Sun Juhyung 201905 Ma Phubbing
Sun Juhyung 201905 Ma Phubbing
Sun Juhyung 201905 Ma Phubbing
by
JUHYUNG SUN
ABSTRACT
This thesis explores phubbing (snubbing someone in the middle of a face-to-face conversation by
individual factors (personality and psychological factors) drive friend phubbing and if friend
phubbing leads to lower levels of friendship satisfaction. Also, this thesis investigates the
mediating role of friend phubbing between proposed predictors and friendship satisfaction.
Participants (n = 472) showed that depression and social anxiety were positively related to friend
phubbing while agreeableness and neuroticism were negatively related to friend phubbing.
Higher levels of friend phubbing resulted in decreased friendship satisfaction. The results
showed that friend phubbing mediates the relationships between each of the proposed predictors
(i.e., depression, social anxiety, agreeableness, and neuroticism) and friendship satisfaction. This
thesis is the first to examine the dynamics of phubbing in friendships from a communication
perspective.
by
JUHYUNG SUN
A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of The University of Georgia in Partial Fulfillment
MASTER OF ARTS
ATHENS, GEORGIA
2019
© 2019
Juhyung Sun
by
JUHYUNG SUN
Electronic Version:
Suzanne Barbour
Dean of the Graduate School
The University of Georgia
May 2019
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Professor Jennifer Samp, for all the support, encouragement, and advice she has provided
throughout past two years of my graduate studies. Her friendly guidance and constructive
feedback have been invaluable throughout all stages of this thesis. This thesis could not have
been accomplished without her. I am also thankful for her consistent support in pursuing a
doctoral degree. It was a great honor to work and study under her direction.
My thanks also go to the members of my thesis committee, Professor Analisa Arroyo, and
Professor Jiaying Liu, for agreeing to serve as my thesis committee and providing many
insightful comments that contributed greatly to improve this thesis and widen my research from
various perspectives.
I will always be thankful to my former graduate school advisor, Professor Namkee Park
at Yonsei University in South Korea. He has always been supportive of me and helped me in
I also thank my best friends, Sangah Park, Sunji Park, and Chaeju Oh, for their moral
support and friendship that I needed. Despite the long distance between us, they always stand by
me, cheer me on, and celebrate each accomplishment. I find myself lucky to have friends like
them in my life.
Last but not least, I must express very profound gratitude to my parents (Kyusoo Sun and
Sunghee Park) and brother (Jihoon Sun). My parents have sacrificed their lives for my brother
and myself and provided unconditional love and unfailing support. Thank them for their love,
iv
prayers, and caring for educating and preparing me for my future. I am so blessed to have them
as my parents. Also, I express my thanks to my brother for his continuous encouragement and
support throughout my life in general. I love them so much, and I would not have accomplished
Finally, my thanks go to all people who have supported me to complete this long journey
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ........................................................................................................... iv
CHAPTER
1 INTRODUCTION .........................................................................................................1
Overview ..................................................................................................................4
3 METHOD ....................................................................................................................24
Measures ................................................................................................................26
4 RESULTS ....................................................................................................................31
vi
Descriptive Statistics..............................................................................................31
5 DISCUSSION ..............................................................................................................40
Conclusion .............................................................................................................50
REFERENCES ..............................................................................................................................52
APPENDICES
vii
LIST OF TABLES
Page
Table 2: Mean, Standard Deviations, Cronbach’s Alphas, and Zero-order Pearson Correlations
viii
LIST OF FIGURES
Page
Figure 2: The Results of Structural Equations Model with the Hypothesized Model .................38
ix
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
our daily life. A recent report showed that over two billion people around the world own a
smartphone (Pew Research Center, 2018a) and about half of smartphone owners (54%) found
that they “couldn’t live without” their smartphones (Pew Research Center, 2015a). Moreover, a
among young adults aged between 18 and 29 (Pew Research Center, 2018b). The possible reason
for this increase is that smartphones provide opportunities for continual connection with others
(Chayko, 2008; Turkle 2011). Moreover, people initiate and maintain their social relationships
with their smartphones by making calls, sending text messages, and interacting on social
networks (Gibbs, Ellison, & Heino, 2006; Ling & Yttri, 2002). As a result, this advanced
communication technology has changed how people spent their time, interact, and manage their
social relationships. However, despite the obvious advantages, the use of smartphones in social
As many people have spent more time and relied heavily on their smartphones, more and
more people are showing problematic behavior in using their smartphones, resulting in negative
consequences concerning smartphone use (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005). In particular, phubbing
which includes four aspects – (a) nomophobia (NP): reflects fear of mobile phone detachment;
(b) interpersonal conflict (IC): concerns perceived conflicts caused by using smartphones during
face-to-face interactions; (c) self-isolation (SI): indicates the use of smartphones to escape from
1
social interactions and isolate themselves from others; and (d) problem acknowledgment (PA):
reflects the perception degree of negative aspects of phubbing behavior (Chotpitayasunondh &
The term is derived from two words: phone and snubbing. The word phone indicates
mobile phones which access to the Internet and support a variety of functions similar to a
computer and a multitude of other devices. The word snubbing stands for ignoring or neglecting,
which in turn results in insulting interlocutors by not paying any attention to them. Taken
together, phubbing refers to the act of snubbing someone in the middle of a face-to-face
and keeping a phone close by during face-to-face conversation with someone. With regard to the
term phubbing, there are two related referents: phubber and phubbee. A phubber is a person who
Recently, we can easily observe phubbing behavior in almost any social setting. Indeed,
Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas (2016) reported that a majority (70%) of people phub others at
least once per day and more than 78 percent of people are phubbed at least once per day. Despite
the pervasiveness of phubbing in our daily life, people have overlooked the negative impact of
this behavior. People still phub others in their social interactions although they have considered it
as a disrespectful behavior to others by making them feel excluded and unimportant (Karadağ et
al., 2015). Indeed, a majority of Americans (95%) think that it is inappropriate to use their
smartphones in real-life social interactions such as during meetings and meal times. However,
2
89% of smartphone owners still use their smartphones during their most recent times with others
(Pew Research Center, 2015b). Of course, people can use their smartphones specifically to avoid
others around them. Nonetheless, this report suggests that individuals do not realize the
frequency with which they phub others around them and more importantly, many individuals are
unaware of how they show such behavior and that phubbing can have a negative impact to
Phubbing behavior does not happen just in a specific type of interpersonal relationships.
People are phubbing casual acquaintances as well as significant others. However, people are
more likely to phub significant others such as a romantic partner, a close friend(s), and other
family members than those who are less close such as a work supervisor, strangers, and
acquaintances. Indeed, a recent study by Al-Saggaf and MacCulloch (2018) found that
smartphone users phub their friends more frequently than they phub strangers.
Although phubbing appears most frequently in friendships, research so far has been
conducted under the lens of marriage and other romantic relationships (e.g., Krasnova,
Abramova, Notter, & Baumann, 2016; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang, Xie, Wang, Wang, & Lie,
2017). No one has examined why people phub more frequently in friendship or whether there
might be certain personality traits and psychological factors that make people phub their
friend(s). Therefore, the present study investigates phubbing behavior in a specific close
relationship – friendship – to fully capture the phubbing behavior and its impact in different
types of interpersonal relationships. The main reason that this study focuses on this type of
relationship is that friendships are also significant relationships which influence our overall
health and well-being (e.g., Chopik, 2017; Demir, Özen, Doğan, Bilyk, & Tyrell, 2011), and
3
phubbing occurs most frequently in friendships compared to other types of close relationships
consequences of phubbing behavior. For instance, some studies identified possible predictors
which lead to phubbing behavior. Among them, smartphone addiction, which refers to the loss of
control over one’s smartphone use or excessive and compulsive smartphone use, appears as the
most significant predictor that increases phubbing (Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas, 2016;
Karadağ et al., 2015). As for consequences of phubbing behavior, other studies found that it
increases conflicts created by such behavior (Roberts & David, 2016), and threatens trust
(Roberts & David, 2017), conversation quality (Abeele, Antheunis, & Schouten, 2016),
relationship satisfaction (Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas, 2018b; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang
et al., 2017), impression formation (Abeele et al., 2016), and engagement (Roberts & David,
2017).
Despite the growing body of literature, there are few studies examining the relationships
empirical study has demonstrated phubbing behavior as a mediator. In other words, both possible
study. Therefore, the present study seeks to expand the findings by exploring the relationships
between individual factors (i.e., personality traits and psychological factors) and phubbing
Overview
The purpose of this study is to investigate: (a) the direct relationship between individual
factors (i.e., personality traits and risk psychological factors) and friend phubbing, (b) the direct
4
relationship between friend phubbing and friendship satisfaction, and (c) the mediating role of
friend phubbing in the association between proposed predictors (i.e., personality traits and risk
relationships, I expect that the results of the present study will provide a mechanism underlying
the phubbing behavior, which will eventually contribute to deepening the understanding of such
behavior.
