavioral tests and, consis-
tently, approximately 20% of rams do not qualify to be kept as breeding subjects
in animal husbandry.
During more precise testing, it was noted that a significant fraction of these
males with little or no reaction in the presence of females are in fact not com-
pletely asexual but have active sexual behavior if they are given another ram
as sexual stimulus. During a quantitative study carried out on more than 700
rams, 51% of subjects offered a choice between a male or female partner
oriented their behavior exclusively toward a female (male-oriented toward
female; MOF), 31% were bisexual, 10% were asexual (did not present any
behavior), and 8% oriented their sexual behavior exclusively toward another
male (male-oriented male; MOM). The intensity of motivation and sexual per-
formance among male-oriented males is not in question. It is specifically the
focus of this behavior that is atypical.
Extensive studies were then carried out to identify the potential causes of this
reversed sexual orientation (see Roselli et al., 2011 for review). It was first sug-
gested that same-sex rearing, which is common in sheep, might promote the
development of these MOM sheep. Comparisons between rams raised in mixed-
sex groups and rams raised in all-male groups revealed small differences in
young adults in the rate of mounting and ejaculations with estrous ewes, but
most rams in both groups later developed a heterosexual mate preference. Also,
a search by several independent groups for genetic determinations of sexual ori-
entation did not provide any conclusive results and selection for reproduction in
ewes did not affect sexual behavior or sexual orientation in male offspring.
58 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
Another idea put forward was that a differential sexual attraction might be
related to the ability to process sensory, in particular olfactory, cues coming
from the partner, and some limited support was obtained for this notion.
Differential endocrine responses (increases in testosterone and/or luteinizing
hormone concentrations) were detected following exposure to ewes in MOF
and MOM (Perkins & Fitzgerald, 1992; Perkins et al., 1992) but some aspects of
these studies make them difficult to interpret [see Roselli et al., (2011) for dis-
cussion]. Furthermore, brain activation, as measured by the induction of the
protein product of the immediate early gene fos, was more intense in the medial
preoptic area of MOF than of MOM, but this was observed after exposure to
stimulus ewes as well as rams (Alexander et al., 1999). The difference observed
might therefore relate to sexual orientation, as well as to an overall difference in
brain activity. Together, available results indicate that none of these mechanisms
are mutually exclusive, but none of them have been demonstrated conclusively
to play a significant role. Attention then focused on potential differences in the
preoptic area that could relate to sexual orientation.
Anatomical studies identified the existence of a sexually dimorphic nucleus
in the preoptic area of sheep similar to that observed in rats. The sexually
dimorphic nucleus of the sheep preoptic area (ovine SDN or oSDN) is approxi-
mately three times larger in males than in females (Figure 4.3) (Roselli et al.,
2004b). The male oSDN has also about four times more neurons than that of
females. It is interesting that the oSDN of the male-oriented male is signifi-
cantly smaller than in the males oriented to females and contains significantly
fewer neurons (Roselli et al., 2004a). It has a structure similar to that observed
in females, with which male-oriented males share the same sexual orientation.
The sheep SDN is also characterized by a dense expression of aromatase.
Quantitative studies of this enzyme in the preoptic area indicate that aromatase
activity is higher in rams than in ewes, just as observed in rodents. In addition,
aromatase activity in the preoptic area was significantly lower in male-oriented
males than in males oriented to females, thus making them more similar to
females (Roselli et al., 2004b). The volume of oSDN can also be measured by
quantifying, in successive sections, the surface of dense expression of aromatase
mRNA. This confirmed measurements of the oSDN obtained by conventional
histological stains indicating that oSDN is larger in MOF than in females and
MOM have an oSDN smaller than MOF.
These studies indicate that several characteristics of the preoptic area of rams
(volume and number of neurons in the SDN, aromatase activity in the preoptic
area) are correlated with sexual orientation (Perkins & Roselli, 2007). This cor-
relation raises again the problem of the nature and direction of causal links
between the volume and structure of oSDN and sexual behavior of rams. Does
the oSDN control sexual orientation or does the orientation determine the size
Biological Determinism of Sexual Orientation in Animals 59
0.5
A
0.4
0.3
0.2
ovine SDN volume (mm3)
0.1
0
MOF Fem. MOM
1.00
B
0.75
0.50
0.25
0
Male Fem. Male Fem.
+T +T
Figure 4.3 Volume of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (oSDN) in
sheep. A. oSDN volume is larger in males directed toward females (MOF) than among
females (Fem.), but the male-oriented males (MOM) have a volume similar to that of
females. B. Effect of treatment with testosterone between days 30 and 90 of gestation on
the volume of oSDN measured at 135 days of gestation (birth at approximately 150 days).
A significant increase in the volume of oSDN is observed among females but the same
treatment has no effect in males [according to Roselli et al., (2004b) and Roselli et al.
(2007)].
of the nucleus? The analysis of the hormonal mechanisms that control the
development of the nucleus provides critical information on this topic.
If rams and ewes are gonadectomized in adulthood (castration of males and
ovariectomy of females) and, after a month without hormone, all subjects are
treated for three weeks with testosterone, at autopsy it can be shown that the
size of the oSDN has not been changed by the manipulations. (The MOF oSDN
remains about twice as large as in females and MOM SDN remains significantly
smaller than that of MOF and thus similar to that of females.) These hormonal
manipulations performed in adulthood suggest that the differentiation affect-
ing the volume of oSDN precedes the establishment of sexual behavior and
potentially its orientation. This idea has been confirmed by the analysis of the
size of oSDN in embryos of sheep (Roselli et al., 2007). During the embryonic
life of the sheep, which lasts about 150 days, between the 50th and 100th days
60 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
BIOLOGY OR EDUCATION?
It is clear that the primary physical sex differences are due almost exclusively
to early hormonal influences. As noted in the previous section, the fusion of
genital folds into a scrotum and the development of the genital tubercle in a
penis are induced in the animal embryo (rats, mice) by the action of testoster-
one, which must be transformed into dihydrotestosterone (DHT) by the action
of 5α-reductase to exert these physical effects. The same is true in humans
(see Chapter 6). Well-known disturbances of these processes, observed either in
individuals with androgen insensitivity (due to a mutation in the receptor to
testosterone and DHT) or in subjects deficient in 5α-reductase, demonstrate the
Gender Differences in Humans 63
indicating that gender differences in math skills have greatly reduced or can-
celed, or even reversed, during the last 20 to 30 years, which have also seen an
increase in girls’ access to higher education in Western countries (Guiso et al.,
2008; Hyde et al., 2008). The intercultural comparison also shows that the more
societies treat boys and girls equally, particularly in education, the more the
difference in math ability is reduced (Guiso et al., 2008). Quite unexpectedly,
however, this more equal treatment for boys and girls increases the difference
in reading ability that already favored girls. It seems that girls have an inher-
ently higher ability to read than boys do, but that difference is partly hidden in
societies where girls are less educated than boys and is fully revealed when we
offer girls an equal opportunity to develop their capacities (Guiso et al., 2008).
It must be pointed out that the magnitude of these differences is generally
very low and there is an overlap between the performances of individuals of
both sexes. Statisticians have developed a tool to quantify and compare differ-
ences between populations. This measure is the effect size. Consider the distri-
bution of scores (see Figure 5.1) obtained by a group of men (light gray) and
women (dark gray) on a psychological test measuring a sexually differentiated
behavior or any physical variable in both sexes. The magnitude of sex differ-
ences involved is very different in the two cases that are illustrated. The size of
the effect is simply the ratio of the difference between mean values observed in
both groups (DM, DM’) to the common standard deviation of samples (SD,
SD’), a measure of the average variance within these groups.
The difference between averages (DM) is relatively important in the case
illustrated on the left, while this difference is small on the right (DM’). The vari-
ability between subjects within the same group remains more or less the same
(SD = ± SD’). The size of an effect is especially important when the ratio of DM
to SD or DM’ to SD’ is large. The size of the effect is considered large if greater
than 0.8, moderate for values between 0.8 and 0.2, and small or negligible for
lower values (< 0.2). When the size of an effect is large (left figure), the score of
DM DM’
% individuals
% individuals
Jack Jack
Lucy
Lucy Mary
John Mary John
SD SD’
an individual can predict his or her sex with a good probability (Lucy and Mary
have scores higher than Jack and John), but this is no longer the case for small
effect sizes. On the right side of the figure, John has a higher score than Lucy for
the variable concerned, while men have an average score lower than women.
The effect size of “sex differences” for many physical variables is large (> 0.8).
This is true for measures such as the height of an individual but also for neuro-
anatomical differences such as that affecting the volume of the dimorphic
nucleus of the preoptic area. This nucleus is indeed about two times larger in
men than in women (Swaab & Fliers, 1985). In contrast, the differences in cog-
nitive abilities (e.g., verbal overall capacity) are often more limited and effect
sizes very often described as low to negligible (< 0.2). There are exceptions. For
example, mental rotation skill of three-dimensional objects is associated with a
sex difference that is considered large (> 0.8, see Figure 5.2).
Many differences in cognitive skills (e.g., mathematical reasoning, verbal
ability, spatial perception) have a low amplitude, even if they are reproducible.
They can be identified by statistics in a relatively large population, but the over-
lap of distribution curves is large, so it is not possible to determine the sex of an
individual from his score on one of these tests. In everyday life, there is no
reason to tolerate any discrimination between the sexes for such tasks. Even if
a job requires a skill known to be sexually differentiated, and for which men are
Men>Women
2.0
1. Height
2. Physical aggression
3. Tridimensional rotations
1.5 4. Math problems
5. Toys choice
6. Aggressive children play
1.0 7. Verbal fluency
Effect size
0.5
1 2 3 4 5 6
0
7
−0.5
Women>Men
Figure 5.2 Magnitude of sex differences affecting various human characteristics.
The amplitude is measured by the effect size (difference between means divided by the
variance of the population). Positive values indicate differences in favor of men, negative
values represent differences in favor of women. The effect size is large (> 0.8) for physical
differences but generally low for behavioral or cognitive differences, except for the
difference in the ability to rotate three-dimensional objects (according to Hines, 2004).
66 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
that support them (if they exist). Sexual orientation itself is highly differenti-
ated between males and females in most species that reproduce sexually (gender
identity does not exist or is impossible to study in animals). One could imagine
that the human species has invented a new way, based on learning, to ensure
that men and women have an orientation and therefore a sexual attraction
toward the opposite sex, thus ensuring successful reproduction. However, it is
worth considering in such circumstances the famous logical rule of thumb
called Occam’s razor (also known as “law of parsimony”): “Entities must not be
multiplied beyond necessity”—in other words, the simplest explanation is likely
to be the best one. The simplest explanation is that the mechanisms existing in
animals have been transmitted to humans, even if cultural controls, unidenti-
fied so far, have been added.
6
We have excellent reasons to believe that the same is true in humans; however,
the nature of the data supporting this conclusion is different. For obvious ethical
reasons, performing a castration in humans purely for experimental purposes is
forbidden, or even to treat subjects chronically with testosterone. Beyond that,
there are obvious methodological difficulties associated with obtaining a reliable
measure of human sexual behavior. The data indicate, however, that male sexual
behavior of the human species is, as in rats, influenced by testosterone. The argu-
ments supporting this argument are threefold.
Comparison with animal species provides a first type of argument. It is
known that human testes produce testosterone identical to that secreted in
rodents such as rats and mice and in most vertebrates. Testosterone circulates
in the blood of adult humans at concentrations similar to those observed in
other vertebrates, and postmortem anatomical studies have shown that the
human brain has receptors for testosterone similar to those of rats or monkeys,
and these receptors are located in exactly the same areas of the brain (Abdelgadir
et al., 1999; Fernández-Guasti et al., 2000; Pelletier, 2000; Kruijver et al., 2001).
This argument is somewhat indirect and may only convince biologists, but it is
strongly reinforced by the analysis of various clinical cases and the outcome of
their medical treatment.
These clinical studies provide a second type of argument, much more direct,
although they are prone to problems of interpretation. For example, there are
human cases of clearly insufficient development of the testes that produce very
low levels of circulating testosterone (less than 10% of values considered normal).
Some of these patients consult endocrinology clinics because they complain of
various sexual disorders, including a lack of desire and sexual motivation, a lack
of erotic fantasies, and weak erections. Clinical studies conducted in controlled
conditions have shown that treatment of these patients with testosterone signifi-
cantly increased all measures of activity and sexual motivation, such as the
number of nocturnal erections, thoughts and sexual fantasies, and masturbation
episodes (Davidson et al., 1979; Bancroft, 1995; Hajjar et al., 1997; Snyder et al.,
2000; Wang et al., 2000). Conversely, pharmacological manipulations that lower
levels of circulating testosterone induce a decrease in libido and various aspects
of sexual behavior in humans (Rosler & Witztum, 1998). These studies are gen-
erally carried out in double-blind conditions in which patients are treated either
by testosterone or by a control solution (vehicle injection), and neither the doctor
nor the patient knows during the study period who received testosterone or pla-
cebo. These studies demonstrate as clearly as possible in humans the behavioral
effects of sex steroids. It must be pointed out that the conclusions that can be
drawn from such studies are not necessarily generalizable to other physiological
situations. For example, other forms of sexual disability will not be improved by
added testosterone if there is not initially a very low circulating level.
The Effects of Sex Steroids in Humans 71
In addition, there are many questions that can only be definitively answered
with experiments that are unethical to carry out on humans, in particular the
detection of brain sites where steroids act to activate the behavior. For such ques-
tions, studies are limited in humans to the use of a third strategy, namely, analysis
of correlations between accidental brain damage and behavioral and endocrine/
physiological variables. When a correlation has been established, its meaning
must be sought by other methods, including comparison with animal studies.
A correlation between two variables does not in fact demonstrate that they
are linked by a direct causal relationship. For example, if we observe that a par-
ticular behavioral characteristic such as frequency of sexual fantasies is corre-
lated with the level of circulating testosterone (either within different individuals
at one time or in an individual over time), it is possible either that testosterone
stimulates behavior, or behavior activates the secretion of testosterone, or that
the two variables are stimulated or inhibited by a third independent variable,
such as the level of stress or relaxation of the individual. Only further studies
can help interpret the observed correlation.
A substantial part of this book is devoted to a review of various types of phys-
ical, functional, and behavioral variables that correlate with homosexuality.
These correlations can be interpreted schematically in three ways: either variable
A is the cause of homosexuality (case 1), or the appearance of A is the result of
homosexuality (case 2), or A and homosexuality are both the result of a cause
that remains to be identified (case 3). It should be noted that these interpreta-
tions are not mutually exclusive and a variable can, for example, represent a par-
tial cause of homosexuality but at the same time be reinforced by the expression
of this sexual orientation (simultaneous occurrence of cases 1 and 2). In the
absence of experimental studies that manipulate the incidence of homosexuality
in a given population (ethically prohibited, obviously), the interpretation of these
correlations will remain somewhat uncertain. The presence of these correlations,
however, clarifies and restricts the possible causes of homosexuality. This is par-
ticularly the case if multiple correlations are considered in parallel and if one
compares these results with those of animal studies. It is clear that only cases 1
and 3 are likely to explain the origins of homosexuality. Condition 2 reveals only
a more or less direct consequence of this orientation. Throughout the following
discussion I shall try to clarify the potential causal significance of various char-
acteristics to be analyzed. In many cases, this meaning is not known with cer-
tainty, so I shall discuss the relative likelihood of each of these solutions.
The remainder of this section is devoted to a brief review of the available evi-
dence that is consistent with the concept and demonstrates in many cases that
the effects of steroid hormones that have been identified in animals are also pres-
ent in humans, although they are sometimes partially masked by other mecha-
nisms. The human species is indeed the result of a long evolution, and there is no
72 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
reason to believe that the mechanisms of endocrine control of behavior that have
been identified in fish and mammals—and are still quite evident in primates—
have completely disappeared in our species. I obviously do not deny the specific-
ity of the human species. But as much as it is characterized by a spectacular
development of the cerebral cortex that has taken control over virtually all aspects
of behavior, this does not exclude the survival of more primitive underlying bio-
logical mechanisms. I consider first the biological mechanisms of sex determina-
tion that are identical in animals and in humans. I then consider the current state
of knowledge on the organizing and activating effects of sex steroids on physical
and behavioral characteristics.
The identification of this gene had to await progress in molecular biology and
was carried out forty years later.
The British group of Andrew Sinclair was able to take advantage of the detailed
study of a small group of individuals in which the correlation between the pres-
ence/absence of a Y chromosome and the man/woman phenotype is not present
(Sinclair et al., 1990). There are indeed very rare cases in which there is a male
phenotype in XX individuals and, vice versa, a female phenotype in XY indi-
viduals. Researchers have found that in XX men, a small fragment of the Y chro-
mosome was transposed (translocated in scientific terms) on the X chromosome.
This translocation occurs exceptionally in one of the cell divisions that lead to
the formation of sperm. A small piece of chromosome “breaks” and joins erro-
neously to another chromosome, here the X chromosome. Conversely, there are
a few individuals with XY chromosomes that have a female phenotype. In these
individuals, the same small piece of the Y chromosome was broken and was lost
during the production of sperm. By studying the DNA contained in this little
piece of Y chromosome that when lost or translocated produces a reverse sex,
British researchers were able to identify in humans the SRY (sex-determining
region of the Y chromosome) gene that encodes the information to produce the
protein TDF (testis-determining factor), which then determines the sex of the
individual. The few cases of translocation or loss of this gene clearly confirm its
key role in humans and animals.
