ШКИЉО Ервин - Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
ШКИЉО Ервин - Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
ШКИЉО Ервин - Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Prepared by
ARRB Group
Austroads membership comprises the six state and two territory road transport and
traffic authorities, the Commonwealth Department of Infrastructure and Transport, the
Australian Local Government Association, and NZ Transport Agency. Austroads is governed
by a Board consisting of the chief executive officer (or an alternative senior executive officer)
of each of its eleven member organisations:
2|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
CONTENTS
1. INTRODUCTION.................................................................................................................- 6 -
1.1. BACKGROUND.....................................................................................................................- 6 -
1.2. PURPOSE AND OUTLINE OF THE PROJECT............................................................................- 6 -
2. METHOD..............................................................................................................................- 7 -
4.1. INTRODUCTION..................................................................................................................- 16 -
4.2. THE NATURE OF ATTENTION............................................................................................- 16 -
4.2.1. AUTOMATIC CAPTURE OF ATTENTION...........................................................................- 16 -
4.2.2. ATTENTIONAL BIASES....................................................................................................- 17 -
4.2.3. LIMITED CAPACITY OF ATTENTION...............................................................................- 18 -
4.3. PERCEPTUAL ISSUES.........................................................................................................- 18 -
4.4. SUMMARY.........................................................................................................................- 19 -
3|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
6.1.5. CONTENT........................................................................................................................- 25 -
6.2. MENTAL WORKLOAD........................................................................................................- 25 -
6.2.1. VISUAL CLUTTER...........................................................................................................- 25 -
6.2.2. DRIVING DEMAND..........................................................................................................- 25 -
6.2.3. CONTENT........................................................................................................................- 25 -
6.3. GAZE DIRECTION..............................................................................................................- 26 -
6.3.1. OFFSET...........................................................................................................................- 26 -
6.3.2. ELEVATION.....................................................................................................................- 26 -
6.4. ROAD ENVIRONMENT........................................................................................................- 26 -
6.4.1. CRASH RATE ASSESSMENT............................................................................................- 26 -
6.4.2. RISK ASSESSMENT..........................................................................................................- 26 -
7. CURRENT GUIDELINES.................................................................................................- 27 -
8. GAP ANALYSIS................................................................................................................- 28 -
9. REFERENCES....................................................................................................................- 29 -
4|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
TABLES
Table 3.1. Conventional billboard and poster formats used in outdoor advertising...............- 9 -
Table 7.1. Key jurisdiction and/or planning authority and industry guidance documents for
roadside advertising..............................................................................................................- 27 -
FIGURES
Figure 3.1. Examples of a freestanding billboard (top), wall-mounted billboard (left) and
bridge-mounted billboard (right)_______________________________________________- 9 -
Figure 3.2: Examples of a mobile billboard (left) and a billboard displayed as part of a bus
shelter (right)_____________________________________________________________- 10 -
Figure 3.3. Examples of digital billboards located within the boundaries of (left) or visible
from (right) state-controlled roads_____________________________________________- 12 -
Figure 3.4. Example of a variable message sign displaying advertising content__________- 13 -
Figure 3.5. Example of an externally illuminated advertising device (left) and an internally
illuminated advertising device (right)__________________________________________- 15 -
Figure 6.1. Austroads Safe Systems diagram____________________________________- 23 -
5|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
1. INTRODUCTION
1.1. Background
Australia’s new National Road Safety Strategy notes that, ‘Driving is a complex task
and sources of driver distraction, both within the vehicle and in the general road environment,
have increased substantially in recent years’ (National Road Safety Strategy 2011–2020, p.
83). While it is recognised that inattentive driving is a contributor to road crashes and that
roadside advertising may be one of the contributors to such inattention, criteria for the
management of roadside advertising devices vary considerably between jurisdictions.
In a number of jurisdictions, responsibility for the criteria resides with the planning
agency, while in others it is a road agency function. A number of road agencies have
sponsored projects to better inform themselves about the safety implications of outdoor
advertising, which also has contributed to variations in jurisdictional practice. Given that the
income derived from outdoor advertising can be significant, particularly on high volume
corridors, the state practice guidelines are able to be ‘played off’ against each other by the
outdoor advertising industry.