This thesis is organized into five chapters beginning with an introduction in Chapter 1. In
Fernandez, Kuss, and Griffiths (2015) is examined to inform my model of friend phubbing. This
model describes the possible predictors of friend phubbing and then explores specific predictors
by introducing the hypotheses related to the relationship between each possible predictors and
friend phubbing. Second, I use the politeness theory (Brown & Levinson, 1987) to support why
the present study focuses on friendships instead of different types of relationships. Afterward, the
present a brief literature review on phubbing behavior to demonstrate whether such behavior can
hypotheses. It provides demographic information on participants and procedure for the data
gathering process. Then, it is described with a review of the instruments used in this study. This
Chapter 4 reports the empirical results of this study. It begins with descriptive statistics
followed by correlation test to investigate the relationships among proposed variables of the
5
present study. It then continues with structural equation modeling (SEM) to test the hypotheses
Lastly, Chapter 5 provides a review of the findings of the study. The findings of the
present study then are interpreted and discussed based on the literature review. Prior to
concluding the study, the chapter addresses the implications and limitations for future research.
6
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
This chapter provides a review of the literature underlying this thesis. Different theories
are presented to explain the relationships among possible predictors, consequences of phubbing
behavior in friendships and further find a mediating role of phubbing behavior in friendships.
introduced. Then, literature with regard to possible predictors is presented. This is followed by a
review of politeness theory (Brown & Levinson, 1987) and social displacement hypothesis
(Kraut et al., 1998) along with consequences of friend phubbing. Based on the literature review,
the mediating role of friend phubbing is discussed. Finally, the hypotheses and overview of the
mobile phone (or smartphone) use” proposed by Billieux and colleagues (2015). Phubbing
behavior can be considered as problematic smartphone use. Indeed, Blachnio and Przepiorka
(2018) argued that phubbing is an umbrella term which covers three pathways of problematic
smartphone use (i.e., excessive reassurance seeking, impulsivity, and extraversion) in that both
and Douglas (2016) also stated that phubbing is one of the problematic behaviors that have
negative impacts on those who phub and who are phubbed. Other studies further argued that
7
phubbing is closely related to problematic smartphone use in that both are regarded as
Billieux and colleagues (2015) proposed three pathways which lead to the problematic
smartphone use including (a) excessive reassurance seeking, (b) impulsivity, and (c)
extraversion. Since people use their smartphones in different ways depending on their various
motivations, each of these pathways within the model is closely related to individual
The first pathway, called the excessive reassurance seeking, describes individuals whose
attachment, higher levels of neuroticism, depression, social anxiety, as well as general anxiety to
obtain reassurance from others in affective relationships (e.g., romantic partners, family
members, and friends). Consequently, these individuals have a tendency to show an addictive
The second pathway, called the impulsive pathway, describes individuals who are
susceptible to show problematic smartphone use because they have poor impulse control and
psychopathic traits, antisocial personality, and ADHD symptoms. These impulsivity traits are
closely related to three different patterns of use, namely addictive, antisocial, and/or risky
Finally, the third pathway, called the extraversion pathway, describes individuals whose
problematic smartphone use is driven by high extraversion to keep communicating with others
and to develop new relationships. These individuals are likely to have high levels of sensation
8
seeking, sensitivity and dependence to rewards. As a result, the extraversion pathway can lead to
either antisocial or risky patterns of smartphone use. To conclude, Billieux and colleagues (2015)
highlighted that one or more described factors and/or pathways can be applied in predicting
pathways in understanding problematic smartphone use. For example, several studies supported
the first pathway of problematic smartphone use. They found that individuals who display
problematic smartphone use are likely to express more depressive symptoms (Elhai, Dvorak,
Levine, & Hall, 2017a; Ha, Chin, Park, Ryu, & Yu, 2008; Kim, Seo, & David, 2015; Smetaniuk,
2014), emotional instability (Smetaniuk, 2014), neuroticism (Butt & Phillips, 2008; Ehrenberg,
Juckes, White, & Walsh, 2008; Takao, 2014), higher levels of anxiety (Elhai, Levine, Dvorak, &
Hall, 2017b; Ha et al., 2008; Lee, Chang, Lin, & Cheng, 2014) and lower self-esteem (Bianchi &
Phillips, 2005; Ehrenberg et al., 2008; Ha et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2014). With regard to the
second pathway which is the impulsive pathway, some studies demonstrated that problematic
smartphone use is closely associated with impulsiveness, (Khang, Kim, & Kim, 2013),
aggressiveness (Lee et al., 2018) and ADHD symptoms (Kim, 2018; Seo, Kim, & David, 2015;
Zheng et al., 2014). Other studies showed that problematic smartphone use is related to high
extraversion (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005; Butt & Phillips, 2008; Ehrenberg et al., 2008; Smetaniuk,
2014; Takao, 2014), supporting the third pathway of problematic smartphone use.
previous studies which found that personality constructs and psychological factors cause
phubbing (Balta, Emirekin, Kicaburun, & Griffiths, 2018), it is theoretically plausible to regard
phubbing behavior as problematic smartphone use. Accordingly, this study examines the
9
associations of these individual characteristics including personality traits and psychological
predictors and phubbing behavior. In the following, hypotheses regarding the influences of each
personality and psychological traits are developed by focusing on friendships because the
relationship is significant for most individuals (Chopik, 2017) and they are more likely to phub
when they are with their friends than when they are with other people (Al-Saggaf & MacCulloch,
2018).
Depression refers to a mood disorder that causes feelings of sadness, loss of interest or
pleasure, emptiness, worthlessness, and low self-worth (Sue, Sue, Sue, & Sue, 2015). Increased
depression results from lower levels of positive reinforcement (Lewinsohn, 1974) and leads to
negative impacts on an individual’s daily life and social interactions (Beck & Alford, 2009).
Many studies have demonstrated that problematic smartphone use is closely related to
depression. Indeed, individuals who have depressive symptoms are more likely to exhibit
problematic smartphone use due to a lack of self-regulation (e.g., Park, 2005; Thomée,
Härenstam, & Hagberg, 2011). Relatedly, Kim and colleagues (2015) found that those who score
high on depression tend to spend more time on communication activities through their
smartphone than those who score low on depression because they expect smartphone would
relieve their negative feelings. These researchers suggested that these people feel less risky to be
rejected by others when they communicate with others via their smartphones.
With regard to phubbing behavior, some studies have demonstrated the relationship
between depression and phubbing behavior. For example, Roberts and David (2016) investigated
10
the relationships among phubbing, relationship satisfaction and depression in marriage and found
that phubbing results in individuals experiencing a greater feeling of depression among romantic
partners. Similarly, Roberts and David (2017) demonstrated that phubbing is associated with
higher levels of depression. In another study regarding the impact of technology interference
romantic relationships, McDaniel and Coyne (2016) found that smartphone use frequently
interrupts interactions such as couple activities, conversations, and meal times with their
partners. They also demonstrated that frequent technological interruptions such as using
smartphones in social interactions (also known as phubbing) are significantly related to higher
behavior, given the relationship between depression and problematic smartphone use (Billieux et
al., 2015) and the empirical previous studies discussed above, it is theoretically possible to
assume that depression is associated with higher levels of phubbing in friendships in that
depressed individuals tend to heavily rely on their smartphones in their social interactions.
Social anxiety is the presence of intense fear or anxiety of being negatively evaluated or
rejected in real or imagined social interactions with others, which is accompanied by feelings of
Specifically, Schlenker and Leary (1982) argued that individuals tend to experience feelings of
11
social anxiety: (a) when “people have inadequate or inappropriate social skills” (i.e., skills deficit
approach; pp. 642), (b) when “people evaluate themselves more negatively” (i.e., cognitive self-
evaluation model; pp. 643), and (c) when “neutral stimuli become paired with aversive social
In social settings, individuals with higher traits of social anxiety are reluctant to spend
time talking with others, manage superficial and broader topics than the specific, convey shorter
stories and end conversations quickly (Depaulo, Epstein, & LeMay, 1990). Often, these
2007).
Several studies have shown that those who are socially anxious tend to experience an
increased level of problematic smartphone use. For example, Demirci, Akgönül, and Akpinar
(2015) investigated the relationship between problematic smartphone use and social anxiety and
found anxiety as a key determinant of problematic smartphone use. Relatedly, Enez Darcin, and
colleagues (2016) revealed that anxious individuals tend to have a greater risk of smartphone
addiction because they not only rely on mobile communication (e.g., texting) to avoid actual
environments.
predicts phubbing behavior, it is plausible to assume that social anxiety is related to higher levels
of phubbing behavior given the significant relationship between social anxiety and problematic
12
smartphone use (Billieux et al., 2015) and the previous studies aforementioned. Thus, I
hypothesize that:
Why do people behave differently in the same situation? Personality traits have been
widely used to answer this question because personality is considered as a stable feature that
shows individual differences (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Among various personality constructs, I
agreeableness, neuroticism, and openness to experience in the present study. The reason to adopt
Although there are many studies investigated the relationship between the Big-Five
personality traits and problematic use of smartphones, the outcomes of the previous studies are
significantly related to problematic smartphone use, an empirical study by Bianchi and Phillips
(2005) showed that the relationship between problematic use of smartphone and neuroticism is
not significant. The discrepancy in the results across studies could be due to the complex nature
of personality traits. In addition, there are scant studies examined personality traits as potential
findings and further clarify relationships of personality traits and phubbing behavior, this study
explores key personality traits. Among different personality traits of the Big-Five, therefore, I
propose that two of personality traits influence friend phubbing: agreeableness and neuroticism.
