0% found this document useful (0 votes)
372 views61 pages

Imagery and Verbal Processes - Introduction

This document is the preface to a book by Allan Paivio on imagery and verbal processes in human learning, memory, perception, and language. The book aims to present a theoretical account of the role of higher mental processes and imagery and verbal codes in these areas. It reviews research evidence to support this approach and integrate prebehavioristic and behavioristic views of thought. The book serves as both a textbook and theoretical monograph for graduate and upper-level undergraduate students. The preface acknowledges contributions from students and colleagues who provided feedback and assistance.

Uploaded by

liuzixin704
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
372 views61 pages

Imagery and Verbal Processes - Introduction

This document is the preface to a book by Allan Paivio on imagery and verbal processes in human learning, memory, perception, and language. The book aims to present a theoretical account of the role of higher mental processes and imagery and verbal codes in these areas. It reviews research evidence to support this approach and integrate prebehavioristic and behavioristic views of thought. The book serves as both a textbook and theoretical monograph for graduate and upper-level undergraduate students. The preface acknowledges contributions from students and colleagues who provided feedback and assistance.

Uploaded by

liuzixin704
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 61

Imagery

and ALLAN PAIVIO


University of Western Ontario
London, Ontario

Verbal Processes

V p Psychology Press
X Taylor & Francis Croup
New York London
To my wife Kathleen
and my children Sandra, Anna Lee, Heather, Eric, and Karina

First Published by
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers
365 Broadway
Hillsdale, New Jersey 07642

Transferred to Digital Printing 2009 by Psychology Press


270 Madison Ave, New York NY 10016
27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex, BN3 2FA

CopyrightCl 1979 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.


All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in
any form, by photostat, microform, retrieval system, or any other
means, without the prior written permission of the publisher.

Library of Congress Catalog Card Number 73-150787

Publisher's Note
The publisher has gone to great lengths to ensure the quality of this reprint
but points out that some imperfections in the original may be apparent.
Preface

In this book I have attempted to present a systematic theoretical and


factual account of the role of higher mental processes in human learning and
memory, and certain aspects of the psychology of perception and language.
The major orienting theme of the book is its dual emphasis on nonverbal
imagery and verbal processes (inner speech) as memory codes and mediators
of behavior. Based on recent experimental evidence, the conceptual approach
in a sense represents an integration of prebehavioristic and behavioristic views
concerning the nature of thought.
The book is intended both as a textbook and as a theoretical monograph.
Beginning with a theoretical orientation, I soon became convinced that I could
not adequately convey the scope of the approach without an extensive review
of the research literature in the various areas it encompasses. Moreover, the
skepticism which behaviorism justifiably fostered in regard to the mentalistic
concept of imagery still persists to some extent among experimental psychologists,
and it is unlikely to be completely dispelled without overwhelming empirical
evidence demonstrating that the construct can have real scientific value. I have
done my best to present such evidence, with the result that the book is somewhat
longer than I had originally intended, but at the same time perhaps more useful
than it might otherwise have been as a textbook and reference book for psychol-
ogy students. It is most relevant as a textbook for graduate and upper-division
undergraduate courses in human learning, memory, psycholinguistics, and
cognitive processes. It could also serve as supplementary reading in courses on
perception. I have assumed throughout that the reader has some familiarity with
the basic facts in the various areas-of the level provided, for example, by
introductory courses in learning and perception, or a course in general experi-
mental psychology.
ALLAN P AIVIO
London, Ontario
July 1971
iii
This page intentionally left blank
Acknowledgements

It is a pleasure for me to express my gratitude to the many people who


in one way or another have contributed to the book or to the enterprise on
which it is based. These include the graduate students and former graduate
students whose names appear throughout the book as collaborators in the
studies that grew out of the ideas we shared and who, individually or in
seminars, have commented on drafts of various chapters-Professors Frank D.
Colman, Stephan A. Madigan, Herb M. Simpson, A. Dan Yarmey, John C.
Yuille, and Dr. Padric C. Smythe; more recently, Carole Ernest, Ian Begg,
Kalman Csapo, Brian O'Neill, Ronald Philipchalk, and Ted Rowe. I am grateful
also to Professor Doreen Kimura, Professor Zenon Pylyshyn, Dr. Colin Berry,
Andrew Feldmar, and Peter Skehan, who read and commented on sections of
the book. It was my good fortune, too, that Professor Ralph Haber read the
entire preliminary manuscript on behalf of Holt, Rinehart and Winston, and
I was able to take advantage of his counsel in preparing the final draft.
Many of the key studies reported in this book grew out of a research program
that has been supported financially by the National Research Council of Canada
(Grant APA 87), the University of Western Ontario Research Fund and the
University's Summer Supplements program. The scope of the research program
would have been severely restricted without their support.
A number of people have provided clerical and other assistance. Mrs. Helen
Smith expertly typed an early mimeographed draft of the book and most of
the final manuscript. Mrs. Patricia Butler and Mrs. Ann Anas proofread the
various drafts and helped compile the references. Typing and general clerical
assistance were ably provided by Mrs. Elizabeth Wilkins.
Above all I am grateful to my wife, who has been patient and encouraging
over the years when I devoted much of my "spare" time during evenings and
weekends to writing. Moreover, despite her own busy life, she found time to
v
vi ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

contribute specifically to the book by typing early drafts of several chapters


and part of the final manuscript. This book is affectionately dedicated to her
and to my children.
In addition to the individuals mentioned above and the sources noted in the
figure legends, credit is due the following for permission to reproduce or adapt
material:

Figure 4.1. Reprinted with permISSIOn of authors and publisher: From Robinson. J. S.,
Brown, L. T., & Hayes, W. H. Test of effects of past experience on perception. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 1964, 18, 953-956.
Figure 4.2. Adapted from Pritchard, R. M., Heron, W., & Hebb, D. O. Visual perception
approached by the method of stabilized images. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1960, 14,
67-77.
Figure 4.3. From Tees, R. C., & More, L. K. Effect of amount of perceptual learning
upon disappearances observed under reduced stimulation conditions. Perception and
Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 565-568.
Figure 4.4. From Wapner, S., & Werner, H. Perceptual development. Worcester, Mass.:
Clark University Press, 1957.
Figure 4.5. From Dick, A. 0., & Mewhort, J. K. Order of report and processing in
tachistoscopic recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 573-576.
Figure 4.6. From Kimura, D. Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening.
Cortex, 1967, 3, 163-178. Reproduced by permission of the author.
Figure 5.1. From Hershenson, M., & Haber, R. N. The role of meaning in the perception
of briefly presented words. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 42-46.
Figure 5.2. From Standing, L., Sell, c., Boss, J., & Haber, R. N. Effect of visualization
and subvocalization on perceptual clarity. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 18, 89-90.
Figure 5.3. From Posner, M. L, & Mitchell, R. F. Chronometric analysis of classification.
Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 392-409. Copyright 1967 by the American Psychological
Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 5.4. From Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. Decay of visual information from a
single letter. Science, 1967, 158, 137-139. Copyright 1967 by the American Association
for the Advancement of Science.
Figure 5.5. From Posner, M. I., Boies, S. J., Eichelman, W. H., & Taylor, R. L. Retention
of visual and name codes of single letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology Mono-
graph, 1969, 79 (1, Pt. 2). Copyright 1969 by the American Psychological Association,
and reproduced by permission.
Figure 5.6. From Haber, R. N., & Nathanson, L. S. Processing of sequentially presented
letters. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 5, 359-361.
Figure 5.7. From Brooks, L. R. The suppression of visualization in reading. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 19, 289-299.
Figure 5.S. From Brooks, L. R. Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 349-368.
Figure 7.2. From Vanderplas, J. M., & Garvin, E. A. The association value of random
shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 147-154. Copyright 1959 by the
American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 7.3. From Feuge, R. L., & Ellis, H. C. Generalization gradients in recognition
memory of visual form: The role of stimulus meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy, 1969, 79, 288-294. Copyright 1969 by the American Psychological Association, and
reproduced by permission.
Figure 7.5. From Noble, C. E. Meaningfulness and familiarity. In C. N. Cofer and B. S.
Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning. Copyright 1963 by McGraw-Hill. Used
with permission of McGraw-Hill Book Company.
Figure 7.S. From Paivio, A., & Madigan, S. A. Noun imagery and frequency in paired-
associate and free recall learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1970, 24, 353-361.
Figure 7.10. From Koeppel, J. C., & Beecroft, R. S. The conceptual similarity effect in
free recall. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9, 213-214.
Figure 7.11. From Paivio, A., Yuille, J. c., & Rogers, T. B. Noun imagery and meaning-
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS vii

fulness in free and serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1969, 79, 509-514.
Copyright 1969 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 7.12. From Paivio, A., Rogers, T. B., & Smythe, P. C. Why are pictures easier
to recall than words? Psychonomic Science, 1968, II, 137-138.
Figure 7.14. From Paivio, A., & Smythe, P. C. Word imagery, frequency, and meaning-
fulness in short-term memory. Psychonomic Science, 1971, 22, 333-335.
Figure 7.15. From Wickens, D. D., & Clark, S. Osgood dimensions as an encoding class
in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 580-584. Copyright
1968 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figures 7.16 and 7.17. From Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. Concrete-image and verbal memory
codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 80, 279-285. Copyright 1969 by the
American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figures 8.3 and 11.1. From Paivio, A., Smythe, P. C., & Yuille, J. C. Imagery versus
meaningfulness of nouns in paired-associate learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
1968, 22, 427-441.
Figure 8.6. From Paivio, A., and Madigan, S. A. Imagery and association value in
paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 76, 35-39. Copyright
1968 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 8.7. From Yuille, J. c., Paivio, A., & Lambert, W. E. Noun and adjective imagery
and order in paired-associate learning by French and English subjects. Canadian Journal
of Psychology, 1969, 23, 459-466.
Figure 8.9. From Asch, S. E., & Ebenholtz, S. M. The principle of associative symmetry.
Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 1962, 106, 135-163.
Figure 9.1. From Kiess, H. O. Effects of natural language mediators on short-term
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 7-13. Copyright 1968 by the
American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 9.2. From Bower, G. H., & Clark, M. C. Narrative stories as mediators for serial
learning. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 14, 181-182.
Figure 10.1. Reprinted with permission of author and publisher: From Smith, R. K., &
Noble, C. E. Effects of a mnemonic technique applied to verbal learning and memory.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 21, 123-134.
Figure 10.2. From Senter, R. J., & Hauser, G. K. An experimental study of a mnemonic
system. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 10, 289-290.
Figure 10.4. From Bugelski, B. R. Images as mediators in one-trial paired-associate
learning. II: Self-timing in successive lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968,
77, 328-334. Copyright 1968 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced
by permission.
Figure 10.6. From Paivio, A. Effects of imagery instructions and concreteness of
memory pegs in a mnemonic system. Proceedings of the 76th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, 1968, 77-78. Copyright 1968 by the American
Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 11.2. From Yuille, J. c., & Paivio, A. Latency of imaginal and verbal mediators
as a function of stimulus and response concreteness-imagery. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 75, 540-544. Copyright 1967 by the American Psychological Associa-
tion, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 11.4. From Yuille, J. C., & Paivio, A. Imagery and verbal mediation instructions
in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 436-441. Copy-
right 1968 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figures 11.5 and 11.6. From Paivio, A., and Yuille, J. C. Changes in associative strategies
and paired-associate learning over trials as a function of word imagery and type of
learning set. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 458-463. Copyright 1969 by
the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 11.7. From Paivio, A., & Foth, D. Imaginal and verbal mediators and noun
concreteness in paired-associate learning: The elusive interaction. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 384-390.
Figure 11.12. Epstein, W., Rock, I., & Zuckerman, C. B. Meaning and familiarity in
associative learning. Psychological Monographs, 1960, 74 (4, Whole No. 491). Copyright
1960 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 12.2. From Katz, J. J., & Fodor, J. A. The structure of a semantic theory.
Language, 1963, 39, 170-210. Copyright 1963 by the Linguistic Society of America.
viii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Figure 12.3. From Miller, G. A., & Isard, S. Some perceptual consequences of linguistic
rules. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 217-228.
Figure 13.3. From Reynolds, J. H. Cognitive transfer in verbal learning: II. Transfer
effects after prefamiliarization with integrated versus partially integrated verbal· perceptual
structures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1968, 59, 133-138. Copyright 1968 by the
American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission.
Figure 13.4. From Sachs, J. S. Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects
of connected discourse. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 437-442.
Figure 13.5. From Begg, I., & Paivio, A. Concreteness and imagery in sentence meaning.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 821-827.
Figure 13.6. From Paivio, A. Imagery and deep structure in the recall of English nominali·
zations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, 10, 1-12.
Figure 14.4. From Pettifor, J. L. The role of language in the development of abstract
thinking: A comparison of hard·of·hearing and normal·hearing children on levels of
conceptual thinking. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 139-156.
Excerpts from Frances A. Yates, The art of memory (Chicago: University of Chicago
Press; London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1966), pp. 2, 4, 6-8, 9-10, 22, 24-25, 35-36.
Copyright 1966 by the University of Chicago Press. Reprinted by permission of the
publishers.
Contents

1
INTRODUCTION, I

2
DEVELOPMENT AND FUNCTIONS OF THE
SYMBOLIC SYSTEMS, II

3
MEANING AND THE SYMBOLIC PROCESSES, 39

4
PERCEPTION AND THE SYMBOLIC
PROCESSES: Effects of Meaning, 87

5
PERCEPTION AND THE EXPERIMENTAL
AROUSAL OF THE SYMBOLIC PROCESSES, 119
ix
x CONTENTS

6
LEARNING AND MEMORY: Classical
Mnemonic Systems, 153

7
STIMULUS ATTRIBUTES AND MEMORY, 177

8
MEANING AND ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING, 245

9
VERBAL MEDIATION IN LEARNING AND
MEMORY, 299

10
IMAGERY MEDIATION IN LEARNING AND
MEMORY, 327

11
DISTINGUISHING IMAGINAL AND VERBAL
MEDIATORS, 353

12
LANGUAGE AND THE SYMBOLIC
PROCESSES: Linguistic Models and
Associationism, 393

13
IMAGERY AND LANGUAGE, 433
CONTENTS xi

14
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN SYMBOLIC
HABITS AND SKILLS, 477

15
EXTENSIONS AND SPECULATIONS, 525

REFERENCES, 535
NAME INDEX, 581
SUBJECT INDEX, 590
This page intentionally left blank
1
Introduction

A remarkable trend is emerging in major areas of psychological inquiry.


The trend involves a renewed interest in mentalistic concepts, among which
imagery and meaning are central. It is remarkable because such concepts, once
prominent in psychology, became anathema in American psychology shortly
after the turn of the century. The negative attitude is generally attributed to the
behavioristic revolution, and the main issues and developments will be consid-
ered primarily in that context. However, in view of the attention given to human
learning and memory in this book, it is important to note that the negative influ-
ence was not restricted to behaviorism proper, but stemmed also from the rote
learning tradition established by Ebbinghaus in 1885. This approach was no less
objective than Watson's behaviorism. Ebbinghaus abandoned the introspective
method and sought to minimize effects attributable to pre-experimental associa-
tive habits and meaning (both mentalistic concepts in that prebehavioristic era)
in order to reveal the factors responsible for the formation of new memory
associations. The latter goal was to be achieved especially by the introduction
of the nonsense syllable as the unit for the experimental study of memory. The
main features of the Ebbinghaus approach persisted in rote learning experiments
with little change for more than seventy years (see Irion, 1959).
The approach has unquestionably yielded important information concerning
effective variables in a variety of tasks, but most of the problems originally
investigated by Ebbinghaus remain essentially unsolved today. The slow devel-
opment of theoretical understanding can be attributed partly to the complexity
of human learning and memory. In addition, however, it is possible that the
attempts (largely unsuccessful, as it turned out) to control rather than sys-
tematically investigate the contributions of meaning and the correlated associative
habits from the outset had a retarding effect on theoretical developments in the
field. Be that as it may, the situation is changing rapidly. The change is mani-
1
2 IMAGERY AND VERBAL PROCESSES

fested in an accumulating research literature on such variables as meaningful-


ness, mediation processes, and organizational factors in learning and memory.
But these departures from the Ebbinghaus tradition have also shown a bias in
the theoretical approach to the mental mechanisms presumed to be the basis
of such phenomena: Verbal processes were emphasized while nonverbal imagery
was almost totally ignored. The verbal emphasis arose directly from behaviorism
and extended beyond memory to the related problems of meaning, association,
perception, and thought in general. The emphasis contrasts sharply with the
yeoman's role played by mental imagery over a period of 2500 years in the
interpretation of such phenomena. It contrasts also with the consideration given
to imagery in the present volume. Does this conceptual revival reflect real theo-
retical and empirical progress in psychology or is it merely a passing, reactionary
trend? That question is the orienting theme of the book. It may not be answered
entirely to the reader's satisfaction, but he will at least find considerable evidence
and argument to weigh. To indicate why the question arises at all, it is neces-
sary to review the relevant historical developments and controversial issues lead-
ing up to the present state of affairs.

Mental Images versus Mental Words


Statements concerning the functions of mental imagery in such phenom-
ena as meaning and memory appeared in early Greek writings, and the funda-
mental ideas then expressed persisted in associationism and structuralism up to
the time of Watson's persuasive attack on mentalistic concepts. Around 500 B.C.,
the poet Simonides aptly summarized the essence of the imagery hypothesis of
linguistic-- meaning in the phrase "Words are the images of things" (Bowra,
1961, p. 363). Some 2500 years later, William James similarly described the
static meaning of concrete words as consisting of "sensory images awakened"
(1890, p. 265), and Titchener (1909) extended this view to encompass abstract
terms as well.
The image appeared in theories of memory in two forms. First, as manifested
in Plato's "wax tablet" model, imagery was the prototype of stimulus-trace the-
ories of memory (see Gomulicki, 1953), according to which perceptions and
thoughts are impressed on the mind as on a block of wax, to be remembered
and known as long as the image lasts. Second, as associative imagery it was
assumed to playa mediational role in various memory techniques, which appar-
ently originated with Simonides and were thereafter elaborated by philosophers
and teachers of rhetoric as a practical art (see Yates, 1966, and Chapter 6 in
this book). This associative view holds that memory images could be evoked
by stimuli with which the imaged objects or events have been associated in the
past, and that these images could themselves combine associatively, as in the
"association of ideas." The image theory of meaning can be regarded as a logical
extension of such associationistic views to the domain of language, with words
presumably serving as the cues for the arousal of the memory image. Common-
Introduction 3

sense experience continues to make the acceptance of such views compelling.


