iCS Echo Chamber Revised+AAM
iCS Echo Chamber Revised+AAM
iCS Echo Chamber Revised+AAM
a
Department of Communication, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada –
elizabeth.dubois@uottawa.ca
b
Oxford Internet Institute, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom –
grant.blank@oii.ox.ac.uk
* Corresponding author:
55 Laurier Ave. East, Room 11-156,
Ottawa, Ontario, K1N 6N5;
613-562-5800 (1478)
This work was supported by Google as part of the Quello Search Project.
1
The echo chamber is overstated: The moderating effect of political
interest and diverse media
Abstract: In a high-choice media environment there are fears that individuals will select
media and content that reinforce their existing beliefs and lead to segregation based on
interest and/or partisanship. This could lead to partisan echo chambers among those who
are politically interested and could contribute to a growing gap in knowledge between
those who are politically interested and those who are not. However, the high-choice
environment also allows individuals, including those who are politically interested, to
consume a wide variety of media which could lead them to more diverse content and
perspectives. This study examines the relationship between political interest as well as
media diversity and being caught in an echo chamber (measured by five different
variables). Using a nationally representative survey of adult Internet users in the United
Kingdom (N = 2,000), we find that those who are interested in politics and those with
diverse media diets tend to avoid echo chambers. This work challenges the impact of
echo chambers and tempers fears of partisan segregation since only a small segment of
the population are likely to find themselves in an echo chamber. We argue that single
media studies and studies which use narrow definitions and measurements of being in an
echo chamber are flawed because they do not test the theory in the realistic context of a
multiple media environment.
2
Introduction
where only certain ideas, information and beliefs are shared (Jamieson & Cappella,
2008; Sunstein, 2009). People inside this setting will only encounter things they already
agree with. Without free movement of ideas and information people inside the echo
chamber will believe that this is all there is. Under these circumstances anyone who
to select information and communities which support existing beliefs as well as through
algorithmic personalization, some worry that the Internet may make it easier for citizens
to find themselves in an echo chamber. Some fear that segregation by interest or opinion
will exacerbate the gap between those who are informed about politics and those who
are not, increase political polarization which will reinforce political divides, and
access news and political information from a diverse array of media and sources (Van
Aelst et al., 2017).1 Since people can select their information sources the Internet may
foster an environment where echo chambers are more common and dangerous. Unlike
information from social media, search, online and offline versions of newspapers,
television broadcasts, radio, and so on. Thus there are two possible outcomes from a
1
We choose the use the term “media environment” to represent the collection of media
available and their interactions. This is essentially the setting in which individuals make
choices about their media use. Others use the terms “media ecosystem” or “media ecology”
which emphasize the interrelated nature and importance of interactions among media in a
system. For our purposes, we see these terms as roughly interchangeable with “media
environment.” Each term aims to capture the environment which individuals find themselves
in, we select one for consistency.
3
diverse media environment. Individuals may be exposed to information and
perspectives which are also diverse or they may select varied media in a way that
produces the echo chamber effect. To date evidence has been conflicting. Examinations
information and ideas they agree with more often (Lawrence, Sides, & Farrell, 2010;
Iyengar and Hahn 2009) but they do not tend to avoid information and ideas which are
conflicting (Garrett, 2009). Even among partisans in the US, the media diet of
Republicans and Democrats is in fact quite similar (Weeks, Ksiazek & Holbert, 2016).
While some have found evidence of echo chambers on Twitter (Conover et al., 2011;
Barberá et al., 2015; Himelboim, McCreery & Smith, 2013), others have shown that the
trend does not persist on Facebook (Bakshy et al., 2015; Goel et al., 2010).
Beyond conflicting evidence, there are two key methodological issues with how
echo chamber work has been conducted. First, many studies are single platform and this
severely limits their generalizability. Even if Twitter is polarized (e.g. Conover et al.