13
First, agreeableness refers to an individual’s tendency to harmonize or get along with
others in social interactions (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Agreeable individuals are described as
being kind, sympathetic, well-mannered, considerate, cooperative, and trusting (McCrae & John,
1992). Unlike the rest of the other four Big-Five personality traits which are highly related to
“self”, agreeableness places more emphasis on relationships with “others” based on equality and
honesty (Phillips, Butt, & Blasczynski, 2006). In this regard, agreeableness can represent the
most relevant trait in examining phubbing since such behavior occurs in the presence of others.
Considering individuals with higher traits of agreeableness avoid harmful consequences (e.g.,
conflicts) to maintain social harmony with others (McCrae & John, 1992), it is reasonable to
expect that agreeable people will focus more on their interlocutor instead of using their
associated with problematic smartphone use. For example, Andreassen, Griffiths, Gjertsen,
Krossbakken, Kvam, and Pallesen (2013) revealed that disagreeable individuals (i.e., low levels
of agreeableness) are more likely to exhibit problematic smartphone use than those with higher
scores on agreeableness. With regard to phubbing behavior, there is a study examining the
relationship between agreeableness and phubbing behavior. For instance, a recent study by Fritz
(2018) predicted that agreeableness can play a significant role in predicting phubbing behavior
by proposing a new conceptual model of phubbing. This study demonstrated that increased
agreeableness is directly associated with decreased phubbing behavior. Considering the previous
14
Second, neuroticism is a tendency to experience emotional instability and maladjustment
impulsiveness and worrying (McCrae & Costa, 1987). Neurotic individuals are emotional and
react sensitively to various stimuli interpreting common situations as unpleasant and threatening
(Anastasi & Urbina, 1997). While nonneurotic individuals locate themselves in traditional
communications such as face-to-face interactions, neurotic individuals are prone to place greater
importance on mediated communication such as the Internet and mobile phones when they
factor related to problematic smartphone use. For example, Ehrenberg and colleagues (2008)
found a significant relationship between neuroticism and problematic smartphone use tendencies
among young adults and reported that individuals with higher traits of neuroticism tend to spend
more time sending and receiving text messages. More recently, Roberts and colleagues (2015)
revealed that those who score higher on neuroticism reported greater levels of smartphone
addiction. These findings argued that neurotic individuals tend to show heavy smartphone
dependency which eventually leads to a higher likelihood of problematic smartphone use since
they use their smartphones as a means of dealing with their stress and recovering their negative
feelings.
colleagues (2015) and the majority of empirical previous studies, I assume that neuroticism also
15
Why friendship? – Politeness theory
time, which is intended to facilitate socio-emotional goals of the participants, and may involve
varying types and degree of companionship, intimacy, affection, and mutual assistance (Hays,
1988, pp.395).” That is, friendship is an affective relationship which is based on a stronger
mutual tie between people. Relationships, especially friendships, play a critical role in
individuals’ lives in that the relationship influences our health, happiness, social and emotional
Then, why I consider friendships in the present study? As mentioned earlier, most of the
studies related to phubbing behavior have focused on romantic relationships (e.g., Krasnova et
al., 2016; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang et al., 2017) in that romantic partners have higher
attentiveness to each other when they are spending time together (Miller-Ott & Kelly, 2015).
Also, romantic partners tend to provide security and enduring emotional supports (Florian,
Mikulincer, & Bucholtz, 1995). However, friendships play a central role too in that friends also
serve emotional supports and friendships have a far greater impact on our overall health,
happiness, and personal well-being than different types of relationships (Chopik, 2017; Demir et
al., 2011). Above all, phubbing occurs more frequently in friendships than other relationships
(Al-Saggaf & MacCulloch, 2018). Given the significance of friendships and the pervasiveness of
phubbing behavior in friendships, it is necessary to understand how people use their smartphones
Why does phubbing frequently occur in friendships? This question can be addressed
through the lens of politeness theory (Brown & Levinson, 1987). As Goffman (1967) introduced,
16
politeness is particularly related to the central role of face in the interactions. He conceptualized
that an individual, who is a social actor, shows linguistic, behavioral, and gestural performances
to provide (also protect) his/her positive self-image. Relatedly, Brown and Levinson (1987)
proposed that individuals have two different faces: (a) positive face - a desire for approval and
connection with others, and (b) negative face - a desire for freedom and independence from
imposition by others. These researchers further argued that there are three factors related to
politeness: (a) the relative power, (b) the degree of imposition, and (c) the social distance
between the speaker and the listener. In other word, people are likely to be polite (e.g., using
more polite strategies such as using more polite languages) when they are with individuals who
have a greater power than those who have equality or lower power, when requesting to do a big
favor than a small favor, and when talking with unknown people than familiar individuals.
Consistent with the three aspects of politeness theory approach, the notion of friendship
is based on a belief in balanced power (equality or fairness) and high social distance (familiarity)
(Austin, 1980; Lim & Bowers, 1991). It suggests that individuals tend to be less polite which in
turn leads to being less concerned about their self-images when they are with their friends. With
regard to two different faces, Miller-Ott and Kelly (2017) demonstrated that the use of
smartphones in the presence of friends violates positive faces conveying possible messages such
as that a person who uses a smartphone is bored and that he does not the relationship seriously.
often feel that they do not have to present (or protect) their public self-images and do not worry
about being perceived as impolite (Brown & Levinson, 1987). That is, not being attentive during
face-to-face interaction does not influence in losing their faces in friendships. Indeed, based on
17
the politeness theory, Harrison, Bealing, and Salley (2015) suggested that people feel free to use
Despite the pervasiveness of phubbing behavior in friendship, friend phubbing and its
association with friendship satisfaction are surprisingly sparse. Therefore, the present study
examines friend phubbing and its impact on friendship satisfaction based on the politeness
theory.
I adopt the view of the displacement hypothesis to explain the detrimental effect of friend
postulates that time spent on media such as smartphones would displace (or reduce) important
activities with significant others since time is a limited resource (i.e., a zero-sum relationship)
(Kraut et al., 1998). That is, when individuals spend more time on communication technologies,
they spend less time on meaningful interactions with their close others (e.g., a romantic partner,
family members, and close friends). Consequently, this argument asserts that despite increased
which posited that mediated communication allows people to expand their social networks
permitting them to connect to others more frequently (Walther, 1996), the displacement has a
negative impact on social interactions with their close others such as reducing social involvement
and psychological well-being (Kraut et al., 1998; Nie, 2001; Valkenburg &Peter, 2007).
Empirically, several studies have supported the social displacement hypothesis. For
example, Nie, Hillygus, and Erbring (2002) conducted a survey on the social impact of Internet
use and found that excessive Internet use causes a negative impact on face-to-face interactions by
18
reducing time spent with family members and friends. More recently, Ahn and Shin (2013) found
perceived quality and quantity of social relationships. These findings suggested that problematic
Even the mere presence of a smartphone distracts and has negative consequences for
social interactions. Indeed, Thornton, Faires, Robbins, and Rollins (2014) demonstrated that the
presence of a smartphone serves as a distractor and then result in attentional and performance
deficits, which ultimately leads to detrimental effects on social interactions with others. With
regard to phubbing behavior, Halpern and Katz (2017) suggested a theoretical model in which
constant media use such as frequency of text messaging is positively related to partner phubbing
(Pphubbing) with increased conflicts and lack of intimacy, which ultimately reduces perceived
relationship quality. Similarly, Roberts and David (2016) examined partner phubbing
(Pphubbing) based on the displacement hypothesis and found that partner phubbing negatively
As the social displacement hypothesis (Kraut et al., 1998) and the empirical results
showed, it is possible to argue that the displacement hypothesis can be applied to explain
phubbing behavior in friendships. Accordingly, I assume that phubbing behavior will lead to
having a detrimental effect on social interactions, especially with friends. In the following, a
displacement hypothesis.
19
Consequences of Friend Phubbing
Friendship satisfaction
Friendship satisfaction varies according to the degree to which a person perceives that
his/her friends fulfill various needs and desires (McAdams, 1988). Friendship satisfaction is an
important determinant of an individual’s overall life satisfaction and social integration (Demir &
relationship maintenance and the likelihood of relational closeness within social relationships
commitment, and relational certainty (e.g., Guerrero, Anderson & Afifi, 2011; Rusbult & Buunk,
1993).
smartphone use including phubbing remains deficient. Only one study by Bae (2015), which
focused on problematic smartphone use among Korean adolescents, found that high levels of
With regard to phubbing behavior, Miller-Ott and Kelly (2017) discussed the dilemma of
smartphone use in the presence of friends and offered some important implications for phubbing
behavior whether it threatens their face or not. However, this study did not investigate the impact
of phubbing behavior on friendship satisfaction. Therefore, the present study suggests that the
I hypothesize that:
20
The Mediating Role of Friend Phubbing
Despite the growing attention of phubbing behavior, most existing studies have focused
either on the antecedents or on the effects of phubbing behavior. In other words, very little is
known about the mediating role of phubbing behavior which examines both possible predictors
and consequences of phubbing behavior. As the theoretical assumptions and the empirical studies
argued above, it is reasonable to expect that friend phubbing will mediate the relationships
between proposed predictors and consequence. Therefore, the present study proposes a complex
model in which risk psychological factors (i.e., depression and social anxiety), personality traits
(i.e., agreeableness and neuroticism), friend phubbing and friendship satisfaction will be
significantly associated and in particularly friend phubbing will mediate those proposed
H3a: Friend phubbing mediates the relationship between depression and friendship
satisfaction.