Occasionally, when I have been required to list the names of my colleagues
from memory, I have found myself visualizing the hallways in which their offices
are located, systematically moving past these offices, then picturing and naming
the occupants. Shepard described a similar personal observation as follows:
" ... if I am now asked about the number of windows in my house, I find that
I must picture the house, as viewed from different sides or from within different
rooms, and then count the windows presented in these various mental images.
No amount of purely verbal machinations would seem to suffice" (1966, p. 203).
The crucial point about such experiences is that the eliciting question and the
behavioral expression of recall may be entirely verbal, but the mediating mecha-
nism apparently consists of nonverbal imagery associatively evoked by the words.
It is necessary only to add that such views were extended by the structural-
ists to the interpretation of perception as a combination of sensations and
memory images, and to thought in general as the manipulation of mental images,
and the story is complete. Memory, meaning, association, perception, thought-
all of these in one way or another implicated mental imagery as a crucial mech-
anism.
It is precisely such views that were rejected by Watson (1913). Partly on
philosophical grounds and partly on the basis of experimental evidence then
available on the issue, he concluded that mental images are mere ghosts, with-
out functional significance. The image theory of memory was shaken by find-
ings that revealed that images are not faithful reproductions of reality, even
for those individuals who claim "photographic" memories (e.g., Fernald, 1912).
Furthermore, the Wiirzburg experiments yielded abundant evidence that think-
ing may go on without reportable conscious content in the form of concrete-
object or verbal images (see Humphrey, 1951; Mandler & Mandler, 1964).
Experimental data thus raised serious questions concerning both the repre-
sentational (memory) and associative or mediational (thought) functions that
traditionally had been attributed to images, and such findings apparently
prompted Watson to conclude that imagery had no functional significance.
Moreover, the concept was unacceptably subjective and was to be banished
along with the rest of the mentalistic baggage that introspectionism produced.
In Watson's approach, the functions that had been attributed to images in
thought and memory became the burden of words-verbal responses-or their
gestural substitutes (e.g., Watson, 1930, pp. 265-268). Verbal recollections
of remembered objects and events are simply habits, learned responses that
occurred originally in the particular setting being described and are re-evoked
later by cues that originally accompanied their occurrence. To the extent that
thought is involved in such behavior, as when one thinks about the original
events before responding, the mediating process is also verbal, that is, it con-
sists of talking to oneself. Meaning, too, was to be viewed in terms of the
organized verbal and nonverbal responses that a given object evokes in an
individual.
4 IMAGERY AND VERBAL PROCESSES

Contemporary Status of the Controversy


Watson's stand on imagery and the factors leading up to it effectively
suppressed interest in the concept, particularly in America. This was reflected
in a marked reduction in the number of empirical studies involving the con-
cept in comparison with the volume of research that followed Galton's pioneer-
ing studies on individual differences in imagery in 1880 and continued until a
few years after Watson's attempted coup de grace. As the interest in imagery
declined, an emphasis on verbal processes quite in accord with Watson's views
increasingly dominated empirical and theoretical approaches to problems of
meaning and mediation processes in verbal learning, memory, association, and
language (see Goss, 1961). The meaningfulness of verbal units came to be
defined most often in terms of such measures as association value (Glaze, 1928)
or meaningfulness (m; Noble, 1952a), which assume that verbal associations
are the basis of meaning. Consistent with this definition, effects of meaningful-
ness on associative learning and memory were interpreted in terms of the avail-
ability of implicit verbal associates as potential mediators of stimulus-response
connections, as in the associative probability or "grapnel" theory (Glanzer,
1962; Underwood & Schulz, 1960, p. 296)-the greater the number of verbal
associations that item A evokes, the greater the probability that one of these
associations will provide a common link between A and B. A similar verbal
emphasis has been generally apparent in research on mediated transfer and
generalization, organizational factors in memory and language, and so-called
"natural language" mediators (e.g., Adams, 1967; W. A. Bousfield, 1961;
Cofer, 1965; Deese, 1965; J. J. Jenkins, 1963). Even in perception, higher-
order coding processes have been interpreted as primarily verbal in nature
(e.g., Glanzer & Clark, 1963a; Haber, 1966). Explicitly or implicitly, the domi-
nant view in America has been that effective meaning and mediational phe-
nomena generally are founded on verbal associative mechanisms. The possibility
that nonverbal imagery may be functionally involved was generally ignored and
occasionally rejected for reasons that echo Watson's original objections to the
concept.

The logical argument The current attitude toward imagery and the rea-
sons for it have been summarized recently by Deese (1965) in reference to
association and language. The arguments apply generally to the phenomena
being considered here and, because they express a rather general viewpoint,
they merit careful consideration. Thus:

The modern experimental psychologist works almost exclusively with linguistic


associations for the good reason that these provide controllable material for
his laboratory studies; he ignores the extra existence of perceptual imagery.
Without necessarily denying either their reality or their importance, the con-
temporary psychologist finds images difficult to manage in empirical study.
Partly for this reason and partly for others, association theory in modern
Introduction 5

psychology has become a theory of the succession of elements in verbal be-


havior (p. 4).

This is one of the classical behavioristic arguments-imagery is subjective


and inferential, words are objective and manageable. It constitutes a valid case
against imagery if the interests of contemporary psychologists remained at the
empirical level, but they obviously do not. As Deese goes on to say:

We study associations in order to make inferences about the nature of human


thought .... The whole ot the current concern is with the associative prop-
erties ot explicit verbal behavior as a model tor the implicit verbal process ot
thought (1965, p. 4, italics added).

With that the empirical superiority of words over images ends, for implicit
verbal processes are no less inferential than perceptual images. One might argue
that it is more direct and, therefore, more parsimonious to infer a verbal medi-
ating process when the response is verbal, but this follows only if one assumes
a one-to-one relation between the overt associative reaction and the mental
process that caused it. Such an assumption would be unsound, for one can
respond verbally to pictures as well as to words and so, by analogy, one's verbal
response could just as logically be mediated by a "mental picture" as by "men-
tal words" (that is, implicit verbal representations). Both are pure inferences
and which one it is more logical to infer in research concerned with such prob-
lems depends on the total set of conditions in a given study, not merely on the
mode of the overt response. The problem is empirical as well as logical, and
it is the experimenter's burden to devise conditions that will permit him to make
reasonable inferences about the nature of the effective underlying processes
independent of their particular behavioral expression.

The factual argument Watson's second major objection to the concept


of imagery, which also persists in contemporary discussions (e.g., R. W. Brown,
1958), was based on the lack of factual evidence that images have any func-
tional significance, even in memory tasks where vivid images should be most
useful. The conclusion that images have no relation to memory because they
do not accurately reflect reality is unjustified, however, simply because all mem-
ory is imperfect. Bartlett (1932) clearly recognized the inaccuracy but did not
find it necessary therefore to conclude that the concept of memory is unnec-
essary, nor that images have no function in .relation to it. Instead, his conclu-
sion was that remembered events are partly constructions rather than faithful
reproductions of reality. The imageless thought experiments likewise did not
indicate that images had no mediational function, only that complex mental
operations could occur without conscious content. Images were, in fact, reported
frequently in those experiments, and Humphrey (1951) concluded from this
that the Wiirzburg psychologists underplayed the significance of imagery as
inferred from the reports of their subjects. Moreover, inferences concerning the
6 IMAGERY AND VERBAL PROCESSES

function of imagery were based almost exclusively on individual differences in


the reported vividness of images, a defining operation that still fails to reveal
the functional significance of imagery as clearly as other objective procedures,
as will be seen later.
Finally, just as verbal mediation processes are no less inferential than images,
so too are they open to the same factual criticisms: Verbal associative memory
is imperfect, and conscious verbal processes were absent in imageless thought
(Humphrey, 1951). Should we conclude from such observations that the con-
cept of verbal mediation is without scientific value? I presume that few would
want to do so and we must accordingly reject such a conclusion in regard to
imagery as well. The problem in the case of both of these postulated processes
is to clarify their functions, that is, to determine the conditions under which
mental images and mental words are aroused and to identify the nature of their
effects on overt behavior. Both are theoretical constructs and whether or- not
it is useful to postulate either, or both, depends on the adequacy of the defining
operations and the research procedures used to test the properties that have
been theoretically attributed to them.

Revival of the image Perhaps partly for the reasons just discussed and
partly because the concept is somehow a valid reflection of mental processes,
Watson's attack did not succeed in burying "the image." Research concerned
explicitly with mental imagery continued to appear sporadically in psychological
literature even during the most arid period in the 1920s and thirties, and the
classical problems associated with imagery continued to be discussed in influ-
ential textbooks of the day (e.g., Woodworth, 1938). In addition, concepts with
essentially the same functional properties as imagery turned up in behavioristic
writings. Perhaps the most obvious example is Tolman's (e.g., 1932, 1948)
cognitive approach to behavior theory, in which such terms as expectancy,
sign learning, and cognitive maps substituted for the various functions that had
been attributed earlier to mental images. Hull's (1931) fractional anticipatory
goal response (rg-sg) mechanism and Osgood's (1953) elaboration of it under
the label of representational mediation process are functionally parallel to Tol-
man's sign-Gestalt-expectation and, on close inspection, all of the concepts can
be seen to retain some of the cognitive flavor of the imagery concept. Perhaps
this was only to be expected. As pointed out by Morgan (1943) and Hebb
( 1949), mental variables have been repeatedly thrown out because there was
no place for them in stimulus-response psychology, but they repeatedly find their
way in again in one form or another because they are necessary to a full account
of behavior.
Thus it is not surprising that we find the concept of imagery reappearing
essentially in its pristine form but with its respectability enhanced by a behavior-
istic cloak. On the basis of results from experimental investigation involving
a classical conditioning paradigm, Leuba (1940) felt justified in referring to
images as conditioned sensations. Skinner (1953) similarly discussed visual
imagery as "conditioned seeing." The possibility has even been suggested that
Introduction 7

conditioned images may play a mediational role in classical conditioning in-


volving lower animals (Mowrer, 1960; Sheffield, 1965). The most elaborate
treatment of this kind can be found in Beritoff's (1965) analysis of aspects of
higher vertebrate behavior in terms of images, although in it he distinguished
"image-driven" behavior from conditioned reflexes. Other theorists with diverse
interests (e.g., Arnheim, 1969; Bruner, Olver, & Greenfield, 1966; Greenwald,
1970; Hebb, 1966, 1968; Holt, 1964; M. J. Horowitz, 1967; G. A. Miller,
Galanter, & Pribram, 1960; Singer, 1966; Tomkins, 1962) have also devoted
considerable attention to the concept without necessarily placing it into the
framework of classical conditioning, which is itself but poorly understood theo-
retically.
At the empirical level, the experimental study of dreaming as introduced by
Aserinsky and Kleitman (1953) has drawn attention to imagery in a manner
that is relevant not only to dream phenomena but also to the problem of non-
verbal mediation of verbal behavior. The reality of imagery and the validity
of the dream report as evidence of imagery are assumed in such research, with
the implication that descriptive verbal behavior (the dream report) is caused
by nonverbal central events (the dream image). Behavioral effects are also
attributed to verbally aroused imagery in Wolpe's (1958) approach to psy-
chotherapy.
The view that images may have functional significance in behavior after all
extends to the phenomena that are most relevant here, namely, meaning and
mediation processes in perception, verbal learning, memory, and language.
Osgood (1953, 1961) has consistently argued in favor of nonverbal mediating
processes in his theory of meaning, where meaning is identified with the con-
ditioned capacity of a sign (e.g., a word) to arouse a fractional component of
the reaction pattern originally evoked by a stimulus object. Staats (1961) has
shown that imagery could be incorporated into the model by interpreting an
image as the fractional component of the original sensory response to an object,
which can become conditioned to a word and represent its denotative meaning.
Essentially the same idea has been proposed independently by Mowrer (1960)
and by Sheffield (1961). Except in their adoption of conditioning as the ex-
planatory mechanism, such a view appears to differ little from the ancient view
that "words are the images of things." That we may indeed have come around
full circle in our theorizing is evidenced further by an increasing recent interest
in research on the possible role of mediating imagery specifically in verbal learn-
ing and memory (see Paivio, 1969). While the trend is unlikely to result in a
return to the "wax tablet" model of memory or the naive view of associative
imagery that was characteristic of the earlier period, it reflects the vitality of
the concept and the compelling nature of the phenomena that led to its revival.
But old fads also have a habit of returning, and if imagery proves to be no
more than that after a hard look, we must be prepared to reject it once more.
The ultimate question to be faced concerns the scientific usefulness of postulating
such a process. This question can be answered only within a framework based
on an empirical foundation and constructed according to a theoretical blue-
8 IMAGERY AND VERBAL PROCESSES

print. Imagery, like all inferential concepts, can have functional significance
only to the extent that it can be differentiated from other concepts theoretically,
and to the extent that these distinctive theoretical properties are open to em-
pirical test. It is essential, therefore, to compare and contrast the concept of
imagery with other concepts that have distinct theoretical properties and at
the same time can be distinguished operationally from imagery. Within the
limitations of present knowledge, this is the approach followed in the present
volume, the aim of which is to compare and contrast the roles of imagery and
of verbal symbolic processes in relation to a variety of psychological phenomena.

AN OVERVIEW OF THE PRESENT APPROACH

The emphasis throughout is on the functional significance of the two


postulated processes, with interest centered on imagery because verbal processes
have received the lion's share of the attention in recent psychological research
and theory. Restricting our conceptual treatment to images and verbal proc-
esses does not imply that these are the only modes of representation and media-
tion. Reference has already been made to Osgood's theory, which assumes that
the mediation process may involve anything from implicit muscular responses
to cortical reactions, and multiple modes of representation have been explicitly
postulated by a number of other theorists whose views are reviewed in Chap-
ter 2. Although relevant problems are considered from time to time, no attempt
will be made to include a systematic treatment of the possible contributions of
emotional and motivational processes to the various phenomena. This omission
may seem regrettable in view of the emphasis given to affective meaning in
Osgood's approach to psycholinguistics, and the importance ascribed to motiva-
tional factors in directed thinking by Berlyne (1965). However, the main con-
cern here is with cognitive processes that directly serve a symbolic, representa-
tional function and other processes or functions will be considered only in
passing.

The theoretical approach Images and verbal processes are viewed as


alternative coding systems, or modes of symbolic representation, which are
developmentally linked to experiences with concrete objects and events as well
as with language. In a given situation, they may be relatively directly aroused
in the sense that an object or an event is represented in memory as a perceptual
image and a word as a perceptual-motor trace, or they may be associatively
aroused in the sense that an object elicits its verbal label (or images of other
objects) and a word arouses implicit verbal associates or images of objects.
In addition, it is assumed that chains of symbolic transformations can occur
involving either words or images, or both, and that these can serve a mediational
function in perception, verbal learning, memory, and language.
The arousal and mediation functions of both processes, but of images par-
Introduction 9

ticularly, are theoretically coordinated to an abstract-concrete dimension of


stimulus meaning or task characteristics. The more concrete or "thing-like" the
stimulus or the task situation, the more likely is it to evoke memory images
that can be functionally useful in mediating appropriate responses in that situa-
tion. Verbal processes presumably are less dependent on concreteness for their
arousal and functioning, hence their relative usefulness accordingly increases
as the task becomes more abstract. Stated differently, both symbolic modes are
readily aroused and can be functionally useful when the situation is relatively
concrete, whereas verbal processes will be differentially favored when the situa-
tion is relatively abstract. Many situations likely involve an interaction of imaginal
and verbal processes, however, and the latter would necessarily be involved at
some stage whenever the stimuli or responses, or both, are verbal, as is the case
with most of the phenomena that are relevant here.
A second postulated distinction is made in terms of the relative efficiency
of the symbolic modes as parallel and sequential processing systems. To over-
simplify for the moment, visual imagery is regarded primarily as a parallel
processing system, specialized for the storage and symbolic manipulation of
information concerning spatially organized objects and events. The verbal sys-
tem, on the other hand, is specialized for sequential processing, as in serial
memory tasks, by virtue of its auditory-motor nature. Finally, the systems are
distinguished in terms of a static-dynamic dimension, with imagery viewed as
the more dynamic process, capable of flexible and swift symbolic transforma-
tions. (These statements are appropriately qualified in Chapter 2.)

The empirical approach The theoretical approach is tied to an em-


pirical one involving three converging operations on the independent variable
side, all conceptually linked by the postulated imaginal and verbal symbolic
processes. These include (1) stimulus characteristics, with particular emphasis
on their abstractness-concreteness and verbal associative meaning; (2) experi-
mental manipulations such as differential task instructions, presentation rates,
and task demands; and (3) individual differences in imaginal and verbal asso-
ciative abilities. All of these are designed to affect the availability or accessibility
of one or other of the symbolic systems in a given task. On the dependent
variable side, physiological reactions as well as overt behavioral effects are
predicted from the independent variables, the predictions frequently including
complex interactions of two or more variables.
Our goal of differentiating the two symbolic processes is an extraordinarily
difficult empirical problem at best, but is especially so when the theoretical goal
is the explanation of verbal behavior, for here it is difficult to rule out verbal
mediation as the most parsimonious interpretation and at the same time isolate
whatever contribution may have been made by imagery. Thus the key issue is
whether or not it is necessary, or at least useful, to postulate both kinds of sym-
bolic processes, nonverbal as well as verbal, to account for effects that have
been observed in a variety of situations.
10 IMAGERY AND VERBAL PROCESSES