2010), it is only one part of a much larger media environment. Individuals tend to use
multiple media to access news and political information (Newman et al., 2017; Dutton
et al., 2017; Ahlers, 2006) and the characteristics of Twitter or any other single medium
may not give us useful information about how political information flows across offline
media or other online media. It is important to consider the entire range of media
for the complex ways individuals can actually use the assortment of media they have
access to. Measuring exposure to conflicting ideas on a single platform or medium does
not account for the ways in which individuals collect information across the entire
media environment. For example, someone might learn about an issue on Facebook
4
which they fact-check using search. We argue that to understand whether a person is in
an echo chamber it is important to consider how they interact with their entire media
environment. Incorporating more than one measure also helps respond to the known
problem that individuals tend to over-estimate how often they see conflicting views in
reported political interest and diversity of media across many different channels of
political information. The following section reviews past work related to selective
exposure and echo chambers before reviewing our key independent variables: media
diversity and political interest, and dependent variables: acts individuals take which
prevent/avoid being caught in echo chambers. We then review our survey methodology.
Next, we analyse the effect of political interest and media diversity on echo chambers
considering the entire media environment when assessing the news and political
Background
The Internet and echo chambers
Echo chambers occur when people with the same interests or views interact primarily
within their group. They seek and share information that both conforms to the norms of
their group and tends to reinforce existing beliefs (Jamieson & Cappella, 2008;
Sunstein, 2009). Social psychology has long shown this tendency to associate with like-
minded others is common cross-culturally. However, there is new fear that the current
media system is helping people enter echo chambers more easily than ever before.
5
Psychological and social psychological research in the 1950s found that people
tend to avoid dissonance and gravitate toward agreement (Festinger, 1957). It is related
to concepts such as groupthink (Janis, 1982) and selective exposure theory (Kapper,
1960). On social media, there are related theories about homophily; the tendency form
social ties with similar others (McPherson, Smith-Lovin & Cook, 2001).
There are two main ways that the Internet and related technologies might
support the development of echo chambers: allowing individuals to make choices that
reinforce existing preferences and algorithmic filter bubbles. The filter bubble argument
suggests algorithmic filtering which personalizes content presented on social media and
through use of search engines could exacerbate the tendency for people to select media
and content which reinforce their existing preferences (Pariser, 2011). We are primarily
concerned with the choices individuals make in their news and political information
seeking practices in this study rather than the impact of algorithmic filtering.
In communication and media studies fragmentation and polarization are key features of
audiences which are relevant for discussions of how individuals might reinforce their
mass audience, which was once concentrated on three or four viewing options, becomes
more widely distributed” (Webster, 2005, p. 367). Polarization occurs when audiences
diverge and are segmented based on an issue or interest (Sunstein, 2002). In a high-
choice media environment individuals can select media and content from a wide range
of options which means audiences are fragmented and potentially polarized based on
preferences which drive individuals’ choices (Prior, 2007; Sunstein, 2002; Webster
6
2005). More recently the term echo chamber has become a popular description of this
basic mechanism.
information and news. The first is a divide between those who are informed and those
who are not informed about politics. The second is political polarization among those
who exhibit at least minimal political interest or awareness. Since democratic political
systems require people talk to each other to work out compromises and/or to become
informed, the emergence of an echo chamber could have serious negative consequences.
conflicting results. While audiences are fragmented, most individuals continue to rely
on at least some more general sources of news and political information such as non-
partisan newspapers (online and offline) or television broadcasts (Newman et al., 2017;
Weeks, Ksiazek & Holbert, 2016). Furthermore, when selecting media, individuals may
choose to access information that confirms their beliefs more frequently but they are
less likely to actively avoid information that contradicts their views (Garrett, 2009).
partisan news websites, blogs, and some social media (Conovor et al., 2011; Lawrence,
Sides & Farrell, 2010), these are not the only or even the main sources of news and
political information the general public report relying on (Newman et al., 2017). Even
considering social media, which show the clearest evidence of echo chambers, most
individuals are in fact exposed to a variety of views and sources of political information
(Messing and Westwood, 2014). That said, Bakshy and colleagues consider the case of
Facebook and show that while individuals may be exposed to heterogonous information
they are more likely to click on stories which are in line with their existing views than
7
One reason past work reports conflicting results is that much echo chamber
research has been focused on only a limited number of social media platforms, often
Twitter. Single-platform studies are problematic because political information and news
also experimental evidence that people put more effort into thinking about information
that comes from multiple sources instead of just one source. People appear to believe
that that information from multiple sources is more likely to be reliable, and thus worth
more serious consideration, than is information that comes from only one source
studies of political polarization in social media, especially Twitter (e.g. Adamic &
Glance, 2005; Himelboim, McCreery & Smith, 2013). A typical paper is Conover et al.