H3b: Friend phubbing mediates the relationship between social anxiety and friendship
satisfaction.
H3c: Friend phubbing mediates the relationship between agreeableness and friendship
satisfaction.
H3d: Friend phubbing mediates the relationship between neuroticism and friendship
satisfaction.
psychological factors (i.e., depression and social anxiety) and personality traits (i.e.,
21
agreeableness and neuroticism) are independent variables, phubbing behavior in friendship
(friend phubbing) is a mediator variable, and friendship satisfaction is the dependent variable. It
was hypothesized that depression, social anxiety, and neuroticism will be positively associated
with friend phubbing, whereas agreeableness will be negatively associated with friend phubbing.
With regard to the indirect effect of friend phubbing among all the variables of this study, it was
hypothesized that friend phubbing will mediate the relationships between (1) depression and
friendship satisfaction; (2) social anxiety and friendship satisfaction; (3) agreeableness and
friendship satisfaction; and (4) neuroticism and friendship satisfaction (see Figure 1).
22
Figure 1. The hypothesized model for the study
Notes. Fphubbing: Friend phubbing, NP: Nomophobia, IC: Interpersonal Conflict, SI: Self-
23
CHAPTER 3
METHOD
This chapter explains the methodology used to test the hypotheses and the hypothesized
model provided in the second chapter. It begins by presenting demographic information of the
participants whose participants were and how they were sampled. The procedure that was
followed to complete an online survey is also included. Additionally, the instrument that was
used for this study is described. Lastly, I discuss the method used to analyze the data for the
First, all materials related to the present study were approved by university IRB. Then,
participants were recruited from a research pool, accessible to undergraduate students enrolled in
classes in the Department of Communication Studies at the University of Georgia in the United
States. Students enrolled in COMM1100, COMM1500 or other upper levels of COMM courses
participated in the survey on a voluntary basis. An email invitation was sent to these students
with a consent form which included the purpose of the study, procedures, risks/discomforts,
benefits, confidentiality, compensation and contact information of the researcher and a link to the
The online survey consisted of four sections (Appendix A). First, demographic
information (e.g., gender, age, ethnicity, and current relationship status) was presented to define
24
the sample. Second, participants were asked to report time spent on smartphone use in a typical
day and which functions/applications (e.g., messaging, SNS, calling, and etc.) they mostly used
to understand duration and usage patterns of smartphone use. The next section included
neuroticism), psychological factors (i.e., depression and social anxiety) and friendship
satisfaction. Because the present study mainly focused on friendship, participants were asked to
think about their current friendships that meet up together or have contact within two weeks.
Lastly, participants were asked to indicate their full name, course name, and their instructor’s
As a result, a total of 519 students participated in the survey. Among 519 participants,
472 were valid (n = 472) for further statistical analysis because 3 did not complete the survey, 4
did not have their own smartphone and the rest (40 participants) were regarded as influential
outliers which observed farthest from the centroid (create heteroscedasticity) indicating less
Of the 472 participants, 299 were female (63.3%), 171 were male (36.2%), and 2 did not
prefer to answer. The average age of the participants was 19.85 ranged between 18 and 28 years
were Asian; 20 (4.2%) were Black/African American; 19 (4.0%) were Hispanic/Latino; 9 (1.9%)
were Biracial or Multiracial; and 2 participants reported other ethnicities. Current relationship
status was measured with three categories. More than half of the participants (n = 283) were
single; 159 (33.7%) were in a relationship; and 30 (6.4%) reported that it is complicated.
25
Measures
Smartphone use. Participants were asked to indicate duration and usage patterns of
smartphone use by the following two questions (Haug et al., 2015): “What is your duration of
smartphone use in a typical day?” by choosing one of five options: (1) less than 10 minutes; (2)
11 – 60 minutes; (3) 1 – 2 hours; (4) 3 – 4 hours; and (5) more than 5 hours and “What
choosing three options among 11 options (1) text messaging; (2) social networking; (3) calling;
(4) gaming; (5) searching; (6) taking pictures/videos; (7) watching movies; (8) reading
news/checking weather; (9) listening to music; (10) using maps; and (11) other.
answer the following question: “Do you know what the term phubbing means?” with “Yes” or
“No” to assess familiarity with the term phubbing. Then, friend phubbing of the participants was
assessed by using a modified version of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP) which was
recently developed by Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas (2018a). Friend phubbing was measured
with four factors (i.e., nomophobia, interpersonal conflict, self-isolation, and problem
participants’ intention or willingness to phub their friends during an interaction. Since the present
study focused on phubbing behavior in friendships, the referent was replaced from others to my
friend(s) who meet up together or contact them within two weeks. The modified GSP comprises
15 items on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always) with four factors:
Nomophobia (NP) (e.g., “I worry that I will miss something important if I do not check my
phone), Interpersonal Conflict (IC) (e.g., “I have conflicts with my friend(s) because I am using
my phone), Self-isolation (SI) (e.g., “I would rather pay attention to my phone than talk to my
26
friend(s)”), and Problem Acknowledgement (PA) (e.g., “I pay attention to my phone for longer
In order to test how well this modified scale measure friend phubbing representing the
number of constructs required in the data and to assess the measurement validity, confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) was used. As a result of CFA, it was observed that Item 3 (i.e., “I place my
phone where I can see it”, see Appendix A) was unacceptable because of unsatisfied standardized
regression weights (<.30) and squared multiple correlations (<.10) (Schumacker& Lomax,
2004). Consequently, this item was removed from the analyses and the result showed that the
scale was valid for assessing four factors among the participants to measure phubbing behavior
in friendships displayed a good fit: χ2 (71, N = 472) = 195.41, p <.001, χ2/df = 2.75, CFI = .93,
NFI = .92, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06. The reliabilities of the subscales – Nomophobia (M =
Depression. The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D scale)
developed by Radloff (1977) was designed to measure levels of depression. The CES-D scale
includes 20 items (e.g., During the past week “I felt depressed” and “I had trouble keeping my
mind on what I was doing”) on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Rarely or none of the time
- less than 1 day) to 4 (Most or all of the time -5-7 days). To test the validity of this scale,
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted. The result showed that Item 7 (i.e., “I felt
that everything I did was an effort”, see Appendix A) should be removed because it did not meet
the criteria of the acceptance obtained lower standardized regression weights (<.30) and squared
multiple correlations (<.10) (Schumacker & Lomax, 2004). This item was removed from the
27
further analysis and then revealed that the scale was valid to measure participants’ depression
levels resulted in a satisfactory fit: χ2 (141, N = 472) = 330.99, p <.001, χ2/df = 2.35, CFI = .94,
NFI = .90, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .05. Then, to compute the variable for depression, reverse
items (i.e., item 4, 8, 12, and 16) were recoded so that individuals with scores tend to have
Social Anxiety. The Interaction Anxiousness Scale (IAS) developed by Leary (1983) was
used to measure trait levels of social anxiety. The IAS includes 15 items (e.g., “I often feel
nervous even in casual get-togethers” and “In general, I’m a shy person”) on a 7-point Likert
scale ranging from 1 (Not at all characteristic of me) and 7 (Extremely characteristic of me). To
assess validity of this scale, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted and showed that
it was valid to assess the levels of social anxiety resulting in a satisfactory fit: χ2 (84, N = 472) =
273.36, p <.001, χ2/df = 3.25, CFI = .93, NFI = .90, SRMR = .06, RMSEA = .07. Four items (i.e.,
item 3, 6, 10, and 15) were recoded and computed so that individuals with higher scores tend to
have higher traits of anxiousness in social interactions (M = 2.73, SD = .63, Cronbach’s α = .84).
