Pitzer Et Al. 2011 Development of PARQ
Pitzer Et Al. 2011 Development of PARQ
Pitzer Et Al. 2011 Development of PARQ
LINDSAY PITZER
KAREN L. FINGERMAN
Purdue University
EVA S. LEFKOWITZ
Pennsylvania State University
ABSTRACT
*This study was supported by grant R01AG17916 “Problems Between Parents and Offspring
in Adulthood” and R01 AG027769, “The Psychology of Intergenerational Transfers” from
the National Institute of Aging.
111
The parent-child tie is both a source of support and a source of irritation across
the lifespan. Moreover, qualities of relationships with parents and adult off-
spring have been linked to well-being (Fingerman, Pitzer, Lefkowitz, Birditt, &
Mroczek, 2008; Lowenstein, 2007; Ward, 2008). Yet, it remains unclear whether
it is positive qualities, negative qualities, or a mixture of sentiments that account
for these associations. Because the study of parent-adult child relationships
is multi-faceted, it is important to provide: (a) a measure that is brief in length;
(b) assesses positive and negative facets of this relationship with regard to balance
in the tone of the questions; and (c) includes items that are applicable to all
parent-adult child relationships, regardless of distance. Such a measure would
serve to complement the study and measurement of other aspects of the parent-
adult child relationship.
Early research regarding parent-adult child relationships involved the inter-
generational solidarity perspective, which provides evidence of cohesion in
parent-adult child relationships (Bengtson & Schrader, 1982). Over the past
decade, however, the field turned to the intergenerational ambivalence model
(Lowenstein, 2007; Luescher & Pillemer, 1998; Pillemer & Suitor, 2002).
This model is conceived of as: (a) feeling mixed or torn in the relationship; or
(b) experiencing both positive and negative sentiments toward a parent or grown
child (Luescher & Pillemer, 1998; Priester & Petty, 2001). The latter definition,
the social psychological perspective, has received the majority of research atten-
tion (Fingerman, Chen, Hay, Cichy, & Lefkowitz, 2006; Fingerman et al., 2008;
Peters, Hooker, & Zvonkovic, 2006; Willson, Shuey, & Elder, 2003). This
model, however, is also hampered by measurement issues. Despite strong research
interest in this topic, there is a lack of adequate measurement for the construct.
Therefore, the Parent Adult Relationship Quality (PARQ) scale was designed to
be a useful measure of positive and negative relationship qualities for researchers
who are seeking to enhance and broaden understanding of intergenera-
tional relationships, ambivalence, and well-being. This measure is an emotionally
balanced, concise (in terms of number of items), and useful measurement tool
for parent-adult child ties across varying geographic differences.
separation from parents, offspring who reside at a distance from parents typically
report positive and negative relationship qualities comparable to qualities reported
by more proximate offspring (Fingerman, Hay, Cichy, Tang, Chen, & Lefkowitz,
2003; Lawton, Silverstein, & Bengtson, 1994).
The following studies assess the Parent and Adult Relationship Questionnaire
(PARQ) as a concise and emotionally balanced instrument of positive and
negative qualities in the parent-adult child tie, regardless of the geographical
distance between parents and children. The literature regarding adults and
parents uses the term “relationship quality” broadly in reference to either
positive and negative feelings (Lefkowitz & Fingerman, 2003), supportive
behaviors or demands (Umberson, 1989, 1992), manifestations of respect or
affection (Bengtson & Schrader, 1982), and/or interpersonal problems (Birditt,
Miller, Fingerman, & Lefkowitz, 2009). Likewise, we use a broad definition of
“quality” here.