This project is designed to facilitate the harmonisation of agency criteria for the
management of roadside advertising devices and promote improved and consistent practice by
road agencies. Most importantly, it will assist road agencies to understand and address a
significant emerging safety issue – the use of digital display technology for outdoor
advertising signs.
review the human factors elements relevant to understanding the possible safety
implications of roadside advertising
develop ‘best practice’ guiding principles and guidelines for the placement of
outdoor advertising signs.
6|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
7|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
2. METHOD
The research method included extensive desktop research, including internet, library
and database searches to locate all relevant material (English language only).
This process was conducted with the aid of the M.G. Lay Library. The M.G. Lay
Library contains the most comprehensive and up-to-date collection of international literature
on land transport issues (particularly roads) in Australia, and is one of the leading technical
libraries in its field in the world.
The library is staffed by a team of full time professionally qualified staff. The ARRB
Group library has close contact with major libraries both in Australia and overseas, for
example the Library at the UK Transport Research Laboratory. Inter-library loans are easily
arranged, or document abstracts in other libraries can be accessed via on-line communications.
The Australian Transport Index (ATRI), the International Transport Research Documentation
database (ITRD), and the Transportation Research Information Services (TRIS) were all
searched for relevant information.
The database contains information from 40 major technical institutes from 24 countries
and more than 350 000 references including an informative abstract. TRIS is a database
prepared by the US Transportation Research Board and covers all modes of transport. It
includes publications and descriptions of research projects and contains over 450 000
references.
8|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
These devices refer to large advertising signs, greater than 4 m2, with messages that
incorporate words, symbols or pictorial displays and are printed on paper or alternative
materials such as computer generated woven polyester panels or ‘skins’. As illustrated in
Figure 3.1, the advertising copy may be mounted on freestanding structures or attached to
building walls, roofs and overhead transport infrastructure (e.g. bridges and overpasses). The
messages displayed on these conventional devices do not change unless manually replaced on-
site.
A variety of sizes are used; the 24 Sheet poster, or traditional ‘Billboard’, is the most
frequently used format in outdoor advertising. The messages may be illuminated through
external power sources, although this does not usually achieve the same perceived brightness
as the digital billboards described in Section 3.2.2. Table 3.1 provides an overview of the
different formats of poster used in roadside advertising in Australia.
9|Page
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Figure 3.1. Examples of a freestanding billboard (top), wall-mounted billboard (left) and bridge-
mounted billboard (right)
Table 3.1. Conventional billboard and poster formats used in outdoor advertising
10 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Other contexts in which conventional billboard or poster formats are displayed are
described below and illustrated in Figure 3.2.
Mobile/portable billboards
Mobile or portable billboards generally consist of posters mounted on small
commercial vehicles or trailers, sometimes illuminated and with two-sided displays. The
vehicle remains motionless while the advertisement is displayed.
Figure 3.2: Examples of a mobile billboard (left) and a billboard displayed as part of a bus shelter
(right)
Other
There are numerous other miscellaneous formats of non-changeable advertising devices
that are commonly used on the road network, although often prohibited on some roads such as
freeways and motorways. These include but are not limited to:
local business, community and event signs
real estate signs
tourist information signs
banners and flags
paintings or murals on building walls
building wrap and hoarding
transit displays (i.e. on moving vehicles such as buses, trams and taxis)
aerial displays.
11 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
3.2.1. Mechanically-changed
These devices allow the presentation of two or more static messages that are rotated
mechanically (i.e. by a motor) through a pre-determined sequence at regular intervals, while
the supporting structure remains stationary. There are motionless periods in between the
presentation of different messages and the number of messages that can be displayed is
restricted. Unlike electronic devices, the change between advertising messages cannot be
instantaneous.
Trivision
In trivision devices, messages are printed onto a series of adjacent vertical prisms
(usually three-sided), which when aligned display a single advertising image. The prisms are
rotated in unison, typically every four to ten seconds, to show one of three messages. They are
also referred to as ‘tri-action’, ‘tri-panel’ or ‘changing slat’ signs. These devices are typically 3
m x 6 m or 12 m x 3 m in dimension.
Multi-advertisement scrolling
Also referred to as rolling devices, these devices have multiple advertisements printed
onto a looped canvas or connected to form a single scroll. The scroll is usually wound around
a vertical axis using a motorised spool, so that the adverts are sequentially presented in the
front display panel. These are often smaller signs installed at street level or incorporated into
public transport infrastructure such as bus stops. The advertisements are often illuminated or
backlit.