The equivalent gene (SRY) exists in mice, and experimental evidence of the
role of SRY in humans is linked in part to the comparison with the animal (see
Chapter 3). By genetic manipulations, it has been demonstrated in mice that
specific deletion of the SRY gene leads to the development of subjects that have
a completely female phenotype even though the rest of their genetic sex is male.
Conversely the “graft” on a non–sex chromosome of a female of the SRY gene
produces a female that will develop testes, and the hormones secreted by the
testes will masculinize the physical, functional, and behavioral traits of the
subject (De Vries et al., 2002; Arnold & Chen, 2009).
Physical Traits
In the embryonic stage, all individuals have the tissues that are the precursors
of male and female reproductive tracts. Thus the fetus at an early stage has a
genital tubercle that can form either a penis or a clitoris and the genital folds
that can form the lips of the vulva in females or testicular scrotum in males
(Figure 3.5). At an early stage, the embryo also contains two sets of ducts that
74 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
connect the gonads to the outside of the body: the Wolffian and Müllerian ducts
(Figure 3.4).
In humans as in other male mammals, testosterone produced by Leydig cells
of fetal testes causes the differentiation of Wolffian ducts into epididymis, vas
deferens, and seminal vesicles. Sertoli cells will produce a peptide hormone, the
anti-Müllerian hormone, or AMH, which as its name suggests, induces rapid
and complete regression of Müllerian ducts (Figure 3.4).
In the female embryo, the ovary secretes little or no steroid hormones. The
production of testosterone is virtually absent, and if estradiol is produced, it is
in low concentrations that will bind to a circulating embryonic protein called
alpha-fetoprotein that prevents the action of the steroid at the intracellular level.
Consequently, Müllerian ducts spontaneously develop while Wolffian ducts
regress completely. Sexual differentiation of the internal reproductive organs
takes place along two independent axes: (1) demasculinization-masculinization
and (2) defeminization-feminization. For normal development of the male
reproductive system, the embryonic organ precursors undergo defeminizing
hormonal effects (regression of Müllerian ducts by anti-Müllerian hormone
produced in Sertoli cells) and masculinizing effects (development of Wolffian
ducts promoted by testosterone). The female reproductive tract will appear as a
result of feminization (development of Müllerian ducts) and demasculinization
(regression of Wolffian ducts) and does not depend a priori on any hormone
production. This dual mechanism directs the main changes leading to the acqui-
sition of physical gender.
While the internal reproductive organs differentiate through development
and regression of two separate drafts originally present in both sexes, this is not
the case for the external genitalia. They differentiate in effect from the same
embryonic structures (the genital tubercle and genital folds). Depending on
whether testosterone is or is not present at a critical stage, these will change in
the male or female external genitalia (Figure 3.5). The presence of testosterone
[specifically its androgenic metabolite, 5α-dihydrotestosterone (DHT) produced
by the enzyme 5α-reductase], induces the development of the male external
genitals by fusion of the folds to form a scrotum and the development of the
genital tubercle to form a penis. In the absence of testosterone, the genital folds
do not fuse and instead form the vaginal lips, while the genital tubercle develops
only slightly and turns into a clitoris. The same result is obtained in the absence
of gonad. Thus, in humans, as in other mammals, hormone production is not
required for normal development of the female reproductive system; hence it is
generally regarded as the “default” developing sex or neutral sex.
This difference between the development of internal and external structures
has important implications for individuals exposed for various reasons to
abnormal hormonal conditions. In a series of clinical conditions, it is indeed
The Effects of Sex Steroids in Humans 75
theoretically possible to obtain adults who possess in parallel the internal sexual
organs typical of the male and female. In contrast, external structures are more
or less male or female but never both. The genital tubercle can develop to vary-
ing degrees ranging from a “normal” clitoris to a “normal” penis through all
stages of more or less enlarged clitoris, but there is never the simultaneous pres-
ence of two types of structures, since they derive from the same embryonic
tissue.
Behavioral Traits
It should also be noted that the principles that apply to sexual differentiation of
the anatomy of the reproductive system in animals and in humans are also
involved in the emergence of many differences affecting sexual behavior. It is
clearly established in animals that typical male behaviors are generated by mas-
culinization of the male under the influence of embryonic testosterone. Typical
female behavior is present in the absence of hormones, but typical female behav-
ior would be lost following embryonic exposure to testosterone (defeminiza-
tion). Typical male and female behaviors can be considered as independent
entities that depend heavily on different areas of the brain. It is therefore con-
ceivable that these two types of behavior can persist simultaneously in an adult
individual, and this condition has been observed in animals subjected to specific
hormonal treatment during their development. Sexual differentiation of behav-
ior does not occur, therefore, along a single axis from more masculine to more
feminine, but along two more or less independent axes involving structures con-
trolling male and female behavior. We shall see later how these concepts can be
applied to the human species.
Figure 6.1 Photograph of four XY women (XY genotype) affected by the syndrome of
androgen insensitivity (AIS). Presumably normal levels of testosterone were present in
these individuals during embryonic life but could not act due to the mutation of the
androgen receptor. Completely female morphology has therefore developed. (Google
Image Search, AIS syndrome).
the genital tubercle into a penis and fusion of genital folds to form a scrotum).
DHT is an androgen more potent than testosterone and is the only one capable
of masculinizing external genital structures; the embryonic testosterone con-
centration is insufficient to achieve this result.
The conversion of testosterone into DHT is catalyzed (promoted) by a protein
enzyme called 5α-reductase, which is expressed at high levels in the genital tuber-
cle and genital folds. In the early 1970s, Imperato-McGinley and her colleagues
identified a mutation of the 5α-reductase enzyme that makes it functionally
unable to produce DHT (Imperato-McGinley, 1994; Imperato-McGinley & Zhu,
2002). This mutation in the gene controlling the production of 5α-reductase is
recessive. It is only visible when present in a homozygous state (on both chromo-
somes of the pair). It has been observed in island societies where circumstances
make inbreeding unusually common; it was first discovered in the Dominican
Republic, but cases have been found subsequently in Papua New Guinea, Europe,
and the United States.
Male embryos (XY chromosomes) affected by this mutation are born with
external genital structures only partially masculinized (see Figure 6.2). They
develop apparently normal testes that secrete the anti-Müllerian hormone that
induces regression of structures derived from Müllerian ducts (fallopian tubes
and uterus). Testosterone also promotes the development of internal sexual
structures typical of the male, but at the level of external genitalia, the genital
tubercle either doesn’t develop or develops very little and remains about the size
of a clitoris. There is no fusion of genital folds: we observe at birth the presence
of lips surrounding an opening to a blind vagina. There is therefore no scrotum,
and testes are located under the skin around the lips of the blind genital opening
or in the inguinal canal.
In adolescence, dramatic increases in the circulating levels of testosterone
induce a partial masculinization of the genital structures. The skin of the genital
folds’ wrinkles darkens so it looks more like a scrotum, the testicles will continue
their descent and will be found in the genital folds, and the genital tubercle will
develop somewhat to resemble a small penis. The opening of the urethra remains
on the base of the “penis,” a medical condition called hypospadias. Pubertal
male testosterone also affects various other physical aspects: It will increase the
muscles and lower the tone of the voice. From a superficial point of view, these
individuals look as though they have changed sex at least partially.
Male individuals affected by this change are essentially raised as girls and
adopt a female sexual identity during childhood. In communities that are famil-
iar with this syndrome, affected boys are clearly identified at birth, however,
and the local language has a specific term to designate them (Guevedoche in the
Dominican Republic, which in Spanish means “eggs,” i.e., testes, and “twelve
years”; or Kwolu-aatmwol in Papua, which means “female thing transforming
into male thing”). One can imagine that those affected are to some extent con-
sidered intersex individuals. Intersex morphology at birth and partial change of
sex morphology at puberty are also associated with the changes in identity and
sexual role that I shall discuss in following sections.
secreted by the adrenal gland will have little influence because they are simply
added to the large amounts of androgens produced by the testicles. In addition
to the metabolic problems associated with the absence of cortisol, we observe in
affected female individuals a more or less profound masculinization of the
external genital structures. In the most severely affected individuals, the genital
tubercle has developed into a fully formed penis with a size almost equal to that
seen in male babies. In addition there is a more or less complete fusion of the
genital lips that form a scrotum (Figure 6.3). There are, of course, no testicles.
The sex of these children is female. SRY is absent and consequently ovaries are
formed. The anti-Müllerian hormone was not produced and the Wolffian ducts
have developed into a uterus and oviducts. The internal reproductive organs are
those of normal girls.
This more or less complete masculinization of external genital structures in
CAH girls is usually detected at birth. These children will be treated during
their entire life by administration of glucocorticoids. This treatment, by feed-
back on the activity of the pituitary, also suppresses the pathological production
of androgens by the adrenal glands. Masculinized external genital structures
are often “corrected” surgically, removing part of the penis to reproduce the
clitoris and incising the genital folds to reopen the vaginal opening. These chil-
dren are then raised as girls as far as we can know. From a scientific point of
view, CAH girls allow one to assess, as accurately as possible in humans, the
consequences of embryonic androgenization independent of postnatal endo-
crine changes (which are medically corrected). This helps in assessing the con-
sequences of embryonic androgenization on the behavior of girls who are
normally raised as such. [See the work of Melissa Hines, including (Hines &
Kaufman, 1994; Brown et al., 2002b; Hines et al., 2003; Hines et al., 2004).]
I shall return to this topic.
and seem to result mainly from the differential education of boys and girls.
Others have a much greater magnitude and relate in particular to sexual orien-
tation and sexual identity, two behavioral characteristics for which 90–99% of
the population shows a marked sex difference closely correlated to the biologi-
cal sex. The second part of this book is mainly devoted to the analysis of various
arguments that strongly suggest that sexual orientation is determined by bio-
logical factors acting predominantly during the embryonic period.
Before addressing these arguments, it is important to ask whether steroids
have organizing effects on behavior in humans and animals. In other words, does
the hormonal milieu, in addition to determining genital form, influence the
organization of behavior in a more masculine or feminine way? Given the impos-
sibility for ethical reasons of implementing any type of experimentation on the
subject, it is only through the analysis of clinical cases that we can try to answer
this question. Moreover, because behavior is strongly influenced by education
and social influences, we can hope to find a trace of an interpretable embryonic
hormonal influence only in clinical cases where this hormonal influence is acting
against the standards imposed by education.
For example, individuals affected by the syndrome of androgen insensitivity
(AIS) teach us very little about the possible role of hormones in the differen-
tiation of behavior. These individuals, with male sex chromosome (XY), have
testes that secrete testosterone, but the steroid cannot act due to a mutation in
the androgen receptor. Therefore they are born with female genitalia but are also
raised as girls. They thus assume as adults the sexual orientation and gender
identity of women, but it is impossible to know whether this is due to their edu-
cation as a girl or the lack of effect of testosterone in the male embryo. These
cases do demonstrate that the sex chromosomes are not very important as such:
XY individuals may well develop a female phenotype. It is the hormonal action
in the embryo that is critical for determining the phenotype at birth. Whether
female behavioral traits observed in adulthood are the direct consequence of this
embryonic hormonal action or a byproduct of the education of these subjects
raised as girls remains impossible to ascertain.
Girls with the syndrome of Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia (CAH) are, from
this point of view, much more interesting and have therefore been the subject of
many behavioral studies. Females with CAH were indeed exposed in utero to a
hormonal milieu typical of the male (presence of large quantities of androgens of
adrenal origin) but are generally raised as girls after postnatal surgical correction
of their external genitals. If embryonic androgens affect the differentiation of
sexual behavior, it could be expected that these girls would be to some extent
behaviorally masculinized. And that is indeed what has been observed in several
studies (Hines, 2003; Hines et al., 2003; Hines et al., 2004). The author of these
studies has summarized the results in a book for the general public (Hines, 2004).
The Effects of Sex Steroids in Humans 83
A classic behavioral difference between boys and girls concerns the type of
games that they choose freely in a controlled test situation. If we introduce indi-
vidual boys or girls in a room containing various types of toys, the girls spontane-
ously chose dolls while boys prefer to take cars or trains. Spontaneous games of
boys are also more violent than in girls. One might think that this sex difference
in children’s behavior is entirely cultural and driven solely by the differential treat-
ment of children by parents and society. In fact, it is not. It was demonstrated that
these spontaneous toy choices are clearly masculinized in CAH in girls who have
been exposed to abnormally high androgen levels during their life in utero but
not during the postnatal period (thanks to medical treatment). In addition, play
of CAH girls is more violent than in control girls not affected by prenatal andro-
genization (Figure 6.4) (Berenbaum & Snyder, 1995; Nordenstrom et al., 2002).
An interesting finding is that an offer of the same selection of toys to young
vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus) produces the same choice dif-
ferences between sexes as found in humans: males are more interested in cars
or balls whereas females are more interested in dolls (Figure 6.5a) (Alexander
& Hines, 2002; Hassett et al., 2008).
400 400
200 200
0
Ctrl CAH Ctrl CAH Ctrl CAH Ctrl CAH
Girls Boys Girls Boys
Figure 6.4 The type of toy used predominantly by boys and girls is sexually
differentiated and influenced by the embryonic hormonal environment. Boys spend
more time with toys such as cars or buildings tools (Boys Games), and girls with dolls,
doll clothes, and kitchen utensils (Girls Toys). These preferences are significantly
masculinized (increased time spent with the boys’ toys) and defeminized (decrease of the
time spent with the girls’ toys) in CAH girls. The boys already have a lot of testosterone
and their preference is not affected by additional testosterone related to CAH status. The
results of the four types of subjects are represented by columns in which the intensity of
gray is proportional to the concentration of testosterone supposed to have been present
during embryonic life (according to Berenbaum & Snyder, 1995).
84 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
12
400
200
4
0 0
Males Females Boys Girls
Sex of experimental subjects
Figure 6.5 Young monkeys show the same preferences for different types of toys as
human children. A. Photographs showing the play of young vervet monkeys with a doll
(left) or a car (right). (Reproduced from Alexander & Hines, 2002, with permission.)
B. Sex difference in time spent playing with boy or girl toys in rhesus monkeys or in
humans (redrawn from Hassett et al., 2008; Berenbaum & Hines, 1992).
in this study, in the rhesus as in humans, the sex difference of use of toys for
boys (cars) is much larger than the difference in the use of toys for girls (dolls,
stuffed animals).
One might ask why monkeys differentially use toys that are obviously not of
direct significance for them (cars or trains). The answer to this question should
probably be sought in the properties of toys studied. Toys with wheels can, for
example, allow games that are more active and could be preferred by males,
given their propensity to engage in these types of games [see Williams & Pleil
(2008) for a more detailed discussion on this subject]. These data suggest that
sexually differentiated preferences for characteristics of objects (color, shape,
movement) emerged very early in the lineage leading to human evolution. They
may have been selected by evolutionary pressures related to the differential
roles played by men and women and still be operant in the determinism of the
choice of toys shown by small children.
The notion that these preferences are imposed only by society is contra-
dicted by these observations. First, the monkeys had never been able to interact
with the toys before the test, so it is impossible that education played a role.
In addition, the CAH girls, although raised as girls, show preferences more or
less masculinized for games that seem consistent with the hormonal milieu to
which they were exposed in utero. The preferences observed are, instead, related
to the general nature of the objects (e.g., preferences of boys and male monkeys
for moving objects with wheels and more active games) (Hines & Alexander,
2008; Williams & Pleil, 2008) At the proximal level, these preferences appear to
be controlled, at least in part, by the degree of in utero exposure to testosterone,
as suggested by the study of CAH girls.
A study in Japan suggests, moreover, that there are sex differences in the types
of drawings made voluntarily by children and that these drawings are masculin-
ized in females affected by the CAH syndrome (Iijima et al., 2001). If we provide
young children, five to six years old, with a set of colored pencils, boys on aver-
age tend to draw mechanical or moving objects with dark or cold colors (blue,
green), while girls prefer drawing human beings (especially girls and women),
flowers, or butterflies with warm colors (yellow, red). The drawings of boys are
also more often drawings of objects seen from above, while girls tend to align
objects at ground level. An analysis of drawings made by girls affected by the
CAH syndrome has shown that their drawings did not have the usual female
characteristics but were heavily masculinized and included all the features nor-
mally observed in the drawings of boys. Again, these observations strongly sug-
gest that the embryonic hormonal environment plays a role in organizing this
aspect of behavior. It should be noted that during early childhood (from more
or less a year of age until the approach of puberty), levels of circulating testos-
terone are the same in boys and girls. Any influence of testosterone thus can
86 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
Small ball
attached
here
other studies of the group of Simon Baron-Cohen have shown that the amount
of eye contact with parents of 12-month-old children was inversely correlated
to the concentration of embryonic testosterone measured by amniocentesis
between 14 and 21 weeks of gestation (Lutchmaya et al., 2002). There are there-
fore good reasons to believe that embryonic testosterone could be one of the
factors responsible for individual and sex differences that affect certain aspects
of social relationships (refer to the excellent popular books by Simon Baron-
Cohen on the subject (Baron-Cohen, 2004; Baron-Cohen, 2006)).
BIRTH PUBERTY
Embryonic life Childhood Adult life Aged men
conceivable that the newborn may have a genital form (determined early in
gestation) that does not correspond completely with some traits of his person-
ality, such as his gender identity or sexual orientation, which are determined
much later.