To determine whether the new measure adequately captures positive and
negative qualities, convergent and discriminant validity should be evident
(Campbell & Fiske, 1959). An appropriate assessment of positive and negative
relationship qualities should be associated with existing measures of relationship
quality and not associated with constructs conceptually distinct from relationship
qualities. We expected that the PARQ would be associated with other measures
of positive relationship qualities (Bengtson & Schrader, 1982; Cicirelli, 1991,
1993; Koropeckyj-Cox, 2002; Sweet, Bumpass, & Call, 1988), and of positive and
negative qualities in this tie (ACL; Fingerman et al., 2006; Umberson, 1992;
Willson et al., 2003) We also expected the PARQ would not be associated with
conceptually dissimilar measures of other aspects of the parent-child tie, such
as filial anxiety (offspring’s worries about parents’ future care needs; Cicirelli,
1988) or frequency of face-to-face contact. We also considered several other
important aspects of measurement development. Namely, it is important that
such a measure assess qualities of the tie in the absence of face-to-face contact.
We developed the PARQ with relevant items for adults and parents regardless
of geographic distance between the parties.
We also sought to develop a measure that is appropriate for survey research.
Survey researchers have become increasingly interested in measures that can be
administered via a variety of methodologies (e.g., paper, telephone, and internet;
Boyer, Olson, Calantone, & Jackson, 2002; Herzog & Rodgers, 1988; Knapp &
Kirk, 2003). We administered the PARQ through multiple approaches to ensure
that reliability and validity of the PARQ was comparable across methods.
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ / 115
Methods
Study 1 had three aims. First, we examined construct validity in a sample
of young adults through: (a) exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses and
(b) internal consistency, a. Next, we determined reliability using test-retest
methods. Finally, given that relationship qualities with mother and with father
may differ, we assessed reliability for mothers and fathers separately.
Participants
Procedures
Participants were recruited from five undergraduate classes which offered the
PARQ as an option to participate in research. All undergraduate students were
volunteers. In some classes, participation in research was a class requirement and
completing the PARQ was one option to fulfill the requirement. In other classes,
students received extra credit for participation in research. Other research using
college student samples have used similar methods of recruitment (e.g., Coles,
Cook, & Blake, 2007; Falkenbach, Poythress, & Creevy, 2008).
To establish test-retest reliability, participants completed the measure twice,
approximately 4 weeks apart. Students completing the PARQ at both time points
received class credit. We randomized whether participants answered questions
about their mother or father first. All participants included in this study answered
on both their mother and father. The completion rate at Time 2 was 68% of the
Time 1 sample. Although completion rates were relatively low, other studies that
have used college student samples have the same issue (e.g., Cranford, McCabe,
Boyd, Slayden, Reed, Ketchie, et al., 2008; Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, 2009).
Measures
PARQ
Participants completed the four positive and four negative quality items
regarding their mother and father. The PARQ items are measured on a 5-point
Likert scale, where 1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, and 5 = always.
Items included in the PARQ are given in the Appendix, Table A1. The means and
standard deviations of the PARQ items can be found in the Appendix, Table A2.
Results
We first conducted a series of exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses
to determine which items best represented the two-factor construct of relation-
ship qualities. We began by conducting a principal components factor analysis
(PCA) with varimax rotation of the eight PARQ items (Birditt, Fingerman,
Lefkowitz, & Kamp-Dush, 2008; Bryant, 2000) and constrained the solution
to two factors. Items were considered as a part of a factor if it had loadings of .30
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ / 117
on their respective factors and lower loadings on the other (Borjesson, Aarons,
& Dunn, 2003; Stoppelbein, Greening, Jordan, Elkin, Moll, & Pullen, 2005).
Indeed, we found that the four positive and four negative items loaded .30 or
higher on each of their respective subscales, with each component (or subscale)
having eigenvalues of 1.0 or higher (see Table 1).
Table 1 also shows instances where items loaded highly on both scales. For
instance, “supportive of decisions” loaded highly on the “negative” factor and
“insensitivity or unsympathetically” loaded highly on the “positive” factor. This
could have happened for a couple of reasons, including the fact that these items
were strongly correlated with the items from the opposite factor and also with
their respective factors. Furthermore, the extraction and rotation method we
employed assumes correlation among factors (oblique rotation) and the variables
(PCA). Indeed, these are situations that may generate the cross-loading of certain
items on the factors. Examination of the factor loadings from confirmatory
factor analysis, however, revealed that these items did not load as highly on
the opposite factors as PCA suggested. For example, for offspring reporting
on mothers, “Supportive of decisions” loaded –.32 on the negative factor and
Discussion
This study shows that the PARQ involves two distinct subscales with high
internal consistency. Test-retest reliability also demonstrated stability in responses
over time. However, Study 1 relied on an undergraduate student sample. Given
the widespread use of college student samples in research on adult relationships,
it is important to demonstrate the PARQ’s measurement features in this popu-
lation of offspring. The PARQ, however, is intended for use with a wider popula-
tion including older offspring, non-college students, and parents.