3.2.2. Electronically-changed
These devices use digital technology to display bright, high quality electronic images
which are uploaded and changed using a computer and modem via a secure network. Digital
billboards feature LED (light emitting diode) technology which enables luminance to be
controlled and adjusted automatically. Within Australia, Victoria was the first state to permit
these types of signs on its road network.
Digital billboards
Similar to conventional billboards, digital billboards are generally large signs with
dimensions greater than 4 m2 displaying messages which incorporate text, symbols and other
pictorial or graphical images.
12 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
13 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Digital billboards can utilise static electronic displays or non-static electronic displays.
They are also known by a large variety of terms including electronic billboards, electronic
message displays, dynamic message signs, commercial electronic variable message signs,
video billboards and moving or animated electronic signs.
Static electronic displays contain static images only which are presented successively
but do not contain or imply motion within the message itself. The device is
programmed to alternate the static images at short intervals. Dwell time, transition time
and luminance can all be controlled and changed electronically. Different approaches
can be taken to the transition between messages e.g. scroll, dissolve, fade or fly-in. In
the USA, typical dwell times for digital billboards range between four and ten seconds
(with restrictions on proximity to entry and exit ramps), with transition times varying
between instantaneous to four seconds (OMA 2010). These times are comparable to
standards in other countries such as Canada and the UK. Some Australian states
however currently utilise longer dwell times, as detailed in Section 7.
Figure 3.3. Examples of digital billboards located within the boundaries of (left) or visible from (right)
state-controlled roads
14 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
VMS are primarily used by road authorities with the purpose to present messages to
motorists to facilitate more effective management of traffic and to promote road safety. VMS
have the capability to present text and/or graphical displays.
In Australia, traffic VMS are generally static electronic text-only displays and are most
commonly used to display a single message for a significant period of time. Road agencies
usually prescribe detailed specifications regarding the format and content of these signs,
including size of text, use of colour and permitted words. Messages may be tactical (e.g.
incident warnings) or advisory (e.g. safe driving advice or journey time information). Under
normal traffic conditions, when there is no need for a safety-critical instruction, the road
authority may authorise use of the sign to display other information which may have relevance
for both traffic management and advertising; for example, details of upcoming special events.
VMS can also be used solely for commercial advertising purposes. These are often in
the form of portable devices located on lower speed roads and adjacent to business premises,
as illustrated in Figure 3.4.
15 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
3.3. Illumination
It is important that advertising devices are illuminated appropriately for the ambient
light conditions to ensure there is no unacceptable glare (making it difficult to read the sign
because of excessive external light sources) or reflectance (making the sign itself so bright that
it is distracting) that may result in safety issues for road users or that will produce
unacceptable light spillage to the local environment. Advertising devices can be classified
according to the following definitions:
Externally illuminated devices have an external light source which is used to illuminate
the advertising copy (see Figure 3.6). For example, through the use of fluorescent and/or
incandescent bulbs. They may also be referred to as floodlit signs, and most commonly consist
of conventional billboards and posters.
Internally illuminated devices have internal lighting to illuminate the advertising copy,
see Figure 3.6. This includes digital billboards which use LED technology, as well as devices
which contain lights or illuminated tubes arranged as an advertisement such as neon signs. The
lighting can be adjusted, either automatically using sensors or manually, to match the
appropriate luminance for ambient light conditions.
16 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Figure 3.5. Example of an externally illuminated advertising device (left) and an internally illuminated
advertising device (right)
17 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
While the notion of attention is to some extent synonymous with voluntary, goal-
directed activity, nevertheless it appears that attention may sometimes be captured
involuntarily by certain events. For example, most people would have had the experience of
sudden movement in their peripheral vision resulting in a seemingly automatic orienting in
that direction.
The question for the current purpose is, when and to what extent this is likely to occur.
If one is walking alone on a dark street in a bad neighbourhood then the answer is likely to be;
frequently and dramatically. But what about when it is not important, or not desirable, to
18 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
display such vigilance? What happens when a digital billboard changes or animates in
peripheral vision when driving? Can we avoid being distracted by such stimuli?
In recent years researchers have been investigating to what extent this attentional
capture is outside of voluntary control and what kinds of stimuli give rise to it. This interest
has been driven by purely theoretical considerations, but obviously has important implications
for understanding the distraction potential of various kinds of roadside advertising. While
there is still debate over some of the theoretical subtleties in this research, there are some clear
findings of relevance to the issue of the distraction potential of roadside advertising.