Personality Traits – Agreeableness and Neuroticism. The subscales of the Big Five Trait
Taxonomy developed by John and Srivastava (1999) were designed to measure the
Agreeableness and the Neuroticism of the participants. The Big Five Trait Taxonomy-
agreeableness and neuroticism consist of 9 items (e.g., I see myself as someone who “is helpful
and unselfish with others” and “has a forgiving nature”) and 8 items (e.g., I see myself as
someone who “can be tense” and “gets nervous easily”) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1
(Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree). Prior to the computation of each personality trait,
reverse items (agreeableness – item 1, 3, 6, and 8; neuroticism – item 2, 5, and 7) were recoded,
28
showed that each subscale was valid to measure participants’ agreeableness and neuroticism,
respectively displayed a satisfactory fit: χ2 (24, N = 472) = 91.35, p <.001, χ2/df = 3.81, CFI
= .93, NFI = .91, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .07 for Agreeableness; and χ2 (19, N = 472) = 73.89, p
<.001, χ2/df = 3.89, CFI = .95, NFI = .94, SRMR = .04, RMSEA = .07 for Neuroticism. The
the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS) (Hendrick, 1988). Referents were replaced from
partner to friend(s) and from relationship to friendship to investigate the participants’ satisfaction
in their current friendship. The modified RAS consists of 7 items (e.g., “How well does your
friend(s) meet your needs?” and “In general, how satisfied are you with your current
well/Extremely satisfied). Negatively worded items (i.e., item 4 and 7) were recoded and
computed so that higher scores indicate high satisfaction in their current friendships.
To test how well the modified scale measures friendship satisfaction and to check the
measurement validity, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was applied. The results of the
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) revealed that Item 7 (i.e., “How many problems are there in
your friendships?”) had unacceptable standardized regression weights (<.30) and squared
multiple correlations (<.10) (Schumacker& Lomax, 2004). Consequently, this item was removed
from the analyses and the result showed that the scale was valid for assessing the friendship
satisfaction of the participants resulted in satisfactory fit: χ2 (9, N = 472) = 43.34, p <.001, χ2/df =
4.82, CFI = .98, NFI = .97, SRMR = .03, RMSEA = .06. The reliability of this scale was also
29
Data Analysis
Data were analyzed using SPSS version 25.0 and AMOS version 25.0 software for
Windows. In order to identify the relationships among proposed predictors (i.e., depression,
social anxiety, agreeableness, and neuroticism), friend phubbing and friendship satisfaction,
structural equation modeling (SEM) was employed. Before using structural equation models,
frequency, descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) were analyzed
with SPSS. Then, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to check the validity of
each scale and examine the hypothesized model fit with the AMOS.
In terms of sample size, Kline (2005) recommended that the minimum sample size
should be greater than 200 for path analysis or structural equation modeling (SEM). As Preacher
and Hayes (2004) suggested, a bootstrapping procedure with 1000 bootstrap samples and a 95%
bias-corrected confidence intervals (CI) was conducted to test the mediating effect of friend
Employing maximum likelihood estimation, the following criteria were used to examine
the overall measurement fit in the CFA and structural equation models (SEM) (Bentler &
Bonnet, 1980; Hooper, Coughlan, & Mullen, 2008; Kline, 2005): (1) the chi-square (χ2) statistic
(non-significant, χ2/df < .05); (2) the comparative fit index (CFI, > .90); (3) the normed-fit index
(NFI, > .90); (4) the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR, < .08); (5) the root mean
30
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
This chapter describes the empirical results of this study. It begins with descriptive
statistics followed by correlation test to investigate the relationships among all variables of the
present study. It then continues with structural equation models to test the hypotheses and the
Descriptive statistics
In terms of smartphone use, more than half of the participants (n = 250, 53.0%) indicated
that they spent ‘3 – 4 hours’ using their smartphones in a day; 133 (28.2%) spent ‘1 – 2 hours’;
80 (16.9%) spent ‘more than 5 hours’; 9 (1.9%) spent ‘11 – 60 minutes’ and no one reported ‘less
functions/applications (or apps), participants indicated that social networking, text messaging
With regard to knowledge about the phubbing term, interestingly, most of the participants
(94.7%) indicated that they do not know about the term phubbing (n = 447), while 25 (5.3%)
reported that they know what the phubbing term means (see Table 1).
31
Gender
Ethnics
Asian 73 15.5
Other 2 0.4
Relationship status
Smartphone ownership
No 0 0.0
32
More than 5 hours 80 16.9
Searching 97 20.6
Calling 75 15.9
Gaming 32 6.8
News/Weather 12 2.5
No 447 94.7
Yes 25 5.3
First, correlation analyses were run to examine the relationships among all variables of
this study. In addition, multicollinearity within a set of independent variables was tested through
variance inflation factor analysis (VIF). The result showed that the study variables were
significantly correlated (Table 2). The outcomes of the VIF showed that collinearity statistics
obtained VIF values of 1.543 (depression), 1.390 (social anxiety), 1.189 (agreeableness), and
33
1.923 (neuroticism), indicating that the VIF values are obtained ranged from 1 to 10 (i.e., VIF <
10 is acceptable) (Hair, Black, Banbin, & Anderson, 2010). It was concluded that there are no
34
Table 2. Mean, Standard Deviations, Cronbach’s alphas, and Zero-order Pearson Correlations among the Study Variables (n = 472)
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Friend phubbing -
2. Depression .29** -
35
To test the hypothesized model as depicted in Figure 1 and relationships among the
study variables, structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted with 5 observed variables
(i.e., depression, social anxiety, agreeableness, neuroticism, and friendship satisfaction) and 1
latent variable (i.e., friend phubbing). To be specific, each of the individual factors (i.e.,
personality traits and psychological factors) was regressed onto friend phubbing, and friend
phubbing was regressed onto friendship satisfaction. Overall, the hypothesized model resulted in
a good model fit: χ2 (19, N = 472) = 87.26, p <.001, χ2/df = 4.59, CFI = .93, NFI = .92, SRMR
estimates (squared multiple correlations) for the endogenous variable were shown in Figure 2. As
this study predicted (Figure 1), three of the formal hypotheses related to the predictors of friend
phubbing were fully supported: (a) H1a was supported; the path from depression to friend
phubbing was significant and positive (.32, p < .001), (b) H1b was supported; the path from social
anxiety to friend phubbing was significant and positive (.24, p < .001), (c) H1c was supported; the
path from agreeableness to friend phubbing was significant and negative (-.27, p < .001), and (d)
H1d was not supported; the path from neuroticism to friend phubbing was significant but not
positive (-.18, p < .05). Among the four predictors of friend phubbing, the results showed that the
anxiety.
36
In regard to the direct relationship between friend phubbing and friendship satisfaction
(H2), it was supported; the path from friend phubbing to friendship satisfaction was significant
As predicted in this study, friend phubbing played a role as a mediator between proposed
independent and dependent variables. Specifically, H3a was supported; the path from depression
to friendship satisfaction through friend phubbing (indirect effect of friend phubbing) was
significant (-.04, 95% CI [-.10, -.01]) were significant and negative. H3b was supported; the path
from social anxiety to friendship satisfaction through friend phubbing was significant (-.03, 95%
CI [-.07, -.01]). H3c was supported; the path from agreeableness to friendship satisfaction through
friend phubbing (indirect effect of friend phubbing) was significant (.03, 95% CI [.01, .08]). H3d
was supported; the path from neuroticism to friendship satisfaction through friend phubbing was
To summarize, the results of this study are as follows: (a) depression was directly and
indirectly associated with friendship satisfaction via friend phubbing; (b) social anxiety was
indirectly associated with friendship satisfaction via friend phubbing; (c) agreeableness was
directly and indirectly associated with friendship satisfaction via friend phubbing, and (d)
neuroticism was indirectly associated with friendship satisfaction via friend phubbing.
In regard to R2, a total of 24.7% variance in friend phubbing and 29.2% of the variance in
37
Figure 2. The results of structural equations modeling with the hypothesized model.
Notes. Fphubbing: Friend phubbing, NP: Nomophobia, IC: Interpersonal Conflict, SI: Self-
isolation, and PA: Problem Acknowledgement.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
38
Table 3. Summary of Hypothesis Testing Results
39
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION
This chapter interprets and discusses the results presented in the previous chapter
(Chapter 4). The implications of this study are then presented. Finally, limitations and
Although the use of smartphones has permeated into a wide number of areas of our life
and that phubbing frequently occurs in friendships, previous research primarily has examined
phubbing in the context of romantic relationships so far. The primary purpose of this
investigation was to uncover the mechanisms underlying phubbing behavior and its relationship
with other proposed variables in friendships. Specifically, this study presented an integrated
model that explains such associations by finding the significant (a) effects of depression, social
satisfaction, and (c) mediating role of friend phubbing in the associations between each of
proposed predictors (i.e., depression, social anxiety, agreeableness, and neuroticism) and
friendship satisfaction.
To support the arguments and test the hypotheses, this study presented three theoretical
approaches: problematic smartphone use model (Billieux et al., 2015), politeness theory (Brown
& Levinson, 1987) and social displacement hypothesis (Kraut et al., 1998). Consequently, I
found that four individual characteristics such as depression, social anxiety, agreeableness, and
neuroticism are significantly linked with friend phubbing, and this ultimately leads to having a
detrimental impact on friendship satisfaction. Further, the results revealed that friend phubbing
40
mediates such relationships (between each individual characteristic and friendship satisfaction).