Table 2. Unstandardized Loadings (Standard Errors) and Standardized Loadings for Two-Factor
Confirmatory Model of the PARQ Scale for Studies 1 through 3
Study 1 Study 1
Offspring about mother Offspring about father Study 2 Study 3
Positive
Warm or affectionate 1.15 (.11) .76 1.37 (.10) .83 1.25 (.15) .64 1.24 (.10) .70
Thoughtful or considerate 1.25 (.12) .86 1.42 (.10) .93 1.55 (.16) .91 1.36 (.10) .81
Favors or other little things 0.99 (.11) .62 1.31 (.10) .78 1.27 (.15) .69 0.96 (.08) .61
Supportive of decisions 1.00 (—) .58 1.00 (—) .69 1.00 (—) .59 1.00 (—) .58
Negative
Angry or hostile 1.29 (.16) .71 1.15 (.11) .71 1.06 (.13) .68 1.04 (.09) .69
Insensitivity or unsympathetically 1.43 (.18) .79 1.60 (.14) .88 1.16 (.13) .76 1.22 (.10) .78
Demands for favors 1.16 (.18) .49 0.86 (.11) .48 1.19 (.17) .55 0.76 (.09) .42
Questioned or doubted decisions 1.00 (—) .49 1.00 (—) .62 1.00 (—) .64 1.00 (—) .61
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ
/ 119
120 / PITZER, FINGERMAN AND LEFKOWITZ
Methods
Participants
Included in this study were 153 mothers and 101 fathers (N = 254). Participants
were aged 41 to 88 (M = 55.48, SD = 8.34); mothers and fathers did not differ
by age. These parents reported on 141 adult daughters and 113 adult sons at
Time 1, who ranged in age from 18 to 57 (M = 28.21, SD = 8.41). This sample was
98% White.
A subset of parents completed the survey at Time 2 to establish test-retest
reliability. This subsample included 79 mothers and 50 fathers reporting on 67
adult daughters and 62 adult sons (n = 129). We examined possible differences
between the subset of participants who completed the survey only at Time 1
(n = 125) and the subset of participants of participants who completed the survey at
Times 1 and 2 with regard to age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, marital status,
physical health, child’s gender, and child’s age. Independent t-tests revealed no
significant differences between participants on these background characteristics.
The completion rate at Time 2 was 51%.
Procedures
Measures
Results
Discussion
The results of Study 2 further established validity and reliability among a
sample of parents of grown children. With regard to validity, the PARQ subscales
converged with other measures of positive and negative behaviors in the expected
directions. We also found high test-retest reliability. A particular strength of Study
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ / 123
PARQ PARQ
Positive Negative
Measure Subscale Subscale
Mothers
Adult Attachment Scale .26*** –.16**
Americans Changing Lives positive quality scale .69*** –.32***
Americans Changing Lives negative quality scale –.35*** .71***
Global relationship quality .65*** –.24***
Fathers
Adult Attachment Scale .46*** –.36**
Americans Changing Lives positive quality scale .79*** –.56***
Americans Changing Lives negative quality scale –.35*** .77***
Global relationship quality .73*** –.54***
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
2 was the ability to look at different survey formats. Indeed, properties of the
PARQ were consistent in both web-based and paper and pencil formats for
mothers and fathers.
Limitations of this study involve the sample being almost exclusively White.
Additionally, participants chose their preference for web and paper surveys.
Reliability might be lower if participants were randomly assigned to format.
Also, convenience and word-of-mouth sampling present possible biases: a less
representative sample of the population and a limited generalizability of the
results. Another limitation of Study 2 is that the measures used to validate the
PARQ were all self-report and, therefore, validation did not include observational
measures of relationship quality. Study 3 addresses these concerns.