While some early research suggested that the appearance of new objects in the visual
field was the key to predicting attentional capture (e.g. Yantis & Hillstrom 1994), other
research suggested that luminance changes were necessary to capture attention (Theeuwes
1995).
More recent research appears to suggest that the presence of unique sensory transients
may be the key to predicting attentional capture (Hollingworth, Simons & Franconeri 2010).
That is, in order to capture attention there must be a salient change in the environment that
creates a new event in the observer’s sensory system. This could be luminance changes, which
could arise from the appearance of a new object, or motion in a previously immobile object.
With respect to the issue of the extent to which the capture of attention is involuntary;
the research is similarly complicated. While some research appears to show that involuntary
attentional capture by environmental events does occur, other research suggests that this
attentional capture can be suppressed (Yantis & Jonides 1990). The key seems to be that this
suppression is more likely if the primary task is very demanding and requires a focussed
attentional state, but that such suppression becomes less likely as the primary task becomes
less demanding, requiring a less focussed attentional state (Lamy & Tsal 1999; Ruz &
Lupianez 2002). The results of Young et al. (2009) showing poorer recall of road signs
(suggesting greater attention to roadside advertisements) are consistent with this and are
discussed in more detail below.
Most et al. (2005) provided another demonstration of how the emotionality of material
may distract attention away from critical target material. They presented a series of
photographs and asked participants to respond to a particular target. When the target was
19 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
preceded by a photograph with a negative emotional content, participants more often missed
the target than when it was preceded by a neutral photograph.
20 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
This ‘blindness’ was evident up to 800 msec after the presentation of the emotional
photograph. Participants who scored low on harm avoidance were more easily able to modify
their cognitive processing to reduce the induced blindness when given appropriate instructions
than were participants who scored high on harm avoidance.
These considerations suggest that billboards with emotional content have a greater
capacity to attract and hold the attention of individuals for whom that emotional content is
significant, and this may result in decrements in driver performance.
However, drivers can drive quite successfully most of the time while having a
conversation. This is because large chunks of the task of driving are relatively automated
and/or do not draw on the same processing resources. When this is not the case driving
performance is apt to suffer.
Even if billboards do not deflect gaze direction away from the forward roadway, to the
extent that they have captured attention they are likely to reduce the processing capacity
available for other visual information processing required for driving. Furthermore, as Strayer
and Johnston (2001) have shown in the case of mobile phone conversations, some driving-
irrelevant stimuli can sometimes be so engaging that essentially all spare capacity is recruited
to the secondary task, with serious consequences for driving performance. A billboard that
was this engaging would undoubtedly be a serious safety risk for driving.
21 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
In a key finding in this area, Klauer et al. (2006), in an analysis of the 100-Car
Naturalistic Driving Study, found that glances away from the forward roadway for more than
two seconds doubled the near-crash and crash risk compared to baseline. This result is
averaged across all road types and traffic conditions. One can imagine that in challenging road
environments in heavy traffic this risk would be much greater. At 70 km/h a two second
glance away from the forward roadway equates to just under 40 m of travel down the
roadway.
In certain road environments and in heavy traffic it becomes quite likely that conditions
in the forward roadway will have changed over this distance and hence that a driver not
looking ahead will not be able to respond appropriately to these changes.
4.4. Summary
Most drivers, in most driving situations, most of the time, probably possess substantial
spare cognitive capacity for the processing of driving-irrelevant information. Given this, and
given the exploratory nature of human cognition and the likelihood that drivers attempt to
maintain an optimal level of arousal via task difficulty homeostasis (Fuller 2005), it may be
very difficult to prevent drivers from directing attention away from the driving task (Trick &
Enns 2009).
The key question is whether there are situations or individuals where processing is
recruited or interfered with by driving-irrelevant material to the detriment of driving
performance. The considerations reviewed above suggest that the answer to this is in the
affirmative.
22 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
However, this analysis does not directly answer the question of whether roadside
advertising is actually distracting in any real driving environments, to such an extent that it
leads to reduced safety and contributes to crashes. In order to evaluate this issue further the
review below first discusses the evidence for the involvement of distraction in crashes and
then the evidence for the involvement of roadside advertising in distraction and crashes.
In one of the most compelling studies to date, Klauer et al. (2006) analysed the
consequences of driver inattention using data from the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study.