(2011), which applies network methods to data from the USA to show that Democrats’
population, about one-quarter of the UK, which is younger, wealthier and better-
educated than Britain as a whole (Blank, 2017). It is an influential segment, but it is not
consistently the least trusted medium in cross-national studies (e.g. Dutton et al. 2017).
Ultimately, there is little agreement about the extent to which echo chambers
form, whether they persist across media, and what their democratic impact is.
In the current media environment individuals may access political information through a
wide variety of channels such as via television, radio, social media, search, online news
sites, and face-to-face communication to name a few (Van Aelst et al., 2017). They may
8
choose to combine these media or use them singularly. These media are diverse and at
times overlapping.
way. Individuals tend to develop media habits which are repeated media consumption
behaviours (LaRose, 2010) and news or political information media repertoires which
is the collection of media an individual uses to access news and political information
regularly (Heeter, 1985; Wolfsfeld et al., 2016). Research has convincingly argued that,
given multiple media to choose from individuals tend to habitually make use of a
smaller subset of media available (Heeter, 1985; Wolfsfeld et al., 2016). These
repertoires differ in terms of how many media are included, which media, and how
those media might be combined. We call the regular, daily set of media individuals use
Individuals tend to use multiple media to access news and political information
(Newman et al., 2017; Dutton et al., 2017). For example, just 2% of individuals in the
US rely only on social media for news (Newman et al., 2017). Furthermore, social
media, search engines and news aggregators are becoming increasingly popular as a
way to access news cross-nationally with 65% of the cross-national sample in the
Reuters Digital News (2017) survey reporting a preference for accessing news brands
indirectly. That said, going directly to a news source such as the BBC remains more
Notably, not all media are used in the same way or provide the same type of
political information. For example, Nikolov and colleagues show that social media
provide a narrower array of political content than search engines (2015). Indeed, a news
consumer has no control over what a television news program displays in contrast to
their own Twitter feed which they can curate at a granular level. Similarly, newspapers
9
are often broad in topical scope while personalized and niche content is more readily
available via various social media sites and online news aggregators.
The media individuals select is often related to their political engagement and
their partisan preferences. Stroud shows that the media individuals choose to
incorporate into their media diets can be predicted by their partisan pre-dispositions
news consumption was not (Ksiazek et al., 2010). Although Ksiazek et al. (2010) treat
the Internet as a single monolithic medium, others have since attempted to tease out
differences across Internet enabled media. Scholars have commonly compared social to
traditional media, acknowledging that traditional media may be accessed via websites.
Social media, search engines and online newspapers each play a potentially varied and
important role. A core problem with this line of research is that most studies select only
one or a few media to focus on and so the comparative utility or effects of use of media
Relatedly, some media are valued and/or trusted more than others. For example,
interviews with Canadians who actively discuss politics on Twitter showed that these
individuals rely on mainstream news media and face-to-face conversations with friends
when seeking information about a political issue they think is important instead of posts
on Twitter and Facebook, despite themselves contributing posts online (Dubois, 2015).
Cross-national surveys also suggest people tend to trust social media less than other
how the many media in a high-choice media environment are integrated into daily life.
10
media literacy campaigns often suggest that relying on more than one medium is an
important way individuals can avoid echo chambers. This mechanism has been implied
in much work on media literacy yet has not been clearly tested. We do this as we
consider how different media are used in conjunction with one another, such as fact
checking, in order to establish a more nuanced set of measures for identifying when
individuals are caught in an echo chamber or not. These and other variables are
described below.
Media diversity
Media repertoires can differ in terms of how many media are included, which media,
and how those media might be combined. Media diversity, a key independent variable
in this study, is concerned with the number of media in a persons’ repertoire. The
greater the number of media a citizen uses the more the opportunity to be exposed to
differing political opinions and news. Citizens could exist within a cross-platform echo
First, even individuals who have strong partisan affiliation report using both
general news sites which are largely non-partisan and include a variety of issues as well
as niche news sites which may be partisan or focused on specific issues – Republicans
and Democrats have media diets which are quite similar (Weeks, Ksiazek & Holbert,
news whether you are interested in it or not (Wojcieszak & Mutz, 2009). Third, not all
media are used in the same way and for the same content which means that as media
diversity increases, there is also an increase in the diversity of content. While one might
11
might hear a debate between representatives from various perspectives on a television
news broadcast.