The aims of Study 3 were twofold. First, we assessed construct validity of the
PARQ by considering the factor structure, internal consistency, and convergent
and discriminant validity in a distinct sample including parents and their grown
children. Convergent validity was assessed through association with extant self-
report measures of relationship quality, while discriminant validity was assessed
through non-association with unrelated features of the relationship (e.g., filial
anxiety). Second, we also compared responses on the PARQ to observations of
124 / PITZER, FINGERMAN AND LEFKOWITZ
Methods
Participants
Data were from the Adult Family Study, a study of 213 families with adults
aged 22 to 49 (M = 34.97, SD = 7.28), their mother (mean age = 61.26, SD = 8.79)
and father (mean age = 63.00. SD = 9.27) conducted in the greater Philadelphia
area (N = 639). Study 3 includes a subset of those participants (158 families;
n = 474) who completed telephone interviews, face-to-face videotaped inter-
views, and self-report questionnaires. This subset did not differ from the
larger sample and was collected at the same time as the rest of the data from
the Adult Family Study (see Fingerman et al., 2006, for details). Both parents
and the grown child completed the face-to-face videotaped interviews, allow-
ing examination of convergent validity with observational measures of
positive and negative emotional behaviors. Approximately one-third of the
sample was African American, while the remaining two-thirds was European
American.
Procedure
Measures
correlations ranged from .94 to .97 for positive and .94 to .96 for negative codes.
Reliability coefficients for the scales of combined codes were a = .76 for the
positive scale and a = .82 for the negative scale.
Background information and contact—Participants provided information
about their age, gender, and ethnicity. Participants also rated often how they
see each other in person from 1 (Less than once a year) to 8 (Once a week or
more often) and how often they communicate with each other via phone, e-mail,
or other forms of contact rated 1 (Twice a year or less often) to 8 (Every day).
PARQ PARQ
Positive Negative
Measure Subscale Subscale
PARENTS
Convergent Measures
Americans Changing Lives negative quality scale –.27*** .47***
Americans Changing Lives positive quality scale .51*** –.34***
Positive emotional experiences .56*** –.51***
Negative emotional experiences –.40*** .63***
Bengtson Positive Affect Index .51*** –.41***
Observed behaviors
Observed positive behaviors .16*** –.05
Observed negative behaviors –.12*** .28***
Divergent Measures
Filial anxiety — —
OFFSPRING
Convergent Measures
Americans Changing Lives negative quality scale –.19*** .62***
Americans Changing Lives positive quality scale .69*** –.37***
Positive emotional experiences .73*** –.45***
Negative emotional experiences –.35*** .63***
Bengtson Positive Affect Index .61*** –.50***
Observed behaviors
Observed positive behaviors .26*** –.16**
Observed negative behaviors –.15* .29***
Divergent Measures
Filial anxiety –.13* .11*
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
128 / PITZER, FINGERMAN AND LEFKOWITZ
GENERAL DISCUSSION
conflict (e.g., doubting a decision; Clarke, Preston, Raskin, & Bengtson, 1999;
Fingerman, 1996).
As expected, the PARQ was associated with self-reported and observed
measures of relationship quality. Associations between the PARQ and self-
reported measures of relationship quality suggest that the underlying constructs
of the PARQ include positive and negative qualities. Most importantly, however,
associations with observed behaviors demonstrate that the PARQ may pick up
on actual interactions between adults and parents that also can be seen by trained
raters. Because observations of behaviors are time consuming and costly, the
PARQ represents a concise and low-cost means of assessing these qualities of
the relationship. To the best of our knowledge, no other measure of positive
and negative qualities in the parent-adult child tie has been validated using
observed behaviors.
Further, associations were not evident between frequency of contact and the
two subscales of the PARQ, suggesting that the measure taps aspects of relation-
ship quality distinct from amount of contact. Prior studies suggest that offspring
may retain strong emotional bonds with their parents, despite geographic separ-
ation and absence of frequent face-to-face contact (Fingerman et al., 2006; Rossi
& Rossi, 1990), and that negative qualities also are evident among offspring who
reside at a distance from parents (Fingerman et al., 2003). The PARQ successfully
assesses these qualities for both proximate and distant offspring.
Limitations
The three studies have limitations, however. Studies 1 and 2 were primarily
limited to White participants. Further, although the sample in Study 3 was
one-third Black, we only assessed construct validity with this sample. Future
studies will need to determine whether the PARQ is a stable measure of relation-
ship qualities in more diverse samples. Second, the PARQ is a self-report assess-
ment, which can elicit (or are susceptible to) socially desirable responding (Birditt
et al., 2008). We addressed this problem to some extent in Study 3 by finding
associations between the PARQ and observed behaviors between adults and
their parents. Finally, the mean scores on the PARQ negative scales were low,
suggesting that individuals did not experience many negative feelings for parents
or offspring. (Fingerman, 2001; Fingerman et al., 2006; Lefkowitz & Fingerman,
2003; Willson et al., 2003).
Overall, the PARQ is an important addition to the assessment of positive and
negative qualities in the parent-adult child tie. Findings suggest that the PARQ is a
stable and balanced measure of relationship quality over time among adults and
their parents. This measure assesses positive and negative relationship qualities
among adults and their parents in a concise and emotionally balanced manner,
above and beyond what other assessments of relationship qualities and ambiv-
alence can provide.
130 / PITZER, FINGERMAN AND LEFKOWITZ
APPENDIX
Table A1. PARQ Items
How often has your (mother/father/child) How often has your (mother/father/child)
acted warm or affectionate toward you? acted angry or hostile toward you?
How often has your (mother/father/child) How often has your (mother/father/child)
acted thoughtful or considerate toward acted insensitively or unsympathetically
you? toward you?
How often has your (mother/father/child) How often has your (mother/father/child)
done favors or other little things for you? made demands for favors or other little
things from you?
How often has your (mother/father/
child) been supportive of the How often has your (mother/father/child)
decisions you’ve made? questioned or doubted your decisions?
Study 1 Study 1
Offspring Offspring
about about Study 2 Study 3
mother father
Item M SD M SD M SD M SD
Positive
Warm or affectionate 4.41 .66 3.65 1.00 4.11 .84 4.00 1.01
Thoughtful or considerate 4.43 .64 3.90 .92 4.07 .72 4.18 .86
Favors or other little things 4.35 .70 3.69 1.02 3.44 .78 3.98 .84
Supportive of decisions 4.22 .75 3.98 .88 3.96 .73 4.11 .90
Negative
Angry or hostile 2.31 .70 2.44 .83 2.01 .69 1.62 .71
Insensitivity or unsympathetically 2.04 .69 2.42 .93 2.02 .67 1.67 .74
Demands for favors 2.59 .91 2.43 .91 2.19 .95 1.85 .85
Questioned or doubted decisions 2.32 .79 2.32 .84 2.12 .70 1.94 .77
Subscales
Positive 4.35 .54 3.80 .82 3.89 .60 4.07 .70
Negative 2.31 .57 2.40 .68 2.09 .57 1.82 .65
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ / 131
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Dr. Jason Newsom and Drs. Nishishiba,
Morgan, and Rook for generously granting us permission to further develop
the PARQ using their initial items. We are grateful to Drs. Erina MacGeorge,
Charles Calahan, Shawn Whiteman, Sara Kubik, as well as Amber Seidel for
assistance in collecting data. We also thank Drs. Kira Birditt and Jori Sechrist
for thoroughly reading a draft of this article. Finally, we thank Laura Miller and
Shelley Hosterman, as well as Drs. Kelly Cichy and Elizabeth Hay for assistance
with all aspects of the AFS study.