The results of structural equation models (SEM) in this study showed that all the hypotheses
except H1d were empirically supported and found that the hypothesized model of this study
displays goodness-of-fit. The results of the present study are discussed in greater detail below
with three main conclusions: (a) predictors of friend phubbing, (b) consequences of friend
An important contribution is that this study supports that phubbing behavior can be
regarded as problematic smartphone use consistent with prior literature (e.g., Blachnio &
Przepiorka, 2018; Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas, 2016). That is, the findings of this study
provide evidence that the key predictors of problematic smartphone use advanced by the
theoretical model by Billieux and colleagues (2015) also predict friend phubbing.
First, H1a suggested that those with high levels of depression would have increased
friend phubbing. The hypothesis was supported by showing a significant positive correlation
between depression and friend phubbing. This is congruent with the problematic smartphone use
approach (Billieux et al., 2015) as well as the previous studies (e.g., Kim et al., 2015; Park,
2005) that have argued that depressed individuals tend to have higher levels of problematic
smartphone use. Kim and colleagues (2015) empirically demonstrated that individuals with
line with the recent study by Roberts and David (2016) who demonstrated that phubbing in
41
romantic relationships is significantly related to higher levels of depression. However, it is
important to note that previous studies related to phubbing and depression (e.g., Elhai et al.,
2017b; McDaniel & Coyne, 2016; Robert & David, 2016; Roberts & David, 2017) were
examined in romantic relationships and the causal direction of the relationship was unclear.
Unlike the prior studies, the present study extends existing research by examining whether
depression can directly predict friend phubbing. In addition, the findings of the present study
confirmed that depression was found to be the most predominant predictor of friend phubbing
Second, H1b advanced that social anxiety is positively associated with friend phubbing.
This hypothesis was supported by showing a significant positive association. That is, anxious
individuals are more likely to use their smartphones in the presence of their friends. This is
consistent with the problematic smartphone use (Billieux et al., 2015) and the previous studies
(e.g., Demirci et al., 2015; Elhai et al., 2017b) that have demonstrated social anxiety as a
significant predictor of problematic smartphone use. For instance, Demirci and colleagues (2015)
found that anxious college students tend to spend more time using their smartphones resulting in
higher levels of problematic smartphone use. In addition, Elhai and colleagues (2017b) showed
the positive relationship between social anxiety and problematic smartphone use suggesting that
smartphone activities are less likely to provoke anxiety than face-to-face interactions for anxious
individuals.
Similar to the outcomes of the depression (H1a), the result of social anxiety (H1b) also
contributes to support the argument that it is reasonable to apply the previous research which has
demonstrated the relationship between social anxiety and problematic smartphone use to friend
phubbing.
42
Third, H1c suggested that agreeableness is negatively associated with friend phubbing.
agreeableness and friend phubbing. In other words, disagreeable individuals are more likely to
show a higher propensity to phub in the presence of their friends. This finding is supported by
the previous studies (e.g., Andreassen et al., 2013; Fritz, 2018) that have shown the negative
smartphone addiction and phubbing. For example, Andreassen and colleagues (2013) revealed
positive interpersonal relationships and to avoid negative consequences with others (e.g.,
interpersonal conflicts) caused by behavior addictions. The result was also supported by Fritz’s
(2018) findings which demonstrated that increased agreeableness is directly related to decreased
phubbing behavior.
In line with these studies, the result about the influence of agreeableness on friend
phubbing suggests that disagreeable individuals may not care what others feel and think in using
their smartphones in the presence of others. Consequently, individuals who score low on
agreeableness (disagreeable individuals) may feel free to use their smartphones whenever they
want even in inappropriate places. On the other hand, individuals with higher traits of
agreeableness (agreeable individuals) may hold a high emphasis on social harmony striving to
maintain interpersonal relationships and to have a strong motive to avoid interpersonal conflicts
caused by using their smartphones. Thereby, agreeable people may be less likely to engage with
phubbing behavior in friendships. These results further contribute to advance existing theoretical
model of problematic smartphone use (Billiexu et al., 2015) by adding a new variable (i.e.,
43
Finally, H1d suggested that neuroticism is positively associated with friend phubbing.
However, the findings of this study showed the opposite result. The hypothesis was not
supported, indicating a significant but not positive correlation between neuroticism and friend
phubbing. Despite the significant association between neuroticism and friend phubbing, it was
negatively related. In other words, neurotic individuals are less likely to use their smartphones in
the presence of their friends. Contrary to previous studies that have demonstrated that neurotic
individuals tend to show problematic smartphone use (Billieux et al., 2015; Tang, Chen, Yang,
Chung, & Lee, 2016), the results may seem to be unexpected, but there is a possible explanation.
The difference in results may be due to how neurotic individuals regard a smartphone as an
appealing device (e.g., Devaraj, Easley, & Crant, 2008; Bianchi & Phillips, 2005). For instance,
Devaraj and colleagues (2008) argued neurotic individuals have negative beliefs on technological
devices because the device itself is threatening and stressful. Another study (Bianchi & Phillips,
2005) also revealed that those who are neurotic believe that mobile phones have irritating
features (e.g., ring tones). As these studies argued, individuals with higher traits of neuroticism
are possibly less engaged in friend phubbing. The result of H1d, further, suggests another coping
strategy for neurotic individuals in using smartphones. Future research is needed to examine the
association between neuroticism and friend phubbing to explain why neurotic people are less
A second important contribution is that the present study provides a model of the process
by which friend phubbing impacts friendship satisfaction. H2 suggested that friend phubbing is
negatively associated with friendship satisfaction. As expected, this hypothesis was supported by
44
showing a significant negative correlation between friend phubbing and friend satisfaction (see
Figure 2). In other words, the level of friend phubbing is associated with the level of friendship
satisfaction. This finding is in line with the politeness theory (Brown & Levinson, 1987) in that
people feel free to behave in using their smartphones regardless of their public self-images when
they are in comfortable surroundings and the social displacement hypothesis (Kraut et al., 1998)
which states that time spent using smartphones displaces significant conversations with friends.
In addition, the same results were obtained in previous studies (e.g., Bae, 2015; Przybylski &
Weinstein, 2012). For example, Przybylski and Weinstein (2012) designed two different
experiments (e.g., present condition and absence condition of a mobile phone) and demonstrated
that the mere presence of a mobile phone has negative impacts on relational quality and
perceived closeness. More recently, Bae (2015) found that problematic smartphone use has a
related behavior in the presence of friends may distract their casual or meaningful conversations,
resulting in lower levels of friendship satisfaction. Although this study demonstrated that friend
phubbing decreased friendship satisfaction, future research further needs to confirm this result
reported here to offer some insight into how friend phubbing influences friendship satisfaction.
Another contribution of this study concerns the mediating role of friend phubbing
between each individual characteristics and friendship satisfaction. First, H3a suggested that
friend phubbing mediates the relationship between depression and friendship satisfaction. The
45
depression and friendship satisfaction. The result of H3a revealed that depression increases friend
phubbing, which in turn is negatively related to friendship satisfaction. This result, along with
the results from H1a and H2 discussed above, is consistent with those of previous studies. For
example, Fincham, Beach, Harold, and Osborne (1997) found that individuals who have higher
levels of depression tend to less satisfy their marital relationships although the causal direction of
the association is unclear. Also, Cramer (2004) showed that there is a direct relationship between
depression and relationship satisfaction in romantic relationships. Therefore, the result of H3a
H3b suggested that phubbing mediates the relationship between social anxiety and
friendship satisfaction. The hypothesis was supported by showing that social anxiety is indirectly
associated with friendship satisfaction through friend phubbing. This result revealed an indirect
effect only of friend phubbing. That is, social anxiety was positively related to friend phubbing
and in turn, friend phubbing was negatively related to friend satisfaction while the direct effect
It is somewhat surprising that social anxiety did not play a significant role in decreasing
friendship satisfaction. Nevertheless, this result of H3b can be explained by the fact that the
relationship between social anxiety and friendship satisfaction still remains to be elucidated
(Rodebaugh, Lim, Shumaker, Levinson, & Thompson, 2015). Also, as La Greca and Lopez
(1998) found, the association between social anxiety and friendship satisfaction can vary
depending on gender and age. These studies suggest that inconsistent findings of the association
make the current result difficult to interpret. Therefore, more research is needed to enhance our
understanding of such relationship whether friend phubbing play a role in the association
46
As another mediation result, H3c suggested that friend phubbing mediates the
relationship between agreeableness and friendship satisfaction. This hypothesis was supported by
satisfaction. The result revealed that agreeableness decreases friend phubbing, which in turn is
positively related to friendship satisfaction. This result, along with the results from H1c and H2
discussed above, is in line with prior studies. For example, White, Hendrick, and Hendrick
satisfaction. More recently, Malouff, Thorsteinsson, Schutte, Bhullar, and Rooke (2010) found
that agreeable individuals are more likely to satisfy their marital relationships. Thus, the result of
Finally, H3d suggested that friend phubbing mediates the relationship between
neuroticism and friendship satisfaction. The hypothesis was supported by showing that
neuroticism is indirectly associated with friendship satisfaction through friend phubbing. In other
words, neuroticism was negatively related to friend phubbing and in turn, friend phubbing was
negatively related to friend satisfaction while the direct effect from neuroticism and friend
associated with relationship satisfaction assuming that neurotic individuals report being less
satisfied in their relationships (e.g., White et al., 2004). However, similar to social anxiety, the
effect of neuroticism on relationship satisfaction has been unclear and inconsistent (e.g., Wilson,
The differences in results for the relationship between neuroticism and relationship
satisfaction may be due to the duration of a relationship (e.g., how long such a relationship has
been maintained). For example, Karney and Bradbury (1997) found that neuroticism is
47
significantly related to relationship satisfaction only at the beginning of a relationship and no
effects are observed in time. It suggests that neuroticism may not play a significant role in
relationship taking time to develop and maintain. Since this study asked participants to think
about their existing friendships not initiating friendships, it is possible that neuroticism was not
significantly associated with friendship satisfaction. Additionally, the differences in results may
be explained that neurotic individuals only care about themselves whereas agreeable individuals
Considering phubbing occurs in the presence of the “others”, it is possible that neurotic
people who care only about the “self” was not significantly related to decreased friendship
satisfaction. Moreover, friendships may be less influenced by high neuroticism in that these
individuals tend to express their neuroticism to their romantic partners more often than to their
friends (Slatcher & Vazire, 2009). Therefore, further studies are required to examine the
whether an individual’s characteristics such as neuroticism can support a causal conclusion that
To my knowledge, this study is the first report to examine the dynamics of phubbing
behavior in friendships from a communication perspective. Yet, there are several limitations that
should be acknowledged. First of all, this study was a cross-sectional survey which is limited to
draw conclusions about causality. Although this study built on the theoretical frameworks and
48
earlier studies, it is hard to generalize the current findings and the causal relationships because of
its correlational nature. Therefore, future research should adopt a longitudinal design considering
either a shorter or longer length of time to reflect all patterns of an individual’s characteristics,
friend phubbing, and friendship satisfaction because different findings may be found for data
Second, a sample was limited to college students at one university. I believe that the data
is appropriate for the smartphone-related study because college students are a major group of
smartphone users. In addition, young adults are more likely to spend more time using their
smartphones than older generations. However, phubbing can be seen in different age groups. In
the future, therefore, it is necessary to collect data from a wide range of ages.