Plan of the Book


The book expands on the above approach by defining and elaborating on
the theoretical properties of the postulated symbolic processes and then review-
ing empirical evidence relevant to those properties, their behavioral implications,
and the general usefulness of the model as a theoretical approach to the analysis
of the psychological phenomena in question.
The basic concepts are defined in Chapter 2, which is concerned with the
nature and development of imagery and verbal symbolic processes. Chapter 3
deals with the relation between meaning and the symbolic processes, in antici-
pation of the subsequent chapters in which meaning is one of the predictors of
experimental results. The remaining chapters deal in turn with perception,
memory, associative learning, mediated learning, language, individual differ-
ences in symbolic habits and skills, and, finally, theoretical implications of the
basic approach for such problems as creativity and education.
References
Aaronson, D. Temporal factors in perception and short-term memory. Psychological
Bulletin, 1967, 67, 130–144.
Adams, J. A. Human memory. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1967.
Adams, J. A. , & McIntyre, J. S. Natural language mediation and all-or-none learning.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1967, 21, 436–449.
Adams, J. A. , & Montague, W. E. Retroactive inhibition and natural language mediation.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 528–535.
Adams, J. A. , Thorsheim, H. I. , & McIntyre, J. S. Item length, acoustic similarity, and
natural language mediation as variables in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1969, 80, 39–46.
Allport, D. A. The rate of assimilation of visual information. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12,
231–232.
Allport, F. H. Social psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1924.
Allport, F. H. Theories of perception and the concept of structure. New York: Wiley, 1955.
Allport, G. W. The eidetic image and the after image. American Journal of Psychology,
1928, 40, 418–425.
Allport, G. W. , Vernon, P. E. , & Lindzey, G. Study of values. (2nd ed.) Boston: Houghton
Mifflin, 1951.
Anastasi, A. , & Levee, R. F. Intellectual defect and musical talent: A case report. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1959, 64, 695–703.
Anderson, R. C. & Hidde, J. L. Imagery and sentence learning. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Illinois, 1970.
Angell, J. R. Methods for the determination of mental imagery. Psychological Monographs,
1910, 13 (53), 61–107.
Anisfeld, M. , & Knapp, M. Association, synonymity, and directionality in false recognition.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 171–179.
Antrobus, J. S. , Antrobus, J. S. , & Singer, J. L. Eye movements accompanying
daydreaming, visual imagery, and thought suppression. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1964, 69, 244–252.
Antrobus, J. S. , & Singer, J. L. Visual signal detection as a function of sequential variability
of simultaneous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 603–610.
Antrobus, J. S. , Singer, J. L. , & Greenberg, S. Studies in the stream of consciousness:
Experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 23, 399–417.
Archer, E. J. A re-evaluation of the meaningfulness of all possible CVC trigrams.
Psychological Monographs, 1960, 74 (10, Whole No. 497).
Arnheim, R. Visual thinking. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press,
1969.
Arnold, M. B. Emotion and personality, Volume II: Neurological and physiological aspects.
New York: Columbia University Press, 1960.
Arnoult, M. D. Familiarity and recognition of nonsense shapes. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1956, 51, 269–276.
Asch, S. E. The metaphor: A psychological inquiry. In R. Tagiuri and L. Petrullo (Eds.),
Person perception and interpersonal behavior. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958.
Asch, S. E. , Ceraso, J. , & Heimer, W. Perceptual conditions of association. Psychological
Monographs, 1960, 57 (3).
Asch, S. E. , & Ebenholtz, S. M. The principle of associative symmetry. Proceedings of the
American Philosophical Society, 1962, 106, 135–163.
Asch, S. E. , & Lindner, M. A note on “strength of association.” Journal of Psychology,
1963, 55, 199–209.
Aserinsky, E. , & Kleitman, N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility and concomitant
phenomena during sleep. Science, 1953, 18, 274–284.
Atkinson, R. C. , & Shiffrin, R. M. Human memory: A proposed system and its control
processes. In K. W. Spence and J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and
motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. II. New York: Academic Press, 1968.
Attneave, F. Some informational aspects of visual perception. Psychological Review, 1954,
61, 183–193.
Attneave, F. Physical determinants of the judged complexity of shapes. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1957, 53, 221–227.
Attneave, F. , & Benson, B. Spatial coding of tactual stimulation. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1969, 81, 216–222.
Atwood, G. E. Experimental studies of mnemonic visualization. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Oregon, 1969.
Ausubel, D. P. A cognitive structure view of word and concept meaning. In R. C. Anderson
and D. P. Ausubel (Eds.), Readings in the psychology of cognition. New York: Holt, 1965.
Ayres, J. J. Some artifactual causes of perceptual primacy. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1966, 71, 896–901.
Baddeley, A. D. Semantic and acoustic similarity in short-term memory. Nature, 1964, 204,
1116–1117.
Baddeley, A. D. Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic
and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 18, 362–365. (a)
Baddeley, A. D. The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity of long-term memory for
word sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 18, 302–309. (b)
Baddeley, A. D. , Conrad, R. , & Hull, A. J. Predictability and immediate memory for
consonant sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 17, 175–177.
Baddeley, A. D. , & Dale, H. C. A. The effect of semantic similarity on retroactive
interference in long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1966, 5, 417–420.
Bahrick, H. P. Discriminative and associative aspects of pictorial paired-associate learning:
Acquisition and retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 80, 113–119.
Bahrick, H. P. , & Boucher, B. Retention of visual and verbal codes of the same stimuli.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 417–422.
Bakan, P. , & Svorad, D. Resting EEG alpha and asymmetry of reflective lateral eye
movements. Nature, 1969, 223, 975–976.
Ball, W. W. R. Calculating prodigies. From Mathematical recreations and essays, 1892.
Reprinted in J. R. Newman (Ed.), The world of mathematics, Vol. I. New York: Simon and
Shuster, 1956, 467–487.
Barber, T. X. The afterimages of “hallucinated” and “imagined” colors. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 136–139. (a)
Barber, T. X. The “eidetic image” and “hallucinatory” behavior: A suggestion for further
research. Psychological Bulletin, 1959, 56, 236–239. (b)
Barnes, J. M. , & Underwood, B. J. “Fate” of first-list associations in transfer theory. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 58, 97–105.
Barnes, L. D. , & Schulz, R. W. Meaningfulness and the associative phase of paired-
associate learning. Paper presented at the meetings of the Midwestern Psychological
Association, Chicago, Ill., 1966.
Barratt, P. E. Imagery and thinking. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1953, 5, 154–164.
Barratt, P. E. Use of EEG in the study of imagery. British Journal of Psychology, 1956, 47,
101–114.
Bartlett, F. C. Remembering. Cambridge (England): Cambridge University Press, 1932.
Barton, M. I. , Goodglass, H. , & Shai, A. Differential recognition of tachistoscopically
presented English and Hebrew words in right and left visual fields. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 1965, 21, 431–437.
Bastian, J. Associative factors in verbal transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961,
62, 70–79.
Battig, W. F. Evidence for coding processes in “rote” paired-associate learning. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 177–181.
Battig, W. F. Paired-associate learning. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal
behaviour and general behaviour theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall,
1968.
Battig, W. F. , & Koppenaal, R. J. Associative asymmetry in S-R vs. R-S recall of double-
function lists. Psychological Reports, 1965, 16, 287–293.
Begg, I. Recognition memory for sentence meaning and wording. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, 10, 114–119.
Begg, I. , & Paivio, A. Concreteness and imagery in sentence meaning. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 821–827.
Beighley, K. C. An experimental study of the effect of four speech variables on listener
comprehension. Speech Monographs, 1952, 19, 249–258.
Bennet-Clark, H. C. , & Evans, C. R. Fragmentation of patterned targets when viewed as
prolonged after-images. Nature, 1963, 199, 1215–1216.
Berger, S. M. Persons as mediators: A three-stage paradigm. Psychonomic Science, 1965,
3, 53–54.
Beritoff, J. S. Neural mechanisms of higher vertebrate behavior. (Translated and edited by
W. T. Liberson .) Boston: Little, Brown, and Co., 1965.
Berlyne, D. E. Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley, 1965.
Berlyne, D. E. Mediating responses: A note on Fodor’s criticisms. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 408–411.
Betts, G. H. The distribution and functions of mental imagery. New York: Teachers College,
Columbia University, 1909.
Bevan, W. , Dukes, W. F. , & Avant, L. L. The effect of variation in specific stimuli on
memory for their superordinates. The American Journal of Psychology, 1966, 79, 250–257.
Beveridge, W. I. B. The art of scientific investigation. New York: Random House, 1957.
Binet, A. Psychologie des grand calculateurs et joueurs d’échec. Paris: Hachette, 1894.
Binet, A. (Translated by M. L. Simmel and S. B. Barron .) Mnemonic virtuosity: A study of
chess players. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1966, 74, 127–162.
Birnbaum, I. M. Context stimuli in verbal learning and the persistence of associative factors.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 71, 483–489.
Bloomer, R. H. Concepts of meaning and the reading and spelling difficulty of words.
Journal of Educational Research, 1961, 54, 178–182.
Blumenthal, A. Prompted recall of sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1967, 6, 203–206.
Blumenthal, A. & Boakes, R. Prompted recall of sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 674–676.
Bobrow, S. A. , & Bower, G. H. Comprehension and recall of sentences. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 80, 455–461.
Boersma, F. J. , Conklin, R. C. , & Carlson, J. E. Effects of reporting associative strategies
on the retention of paired-associates. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 463–464.
Bolinger, D. The atomization of meaning. Language, 1965, 41, 555–573.
Boomer, D. Hesitation and grammatical encoding. Language and Speech, 1965, 8,
148–158.
Boring, E. G. A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1950.
Borkowski, J. G. , & Eisner, H. C. Meaningfulness and abstractness in short-term memory.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 76, 57–61.
Borkowski, J. G. , Spreen, O. , & Stutz, J. Z. Ear preference and abstractness in dichotic
listening. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 3, 547–548.
Bourne, L. E., Jr. Human conceptual behavior. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1966.
Bousfield, A. K. , & Bousfield, W. A. Measurement of clustering and of sequential
constancies in repeated free recall. Psychological Reports, 1966, 19, 935–942.
Bousfield, W. A. The occurrence of clustering in recall of randomly arranged associates.
Journal of General Psychology, 1953, 49, 229–240.
Bousfield, W. A. The problem of meaning in verbal behavior. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), Verbal
learning and verbal behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.
Bousfield, W. A. , & Barry, H. The visual imagery of a lightning calculator. American Journal
of Psychology, 1933, 45, 353–358.
Bousfield, W. A. , & Cohen, B. H. The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly
arranged words of different frequencies of usage. Journal of General Psychology, 1955, 52,
83–95.
Bousfield, W. A. , Esterson, J. , & Whitmarsh, G. A. The effects of concomitant colored and
uncolored pictorial representations on the learning of stimulus words. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 1957, 41, 165–168.
Bousfield, W. A. , Puff, C. R. , & Cowan, T. M. , The development of constancies in
sequential organization during repeated free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1964, 3, 489–495.
Bousfield, W. A. , & Samborski, G. The relationship between strength of values and the
meaningfulness of value words. Journal of Personality, 1955, 23, 375–380.
Bousfield, W. A. , Steward, J. R. , & Cowan, T. M. The use of free associational norms for
the prediction of clustering. Journal of General Psychology, 1964, 70, 205–214.
Bower, G. H. A multicomponent theory of the memory trace. In K. W. Spence & J. T.
Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. I. New York: Academic
Press, 1967.
Bower, G. H. Mental imagery and associative learning. Fifth annual symposium on
cognition. Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Penn., 1969. To be published in Lee
Gregg (Ed.), Cognition in learning and memory, New York: Wiley.
Bower, G. H. Imagery as a relational organizer in associative learning. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 529–533. (a)
Bower, G. H. Mnemonics. Symposium address, American Educational Research
Association meeting, Minneapolis, March, 1970. (b)
Bower, G. H. , & Bolton, L. S. Why are rhymes easy to learn? Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1969, 82, 453–461.
Bower, G. H. , & Clark, M. C. Narrative stories as mediators for serial learning.
Psychonomic Science, 1969, 14, 181–182.
Bower, G. H. , Clark, M. C. , Lesgold, A. M. , & Winzenz, D. Hierarchical retrieval schemes
in recall of categorized word lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8,
323–343.
Bower, G. H. , Lesgold, A. M. , & Tieman, D. Grouping operations in free recall. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 481–493.
Bower, G. H. , & Winzenz, D. Comparison of associative learning strategies. Psychonomic
Science, 1970, 20, 119–120.
Bowers, H. Memory and mental imagery. An experimental study. British Journal of
Psychology, 1931, 21, 271–282.
Bowers, H. Visual imagery and retention. British Journal of Psychology, 1932, 23, 180–195.
Bowra, C. M. Greek lyric poetry. London: Oxford University Press, 1961.
Braine, L. G. Asymmetries of pattern perception observed in Israelis. Neuropsychologia,
1968, 6, 73–88.
Braine, M. D. S. On learning the grammatical order of words. Psychological Review, 1963,
70, 323–348.
Bregman, A. S. , & Strasberg, R. Memory for the syntactic form of sentences. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 396–403.
Brener, L. R. An experimental investigation of memory span. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1940, 26, 467–482.
Brent, S. B. Linguistic unity, list length, and rate of presentation in serial anticipation
learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 70–79.
Broadbent, D. E. A mechanical model for human attention and immediate memory.
Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 205–215.
Brodey, W. The other-than-visual world of the blind. Ekistics, 1969, 28, 100–103.
Brogden, W. J. Sensory pre-conditioning of human subjects. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1947, 37, 527–539.
Brooks, L. R. The suppression of visualization in reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 19, 289–299.
Brooks, L. R. Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1968, 22, 349–368.
Brosgole, L. , & Whalen, P. M. The effect of meaning on the allocation of visually induced
movement. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 275–277.
Brower, D. The experimental study of imagery. I. The relation of imagery to intelligence.
Journal of General Psychology, 1947, 36, 229–231.
Brown, B. B. Specificity of EEG photic flicker responses to color as related to visual imagery
ability. Psychophysiology, 1966, 2, 197–207.
Brown, J. Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1958, 10, 12–21.
Brown, R. W. Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1957, 55, 1–5.
Brown, R. W. Words and things. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press, 1958.
Brown, R. W. , & Berko, J. Word association and the acquisition of grammar. Child
Development, 1960, 31, 1–14.
Brown, R. W. , & Lenneberg, E. H. A study in language and cognition. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 454–462.
Brown, S. W. , Guilford, J. P. , & Hoepfner, R. Six semantic-memory abilities. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 1968, 28, 691–718.
Brown, W. P. , & Ure, D. M. J. Five rated characteristics of 650 word association stimuli.
British Journal of Psychology, 1969, 60, 233–249.
Bruner, J. S. Neural mechanisms in perception. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 340–358.
Bruner, J. S. The course of cognitive growth. American Psychologist, 1964, 19, 1–15.
Bruner, J. S. , Olver, R. R. , & Greenfield, P. M. Studies in cognitive growth. New York:
Wiley, 1966.
Bruner, J. S. , & Postman, L. On the perception of incongruity: A paradigm. Journal of
Personality, 1949, 18, 206–223.
Bruner, J. S. , & Potter, M. C. Interference in visual recognition. Science, 1964, 144,
424–425.
Bryden, M. P. Tachistoscopic recognition of non-alphabetic material. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1960, 14, 78–86.
Bryden, M. P. The role of post-exposural eye movements in tachistoscopic perception.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 15, 220–225.
Bryden, M. P. Left-right differences in tachistoscopic recognition: Directional scanning or
cerebral dominance? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 23, 1127–1134.
Bryden, M. P. A model for the sequential organization of behaviour. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1967, 21, 37–56.
Bryden, M. P. , Dick, A. O. , & Mewhort, D. J. K. Tachistoscopic recognition of number
sequences. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 52–59.
Bryden, M. P. , & Rainey, C. A. Left-right differences in tachistoscopic recognition. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 568–571.
Bugelski, B. R. An introduction to the principles of psychology. New York: Rinehart, 1960.
Bugelski, B. R. Presentation time, total time, and mediation in paired-associate learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 63, 409–412.
Bugelski, B. R. Images as mediators in one-trial paired-associate learning. II: Self-timing in
successive lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 328–334.
Bugelski, B. R. Learning theory and the reading process. In The 23rd annual reading
conference. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1969, in press.
Bugelski, B. R. Words and things and images. American Psychologist, 1970, 25,
1002–1012.
Bugelski, B. R. , Kidd, E. , & Segmen, J. Image as a mediator in one-trial paired-associate
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 76, 69–73.
Bugelski, B. R. , & Scharlock, D. P. An experimental demonstration of unconscious
mediated generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1952, 44, 334–338.
Butter, M. J. Differential recall of paired associates as a function of arousal and
concreteness-imagery levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 84, 252–256.
Calkins, M. W. Short studies in memory and in association from the Wellesley College
psychological laboratory. Psychological Review, 1898, 5, 451–462.
Canestrari, R. E., Jr. Age changes in acquisition. Human aging and behavior, New York:
Academic Press, 1968, 169–188.
Carey, N. Factors in the mental processes of school children. I. Visual and auditory
imagery. British Journal of Psychology, 1915, 7, 452–490.
Carmichael, L. , Hogan, H. P. , & Walter, A. A. An experimental study of the effect of
language on the reproduction of visually perceived form. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1932, 15, 73–86.
Carnegie, D. Public speaking and influencing men in business. New York: Association
Press, 1937.
Cartwright, D. Lewinian theory as a contemporary systematic framework. In S. Koch (Ed.),
Psychology: A study of a science, Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
Chomsky, N. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton, 1957.
Chomsky, N. Review of Verbal behaviour by B. F. Skinner. Language, 1959, 35, 26–58.
Chomsky, N. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1965.
Chomsky, N. Cartesian linguistics. New York: Harper & Row, 1966.
Chomsky, N. Language and the mind. Psychology Today, 1968, 1 (9), 48–51; 66–68.
Christal, R. E. Factor analytic study of visual memory. Psychological Monographs, 1958,
72, (13, Whole No. 466).
Christensen, P. R. , Guilford, J. P. , Merrifield, P. R. , & Wilson, R. C. Alternate uses, Form
A. Sheridan Supply Company, Beverly Hills, California, 1960.
Christiansen, T. Sex and level of mediator abstractness as variables in mediate association.
Psychonomic Science, 1969, 14, 160–162.
Christiansen, T. , & Stone, D. R. Visual imagery and level of mediator abstractness in
induced mediation paradigms. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1968, 26, 775–779.
Clark, H. H. Some structural properties of simple active and passive sentences. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 365–370.
Clark, H. H. Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 1969, 76,
387–403.
Clark, H. J. Recognition memory for random shapes as a function of complexity,
association value, and delay. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 590–595.
Clark, H. J. Random shape recognition at brief exposure durations. Psychonomic Science,
1968, 12, 245–246.
Clark, L. L. , Lansford, T. G. , & Dallenbach, K. M. Repetition and associative learning.
American Journal of Psychology, 1960, 73, 22–40.
Clarke, F. J. J. , & Evans, C. R. Comments on reports: Luminous-design phenomena.
Science, 1964, 144, 1359–1360.
Cofer, C. N. On some factors in the organizational characteristics of free recall. American
Psychologist, 1965, 20, 261–272.
Cofer, C. N. Conditions for the use of verbal associations. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 68
(1), 1–12.
Cofer, C. N. , Diamond, F. , Olsen, R. A. , Stein, J. S. , & Walker, H. Comparison of
anticipation and recall methods in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 75, 545–558.
Cohen, B. H. An investigation of recoding in free recall. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1963, 65, 368–376.
Cohen, R. L. & Granström, K. Reproduction and recognition in short-term visual memory.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 22, 450–457.
Cole, M. , Frankel, F. , & Sharp, D. The development of free recall learning in children.
Unpublished paper, Department of Psychology, University of California, Irvine, 1969.