While brief glances away from the forward roadway for the purpose of scanning the driving
environment were found to actually decrease the crash risk, glances of two seconds or more
doubled the crash risk. In addition, this risk was further increased for certain demanding traffic
environments such as intersections and high density traffic.
Some of the riskiest kinds of inattentive driving that contributed to crashes and near
crashes in the Klauer et al. (2006) study originated from either drowsiness or in-vehicle
distractions. Importantly, looking at an external object exhibited the second highest significant
odds ratio of all distractions, (reaching for a moving object produced the highest significant
odds ratio) with a driver 3.7 times more likely to have a crash or near crash when looking at an
external object.
However this kind of distraction accounted for less than 1% of all crashes and near
crashes in the study. Thus while looking at an external object appears to be quite risky
behaviour when it is engaged in, it is not a frequent cause of crashes overall.
23 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Crundall et al. (2006) showed participants in their study video clips taken from the
driver’s perspective and asked them to either scan for hazards only or to look for
advertisements also. Advertisements were either at street level or raised 3 m above street level.
The core finding from this study was that street level advertisements attracted more attention
than raised advertisements when drivers were instructed to look for hazards. Crundall et al.
(2006) suggest that this occurs because street level advertisements fall within the normal
window within which drivers habitually scan for hazards and that advertisements within this
window are inappropriately capturing attention.
Of course this study is somewhat removed from the experience of actually driving,
simply requiring, as it does, that drivers passively watch a video (although note that Crundall
et al. (2006) discuss why there is good reason to believe that their methodology in this study
appropriately taps the key aspects of the driving task).
This concern does not arise in the study by Lee, McElheny and Gibbons (2007). In this
naturalistic study drivers drove an instrumented vehicle around a 50 mile loop in Cleveland
Ohio. They found that drivers took longer glances at digital billboards than at conventional
billboards and baseline sites. While there has been some criticism of their methodology and
conclusions (Wachtel 2009) it would be agreed by all parties that Lee, McElheny and
Gibbon’s results show that in real world driving, digital billboards can be more distracting
than conventional billboards.
The negative impact of roadside advertising on lateral control has also been reported by
Bendak and Al-Saleh (2010) in their simulator study. While the frequency of ‘crashes’ in
Young et al.’s study was too low for statistical analysis, it is worth noting that there were three
times as many crashes in the presence of billboards compared to driving conditions where
billboards were absent. Interestingly, they also found that participants displayed significantly
poorer recall of traffic control in the motorway and rural driving conditions, compared to
urban driving conditions, suggesting that participants were spending more time processing
advertisements in these less demanding driving scenarios, at the expense of attending to
information that is important for safe driving.
24 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
25 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
In recent times, very few studies have attempted to investigate the impact of roadside
advertising on actual crash rates. Smiley et al. (2005) investigated the impact of video
advertising in Toronto on driving performance in a series of studies, including a before – after
installation comparison of crash rates. While Smiley et al. found no statistically significant
effect on crash rates overall, they note that sample sizes were not large enough to detect any
effect that might accrue from the presence of the billboards. The descriptive statistics in this
study however, are consistent with a relative increase in collisions, of all the various types, at
the approaches to the video advertising sites.
There are a number of much older studies investigating the effect of roadside
advertising on crash rates, but of course these do not deal with modern digital technology. In a
review of these older studies, Wallace (2003) concluded that, while many are correlational,
thus making it difficult to unambiguously attribute causality, nevertheless, ‘the case for
arguing that visual ‘clutter’ at junctions (associated with billboards and signs) can lead to
unsafe driving is very strong.
5.3. Summary
There is compelling evidence that distraction is a major contributor to crashes.
However, studies providing direct evidence that roadside advertising plays a significant role in
these distraction based crashes are currently not available. The studies that have been
conducted show convincingly that roadside advertising is distracting and that it may lead to
poorer vehicle control. However, the evidence is presently only suggestive of, although clearly
consistent with, the notion that this in turn results in crashes.
It is also worth noting, on the basis of Klauer et al.’s (2006) results, that while looking
at an external object increased the crash risk by nearly four times, less than 1% of all crashes
and near crashes were from this source of distraction. A substantial proportion of these
external objects would not have been advertising signs. Thus, while it is not possible to tell
from the reported results, it is reasonable to conclude that far less than 1% of all crashes and
near crashes involved distraction from roadside advertising.