Third, the data were collected from only one country which does not represent all other
cultural and social factors. The generalizability of the findings was limited. Therefore, future
studies should target a more representative sample to generalize the findings of this study. In
addition, these results did not consider demographic differences to the hypothesized model. The
relationships of this study may be different (e.g., less significant) depending on the participants’
phubbing behavior in friendships. It would bring new insights into phubbing behavior.
Fourth, this study is based on the self-report data, which may not accurately reflect
participants’ phubbing behavior. Considering such behavior can occur unconsciously and people
can unaware that such behavior will have negative impacts on themselves and others, self-
reported information regarding phubbing behavior may be different from actual phubbing
behavior as well as other variables in the present study. Thus, future studies could use an
objective measure of the participants’ phubbing behavior such as experimental studies in natural
49
settings. It may help to reflect actual levels of phubbing behavior and overcome the
In this study, 25% of the friend phubbing and 29% of the friendship satisfaction were
explained by the proposed variables. In this regard, future studies should consider unexplained
variables or possible different factors such as other psychological, personality traits and cultural
variables. In addition, the interpretation of the findings was limited because there was a lack of
research related to phubbing, and the proposed variables, especially independent variables, were
Lastly, the present study confined its context to phubbers’ perspective to understanding
which individual characteristics lead to friend phubbing by examining the relationships between
each individual difference and friend phubbing. Since such behavior also has a negative impact
to co-present person who is a victim of phubbing behavior, it is worth considering both actor
(phubber) and actor-partner (phubbee) effect of phubbing to gain further insights into phubbing
behavior.
Despite these limitations, these findings of the present study may have some important
Conclusion
traits and psychological factors are significant predictors of friend phubbing. Specifically, people
who have higher traits of depression or social anxiety were more likely to phub their friends
50
while individuals who have lower traits of agreeableness and neuroticism were more likely to use
The findings of this study also showed that friend phubbing has a detrimental impact on
friendship satisfaction. More interestingly, the mediation analysis revealed that friend phubbing
appears to mediate the proposed relationships between each possible predictor of friend
phubbing and friendships satisfaction: (a) friend phubbing mediated the relationship between
depression and friendship satisfaction; (b) friend phubbing mediated the relationship between
social anxiety and friendship satisfaction; (c) friend phubbing mediated the relationship between
agreeableness and friendship satisfaction; and (d) friend phubbing mediated the relationship
51
REFERENCES
Abeele, M. M. V., Antheunis, M. L., & Schouten, A. P. (2016). The effect of mobile messaging
Ahn, D., & Shin, D. H. (2013). Is the social use of media for seeking connectedness or for
avoiding social isolation? Mechanisms underlying media use and subjective well-
Al-Saggaf, Y., & MacCulloch, R. (2018). Phubbing: How Frequent? Who is Phubbed? In Which
Situation? And Using Which Apps? In Paper presented at the Thirty ninth International
Conference on Information Systems (ICIS), December 13-16, San Francisco, CA, USA.
Amichai-Hamburger, Y., Wainapel, G., & Fox, S. (2002). "On the Internet no one knows I'm an
Anastasi, A. & Urbina, S. (1997). The evolution of psychology: Fifty years of the American
Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Gjertsen, S. R., Krossbakken, E., Kvam, S., & Pallesen, S.
(2013). The relationships between behavioral addictions and the five-factor model of
Austin, W. (1980). Friendship and fairness: Effects of type of relationship and task performance
on choice of distribution rules. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6(3), 402-
408.
52
Bae, S. M. (2015). The relationships between perceived parenting style, learning motivation,
friendship satisfaction, and the addictive use of smartphones with elementary school
students of South Korea: Using multivariate latent growth modeling. School Psychology
Balta, S., Emirtekin, E., Kircaburun, K., & Griffiths, M. D. (2018). Neuroticism, Trait Fear of
Missing Out, and Phubbing: The Mediating Role of State Fear of Missing Out and
Problematic Instagram Use. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 1-12.
Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (2009). Depression: Causes and treatment. University of
Pennsylvania Press.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of
Bianchi, A., & Phillips, J. G. (2005). Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone
Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can
Blachnio, A., & Przepiorka, A. (2018). Be aware! If you start using Facebook problematically
you will feel lonely: Phubbing, loneliness, self-esteem, and Facebook intrusion. A cross-
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge
53
initial test. Human Communication Research, 4, 129–142.
Butt, S., & Phillips, J. G. (2008). Personality and self-reported mobile phone use. Computers in
Caplan, S. E. (2007). Relations among loneliness, social anxiety, and problematic Internet
Chayko, M. (2008). Portable communities: The social dynamics of line and mobile
Chopik, W. J. (2017). Associations among relational values, support, health, and well‐being
Chotpitayasunondh, V., & Douglas, K. M. (2016). How “phubbing” becomes the norm: The
Development and Validation of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP) and the Generic
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO
conflict. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 77(4), 449-
461.
54
Demir, M., Özen, A., Doğan, A., Bilyk, N. A., & Tyrell, F. A. (2011). I matter to my friend,
Demir, M., & Weitekamp, L. A. (2007). I am so happy cause today I found my friend: Friendship
Demirci, K., Akgönül, M., & Akpinar, A. (2015). Relationship of smartphone use severity with
DePaulo, B. M., Epstein, J. A., & LeMay, C. S. (1990). Responses of the socially anxious to the
Devaraj, S., Easley, R. F., & Crant, J. M. (2008). Research note—how does personality matter?
Relating the five-factor model to technology acceptance and use. Information Systems
Ehrenberg, A., Juckes, S., White, K. M., & Walsh, S. P. (2008). Personality and self-esteem as
predictors of young people's technology use. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 11(6), 739-
741.
Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017a). Problematic smartphone use: A
conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2017b). Non-social features of
smartphone use are most related to depression, anxiety and problematic smartphone
55
Enez Darcin, A., Kose, S., Noyan, C. O., Nurmedov, S., Yılmaz, O., & Dilbaz, N. (2016).
Smartphone addiction and its relationship with social anxiety and loneliness. Behavior
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R., Harold, G. T., & Osborne, L. N. (1997). Marital satisfaction and
Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Bucholtz, I. (1995). Effects of adult attachment style on the
perception and search for social support. The Journal of Psychology, 129(6), 665-676.
Fritz, K. (2018). Do touchscreens make us lose touch? The relationship between personality
traits and phubbing among emerging adults (Master’s thesis). Retrived from
http://scriptiesonline.uba.uva.nl/document/656594
Gibbs, J. L., Ellison, N. B., & Heino, R. D. (2006). Self-presentation in online personals: The
Doubleday.
Guerrero, L. K., Andersen, P. A., & Afifi, W. A. (2011). Communicating closeness: affection,
125-150.
Ha, J. H., Chin, B., Park, D. H., Ryu, S. H., & Yu, J. (2008). Characteristics of excessive cellular
Hair, J. F., Black, W., Banbin, B., & Anderson, R. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis (7th ed.).