It therefore seems possible that an individual who has experienced “normal”
conditions during the first part of his or her embryonic development has
genital structures of one sex but that, subsequently, hormonal changes occur
and may change his or her gender identity (transsexual) or sexual orientation
(homosexual). These characteristics of an individual are firmly established in
early childhood. The development of sexual orientation, however, does not
become evident until later in life (puberty). Various clinical and epidemiologi-
cal data suggest that sexual differentiation of these two characteristics could
begin during the second half of embryonic life (Swaab, 2007).
The presence or absence of testosterone at different critical periods during
development is not the only factor that can potentially generate discordance
between the genetic and gonadal sex of an individual, on the one hand, and
some of his or her behavioral characteristics, on another hand. To exert its
actions, testosterone must be transformed into active metabolites (estradiol,
DHT) that must bind to proper intracellular receptors and activate a cascade of
biochemical events that will eventually lead to the biological response. Localized
changes in any aspect of this reaction chain can therefore produce discordance
between the actions of the steroids at two loci. The profile of plasma testoster-
one concentrations could thus be perfectly normal in a male subject, but local
changes in testosterone action in the brain could prevent the full spectrum of
The Effects of Sex Steroids in Humans 89
different from what happens in humans, and we have as yet no reason to believe
that the difference in volume of the SDN-POA present in humans develops by
mechanisms different from those described in rats or sheep. But we do not have
any argument beyond evolutionary continuity to confirm the identity of these
mechanisms.
Some authors (e.g. Vidal, 2007, p. 13) argue that the sexually dimorphic
nucleus of the hypothalamus would be unable to control sexual orientation.
C. Vidal announces a size of 50 microns of this sexually dimorphic nucleus in
women and homosexuals and 100 microns in heterosexual men (cubic microns,
I assume). Although small (between 0.05 mm3 in homosexuals and 0.10 mm3
in heterosexuals), the human SDN contains a significant number of neurons,
approximately 1,800. It is generally accepted that each neuron in the human
brain on average makes 100 to 1,000 connections with other neurons. This means
that the SDN is directly connected to a number of neurons ranging between
18,000 and 180,000, themselves part of a network that is branching in an expo-
nential manner. Such a network is capable, I believe, of managing complex infor-
mation. Excluding a priori that it could be a significant part of the mechanisms
that control sexual orientation is not based on any rational argument, even if the
author of this book was trained as a neurobiologist and director of research at the
Pasteur Institute. The denial of the role of the brain is here based on a priori
excluding the idea that there may be significant differences between the male
and female brains. If the ultimate goal of this claim is laudable (fight against sex
discrimination), the means are not scientifically acceptable.
7
BEHAVIOR
If the reality of the effects of sex steroids on the morphology (including genital)
and the physiology of reproduction does not appear to be contested, there is in
contrast a widespread reluctance to accept the idea that sex steroids could play
a role in control of behavior in general and sexual behavior in particular. This
resistance is particularly important in Latin societies. For instance, in Men and
Women: Do We Have the Same Brain? Catherine Vidal wrote, “In animals, the
action of hormones on the brain induces mating behavior and mating periods
are associated with ovulation of the female. Sexuality and reproduction go hand
in hand. In contrast the human being escapes this determinism” (Vidal, 2007).
There is, in my opinion, no statement more false than her last sentence.
Human sexual behavior is very complex, and its control involves multi-
ple cognitive aspects (experience, perception of social expectations of the
environment, individual preferences, social conventions, varied religious and
philosophical influences, etc.), the importance of which relates to the major
development of the cerebral cortex that characterizes our species. Accordingly,
the mere presence of testosterone in the blood and brain of a man and of estra-
diol in a woman will not automatically lead them to have sex (mating) within
minutes of their first meeting. It is also not the case in animals. The specific
studies conducted in particular in rats and mice are showing the influence of
multiple variables on sexual behavior in these species, such as prior experience,
learning, individual preferences, and the effect of an audience on the social
behavior achieved. Only in the study of instinctive reactions in invertebrates
such as insects do we find to some degree the automatic causal reactions
The Effects of Sex Steroids in Humans 93
EVOLUTIONARY ARGUMENT
The brain regions that are known to control sexual behavior in rodents and
primates have changed very little from a physical or neurochemical point of
view between mammals and humans. Humans possess almost all the nuclei
(clusters of neurons) of the preoptic area, hypothalamus, and limbic systems
that control sexual behavior in the rat brain. In addition to this anatomical
consistency, the same neurotransmitters as in animals and steroid hormone
receptors (androgen and estrogen receptors) that have been characterized in
other vertebrates are also found in these regions of the human brain. The distri-
bution of these receptors is very consistent from fish to mammals (Kelley &
Pfaff, 1978; Morrell & Pfaff, 1978), and man is no exception (Kruijver et al.,
2001; Kruijver et al., 2002; 2003). The cellular and neurochemical “machinery”
94 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
6
Coital risk
0
45.2 120.4 195.6 270.7 345.9
−1SD Mean +1SD +2SD +3SD
Testosterone in saliva (pmole/liter)
Figure 7.1 Relationship between the concentration of testosterone in saliva and
the probability of occurrence of complete sexual (coital) behavior in adolescents
(age 12–13 years at baseline). Saliva samples were collected at regular intervals over
a period of two years during which the subjects filled out questionnaires about their
weekly sexual activity. The average risk of coital sexual behavior is adjusted to 1 for
average values of testosterone (120.4 pmol/l). It increases in an exponential manner
when concentrations of testosterone increased by 1, 2, or 3 standard deviations (SD)
above average (redrawn from Halpern et al. 1998).
5 Injection Injection
6
Sexual excitation
Sexual desire
4 5
E+A E+A
4
3
3
2 E E
2
1 1
Ctrl Ctrl
0 0
0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4
Weeks after injection
Figure 7.2 Evolution over time of various aspects of sexuality in ovariectomized women
treated with estrogen alone (E) or estrogen combined with androgens (E+A), or
receiving a control (Ctrl) treatment (redrawn from Sherwin & Gelfand 1987).
400
Normal
300 cycles
Dollars earned
200
Pill
users
100
0
Menstrual Fertile Luteal
Cycle phase
Figure 7.3 Variations during the menstrual cycle of female attractiveness assessed
indirectly by the dollar amount of tips obtained by seminude dancers in cabaret.
A peak gain is observed in ovulatory phase (fertile) compared to menstrual and luteal
phases. This temporal variation is not present in women using an oral contraceptive
and who therefore have a constant level of estrogen and progesterone during the cycle
(redrawn from Miller et al. 2007).
Same
80
Better
Worse
60
% of subjects
40
20
0
Control Testosterone
Figure 7.4 Percentage of male subjects suffering from hypogonadism in which sex life
had improved, deteriorated, or remained the same after two years of treatment with
testosterone or a control treatment (according to Hajjar et al. 1997).
(which is questionable, as we have seen), orientation remains the same and they
are still attracted by children (boys, girls, or both), thus indicating that sexual
orientation is not controlled by the action of hormones in adulthood, as I dis-
cuss in Chapter 8.
Nevertheless, all studies of subjects with spontaneously or experimentally
very low levels of testosterone agree in demonstrating that the addition of tes-
tosterone increases sexual activity or sexual fantasies (Davidson et al., 1979;
Hajjar et al., 1997; Snyder et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2000). In contrast, improve-
ment is not observed following administration of testosterone to subjects suf-
fering from lack of sexual desire if the concentration of circulating testosterone
is normal. Testosterone does not affect erectile potential in response to erotic
stimuli (Carani et al., 1992). It would only be involved in the activation of sexual
motivation.
8
Visual-Spatial Tasks
Several studies have shown that homosexual men have performances inferior
to those of heterosexuals in the execution of many tasks, including visual-
spatial mental rotation tasks, evaluation of the orientation of a straight line, and
aiming at a specific target (Hall & Kimura, 1995; Neave et al., 1999; Rahman
& Wilson, 2003b). Gay men generally have performances similar to those of
women or intermediate between men and women. Some studies, however, have
not shown such differences between homosexual and heterosexual males for
some of these characteristics (Gladue et al., 1990; Tutle & Pillard, 1991).
Studies conducted in women, which according to this theory should identify
masculinized performances in lesbians, have yielded results that are less clear.
A recent study indicates that homosexual women perform mental rotations
106 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
slightly better than heterosexual women, but the difference only affects the
speed of execution of the task, not its accuracy (Rahman & Wilson, 2003b).
Aggression
In general, men are more aggressive than women, and two separate studies have
indicated that gay men are less physically aggressive than heterosexual men
(Ellis et al., 1990). No difference in aggression has been identified between les-
bians and heterosexual women (Gladue & Bailey, 1995). One can imagine that
the basal level is very low, so a small increase can pass unnoticed.
Verbal Fluency
A 1991 study indicated that homosexual men have a higher verbal performance
than heterosexuals, a finding that conforms with the notion that they were not
completely masculinized and therefore are more feminine from this point of
view (McCormick & Witelson, 1991). Two subsequent studies have not been
able to confirm this difference (Gladue et al., 1990). In addition, a recent analy-
sis based on a large number of subjects reported that in verbal fluency tests,
both gay men and gay women have performances that are atypical for their
gender (Rahman et al., 2003a). Thus, in one of these tests, gay men were better
than all other groups (heterosexual men and women regardless of their orienta-
tion). In another test, gay men and heterosexual women were better than lesbi-
ans and heterosexual men. These data indicate that for these features, gay men
are more similar to women, whereas lesbians are more like men.
D2 D4
0.975
0.970
0.965
D2:D4 ratio
0.960
0.955
0.950
0.945
ratio than in “normal” women (Brown et al., 2002b). A recent study also indi-
cates that the D2:D4 ratio is higher in XY women affected by the complete
androgen insensitivity syndrome (AIS) than in control XY males (Berenbaum
et al., 2009). Moreover, this sex difference in the relative length of fingers is
found in various animal species as well (Brown et al., 2002a; Romano et al.,
2005) and is in animals significantly affected by early treatments with andro-
gens (Lutchmaya et al., 2004; Romano et al., 2005; Manning et al., 2006).
Together, these facts strongly suggest that the D2:D4 ratio is indeed affected by
prenatal androgen levels [see Breedlove (2010) for a recent review], but it has
been pointed out that the real world might actually be more complex than sug-
gested by this simple conclusion. The ratio might be related more directly to the
ratio of androgens to estrogens and might reflect a difference in adiposity rather
than a true difference in finger lengths (Wallen, 2009).
Sex Differences Suggest Homosexuality Is an Endocrine Phenomenon 109
Several independent groups have shown that the D2:D4 ratio is significantly
smaller (and therefore similar to the ratio observed in men) in lesbians than in
heterosexual women, which is in perfect agreement with the theory that assigns
a role for prenatal hormones in the development of homosexuality (McFadden &
Shubel, 2002; Rahman & Wilson, 2003c; Kraemer et al., 2006). This difference
was recently confirmed by a meta-analysis of multiple studies involving thou-
sands of subjects (Grimbos et al., 2010). It was also shown that only lesbians who
identify as masculine (known as “butch”) have this decreased D2:D4 ratio (Brown
et al., 2002a). Another study also confirmed these results independently, showing
that among monozygotic twins (true twins), where one is homosexual and the
other not, the lesbian sister had a smaller (masculine) D2:D4 ratio compared
with the heterosexual sister (Hall & Love, 2003). This work also shows that these
variations of the D2:D4 ratio are independent of genes, since the study is of twins
that have an identical genetic heritage. A more recent study based on a larger
number of subjects, however, indicates a genetic contribution in conjunction
with a contribution of the prenatal environment in the determination of the
D2:D4 ratio (Gobrogge et al., 2008). These studies therefore imply that if testos-
terone is a key factor that determines this ratio, twins may be exposed to different
concentrations of testosterone, which so far has not been experimentally tested.
However, it is also possible that the twins could be exposed to the same concen-
tration of steroids but be differentially sensitive (e.g., differences in the density of
androgen receptors in the intracellular metabolism of the hormone, etc.; see
Chapter 3). To my knowledge, only one study has not replicated the relationship
between the D2:D4 ratio and sexual orientation in women (Lippa, 2003a).
Four similar studies were conducted among men, in whom it could be
expected that homosexuality is associated with a more feminine (higher) D2:D4
ratio, with conflicting results (only one of the four studies conducted among
men jibes with the predictions of the prenatal hormonal theory).
It was shown that the length of the bones that become sexually dimorphic in
infancy was significantly different between homosexual and heterosexual indi-
viduals, while the bones that become different between men and women after
puberty are not modified according to sexual orientation. Thus, people who have
a sexual preference for men (heterosexual women and homosexual men) have a
smaller growth of the bones of the arms, legs, and hands than subjects who have
a sexual preference for women (heterosexual men and homosexual women).
These data support the idea that gay men have experienced a reduced exposure
to sex steroids during development, and conversely that the homosexual women
were exposed to higher concentrations of sex steroids than heterosexual women,
or alternatively that the sensitivity to these hormones is different in relation to
sexual orientation.
Before this major study based on over 500 subjects (Martin & Nguyen, 2004),
other less systematic work had already identified in males differences in ratios
of the lengths of various parts of the body associated with sexual orientation
[see references in Martin & Nguyen (2004)]. It is therefore quite likely that the
results of this study are reproducible.
Other studies have identified differences compatible with the existence of
endocrine disruption during development in homosexuals, but they appear to
be less reproducible at the present stage of knowledge. These studies should
certainly be pursued.
PHYSIOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES
Oto-Acoustic Emissions
It was discovered in the late 1970s that the inner ear, in addition to its obvious
function in hearing, also emits sounds in the form of barely audible clicks that
can be recorded by placing a sensitive microphone in the canal of the ear. The
subjects do not usually hear these sounds, which are called oto-acoustic emis-
sions (OAE). They are produced either spontaneously or in response to external
short noises (e.g., clicks; Figure 8.2). It is interesting, for the purposes of this
book, that oto-acoustic emissions are different in men and women. Women
produce more oto-acoustic emissions, with a greater amplitude, than men do in
the same situation. This difference is already present during childhood. The
functional significance of oto-acoustic emissions is unclear (they seem to be an
indirect consequence of the functioning of the inner ear), but it is thought that
the sex difference that affects this sound production is largely the result of the
prenatal exposure to androgens in male embryos (McFadden, 2002; 2011).
This hypothesis is supported by studies of OAE in animals. These OAE are
indeed found, mutatis mutandis, both in sheep and in monkeys, and they are
Sex Differences Suggest Homosexuality Is an Endocrine Phenomenon 111
25
−5
−15
2 4 6 8
Frequency (Hz)
Amplitude of induced OAE Number of spontaneous OAE
0
16 (Clicks at 69 dB)
12
Figure 8.2 The oto-acoustic emissions (OAE) are sexually differentiated and
significantly masculinized in homosexual females. The upper figure shows the
distribution of frequencies and amplitudes of a spontaneous OAE recorded directly
into the ear canal. The individual has a peak of emission at 1,700 Hz. Some individuals
have such peaks at multiple frequencies. The lower figure represents the average
number of spontaneous OAE and OAE amplitude evoked by clicks sounds
measured in the right ear of hetero- or homosexual males and females
(according to McFadden, 2002; 2008).
112 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
also sexually differentiated (female OAE are more frequent and of greater
amplitude than in males). OAE were studied in hyenas because in this species,
females are strongly masculinized by androgens during fetal life and immedi-
ately postnatally, so that their external genital structures are at first sight not
different from those of males. The clitoris is greatly enlarged and, for a naïve
observer, seems to have the same form as the male penis. Correspondingly, it
was noted that in this case, the OAE of the females do not have a greater mag-
nitude than those of males. This magnitude is even slightly lower. The OAE of
hyenas are affected by prenatal androgens, because if pregnant mothers are
treated with an antiandrogen (compound that blocks the action of androgens at
their receptor), an amplitude OAE well above normal is observed in treated
young (McFadden, 2008).
Furthermore, in sheep that show the classic sex difference in OAE (female >
male), it was shown that prenatal treatment of female embryos with testoster-
one considerably reduced the magnitude of their OAE (McFadden et al., 2009).
Finally, in both species (sheep and hyena), castration in adulthood does not
affect the OAE, thus confirming that the sex difference affecting this character-
istic reflects the physiological prenatal hormonal milieu but not activation by
steroids in adulthood. In humans also, the sex difference in OAE seems to result
mostly from prenatal organizing effects of steroids, but there is also some evi-
dence for limited activational effects of steroids on this physiological response
(McFadden, 2011). It has been shown, for example, that OAE are masculinized
in women taking oral contraceptives.
Researchers at the University of Texas at Austin found that lesbians and bisex-
ual women have oto-acoustic emissions and acoustic evoked potentials that are
partially male: they emit significantly fewer OAE than heterosexual women, and
these OAE have a lower amplitude (McFadden & Pasanen, 1998; 1999; McFadden,
2002). These data are consistent with the hormonal theory of homosexuality
stating that female fetuses destined to become homosexual or bisexual have
been exposed to abnormally high levels of androgens. These same researchers
also demonstrated that women who had a twin brother and therefore had poten-
tially been exposed to slightly higher levels of testosterone during embryonic life
(testosterone produced by the twin would have diffused to the female embryo)
had masculinized OAE, thus confirming sensitivity to androgens of the response
in humans. However, the same researchers found no difference between the
OAE of homo- and heterosexual males.
are also different in men and women, and in (partly masculinized) homosexual
or bisexual women compared with heterosexual women. By contrast, other
characteristics of these evoked acoustic potentials were hypermasculinized in
homosexual men when compared with values observed in heterosexual men
(McFadden & Champlin, 2000). This study is again consistent with the idea that
lesbians were exposed to abnormally high levels of androgens during fetal life
but that, contrary to what one might intuit, gay men have been masculinized as
much or even more than heterosexual men. Indeed, if we remain in the context
of an interpretation based on differences in the concentrations of testosterone
that embryos have been exposed to, we should conclude that if lesbians were
exposed to concentrations higher than heterosexual women, they would have
been masculinized and therefore present a male-typical sexual orientation
(preference for women), which is consistent with the theory. In contrast, these
data would suggest that gay men were exposed in utero to testosterone at above-
normal levels. No definitive explanation for these paradoxical observations
about gay men has been provided to date, but several potential mechanisms
have been suggested (McFadden, 2011). Since it is a recurring paradox that
I have noted before, it may be that we still have an imperfect understanding of
some hormonal effects.