Coleman, E. B. Approximations to English: Some comments on the method. American
Journal of Psychology, 1963, 76, 239–247.
Coleman, E. B. Responses to a scale of grammaticalness. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 521–527.
Colman, F. , & Paivio, A. Pupillary dilation and mediation processes during paired-associate
learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1970, 24, 261–270.
Conrad, R. Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 1964,
55, 75–84.
Conrad, R. , Freeman, P. R. , & Hull, A. J. Acoustic factors versus language factors in
short-term memory. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 3, 57–58.
Corsini, D. A. , Jacobus, K. A. , & Leonard, S. D. Recognition memory of preschool children
for pictures and words. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 16, 192–193.
Craik, F. I. M. The fate of primary memory items in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 143–148.
Creelman, M. B. The experimental investigation of meaning. New York: Springer, 1966.
Crovitz, H. F. Galton’s Walk. New York: Harper and Row, 1970.
Crovitz, H. F. , & Daves, W. Tendency to eye movement and perceptual accuracy. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 63, 495–498.
Crowder, R. G. Evidence for the chaining hypothesis of serial verbal learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 76, 497–500.
Crowder, R. G. , & Morton, J. Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS). Perception and
Psychophysics, 1969, 5, 365–373.
Csapo, K. The effects of presentation rate and item attribute in paired-associate learning.
Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1968.
Dainoff, M. , & Haber, R. N. How much help do repeated presentations give to recognition
processes? Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 131–136.
Dallett, K. M. Implicit mediators in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 209–214.
Darley, F. L. , Sherman, D. , & Siegel, G. M. Scaling of abstraction level of single words.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1959, 2, 161–167.
Daves, W. F. , & Adkins, M. Stimulus variation and free recall: A confirmation. American
Journal of Psychology, 1969, 82, 122–124.
Davidson, R. E. Mediation and ability in paired-associate learning. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 1964, 55, 352–356.
Davidson, R. E. , & Adams, J. F. Verbal and imagery processes in children’s paired-
associate learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, in press.
Davidson, R. E. , Schwenn, E. A. , & Adams, J. F. Semantic effects in transfer. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 212–217.
Davis, F. C. The functional significance of imagery differences. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1932, 15, 630–661.
Deckert, G. H. Pursuit eye movements in the absence of a moving visual stimulus. Science,
1964, 143, 1192–1193.
Deese, J. Influence of inter-item associative strength upon immediate free recall.
Psychological Reports, 1959, 5, 305–312.
Deese, J. Frequency of usage and number of words in free recall: The role of association.
Psychological Reports, 1960, 7, 337–344.
Deese, J. From the isolated verbal unit to connected discourse. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), Verbal
learning and verbal behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.
Deese, J. The structure of associations in language and thought. Baltimore: The Johns
Hopkins Press, 1965.
Deese, J. Association and memory. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior
and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Deese, J. , & Hulse, S. H. The psychology of learning. (3rd ed.) New York: McGraw-Hill,
1967.
Delin, P. S. Success in recall as a function of success in implementation of mnemonic
instructions. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12, 153–154.
Delin, P. S. The learning to criterion of a serial list with and without mnemonic instructions.
Psychonomic Science, 1969, 16, 169–170.
Dember, W. N. Psychology of perception. New York: Holt, 1960.
Dement, W. C. An essay on dreams: The role of physiology in understanding their nature.
In New directions in psychology II. New York: Holt, 1965.
DeSoto, C. , London, M. , & Handel, S. Social reasoning and spatial paralogic. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 513–521.
Dick, A. O. , & Mewhort, J. K. Order of report and processing in tachistoscopic recognition.
Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 573–576.
Dilley, M. G. , & Paivio, A. Pictures and words as stimulus and response items in paired-
associate learning of young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1968, 6,
231–240.
DiVesta, F. J. , & Walls, R. T. Factor analysis of the semantic attributes of 487 words and
some relationships to the conceptual behavior of fifth-grade children. Journal of Educational
Psychology Monograph, 1970, 61 (6, Pt. 2).
Dixon, T. R. , & Horton, D. L. (Eds.) Verbal behavior and general behavior theory.
Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Dixon, T. R. , & Moulton, A. E. Effects of questioning unaware problem solvers in a “verbal
conditioning” task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 83, 431–434.
Dominowski, R. L. , & Gadlin, H. Imagery and paired-associate learning. Canadian Journal
of Psychology, 1968, 22, 336–348.
Donderi, D. C. , & Kane, E. Perceptual learning produced by common responses to
different stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 15–30.
Dong, T. , & Kintsch, W. Subjective retrieval cues in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 813–816.
Doob, L. W. Eidetic images among the Ibo. Ethnology, 1964, 3, 357–363.
Doob, L. W. Exploring eidetic imagery among the Kamba of Central Kenya. Journal of
Social Psychology, 1965, 67, 3–22.
Doob, L. W. Eidetic imagery: A cross-cultural will-o’-the-wisp? Journal of Psychology, 1966,
63, 13–34.
Dooling, D. J. Effects of surrogate processes and syntactic constraint on the retention of
connected discourse. Paper read at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association,
Washington, 1968.
Dornbush, R. L. Shadowing in bisensory memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1968, 20, 225–231.
Drever, J. Some observations on the occipital alpha rhythm. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1955, 7, 91–97.
Ducharme, R. , & Fraisse, P. Etude génétique de la mémorisation de mots et d’images.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 253–261.
Dukes, W. F. , & Bastian, J. Recall of abstract and concrete words equated on
meaningfulness. Journal of Verbal Learnings and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 455–458.
Dunn, F. Poetic space. Scottish International, January, 1968, 56–58.
Dunn, S. , Bliss, J. , & Siipola, E. Effects of impulsivity, introversion and individual values
upon association under free conditions. Journal of Personality, 1958, 26, 61–76.
Dunne, M. M. The effect of syntactic structure on learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 458–463.
Eagle, M. , Wolitzky, D. L. , & Klein, G. S. Imagery: Effect of a concealed figure in a
stimulus. Science, 1966, 151, 837–839.
Earhard, B. , & Earhard, M. Mediation and pseudomediation: A reply to Horton. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 308–322.
Earhard, B. , & Mandler, G. Mediated associations: Paradigms, controls, and mechanisms.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 346–378.
Eastman, R. The relative crossmodal transfer of a form discrimination. Psychonomic
Science, 1967, 9, 197–198.
Ebbinghaus, H. Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Dover,
1964. (Originally published: Leipzig: Duncker & Humblot, 1885.)
Ebenholtz, S. M. Serial learning: Position learning and sequential associations. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 353–362.
Edmonds, E. M. , & Evans, S. H. Schema learning without a prototype. Psychonomic
Science, 1966, 5, 247–248.
Eiles, R. R. Effects of mediational and motivational instructions on forward and backward
recall of concrete paired-associate nouns. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State
University, 1970.
Ekstrand, B. R. Backward associations. Psychological Bulletin, 1966, 65, 50–64.
Ekstrand, B. R. , Wallace, W. P. , & Underwood, B. J. A frequency theory of verbal-
discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 556–578.
Eliava, N. L. A contribution to the problem of the role of set and attitude in perceptual
processes. Voprosy psikhologii, 1961, No. 1. English translation: Soviet psychology and
psychiatry, 1962, 1, 16–20.
Ellis, H. C. , & Muller, D. G. Transfer in perceptual learning following stimulus
predifferentiation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 388–395.
Ellis, H. C. , Muller, D. G. , & Tosti, D. T. Stimulus meaning and complexity as factors in the
transfer of stimulus predifferentiation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 71,
629–633.
Ellson, D. G. Hallucinations produced by sensory conditioning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1941, 28, 1–20. (a)
Ellson, D. G. Experimental extinction of an hallucination produced by sensory conditioning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1941, 28, 350–361. (b)
Epstein, W. The influence of syntactical structure on learning. American Journal of
Psychology, 1961, 74, 80–85.
Epstein, W. A further study of the influence of syntactical structure on learning. American
Journal of Psychology, 1962, 75, 121–126. (a)
Epstein, W. Backward association as a function of meaningfulness. Journal of General
Psychology, 1962, 67, 11–20. (b)
Epstein, W. Temporal schemata in syntactically structured material. Journal of General
Psychology, 1963, 68, 157–164.
Epstein, W. Varieties of perceptual learning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1967.
Epstein, W. , Rock, I. , & Zuckerman, C. B. Meaning and familiarity in associative learning.
Psychological Monographs, 1960, 74, (4, Whole No. 491).
Epstein, W. , & Streib, R. The effect of stimulus meaningfulness and response
meaningfulness in the absence of response learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1962, 1, 105–108.
Erickson, R. L. Differential effects of stimulus and response isolation in paired-associate
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 317–323.
Eriksen, C. W. Unconscious processes. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on
motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1958.
Eriksen, C. W. Temporal luminance summation effects in forward and backward masking.
Perception and Psychophysics, 1966, 1, 87–92.
Ernest, C. H. , & Paivio, A. Imagery ability in paired-associate and incidental learning.
Psychonomic Science, 1969, 15, 181–182.
Ernest, C. H. , & Paivio, A. Imagery and sex differences in incidental recall. British Journal
of Psychology, 1971, 62, 67–72.
Ervin, S. M. Changes with age in the verbal determinants of word association. American
Journal of Psychology, 1961, 74, 361–372.
Esper, E. A. Mentalism and objectivism in linguistics. New York: American Elsevier, 1968.
Evans, C. R. Further studies of pattern perception and a stabilized retinal image: The use of
prolonged after-images to achieve perfect stabilization. British Journal of Psychology, 1967,
58, 315–327.
Feinaigle, G. von . The new art of memory. (3rd ed.) London: Sherwood, Neely and Jones,
1813.
Feldmar, A. Syntactic structure and speech decoding: The judgment of sequence in
auditory events. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1969.
Ferguson, G. A. On learning and human ability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1954, 8,
95–112.
Ferguson, G. A. On transfer and the abilities of man. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
1956, 10, 121–131.
Fernald, M. R. The diagnosis of mental imagery. Psychological Monographs, 1912, No. 58.
Feuge, R. L. , & Ellis, H. C. Generalization gradients in recognition memory of visual form:
The role of stimulus meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 288–294.
Fillenbaum, S. Memory for gist: Some relevant variables. Language and Speech, 1966, 9,
217–227.
Fiss, H. , Goldberg, F. H. , & Klein, G. S. Effects of subliminal stimulation on imagery and
discrimination. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1963, 17, 31–44.
Flavell, J. H. , Beach, D. R. , & Chinsky, J. M. Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory
task as a function of age. Child Development, 1966, 37, 283–299.
Fleishman, E. A. Human abilities and the acquisition of skill. In E. A. Bilodeau (Ed.),
Acquisition of skill. New York: Academic Press, 1966. (a)
Fleishman, E. A. Human abilities and verbal learning. In R. Gagné (Ed.), Learning and
individual differences. Columbus: Merrill, 1966. (b)
Fleishman, E. A. , & Rich, S. Role of kinesthetic and spatial-visual abilities in perceptual-
motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 6–11.
Flesch, R. Measuring the level of abstraction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1950, 34,
384–390.
Fodor, J. A. Could meaning be an rm? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1965, 4, 73–81. (a)
Fodor, J. A. A review of Language and thought, by J. B. Carroll. The Modern Language
Journal, 1965, 49 (6), 384–386. (b)
Fodor, J. A. , & Bever, T. G. The psychological reality of linguistic segments. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 414–420.
Forgays, D. G. The development of a differential word recognition. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1953, 45, 165–168.
Forrester, W. E. , & Spear, N. E. Coding processes in verbal learning as a function of
response pronounciability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 74, 586–588.
Forster, K. I. Left-to-right processes in the construction of sentences. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 285–291.
Fozard, J. L. Apparent recency of unrelated pictures and nouns presented in the same
sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 86, 137–143.
Fozard, J. L. , & Lapine, R. Comparison of discrimination of recency of pictures and names
of common objects. Paper presented at Eastern Psychological Association meetings,
Washington, D. C., 1968.
Fracker, G. C. On the transference of training in memory. Psychological Monographs, 1908,
9 (38), 56–102.
Fraisse, P. Recognition time measured by verbal reaction to figures and words. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 1960, 11, 204.
Fraisse, P. Le temps de réaction verbale. Année Psychologique, 1964, 64, 21–46.
Fraisse, P. Motor and verbal reaction times to words and drawings. Psychonomic Science,
1968, 12, 235–236.
Fraser, C. , Bellugi, U. , & Brown, R. Control of grammar in imitation, comprehension, and
production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 121–135.
Frederiksen, J. R. Cognitive factors in the recognition of ambiguous auditory and visual
stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 7, Monograph No. 639.
French, J. , Ekstrom, R. , & Price, L. Kit of reference tests for cognitive factors. Princeton,
N.J.: Educational Testing Service, 1963.
Frincke, G. Word characteristics, associative-relatedness, and the free-recall of nouns.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 366–372.
Furst, B. Stop forgetting. New York: Garden City, 1948.
Furst, B. The practical way to a better memory. New York: Fawcett World Library, 1957.
Furth, H. G. Thinking without language. New York: Free Press, 1966.
Gagné, R. M. (Ed.). Learning and individual differences. Columbus, Ohio: Merrill, 1967.
Galton, F. Inquiries into human faculty and its development. (1st ed.) London: MacMillan,
1883. (2nd ed.) London: J. M. Dent & Sons, 1907.
Garrett, M. , & Fodor, J. A. Psychological theories and linguistic constructs. In T. R. Dixon
and D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs,
New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Gavurin, E. I. The relationship between spatial aptitude and verbal problem solving. Paper
read at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York, 1966.
Gibson, E. J. A systematic application of the concepts of generalization and differentiation
to verbal learning. Psychological Review, 1940, 47, 196–229.
Gibson, E. J. Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts, 1969.
Gibson, E. J. , Bishop, C. H. , Schiff, W. , & Smith, J. Comparison of meaningfulness and
pronunciability as grouping principles in the perception and retention of verbal material.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 67, 173–182.
Gibson, J. J. Observations on active touch. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 477–491.
Gibson, J. J. The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin,
1966.
Glanzer, M. Grammatical category: A rote learning and word association analysis. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1962, 1, 31–41.
Glanzer, M. , & Clark, W. H. Accuracy of perceptual recall: An analysis of organization.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 1, 289–299. (a)
Glanzer, M. , & Clark, W. H. The verbal loop hypothesis: Binary numbers. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 301–309. (b)
Glanzer, M. , & Clark, W. H. The verbal-loop hypothesis: Conventional figures. American
Journal of Psychology, 1964, 77, 621–626.
Glanzer, M. , & Cunitz, A. R. Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 351–360.
Glanzer, M. , & Fleishman, J. The effect of encoding training on perceptual recall.
Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 561–564.
Glaze, J. A. The association value of nonsense syllables. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
1928, 35, 255–267.
Glucksberg, S. , & Danks, J. H. Grammatical structure and recall: A function of the space in
immediate memory or of recall delay? Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 6, 113–117.
Goldman-Eisler, F. Speech production and the predictability of words in context. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 10, 96–106.
Goldman-Eisler, F. Hesitation and information in speech. In C. Cherry (Ed.), Information
theory. London: Butterworths, 1961.
Goldstein, K. , & Scheerer, M. Abstract and concrete behavior: An experimental study with
special tests. Psychological Monographs, 1941, 53, No. 2.
Golla, F. L. , & Antonovitch, S. The respiratory rhythm in its relation to the mechanism of
thought. Brain, 1929, 52, 491–510.
Golla, F. L. , Hutton, E. L. , & Walter, W. G. The objective study of mental imagery. Journal
of Mental Science, 1943, 89, 216–223.
Gomulicki, B. R. The development and the present status of the trace theory of memory.
British Journal of Psychology Monograph Supplement, 1953, No. 29.
Goodglass, H. , & Barton, M. Handedness and differential perception of verbal stimuli in left
and right visual fields. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1963, 17, 851–854.
Gorman, A. M. Recognition memory for nouns as a function of abstractness and frequency.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961, 61, 23–29.
Goss, A. E. Early behaviorism and verbal mediating responses. American Psychologist,
1961, 16, 285–298.
Goss, A. E. , & Nodine, C. F. Paired-associate learning: The role of meaningfulness,
similarity, and familiarization. New York: Academic Press, 1965.
Goss, A. E. , Nodine, C. F. , Gregory, B. N. , Taub, H. A. , & Kennedy, K. E. Stimulus
characteristics and percentage of occurrence of response members in paired-associates
learning. Psychological Monographs, 1962, 76 (12, Whole No. 531).
Gough, P. B. Grammatical transformations and speed of understanding. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 107–111.
Gough, P. B. The verification of sentences: The effects of delay of evidence and sentence
length. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 492–496.
Goulet, L. R. Retroaction and the “fate” of the mediator in three stage mediation paradigms.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 172–176.
Goulet, L. R. Verbal learning in children: Implications for developmental research.
Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 69, 359–376.
Graham, K. R. Eye movements during visual mental imagery. Paper presented to the
Eastern Psychological Association, Atlantic City, April, 1970.
Greeno, J. G. Verbal fluency, free recall and paired-associate learning speed.
Psychological Reports, 1965, 16, 659–660.
Greeno, J. G. Associative effects vs. differentiation: Comment on Saltz and Wickey’s reply
to Birnbaum. Psychological Reports, 1968, 22, 347–350.
Greenwald, A. G. Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special
reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 73–99.
Griffitts, C. W. Individual differences in imagery. Psychological Monographs, 1927, No. 172.
Groninger, L. D. Natural language mediation and covert rehearsal in short-term memory.
Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 135–136.
Groninger, L. D. Mnemonic imagery and forgetting. Psychonomic Science, 1971, in press.
Grover, D. E. , Horton, D. L. , & Cunningham, M. Mediated facilitation and interference in a
four-stage paradigm. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 42–46.
Gruber, H. E. , Kulkin, A. , & Schwartz, P. The effect of exposure time on mnemonic
processing in paired associate learning. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological
Association meeting, Atlantic City, 1965.
Guilford, J. P. An inventory of factors STDCR. Beverly Hills, California: Sheridan Supply
Co., 1940.
Guilford, J. P. The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1967.
Guilford, J. P. , & Zimmerman, W. S. The Guilford-Zimmerman aptitude survey. Beverly
Hills, California: Sheridan Psychological Services, 1948.
Gupton, T. , & Frincke, G. Imagery, mediational instructions, and noun position in free recall
of noun-verb pairs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 86, 461–462.
Haber, R. N. A replication of selective attention and coding in visual perception. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1964, 67, 402–404. (a)
Haber, R. N. Effects of coding strategy on perceptual memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1964, 68, 357–362. (b)
Haber, R. N. The effect of prior knowledge of the stimulus on word recognition processes.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 282–286.
Haber, R. N. Nature of the effect of set on perception. Psychological Review, 1966, 73,
335–351.
Haber, R. N. Repetition as a determinant of perceptual recognition processes. In W.
Watken-Dunn (Ed.), Symposium on models for the perception of speech and visual form.
Cambridge: M.I.T. Press, 1967, 202–212.
Haber, R. N. Temporal integration of suprathreshold perceptual processes. Paper
presented at the American Psychological Association meeting, San Francisco, 1968.
Haber, R. N. , & Haber, R. B. Eidetic imagery: I. Frequency. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
1964, 19, 131–138.
Haber, R. N. , & Nathanson, L. S. Processing of sequentially presented letters. Perception
and Psychophysics, 1969, 5, 359–361.
Haber, R. N. , & Standing, L. G. Direct measures of short-term visual storage. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 21, 43–54.
Hake, H. W. , & Eriksen, C. W. Role of response variables in recognition and identification
of complex visual forms. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1956, 52, 235–243.
Hall, J. F. Learning as a function of word-frequency. American Journal of Psychology, 1954,
67, 138–140.
Hall, J. F. The psychology of learning. New York: J. B. Lippincott, 1966.