26 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
27 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
28 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
However, as noted earlier, the human factors issues are not straightforward when
attempting to be definitive about what is and is not desirable from a distraction perspective.
Firstly, in some environments, some level of appropriate roadside ‘distraction’ may be
desirable. Secondly, it seems very likely that if drivers are not completely engaged by the
driving environment they will spontaneously engage in other ‘distracting’ activities. Finally, it
appears that in many cases drivers regulate their engagement with potentially distracting
stimuli so that its distraction potential is controlled to some extent.
This does not mean that roadside advertising is of no concern, but it does mean that
there are situations where it is unlikely to compromise the integrity of the Safe System. The
key is to specify the principles that are important in determining those situations.
Based on the human factors issues and the specific research outlined above, the
following principles should be considered when formulating guidelines for the approval and
placement of roadside advertising.
6.1.1. Movement
The potential for sudden movement and change in the environment to capture attention
in a way that is outside volition suggests that digital billboards should not display moving or
flashing images (or lighting) or change in a way that produces an impression of movement.
6.1.4. Luminance
Signs that have luminance levels that are high relative to other objects in the
environment are likely to gain preferential attention and be particularly good at capturing
attention when they change. As a result, digital signs should have luminance levels no greater
than any other sign and preferably lower than non-changeable signs.
29 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
6.1.5. Content
As some content, particularly emotional content, can capture attention automatically, it
is undesirable for such content to be used in roadside advertising. For a similar reason, content
that mimics the content of traffic signs would also be undesirable.
6.2.3. Content
The greater the quantity of information in an advertising display, the longer it will take
to process and hence the longer a driver’s eyes will be off the road. This suggests that the
informational load of the advertising message should be minimised as much as possible so that
the content can be processed as rapidly as possible. This will minimise the time during which
drivers’ eyes are off the road.
30 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
This consideration suggests that roadside advertising is best located in the line of sight
of the forward roadway, provided that it does not obscure or background critical other signage,
signals or infrastructure. The following principles are suggested.
6.3.1. Offset
Roadside advertising should not be substantially offset from the travel lane it is desired
to be viewed from as this could move gaze direction away from the forward roadway.
6.3.2. Elevation
Roadside advertising should not be elevated to the extent that it draws gaze away from
the forward roadway.
31 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
7. CURRENT GUIDELINES
Table 7.1. Key jurisdiction and/or planning authority and industry guidance documents for roadside
advertising
32 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
8. GAP ANALYSIS
While many of the best practice principles identified are covered in at least some of the
road authorities’ existing guidelines, a number of issues are not yet considered. In particular,
the following issues typically are not covered:
specification of appropriate dwell and transition times relative to road speed limit or
speed environment as well as the sight distance to the device
restrictions in relation to the use of:
- special effects for transition between messages e.g. ‘fade’, ‘zoom’ or ‘fly-in’
effects
- message sequencing e.g. the maximum number of sequential messages
permitted and/or the minimum and maximum time duration for the entire
sequence
- emotive content – although this may be covered more comprehensively in
general guidelines for advertising content, it is not typically being considered in
relation to the impact on road safety
- audio, interactive or personalised electronic message displays
33 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
9. REFERENCES
AASHTO 1989. Roadside design guide, American Association of State Highway and
Transportation Officials, Washington, DC, USA.
Austroads 2011, Innovative road safety measures to address fatigue: review of
research and results from a treatment trial, AP-R379/11, Austroads, Sydney, New South
Wales, Australia.
Australian Transport Council 2009, National road safety action plan: 2009 and 2010,
ATSB, Canberra, ACT.
Bendak, S & Al-Saleh, K 2010, ‘The role of roadside advertising signs in distracting
drivers’, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 40, no.3, pp. 233-36.
Chattington, M, Reed, N, Basacik, D, Flint, A & Parkes, A 2009, Investigating driver
distraction: the effects of video and static advertising: a driving simulator study, report 409,
TRL, Crowthorne, UK.
Crundall, D, Van Loon, E & Underwood, G 2006, ‘Attraction and distraction of
attention with roadside advertisements’, Accident Analysis & Prevention, vol. 38, no. 4, pp.
671-7.
Department of Planning NSW 2007, Transport corridor outdoor advertising and
signage guidelines: assessing development applications under SEPP 64, Department of
Planning, Sydney, NSW.
Department of Planning and Community Development Vic 2007, Advisory committee
reviewing advertising sign provisions in Victorian planning schemes: issues and options
paper, Advisory Committee, DPCD, Melbourne, Vic.
Dingus, T.A., Antin, J.F., Hulse, M.C., and Wierwille, W.W. 1989, Attentional
Demand Requirements of an Automobile Moving-Map Navigation System, Transportation
Research, 23A ( 4), 301-315
Edquist, J, Horberry, T, Hosking, S & Johnston, I 2011, ‘Effects of advertising
billboards during simulated driving’, Applied Ergonomics, vol. 42, no. 4, pp. 619-26.
Edquist, J, Hosking, S, Horberry, T & Johnston, I, n.d., ‘Investigating the effects of
visual clutter in road environments’.
Franconeri, SL & Simons, DJ 2003, ‘Moving and looming stimuli capture attention’,
Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 65, no. 7, pp. 999-1010.
Fuller, R 2005, ‘Towards a general theory of driver behaviour’, Accident Analysis &
Prevention, vol. 37, no. 3, pp. 461-72.
Greist-Bousquet, S., Schiffman, N. (1992). The effect of Task interruption and closure
on perceived duration. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30(1), 9-11).
Ho, G, Scialfa, CT, Caird, JK & Graw, T 2001, ‘Visual search for traffic signs: the
effects of clutter, luminance, and aging’, Human Factors, vol. 43, no. 2, pp.194-207.
34 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
35 | P a g e
Impact of roadside advertising on road safety
Roads and Traffic Authority 2011, RTA supplement to the Austroads guide to road
design: part 2 (2006): design considerations, RTA/Pub. 11.091, RTA NSW, Sydney, NSW.
Rosenholtz, R, Li, Y & Nakano, L 2007, ‘Measuring visual clutter’, Journal of Vision,
vol. 7 no. 2, pp. 1-22.
Ruz, M & Lupianez, J 2002, ‘A review of attentional capture: on its automaticity and
sensitivity to endogenous control’, Psicológica, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 283-309.
Shinar, D, Meir, M & Ben-Shoham, I 1998, ‘How automatic is manual gear shifting?’
Human Factors, vol. 40, no. 4, pp.647–54.
Smiley, A, Persaud, B, Bahar, G, Mollett, C, Lyon, C., Smahel, T & Kelman WL 2005,
‘Traffic safety evaluation of video advertising signs’, Transportation Research Record, no.
1937, pp. 105-112.
Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. 2001, Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of
simulated driving and onversing on a cellular phone Psychological Science, 12, 462-466
Stutts, JC, Reinfurt, DW, Staplin, L, Rodgman, EA, 2001, The role of driver distraction
in traffic crashes, AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, Washington, DC, USA.
Theeuwes, J, 1995, ‘Abrupt luminance change pops out; abrupt colour changes does
not’, Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 57, no. 5, pp. 637-44.
Trick, LM & Enns, JT 2009, ‘A two-dimensional framework for understanding the role
of attentional selection in driving’, in Castro, C (ed), Human factors of visual and cognitive
performance in driving, CRC Press, Boca Raton, USA, pp. 63-73.
Wachtel, J 2009, Research for AASHTO Standing Committee on Highways task 256:
safety impacts of the emerging digital display technology for outdoor advertising signs,
National Cooperative Highway Research Program, Washington, DC, USA.
Wallace, B 2003, External-to-vehicle driver distraction, Transport Research Planning
Group, Edinburgh, UK.
Wang, JS, Knipling, RR & Goodman, MJ 1996, ‘The role of driver inattention in
crashes: new statistics from the 1995 crashworthiness data system’, Association for the
advancement of automotive medicine conference, 40th, 1996, Vancouver, Canada, AAAM,
Des Plaines, Illinois, USA, pp. 377-92.
Wickens, CD 2002, ‘Multiple resources and performance prediction’, Theoretical
Issues in Ergonomics Science, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 159-77.
Yantis, S & Hillstrom, AP 1994, ‘Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from
equiluminant visual objects’, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 95-107.
Yantis, S & Jonides, J 1990, ‘Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary
versus automatic allocation’, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 121-34.
Young, MS, Mahfoud, JM, Stanton, NA, Salmon, PM, Jenkins, DP & Walker, GH
2009, ‘Conflicts of interest: the implications of roadside advertising for driver attention’.
36 | P a g e