An independent study in London has also focused on the analysis of another
physiological response known to be sexually differentiated—the particular fear
response called the alarm or startle response. Following a loud noise, there is a
blink of the eyes, but this response is partially inhibited if the loud noise is pre-
ceded by a lower noise. This inhibition by the preceding sound (prepulse inhibi-
tion) is usually less pronounced among women than among men. The group of
Rahman and his colleagues recently showed that inhibition of the blink by a low
alarm is stronger among lesbians than among heterosexual women (Rahman
et al., 2003b). This response is masculinized in agreement with the prenatal hor-
monal theory of homosexuality. However, here again, the authors have not found
this difference in inhibition between homosexual and heterosexual men.
hormonal mechanisms that differentiate sexual behavior. The male embryos are
masculinized and defeminized by early exposure to testosterone, whereas the
female phenotype develops in the relative absence of hormones (Chapter 3).
The injection of testosterone in female embryos of rats induces the loss of the
LH peak in response to estradiol, but males castrated immediately after birth
are able to produce such a peak.
Having suggested that human male homosexuality could be the result of
inadequate masculinization during embryonic life, Dörner wondered whether
the positive feedback of estrogen on LH secretion might be different in homo-
sexual and heterosexual males (Dörner, 1969). In women, an estrogen injection
at an appropriate time of the ovarian cycle induced a strong increase of circulat-
ing levels of LH, whereas this response was not observed in men. In keeping
with his theory, Dörner showed that gay men react to an injection of estrogen
by a significant increase in blood levels of LH. This increase is lower than that
observed in women, but it is still significantly higher than among heterosexual
men (Dörner, 1972; 1976; 1980).
These publications raised a huge controversy, in part because of the research-
er’s views concerning preventive treatments that he suggested should be estab-
lished to prevent the emergence of gays, but also because the positive feedback
of estrogen on LH that is quite evident in the rat is apparently not present in
monkeys (Baum et al., 1985), a finding that raises the question of its existence
in humans. Many researchers have thus doubted the reality of the effect identi-
fied by Dörner. In an attempt to resolve this controversy, American neuroendo-
crinologist Brian Gladue therefore decided to test the reproducibility of the
effect. In a study published in Science in 1984, Gladue and colleagues clearly
demonstrated the reproducibility of the effect identified by Dörner. After a
single injection of Premarin, a compound with estrogenic action, women
showed a major increase in LH 72 to 96 hours later (Gladue et al., 1984). This
increase was not observed in men, but again an increase of intermediate mag-
nitude was observed in men who reported having had a homosexual orienta-
tion during their life (see Figure 8.3).
This LH response to the injection of an estrogenic compound looks similar,
but is not identical, to the positive feedback observed in the control of ovulation
in rats [see for discussion Baum et al. (1985)]. It is important that there is a dif-
ference in this endocrine response in men according to their sexual orientation.
However, based on available data, it is impossible to know whether this differ-
ence concerns a sexually differentiated neuroendocrine mechanism, which
would suggest that homosexuals were exposed to “abnormal” endocrine condi-
tions during their ontogeny. Alternatively, the differential response of LH to
estrogens might just reflect a stable but unidentified difference in adult testicular
Sex Differences Suggest Homosexuality Is an Endocrine Phenomenon 115
Women
200
LH (% of baseline)
150 Homosexual
men
100
Men
50
Base 24 48 72 96
line Hours after
injection
Premarin
injection
Figure 8.3 Changes in blood levels of luteinizing hormone (LH) in response to injection
of a single dose of an estrogenic compound (Premarin) in heterosexual men and women
and in homosexual men. The injection induced in women after 72–96 hours an increase
in LH that was not present in men but was seen in an attenuated form in homosexuals
(according to Gladue et al., 1984).
activity that could interfere with the positive and negative feedback on LH
(Baum et al., 1985; Gladue, 1985).
Further studies should be conducted on this issue, but the fact remains that
all this work indicates the existence of a (neuro)endocrine difference between
homosexual and heterosexual men. We cannot conclude, at present, that this
difference concerns a neuroendocrine mechanism, located most likely in the
hypothalamus, that is known in rats to be differentiated (defeminized) during
ontogenesis after exposure to testosterone rather than an aspect of testicular
physiology associated in a less specific or even unknown manner to the differ-
entiation of sexual behavior and brain. In either case, these data demonstrate
the existence of a difference in the function of the hypothalamic-pituitary-
testicular axis associated with homosexuality.
116 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
Heterosexuals Homosexuals
Men Women Men Women
AND − + + −
EST + − − −
Figure 8.4 Exposure to potential pheromones differentially activates the hypothalamic
area of males and females, and the response is modified based on their sexual
orientation. The upper figure presents a schematic view of a sagittal section of human
brain showing the region (arrow and black square) where metabolic activations were
detected by medical imaging. The table indicates whether these hypothalamic activations
are present (+) or absent (–) depending on the sex of the individual, his or her sexual
orientation, and the compound used. AND = compound produced by men; EST =
compound produced by women (according to Savic et al., 2005; Berglund et al., 2006).
of sexual orientation (Kruijver et al., 1993). And since there is also no evidence
that the volume of the SCN is sexually differentiated (greater or smaller in men
than in women), the difference observed between homosexual and straight
men does not provide any argument that might suggest a lack of masculiniza-
tion of the brain.
15
Mid-sagittal surface (mm2)
12
cognitive abilities. However, some studies have failed to confirm these differ-
ences in cognitive abilities between heterosexual and homosexual men, so they
must be treated with caution even though many aspects of experimental proto-
cols, and in particular the selection of subjects, are likely to explain these fail-
ures of replication.
It is important to note here, once again, that the neuroanatomical difference
in gay men affecting the size of the anterior commissure, a structure that has no
direct link with sexuality, is an additional strong argument suggesting that
homosexuality is not the result of a choice, but instead a complex phenotypic
change that goes far beyond the field of sexuality and has in all likelihood bio-
logical bases beyond the control of the individuals.
They observed that the isthmus of the corpus callosum was significantly greater
among homosexuals, thus indicating that they have, like women, a less pro-
nounced brain asymmetry than do heterosexual men. Right-handed homosexu-
als thus have a cerebral functional asymmetry less developed than in heterosexual
men, and the motor lateralization is partially dissociated in these homosexuals
from the cerebral lateralization. Moreover, during this same study, researchers
were able to show that statistical analysis could correctly identify the sexual orien-
tation of the subject in 21 cases out of 22 based not only on the size of the isthmus
of the corpus callosum but also on the basis of different cognitive tests already
mentioned in this chapter. These data demonstrate an association between a neu-
roanatomical structure and cognition, and join many other results that indicate
the existence of anatomical differences between the brains of homosexual and
heterosexual men that indirectly support the hypothesis of a biological basis to
sexual orientation (Witelson et al., 2008). Remember also that being left-handed
or right-handed is a characteristic that appears early in development (Hepper
et al., 1991). This feature cannot be a personal choice. The fact that it is correlated
with sexual orientation suggests that sexual orientation is determined early.
(nuclei) in this region. They called these nuclei the INAH 1 to 4 for the interstitial
nuclei of the anterior hypothalamus. Two of these nuclei were found to be larger in
men than in women (INAH 2 and 3) (Allen et al., 1989). It was shown a little later
that the INAH 3 was significantly smaller in homosexual men than heterosexual
men. Its average size was in fact similar to the size of INAH 3 observed among
women (LeVay, 1991) (Figure 8.6).
Bill Byne’s independent study based on different brains confirmed the reduced
size of INAH 3 in male homosexuals compared to heterosexuals, though the
magnitude of the difference observed in this replication was lower than in the first
study of LeVay and thus not statistically significant (Byne et al., 2001). This study
also showed greater cell density (more cells per unit volume) in the INAH 3 of
homosexuals than in heterosexuals. The nucleus of homosexuals thus has a cel-
lular composition identical to that of heterosexuals (same number of neurons),
but the neurons are closer to each other on average, possibly because they have
H 4
INA2 3
1
SO
3rdV
OC
AIDS
0.20 Other
INAH 3 volume (mm3)
0.15
0.10
0.05
0.00
Women Men Homosexual
men
Figure 8.6 Schematic representation of the human hypothalamus illustrating the
position of the four interstitial nuclei of the anterior hypothalamus (INAH) in relation
to the 3rd ventricle (3rdV), the optic nerve chiasma (OC), and supraoptic nucleus (SO).
The lower figure represents the values of the volume of INAH 3 measured in women,
men, and gay men who died of AIDS or of another cause. The horizontal bars represent
the average for each group of subjects (redrawn from LeVay, 1991).
124 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
formed fewer synapses during development. This observation and its interpreta-
tion, however, need to be confirmed.
Many critics have focused on the fact that many gay brains used in these stud-
ies came from individuals who had died of AIDS. It was therefore argued that the
small size of the INAH 3 in these subjects arose from either AIDS or the inten-
sive medical treatments to which these patients had been submitted. In the pres-
ent state of knowledge, this critique is unfounded. Indeed, LeVay and Byne were
perfectly aware of this problem and whenever possible included homosexuals in
their samples who had died of a cause other than AIDS, as well as heterosexuals
who died of AIDS. These data have not provided an argument to suggest that
AIDS and its treatment can reduce the size of the SDN (see Figure 8.6). Thus, in
the study of LeVay, the size of the SDN of heterosexuals who had died of AIDS
was larger than the average size of that of homosexuals. In addition, the study of
Byne and colleagues included nine heterosexual men who had died of AIDS and
22 who had died of other causes. A difference in volume of INAH 3 could not be
detected between these two subgroups. It should also be recalled here that in
rats, SDN size is determined during early life and can no longer be affected by
any known treatment in adulthood.
It was also said that the number of brains used in these studies was too
limited. This objection is clearly based on a misreading of the results. LeVay’s
study was based on the brains of 41 people, including 19 homosexuals, which
given the difficulty inherent in obtaining such samples is a respectable or even
remarkable sample size. The difference he observed in the volume of INAH 3
according to gender and sexual orientation was quite significant (p < 0.00014,
or less than 2 chances in 10,000 of obtaining a difference of such an amplitude
by random fluctuations in sampling). So it is clearly a misunderstanding of the
meaning of statistical tests to say that the observed difference could simply be
caused by fluctuations of sampling. The result of LeVay was by and large repro-
duced independently by Bill Byne, which reinforces its validity.
The presence of a smaller SDN in male homosexuals (IANH 3) acquires an
especially important meaning when connecting this observation to the animal
studies that have been described in detail previously. Remember the following:
1. The nucleus is located in the center of the preoptic area, which plays
a key role in controlling male sexual behavior (Nelson, 2005).
2. The lesion of the SDN in several mammalian species induces a change
in male sexual orientation from strictly heterosexual to either
homosexual or bisexual (Paredes & Baum, 1995).
3. The larger size of SDN in males compared to females is determined
exclusively by the action of sex steroids during embryonic and
postnatal life (masculinization by testosterone and its estrogenic
Sex Differences Suggest Homosexuality Is an Endocrine Phenomenon 125
One could thus imagine that the small size of the SDN in homosexuals is
induced by an aspect of their behavior or lifestyle, rather than being the cause of
their orientation. It has been observed that many homosexuals living in New
York and California, from which came the subjects in LeVay’s study, had sexual
activity involving large numbers of relationships with many partners. One might
therefore think that the smaller size of SDN could be the result of that intense
and diversified sexual activity. This argument cannot be formally rejected, but
all cases of behavior-induced plasticity in the nervous system that have been
described both in animals and in humans never concern the hypothalamus and
limbic system but instead are almost always related to the cerebral hemispheres.
These brain areas seem to react to plastic life experiences of the subject, whereas
the hypothalamus appears to be controlled by intrinsic physiological mecha-
nisms based largely on hormonal changes. It is not excluded that in the future
researchers will discover changes in the hypothalamus induced by experience,
but in the current state of knowledge, data do not support this hypothesis.
In a minimalist interpretation, one can imagine that the small size of the
homosexual INAH 3 represents a signature of the early hormonal environment
in which these individuals have developed, but the size of the nucleus is not
responsible for the sexual orientation of the individuals. In other words, the
small size of the INAH 3 and the sexual orientation of the concerned individual
would have both been induced by the presence of abnormal embryonic hor-
monal conditions (probably an overly low concentration of testosterone), but
these two features would not be directly related in a causal manner.
Alternatively, if we consider that lesions of the preoptic area (including SDN) of
rats or ferrets change their sexual orientation, it is also possible that there is actually
a causal chain that, from a low level of embryonic testosterone, leads to the develop-
ment of a small INAH 3 that is the cause of the homosexual orientation. To defend
this hypothesis further, it should be previously determined by what mechanism a
small SDN/INAH 3 can be the cause of a reversed sexual orientation.
Finally, we should note that even if we consider the latter interpretation of
these data sympathetically, the fact remains that the size of INAH 3 and cellular
changes that must necessarily accompany the changes in the volume of this
nucleus cannot be the only cause of homosexuality. Indeed, although the average
volume of INAH 3 in homosexuals is statistically smaller than in heterosexuals,
128 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
there is some overlap between the volumes observed in both groups. In other
words, the smallest of the INAH 3 observed in heterosexuals have a volume
smaller than the largest nuclei observed in homosexuals. The size of this nucleus
thus cannot be the sole cause of homosexuality. This feature may predispose to
sexual orientation, but not produce it by itself.
with peripheral changes (if the circulating testosterone was affected) but
that these correlations are not present among other subjects (for whom
brain sensitivity to testosterone is changed).
4. The dose-response relationships that link the concentration of
circulating testosterone during embryonic development with the
mechanism controlling sexual orientation and its various correlates
are not known and could be different. Studies of animal behavior and
physiology have shown that among a group of responses controlled by
hormones, some are affected by lower doses than others. If we
imagine that human sexual orientation is more sensitive to small
variations in testosterone levels than other hormonal responses
described above (to which it is sometimes correlated), we can easily
obtain homosexual individuals in whom the trait associated with
homosexuality is affected (strong hormonal changes) and others for
whom it is not (more subtle hormonal changes).
5. In the same way, the critical period during which sexual orientation
and its various correlates are determined might be partially different,
as is the case for various responses studied in animals. Hormonal
events occurring at a specific stage of development can affect one
response without affecting the others significantly. Since we know
nothing of these critical periods, this possibility is difficult to assess.
All the interpretations set out in items 3 to 5 of this list are derived
directly from studies on animal models and could easily be tested on
animals. In humans, obvious ethical reasons prohibit deliberate
manipulations of embryonic hormones, and it is therefore much more
difficult to obtain information on the mechanistic aspects of
hormonal action during ontogeny.
6. Finally, any prenatal endocrine phenomenon that leads to
homosexuality is obviously not completely deterministic but simply
modulates the probability of occurrence of this orientation. It is
possible and even likely that to determine sexual orientation in
adulthood, it is necessary that various hormonal and biological
changes accumulate in the same subject or that the predisposition to
homosexuality induced by embryonic hormones is strengthened by
not-yet-identified aspects of interaction with parents, teachers, or
society in general. It is possible that a predisposition to homosexuality
that would be controlled by prenatal biological factors can only be
manifested in particular circumstances of the environment. These
have not been identified. If these “social” influences exist, their
importance is probably less than that of the embryonic hormonal
factors, since they are so far much better identified.
9
probably represent the best possible source of information. The information that
was collected using this approach will be considered here in detail.
in utero to abnormally high levels of androgens (see Chapter 6). We have already
seen that these girls display a masculinization of certain character traits during
childhood, such as the type of toys used or drawings made freely (Berenbaum
& Snyder, 1995; Berenbaum et al., 2000; Iijima et al., 2001). They also show a
high level of physical activity, which is typical of boys.
There is an increase in CAH women of the probability of commitment or desire
to engage in a homosexual relationship in comparison to a population of control
females or to unaffected sisters of these androgenized women (Money et al., 1984;
Dittmann et al., 1992; Zucker et al., 1996). Although there is an incidence of
female homosexuality of 10–12% in a control population (Kinsey et al., 1953), or
somewhat below 5% (Mosher et al., 2005), a study by John Money and his col-
leagues reported an incidence of homosexuality or bisexuality of 37% in CAH
women (Figure 9.1). Recent studies have produced similar figures (Hines, 2006;
Meyer-Bahlburg, 2009; Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 2008). A review by Meyer-Bahlburg
and collaborators also presented a summary of 17 studies on the subject in which
most studies showed an increased incidence of homosexuality or bisexuality in
CAH girls.