Hall, J. F. , & Ugelow, A. Free association time as a function of word frequency. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1957, 11, 29–32.
Handel, S. , & Garner, W. R. The structure of visual pattern associates and pattern
goodness. Perception and Psychophysics, 1966, 1, 33–88.
Harcum, E. R. Visual hemifield differences as conflicts in direction of reading. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1966, 72, 479–480.
Harcum, E. R. , & Dyer, D. W. Monocular and binocular reproduction of binary stimuli
appearing right and left of fixation. American Journal of Psychology, 1962, 75, 56–65.
Harcum, E. R. , & Finkel, M. E. Explanation of Mishkin and Forgay’s results as a directional-
reading conflict. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1963, 17, 224–234.
Harcum, E. R. , & Friedman, S. M. Reversal reading by Israeli observers of visual patterns
without intrinsic directionality. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1963, 17, 361–369.
Hare, R. D. Cognitive factors in transfer of meaning. Psychological Reports, 1964, 15,
199–206.
Harrigan, J. E. , & Modrick, J. A. S-R and R-S acquisition as a function of meaningfulness.
Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9, 199–200.
Harris, C. S. , & Haber, R. N. Selective attention and coding in visual perception. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 65, 328–333.
Hart, J. T. Luminous figures: Influence of point of fixation on their disappearances. Science,
1964, 143, 1193–1194.
Havelka, J. The nature of the creative process in art. The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus
Nijhoff, 1968.
Hayakawa, S. I. Language in thought and action. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1949.
Hebb, D. O. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1949.
Hebb, D. O. The semiautonomous process: Its nature and nurture. American Psychologist,
1963, 18, 16–27.
Hebb, D. O. A textbook of psychology. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1966.
Hebb, D. O. Concerning imagery. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 466–477.
Hebb, D. O. , Lambert, W. E. , & Tucker, G. R. Language, thought and experience.
Unpublished manuscript, McGill University, 1970.
Hebb, D. O. , & Thompson, W. R. The social significance of animal studies. In G. Lindzey
(Ed.), Handbook of social psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1954, 1,
532–561.
Hefferline, R. F. , & Perera, T. B. Proprioceptive discrimination of a covert operant without
its observation by the subject. Science, 1963, 139, 834–835.
Heidbreder, E. The attainment of concepts: I. Terminology and methodology. Journal of
General Psychology, 1946, 35, 173–189.
Herman, T. , Broussard, I. G. , & Todd, H. R. Intertrial interval and the rate of learning serial
order picture stimuli. Journal of General Psychology, 1951, 45, 245–254.
Heron, W. Perception as a function of retinal locus and attention. American Journal of
Psychology, 1957, 70, 38–48.
Herring, B. S. , & Bryden, M. P. Memory colour effects as a function of viewing time.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1970, 24, 127–132.
Hershenson, M. Stimulus structure, cognitive structure, and the perception of letter arrays.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 327–335.
Hershenson, M. , & Haber, R. N. The role of meaning in the perception of briefly presented
words. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 42–46.
Hintzman, D. L. Classification and aural coding in short-term memory. Psychonomic
Science, 1965, 3, 161–162.
Hochberg, J. E. Perception. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1964.
Hochberg, J. E. In the mind’s eye. In R. N. Haber (Ed.), Contemporary theory and research
in visual perception. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1968.
Hochberg, J. , & Galper, R. E. Recognition of faces: I. An exploratory study. Psychonomic
Science, 1967, 9, 619–620.
Hockett, C. D. The origin of speech. Scientific American, 1960, 203 (3), 88–96.
Holt, R. R. Imagery: The return of the ostracized. American Psychologist, 1964, 19,
254–264.
Horowitz, L. M. , Brown, Z. M. , & Weissbluth, S. Availability and the direction of
associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 541–549.
Horowitz, L. M. , Lampel, A. K. , & Takanishi, R. N. The child’s memory for unitized scenes.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1969, 8, 375–388.
Horowitz, L. M. , Norman, S. A. , & Day, R. S. Availability and associative symmetry.
Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 1–15.
Horowitz, L. M. , & Prytulak, L. S. Redintegrative memory. Psychological Review, 1969, 76,
519–531.
Horowitz, M. J. Visual imagery and cognitive organization. American Journal of Psychiatry,
1967, 123, 938–946.
Horton, D. L. The effects of meaningfulness, awareness, and type of design in verbal
mediation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 187–194.
Horton, D. L. , & Kjeldergaard, P. M. An experimental analysis of associative factors in
mediated generalizations. Psychological Monographs, 1961, 75 (11, Whole No. 515).
Horton, D. L. , Grover, D. E. , & Wiley, R. E. Mediation, recall and instructional effects.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 539–542.
Houston, J. P. Ease of verbal S-R learning as a function of the number of mediating
associations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 326–329.
Howe, M. J. A. Digram-frequency and immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology,
1967, 58, 57–62.
Huey, E. B. The psychology and pedagogy of reading. New York: MacMillan, 1908.
(Reprinted, Cambridge, Mass. by M.I.T. Press, 1968).
Hulicka, I. M. , & Grossman, J. L. Age-group comparisons for the use of mediators in
paired-associate learning. Journal of Gerontology, 1967, 21, 46–51.
Hull, C. L. Goal attraction and directing ideas conceived as habit phenomena.
Psychological Review, 1931, 38, 487–506.
Hull, C. L. The meaningfulness of 320 selected nonsense syllables. American Journal of
Psychology, 1933, 45, 730–734.
Hull, C. L. The problem of stimulus equivalence in behavior theory. Psychological Review,
1939, 46, 9–30.
Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.
Hulme, T. E. Further speculations ( S. Hynes , Ed.). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press, 1955.
Humphrey, G. Thinking. London: Methuen, 1951.
Hunter, I. M. L. Memory. Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1964.
Hunter, W. S. The delayed reaction in animals and children. Behavior Science Monographs,
1913, 2, No. 6.
Huttenlocher, J. Constructing spatial images: A strategy in reasoning. Psychological
Review, 1968, 75, 550–560.
Huttenlocher, J. , Eisenberg, K. , & Strauss, S. Comprehension: Relation between
perceived actor and logical subject. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968,
7, 527–530.
Huttenlocher, J. , Higgins, E. T. , Milligan, C. , & Kaufman, B. The mystery of the “negative
equative” construction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 334–341.
Hyman, J. A. Performance of high and low imagers on two complex tactual discrimination
tasks following imagery and verbal pretraining. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of
Western Ontario, 1966.
Imae, K. Wordness and abstractness of the so-called nonsense verbal materials. Japanese
Journal of Psychology, 1968, 39, 200–211.
Imai, S. , & Garner, W. R. Structure in perceptual classification. Psychonomic Monograph
Supplements, 1968, 2, 153–172.
Irion, A. L. Rote learning. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science. New York:
McGraw-Hill, 1959.
Iscoe, I. , & Semler, I. J. Paired-associate learning in normal and mentally retarded children
as a function of four experimental conditions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1964, 57, 387–392.
Jaensch, E. R. Eidetic imagery and typological methods of investigation. New York:
Harcourt Brace, 1930.
Jakobovits, L. A. Mediation theory and the single-stage S-R model: Different? Psychological
Review, 1966, 73, 376–381.
Jakobovits, L. A. , & Lambert, W. E. Stimulus-characteristics as determinants of semantic
changes with repeated presentation. American Journal of Psychology, 1964, 77, 84–92.
Jakobson, R. , & Halle, M. Fundamentals of language. Pt. 2, Two aspects of language and
two types of aphasie disorders.’s Gravenhage: Mouton, 1956.
James, C. T. , & Hakes, D. T. Mediated transfer in a four-stage stimulus equivalence
paradigm. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 89–93.
James, W. Principles of psychology. New York: Holt, 1890. (Reprinted by Dover, 1950.)
Jampolsky, M. Étude de quelques épreuves de reconnaissance. Année Psychologique,
1950, 49, 63–97.
Jenkin, A. M. Imagery and learning. British Journal of Psychology, 1935, 26, 149–164.
Jenkins, J. J. Mediated associations: Paradigms and situations. In C. N. Cofer and B. S.
Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963.
Jenkins, J. J. Mediation theory and grammatical behavior. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Directions
in psycholinguistics. New York: MacMillan, 1965.
Jenkins, J. J. Individual differences in verbal learning. In R. M. Gagné (Ed.), Learning and
individual differences. Columbus, Ohio: Merrill, 1967.
Jenkins, J. J. , & Foss, D. J. An experimental analysis of pseudomediation. Psychonomic
Science, 1965, 2, 99–100.
Jenkins, J. J. , & Palermo, D. S. Mediation processes and the acquisition of linguistic
structure. In U. Bellugi and R. W. Brown (Eds.), The acquisition of language. Monographs
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1964, 29 (1), 141–169.
Jenkins, J. J. , & Russell, W. A. Associative clustering during recall. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 818–821.
Jenkins, J. J. , Russell, W. A. , & Suci, G. J. An atlas of semantic profiles for 360 words.
American Journal of Psychology, 1958, 71, 688–699.
Jenkins, J. R. , Neale, D. C. , & Deno, S. L. Differential memory for picture and word stimuli.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 1967, 58, 303–307.
Jensen, A. R. Transfer between paired-associate and serial learning. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1962, 1, 269–280.
Jensen, A. R. Rote learning in retarded adults and normal children. American Journal of
Mental Deficiency, 1965, 69, 828–834.
Jensen, A. R. , & Rohwer, W. D. Verbal mediation in paired-associate and serial learning.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 1, 346–352.
Jensen, A. R. , & Rohwer, W. D. Syntactical mediation of serial and paired-associate
learning as a function of age. Child Development, 1965, 36, 601–608. (a)
Jensen, A. R. , & Rohwer, W. D. What is learned in serial learning? Journal of Verbal
Behavior, 1965, 4, 62–72. (b)
Jensen, G. D. Effect of past experience upon induced movement. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 1960, 11, 281–288.
John, E. R. Mechanisms of memory. New York: Academic Press, 1967.
Johnson, N. F. The psychological reality of phrase-structure rules. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 469–475.
Johnson, N. F. On the relationship between sentence structure and the latency in
generating the sentence. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5,
375–380.
Johnson, N. F. Sequential verbal behavior. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal
behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Johnson, R. C. Latency and association value as predictors of rate of verbal learning.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 77–78.
Johnson, R. C. , Frincke, G. , & Martin, L. Meaningfulness, frequency, and affective
character of words as related to visual duration thresholds. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1961, 15, 199–204.
Johnson, R. C. , Thomson, C. W. , & Frincke, G. Word values, word frequency, and visual
duration thresholds. Psychological Review, 1960, 67, 332–342.
Jones, D. , & Spreen, O. Dichotic listening by retarded children: The effects of ear order
and abstractness. Child Development, 1967, 38 (1), 101–105.
Kahneman D. Method, findings and theory in studies of visual masking. Psychological
Bulletin, 1968, 70, (6, Pt. 1), 404–425.
Kamman, R. Associability: A study of the properties of associative ratings and the role of
association in word-word learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph, 1968,
78, (4, Pt. 2).
Kanungo, R. Paired-associate learning of function words. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 10,
47–48.
Kaplan, S. , Kaplan, R. , & Sampson, J. R. Encoding and arousal factors in free recall of
verbal and visual material. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12, 73–74.
Karwoski, T. R. , Gramlich, F. W. , & Arnott, P. Psychological studies in semantics. I. Free
association reactions to words, drawings and objects. Journal of Social Psychology, 1944,
20, 233–247.
Kasschau, R. A. , & Pollio, H. R. Response transfer mediated by meaningfully similar and
associated stimuli using a separate-lists design. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967,
74, 146–148.
Katz, J. J. , & Fodor, J. A. The structure of a semantic theory. Language, 1963, 39,
170–210.
Katz, J. J. , & Postal, P. M. An integrated theory of linguistic description. Cambridge, Mass.:
The M.I.T. Press, 1964.
Kausler, D. H. (Ed.) Readings in verbal learning. New York: Wiley, 1966.
Kausler, D. H. , & Deichmann, J. W. Mediation vs. pseudomediation in kindergarten
children. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 13, 119–120.
Kendler, H. H. , & Kendler, T. S. Vertical and horizontal processes in problem solving.
Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 1–16.
Keppel, G. , & Underwood, B. J. Proactive inhibition in short-term retention of single items.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1962, 1, 153–161.
Keppel, G. , & Zavortink, B. Further test of the use of images as mediators. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 82, 190–192.
Kiess, H. O. Effects of natural language mediators on short-term memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 7–13.
Kiess, H. O. , & Montague, W. E. Natural language mediators in paired-associate learning.
Psychonomic Science, 1965, 3, 549–550.
Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis’ “conditioning and learning.” New York: Appleton, 1961.
Kimura, D. Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1961, 15, 166–171.
Kimura, D. Right temporal-lobe damage: Perception of unfamiliar stimuli after damage.
Archives of Neurology, 1963, 8, 264–271.
Kimura, D. Left-right differences in the perception of melodies. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1964, 14, 355–358.
Kimura, D. Dual functional asymmetry of the brain in visual perception. Neuropsychologia,
1966, 4, 275–285.
Kimura, D. Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 1967, 3,
163–178.
Kimura, D. Asymmetries in perception related to hemispheric differentiation of function. In
M. Kinsbourne (Ed.), Hemispheric asymmetry of function. London: Tavistock, in press.
Kimura, D. , & Folb, S. Neural processing of backwards-speech sounds. Science, 1968,
161, 395–396.
Kirkpatrick, E. A. An experimental study of memory. Psychological Review, 1894, 1,
602–609.
Kjeldergaard, P. M. Transfer and mediation in verbal learning. In T. R. Dixon and D. L.
Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.:
Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Klapp, S. T. Individual differences in nonverbal memory for visually presented material.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, 1969.
Klima, E. S. , & Bellugi-Klima, U. Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In D. A.
Reibel and S. A. Schane (Eds.), Modern studies in English: Readings in transformational
grammar. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1969.
Klüver, H. Studies on the eidetic type and eidetic imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 1928, 25,
69–104.
Klüver, H. The eidetic child. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of child psychology.
Worcester, Mass.: Clark University Press, 1931.
Klüver, H. Eidetic phenomena. Psychological Bulletin, 1932, 29, 181–203.
Koen, F. An intra-verbal explication of the nature of metaphor. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 129–133.
Koen, F. Codability of complex stimuli: Three modes of representation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 435–441.
Koeppel, J. C. , & Beecroft, R. S. The conceptual similarity effect in free recall.
Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9, 213–214.
Kohler, I. (Translated by H. Fiss.) The formation and transformation of the perceptual world.
Psychological Issues, 1964, 3 (12, Whole No. 4).
Köhler, W. Gestalt psychology, New York: Liveright, 1929.
Kolers, P. A. Some formal characteristics of pictograms. American Scientist, 1969, 57,
348–363.
Kristofferson, A. B. Word recognition, meaningfulness, and familiarity. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 1957, 7, 219–220.
Krueger, W. C. F. The relative difficulty of nonsense syllables. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1934, 17, 145–153.
Kuhlman, C. K. Visual imagery in children. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Radcliffe College,
1960.
Kuhlmann, F. On the analysis of the memory consciousness for pictures of familiar objects.
American Journal of Psychology, 1907, 18, 389–420.
Kurtz, K. H. , & Hovland, C. I. The effect of verbalization during observation of stimulus
objects upon accuracy of recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1953,
45, 157–164.
Kusyszyn, I. , & Paivio, A. Transition probability, word order, and noun abstractness in the
learning of adjective-noun paired associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966,
71, 800–805.
Lachman, R. , & Dooling, D. J. Connected discourse and random strings: Effects of number
of inputs on recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 517–522.
Laffal, J. Response faults in word association as a function of response entropy. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 50, 265–270.
Laffal, J. Pathological and normal language. New York: Atherton Press, 1965.
Lambert, W. E. Associational fluency as a function of stimulus abstractness. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1955, 9, 103–106.
Lambert, W. E. , & Paivio, A. The influence of noun-adjective order on learning. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1956, 10, 9–12.
Landauer, T. K. Rate of implicit speech. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1962, 15, 646.
Langer, S. K. Philosophy in a new key. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1942.
(Reprinted by Mentor Books, New York, 1948)
Lantz, D. , & Stefflre, V. Language and cognition revisited. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 1964, 69, 472–481.
Lashley, K. S. The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral
mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon symposium. New York: Wiley, 1951.
Lawrence, D. H. , & LaBerge, D. L. Relationship between recognition accuracy and order of
reporting stimulus dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1956, 51, 12–18.
Lay, C. H. , & Paivio, A. The effects of task difficulty and anxiety on hesitations in speech.
Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 1969, 1, 25–37.
Leask, J. , Haber, R. N. , & Haber, R. B. Eidetic imagery in children: II. Longitudinal and
experimental results. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1969, 3 (3, Whole No. 35),
25–48.
Lee, S. S. , & Jensen, A. R. The effect of awareness on three-stage mediated association.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 1005–1009.
Leeper, R. A study of a neglected portion of the field of learning—the development of
sensory organization. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1935, 46, 41–75.
Lenneberg, E. H. Color naming, color recognition, color discrimination: A reappraisal.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1961, 12, 375–382.
Lenneberg, E. H. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley, 1967.
Leuba, C. Images as conditioned sensations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1940,
26, 345–351.
Leuba, C. , & Dunlap, R. Conditioning imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1951,
41, 352–355.
Levin, J. R. Factors related to the sentence facilitation of paired-associate learning: Some
characteristics of verbs. Journal of Educational Psychology, in press.
Liberman, A. M. , Cooper, F. S. , Shankweiler, D. P. , & Studdert-Kennedy, M. Perception of
the speech code. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 431–461.
Lieberman, L. R. , & Culpepper, J. T. Words versus objects: Comparison of free verbal
recall. Psychological Reports, 1965, 17, 983–988.
Lindley, R. H. Effects of controlled coding cues in short-term memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 580–587.
Lindley, R. H. , & Nedler, S. E. Supplementary report: Further effects of subjectgenerated
recoding cues on short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69,
324–325.
Lobb, H. Vision versus touch in form discrimination. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965,
19, 175–187.
Lobb, H. Cross-modal transfer of training in form discrimination. Address to the Annual
Meeting, Canadian Psychological Association, 1968.
Lobb, H. Asymmetrical transfer of form discrimination across sensory modalities in human
adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 86, 350–354.
Lockhart, R. S. Retrieval asymmetry in the recall of adjectives and nouns. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 12–17.
Loess, H. Short-term memory, word class, and sequence of items. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 74, 556–561.
Long, E. R. , Reid, L. S. , & Henneman, R. H. An experimental analysis of set: Variables
influencing the identification of ambiguous, visual stimulus-objects. The American Journal of
Psychology, 1960, 73, 553–562.
Lumsdaine, A. A. Effectiveness of pictures versus printed words in learning simple verbal
associations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1949.
Luria, A. R. (Translated by L. Solotaroff .) The mind of a mnemonist. New York: Basic
Books, 1968.
MacCorquodale, K. On Chomsky’s review of Skinner’s Verbal behavior . Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 83–99.
MacCorquodale, K. , & Meehl, P. E. Edward C. Tolman . In Modern learning theory. New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1954.
MacDonald, V. N. Effects of word concreteness, mediation set, and retention interval on
reported mediators and recall of noun paired associates. Unpublished master’s thesis,
University of Western Ontario, 1967.
Mackworth, J. F. Auditory short-term memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1964, 18,
292–303.
Maclay, H. , & Osgood, C. E. Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech. Word,
1959, 15, 19–44.
Maltzman, I. On the training of originality. Psychological Review, 1960, 67, 229–242.
Mandler, G. Associative frequency and associative prepotency as measures of response to
nonsense syllables. American Journal of Psychology, 1956, 68, 662–665.
Mandler, G. Organization and memory. In K. W. Spence and J. T. Spence (Eds.), The
psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. New York:
Academic Press, 1967.
Mandler, G. Association and organization: Facts, fancies, and theories. In T. R. Dixon and
D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New
Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Mandler, G. , & Campbell, E. H. Effect of variation in associative frequency of stimulus and
response members on paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1957, 54, 269–273.
Mandler, G. , & Earhard, B. Pseudomediation: Is chaining an artifact? Psychonomic
Science, 1964, 1, 247–248.
Mandler, G. , & Huttenlocher, J. The relationship between associative frequency,
associative ability, and paired-associate learning. American Journal of Psychology, 1956,
69, 424–428.
Mandler, J. , & Mandler, G. Thinking: From association to gestalt. New York: Wiley, 1964.
Margrain, S. A. Short-term memory as a function of input modality. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1967, 19, 109–114.
Marks, L. E. , & Miller, G. A. The role of semantic and syntactic constraints in the
memorization of English sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964,
3, 1–5.
Marshall, G. R. The effect of concrete noun and picture mediation on the paired associate
learning of abstract nouns. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological
Association, Atlantic City, 1965.
Marshall, G. R. , & Cofer, C. N. Associative indices as measures of word relatedness: A
summary and comparison of ten methods. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1963, 1, 408–421.
Marshall, J. F. , Rouse, R. O., Jr., & Tarpy, R. M. Acoustic versus associative models of
short-term memory coding. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 14, 54–55.
Martin, C. J. , Boersma, F. J. , & Cox, D. L. A classification of associative strategies in
paired-associate learning. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 3, 455–456.
Martin, C. J. , Cox, D. L. , & Boersma, F. J. The role of associative strategies in the
acquisition of P-A material: An alternate approach to meaningfulness. Psychonomic
Science, 1965, 3, 463–464.
Martin, E. Stimulus meaningfulness and paired-associate transfer. Psychological Review,
1968, 75, 421–441.
Martin, E. , & Roberts, K. H. Grammatical factors in sentence retention. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 211–218.
Martin, E. , & Roberts, K. H. Sentence length and sentence retention in the free learning
situation. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 8, 535–536.
Martin, R. B. , & Dean, S. J. Implicit and explicit mediation in paired-associate learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 21–27.
Martin, R. B. , & Dean, S. J. Reported mediation in paired-associate learning. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 23–27.
Matthews, W. A. The relation between association norms and word frequency. British
Journal of Psychology, 1965, 56, 391–399.
Matthews, W. A. Continued word associations and free recall. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1966, 18, 31–38.
Matthews, W. A. A frequency analysis of controlled, continuous word association. British
Journal of Psychology, 1967, 58, 227–236.
Matthews, W. A. Transformational complexity and short-term recall. Language and Speech,
1968, 11, 120–128.
McCawley, J. D. The role of semantics in a grammar. In E. Bach and R. T. Harms (Eds.),
Universals in linguistic theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1968.
McCullers, J. C. Associative strength and degree of interference in children’s verbal paired-
associate learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1967, 5, 58–68.
McGeoch, J. A. , & Irion, A. L. The psychology of human learning. New York: Longmans,
1952.
McGeoch, J. A. , & Whitely, P. L. The recall of observed material. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 1926, 17, 419–425.
McGuigan, F. J. (Ed.). Thinking: Studies of covert language processes. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.
McGuire, W. J. A multiprocess model for paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1961, 62, 335–347.
McKellar, P. Imagination and thinking. New York: Basic Books, 1957.
McKinney, J. P. Disappearance of luminous designs. Science, 1963, 140, 403–404.
McKinney, J. P. Lateral asymmetry in the stability of the visual field. Psychonomic Science,
1966, 5, 175–176. (a)
McKinney, J. P. Verbal meaning and perceptual stability. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
1966, 20, 237–242. (b)
McKinney, J. P. Handedness, eyedness, and perceptual stability of the left and right visual
fields. Neuropsychologia, 1967, 5, 339–344.
McMahon, L. Grammatical analysis as part of understanding a sentence. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, 1963.
McNeill, D. On theories of language acquisition. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.),
Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall,
1968.
McNulty, J. A. The effects of “instructions to mediate” upon paired-associate learning.
Psychonomic Science, 1966, 4, 61–62. (a)
McNulty, J. A. The measurement of “adopted chunks” in free recall learning. Psychonomic
Science, 1966, 4, 71–72. (b)
Mednick, S. A. The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 1962,
69, 220–232.
Mehler, J. Some effects of grammatical transformations on the recall of English sentences.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 346–351.
Mehler, J. , & Carey, P. Role of surface and base structure in the perception of sentences.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 335–338.
Melton, A. W. Implications of short-term memory for a general theory of memory. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 1–21.
Merikle, P. M. , Lowe, D. G. , & Coltheart, M. Evidence against left to right processing of
input to short-term memory. Paper presented at the meeting of the Canadian Psychological
Association, Winnipeg, April, 1970.
Merryman, S. S. , & Merryman, C. T. Differential recall of stimuli and responses following
paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 345–346.
Mewhort, D. J. K. Sequential redundancy and letter spacing as determinants of
tachistoscopic recognition. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1966, 20, 435–444.
Mewhort, D. J. K. , Merikle, P. M. , & Bryden, M. P. On the transfer from iconic to short-term
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 81, 89–94.
Michon, J. A. On the internal representation of associative data networks. Nederlands
Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie, 1968, 23, 428–457.
Middleton, A. E. All about mnemonics. London: Pitman, 1887.
Milgram, N. A. Verbal context versus visual compound in paired-associate learning by
children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1967, 5, 597–603.
Miller, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two. Psychological Review, 1956,
63, 81–97.
Miller, G. A. Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist, 1962, 17,
748–762.
Miller, G. A. , Galanter, E. , & Pribram, K. H. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York;
Holt, 1960.
Miller, G. A. , & Isard, S. Some perceptual consequences of linguistic rules. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 217–228.
Miller, G. A. , & McNeill, D. Psycholinguistics. In G. Lindzey and E. Aranson (Eds.), The
handbook of social psychology, Vol. III. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1969.
Miller, G. A. , & Selfridge, J. A. Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material.
American Journal of Psychology, 1950, 63, 176–187.
Miller, N. E. Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extension to conflict behavior, motivation,
and social learning. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 2. New York:
McGraw-Hill, 1959.
Miller, N. E. , and Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale University
Press, 1941.
Milner, B. , & Teuber, H. L. Alteration of perception and memory in man: Reflections on
methods. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Analysis of behavioral change. New York: Harper & Row,
1968.
Mishkin, M. , & Forgays, D. G. Word recognition as a function of retinal locus. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1952, 43, 43–48.
Montague, W. E. , Adams, J. A. , & Kiess, H. O. Forgetting and natural language mediation.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 72, 829–833.
Montague, W. E. , & Kiess, H. O. The associability of CVC pairs. Journal of Experimental
Psychology Monograph, 1968, 78 (2, Pt. 2).
Montague, W. E. , & Wearing, A. J. The complexity of natural language mediators and its
relation to paired-associate learning. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 7, 135–136.
Monty, R. A. , & Karsh, R. Spatial encoding of auditory stimuli in sequential shortterm
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 81, 572–575.
Monty, R. A. , Taub, H. A. , & Laughery, K. R. Keeping track of sequential events: Effects of
rate, categories, and trial length. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 224–229.
Moore, T. V. The temporal relations of meaning and imagery. Psychological Review, 1915,
22, 177–225.
Moore, T. V. Image and meaning in memory and perception. Psychological Monographs,
1919, 27 (Whole No. 119).
Morelli, G. Pictures and competing pictures as mediators in paired-associate learning.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1970, 30, 729–730.
Morgan, C. T. Physiological psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1943.
Morin, R. E. , Konick, A. , Troxell, N. , & McPherson, S. Information and reaction time for
“naming” responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 309–314.
Morris, C. Signs, language and behavior. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1946.
Morris, V. A. , Rankine, F. C. , & Reber, A. S. Sentence comprehension, grammatical
transformations and response availability. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1968, 7, 1113–1115.
Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and the symbolic processes. New York: Wiley, 1960.
Mueller, D. J. , & Travers, R. M. W. Temporal relations and meaningfulness in paired-
associate learning. Psychological Reports, 1965, 17, 491–497.
Mueller, J. H. & Jablonski, E. M. Instructions, noun imagery, and priority in free recall.
Psychological Reports, 1970, 27, 559–566.
Mueller, M. R. , Edmonds, E. M. , & Evans, S. H. Amount of uncertainty associated with
decoding in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 75, 437–443.
Murdock, B. B., Jr. The immediate retention of unrelated words. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1960, 60, 222–234.
Murdock, B. B., Jr. The serial position effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1962, 64, 482–488.
Murdock, B. B., Jr. Auditory and visual stores in short term memory. Acta Psychologica,
1967, 27, 316–324. (a)
Murdock, B. B., Jr. Recent developments in short-term memory. British Journal of
Psychology, 1967, 58, 421–433. (b)
Murdock, B. B., Jr. Modality effects in short-term memory: Storage or retrieval? Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 79–86.
Murray, D. J. The role of speech responses in short-term memory. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1967, 21, 263–276.
Murray, D. J. Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 679–684.
Murray, D. J. , & Roberts, B. Visual and auditory presentation, presentation rate, and short-
term memory in children. British Journal of Psychology, 1968, 59, 119–125.
Murray, H. A. Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938.
Neisser, U. An experimental distinction between perceptual process and verbal response.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1954, 47, 399–402.
Neisser, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton, 1967.
Neisser, U. Visual imagery as process and as experience. Paper presented at the Center
for Research in Cognition and Affect, New York, June, 1968.
Neisser, U. , & Beller, H. K. Searching through word lists. British Journal of Psychology,
1965, 56, 349–358.
Nickerson, R. S. Short-term memory for complex meaningful visual configurations: A
demonstration of capacity. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 155–160.
Nickerson, R. S. A note on long-term recognition memory for pictorial material.
Psychonomic Science, 1968, 11, 58.
Noble, C. E. An analysis of meaning. Psychological Review, 1952, 59, 421–430. (a)
Noble, C. E. The role of stimulus meaning (m) in serial verbal learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1952, 43, 437–446; 1952, 44, 465. (b)
Noble, C. E. The meaning-familiarity relationship. Psychological Review, 1953, 60, 89–98.
Noble, C. E. The familiarity-frequency relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1954, 47, 13–16.
Noble, C. E. Verbal learning and individual differences. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), Verbal learning
and verbal behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.
Noble, C. E. Meaningfulness and familiarity. In C. N. Cofer and B. S. Musgrave (Eds.),
Verbal behavior and learning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963.
Noble, C. E. , & McNeely, D. A. The role of meaningfulness (m) in paired-associate verbal
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1957, 53, 16–22.
Noble, C. E. , Showell, F. A. , & Jones, H. R. Serial CVC learning with varied m′ but equal a
values. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 4, 217–218.
Norman, D. A. (Ed.), Models of human memory. New York: Academic Press, 1970.
Nunnally, J. C. , & Flaugher, R. L. Psychological implications of word usage. Science, 1963,
140, 775–781.
Oldfield, R. C. Memory mechanisms and the theory of schemata. British Journal of
Psychology, 1954, 45, 14–23.
Oldfield, R. C. Things, words, and the brain. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1966, 18, 340–353.
Olson, D. R. Language and thought: Aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics.
Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 257–273.
Olton, R. M. The effect of a mnemonic upon the retention of paired-associate verbal
material. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 43–48.
Olver, M. A. Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in recognition and free recall.
Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1965.
O'Neill, B. Word attributes in dichotic recognition and memory. Unpublished doctoral thesis,
University of Western Ontario, 1971.
Osgood, C. E. Method and theory in experimental psychology. New York: Oxford, 1953.
Osgood, C. E. Motivational dynamics of language behavior. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska
symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1957.
Osgood, C. E. Comments on Professor Bousfield’s paper. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), Verbal
learning and verbal behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.
Osgood, C. E. Psycholinguistics. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 6.
New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963.
Osgood, C. E. Meaning cannot be rm? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1966, 5, 402–407.
Osgood, C. E. Toward a wedding of insufficiencies. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.),
Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall,
1968.
Osgood, C. E. Is neo-behaviorism up a blind alley? Unpublished mimeo, Institute of
Communications Research, The University of Illinois, 1969.
Osgood, C. E. , Suci, G. J. , & Tannenbaum, P. H. The measurement of meaning. Urbana,
Ill.: University of Illinois Press, 1957.
Oswald, I. The EEG, visual imagery and attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1957, 9, 113–118.
Oswald, I. Number-forms and kindred visual images. Journal of General Psychology, 1960,
63, 81–88.
Oswald, I. Sleeping and waking. New York: Elsevier, 1962.
Otto, W. The differential effects of verbal and pictorial representations of stimuli upon
responses evoked. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1962, 1, 192–196.
Overton, W. , & Wiener, M. Visual field position and word-recognition threshold. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1966, 71, 249–253.
Paivio, A. Learning of adjective-noun paired-associates as a function of adjective-noun
word order and noun abstractness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1963, 17, 370–379.
Paivio, A. Generalization of verbally conditioned meaning from symbol to referent.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1964, 18, 146–155.
Paivio, A. Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 32–38.
Paivio, A. Latency of verbal associations and imagery to noun stimuli as a function of
abstractness and generality. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1966, 20, 378–387.
Paivio, A. Paired-associate learning and free recall of nouns as a function of concreteness,
specificity, imagery, and meaningfulness. Psychological Reports, 1967, 20, 239–245.
Paivio, A. A factor-analytic study of word attributes and verbal learning. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 41–49. (a)
Paivio, A. Effects of imagery instructions and concreteness of memory pegs in a mnemonic
system. Proceedings of the 76th Annual Convention of the American Psychological
Association, 1968, 77–78. (b)
Paivio, A. Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychological Review, 1969,
76, 3, 241–263.
Paivio, A. Imagery and natural language. Symposium address, American Educational
Research Association meeting, Minneapolis, March, 1970. (a)
Paivio, A. On the functional significance of imagery. In H. W. Reese (Chm.), Imagery in
children’s learning: A symposium. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, 73, 385–392. (b)
Paivio, A. Imagery and deep structure in the recall of English nominalizations. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, 10, 1–12.
Paivio, A. , & Begg, I. Imagery and associative overlap in short-term memory. Research
Bulletin No. 163, Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, 1970. (a)
Paivio, A. , & Begg, I. Imagery and comprehension latencies as a function of sentence
concreteness and structure. Research Bulletin No. 154, Department of Psychology,
University of Western Ontario, 1970. (b)
Paivio, A. , & Csapo, K. Concrete-image and verbal memory codes. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 80, 279–285.
Paivio, A. , & Foth, D. Imaginai and verbal mediators and noun concreteness in paired-
associate learning: The elusive interaction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1970, 9, 384–390.
Paivio, A. , & Madigan, S. A. Imagery and association value in paired-associate learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 76, 35–39.
Paivio, A. , & Madigan, S. A. Noun imagery and frequency in paired-associate and free
recall learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1970, 24, 353–361.
Paivio, A. , & Okovita, H. W. Word imagery modalities and associative learning in blind and
sighted subjects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, in press.
Paivio, A. , & Olver, M. Denotative-generality, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired
associate learning of nouns. Psychonomic Science, 1964, 1, 183–184.
Paivio, A. , & O'Neill, B. J. Visual recognition thresholds and dimensions of word meaning.
Perception and Psychophysics, 1970, 8, 273–275.
Paivio, A. , Rogers, T. B. , & Smythe, P. C. Why are pictures easier to recall than words?
Psychonomic Science, 1968, 11, 137–138.
Paivio, A. , & Rowe, E. J. Noun imagery, frequency, and meaningfulness in verbal
discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 85, 264–269.
Paivio, A. , & Simpson, H. M. The effect of word abstractness and pleasantness on pupil
size during an imagery task. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 55–56.
Paivio, A. , & Simpson, H. M. Pupillary responses during imagery tasks as a function of
stimulus characteristics and imagery ability. Research Bulletin No. 45, Department of
Psychology, University of Western Ontario, 1967.
Paivio, A. , & Simpson, H. M. Magnitude and latency of the pupillary response during an
imagery task as a function of stimulus abstractness and imagery ability. Psychonomic
Science, 1968, 12, 45–46.
Paivio, A. , & Smythe, P. C. Word imagery, frequency, and meaningfulness in shortterm
memory. Psychonomic Science, 1971, 22, 333–335.
Paivio, A. , Smythe, P. C. , & Yuille, J. C. Imagery versus meaningfulness of nouns in
paired-associate learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 427–441.
Paivio, A. , & Steeves, R. The relations between personal values and the imagery and
meaningfulness of value words. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1967, 24, 357–358.
Paivio, A. , & Yarmey, A. D. Abstractness of the common element in mediated learning.
Psychonomic Science, 1965, 2, 231–232.
Paivio, A. , & Yarmey, A. D. Pictures versus words as stimuli and responses in paired-
associate learning. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 235–236.
Paivio, A. , & Yuille, J. C. Word abstractness and meaningfulness, and paired-associate
learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1966, 4, 81–89.
Paivio, A. , & Yuille, J. C. Mediation instructions and word attributes in paired-associate
learning. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 8, 65–66.
Paivio, A. , & Yuille, J. C. Changes in associative strategies and paired-associate learning
over trials as a function of word imagery and type of learning set. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1969, 79, 458–463.
Paivio, A. , Yuille, J. C. , & Madigan, S. Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values
for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 1968, 76 (1,
Pt. 2).
Paivio, A. , Yuille, J. C. , & Rogers, T. B. Noun imagery and meaningfulness in free and
serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 509–514.
Paivio, A. , Yuille, J. C. , & Smythe, P. C. Stimulus and response abstractness, imagery,
and meaningfulness, and reported mediators in paired-associate learning. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1966, 20, 362–377.
Palermo, D. S. & Jenkins, J. J. Word association norms: Grade school through college.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1964.
Pear, T. H. The relevance of visual imagery to the process of thinking, I. British Journal of
Psychology, 1927, 28, 1–14.
Pelton, L. H. Mediational construction vs. mediational perception in paired-associate
learning. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 17, 199–200.
Penfield, W. The permanent record of the stream of consciousness. Proceedings and
papers, 14th International Congress of Psychology, June, 1954, 47–69.
Perfetti, C. A. Sentence retention and the depth hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 101–104.
Perky, C. W. An experimental study of imagination. American Journal of Psychology, 1910,
21, 422–452.
Peters, H. N. The relationship between familiarity of words and their memory value.
American Journal of Psychology, 1936, 48, 572–584.
Peterson, M. J. Effects of knowledge of results on a verbal mediating response. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 394–398.
Peterson, M. J. , & Blattner, K. C. Development of a verbal mediator. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 72–77.
Peterson, M. J. , Colavita, F. B. , Sheahan, D. B. , & Blattner, K. C. Verbal mediating chains
and response availability as a function of the acquisition paradigm. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 11–18.
Peterson, L. R. , & Peterson, M. J. Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1959, 58, 193–198.
Pettifor, J. L. The role of language in the development of abstract thinking: A comparison of