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
56
Halpern, D., & Katz, J. E. (2017). Texting's consequences for romantic relationships: A cross-
lagged analysis highlights its risks. Computers in Human Behavior, 71, 386-394.
Harrison, M. A., Bealing, C. E., & Salley, J. M. (2015). 2 TXT or not 2 TXT: College students’
reports of when text messaging is social breach. The Social Science Journal, 52(2), 188-
194.
Haug, S., Castro, R. P., Kwon, M., Filler, A., Kowatsch, T., & Schaub, M. P. (2015). Smartphone
Hirschmüller, S., Egloff, B., Schmukle, S. C., Nestler, S., & Back, M. D. (2015). Accurate
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modeling: Guidelines for
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
Theory and Research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
Karadağ, E., Tosuntaş, Ş. B., Erzen, E., Duru, P., Bostan, N., Şahin, B. M., ... & Babadağ, B.
57
(2015). Determinants of phubbing, which is the sum of many virtual addictions: A
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the trajectory of
Khang, H., Kim, J. K., & Kim, Y. (2013). Self-traits and motivations as antecedents of digital
media flow and addiction: The Internet, mobile phones, and video games. Computers in
Kim, J. H., Seo, M., & David, P. (2015). Alleviating depression only to become problematic
Kim, J. H. (2018). Psychological issues and problematic use of smartphone: ADHD's moderating
role in the associations among loneliness, need for social assurance, need for immediate
390-398.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY:
Krasnova, H., Abramova, O., Notter, I., & Baumann, A. (2016). Why Phubbing is Toxic for your
Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukopadhyay, T., & Scherlis, W. (1998).
Internet paradox: A social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological
58
La Greca, A. M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with peer
Leary, M. (1983). Social anxiousness: The construct and its measurement. Journal of Personality
Lee, J., Sung, M. J., Song, S. H., Lee, Y. M., Lee, J. J., Cho, S. M., ... & Shin, Y. M. (2018).
Lee, Y. K., Chang, C. T., Lin, Y., & Cheng, Z. H. (2014). The dark side of smartphone usage:
M. Katz (Eds.), The Psychology of Depression: Contemporary Theory and Research (pp.
Lim, T. S., & Bowers, J. W. (1991). Facework solidarity, approbation, and tact. Human
Ling, R., & Yttri, B. (2002). 10 Hyper-coordination via mobile phones in Norway. Perpetual
Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., Schutte, N. S., Bhullar, N., & Rooke, S. E. (2010). The
McAdams, D. P. (1988). Power, intimacy, and the life story: Personological inquiries into
59
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across
instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(1), 81-90.
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five‐factor model and its
couple relationships and implications for women’s personal and relational well-
McElroy, J. C., Hendrickson, A. R., Townsend, A. M., & DeMarie, S. M. (2007). Dispositional
factors in internet use: personality versus cognitive style. MIS quarterly, 809-820.
Miller-Ott, A. E., & Kelly, L. (2015). The presence of cell phones in romantic partner face-to-
Miller-Ott, A. E., & Kelly, L. (2017). A Politeness theory analysis of cell-phone usage in the
Nie, N. H. (2001). Sociability, interpersonal relations, and the Internet: Reconciling conflicting
Nie, N. H., Hillygus, D. S., & Erbring, L. (2002). Internet use, interpersonal relations, and
Park, W. K. (2005). Mobile phone addiction. In R. Ling & P. E. Pedersen (Eds.), Mobile
Springer.
60
Pew Research Center. (2015a). U.S. Smartphone Use in 2015. Retrieved on January 16, 2019
Pew Research Center. (2015b). Americans’ views on mobile etiquette. Retrieved on January 16,
etiquette/.
Pew Research Center. (2018a). Number of smartphone users worldwide from 2014 to 2020 (in
https://www.statista.com/statistics/330695/number-of-smartphone-users-worldwide/.
Pew Research Center. (2018b). Mobile Fact Sheet. Retrieved on January 16, 2019 from:
http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/03/14/about-a-quarter-of-americans-report-
going-online-almost-constantly/
Phillips, J. G., Butt, S., & Blaszczynski, A. (2006). Personality and self-reported use of mobile
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects
36(4), 717-731.
Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2012). Can you connect with me now? How the presence of
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general
Roberts, J. A., & David, M. E. (2016). My life has become a major distraction from my cell
61
partners. Computers in Human Behavior, 54, 134-141.
Roberts, J. A., & David, M. E. (2017). Put down your phone and listen to me: How boss
Roberts, J. A., Pullig, C., & Manolis, C. (2015). I need my smartphone: A hierarchical model of
personality and cell-phone addiction. Personality and Individual Differences, 79, 13-19.
Rodebaugh, T. L., Lim, M. H., Shumaker, E. A., Levinson, C. A., & Thompson, T. (2015). Social
anxiety and friendship quality over time. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, 44(6), 502-511.
Seo, M., Kim, J. H., & David, P. (2015). Always connected or always distracted? ADHD
symptoms and social assurance explain problematic use of mobile phone and
Slatcher, R. B., & Vazire, S. (2009). Effects of global and contextualized personality on
Sue, D., Sue, D. W., Sue, S., & Sue, D. M. (2015). Understanding abnormal behavior (11th ed.).
62
Takao, M. (2014). Problematic mobile phone use and big-five personality domains. Indian
Tang, J. H., Chen, M. C., Yang, C. Y., Chung, T. Y., & Lee, Y. A. (2016). Personality traits,
Thomée, S., Härenstam, A., & Hagberg, M. (2011). Mobile phone use and stress, sleep
Thornton, B., Faires, A., Robbins, M., & Rollins, E. (2014). The mere presence of a cell phone
Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from each
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Online communication and adolescent well-being: Testing
Wang, X., Xie, X., Wang, Y., Wang, P., & Lei, L. (2017). Partner phubbing and depression
among married Chinese adults: The roles of relationship satisfaction and relationship
White, J. K., Hendrick, S. S., & Hendrick, C. (2004). Big five personality variables and
Wilson, R. E., Harris, K., & Vazire, S. (2015). Personality and friendship satisfaction in daily
63
life: Do everyday social interactions account for individual differences in friendship
Zheng, F., Gao, P., He, M., Li, M., Wang, C., Zeng, Q., ... & Zhang, L. (2014). Association
between mobile phone use and inattention in 7102 Chinese adolescents: a population-
64
APPENDIX A
PART 1. DEMOGRAPHICS
1. What is your gender?
(1) Female
(2) Male
(3) Transgender
(4) Other (please specify) _____
(5) Prefer not to answer
4. I am …
(1) an undergraduate student
(2) a graduate student
(3) not in school
5. Relationship Status
65
(1) Single
(2) In a relationship
(3) It’s complicated
66
PART 2. DURATION AND PATTERNS OF SMARTPHONE USE
2. What functions/applications on your smartphone do you use most frequently in a typical day?
(Choose three options that you use most frequently)
67
PART 3. THE TERM PHUBBING
(1) No
(2) Yes
68
PART 4. PHUBBING BEHAVIOR – Revised Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP)
Please read each item carefully and indicate how often each of the statements below is
descriptive of you.
Please think about their current friendships: meet up together or contact them within two weeks
1 = Never
2 = Rarely
3 = Occasionally
4 = Sometimes
5 = Frequently
6 = Usually
7 = Always
69
PART 5. DEPRESSION – Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D)
Below is a list of the ways you might have felt or behaved. Please indicate how often you have
felt this way during the past week.
70
PART 6. SOCIAL ANXIETY - The Interaction Anxiousness Scale (IAS)
Please read each item carefully and indicate how often each of the statements below is
descriptive of you.
71
PART 7. AGREEABLENESS – SUBSCALE OF THE BIG FIVE TRAIT TAXONOMY
Here are a number of characteristics that may or may not apply to you. Please read each item
carefully and indicate how often each of the statements below is descriptive of you.
1 = Strongly disagree
2 = Somewhat disagree
3 = Neither agree nor disagree
4 = Somewhat agree
5 = Strongly agree
72
PART 8. NEUROTICISM – SUBSCALE OF THE BIG FIVE TRAIT TAXONOMY
Here are a number of characteristics that may or may not apply to you. Please read each item
carefully and indicate how often each of the statements below is descriptive of you.
1 = Strongly disagree
2 = Somewhat disagree
3 = Neither agree nor disagree
4 = Somewhat agree
5 = Strongly agree
73
PART 9. RELATIONSHIP SATISFACTION – Revised Relationship Assessment Scale
(RAS)
Please read each item carefully and decide to what extent it is characteristic of your feelings and
behavior.
Please think about their current friendships: meet up together or contact them within two weeks
1 = Poorly/Unsatisfied
2 = Not that well/Not very satisfied
3 = About average
4 = Well/Satisfied
5 = Extremely well/Extremely satisfied
74
PART 10. RESEARCH POOL & EXTRA CREDIT PARTICIPANTS
Please provide the following information in order to get CREDIT for participation (only for
UGA students)
The course for which you want course research credit by completing this survey:
Course Number (e.g., COMM 1500) __________
75