The prenatal endocrine change corrected at birth is thus associated with a
significant reduction of conventional heterosexual orientation. The explanatory
interest of this medical condition is that postnatal treatment of the subjects
40
Homosexual orientation (%)
30
20
10
0
Homo. CAH
(Kinsey)
Bi-/homo. Refused Hetero.
to answer
Figure 9.1 Effect of prenatal androgenization linked to the syndrome of congenital
adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) on sexual orientation. One can observe in affected women a
significant increase in the percentage of bisexual or homosexual orientation compared
with control subjects and a large number of subjects refusing to answer questions
relating to this topic (drawn from data in Money et al., 1984).
134 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
(as a girl) is expected to act in the opposite direction of the prenatal hormonal
influence (masculinization). The masculinization of a behavioral trait (female
homosexuality, or sexual attraction for women, a trait normally male) should
thus in theory result from prenatal hormonal effects.
There are two possible alternative explanations, however, to this increased
incidence of homosexual or bisexual orientation. First of all, the change of
sexual orientation could be caused indirectly by the masculinization of the
external genital structures. Although these were surgically corrected at birth,
they still do not have an ideal structure in some women and therefore allow
little or no penetrative heterosexual relationships. This could be one reason for
engaging in homosexual rather than heterosexual relationships. It is also pos-
sible, theoretically, that the increase in nonheterosexual behavior and attraction
in CAH women is induced indirectly by the reaction of the parents of the girl
with masculinized genital structures. Even though these genital structures are
surgically corrected at birth and adequate hormonal treatment is in place to
block the secretion of androgens, the parents may not educate their daughter in
the same way as an unaffected girl. However, one might reasonably assume that
if parents have a modified reaction toward CAH girls, they should intentionally
promote the feminine behavioral traits of these girls. Homosexual attraction in
women is a trait normally found in men, and therefore one should expect that
the action of parents would decrease this aspect of behavior. Since several stud-
ies have observed increased homosexual orientation in CAH girls, one would,
in this interpretation, be lead to believe that parental efforts systematically pro-
duce an effect opposite to the intended effect. This would be paradoxical, to say
the least.
Therefore, the most logical interpretation of the change of sexual orientation
observed is that it was caused by the action of prenatal androgens. Of course,
adrenal hyperplasia is a rare disease and cannot play a role in determining the
sexual orientation of the majority of lesbians. Also note that the effect of prenatal
androgens in CAH girls, even if it is very significant, has moderate amplitude.
Only about 30% of the population of affected girls is not strictly heterosexual,
and statistical calculation of the effect size of this difference (a measure of the
difference with a control population taking into account the difference between
means and the normal variation of this characteristic; see Chapter 5 for the def-
inition of that term) demonstrates that it is five times smaller than the effect of
the adrenal hyperplasia on children’s games, for example (Hines, 2006). That
said, the results nonetheless fit within the expected parameters.
The potential limits to the effects of embryonic androgens on sexual orienta-
tion might include the existence of limited periods of sensitivity to androgens
that overlap only partially with the times during which these hormones are
present in high concentrations in the blood (problem of time-response); the fact
Sexual Orientation in Clinical Cases 135
that children’s games are more sensitive to androgens than sexual orientation
(problem of dose-response); or finally that the part of the sexual orientation
controlled by androgens is limited, and other prenatal or postnatal factors,
including environmental or social and parental influences, are required. There
is no way to discriminate between these interpretations at present.
40
Homosexual orientation (%)
30
20
10
DEFICIENCY IN 5 α -REDUCTASE
As we saw in Chapter 6, a genetic deficiency affects an enzyme called 5α-reductase
that converts testosterone into dihydrotestosterone (DHT). The patients are
exposed during fetal life to estrogen and androgen (testosterone) but not DHT.
At birth, affected boys have poorly masculinized or nonmasculinized external
genitalia, and they adopt a female gender identity in their childhood. Under the
influence of massive secretions of testosterone that occur during puberty, their
genitals are partially masculinized, and these children usually change their sexual
identity in adulthood to live as men. This change of identity and gender role has
even been observed in some individuals who had been married very young to a
man as a girl and who remarried with a woman after puberty.
Sexual Orientation in Clinical Cases 137
CLOACAL EXSTROPHY
Cloacal exstrophy is a rare, complex, genito-urinary malformation occurring
during embryonic development that results in the birth of XY males who, in
addition to various malformations of the pelvis, have no penis. This is not an
endocrine disease, in that the testes are apparently normal both physically and
functionally. In many cases, the XY individuals are assigned at birth a female sex
138 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
both at the legal and social levels. They are also subjected to corrective surgery,
including removal of the testes and vaginoplasty. Several recent studies have fol-
lowed the psychosexual development of affected subjects, and one study observed
that in a significant number of cases (sometimes up to 55%, 8/14), adult subjects
chose to adopt a male identity and gender role (Reiner & Gearhart, 2004; Meyer-
Bahlburg, 2005). A clear masculinization of games played by these XY children
reared as girls was also present [preference for highly physical sports (football)
or aggressive sports (karate)] (Schober et al., 2002; Reiner & Gearhart, 2004).
In many cases, a typical male sexual orientation (attraction to women) was also
observed.
These data again suggest that hormonal imprinting of the embryo by andro-
gens could be responsible, at least in part, for the determinism of sexually dif-
ferentiated characteristics such as gender identity and sexual orientation. This
would result in a significant increase in the change of gender identity and sexual
orientation later in life in subjects exposed in utero to androgens, despite the
fact that they have subsequently been raised as girls. This change, however, does
not concern all individuals. If this type of change is consistent with a role of
prenatal androgens, it also indicates that androgens are probably not the only
determinant and the postnatal environment is potentially also involved. I will
come back to this idea.
IN CONCLUSION
I have in this chapter reviewed a number of medical conditions that are all
related, at least in part, to sexual orientation at odds with the genetic sex of the
affected subjects. In several of these conditions, a sexual orientation was finally
adopted that was in opposition to the sex assigned at birth, but was consistent
with the type of presumed hormonal exposure during intrauterine life (attrac-
tion to men given absence of androgens and to women given presence of andro-
gens during embryogenesis). Most of the effects described here were reproduced
in independent studies on populations of unrelated subjects, which adds to their
credibility. It must be recognized that, occasionally, these effects could not be
replicated in other research. This is not necessarily surprising given the com-
plexity of these problems, including the selection of subjects and their controls
and the difficulties associated with an accurate determination of gender identity
and sexual orientation in subjects whose education and hormonal history were
potentially disturbed.
Taken together, these data suggest that embryonic hormones could, in humans
as in animals, play a significant role in determining sexual orientation. This con-
clusion is consistent with the results presented in the previous section indicating
that homosexual orientation is often associated with a change in physical, func-
tional, or behavioral traits supposed to differentiate themselves under the influ-
ence of prenatal sex steroids.
The most convincing medical cases suggesting a role of embryonic hormones
on sexual orientation are those in which hormonal effects and education are sup-
posed to pull in opposite directions, such as in congenital adrenal gland hyper-
trophy, prenatal DES exposure, 5α-reductase deficiency, and cloacal exstrophy.
As in any clinical study, it is impossible to be completely sure that the sex in
which subjects were educated was entirely consistent with what was desired.
It would be surprising that in all cases, parents have unintentionally encouraged
the development of a sexual attraction and sexual identity that would be con-
trary to their intentions (XX girls with adrenal hyperplasia raised as girls who
become attracted to girls, XY boys with 5α-reductase deficiency or cloacal exstro-
phy raised as girls but adopting male gender identity and male sexual orientation
140 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
Attraction Attraction
for men for women
Threshold
concentration
Number of subjects
Women Men
Heterosexual Heterosexual
women men
Low High
(T Concentration during
the critical period)
Homosexual Homosexual
men women
Figure 10.1 Model showing how the fluctuations around the average concentration of
testosterone during embryonic life could lead to a homosexual orientation in a fraction
of the population.
A Genetic or Immunological Mechanism Underlying Homosexuality? 143
100 Monozygotic
Dizygotic
80
% of concordance
60
40
20
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 Mean
Studies
Figure 10.2 Percentage of concordance of homosexual orientation among true twins
(monozygotic) or false (dizygotic) twins observed in six independent studies involving a
total of 270 pairs of true and 271 pairs of false fraternal twins and average of the results.
Note that a concordance of 100% does not indicate the presence of 100% of homosexuals
in the population, but only that if in a twin pair one of the subjects was homosexual, the
other was as well (according to data in Diamond, 1993).
more recent studies using more sophisticated methods of selection have resulted
in a qualitatively similar conclusion showing a greater concordance of sexual
orientation between true than in fraternal twins. Taken together, these studies
indicate that 50–60% of the variance in sexual orientation in men is genetic in
the socio-cultural conditions typical of Western societies (LeVay & Hamer,
1994; Rahman & Wilson, 2003a; Swaab, 2007). These studies may still be influ-
enced by bias in the recruitment of subjects. Two recent very strict studies based
on very large populations of Australian and U.S. twins show a concordance
between monozyotic twins that is substantially lower, equal to about 30%.
A separate analysis of Australian data also shows a concordance of 26% for men
but 58% for women (see Rahman & Wilson, 2003a for more detail).
Whatever the precise value hiding behind these estimates, it is clear that all
studies show a better concordance in real than in fraternal twins. This difference
should be genetic. Identical twins in fact developed from the splitting of a single
fertilized egg and therefore share (in first approximation) exactly the same genetic
material, whereas dizygotic twins are not more similar genetically than siblings
born at different times. This genetic identity is likely to cause the better concor-
dance of sexual orientation. The only alternative explanation would be to assume
A Genetic or Immunological Mechanism Underlying Homosexuality? 145
that the two types of twins are raised in a different way (that is partially the case)
and that these educational differences induce a better concordance in sexual
orientation. Given the lack of data demonstrating a role of education on sexual
orientation (Green, 1978), this interpretation seems unlikely at present. Absolute
demonstration of the genetic interpretation of these data could be obtained by
comparing two types of twins separated at birth, but due to the extreme rarity of
this combination of already minority events (frequency of twins, homosexuality,
separation at birth), this study will probably always remain impossible.
100
Xq28 alleles
p=0.0001 Not shared
80 Shared
p=0.04 p=0.04 p=0.0001
60
Percentage
n.s.
40
20
0
Hamer Hu Sanders Rice Total
et al. et al. et al. et al. q28
1993 1995 2003 1999
Figure 10.3 Four independent studies indicate a link between the transmission of
male homosexuality and genetic markers located in the Xq28 region of chromosome X.
The diagram on the right shows the location at the subtelomeric end of this set of genes
on chromosome X (redrawn from Bocklandt & Vilain, 2007).
that this last study confirmed the link with maternal inheritance but had a lower
statistical power and was therefore less likely to identify a significant link with
Xq28 (Bocklandt & Vilain, 2007).
A comprehensive analysis of all available data, including negative results,
however, indicates that approximately 64% of gay brothers have common alleles
in the Xq28 region of chromosome X (Figure 10.3) (Bocklandt & Vilain, 2007).
This association is significantly associated with a probability of less than 0.0001,
which means there is less of a chance in ten thousand that the association
appeared by chance in the data and does not reflect a real connection. That
being said, it should be made immediately clear that this chromosomal region
remains quite broad and could potentially contain many genes. We are still far
from the identification of the responsible genes.
to try unravel the genetic mechanisms potentially involved. A recent study has
particularly been interested in the phenomenon of gene inactivation in the X
chromosome. The male cells indeed contain a single copy of the X chromosome,
while female cells contain two. So women (females) should theoretically pro-
duce a double amount of proteins whose genes are located on the X chromo-
some as compared to what is seen in men (male). To counteract this, each cell of
a female embryo inactivates randomly one of the two X chromosomes. This
inactivation is random in most cells, and it remains present in all cells that derive
from a given cell in the adult tissues: all derived cells have one or the other X
chromosome inactivated. Comparing this inactivation in mothers of homosex-
ual or heterosexual sons, Bockland and collaborators have recently shown that
inactivation showing an extreme asymmetry was present more frequently in the
first group (13 of 97 = 13% in mothers of homosexuals) than in the second (4 of
103 = 4% in mothers of heterosexuals) and even more frequently in mothers of
two or more gay boys (10/44 = 23%) (Bocklandt et al., 2006).
It is not known whether this unusual pattern of X chromosome inactivation
is partly responsible for the homosexual orientation of the sons or is a conse-
quence of an unidentified mechanism that directs this orientation. One possible
explanation would be that one or more genetic factors that influence sexual
orientation could also alter the survival of cells in the mother (white blood cells
or stem cells), leading to a selection of cells that inactivate the one or the other
allele. The fact remains that these data indicate that a mechanism that affects
male sexual orientation is visible in the blood of the mothers. The unusual inac-
tivation of the X chromosome among mothers of homosexuals also reinforces
the idea that this chromosome influences sexual orientation.
The influence of the X chromosome is only partial and it is therefore likely
that other genes located on nonsex chromosomes (autosomes) are also involved.
A gene-linking study taking into account the entire genome was undertaken
recently through advances in techniques of molecular biology (Mustanski et al.,
2005). Unlike the studies focused on the X chromosome that only considered
subjects with proven maternal transmission, this study did not use any criterion
for exclusion of individuals. By mathematical techniques too complex to explain
here, the study allowed for the identification of the maternal and paternal con-
tribution to the transmission of homosexuality by the measure of a score called
Lod (logarithm of odds). The largest parental genetic contributions to sexual
orientation have been identified on chromosome 7 (position 7q36, maternal
and paternal inheritance roughly equal) and chromosome 8 (position 8p12,
again equal inheritance from both parents). An exclusively maternal effect was
also identified on chromosome 10 (10q26). A similar study on a larger scale
(more than 1,000 gay and heterosexual controls) is under way to confirm these
results (see Bocklandt & Vilain, 2007; Ngun et al., 2011 for details).
148 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
% of homosexuals 70
60
50
40
0 1 2 3 ≥4
Number of older brothers
Figure 10.4 Relationship between the number of older brothers born to the same
mother and the percentage of gay men in a population of 302 gay men and 302
heterosexual matched subjects used as controls. Because the homosexual and
heterosexual subjects were matched for this study, there was 50% of homosexuals
in the population studied, which is of course much higher than what is found in
a normal population (redrawn from Blanchard & Bogaert, 1996).
that a fourth boy in a family is gay is thus about twice higher than for the first-
born boy (Blanchard, 1997; Blanchard, 2004). Such a correlation could have
many causes and interpretations. But given the robustness of the phenomenon
and because it has been known for many years, it was possible by statistical
analysis to exclude most of these alternative interpretations, and the authors of
these studies now focus on an immune theory (Bogaert & Skorska, 2011), which
I discuss below. Let us first consider all the most likely factors that have been
considered but do not appear to explain this correlation.
It has been shown that the effect of older brothers is not observed for younger
brothers (no increase in homosexuality, according to the number of brothers
born after the proband) nor for sisters whether they are born before or after the
proband. The effect does not depend on the number of brothers that were raised
at the same time as the proband (a social effect that would be derived from an
infancy spent with a lot of other boys) but only on the number of brothers pre-
viously born from the same mother (Bogaert, 2006). This effect is not found in
girls. There is no increase in female homosexuality associated with the number
of brothers or sisters born before (or after) the subject. All these data suggest a
specific link with the sex of the children and that the sequence of births rather
than the number of boys in a family is the relevant factor. This tends to show
150 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
that the cause is not an educational factor but rather a biological factor associ-
ated with multiple pregnancies. This notion is further strengthened by more
recent analyses showing that the effect of older brothers (born before) is still
observed if the subjects considered are raised in different families (because of
divorce and reconstituted families), but that this effect does not occur for half-
brothers (born to a different mother) or adopted brothers (Blanchard et al.,
2006). These data therefore exclude, as effectively as possible in a psychological
study in humans, the influence of postnatal educational effects of numerous
siblings.
Furthermore, we know that parental age affects some characteristics of chil-
dren, the best-known example being the rate of trisomy 21 (a genetic disorder
that is one of two causes of Down syndrome), which increases with parental
age. Since boys who have many brothers were born to mothers (and fathers) on
average older than subjects with fewer older brothers, one would also have
thought that the age of the parents was the explanatory factor. However, given
the large samples (more than 10,000 subjects available), detailed statistical anal-
ysis (by multiple correlations and partial regressions) could be used to reject
this idea.
Having rejected over the years all the alternative hypotheses that could explain
this effect, the only remaining explanation at present is based on a type of
immune response of the pregnant mother. This interpretation considers that the
mother who carries male embryos develops a progressive immune response
over successive pregnancies against these embryos, which are considered a for-
eign body that secretes foreign proteins in greater quantities than female embryos
(Blanchard & Bogaert, 1996; Blanchard & Klassen, 1997). This is the hypothesis
of progressive maternal immunization. The accumulation of antibodies is simi-
lar to that seen if a Rhesus-negative mother sequentially carries in her womb
several Rhesus-positive embryos. The first embryo is almost unaffected, but the
following ones are quite severely attacked by the maternal antibodies and must
often be prematurely removed from the uterus by Caesarean section or some-
times have to undergo an complete change of blood at birth to prevent damage
caused by maternal antibodies. The immune hypothesis to explain the effect of
older brothers assumes that mothers who had boys previously produce antibod-
ies against unidentified male proteins and that these antibodies affect the devel-
opment of certain aspects of the brain that are involved in determining sexual
orientation.
Consistent with this hypothesis, it was shown that the birth weight of boys
with older brothers is significantly lower than the weight of matched boys with
the same number of older sisters (Blanchard & Klassen, 1997; Blanchard, 2001).
Moreover, within this population of subjects with older brothers, gay men had a
lower body weight at birth than heterosexual men (about 6 ounces of difference,
A Genetic or Immunological Mechanism Underlying Homosexuality? 151
which is low but significant in the sample). These differences may be explained
by the effects of hypothetical antibodies produced by the mother against the
antigens typical of the male.
To mediate such an immune effect on the development of sexual orientation,
a protein should theoretically fulfill a number of criteria (Bogaert & Skorska,
2011). Namely: (1) it should be expressed more or less specifically in the brain,
since sexual orientation is obviously controlled by the brain and homosexuality
is not associated with any major change in body structure or function, (2) it
should be expressed at the surface of brain cells in order to be accessible to
maternal antibodies, (3) it should be a protein specific to males, (4) fetal mate-
rial containing this protein should enter the maternal circulation and should
induce the formation of antibodies in the mother and (5) the concentration of
these antibodies in the maternal circulation should increase with the number of
male embryos that the mother has been exposed to. A small number of proteins
have been identified that fulfill most of but not all these criteria. However, there
is as of this date no formal proof that these proteins actually play a role in the
control of sexual orientation and in particular in the increased incidence of
homosexuality associated with the older brother effect. It is therefore premature
to discuss these proeins here, but the interested reader can conveniently find a
full discussion of this topic in a recent review (Bogaert & Skorska, 2011).
In summary, the mechanism underlying the effect of older brothers, and more
specifically its immune aspects, remains unknown to date. It is possible to imag-
ine an effect of the antibodies (which are known to penetrate the embryo) on the
expression of genes and their interaction with the environment that are indis-
pensable for the construction of an individual. However, there is no evidence of
interaction with a genetic system, and the possible immune effects could result
only from interactions with membrane proteins. Nevertheless, the effect of older
brothers is without any doubt an argument of great weight supporting the idea
that sexual orientation is significantly determined before birth.
assistance could have favored the survival of children and thus contributed to the
transmission of genes promoting homosexuality. Many animal societies have
been identified in which some individuals do not reproduce, but simply help raise
the offspring of their close relatives. This reproduction, based on a few individuals
helped by others closely related to them genetically, is almost always observed in
difficult environmental conditions (e.g., deserts) and is expected to be maintained
because the nonbreeding individuals still spread their genes through the repro-
duction of their close relatives. This seemingly altruistic behavior would be still
“interested” from an evolutionary point of view.
It is also possible that genes controlling homosexuality are only harmful to
reproduction and therefore have a tendency to disappear on a continuous basis.
They would be maintained in humans by repeated mutations that would fre-
quently make them reappear in a more or less similar form. Sites on the chromo-
somes are known that are actually subject to very frequent mutations, but so far
none has been associated with homosexuality.
It is clear that as long as the gene or genes favoring homosexuality have not
been identified, it remains impossible to test these different interpretations. They
are presented here simply to show that the existence of a gene to a phenotypic trait
obviously associated with a decreased reproductive success is not inconceivable.
11
General Conclusions
At the end of this journey into the world of neurobiology in relation to sexual-
ity, it is important to summarize the accumulated knowledge and the questions
that remain unanswered, and then see how scientific information collected over
the past 20 or 30 years can and should probably change certain aspects of med-
ical practice and also, in a much more important manner, the general attitude
of society toward homosexuals (and also probably transsexuals).
In Animals
1. The action of hormones on the hypothalamus and preoptic area in
rodents and primates precisely determines the type of sexual
behavior (male or female) expressed by an individual. This
determination is largely independent of the genetic sex of the
animal. It is controlled by the type of hormonal imprinting
experienced during embryonic and/or immediately postpartum life
General Conclusions 155
In Humans
5. The sex steroids and their receptors in the human brain are similar
or identical to what they are in animals. One can find in the human
species all the embryonic effects of sex steroids on genital structures
that are identified in animals. Effects of steroids are also present in
the activation of sexual behavior even though they are, as might be
expected given the complexity of human sexuality, more nuanced
than in animals.
6. The sexual differentiation of genital structures during embryonic life
precedes by several months the differentiation of the brain, the organ
controlling behavior. It is therefore possible that hormonal changes
occurring between these two embryonic periods can induce
discrepancies between physical sex and aspects of sexual behavior
that are sexually differentiated (orientation, gender identity).
Such discrepancies could also originate in a differential sensitivity
of responses to steroids (dose-response difference) or in the
existence of different sensitive periods to the action of steroids.
7. Homosexuality is not simply a different sexual orientation; it is
accompanied by complex physical, functional, and behavioral
changes. Homosexuality therefore affects not only a particular aspect
156 T H E B I O L O G Y O F H O M O S E X UA L I T Y
Taken together, all these data thus strongly suggest that hormonal, genetic,
and possibly immunological factors acting to a large extent during the embryonic
life or during early infancy play an import role in the determination of sexual
orientation.
These negative results do not necessarily imply the complete absence of influ-
ence of the postnatal environment on the development of sexual orientation,
but available data strongly suggest that the environment alone cannot deter-
mine homosexuality or heterosexuality, though it is quite conceivable that it
may interact with prenatal biological factors that I have outlined to allow their
full expression. Studies that could potentially identify such interactions should
probably be based on extremely large numbers of subjects, given the low ampli-
tude of the effects studied.
set of psychosocial contexts that are not yet identified. But if this postnatal con-
text is actually an important permissive factor, it is surprising that a quantitative
study has been unable so far to identify aspects of the environment that are
limiting.
Current knowledge does not allow us to discriminate between these interpre-
tations. However, it is clear that biological factors acting during prenatal life
play a significant role in determining sexual orientation and that homosexuality
is not, for most people, a choice of life. This orientation is often or always
an evidence that imposes itself on the individual during his or her teens or life
as a young adult. The recognition of a nonconventional sexual orientation is
very often the occasion of significant psychological suffering. Remember the
suicide rate that is three times higher during adolescence among homosexuals
as compared to the general population. By contrast, the heterosexual orienta-
tion develops spontaneously, often while the individual does not truly realize it.
It is not a matter of choice here. One does not choose to be homosexual any
more than one chooses to be heterosexual. We can choose to accept this orienta-
tion, to act accordingly, and to reveal it or not to the society, but the orientation
itself is not in any way a deliberate choice. Given the complexity of the human
person, this does not mean of course that there is not a minority of gay men for
whom this sexual orientation is a choice of life influenced by past experiences
and/or motivated by various reasons ranging from curiosity to perversity.
There are probably sex perverts among homosexuals, just as there are among
heterosexuals, but homosexuality itself is not a perversion. A large proportion
of homosexuals are born with that sexual orientation, which is revealed to them
in a very progressive way during development and is often accepted at the price
of a significant psychological distress. It is for most of them not a choice (how
could a homosexual change by personal choice his D2:D4 ratio or the function-
ing of his or her inner ear?). Homosexuality is due neither to a perversity nor to
inadequate parents. It is a biological variation of a complex behavioral trait
whose control is obviously multifactorial.
It also should be noted that understanding does not necessarily mean want-
ing to control. Understanding is part of science; the choice of the use of knowl-
edge is a question of morality and politics. It is very important that scientists be
involved in the political choices that are made, particularly when they concern
the use of science and technology, but the two aspects of human activity should
not be confused. It would be foolish to abandon the pursuit of knowledge
because it could later be misused. If this were the case, then we would have had
to abandon the discovery of the structure of the atom. This knowledge has
brought significant benefits (radiotherapy, an enormous source of energy) as
well as major disasters (the atomic bomb). It is up to societies to decide what
they do with scientific knowledge. In our view, understanding the biological
mechanisms that control human behavior should have more positive than neg-
ative effects on the happiness of the human species.
Finally, we must recognize that there are homosexuals who claim their
lifestyle and say, contrary to the argument presented in this book, that their
sexual orientation reflects a free choice of life. I respect this attitude and give
everyone the right to do what he or she wants with the data presented here. The
level of biological determinism that has been identified to date remains impre-
cise and can leave a place for individual choice. Large interindividual variations
in the determinism of homosexuality probably exist and homosexuality is not,
in all probability, a uniform phenomenon. If some think that they are homo-
sexual by choice, there is no reason to try to dissuade them, and it is possible
that they are right. In many other cases, however, homosexuality is seen as
a “defect” or a perversion that is in conflict with deep religious or moral convic-
tions. For many homosexuals, homosexuality was never an option but a differ-
ence difficult to accept and a challenge for social integration. To all these people,
we should be able to say that the difference is essentially the same as the differ-
ence that affects people in relation to their stature, eye color, or hair color.
Determinism of sexual orientation is simply more complex than that of these
physical characteristics.
If we accept these premises, it becomes apparent that homosexuals and their
parents are not responsible for their sexual orientation and there is no objective
reason to reject them. Homosexuals are not perverse (at least not more than
straight people), they are not dangerous (homosexuality is not “contagious”),
and they are generally not responsible for their condition. They should be able
to live their lives according to their nature (biology) without worrying about
issues of personal guilt. In return, society should, as it is increasingly but not yet
uniformly doing, accept them without any form of discrimination.
REFERENCES
Abdelgadir, S.E., Roselli, C.E., Choate, J.V. & Resko, J.A. (1999) Androgen receptor
messenger ribonucleic acid in brains and pituitaries of male rhesus monkeys: studies
on distribution, hormonal control, and relationship to luteinizing hormone secre-
tion. Biol Reprod, 60, 1251–1256.
Adkins-Regan, E. (1999) Testosterone increases singing and aggression but not male-
typical sexual partner preference in early estrogen treated female zebra finches.
Horm. Behav., 35, 63–70.
Adkins-Regan, E. (2011) Neuroendocrine contributions to sexual partner preference in
birds. Front Neuroendocrinol, 32, 155–163.
Adkins-Regan, E. & Ascenzi, M. (1987) Social and sexual behaviour of male and female
zebra finches treated with oestradiol during the nestling period. Anim. Behav., 35,
1100–1112.
Adkins-Regan, E. & Wade, J. (2001) Masculinized sexual partner preference in female
zebra finches with sex-reversed gonads. Horm. Behav., 39, 22–28.
Adkins-Regan, E., Yang, S. & Mansukhani, V. (1996) Behavior of male and female zebra
finches treated with an estrogen synthesis inhibitor as nestlings. Behaviour, 133,
847–862.
Agmo, A. (2007) Functional and dysfunctional sexual behavior. A synthesis of neurosci-
ence and comparative psychology. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Ahmed, E.I., Zehr, J.L., Schulz, K.M., Lorenz, B.H., DonCarlos, L.L. & Sisk, C.L. (2008)
Pubertal hormones modulate the addition of new cells to sexually dimorphic brain
regions. Nat. Neurosci., 11, 995–997.
Alexander, B.M., Rose, J.D., Stellflug, J.N., Fitzgerald, J.A. & Moss, G.E. (1999) Behavior
and endocrine changes in high-performing, low-performing, and male-oriented
domestic rams following exposure to rams and ewes in estrus when copulation is
precluded. J. Anim. Sci., 77, 1869–1874.
Alexander, G.M. & Hines, M. (2002) Sex diferences in response to children’s toys in non-
human primates (Cercopithecus aethios sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior,
23, 467–479.
Allen, L.S. & Gorski, R.A. (1992) Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior
commissure in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 89, 7199–7202.
164 References
Allen, L.S., Hines, M., Shryne, J.E. & Gorski, R.A. (1989) Two sexually dimorphic
cell groups in the human brain. J. Neurosci, 9, 497–506.
Anderson, D.K., Rhees, R.W. & Fleming, D.E. (1985) Effects of prenatal stress on
differentiation of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA) of
the rat brain. Brain Res., 332, 113–118.
Arnold, A.P. & Chen, X. (2009) What does the “four core genotypes” mouse model
tell us about sex differences in the brain and other tissues? Front Neuroendocrinol,
30, 1–9.
Arnold, A.P. & Gorski, R.A. (1984) Gonadal steroid induction of structural sex differ-
ences in the central nervous system. Ann. Rev. Neurosci., 7, 413–442.
Arnold, A.P., Xu, J., Grisham, W., Chen, X.Q., Kim, Y.H. & Itoh, Y. (2004) Minireview:
Sex chromosomes and brain sexual differentiation. Endocrinology, 145, 1057–1062.
Bagatell, C.J., Heiman, J.R., Rivier, J.E. & Bremner, W.J. (1994) Effects of endogenous
testosterone and estradiol on sexual behavior in normal young men. J. Clin. Endocrinol.
Metab., 78, 711–716.
Bagemihl, B. (1999) Biological exuberance. Animal homosexuality and natural diversity.
St. Martin’s Press, New York.
Bailey, J.M., Willerman, L. & Parks, C. (1991) A test of the maternal stress theory of
human male homosexuality. Arch Sex. Behav., 20, 277–293.
Bakker, J., Baum, M.J. & Slob, A.K. (1996a) Neonatal inhibition of brain estrogen
synthesis alters adult neural Fos responses to mating and pheromonal stimulation in
the male rat. Neuroscience, 74, 251–260.
Bakker, J., Brand, T., van Ophemert, J. & Slob, A.K. (1993a) Hormonal regulation of
adult partner preference behavior in neonatally ATD-treated male rats. Behav.
Neurosci., 107, 480–487.
Bakker, J. & Brock, O. (2010) Early oestrogens in shaping reproductive networks:
evidence for a potential organisational role of oestradiol in female brain develop-
ment. J. Neuroendocrinol, 22, 728–735.
Bakker, J., De Mees, C., Douhard, Q., Balthazart, J., Gabant, P., Szpirer, J. & Szpirer, C.
(2006) Alpha-fetoprotein protects the developing female mouse brain from mascu-
linization and defeminization by estrogens. Nat Neurosci., 9, 220–226.
Bakker, J., Honda, S., Harada, N. & Balthazart, J. (2002a) Sexual partner preference
requires a functional aromatase (cyp19) gene in male mice. Horm. Behav., 42,
158–171.
Bakker, J., Honda, S.I., Harada, N. & Balthazart, J. (2002b) The aromatase knock-out
mouse provides new evidence that estradiol is required during development in the
female for the expression of sociosexual behaviors in adulthood. J. Neurosci., 22,
9104–9112.
Bakker, J., Van Ophemert, J. & Slob, A.K. (1993b) Organization of partner preference
and sexual behavior and its nocturnal rhythmicity in male rats. Behav. Neurosci., 107,
1049–1058.
Bakker, J., Van Ophemert, J. & Slob, A.K. (1996b) Sexual differentiation of odor and
partner preference in the rat. Physiol. Behav., 60, 489–494.
Balthazart, J. & Ball, G.F. (2006) Is brain estradiol a hormone or a neurotransmitter?
Trends Neurosci., 29, 241–249.
References 165
Balthazart, J. & Ball, G.F. (2007) Topography in the preoptic region: differential regula-
tion of appetitive and consummatory male sexual behaviors. Front Neuroendocrinol,
28, 161–178.
Balthazart, J., Cornil, C.A., Charlier, T.D., Taziaux, M. & Ball, G.F. (2009) Estradiol,
a key endocrine signal in the sexual differentiation and activation of reproductive
behavior in quail. J Exp Zool Part A Ecol Genet Physiol. 311, 323–345.
Bancroft, J. (1995) The pharmacology of sexual function and dysfunction. Elsevier
Science, Amsterdam.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2004) The Essential Difference: Men, Women and the Extreme Male
Brain. Penguin Press Science, London.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2006) Prenatal testosterone in mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Barres., B.A. (2010) Neuro Nonsense. Plos Biology, 8, e1001005.
Baum, M.J., Carroll, R.S., Erskine, M.S. & Tobet, S.A. (1985) Neuroendocrine response
to estrogen and sexual orientation. Science, 230, 960–961.
Becker, J.B., Berkley, K.J., Geary, N., Hampson, E., Herman, J.P. & Young, E.A. (2008)
Sex differences in the brain. From Genes to behavior. Oxford University Press,
Oxford.
Becker, J.B., Breedlove, S.M., Crews, D. & McCarthy, M.M. (2002) Behavioral
Endocrinology. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.
Bell, A.P., Weinberg, M.S. & Hammersmith, S.K. (1981) Sexual preference: It’s develop-
ment in men and women. Indiana University Press, Bloomington.
Benbow, C.P. (1993) Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability in intellectually
talented preadolescents: Their nature, effects and possible causes. BBS 11:169–232.
Behav. Brain Sci., 16, 187–189.
Bénesteau, J. (2002) Mensonges Freudiens. Pierre Mardaga, Wavre (Belgique).
Berenbaum, S.A., Bryk, K.K., Nowak, N., Quigley, C.A. & Moffat, S. (2009) Fingers as
a marker of prenatal androgen exposure. Endocrinology, 150, 5119–5124.
Berenbaum, S.A., Duck, S.C. & Bryk, K. (2000) Behavioral effects of prenatal versus
postnatal androgen excess in children with 21-hydroxylase-deficient congenital
adrenal hyperplasia. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 85, 727–733.
Berenbaum, S.A. & Hines, M. (1992) Early androgens are related to childhood sex-
typed toy preferences. Psychol. Sci., 3, 203–206.
Berenbaum, S.A. & Snyder, E. (1995) Early hormonal influences on childhood sex-
typed activity and playmate preferences: implications for the development of sexual
orientation. Developmental Psychology, 31, 31–42.
Berglund, H., Lindstrom, P. & Savic, I. (2006) Brain response to putative pheromones in
lesbian women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A, 103, 8269–8274.
Berta, P., Hawkins, J.R., Sinclair, A.H., Taylor, A., Griffiths, B.L., Goodfellow, P.N. &
Fellous, M. (1990) Genetic evidence equating SRY and the testis-determining factor.
Nature, 348, 448–450.
Berthold, A.A. (1849) Transplantation der Hoden. Arch. F. Anat. U. Physiol., 16, 42–46.
Blanchard, R. (1997) Birth order and sibling sex ration in homosexual versus hetero-
sexual males and females. Ann. Rev. Sex Res. 8, 27–67.
Blanchard, R. (2001) Fraternal birth order and the maternal immune hypothesis of
male homosexuality. Horm. Behav., 40, 105–114.
166 References
Blanchard, R. (2004) Quantitative and theoretical analyses of the relation between older
brothers and homosexuality in men. J. Theor. Biol. 230, 173–187.
Blanchard, R. & Bogaert, A.F. (1996) Homosexuality in men and number of older broth-
ers. Am. J. Psychiatry, 153, 27–31.
Blanchard, R., Cantor, J.M., Bogaert, A.F., Breedlove, S.M. & Ellis, L. (2006) Interaction
of fraternal birth order and handedness in the development of male homosexuality.
Horm. Behav., 49, 405–414.
Blanchard, R. & Klassen, P. (1997) H-Y antigen and homosexuality in men. J. Theor.
Biol., 185, 373–378.
Blanchard, R. & Lippa, R.A. (2007) Birth order, sibling sex ratio, handedness, and sex-
ual orientation of male and female participants in a BBC internet research project.
Arch. Sex. Behav., 36, 163–176.
Bloch, G.J. & Gorski, R.A. (1988a) Cytoarchitectonic analysis of the SDN-POA of
the intact and gonadectomized rat. J. Comp. Neurol., 275, 604–612.
Bloch, G.J. & Gorski, R.A. (1988b) Estrogen/progesterone treatment in adulthood affects
the size of several components of the medial preoptic area in the male rat. J. Comp.
Neurol., 275, 613–622.
Bocklandt, S., Horvath, S., Vilain, E. & Hamer, D.H. (2006) Extreme skewing of
X chromosome inactivation in mothers of homosexual men. Hum. Genet., 118,
691–694.
Bocklandt, S. & Vilain, E. (2007) Sex differences in brain and behavior: hormones versus
genes. Adv. Genet., 59, 245–266.
Bodo, C. & Rissman, E.F. (2007) Androgen receptor is essential for sexual differentia-
tion of responses to olfactory cues in mice. Eur. J. Neurosci., 25, 2182–2190.
Bodo, C. & Rissman, E.F. (2008) The androgen receptor is selectively involved in
organization of sexually dimorphic social behaviors in mice. Endocrinology, 149,
4142–4150.
Bogaert, A.F. (2006) Biological versus nonbiological older brothers and men’s sexual
orientation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A, 103, 10771–10774.
Bogaert, A.F. & Skorska, M. (2011) Sexual Orientation, Fraternal Birth Order, and the
Maternal Immune Hypothesis: A Review of Evidence. Front Neuroendocrinol, 32,
247–254.
Bradley, S.J., Oliver, G.D., Chernick, A.B. & Zucker, K.J. (1998) Experiment of nurture:
ablatio penis at 2 months, sex reassignment at 7 months, and a psychosexual fol-
low-up in young adulthood. Pediatrics, 102, 1–5.
Breedlove, S.M. (2010) Minireview: Organizational hypothesis: instances of the finger-
post. Endocrinology, 151, 4116–4122.
Brock, O. & Bakker, J. (2011) Potential contribution of prenatal estrogens to the sexual
differentiation of mate preferences in mice. Horm. Behav., 59, 83–89.
Brown, W.M., Finn, C.J., Cooke, B.M. & Breedlove, S.M. (2002a) Differences in finger
length ratios between self-identified “butch” and “femme” lesbians. Arch. Sex. Behav.,
31, 123–127.
Brown, W.M., Hines, M., Fane, B.A. & Breedlove, S.M. (2002b) Masculinized finger
length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia.
Horm. Behav., 42, 380–386.
Byne, W., Tobet, S., Mattiace, L.A., Lasco, M.S., Kemether, E., Edgar, M.A., Morgello, S.,
Buchsbaum, M.S. & Jones, L.B. (2001) The interstitial nuclei of the human anterior
References 167
Davis, E.C., Shryne, J.E. & Gorski, R.A. (1995) A revised critical period for the sexual
differentiation of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area in the rat.
Neuroendocrinol., 62, 579–585.
de Lacoste-Utamsing, C. & Holloway, R.L. (1982) Sexual dimorphism in the human
corpus callosum. Science, 216, 1431–1432.
De Vries, G.J., Rissman, E.F., Simerly, R.B., Yang, L.Y., Scordalakes, E.M., Auger, C.J.,
Swain, A., Lovell-Badge, R., Burgoyne, P.S. & Arnold, A.P. (2002) A model system for
study of sex chromosome effects on sexually dimorphic neural and behavioral traits.
J. Neurosci., 22, 9005–9014.
Diamond, M. (1993) Some genetic considerations in the development of sexual orien-
tation. In Haug, M., Whalen, R.E., Aron, C., Olsen, K.L. (eds) The development of
sex differences and similarities in behavior. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht,
pp. 291–309.
Diamond, M. & Sigmundson, H.K. (1997) Sex reassignment at birth: long-term review
and clinical implications. Arch. Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 298–304.
Dittmann, R.W., Kappes, M.E. & Kappes, M.H. (1992) Sexual behavior in adolescent
and adult females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
17, 153–170.
Dörner, G. (1969) Zur Frage einer neuroendocrinen Pathogenese, Prophylaxe und
Therapie angeborenen Sexualdeviationen. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift, 94,
390–396.
Dörner, G. (1972) Sexualhormonabhängige Gehirndifferenzierung und Sexualität.
Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York.
Dörner, G. (1976) Hormones and Brain Differentiation. Elsevier, Amsterdam, Oxford,
New York.
Dörner, G. (1980) Sexual differentiation of the brain Vitamins and hormones. Academic
Press, Inc., pp. 325–381, New York.
Dörner, G., Geier, T., Ahrens, L., Krell, L., Munx, G., Sieler, H., Kittner, E. &
Muller, H. (1980) Prenatal stress as possible aetiogenetic factor of homosexuality in
human males. Endokrinologie, 75, 365–368.
Dörner, G., Schenk, B., Schmiedel, B. & Ahrens, L. (1983) Stressful events in prenatal
life of bi- and homosexual men. Exp Clin Endocrinol, 81, 83–87.
Dörner, G. & Staudt, J. (1969) Structural changes in the hypothalamic ventromedial
nucleus of the male rat, following neonatal castration and androgen treatment.
Neuroendocrinol., 4, 278–281.
Dubb, A., Gur, R., Avants, B. & Gee, J. (2003) Characterization of sexual dimorphism
in the human corpus callosum. Neuroimage, 20, 512–519.
Dufresne, T. (2007) Against Freud. Critics talk back. Stanford University Press,
Stan ford, CA.
Ehrhardt, A.A., Meyer-Bahlburg, H.F., Rosen, L.R., Feldman, J.F., Veridiano, N.P.,
Zimmerman, I. & McEwen, B.S. (1985) Sexual orientation after prenatal exposure to
exogenous estrogen. Arch. Sex. Behav., 14, 57–77.
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B. & Taub, E. (1995) Increased cor-
tical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270,
305–307.
References 169
Ellis, L., Ames, M.A., Peckham, W. & Ahrens, L. (1988) Sexual orientation of human
offspring may be altered by severe maternal stress during pregnancy. J. Sex. Res., 25,
152–157.
Ellis, L., Hershberger, S., Field, E., Wersinger, S., Pelis, S., Geary, D., Palmer, C.,
Hoyenga, K., Hetsroni, A. & Karadi, K. (2008) Sex differences: summarizing more than
a century of scientific research. Psychology Press, New York.
Ellis, L., Hoffman, H. & Burke, D.M. (1990) Sex, sexual orientation and criminal and
violent behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1207–1211.
Everitt, B.J. (1990) Sexual motivation: a neural and behavioural analysis of the mecha-
nisms underlying appetitive and copulatory responses in male rats. Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev., 14, 217–232.
Everitt, B.J. (1995) Neuroendocrine mechanisms underlying appetitive and consum-
matory elements of masculine sexual behavior. In Bancroft, J. (ed) The pharmacology
of sexual function and dysfunction. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 15–31.
Fernández-Guasti, A., Kruijver, F.P., Fodor, M. & Swaab, D.F. (2000) Sex differences in
the distribution of androgen receptors in the human hypothalamus. J. Comp. Neurol.,
425, 422–435.
Fine, C. (2010) Delusions of gender: how our minds, society and neurosexism create
difference. W.W. Norton and Company, New York.
Foidart, A., Legros, J.J. & Balthazart, J. (1994) Les phéromones humaines: vestige animal
ou réalité non reconnue. Revue Médicale de Liège, 49, 662–680.
Fraga, M.F., Ballestar, E., Paz, M.F., Ropero, S., Setien, F., Ballestar, M.L., Heine-
Suner, D., Cigudosa, J.C., Urioste, M., Benitez, J., Boix-Chornet, M., Sanchez-
Aguilera, A., Ling, C., Carlsson, E., Poulsen, P., Vaag, A., Stephan, Z., Spector, T.D.,
Wu, Y.Z., Plass, C. & Esteller, M. (2005) Epigenetic differences arise during the life-
time of monozygotic twins. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A, 102, 10604–10609.
Freund, K. & Blanchard, R. (1983) Is the distant relationship of fathers and homo-
sexual sons related to the son’s erotic preference for male partners, or to the sons’
atypical gender identity, or both? Journal of Homosexuality, 9, 7–25.
Freund, K.W. (1974) Male homosexuality: an analysis of the pattern. In Lorraine, J.A.
(ed) Understanding homosexuality: its biological and psychological bases. Elsevier,
Amsterdam, pp. 25–81.
Garcia-Falgueras, A. & Swaab, D.F. (2008) A sex difference in the hypothalamic unci-
nate nucleus: relationship to gender identity. Brain, 131, 3132–3146.
Gladue, B.A. (1985) Neuroendocrine response to estrogen and sexual orientation.
Science, 230, 961.
Gladue, B.A. & Bailey, J.M. (1995) Aggressiveness, competitiveness, and human sexual
orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinol., 20, 475.
Gladue, B.A., Beatty, W.W., Larson, J. & Staton, R.D. (1990) Sexual orientation and
spatial ability in men and women. Psychobiol., 18, 101–108.
Gladue, B.A., Green, R. & Hellman, R.E. (1984) Neuroendocrine response to estrogen
and sexual orientation. Science, 225, 1496–1499.
Gobrogge, K.L., Breedlove, S.M. & Klump, K.L. (2008) Genetic and environmental
influences on 2D:4D finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male
and female twins. Arch. Sex. Behav., 37, 112–118.
170 References
Gorski, R.A. (1984) Critical role of the medial preoptic area in the sexual differentiation
of the brain. In De Vries, G.J., De Bruin, J.P.C., Uylings, H.B.M., Corner, M.A. (eds)
Sex differences in the brain. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 129–146.
Gorski, R.A., Gordon, J.H., Shryne, J.E. & Southam, A.M. (1978) Evidence for a mor-
phological sex difference within the medial preoptic area of the rat brain. Brain Res.,
148, 333–346.
Goy, R.W. & McEwen, B.S. (1980) Sexual differentiation of the brain. The MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA.
Green, R. (1978) Sexual identity of 37 children raised by homosexual or transsexual
parents. Am. J. Psychiatry, 135, 692–697.
Grimbos, T., Dawood, K., Burriss, R.P., Zucker, K.J. & Puts, D.A. (2010) Sexual orienta-
tion and the second to fourth finger length ratio: a meta-analysis in men and women.
Behav. Neurosci., 124, 278–287.
Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P. & Zingales, L. (2008) Diversity. Culture, gender, and
math. Science, 320, 1164–1165.
Hajjar, R.R., Kaiser, F.E. & Morley, J.E. (1997) Outcomes of long-term testosterone
replacement in older hypogonadal males: a retrospective analysis. J. Clin. Endocrinol
Metab., 82, 3793–3796.
Hall, L.S. & Kimura, D. (1995) Sexual orientation and performance on sexually dimor-
phic motor tasks. Arch. Sex. Behav., 24, 395–407.
Hall, L.S. & Love, C.T. (2003) Finger-length ratios in female monozygotic twins discor-
dant for sexual orientation. Arch. Sex. Behav., 32, 23–28.
Halperin, D.M. (1990) One hundred years of homosexuality and other essays on Greek
love. Routledge, New York.
Halpern, C.T., Udry, J.R. & Suchindran, C. (1998) Monthly measures of salivary testos-
terone predict sexual activity in adolescent males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 27,
445–465.
Halpern, C.T., Udry, R. & Suchindran, C. (1997) Testosterone Predicts Initiation of
Coitus in Adolescent Females. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 161–171.
Halpern, M. & Martinez-Marcos, A. (2003) Structure and function of the vomeronasal
system: an update. Prog Neurobiol, 70, 245–318.
Hamer, D.H., Hu, S., Magnuson, V.L., Hu, N. & Pattatucci, A.M.L. (1993) A linkage
between DNA markers on the X chromosome and male sexual orientation. Science,
261, 321–327.
Hassett, J.M., Siebert, E.R. & Wallen, K. (2008) Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy
preferences parallel those of children. Horm. Behav., 54, 359–364.
Hefez, S. (2007) Dans le coeur des hommes. Hachette, Paris.
Henley, C.L., Nunez, A.A. & Clemens, L.G. (2009) Estrogen treatment during develop-
ment alters adult partner preference and reproductive behavior in female laboratory
rats. Horm. Behav., 55, 68–75.
Henley, C.L., Nunez, A.A. & Clemens, L.G. (2011) Hormones of choice: the neuroendo-
crinology of sexual orientation in animals. Front Neuroendocrinol, 32, 146–154.
Hepper, P.G., Shahidullah, S. & White, R. (1991) Handedness in the human fetus. Neuro-
psychologia, 29, 1107–1111.
Hines, M. (2003) Sex steroids and human behavior: prenatal androgen exposure and
sex-typical play behavior in children. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1007, 272–282.
References 171
Jimbo, M., Okubo, K., Toma, Y., Shimizu, Y., Saito, H. & Yanaihara, T. (1998) Inhibitory
effects of catecholamines and maternal stress on aromatase activity in the fetal rat
brain. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. Res., 24, 291–297.
Katz, J.N. (1976) Gay American History: Lesbians and Gay Men in the U.S.A. T.Y. Crowell,
New York.
Kelley, D.B. & Pfaff, D.W. (1978) Generalizations from comparative studies on neuroana-
tomical and endocrine mechanisms of sexual behaviour. In Hutchison, J.B. (ed) Biological
determinants of sexual behaviour. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, pp. 225–254.
Kerchner, M. & Ward, I.L. (1992) SDN-MPOA volume in male rats is decreased by
prenatal stress, but is not related to ejaculatory behavior. Brain Res., 581, 244–251.
Kessler, S.J. & McKenna, W. (1978) Gender: an ethno-methodological approach. Wiley,
New York.
Keverne, E.B. (1999) The vomeronasal organ. Science, 286, 716–720.
Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.R. & Martin, C.E. (1948) Sexual behavior in the human male.
W.B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia.
Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.R., Martin, C.E. & Gebhard, P.H. (1953) Sexual behavior in
the human female. Saunders, Philadelphia.
Kiumura, D. (1999) Sex and cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge.
Kourany, R.F. (1987) Suicide among homosexual adolescents. J. Homosex., 13, 111–117.
Kraemer, B., Noll, T., Delsignore, A., Milos, G., Schnyder, U. & Hepp, U. (2006) Finger
length ratio (2D:4D) and dimensions of sexual orientation. Neuropsychobiology, 53,
210–214.
Kruijver, F.P.M., Balesar, R., Espila, A.M., Unmehopa, U.A. & Swaab, D.F. (2002)
Estrogen receptor-alpha distribution in the human hypothalamus in relation to sex
and endocrine status. J. Comp. Neurol., 454, 115–139.
Kruijver, F.P.M., Balesar, R., Espila, A.M., Unmehopa, U.A. & Swaab, D.F. (2003)
Estrogen-receptor-beta distribution in the human hypothalamus: Similarities and
differences with ERalpha distribution. J. Comp. Neurol., 466, 251–277.
Kruijver, F.P.M., de Jonge, F.H., van den Broek, W.T., van der Woude, T., Endert, E.
& Swaab, D.F. (1993) Lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus do not disturb sexual
orientation of the adult male rat. Brain Res., 624, 342–346.
Kruijver, F.P.M., Fernández-Guasti, A., Fodor, M., Kraan, E.M. & Swaab, D.F. (2001)
Sex differences in androgen receptors of the human mamillary bodies are related to
endocrine status rather than to sexual orientation or transsexuality. J. Clin. Endocrinol.
Metab., 86, 818–827.
Lalumiere, M.L., Blanchard, R. & Zucker, K.J. (2000) Sexual orientation and handed-
ness in men and women: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull, 126, 575–592.
Lebson, M. (2002) Suicide among homosexual youth. J. Homosex., 42, 107–117.
Legros, J.J., Van Cauwenberge, H., Lambotte, R., Bauduin, A., Franchimont, P. &
Legros, J. (1974) Problèmes psycho-endocriniens posés par un cas d’hyperplasie sur-
rénalienne congénitale reconnu tardivement. Revue Médicale de Liège, 29, 73–80.
LeVay, S. (1991) A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and
homosexual men. Science, 253, 1034–1037.
LeVay, S. (1993) The sexual brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
LeVay, S. (1996) Queer science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
References 173
LeVay, S. (2010) Gay, straight, and the reason why. The science of sexual orientation.
Oxford University Press, New York.
LeVay, S. & Hamer, D.H. (1994) Evidence for a biological influence in male homosexu-
ality. Sci. Am., 270, 44–49.
LeVay, S. & Valente, S.M. (2006) Human Sexuallity. Sinauer Associates Inc,
Sunderland, MA.
Lippa, R.A. (2003a) Are 2D:4D finger-length ratios related to sexual orientation? Yes
for men, no for women. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 85, 179–188.
Lippa, R.A. (2003b) Handedness, sexual orientation, and gender-related personality
traits in men and women. Arch. Sex. Behav., 32, 103–114.
Lorenz, K. (1937) Uber die Bildung des Instinktbergiffes (The establishment of the
instinct concept). Die Naturwissenschaften, 25, 280–300, 307–318, 325–331.
Lorenz, K. (1950) The comparative method in studying innate behavior patterns. Symp.
Soc. Exp. Biol., 4, 221–268.
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S. & Raggatt, P. (2002) Foetal testosterone and eye contact
in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behav. Develop, 25, 327–335.
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Knickmeyer, R. & Manning, J.T. (2004) 2nd
to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum. Dev., 77, 23–28.
MacLaughlin, D.T. & Donahoe, P.K. (2004) Sex determination and differentiation.
N. Engl. J. Med., 350, 367–378.
Manning, J.T., Fink, B., Neave, N. & Szwed, A. (2006) The second to fourth digit ratio
and asymmetry. Ann. Hum. Biol., 33, 480–492.
Mansukhani, V., Adkins-Regan, E. & Yang, S. (1996) Sexual partner preference in
female zebra finches: The role of early hormones and social environment. Horm.
Behav., 30, 506–513.
Marler, P. & Hamilton, W.J.I. (1966) Mechanisms of animal behavior. Wiley and Sons,
New York.
Martin, J.T. & Nguyen, D.H. (2004) Anthropometric analysis of homosexuals and
heterosexuals: implications for early hormone exposure. Horm. Behav., 45, 31–39.
McCormick, C.M. & Witelson, S.F. (1991) A cognitive profile of homosexual men
compared to heterosexual men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16, 459–73.
McEwen, B.S. & Alves, S.E. (1999) Estrogen actions in the central nervous system.
Endocr. Rev., 20, 279–307.
McFadden, D. (2002) Masculinization effects in the auditory system. Arch. Sex. Behav.,
31, 99–111.
McFadden, D. (2008) What do sex, twins, spotted hyenas, ADHD, and sexual orienta-
tion have in common? Perspectives Psychol. Sci, 3, 309–323.
McFadden, D. (2011) Sexual orientation and the auditory system. Front. Neuroendocrinol.,
32, 201–213.
McFadden, D. & Champlin, C.A. (2000) Comparison of auditory evoked potentials in
heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual males and females. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol.,
1, 89–99.
McFadden, D. & Pasanen, E.G. (1998) Comparison of the auditory systems of hetero-
sexuals and homosexuals: click-evoked otoacoustic emissions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U S A, 95, 2709–2713.
174 References
Ngun, T.C., Ghahramani, N., Sanchez, F.J., Bocklandt, S. & Vilain, E. (2011) The genet-
ics of sex differences in brain and behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol., 32, 227–246.
Nordenstrom, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson, A. & Wedell, A. (2002) Sex-typed
toy play behavior correlates with the degree of prenatal androgen exposure assessed
by CYP21 genotype in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. J. Clin. Endocrinol.
Metab., 87, 5119–5124.
Nottebohm, F. & Arnold, A.P. (1976) Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of the
songbird brain. Science, 194, 211–213.
Onfray, M. (2010) Le crépuscule d’une idole. L’affabulation freudienne. Bernard Grasset,
Paris.
Op de Beeck, H.P., Haushofer, J. & Kanwisher, N.G. (2008) Interpreting fMRI data:
maps, modules and dimensions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 9, 123–135.
Ozdemir, S.T., Ercan, I., Sevinc, O., Guney, I., Ocakoglu, G., Aslan, E. & Barut, C. (2007)
Statistical shape analysis of differences in the shape of the corpus callosum between
genders. Anat. Rec. (Hoboken), 290, 825–830.
Paredes, R.G. & Baum, M.J. (1995) Altered sexual partner preference in male ferrets
given excitotoxic lesions of the preoptic area anterior hypothalamus. J. Neurosci., 15,
6619–6630.
Paredes, R.G., Tzschentke, T. & Nakach, N. (1998) Lesions of the medial preoptic
area anterior hypothalamus (MPOA/AH) modify partner preference in male rats.
Brain Res., 813, 1–8.
Pelletier, G. (2000) Localization of androgen and estrogen receptors in rat and primate
tissues. Histol. Histopathol., 15, 1261–1270.
Perkins, A. & Fitzgerald, J.A. (1992) Luteinizing hormone, testosterone, and behavioral
response of male-oriented rams to estrous ewes and rams. J. Anim. Sci., 70, 1787–1794.
Perkins, A., Fitzgerald, J.A. & Price, E.O. (1992) Luteinizing hormone and testosterone
response of sexually active and inactive rams. J. Anim. Sci., 70, 2086–2093.
Perkins, A. & Roselli, C.E. (2007) The ram as a model for behavioral neuroendocrinol-
ogy. Horm. Behav., 52, 70–77.
Pinckard, K.L., Stellflug, J., Resko, J.A., Roselli, C.E. & Stormshak, F. (2000) Review:
brain aromatization and other factors affecting male reproductive behavior with
emphasis on the sexual orientation of rams. Domes. Anim. Endocrinol., 18, 83–96.
Poianni, A. (2010) Animal homosexuality. A biological perspective. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge UK.
Rahman, Q., Abrahams, S. & Wilson, G.D. (2003a) Sexual-orientation-related differ-
ences in verbal fluency. Neuropsychology, 17, 240–246.
Rahman, Q., Kumari, V. & Wilson, G.D. (2003b) Sexual orientation-related differ-
ences in prepulse inhibition of the human startle response. Behav. Neurosci., 117,
1096–1102.
Rahman, Q. & Wilson, G.D. (2003a) Born gay? The psychobiology of human sexual
orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1337–1382.
Rahman, Q. & Wilson, G.D. (2003b) Large sexual-orientation-related differences in per-
formance on mental rotation and judgment of line orientation tasks. Neuropsychology,
17, 25–31.
Rahman, Q. & Wilson, G.D. (2003c) Sexual orientation and the 2nd to 4th finger length
ratio: evidence for organising effects of sex hormones or developmental instability?
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 288–303.
176 References
Raisman, G. & Field, P.M. (1971) Sexual dimorphism in the preoptic area of the rat.
Science, 173, 731–733.
Reiner, W.G. & Gearhart, J.P. (2004) Discordant sexual identity in some genetic males
with cloacal exstrophy assigned to female sex at birth. N. Engl. J. Med., 350, 333–341.
Resko, J.A., Perkins, A., Roselli, C.E., Fitzgerald, J.A., Choate, J.V. & Stormshak, F.
(1996) Endocrine correlates of partner preference behavior in rams. Biol. Reprod.,
55, 120–126.
Rhees, R.W., Shryne, J.E. & Gorski, R.A. (1990) Termination of the hormone-sensitive
period for differentiation of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area in
male and female rats. Dev. Brain Res., 52, 17–23.
Rice, G., Anderson, C., Risch, N. & Ebers, G. (1999) Male homosexuality: absence of
linkage to microsatellite markers at Xq28. Science, 284, 665–667.
Richelle, M. (1966) Le conditionnement operant. Delachaux et Niestlé, Neuchatel, Suisse.
Rieger, G., Chivers, M.L. & Bailey, J.M. (2005) Sexual arousal patterns of bisexual men.
Psychol. Sci, 16, 579–584.
Robarts, D.W & Baum, M.J. (2007) Ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus lesions dis-
rupt olfactory mate recognition and receptivity in female ferrets. Horm. Behav., 51,
104–113.
Romano, M., Rubolini, D., Martinelli, R., Bonisoli Alquati, A. & Saino, N. (2005)
Experimental manipulation of yolk testosterone affects digit length ratios in the ring-
necked pheasant (Phasianus colchicus). Horm. Behav., 48, 342–346.
Roselli, C.E., Larkin, K., Resko, J.A., Stellflug, J.N. & Stormshak, F. (2004a) The volume
of a sexually dimorphic nucleus in the ovine medial preoptic area/anterior hypo-
thalamus varies with sexual partner preference. Endocrinology, 145, 478–483.
Roselli, C.E., Larkin, K., Schrunk, J.M. & Stormshak, F. (2004b) Sexual partner preference,
hypothalamic morphology and aromatase in rams. Physiol. Behav., 83, 233–245.
Roselli, C.E., Reddy, R. & Kaufman, K. (2011) The development of male-oriented behav-
ior in rams. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 32, 164–169
Roselli, C.E., Stadelman, H., Reeve, R., Bishop, C.V. & Stormshak, F. (2007) The ovine
sexually dimorphic nucleus of the medial preoptic area is organized prenatally by
testosterone. Endocrinology, 148, 4450–4457.
Rosenzweig, M.R., Breedlove, S.M. & Watson, N.V. (2004) Biological psychology: An intro-
duction to behavioral and cognitive neuroscience. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland MA.
Rosler, A. & Witztum, E. (1998) Treatment of men with paraphilia with a long-acting
analogue of gonadotropin-releasing hormone. N. Engl. J. Med., 338, 416–422.
Rubin, R.T., Reinisch, J.M. & Haskett, R.F. (1981) Postnatal gonadal steroid effects on
human behavior. Science, 211, 1318–1324.
Sanders, A.R. & Dawood, K. (2003) Nature encyclopedia of life sciences. Nature Publishing
Group, London.
Savic, I., Berglund, H., Gulyas, B. & Roland, P. (2001) Smelling of odorous sex hormone-
like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans. Neuron,
31, 661–668.
Savic, I., Berglund, H. & Lindstrom, P. (2005) Brain response to putative pheromones in
homosexual men. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A, 102, 7356–7361.
Schmidt, G. & Clement, U. (1990) Does peace prevent homosexuality? Arch. Sex. Behav.,
19, 183–187.
References 177
Schober, J.M., Carmichael, P.A., Hines, M. & Ransley, P.G. (2002) The ultimate chal-
lenge of cloacal exstrophy. J. Urol., 167, 300–304.
Sherwin, B.B. & Gelfand, M.M. (1987) The role of androgen in the maintenance of
sexual functioning in oophorectomized women. Psychosom. Med., 49, 397–409.
Sinclair, A.H., Berta, P., Palmer, M.S., Hawkins, J.R., Griffiths, B.L., Smith, M.J., Foster,
J.W., Frischauf, A.M., Lovell-Badge, R. & Goodfellow, P.N. (1990) A gene from the
human sex-determining region encodes a protein with homology to a conserved
DNA-binding motif. Nature, 346, 240–244.
Sisk, C.L. & Zehr, J.L. (2005) Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and
behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol., 26, 163–174.
Skinner, B.F. (1965) L’analyse expérimentale du comportement. Pierre Mardaga, Wavre.
Skinner, B.F. (1971) L’analyse expérimentale du comportement (Traduit de l’Amércain par
A.M et M. Richelle). Charles Dessart, Bruxelles.
Snyder, P.J., Peachey, H., Berlin, J.A., Hannoush, P., Haddad, G., Dlewati, A., Santanna, J.,
Loh, L., Lenrow, D.A., Holmes, J.H., Kapoor, S.C., Atkinson, L.E. & Strom, B.L. (2000)
Effects of testosterone replacement in hypogonadal men. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.,
85, 2670–2677.
Sommer, V. & Vasey, P.L. (2006) Homosexual behaviour in animals. An evolutionary
perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Stacey, J. & Biblarz, T.J. (2001) (How) does the sexual orientation of parents matter?
American Sociological Review, 66, 159–183.
Strong, B. & DeVault, C. (1997) Human sexuality. Mayfield Publishing Co, Mountain
View, CA.
Swaab, D.F. (2007) Sexual differentiation of the brain and behavior. Best Pract. Res. Clin.
Endocrinol. Metab., 21, 431–444.
Swaab, D.F. & Fliers, E. (1985) A sexually dimorphic nucleus in the human brain.
Science, 228, 1112–1115.
Swaab, D.F. & Hofman, M.A. (1988) Sexual differentiation of the human hypothalamus:
Ontogeny of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area. Dev. Brain Res., 44,
314–318.
Swaab, D.F. & Hofman, M.A. (1990) An enlarged suprachiasmatic nucleus in homo-
sexual men. Brain Res., 537, 141–148.
Taziaux, M., Keller, M., Bakker, J. & Balthazart, J. (2007) Sexual behavior activity tracks
rapid changes in brain estrogen concentrations. J. Neurosci., 27, 6563–6572.
Titus-Ernstoff, L., Perez, K., Hatch, E.E., Troisi, R., Palmer, J.R., Hartge, P., Hyer, M.,
Kaufman, R., Adam, E., Strohsnitter, W., Noller, K., Pickett, K.E. & Hoover, R. (2003)
Psychosexual characteristics of men and women exposed prenatally to diethylstilbe-
strol. Epidemiology, 14, 155–160.
Tuncer, M.C., Hatipoglu, E.S. & Ozates, M. (2005) Sexual dimorphism and handedness
in the human corpus callosum based on magnetic resonance imaging. Surg. Radiol.
Anat., 27, 254–259.
Tutle, G.E. & Pillard, R.C. (1991) Sexual orientation and cognitive abilities. Arch. Sex.
Behav., 20, 307–318.
Udry, J.R., Billy, J.O., Morris, N.M., Groff, T.R. & Raj, M.H. (1985) Serum androgenic
hormones motivate sexual behavior in adolescent boys. Fertil. Steril., 43, 90–94.
Van Rillaer, J. (1980) Les illusions de la psychanalyse. Pierre Mardaga, Sprimont (Belgique).
178 References
Vasudevan, N. & Pfaff, D.W. (2008) Non-genomic actions of estrogens and their inter-
action with genomic actions in the brain. Front. Neuroendocrinol., 29, 238–257.
Vermeulen, A., Rubens, R. & Verdonck, L. (1972) Testosterone secretion and metabo-
lism in male senescence. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 34, 730–735.
Vidal, C. (ed) (2000) Féminin Masculin Mythes et idéologie. Belin, Paris.
Vidal, C. (2007) Hommes, femmes avons-nous le même cerveau? Le Pommier, Paris.
Wallen, K. (2009) Does finger fat produce sex differences in second to fourth digit
ratios? Endocrinology, 150, 4819–4822.
Wang, C., Swerdloff, R.S., Iranmanesh, A., Dobs, A., Snyder, P.J., Cunningham, G.,
Matsumoto, A.M., Weber, T., Berman, N. & Grp, T.G.S. (2000) Transdermal testosterone
gel improves sexual function, mood, muscle strength, and body composition parameters
in hypogonadal men. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 85, 2839–2853.
Ward, I.L. (1972) Prenatal stress feminizes and demasculinizes the behavior of males.
Science, 175, 82–84.
Ward, I.L. (1984) The prenatal stress syndrome: current status. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
9, 3–11.
Ward, I.L. & Ward, O.B. (1985) Sexual behavior differentiation: effects of prenatal
manipulations in rats Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Reproduction. 7, 77–98.
Weisz, J. (1983) Influence of maternal stress on the developmental pattern of the ste-
roidogenic function in Leydig cells and steroid aromatase activity in the brain of rat
fetuses. Monogr. Neural. Sci., 9, 184–193.
Wellings, K., Field, J., Johnson, A.M. & Wadsworth, J. (1994) Sexual behavior in Britain:
The National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles. Penguin Books, London.
Williams, C.L. & Pleil, K.E. (2008) Toy story: why do monkey and human males prefer
trucks? Comment on “Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel
those of children” by Hassett, Siebert and Wallen. Horm. Behav., 54, 355–358.
Wisniewski, A.B., Migeon, C.J., Meyer-Bahlburg, H.F., Gearhart, J.P., Berkovitz, G.D.,
Brown, T.R. & Money, J. (2000) Complete androgen insensitivity syndrome: long-
term medical, surgical, and psychosexual outcome. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 85,
2664–2669.
Witelson, S.F., Kigar, D.L., Scamvougeras, A., Kideckel, D.M., Buck, B., Stanchev, P.L.,
Bronskill, M. & Black, S. (2008) Corpus Callosum Anatomy in Right-Handed
Homosexual and Heterosexual Men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 37, 857–863
Wysocki, C.J. & Preti, G. (2004) Facts, fallacies, fears, and frustrations with human
pheromones. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol., 281, 1201–1211.
Yang, X., Schadt, E.E., Wang, S., Wang, H., Arnold, A.P., Ingram-Drake, L., Drake, T.A.
& Lusis, A.J. (2006) Tissue-specific expression and regulation of sexually dimorphic
genes in mice. Genome Res., 16, 995–1004.
Zhou, J.N., Hofman, M.A., Gooren, L.J.G. & Swaab, D.F. (1995) A sex difference in the
human brain and its relation to transsexuality. Nature, 378, 68–70.
Zucker, K.J., Bradley, S.J., Oliver, G., Blake, J., Fleming, S. & Hood, J. (1996) Psycho-
sexual development of women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm. Behav.,
30, 300–318.
Index