H1: The more diverse media that individuals are exposed to, the less likely they are to
be in an echo chamber
Political interest
Political interest is associated with higher than average news and political information
consumption (Boulianne, 2011). Strömbäck and colleagues show that news polarization
in Sweden is increasing over time and that political interest is a key driver for news
consumption (2013). Others note that as news consumption increases so do the number
of media an individual incorporates into their diets (Ksiazek et al., 2010; Yuan, 2011).
As Prior argues, political “junkies” are likely to consume a lot of information and
information when they see value in being exposed to those ideas (Knobloch-Westerwick
and Kleinman 2012; Valentino et al. 2009). People who are politically interested often
For these people, there is value and relevance in avoiding echo chamber.
H2: The higher a person’s level of political interest the less likely they are to be in an
echo chamber
whether or not people are exposed to contrasting views from their own.
12
This is problematic because people have a hard time recalling when they have
been exposed to different ideas and so survey research is potentially flawed (Prior
2009). Trace data approaches have emerged as a response to this self-report bias
problem but are also limited because it is hard to measure the type of information and/or
partisan leaning of content accessed across platforms (Wesler et al., 2008). As such,
single platform studies are common. But, being presented with confirmatory opinions
on one platform does not mean other platforms are not used by individuals to help them
avoid being caught in an echo chamber. While our study is limited by self-report we
work to address these concerns. We consider the wide variety of media accessible to
citizens. We also conceptualize our dependent variables in terms of not only what
information people are exposed to but whether or not they take acts to avoid echo
chambers. By using five proxy variables we are able to offer multiple perspectives on
The data
We use data from the Quello Search Project, a study of media use and politics collected
in January 2017 in the United Kingdom. The 2,000 cases are a random sample of the
online population of Britain, including England, Scotland, Wales and Northern Ireland.
Post-stratification weights are used to re-weight them to census proportions for age,
gender and region. The data collection was funded by Google, although Google has had
As control variables we include six demographic variables: age, gender, marital status,
13
position variable and a self-report of skill using the Internet. Political interest is
measured by an item asking “How interested are you in politics?” Responses were
measured on a 4-category Likert scale from “No interest at all” to “very interested”.
Media diversity was measured with two variables. First, the questionnaire asked “When
looking for information about POLITICAL news, issues or elected officials, how often
do you go to…” Responses were measured on a 5-category Likert scale from “Never”
to “Very often”. This item was used for 12 media, 6 online and 6 offline. The 12 items
were formed into a scale by summing the responses, yielding a range from 0-48. We
tried separating online and offline media use, but they are highly correlated and they
cause collinearity problems in the models reported below. Since we could not put them
both in the same model we only use the combined measure of total media diversity.
Second, since social media are an important source of political news, particularly for
certain groups, we measured it separately. Our measure of social media use is a count of
the number of sites on which a respondent has a profile, so the variable ranges from 0 to
12. There is a relationship between political interest and media diversity but the pearson
product moment correlation is relatively small, about 0.43, and it does not cause
collinearity problems.
aspects of an echo chamber. Each variable measures the extent to which people are
exposed to different opinions, i.e. the extent to which respondents find themselves in an
echo chamber. All are based on five-category Likert scales, so they measure the extent
to which a respondent reports being in an echo chamber. For the first three items the
stub was “When looking for news or political information, how often, if ever, do
you…” Each of the following four items follow that stub and are used ad dependent
14
1. “Read something you DISAGREE with?” (which we call ‘Disagree’);
2. “Check a news source that’s different from what you normally read?” (which we
call ‘Different’);
3. “Try to confirm political information you found by searching online for another
source?” (which we call ‘Confirm’).
The fifth dependent variable, which we call ‘Changed’, is about opinion change:
5. “Thinking about recent searches you have done online using a search engine.
How often have you discovered something that CHANGED your opinion on a
political issue?”.
All dependent variables are coded so that lower values mean the respondent is
more likely to be in an echo chamber. This coding implies that, in the regressions,
These variables complement each other in several ways. The variable Disagree
closest to the standard measures of being in an echo chamber used in previous work. It
it. The variable Different measures the extent to which respondents expose themselves
variables Confirm, Different, Changed and Offline measure the extent to which
respondents have taken action to actively remove themselves from an echo chamber. By
using all five dependent variables, we have a much more comprehensive view of
possible echo chambers than other research. This comprehensive view is directly
15
Results
Our five dependent variables have reasonable distributions (see Figure 1), largely
symmetric and well-spread across the entire range of possible values. The symmetry is
noteworthy because it indicates the portion of the population who are less likely to be in
an echo chamber. The exact proportions vary across the five variables, but in general
about the same proportion are on the right side of the centre as on the left side.
60 60
Percent
40
Percent
40
20
20
0
Almost Rarely Sometimes Mostly Nearly 0
never always Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often
Disagree: How often do you disagree with Different: How often do you check a political
political content friends post on social media? news source different from usual?
60 60
Percent
Percent
40 40
20 20
0 0
Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often Never Rarely Occasionally Often
Confirm: How often do you use search Changed: How often have you discovered information
to try to confirm political information? that changed your opinion on a political issue?
60
Percent
40
20
0
Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often
we entered all of our control variables. In the second step we entered our three variables
regression coefficients for the five regressions containing only control variables. The
16
results are fairly consistent across all five dependent variables. Skills and political
participation are always significant. Age is significant and (as expected) negative for
three variables. Gender, marital status, lifestage and right-left political orientation are
each significant only once. Income and education are not significant. The R²s range
from 11% to 23%. We lose over 500 cases because of missing data. This is due mostly
to the income and right-left politics variables, where there are a large number of people
who did not respond (about 350 respondents). Because of the missing data, we remove
income and right-left politics from the remaining regressions. Including them does not
Table 2 adds the variables measuring media diversity and political interest.
These new variables change the results considerably. First, the R²s generally double in
size, increasing by between 12 and 22 percentage points. Second, both skills and
political participation become less significant and weaker. They are not significant at all
17
in the Change regression. Age also becomes weaker and it is no longer significant in the
Different regression.
The media diversity variables have strong effects. They are always significant.
Media diversity is particularly noteworthy because it has by far the strongest effects. It
is between two and six times stronger than the second strongest variable in the model.
Since the media diversity variables are always positive, respondents who have more
diverse media habits are less likely to be in an echo chamber. This confirms hypothesis
1.
Political interest is significant for four of the five dependent variables, only for
Change is it not significant. It is stronger than social media diversity in the Disagree,
Confirm and Offline models. Since it is positive, respondents who are more interested in
18
These results raise two possibilities. First, that respondents with no political
interest are in an echo chamber. We examine this possibility using the regressions in
Table 3. The results in this table are based only on the respondents who said they had
‘No interest at all’ in politics, N = 243. With so few cases, the results are not very
stable, but they follow the same broad pattern that we have seen in prior regressions.
Social media diversity is no longer significant but media diversity remains positive,
significant and strong, so even respondents with no political interest are less likely to be
in an echo chamber when they have diverse media habits. Because of this strong
respondents at the ends of the political spectrum. In other words, it may be that these
relationships hold for people who are in the political middle, but that respondents who
are on the extreme left or the extreme right of the political spectrum are more likely to
19
be in an echo chamber. We repeated these regressions separately (1) for respondents
who said they were fairly or very right wing (N = 273) and (2) for respondents who said
they were fairly or very left wing (N = 393). We do not show these two tables of
regression results because they duplicate the results in Tables 1-3. On both ends of the
political spectrum, political interest, media diversity and social media diversity remain
strong, generally significant and positive. Media diversity is always significant and
always the strongest coefficient in all models. In short, hypothesis 1 and hypothesis 2
Discussion
The main point that comes out of these regressions is that, regardless of how we
measure the presence of an echo chamber, greater interest in politics and more media
diversity reduces the likelihood of being in an echo chamber. These results are strong
It is evident that media diets and choices matter when it comes to assessing the
threat of potential echo chambers. Supporting our first hypothesis, we have shown that
the number of media an individual chooses to incorporate into their habits is related to
their likelihood of becoming caught in an echo chamber. Having a diverse media diet is
a step toward exposure to diverse information and perspectives. Individuals may expose
information they disagree with, to actively checking multiple sources or using other
media to verify information. In each case we have found that media diversity predicts
acts which help the individual avoid an echo chamber. Supporting our second
hypothesis, we have shown that greater interest in politics also reduces the likelihood of
20
Whatever may be happening on any single social media platform, when we look at
the entire media environment there is little apparent echo chamber. People regularly
encounter things that they disagree with. People check multiple sources. People try to
confirm information using search. Possibly most important, people discover things that
change their political opinions. Looking at the entire multi-media environment we find
little evidence of an echo chamber. This applies even to people who are not interested in
politics. Thus, the possibility of being in an echo chamber seems overstated. Of course,
there are a small number of individuals with both very low interest in politics and low
media diversity for whom being stuck in an echo chamber is more likely. We discuss
this segment of the population below but we first review theoretical and methodological
self-report survey or through analysis of trace data from a social media platform such as
Twitter. In line with this work, we have included exposure to different ideas as a
develop strategies to deal with the many media options available, though of course they
do so as they develop their news and political information repertoires (Webster &
Ksiazek, 2012). People also develop strategies for making use of different media, often
21
approach because it does not consider the nuanced and possibly strategic use of multiple
likelihood of being caught in an echo chamber but so too are acts individuals take which
can, intentionally or otherwise, help them avoid echo chambers. We consider the acts
individuals can take to avoid echo chambers as they choose which media to use and
how to use them in a high-choice media environment. We use these acts as a proxy for
likelihood of being caught in an echo chamber. This is because news and political
particularly when few or even one media source is considered or recalled, cannot
Future work on echo chambers should consider the various types of choices
individuals can make in this high-choice media environment including the diversity of
media they make use of and the consequences of that diverse use in terms of how and
when different media are combined. Future studies might draw on the idea of dual-
practices, and other ways in which individuals use media in complementary ways as
seeking practices which means that single platform studies are insufficient for assessing
the threats of echo chambers in the context of a high-choice media environment. The
risk of echo chambers is that they divide society into groups of people who are informed
and people who are not and/or across partisan lines. This societal threat can only be
assessed if the multiple media individuals often rely on are considered together.
22
It seems likely that networks on Twitter are polarized, as Conover and
colleagues’ results show (2011) and networks on other social media may be equally
polarized. But social media are only part of the environment, and they are the least
trusted part. Political information can be sourced through many media channels,
including political websites, websites of offline magazines and newspapers, offline print
media, and above all television. Twitter may be a place where individuals talk to people
with the same political opinions. But a study of Twitter says little about the political
information one is exposed to when they watch CNN or BBC news, or visit the
Economist website or the Washington Post. These are places where individuals may be
exposed to a wider variety of information and political views. This suggests that future
research could profitably focus on the complex ways that people interact with all forms
use diverse media are more likely to be in an echo chamber. They are less likely check
multiple sources or to discover things that change their minds. This is an argument that
an echo chamber exists, but for a subset of the population. While it is concerning that
some individuals are likely to be caught in an echo chamber it is worth noting that this
segment of the population is quite small. In our data 148 respondents, about 8%, have
media diversity scores of 10 or less (out of a possible maximum of 48) and also say they
in an echo chamber they may also benefit from friends and family who have more
diverse media diets and who are interested in politics. Katz and Lazarsfeld famously
identified the opinion leader who is found in every social stratum and who is an above
23
average consumer of news media (1955). The opinion leader shares political
information and opinions with their everyday associates who are normally less
interested in politics and who normally consume less news. Their opinion is one that
people who are less interested in politics may listen to and heed. Though we cannot test
this idea in the present study, this could be a way that people who are less interested in
to what opinion leaders choose to share, with whom, and in what contexts.
Finally, this evidence that an echo chamber may exist for a portion of the
population does suggest that increased media literacy can help people learn to avoid
individuals should not rely solely on social media. These claims are correct; people with
opportunity for future work to examine the kinds of media choices that are most
effective which can in turn inform policy and educational campaigns. This work would
serve efforts at increasing media literacy and shed light on ways we might combat echo
chambers that exist for the relatively small proportion of individuals who are neither
Broader implications
This study has two broader implications. First, the definition and measurement of being
in an echo chamber has so far been overly narrow. This narrowness is a potential
medium studies are not useful to generalize to the broader media environment. Both
relate to the fact that, despite touting the potential of the Internet to expand media and
24
of understanding the political communication practices of individuals and the
individuals the opportunity to choose among a variety of media in order to serve their
own needs and preferences. Yet, empirical work has too often used overly narrow
definitions of being caught in an echo chamber which do not actually encompass the
choices individuals can make. We need to consider factors such as what and how many
media people choose, how they choose to use them, whether this use is overlapping and
assessed by more than a single-self report of exposure to different ideas or trace data
considers the ways individuals actually use media in this high-choice environment.
social network site, are of limited value. Consider that in our data young respondents
age 18-34 say they have accounts on an average of five social media. Studying one of
those five social networking sites, no matter how large the dataset, remains a single case
study. Though valuable for other purposes, they do not help us understand the other four
social media, nor does it help us understand how they consume other online and offline
media. The value of studies of a single medium is waning. Unless we have a special
we believed it we would study individuals and their choices in this environment in all its
25
References
Adamic, L.A. and Glance, N. (2005, August). The political blogosphere and the 2004
U.S. election. Proceedings of the 3rd International workshop on Link Discovery (pp.
36-43). Chicago: Il. ACM: New York.
Ahlers, D. (2006). News consumption and the new electronic media. The Harvard
International Journal of Press/Politics, 11(1): 29–52.
Bakshy, E., Messing, S., and Adamic, L. (2015). Exposure to Ideologically Diverse
News and Opinion on Facebook. Science, May.
Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A., & Bonneau, R. (2015). Tweeting from
left to right: Is online political communication more than an echo
chamber?. Psychological science, 26(10), 1531-1542.
Blank, G. (2017). The digital divide among Twitter users and its implications for social
research. Social Science Computer Review.
Boulianne, S. (2011). Stimulating or reinforcing political interest: Using panel data to
examine reciprocal effects between news media and political interest. Political
Communication, 28(2), 147-162.
Conover, M.D., Ratkiewicz, J., Francisco, M.R., Goncalves, B., Flammini, A., Menczer,
F. (2011). Political polarization on Twitter. In Fifth International Conference on
Weblogs and Social media (ICWSM) (pp. 89-96). AAAI: Palo Alto, CA .
Dubois, E. (2015). The strategic opinion leader: Personal influence and political
networks in a hybrid media system. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:35b1e408-a70a-4ea0-9c41-10d7df024ee9
Dutton, W., Reisdorf, B., Dubois, E. & Blank, G. (2017). Search and Politics. Retrieved
from http://ssrn.com/abstract=2960697, last accessed on 13 June 2017.
Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. New York, NY: Row, Peterson
and Company.
Garrett, R. K. (2009). Echo Chambers Online?: Politically Motivated Selective
Exposure among Internet News users1. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 14 (2): 265–85.
Garrett, R. K., Bimber, B., de Zuniga, H. G., Heinderyckx, F., Kelly, J., & Smith, M.
(2012). New ICTs and the study of political communication. International Journal
of Communication, 6, 214–231.
Goel, S., Mason, W., and Watts, D. (2010). Real and Perceived Attitude Agreement in
Social Networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(4), 611–21.
Harkins, S. G., & Petty, R. E. (1987). Information utility and the multiple source
effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(2), 260.
Heeter, C. (1985). Program selection with abundance of choice: a process model.
Human Communication Research, 12(1), 126–152.
Himelboim, I., McCreery, S., & Smith, M. (2013). Birds of a feather tweet together:
Integrating network and content analyses to examine cross-ideology exposure on
Twitter. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 18(2), 154-174.
Iyengar, S. and Hahn, K. S. (2009). Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological
selectivity in media use. Journal of Communication, 59, 19-39.
Jamieson, K. and Cappella, J. (2008). Echo Chamber: Rush Limbaugh and the
Conservative Media Establishment. London, UK: Oxford UP.
Janis, I. (1972) Groupthink. 2nd edn. Boston, MA: Houghton, Mifflin.
Katz, E. and Lazarsfeld, P. (1955). Personal Influence: The part played by people in the
flow of mass communication. New York, NY: The Free Press.
Klapper, J. T.(1960). The effects of mass communication. Free Press: New York.