hard-of-hearing and normal-hearing children on levels of conceptual thinking. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 139–156.
Philipchalk, R. The development of imaginai meaning in verbal stimuli. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Western Ontario, 1971.
Philipchalk, R. , & Begg, I. Context concreteness and form class in the retention of CVCs.
Research Bulletin No. 155, Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, 1970.
Phillips, L. W. Mediated verbal similarity as a determinant of the generalization of a
conditioned GSR. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 55, 56–62.
Piaget, J. Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton, 1962.
Piaget, J. , & Inhelder, B. L’image mentale chez l’enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de
France, 1966.
Pinkus, A. L. , & Laughery, K. R. Short-term memory: Effects of pronunciability and
phonemic uniqueness of chunks. Proceedings of the 75th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, 1967, 65–66.
Podd, M. H. , & Spear, N. E. Stimulus relatedness and response coding. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 55–60.
Pollack, I. , & Johnson, L. B. Memory-span with efficient coding procedures. American
Journal of Psychology, 1965, 78, 609–614.
Pollio, H. R. Composition of associative clusters. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1964, 67, 199–208.
Pollio, H. R. Associative structure and verbal behavior. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton
(Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey:
Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Pollio, H. R. , Richards, S. , & Lucas, R. Temporal properties of category recall. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 529–536.
Poison, M. C. , Restle, F. , & Poison, P. G. Association and discrimination in paired-
associates learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 47–55.
Pompi, K. F. , & Lachman, R. Surrogate processes in the short-term retention of connected
discourse. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 75, 143–150.
Posner, M. I. Abstraction and the process of recognition. In Spence and Bower (Eds.),
Advances in learning and motivation. Vol. 3. New York: Academic Press, 1970, in press.
Posner, M. I. , Boies, S. J. , Eichelman, W. H. , & Taylor, R. L. Retention of visual and name
codes of single letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph, 1969, 79 (1, Pt. 2).
Posner, M. I. , & Keele, S. W. Decay of visual information from a single letter. Science,
1967, 158, 137–139.
Posner, M. I. , & Keele, S. W. On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1968, 77, 353–363.
Posner, M. I. , & Konick, A. F. Short-term retention of visual and kinesthetic information.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1966, 1, 71–86.
Posner, M. I. , & Mitchell, R. F. Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological
Review, 1967, 74, 392–409.
Postman, L. Extra-experimental interference and the retention of words. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1961, 61, 97–110.
Postman, L. The effects of language habits on the acquisition and retention of verbal
associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 64, 7–19.
Postman, L. Acquisition and retention of consistent associative responses. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1964, 67, 183–190.
Postman, L. , & Phillips, L. W. Short-term temporal changes in free recall. Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 17, Pt. 2, 132–138.
Postman, L. , & Stark, K. Studies of learning to learn. IV. Transfer from serial to paired-
associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 339–353.
Potter, M. C. , & Levy, E. I. Recognition memory for a rapid sequence of pictures. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 81, 10–15.
Pound, E. Literary essays of Ezra Pound. ( T. S. Eliot , ed.) Norfolk: New Directions, 1954.
Pratt, W. (Ed.) The imagist poem. New York: E. P. Dutton & Co., 1963.
Prentice, J. L. Intraverbal associations in sentence behavior. Psychonomic Science, 1968,
10, 213–214.
Prentice, W. C. H. Visual recognition of verbally labeled figures. American Journal of
Psychology, 1954, 67, 315–320.
Pribram, K. H. A review of theory in physiological psychology. Annual Review of
Psychology, 1960, 11, 1–40.
Pribram, K. H. The neurophysiology of remembering. Scientific American, 1969, 220 (1),
73–86.
Pritchard, R. M. Stabilized images on the retina. Scientific American, 1961, 204, 72–78.
Pritchard, R. M. , Heron, W. , & Hebb, D. O. Visual perception approached by the method of
stabilized images. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1960, 14, 67–77.
Prytulak, L. S. Natural language mediation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford
University, 1969.
Puff, C. R. , & Hyson, S. P. An empirical comparison of two measures of intertrial
organization in free recall. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9, 329–330.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. , & Agnew, N. M. Coding in perception and immediate memory. Paper read
at the 17th International Congress of Psychology, Washington, D. C., August, 1963.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. , & Feldmar, A. Grammatical category as mediator. Psychonomic Science,
1968, 13, 115–116.
Quillian, M. R. Word concepts. A theory and simulation of some basic semantic capabilities.
Behavioral Science, 1967, 12, 410–430.
Quintilian . The institutio oratoria. English translation by H. E. Butler. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press, 1953.
Raab, D. H. Backward masking. Psychological Bulletin, 1963, 60, 118–129.
Ranken, H. B. Language and thinking: Positive and negative effects of naming. Science,
1963, 141, 48–50.
Raser, G. A. , & Bartz, W. H. Imagery and paired-associate recognition. Psychonomic
Science, 1968, 12, 385–386.
Ray-Chowdhury, K. Imagery and performance tests of intelligence. Indian Psychological
Bulletin, 1957, 2, 25–30.
Reed, H. B. Associative aids: I. Their relation to learning, retention and other associations.
Psychological Review, 1918, 25, 128–155. (a)
Reed, H. B. Associative aids: II. Their relation to practice and the transfer of training.
Psychological Review, 1918, 25, 257–285. (b)
Reed, H. B. Associative aids: III. Their relation to the theory of thought and to methodology
in psychology. Psychological Review, 1918, 25, 378–401. (c)
Reed, H. B. , & Dick, R. D. The learning and generalization of abstract and concrete
concepts. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 486–490.
Reese, H. W. Imagery in paired-associate learning in children. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 290–296.
Reese, H. W. Imagery and contextual meaning. In H. W. Reese (Chm.), Imagery in
children’s learning: A symposium. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, 73, 404–414.
Reitman, W. R. Cognition and thought. New York: Wiley, 1965.
Reynolds, A. , & Paivio, A. Cognitive and emotional determinants of speech. Canadian
Journal of Psychology, 1968, 22, 164–175.
Reynolds, J. H. Cognitive transfer in verbal learning. Journal of Educational Psychology,
1966, 57, 382–388.
Reynolds, J. H. Cognitive transfer in verbal learning: II. Transfer effects after
prefamiliarization with integrated versus partially integrated verbal-perceptual structures.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 1968, 59, 133–138.
Richardson, A. Mental imagery. New York: Springer, 1969.
Richardson, J. Comparison of S-R and R-S learning of paired-associates. Psychological
Reports, 1960, 7, 225–228.
Richardson, J. Latencies of implicit associative responses and positive transfer in paired-
associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 638–646.
Riegel, K. F. , & Riegel, R. M. Prediction of word-recognition thresholds on the basis of
stimulus-parameters. Language and Speech, 1961, 4, 157–170.
Rimm, D. C. , Alexander, R. A. , & Eiles, R. R. Effects of different mediational instructions
and sex of subject on paired-associate learning of concrete nouns. Psychological Reports,
1969, 25, 935–940.
Roberts, K. H. Grammatical and associative constraints in sentence retention. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 1072–1076.
Robinson, J. S. , Brown, L. T. , & Hayes, W. H. Test of effects of past experience on
perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1964, 18, 953–956.
Roe, A. A Study of imagery in research scientists. Journal of Personality, 1951, 19,
459–470.
Rogers, T. B. Coding instructions and item concreteness in free recall. Unpublished
master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1967.
Rohrman, N. L. The role of syntactic structure in the recall of English nominalizations.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 904–912.
Rohrman, N. L. , & Polzella, D. J. Recall of subject nominalizations. Psychonomic Science,
1968, 12, 373–374.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr. Verbal and visual elaboration in paired associate learning. Project
Literacy Reports, Cornell University, 1966, No. 7, 18–28.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr. Images and pictures in children’s learning: Research results and
instructional implications. In H. W. Reese (Chm.), Imagery in children’s learning: A
symposium. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, 73, 393–403.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr., & Lynch, S. Semantic constraint in paired-associate learning. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 1966, 57, 271–278.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr., & Lynch, S. Form class and intralist similarity in paired-associate
learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 551–554.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr., Lynch, S. , Levin, J. R. , & Suzuki, N. Pictorial and verbal factors in the
efficient learning of paired associates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1967, 58,
278–284.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr ., Lynch, S. , Suzuki, N. , & Levin, J. R. Verbal and pictorial facilitation of
paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1967, 5, 294–302.
Rosenberg, S. Recall of sentences as a function of syntactic and associative habit. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 392–396.
Rosenberg, S. The relation between association and syntax in sentence production.
Studies in language and behavior, Progress Report V. Center for Research on Language
and Language Behavior, University of Michigan, 1967.
Rosenberg, S. Association and phrase structure in sentence recall. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 1077–1081.
Rosenfeld, J. B. Information processing: encoding and decoding. Unpublished doctoral
thesis, Indiana University, 1967.
Rosenstein, J. Perception, cognition and language in deaf children. Exceptional Children,
1961, 27, 276–284.
Ross, J. , & Lawrence, K. A. Some observations on memory artifice. Psychonomic Science,
1968, 13, 107–108.
Rouse, R. O. , & Verinis, J. S. The effect of associative connections on the recognition of
flashed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1962, 1, 300–303.
Rowe, E. J. & Paivio, A. Word frequency and imagery effects in verbal discrimination
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, in press. (a)
Rowe, E. J. & Paivio, A. Effects of imagery and repetition instructions in verbal
discrimination learning. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological
Association, New York, April, 1971. (b)
Rugg, H. Imagination. New York: Harper & Row, 1963.
Runquist, W. N. Functions relating intralist stimulus similarity to acquisition performance
with a variety of materials. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7,
549–553.
Runquist, W. N. , & Farley, F. H. The use of mediators in the learning of verbal paired
associates. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 280–285.
Russell, B. An inquiry into meaning and truth. London: George Allen and Unwin, 1940.
Russell, W. A. , & Jenkins, J. J. The complete Minnesota norms for responses to 100 words
from the Kent-Rosanoff Word Association Test. Technical Report No. 11, Contract No.
N8onr-66216, Office of Naval Research and University of Minnesota, 1954.
Russell, W. A. , & Storms, L. H. Implicit verbal chaining in paired-associate learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1955, 49, 287–293.
Ryan, J. J. Comparison of verbal response transfer mediated by meaningfully similar and
associated stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1960, 60, 408–415.
Sachs, J. S. Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected
discourse. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 437–442. (a)
Sachs, J. S. Recognition of semantic, syntactic and lexical changes in sentences. Paper
presented at Psychonomic Society Meetings, Chicago, Ill., October, 1967. (b)
Sales, B. D. , Haber, R. N. , & Cole, R. A. Mechanisms of aural encoding III: Distinctive
features for vowels. Perception and Psychophysics, 1968, 4, 321–327.
Sales, B. D. , Haber, R. N. , & Cole, R. A. Mechanisms of aural encoding IV: Hear-see, say-
write interactions for vowels. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 6, 385–390.
Saltz, E. Compound stimuli in verbal learning: Cognitive and sensory differentiation versus
stimulus selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 1–5.
Saltz, E. Thorndike-Lorge frequency and m of stimuli as separate factors in paired-
associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 73, 473–478.
Saltz, E. , & Ager, J. W. The role of context cues in learning: A reply to Greeno.
Psychological Reports, 1968, 22, 351–354.
Saltz, E. , & Modigliani, V. Response meaningfulness in paired associates: T-L frequency,
m, and number of meanings (dm ). Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 75,
313–320.
Saltz, E. , & Wickey, J. Further evidence for differentiation effects of context stimuli: A reply
to Birnbaum. Psychological Reports, 1967, 20, 835–838.
Salzinger, K. , Portnoy, S. , & Feldman, R. S. The effect of order of approximation to the
statistical structure of English on the emission of verbal responses. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1962, 64, 52–57.
Sampson, J. R. Free recall of verbal and non-verbal stimuli. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1970, 22, 215–221.
Samuels, S. J. Effect of word associations on the recognition of flashed words. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 1969, 60, 97–102.
Santa, J. L. & Ranken, H. B. Language and memory: Redintegrative memory for shapes
facilitated by naming. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 13, 109–110.
Sassenrath, J. M. , & Yonge, G. D. Meaning and transfer of verbal learning. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 1967, 58, 365–372.
Saufley, W. H., Jr. An analysis of cues in serial learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1967, 74, 414–419.
Savin, H. B. , & Perchonock, E. Grammatical structure and the immediate recall of English
sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1965, 4, 348–353.
Scagnelli, P. Relationships among visual imagery, language and haptics in spatial
perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Duke University, 1969.
Scapinello, K. F. & Yarmey, A. D. The role of familiarity and orientation in immediate and
delayed recognition of pictorial stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 21, 329–331.
Schaub, G. R. , & Lindley, R. H. Effects of subject-generated recoding cues on short-term
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 171–175.
Schlaegel, T. F. The dominant method of imagery in blind as compared to sighted
adolescents. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1953, 83, 265–277.
Schmeidler, G. R. Visual imagery correlated to a measure of creativity. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, 78–80.
Schnorr, J. A. , & Atkinson, R. C. Repetition versus imagery instructions in the short- and
long-term retention of paired-associates. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 15, 183–184.
Scholtz, D. S. Fundamental principles of form perception in touch. Acta Psychologica, 1958,
13, 299–333.
Schuck, J. R. , Brock, T. C. , & Becker, L. A. Luminous figures: Factors affecting the
reporting of disappearances. Science, 1964, 146, 1598–1599.
Schulman, A. I. Word length and rarity in recognition memory. Psychonomic Science, 1967,
9, 211–212.
Schulz, R. W. , Liston, J. R. , & Weaver, G. E. The A-B, B-C, A-C mediation paradigm:
Recall of A-B following A-C learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968,
7, 602–607.
Schulz, R. W. , & Lovelace, E. A. Mediation in verbal paired-associate learning: The role of
temporal factors. Psychonomic Science, 1964, 1, 95–96.
Schulz, R. W. , & Weaver, G. E. The A-B, B-C, A-C mediation paradigm: The effects of
variation in A-C study- and test-interval lengths and strength of A-B or B-C. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 76, 303–311.
Schwartz, M. Instructions to use verbal mediators in paired-associate learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 1–5. (a)
Schwartz, M. Instructions to mediate, recall time, and type of paired-associate list. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 81, 398–400. (b)
Scott, K. G. Clustering with perceptual and symbolic stimuli in free recall. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 864–866.
Segal, S. J. The Perky effect: Changes in reality judgments with changing methods of
inquiry. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12, 393–394.
Segal, S. J. , & Fusella, V. Influence of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual
and auditory signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 83, 458–464.
Segal, S. J. , & Gordon, P. The Perky effect revisited: Paradoxical thresholds or signal
detection error? Paper presented at the 39th annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological
Association, 1968.
Segal, S. J. , & Nathan, S. The Perky effect: Incorporation of an external stimulus into an
imagery experience under placebo and control conditions. Perceptual Motor Skills, 1964,
18, 385–395.
Seibel, R. , Lockhart, R. , & Taschman, C. S. The learning of 100 paired associates in a
single, self-paced trial. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society,
Chicago, October, 1967.
Seidel, R. J. The importance of the S-R role of the verbal mediators in mediate association.
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1962, 16, 170–176.
Semler, I. J. , & Iscoe, I. Concept interference and paired associates in retarded children.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1965, 60, 465–466.
Semmes, J. Hemispheric specialization: A possible clue to mechanism. Neuropsychologia,
1968, 6, 11–26.
Senter, R. J. , & Hauser, G. K. An experimental study of a mnemonic system. Psychonomic
Science, 1968, 10, 289–290.
Shapiro, S. I. Response word frequency in paired-associate learning. Psychonomic
Science, 1969, 16, 308–309.
Shaw, E. E. Some imaginai factors influencing verbal expression. Psychological Review
Monograph Supplement, 1919, 26, No. 113.
Sheehan, P. W. Accuracy and vividness of visual images. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
1966, 23, 391–398. (a)
Sheehan, P. W. Functional similarity of imaging to perceiving: Individual differences in
vividness of imagery. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 23, 1011–1033. (b)
Sheehan, P. W. A shortened form of Betts’ questionnaire upon mental imagery. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 1967, 23, 386–389. (a)
Sheehan, P. W. Reliability of a short test of imagery. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1967, 25,
744. (b)
Sheehan, P. W. , & Neisser, U. Some variables affecting the vividness of imagery in recall.
British Journal of Psychology, 1969, 60, 71–80.
Sheffield, F. D. Theoretical considerations in the learning of complex sequential tasks from
demonstration and practice. In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in programmed
instruction. (NAS-NRS Publication No. 943) Washington, D. C.: National Academy of
Sciences-National Research Council, 1961.
Sheffield, F. D. Relation between classical conditioning and instrumental learning. In W. F.
Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1965.
Shepard, R. N. Learning and recall as organization and search. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 201–204.
Shepard, R. N. Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 156–163.
Shepard, R. N. , & Chipman, S. Second-order isomorphism of internal representations:
Shapes of states. Cognitive Psychology, 1970, 1, 1–17.
Shepard, R. N. , & Teghtsoonian, M. Retention of information under conditions approaching
a steady state. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961, 62, 302–309.
Shipley, W. C. Indirect conditioning. Journal of General Psychology, 1935, 12, 337–357.
Short, P. L. The objective study of mental imagery. British Journal of Psychology, 1953, 44,
38–51.
Shuell, T. J. , & Keppel, G. A further test of the chaining hypothesis of serial learning.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 439–445.
Siipola, E. M. , & Hayden, S. D. Exploring eidetic imagery among the retarded. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 1965, 21, 275–286.
Siipola, E. M. , Walker, W. N. , & Kolb, D. Task attitudes in word association, projective and
nonprojective. Journal of Personality, 1955, 23, 441–459.
Simon, H. A. , & Feigenbaum, E. A. An information-processing theory of some effects of
similarity, familiarization, and meaningfulness in verbal learning. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 385–396.
Simpson, H. M. Inferring cognitive processes from pupillary activity and response time.
Colloquium presented at the University of Western Ontario, January, 1970.
Simpson, H. M. , & Paivio, A. Changes in pupil size during an imagery task without motor
response involvement. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 405–406.
Simpson, H. M. , & Paivio, A. Effects on pupil size of manual and verbal indicators of
cognitive task fulfillment. Perception and Psychophysics, 1968, 3, 185–190.
Simpson, H. M. , Paivio, A. , & Rogers, T. B. Occipital alpha activity of high and low visual
imagers during problem solving. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 7, 49–50.
Singer, J. L. Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner experience.
New York: Random House, 1966.
Singh, S. , Brokaw, S. P. , & Black, J. W. Effects of delayed sidetone, noise, and syntactic
structure on the level and duration of speech. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1967, 6, 629–633.
Skehan, P. The relation of visual imagery to true-false judgment of simple sentences.
Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1970.
Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: MacMillan, 1953.
Skinner, B. F. Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.
Slamecka, N. J. An inquiry into the doctrine of remote associations. Psychological Review,
1964, 71, 61–76.
Slamecka, N. J. An examination of trace storage in free recall. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1968, 76, 504–513.
Slamecka, N. J. Recognition of word strings as a function of linguistic violations. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 377–378.
Slatter, K. H. Alpha rhythms and mental imagery. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology, 1960, 12, 851–859.