REFERENCES
Bengtson, V., Giarrusso, R., Mabry, J., & Silverstein, M. (2002). Solidarity, conflict,
and ambivalence: Complementary or competing perspectives on intergenerational
relationships? Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 568-576. doi: 10.1111/
j.1741-3737.2002.00568.x
Bengtson, V. L., & Schrader, S. S. (1982). Parent-child relations. In D. J. Mangen &
W. A. Peterson (Eds.), Research instruments in social gerontology: Social roles and
social participation (pp. 115-129). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Birditt, K., Miller, L., Fingerman, K., & Lefkowitz, E. (2009).Tensions in the parent
and adult child relationship: Links to solidarity and ambivalence. Psychology and
Aging, 24, 287-295. doi: 10.1037/a0015196
Birditt, K. S., Fingerman, K. L., Lefkowitz, E. S., & Kamp-Dush, C. M. (2008). Parents
perceived as peers: Filial maturity in adulthood. Journal of Adult Development, 15,
1-12. doi: 10.1007/s10804-007-9019-2
Borjesson, W., Aarons, G., & Dunn, M. (2003). Development and confirmatory factor
analysis of the abuse within intimate relationships scale. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 18, 295-309. doi: 10.1177/0886260502250089
Boyer, K., Olson, J., Calantone, R., & Jackson, E. (2002). Print versus electronic surveys:
A comparison of two data collection methodologies. Journal of Operations
Management, 20, 357-373. doi: 10.1016/S0272-6963(02)00004-9
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In
K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Bryant, F. B. (2000). Assessing the validity of measurement. In L. G. Grimm & P. R.
Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understanding more multivariate statistics (pp. 99-146).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bryant, F. B., & Yarnold, P. R. (1995). Principal-components analysis and exploratory
and confirmatory factor analysis. In L. G. Grimm & P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading
and understanding multivariate statistics (pp. 99-136). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Campbell, D., & Fiske, D. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-
multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 81-105. doi: 10.1037/h0046016
Cicirelli, V. (1988). A measure of filial anxiety regarding anticipated care of elderly
parents. The Gerontologist, 28, 478-482. doi: 10.1093/geront/28.4.478
132 / PITZER, FINGERMAN AND LEFKOWITZ
Cicirelli, V. (1991). Attachment theory in old age: Protection of the attached figure. In
K. Pillemer & K. McCartney (Eds.), Parent-child relations throughout life (pp. 25-42).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cicirelli, V. (1993). Attachment and obligation as daughter’s motives for caregiving
behavior and subsequent effect on subjective burden. Psychology and Aging, 8,
144-155. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.2.144
Cicirelli, V. (1995). A measure of caregiving daughters’ attachment to elderly mothers.
Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 89-94. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.9.1.89
Clarke, E., Preston, M., Raskin, J., & Bengtson, V. (1999). Types of conflicts and
tensions between older parents and adult children. The Gerontologist, 39, 261-270.
doi: 10.1093/geront/39.3.261
Coles, M., Cook, L., & Blake, T. (2007). Assessing obsessive compulsive symptoms
and cognitions on the internet: Evidence for the comparability of paper and
internet administration. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2232-2240. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2006.12.009
Cranford, J., McCabe, S., Boyd, C., Slayden, J., Reed, M., Ketchie, J., et al. (2008).
Reasons for nonresponse in a web-based survey of alcohol involvement among
first-year college students. Addictive Behaviors, 33, 206-210. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.
2007.07.008
Falkenbach, D., Poythress, N., & Creevy, C. (2008). The exploration of subclinical
psychopathic subtypes and the relationship with types of aggression. Personality
and Individual Differences, 44, 821-832. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.012
Finch, J., Okun, M., Pool, G., & Ruehlman, L. (1999). A comparison of the influence
of conflictual and supportive social interactions on psychological distress. Journal of
Personality, 67, 581-621. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00066
Fincham, F., Beach, S., Arias, I., & Brody, G. (1998). Childrens attributions in the family:
The children’s relationship attribution measure. Journal of Family Psychology, 12,
481-493. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.12.4.481
Fingerman, K. L. (1996). Sources of tension in the aging mother and adult daughter
relationship. Psychology and Aging, 11, 591-606. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.11.4.591
Fingerman, K. L. (2001). Aging mothers and their adult daughters: A study in mixed
emotions. New York: Springer.
Fingerman, K. L., Chen, P.-C., Hay, E. L., Cichy, K., & Lefkowitz, E. (2006). Ambivalent
reactions in the parent and offspring relationship. Journal of Gerontology: Psycho-
logical Sciences, 61(3), P152-P160.
Fingerman, K. L., Hay, E. L., Cichy, K. E., Tang, C. Y, Chen, P. C., & Lefkowitz, E. S.
(2003, November). Close ties with distant parents: Geographic separation in inter-
generational ties. Poster presented at the Gerontological Society of America Annual
Meeting, San Diego, CA.
Fingerman, K. L., Pitzer, L., Lefkowitz, E. S., Birditt, K. S., & Mroczek, D. K. (2008).
Ambivalent relationship qualities between adults and their parents: Implications
for the well-being of both parties. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
63(6), P362-P371
Flannery, D., Montemayor, R., & Eberly, M. (1994). The influence of parent negative
emotional expression on adolescents’ perceptions of their relationships with their
parents. Personal Relationships, 1, 259-274. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1994.
tb00065.x
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ / 133
Giarrusso, R., Silverstein, M., Gans, D., & Bengtson, V. (2005). Ageing parents and
adult children: New perspectives on intergenerational relationships. In M. L. Johnson
(Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of age and ageing (pp. 413-421). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Greenwell, L., & Bengtson, V. (1997). Geographic distance and contact between middle-
aged children and their parents: The effects of social class over 20 years. Journals
of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 52B(1), S13-S26. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52B.1.S13
Hendrick, S., & Hendrick, C. (2006). Measuring respect in close relationships. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 881-899. doi: 10.1177/0265407506070471
Herzog, A. R., & Rodgers, W. (1988). Interviewing older adults: Mode comparison
using data from a face-to-face survey and a telephone resurvey. The Public Opinion
Quarterly, 52, 84-99. doi: 10.1086/269083
Hingson, R., Zha, W., & Weitzman, E. (2009). Magnitude and trends in alcohol-related
mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18-24, 1998-2005. Journals
of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 16, 12-20.
Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P., & Haynes, O. M. (1993). Stability of emotion
experiences and their relations to traits of personality. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 64, 847-860. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.847
Knapp, H., & Kirk, S. (2003). Using pencil and paper, Internet, and touch-tone phones
for self-administered surveys: Does methodology matter? Computers in Human
Behavior, 19, 117-134. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(02)00008-0
Koropeckyj-Cox, T. (2002). Beyond parental status: Psychological well-being in middle
and old age. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 957-971. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.
2002.00957.x
Kring, A., Smith, D., & Neale, J. (1994). Individual differences in dispositional expressive-
ness: Development and validation of the emotional expressivity scale. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 934-949. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.934
Kubany, E., Leisen, M., Kaplan, A., & Kelly, M. (2000). Validation of a brief measure
of posttraumatic stress disorder: The Distressing Event Questionnaire (DEQ). Psycho-
logical Assessment, 12, 197-209. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.197
Lawton, L., Silverstein, M., & Bengtson, V. (1994). Affections, social contact, and
geographic distance between adult children and their parents. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 56, 57-68.
Lefkowitz, E. S., Cichy, K. E., Hay, E. L., Espinosa-Hernandez, G. E., & Fingerman, K. L.
(2008). Emotionally-charged conversations between adult offspring and their mothers
and fathers. Unpublished manuscript.
Lefkowitz, E. S., & Fingerman, K. L. (2003). Positive and negative emotional feelings
and behaviors in mother-daughters ties in late life. Journal of Family Psychology, 17,
607-617. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.607
Lin, G., & Rogerson, P. (1995). Elderly parents and the geographic availability of their
adult children. Research on Aging, 17, 303-331. doi: 10.1177/0164027595173004
Lowenstein, A. (2007). Solidarity-conflict and ambivalence: Testing two conceptual
frameworks and their impact on quality of life for older family members. Journal
of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 62B, S100-S107.
Luescher, K., & Pillemer, K. (1998). Intergenerational ambivalence: A new approach to
the study of parent-child relations in later life. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60,
413-425.
134 / PITZER, FINGERMAN AND LEFKOWITZ
Martini, T., Grusec, J., & Bernardini, S. (2001). Effects of interpersonal control, per-
spective taking, and attributions on older mothers’ and daughters’ satisfaction with
their helping relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 688-705. doi: 10.1037/
0893-3200.15.4.688
Miller, E., Neal, D., Roberts, L., Baer, J., Cressler, S., Metrik, J., et al. (2002). Test-retest
reliability of alcohol measures: Is there a difference between internet-based assess-
ment and traditional methods. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 56-63. doi:
10.1037/0893-164X.16.1.56
Newsom, J. T., Morgan, D. L., Nishishiba, M., & Rook, K. S. (2000, November). Devel-
opment of new comparable measures of negative and positive social exchanges. Poster
presented at the Gerontological Society of America annual meeting, Washington, DC.
Newsom, J., Nishishiba, M., Morgan, D., & Rook, K. (2003). The relative importance
of three domains of positive and negative social exchanges: A longitudinal model
with comparable measures. Psychology and Aging, 18, 746-754. doi: 10.1037/0882-
7974.18.4.746
Newsom, J., Rook, K., Nishishiba, M., Sorkin, D., & Mahan, T. (2005). Understand-
ing the relative importance of positive and negative social exchanges: Examining
specific domains and appraisals. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
60B, P304-P312. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.6.P304
Peters, C., Hooker, K., & Zvonkovic, A. (2006). Older parents’ perceptions of ambiv-
alence in relationships with their children. Family Relations, 55, 539-551. doi:
10.1111/j.1741-3729.2006.00424.x
Pett, M., Lackey, N., & Sullivan, J. (2003). Making sense of factor analysis: The use
of factor analysis of instrument development in health care research. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pillemer, K., & Suitor, J. J. (2002). Explaining mothers’ ambivalence toward their adult
children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 64, 602-613. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.
2002.00602.x
Priester, J., & Petty, R. (2001). Extending the bases of subjective attitudinal ambivalence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 19-34. doi: 10.1037/00223514.80.1.19
Rook, K. (1998). Investigating the positive and negative sides of personal relationships:
Through a lens darkly? In B. Spitzberg & W. Cupach (Eds.), The dark side of
close relationships (pp. 369-393). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rook, K. (2001). Emotional health and positive versus negative social exchanges:
A daily diary analysis. Applied Developmental Science, 5, 85-97. doi: 10.1207/
S1532480XADS0502_4
Rook, K. (2003). Exposure and reactivity to negative social exchanges: A preliminary
investigation using daily diary data. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
58B, P100-P111. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.P100
Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent-child relations across
the life course. New York: de Gruyter.
Stoppelbein, L., Greening, L., Jordan, S., Elkin, T. D., Moll, G., & Pullen, J. (2005).
Factor analysis of the pediatric symptom checklist with a chronically ill pediatric
population. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 349-355.
Sweet, J., Bumpass, L., & Call, V. (1988). The design and content of the National Survey of
Families and Households (Working Paper NSFH-1). Madison, WI: University of
Wisconsin, Center for Demography and Ecology.
DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE PARQ / 135
Thompson, M., Zanna, M., & Griffin, D. (1995). Let’s not be indifferent about (attitudinal)
ambivalence. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents
and consequences (pp. 361-385). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Uchino, B., Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T., & Bloor, L. (2004). Heterogeneity in social
networks: A comparison of different models linking relationships to psychological
outcomes. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 123-139. doi: 10.1521/
jscp.23.2.123.31014
Umberson, D. (1989). Relationships with children: Explaining parents’ psychological
well being. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 999-1012.
Umberson, D. (1992). Relationships between adult children and their parents: Psycho-
logical consequences for both generations. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
544, 664-674.
Ward, R. A. (2008). Multiple parent-adult child relations and well-being in middle and
later life. Journal of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological and Social Sciences, 63(4),
S239-S247.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1079. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Willson, A., Shuey, K., & Elder, G. (2003). Ambivalence in the relationship of adult
children to aging parents and in-laws. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 1055 1072.
doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.01055.x