26
Knobloch-Westerwick, S., and Kleinman, S. (2012). Preelection Selective Exposure
Confirmation Bias versus Informational Utility. Communication Research, 39 (2),
170–93.
Ksiazek, T, Malthouse, E., and Webster, J. (2010) News-seekers and Avoiders:
Exploring Patterns of Total News Consumption Across Media and the Relationship
to Civic Participation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(4), 551-568.
LaRose, R. (2010). The problem of media habits. Communication Theory, 20(2), 194-
222.
Lawrence, E., Sides, J., and Farrell, H. (2010). Self-Segregation or Deliberation? Blog
Readership, Participation, and Polarization in American Politics. Perspectives on
Politics, 8(1), 141–157.
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., Cook, J. M. (2011) Birds of a Feather: Homophily in
Social Networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444.
Messing, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2014). Selective exposure in the age of social media:
Endorsements trump partisan source affiliation when selecting news
online. Communication Research, 41(8), 1042-1063.
Nikolov, D., Oliveira, D. F., Flammini, A., & Menczer, F. (2015). Measuring online
social bubbles. PeerJ Computer Science, 1.
Pariser, E. (2011). The Filter Bubble: What the Internet is Hiding from You. New York,
NYP: Penguin Press.
Prior, M. (2009). The immensely inflated news audience: Assessing bias in self-
reported news exposure. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(1), 130-143.
-- (2007). Post-broadcast democracy: How media choice increases inequality in
political involvement and polarizes elections. New York, NY: Cambridge UP.
Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Kalogeropoulos, A., Levy, D. and Kleis Nielsen, R. (2017).
Reuters Digital News Report 2017. Oxford: Reuters Institute for the Study of
Journalism. Retrieved from http://www.digitalnewsreport.org/
Stroud, N. (2007). Media Use and Political Predispositions: Revisiting the Concept of
Selective Exposure. Political Behavior, 30(3), 341–66.
Sunstein, C. (2002). The Law of Group Polarization. Journal of Political Philosophy,
10(2), 175–95.
-- (2009). Republic. Com 2.0. New York, NY: Princeton UP.
Vaccari, C., Chadwick, A., & O'Loughlin, B. (2015). Dual screening the political:
Media events, social media, and citizen engagement. Journal of
Communication, 65(6), 1041-1061.
Valentino, N., Banks, A., Hutchings, V. and Davis, A. (2009). Selective Exposure in the
Internet Age: The Interaction between Anxiety and Information Utility. Political
Psychology, 30(4), 591–613.
Van Aelst, P., Strömbäck, J., Aalberg, T., Esser, F., de Vreese, C., Matthes, J.,
Hopmann, D., Salgado, S., Hubé, N., Stępińska, A., Papathanassopoulos, S.,
Berganza, R., Legnante, G., Reinemann, C., Sheafer, T., Stanyer, J. (2017) Political
communication in a high-choice media environment: a challenge for
democracy?. Annals of the International Communication Association, 41(1), 3-27.
Webster, J. G. (2005). Beneath the veneer of fragmentation: Television audience
polarization in a multichannel world. Journal of Communication, 55(2), 366-382.
Webster, J. G., & Ksiazek, T. B. (2012). The dynamics of audience fragmentation:
Public attention in an age of digital media. Journal of Communication, 62(1), 39-56.
Weeks, B. E., Ksiazek, T. B., & Holbert, R. L. (2016). Partisan enclaves or shared
media experiences? A network approach to understanding citizens’ political news
environments. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 60(2), 248-268.
27
Wesler, H. T., Smith, M., Fisher, D., & Gleave, E. (2008). Distilling digital traces:
Computational social science approaches to studying the Internet. The SAGE
handbook of online research methods (pp. 116–140). London, UK: Sage
Publications.
Wojcieszak, M. E., & Mutz, D. C. (2009). Online groups and political discourse: Do
online discussion spaces facilitate exposure to political disagreement?. Journal of
Communication, 59(1), 40-56.
Wolfsfeld, G., Yarchi, M., & Samuel-Azran, T. (2016). Political information repertoires
and political participation. New Media & Society, 18(9), 2096-2115.
Yuan, E. (2011). News consumption across multiple media platforms: a repertoire
approach. Information, Communication & Society, 14(7), 998–1016.
28