Slobin, D. I. Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in childhood and
adulthood. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 219–227.
Slobin, D. L. Recall of full and truncated passive sentences in connected discourse. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 876–881.
Smith, M. G. , & Harleston, B. W. Stimulus abstractness and emotionality as determinants
of behavioral and physiological responses in a word-association task. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 309–313.
Smith, R. K. , & Noble, C. E. Effects of a mnemonic technique applied to verbal learning
and memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 21, 123–134.
Smythe, P. C. The effects of imagery and synonymity in verbal transfer. Unpublished
honors thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1966.
Smythe, P. C. Pair concreteness and mediation instructions in forward and backward
paired-associate recall. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Western Ontario, 1970.
Smythe, P. C. , & Paivio, A. A comparison of the effectiveness of word imagery and
meaningfulness in paired-associate learning of nouns. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 10,
49–50.
Solomon, R. L. , & Howes, D. H. Word frequency, personal values, and visual duration
thresholds. Psychological Review, 1951, 58, 256–270.
Spear, N. E. , Ekstrand, B. R. , & Underwood, B. J. Association by contiguity. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1964, 67, 151–161.
Sperling, G. The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological
Monographs, 1960, 74 (Whole No. 498).
Sperling, G. A model for visual memory tasks. Human Factors, 1963, 5, 19–31.
Sperry, R. W. Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. American
Psychologist, 1968, 23, 723–733.
Spielberger, C. D. , & DeNike, L. D. Descriptive behaviorism versus cognitive theory in
verbal operant conditioning. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 306–326.
Spielberger, C. D. , & Denny, J. P. Visual recognition thresholds as a function of verbal
ability and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 65, 597–602.
Spiker, C. C. Associative transfer in verbal paired-associate learning. Child Development,
1960, 31, 73–87.
Spreen, O. , Borkowski, J. G. , & Benton, A. L. Auditory recognition in relation to
meaningfulness, abstractness, and phonetic structure of words. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 101–104.
Spreen, O. , & Schulz, R. W. Parameters of abstraction, meaningfulness, and
pronunciability for 329 nouns. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5,
459–468.
Staats, A. W. Verbal habit families, concepts, and the operant conditioning of word classes.
Psychological Review, 1961, 68, 190–204.
Staats, A. W. Learning, language, and cognition. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston,
1968.
Staats, A. W. Integrated functional learning theory and language development. In D. I.
Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar: Facts and theories. New York: Academic Press,
in press.
Staats, A. W. , & Staats, C. K. Attitudes established by classical conditioning. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 57, 37–40.
Staats, A. W. , & Staats, C. K. Meaning and ra: Correlated but separate. Psychological
Review, 1959, 66, 136–144.
Staats, A. W. , Staats, C. K. , & Heard, W. G. Language conditioning of meaning to
meaning using a semantic generalization paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1959, 57, 187–192.
Staats, C. K. , & Staats, A. W. Meaning established by classical conditioning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1957, 54, 74–80.
Standing, L. , Conezio, J. , & Haber, R. N. Perception and memory for pictures: Single-trial
learning of 2560 visual stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 19, 73–74.
Standing, L. , Sell, C. , Boss, J. , & Haber, R. N. Effect of visualization and subvocalization
on perceptual clarity. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 18, 89–90.
Steinfeld, G. J. Concepts of set and availability and their relation to the reorganization of
ambiguous pictorial stimuli. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 505–522.
Sternberg, S. Two operations in character recognition: Some evidence from reactiontime
measurements. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 45–53.
Sternberg, S. The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Paper
presented at the Donders Centenary Symposium on reaction-time. Instituut Voor Perceptie
Onderzoek, Eindhoven, Netherlands, 1968.
Stevenson, C. L. Ethics and language. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1944.
Stewart, J. C. An experimental investigation of imagery. Unpublished doctoral thesis,
University of Toronto, 1965.
Stoke, S. M. Memory for onomatopes. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1929, 36, 594–596.
Stolz, W. S. A study of the ability to decode grammatically novel sentences. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 867–873.
Stoyva, J. , & Kamiya, J. Electrophysiological studies of dreaming as the prototype of a new
strategy in the study of consciousness. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 192–205.
Street, R. F. A Gestalt completion test: A study of a cross section of intellect. New York:
Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1931.
Stromeyer, C. F. , & Psotka, J. The detailed texture of eidetic images. Nature, 1970, 225,
346–349.
Stroop, J. R. Interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1935, 18, 643–661.
Stumberg, D. A study of poetic talent. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1928, 11,
219–234.
Supa, M. , Cotzin, M. , & Dallenbach, K. M. “Facial vision”: The perception of obstacles by
the blind. American Journal of Psychology, 1944, 57, 133–183.
Suzuki, N. S. , & Rohwer, W. D., Jr. Verbal facilitation of paired-associate learning: Type of
grammatical unit vs. connective form class. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1968, 7, 584–588.
Taylor, A. M. , & Black, H. B. Variables affecting imagery instruction in children. Research
Monograph No. 6, Audio-Visual Center, Indiana University, April, 1969.
Taylor, A. M. , Josberger, M. , & Prentice, J. L. Imagery organization and children’s recall.
Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Convention, March,
1970.
Taylor, J. A. Meaning, frequency, and visual duration threshold. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1958, 55, 329–334.
Taylor, J. G. The behavioral basis of perception. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1962.
Taylor, R. L. , & Posner, M. I. Retrieval from visual and verbal memory codes. Paper
presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, May,
1968.
Tees, R. C. , & More, L. K. Effect of amount of perceptual learning upon disappearances
observed under reduced stimulation conditions. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2,
565–568. (a)
Tees, R. C. , & More, L. K. Visual disappearances under simplified stimulus conditions
caused by auditory perceptual learning. Perception and Psychophysics, 1967, 2, 627–629.
(b)
Tejirian, E. Syntactic and semantic structure in the recall of orders of approximation to
English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 1010–1015.
Terrace, H. The effects of retinal locus and attention on the perception of words. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1959, 58, 382–385.
Thorndike, E. L. On the function of visual images. Journal of Philosophy, 1907, 4, 324–327.
Thorndike, E. L. Educational psychology. Volume III. New York: Teachers College,
Columbia University, 1914.
Thorndike, E. L. , & Lorge, I. The teacher’s word book of 30,000 words. New York: Bureau
of Publications, Teachers College, 1944.
Thurstone, L. L. Primary mental abilities. Psychometrika Monographs, 1938, No. 1.
Thurstone, L. L. , & Jeffrey, T. E. Flags: A test of space thinking. Chicago: Education
Industry Service, 1956.
Titchener, E. B. Lectures on the experimental psychology of thought processes. New York:
Macmillan, 1909.
Tolman, E. C. More concerning the temporal relations of meaning and imagery.
Psychological Review, 1917, 24, 114–138.
Tolman, E. C. Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century, 1932.
Tolman, E. C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 1948, 55, 189–208.
Tolman, E. C. Principles of purposive behavior. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A Study of a
science. Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
Tomkins, S. S. Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. 1. New York: Springer, 1962.
Tulving, E. Subjective organization in free recall of “unrelated” words. Psychological
Review, 1962, 69, 344–354.
Tulving, E. Intratrial and intertrial retention: Notes towards a theory of free recall verbal
learning. Psychological Review, 1964, 71, 219–237.
Tulving, E. Subjective organization and effects of repetition in multitrial free-recall learning.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 193–197.
Tulving, E. Theoretical issues in free recall. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal
behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J., Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Tulving, E. , & Gold, C. Stimulus information and contextual information as determinants of
tachistoscopic recognition of words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66,
319–327.
Tulving, E. , McNulty, J. A. , & Ozier, M. Vividness of words and learning to learn in free-
recall learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1965, 19, 242–252.
Tulving, E. , & Osier, S. Transfer effects in whole/part free-recall learning. Canadian Journal
of Psychology, 1967, 21, 253–262.
Tulving, E. , & Osier, S. Effectiveness of retrieval cues in memory for words. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 593–601.
Tulving, E. , & Patkau, J. E. Concurrent effects of contextual constraint and word frequency
on immediate recall and learning of verbal material. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1962,
16, 83–95.
Tulving, E. , & Pearlstone, Z. Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for
words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 381–391.
Turner, E. A. , & Rommetveit, R. Focus of attention in recall of active and passive
sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 543–548.
Turvey, M. T. Analysis of augmented recall in short-term memory following a shift in
connotation. British Journal of Psychology, 1968, 59, 131–137.
Tversky, B. Pictorial and verbal encoding in a short-term memory task. Perception and
Psychophysics, 1969, 6, 225–233.
Uhlenbeck, E. M. Some further remarks on transformational grammar. Lingua, 1967, 17,
263–316.
Underwood, B. J. An evaluation of the Gibson theory of verbal learning. In C. N. Cofer
(Ed.), Verbal learning and verbal behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.
Underwood, B. J. Stimulus selection in verbal learning. In C. N. Cofer and B. S. Musgrave
(Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning: Problems and processes. New York: McGraw-Hill,
1963.
Underwood, B. J. , The representativeness of rote verbal learning. In A. W. Melton (Ed.),
Categories of human learning. New York: Academic Press, 1964.
Underwood, B. J. False recognition produced by implicit verbal responses. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 122–129.
Underwood, B. J. Attributes of memory. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 559–573.
Underwood, B. J. , & Ekstrand, B. R. An analysis of some shortcomings in the interference
theory of forgetting. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 540–549.
Underwood, B. J. , Ekstrand, B. R. , & Keppel, G. An analysis of intralist similarity in verbal
learning with experiments on conceptual similarity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 1965, 4, 447–462.
Underwood, B. J. , & Erlebacher, A. Studies of coding in verbal learning. Psychological
Monographs, 1965, 79 (13, Whole No. 606).
Underwood, B. J. , & Freund, J. S. Testing effects in recognition of words. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 117–125.
Underwood, B. J. , & Keppel, G. Coding processes in verbal learning. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 1, 250–257.
Underwood, B. J. , & Postman, L. Extraexperimental sources of interference in forgetting.
Psychological Review, 1960, 67, 73–95.
Underwood, B. J. , & Richardson, J. Some verbal materials for the study of concept
formation. Psychological Bulletin, 1956, 53, 84–95.
Underwood, B. J. , & Schulz, R. W. Meaningfulness and verbal learning. Chicago:
Lippincott, 1960.
Uznadze, D. N. The psychology of set. Translated by B. Haigh . New York: Consultants’
Bureau, 1966.
Vanderplas, J. M. , & Garvin, E. A. The association value of random shapes. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 147–154. (a)
Vanderplas, J. M. , & Garvin, E. A. Complexity, association value, and practice as factors in
shape recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 155–163. (b)
Vanderplas, J. M. , Sanderson, W. A. , & Vanderplas, J. N. Some task-related determinants
of transfer in perceptual learning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1964, 18, 71–80.
Vernon, M. Relationship of language to the thinking process. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 1967, 16, 325–333.
Walker, H. J. Imagery ratings for 338 nouns. Behavior Research Methods and
Instrumentation, 1970, 2, 165–167.
Walkup, L. E. Creativity in science through visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965,
21, 35–41.
Wallace, W. H. , Turner, S. H. , & Perkins, C. C. Preliminary studies of human information
storage. Signal Corps Project No. 1320, Institute for Cooperative Research, University of
Pennsylvania, 1957.
Wallace, W. P. Review of the historical, empirical, and theoretical status of the von Restorff
phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 1965, 63, 410–424.
Wallace, W. P. Implicit associative response occurrence in learning with retarded subjects:
a supplementary report. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1967, 58, 110–114.
Wallace, W. P. , & Underwood B. J. Implicit responses and the role of intralist similarity in
verbal learning by normal and retarded subjects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1964,
55, 362–370.
Wallach, H. , & Averbach, E. On memory modalities. American Journal of Psychology,
1955, 68, 249–257.
Wallach, H. , O'Connell, D. N. , & Neisser, U. The memory effect on visual perception of
three-dimensional form. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1953, 45, 360–368.
Walter, R. D. , & Yeager, C. L. Visual imagery and electroencephalographic changes.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1956, 8, 193–199.
Wanner, H. E. On remembering, forgetting, and understanding sentences: A study of the
deep structure hypothesis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, 1968.
Wapner, S. , & Werner, H. Perceptual development. Worcester, Mass.: Clark University
Press, 1957.
Warren, R. M. , Obusek, C. J. , Farmer, R. M. , & Warren, R. P. Auditory sequence:
Confusion of patterns other than speech or music. Science, 1969, 164, 586–587.
Watson, J. B. Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 1913, 20,
158–177.
Watson, J. B. Behaviorism. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press, 1930.
Watson, J. D. The double helix. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson, 1968.
Waugh, N. C. , & Norman, D. A. Primary memory. Psychological Review, 1965, 72, 89–104.
Wearing, A. J. Vividness in the recall of English nominalizations. Psychonomic Science,
1971, 22, 121–122.
Weaver, G. E. , Hopkins, R. H. , & Schulz, R. W. The A-B, B-C, A-C mediation paradigm: A-
C performance in the absence of study trials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968,
77, 670–675.
Weber, R. J. , & Bach, M. Visual and speech imagery. British Journal of Psychology, 1969,
60, 199–202.
Weber, R. J. & Castleman, J. The time it takes to imagine. Perception and Psychophysics,
1970, 8, 165–168.
Weisstein, N. A Rashevsky-Landahl neural net: Simulation of meta-contrast. Psychological
Review, 1968, 75, 494–521.
Werner, H. , & Kaplan, B. Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to the
psychology of language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley, 1963.
Whorf, B. L. Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge: Technology Press, 1956.
Wickelgren, W. A. Size of rehearsal group and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1964, 68, 413–419.
Wickelgren, W. A. Acoustic similarity and intrusion errors in short-term memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 102–108.
Wickelgren W. A. Auditory or articulatory coding in verbal short-term memory.
Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 232–235.
Wickens, D. D. Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach to meaning.
Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 1–15.
Wickens, D. D. , Born, D. G. , & Allen, C. K. Proactive inhibition and item similarity in short-
term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 440–445.
Wickens, D. D. , & Clark, S. Osgood dimensions as an encoding class in short-term
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 580–584.
Wickens, D. D. , & Engle, R. W. Imagery and abstractness in short-term memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1970, 84, 268–272.
Wicker, F. W. On the locus of picture-word differences in paired-associate learning. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 52–57. (a)
Wicker, F. W. Photographs, drawings, and nouns as stimuli in paired-associate learning.
Psychonomic Science, 1970, 18, 205–206. (b)
Wilgosh, L. R. The interaction between pictures and their labels in the memory of four-year-
old children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, McMaster University, 1970.
Williams, J. R. , & Levin, I. P. Mediated facilitation in a four-stage chaining paradigm.
Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12, 281–282.
Wimer, C. An analysis of semantic stimulus factors in paired-associate learning. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 1, 397–407.
Wimer, C. , & Lambert, W. E. The differential effects of word and object stimuli on the
learning of paired associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 31–36.
Wind, M. , & Davidson, M. Facilitation of paired-associate learning by language context.
Psychonomic Science, 1969, 15, 184–185.
Wing, J. F. Stimulus-trace model of serial acquisition and retention. Proceedings of the 75th
Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 1967, 59–60.
Winnick, W. A. , & Bruder, G. E. Signal detection approach to the study of retinal locus in
tachistoscopic recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 528–530.
Winnick, W. A. , & Kressel, K. Tachistoscopic recognition thresholds, paired-associate
learning, and immediate recall as a function of abstractness-concreteness and word
frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 163–168.
Winograd, E. , Karchmer, M. A. , & Russell, I. S. Role of encoding unitization in cued
recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, in press.
Wirtz, J. & Anisfeld, M. Imagery, synonymity, antonymy, and false recognition of phrases.
Unpublished manuscript, Yeshiva University, 1970.
Witmer, L. R. The association value of three-place consonant syllables. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 1935, 47, 337–359.
Wollen, K. A. Effects of relevant or irrelevant pictorial mediators upon forward and
backward recall. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis,
November, 1968.
Wollen, K. A. Variables that determine the effectiveness of picture mediators in paired-
associate learning. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis,
November, 1969.
Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958.
Wood, G. Mnemonic systems in recall. Journal of Educational Psychology Monographs,
1967, 58 (6, Pt. 2).
Woodworth, R. S. Experimental psychology. New York: Holt, 1938.
Woodworth, R. S. , & Schlosberg, H. Experimental psychology. New York: Holt, 1954.
Worchel, P. Space perception and orientation in the blind. Psychological Monographs,
1951, 65 (15).
Wynne, R. D. , & Cofer, C. N. On meaning, association, and transfer. Unpublished
manuscript, 1958.
Yarmey, A. D. Word abstractness, awareness, and type of design in verbal mediation.
Psychology, 1967, 4, 24–32.
Yarmey, A. D. The person-image and social mediated learning. Psychological Record,
1969, 19, 123–127.
Yarmey, A. D. , & Csapo, K. G. Imaginai and verbal mediation instructions and stimulus
attributes in paired-associate learning. Psychological Record, 1968, 18, 191–199.
Yarmey, A. D. , & O'Neill, B. J. S-R and R-S paired-associate learning as a function of
concreteness, imagery, specificity, and association value. Journal of Psychology, 1969, 71,
95–109.
Yarmey, A. D. , & Paivio, A. Further evidence on the effects of word abstractness and
meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 2, 307–308.
Yates, F. A. The art of memory. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1966.
Yin, R. K. Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 81,
141–145.
Yngve, V. A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American
Philosophical Society, 1960, 104, 444–466.
Yntema, D. B. , & Trask, F. P. Recall as a search process. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 65–74.
Young, R. K. Tests of three hypotheses about the effective stimulus in serial learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 63, 307–313.
Young, R. K. Serial learning. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and
general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968.
Yuille, J. C. Concreteness without imagery in paired-associate learning. Psychonomic
Science, 1968, 11, 55–56.
Yuille, J. C. , & Barnsley, R. H. Visual, auditory, and tactual imagery in paired associate
learning. Paper read at the 19th International Congress of Psychology, London, England,
July, 1969.
Yuille, J. C. , & Paivio, A. Latency of imaginai and verbal mediators as a function of stimulus
and response concreteness-imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 75,
540–544.
Yuille, J. C. , & Paivio, A. Imagery and verbal mediation instructions in paired-associate
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 436–441.
Yuille, J. C. , & Paivio, A. Abstractness and recall of connected discourse. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1969, 82, 467–471.
Yuille, J. C. , Paivio, A. , & Lambert, W. E. Noun and adjective imagery and order in paired-
associate learning by French and English subjects. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1969,
23, 459–466.
Yuille, J. C. , & Pritchard, S. Noun concreteness and verbal facilitation as factors in imaginai
mediation and paired-associate learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 1969, 7, 459–466.
Zajonc, R. B. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology Monograph Supplement, 1968, 9, 1–27.
Zigler, M. J. , & Northrup, K. M. The tactual perception of form. American Journal of
Psychology, 1926, 37, 391–397.
Zuckerman, C. B. , & Rock, I. A reappraisal of the roles of past experience and innate
organizing processes in visual perception. Psychological Bulletin, 1957, 54, 269–296.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy