Ageheuristics - Preprint 2
Ageheuristics - Preprint 2
Ageheuristics - Preprint 2
Author Note
1
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, 417 Chapel Drive, Duke
E-mail: morgan.k.taylor@duke.edu
This work was previously presented at the 2022 Cognitive Aging Conference in Atlanta,
Abstract
In general, research on aging and decision making has grown in recent years. Yet, little
work has investigated how reliance on classic heuristics may differ across adulthood. For
example, younger adults rely on the availability of information from memory when judging the
relative frequency of plane crashes versus car accidents, but it is unclear if older adults are
similarly reliant on this heuristic. In the current study, participants aged 20 to 90 years old made
judgments that could be answered by relying on five different heuristics: anchoring, availability,
differences in the use of the classic heuristics — younger and older adults employed anchoring,
However, replicating past work, we found age-related differences in the sunk cost bias — older
adults were more likely to avoid this fallacy compared to younger adults. We explain these
different patterns by drawing on the distinctive roles stored knowledge and personal experience
People often rely on shortcuts when making decisions. For example, they anchor on the
asking price of a house when making an offer and overestimate the frequency of high profile
events like plane crashes. The current study demonstrates that older adults use these shortcuts at
a similar rate as younger adults. This work highlights that certain decision-making strategies are
preserved with age and is consistent with findings that semantic knowledge remains intact
Younger and older adults alike make many decisions on a daily basis. Some decisions
(e.g., what to eat for breakfast, what to wear to work) are simple and require minimal effort,
while others (e.g., where to invest your money, which medication to take) are more complex and
tap into numerous cognitive resources. In the face of the aging population (Cire, 2016),
researchers have begun to more closely examine how older adults make decisions, especially as
they relate to the preservation or decline of various cognitive resources across adulthood (e.g.,
Finucane et al., 2002; Mata et al., 2015; Strough et al., 2011a; Strough et al., 2020). Much of this
work has focused on older adults’ use of simpler strategies or heuristic processing in general, but
less published work has examined older adults’ usage of classic heuristics (e.g., anchoring,
availability), which younger adults often rely on to make decisions. Here, we investigate whether
there are age-related differences in the use of four classic heuristics: anchoring, availability,
How can the existing literature on aging and decision making inform whether there are
adult age differences in classic heuristic use? In general, older adults’ use of simpler decision
strategies is well documented. In a meta-analysis, Mata and Nunes (2010) found that compared
to younger adults, older adults search for less information before making decisions. Older adults
also favor smaller sets of options (Reed et al., 2008; Reed et al., 2013) and prefer to consider one
important attribute (take-the-best) instead of several (weighted additive rule) when choosing
between alternatives (Mata et al., 2007). These instances all reflect older adults’ tendency to
engage in satisficing behavior (Bruine de Bruin et al., 2016); they choose the option that is “good
enough” for their purposes and not necessarily “the best” with respect to maximizing.
4
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Researchers have proposed several explanations for why older adults rely on simpler
strategies when making decisions. First, consistent with the theory of bounded rationality
(Simon, 1990), older adults’ declining fluid intelligence may leave them with fewer cognitive
resources, making less-demanding strategies appealing (Finucane et al., 2005). Supporting this
idea, one study found that lower fluid cognitive ability mediated older adults’ poorer
performance on a battery of decision making tasks (Adult Decision Making Competence (A-
DMC); Bruine de Bruin et al., 2012). Subsequent studies have documented associations between
specific fluid and crystalized cognitive abilities and age-related differences in decision making
(for a review, see Strough et al., 2020). Second, most laboratory decision making tasks are
hypothetical and the choices made are often not meaningful outside of the lab; older adults may
not be motivated to expend effort on these tasks because they are less relevant to their personal
goals (Hess et al., 2013). On a more positive note, older adults have more knowledge and life
experience, allowing them to adaptively adjust their decision making strategies based on the
cognitive resources and using specific heuristics because they are cognitively efficient. Broadly,
a heuristic is defined as “a strategy that ignores part of the information, with the goal of making
decisions more quickly, frugally, and/or accurately than more complex methods” (Gigerenzer &
Gaissmaier, 2011, p. 454). Classic work has highlighted many common heuristics and detailed
how they may lead to biased and systematic errors (e.g., Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). Yet, more
recent work contends that despite their occasional faults, heuristics can lead to more accurate
inferences under uncertainty than more complicated strategies (Neth & Gigerenzer, 2015).
Heuristics have developed and are continually used because they are simple, quick, and often
5
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
yield accurate answers. Some research has focused on the development of such heuristics,
adults (e.g., Jacobs & Potenza, 1991; Jetter & Walker, 2020; Guerten et al., 2015). In contrast,
there is a dearth of research on older adults’ use of many of the classic heuristics outlined by
Tversky and Kahneman (1974), even though adulthood provides many opportunities for
individuals to learn that heuristics offer efficient ways to make good choices. As such, we predict
that many common heuristics are implemented equally across the adult lifespan.
representativeness. We selected these heuristics because they are well known in the literature,
remain highly cited (e.g., in 2021 there were 1,142 publications that cited Tversky & Kahneman,
1974), and have few to no empirical studies conducted with older adults. In the anchoring
heuristic, people make estimates by starting from an initial value and adjusting to the final
answer. Different starting points garner different estimates, which are biased toward the initial
values (e.g., participants’ estimates of the percentage of African countries in the United Nations
(25% vs. 45%) differed based on the initial value they were given (10% vs. 65%); Tversky &
Kahneman, 1974). Availability is used when the frequency or probability of an event is judged
by the ease with which an example comes to mind (e.g., incorrectly estimating there are more R
words in the English language because it is easier to recall words beginning with the letter R than
words with R as their third letter; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). In recognition, if one of two
alternatives is recognized and the other is not, then one can infer that the recognized alternative
has the higher value with respect to the criterion (e.g., incorrectly estimating that Minneapolis,
MN has a higher population than Mesa, AZ because Minneapolis is the more familiar city;
Pachur et al., 2009). Finally, representativeness involves evaluating the probability that
6
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
something is true based on the degree to which one thing resembles another (e.g., assuming it is
more likely that Steve, a shy, withdrawn man with a passion for detail, is a librarian instead of a
For comparison purposes, we also examined the sunk cost fallacy, a bias where there are
known adult age differences. People are said to be susceptible to the sunk cost fallacy when they
continue a behavior or action because of their previously invested resources (i.e., time, money,
effort), even if the behavior or action is no longer rewarding (e.g., continuing to watch a movie
you dislike because you already paid for the ticket; Arkes & Blumer, 1985). Studies have shown
that older adults, perhaps due to their decreased time horizons, greater life experience, or
improved emotion regulation, are less susceptible to the sunk cost fallacy than younger adults
(e.g., Strough et al., 2008; Strough et al., 2014; Bruine de Bruin, 2014).
To our knowledge, there is only one study on adult age difference in the availability
heuristic (Maley, 2000), two studies on adult age differences in the recognition heuristic (Pachur
et al., 2009), and no existing studies on adult age differences in either the anchoring heuristic or
representativeness heuristic. None of these studies found age differences between younger and
older adults, but their sample sizes were small (n = 18 per age group in Maley, 2000; an average
of 50 per age group in Pachur et al., 2009). Of course, there may be many unpublished studies
that examine adult age differences in heuristics; studies showing null effects are much more
likely to be relegated to the file drawer. Yet, null effects of aging are not uninteresting, as it is
important to understand what abilities are spared with age (Lakens et al., 2020; Isaacowitz,
2020). Null results in this case would suggest that heuristic use is preserved as one gets older.
Such results would be consistent with findings that crystallized intelligence, or the accumulated
knowledge one gains about the world through socialization, experience, and education, increases
7
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
and stabilizes across the lifespan (Craik & Bialystok, 2006) and contributes to the preservation
and/or improvement in decision making with age (Samanez-Larkin & Knutson, 2015). However,
In the present study, a sample of over 300 participants aged 20 to 90 completed a series
of tasks to clarify potential similarities or differences in heuristic use across adulthood. Heuristic
use is automatic and widespread among younger adults—even among experts (e.g., Brannon &
Carson, 2003; Englich et al., 2006)—leading us to predict that older adults should be at least as
likely as younger adults to rely on heuristics. Given the few existing studies that found null age
effects and the lack of specific theoretical predictions for increased reliance with age, we
hypothesized that we would not find any age differences across adulthood in the use of
our hypothesis – we found no evidence for adult age differences in the use of these classic
heuristics. In contrast (and consistent with previous literature), we found age differences in the
sunk cost fallacy; younger adults were more susceptible to this bias than older adults.
Method
Transparency and Openness. We report how we determined our sample size and
describe all manipulations and measures in the study. The de-identified data, materials, and
analytic code are available on OSF (see Author’s Note). Data were analyzed using JASP, version
0.16.2 (JASP Team, 2022). This study’s design and analyses were not pre-registered.
Participants. Three hundred and eight participants (Mage = 54.20, SD = 18.99, range =
20-90, 166 female, 42.8% White, 29.2% African American, 28% Hispanic/Latinx) were
recruited through Qualtrics Panels. We under sampled White participants and over sampled
8
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
the latter populations in cognitive aging research and increase the generalizability of our
findings. All participants were included in the analysis. Our sample size was calculated in
G*Power to be able to identify small main effects of age (f2 = .03) with 0.85 power using an α of
.05. This effect size was based on previous studies that found age-related differences in the sunk
cost bias (e.g., Bruine de Bruin et al., 2014; Strough et al., 2008; Strough et al., 2016). This study
was approved by the Duke University Health System Institutional Review Board.
Table 1
Participant demographics
Number of 87 88 87 46
participants
Average Age (SD) 31.02 (5.21) 48.83 (5.95) 67.55 (4.64) 83.02 (2.98)
Number 57 | 30 58 | 30 47 | 40 4 | 42
female | male
% White | African 35% | 32% | 33% 34% | 32% | 34% 35% | 33% | 32% 91% | 7% | 2%
American | Latinx
Average Years of 14.55 (2.02) 14.25 (1.87) 14.69 (1.90) 15.39 (2.79)
Education (SD)
Note. For simplicity, age is grouped into four categories above, but is treated as continuous in
analysis. Only the oldest age group (80-90) significantly differs from the others in gender,
ethnicity, and years of education; these demographics are equivalent in all other age groups.
9
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Materials. Three to six scenarios demonstrating each heuristic were adapted from
various sources (see Appendix). As these heuristics are prompted in different ways across the
cost fallacy in order to capture a fuller range of heuristic responding. We included six instances
of recognition to allow for three examples each of the disease prevalence and city population
Anchoring Heuristic. We used scenarios listed in Furnham and Boo (2011): height of the
tallest redwood tree, length of the Mississippi River, and gestation period of an African elephant.
Participants first decided whether the height, length, or gestation period were greater than or less
than the given anchor and then estimated the exact measure.
shortened version of the “famous names problem” that was implemented in Braga et al. (2015).
Sixteen names were given: five were famous men and one was a famous woman (e.g., David
Beckham, Britney Spears). The other ten names were taken from the 2011 census list of the most
frequent U.S. names (e.g., Richard Miller, Sarah Wilson). After the names were listed in random
order one by one, participants decided whether more men or women’s names were presented
(note: an equal number of men’s and women’s names were shown). We also used the letter
likelihood scenario from Tversky and Kahneman (1974) for the letters R and K. Participants
decided whether it was more likely that a randomly sampled English word started with the target
letter or had the target letter as its third letter (the correct answer is the third letter, but it is easier
Recognition Heuristic. We adapted the disease prevalence and city population inference
tasks used in Pachur et al. (2009). Participants saw two options and chose the one they believed
has higher prevalence in the U.S. (e.g., shigellosis vs. E. coli) or has the higher population (e.g.,
Dublin vs. Minsk), respectively. Choice of the more familiar option (i.e., E. coli or Dublin)
Representativeness Heuristic. We used the Linda problem from Tversky and Kahneman
(1983), the lawyer vs. engineer base rate problem from Kahneman and Tversky (1973), and the
marble problem from Kahneman and Tversky (1972). In the Linda problem, participants were
given a description of Linda (e.g., bright, outspoken 31-year-old woman concerned with issues
of social justice) and asked to indicate whether it is more probable that Linda is 1) a bank teller
or 2) a bank teller that is active in the feminist movement. In the lawyer vs. engineer problem,
participants were told that 30 engineers and 70 lawyers were interviewed by psychologists. They
then received one randomly chosen description of one of the interviewees (e.g., Jack has no
political interests and enjoys carpentry and math puzzles) and were asked to predict the
probability (0-100%) that the man is an engineer. In the marble problem, five children are
playing a game and 20 marbles are distributed randomly among them. Participants selected
which of two distributions (Distribution A: 4,4,5,4,3; Distribution B: 4,4,4,4,4) was more likely
to occur after several rounds of the game. In all scenarios, reliance on the representativeness
heuristic means choosing the option that matches one’s expectations (i.e., knowledge about the
Sunk Cost Fallacy. We used three scenarios adapted from Bruine de Bruin et al. (2014)
and Strough et al. (2014) and participants decided on a scale from 1 (most likely to stick with
plans) to 6 (most likely to cancel plans) how willing they were to forego their plans. For
11
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
example, participants read a scenario about losing interest in a project and then rated their
information about their age, sex, education level, and ethnicity. They then completed three
blocks of decision scenarios. Each block contained two recognition scenarios and one scenario
tapping each of the following heuristics: sunk cost bias, availability, representativeness, and
anchoring. Presentation of each heuristic within a block was randomized and high versus low
anchors were counterbalanced across participants. Finally, participants were asked seven open-
ended response questions, including whether they looked up the answers to any questions and
whether they had prior exposure to the scenarios used in the study. Then, for each of the five
heuristics, participants were asked to describe what strategy they used to make their decision.
Finally, participants were compensated for their participation directly through Qualtrics.
Results
confirm that the items used in the study produced anchoring effects. Anchoring occurred when
participants estimated the height of the tallest redwood tree and the length of the Mississippi
River. The redwood tree was estimated as shorter (M = 128, SD = 126) when participants were
given a low anchor (65 feet) versus a high anchor (500 feet; M = 630, SD = 972); t(306) = -6.17,
p < .001, d = -.71. Similarly, the Mississippi River was estimated as shorter when participants
were given a low anchor of 200 miles (M = 939, SD = 1,729) versus a high anchor of 20,000
miles (M = 15,980, SD = 35,670); t(306) = -5.29, p < .001, d = -.60. Participants did not show an
anchoring effect when estimating the gestation period of an African elephant. Estimates were
12
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
similar regardless of whether participants were given a low anchor (12 months; M = 23.7, SD =
45.1) or high anchor (32 months; M = 25.2, SD = 12.1); t(306) = -.388, p = .698, d = -.044.
Next, we put participants’ estimates on the same scale by calculating the absolute
proportional difference from the anchor. That is, we computed the absolute difference between
the estimate and the anchor and divided it by the anchor. For completeness, we report the age
effect results for the two items that showed anchoring and the one that did not separately below.
Height of tallest redwood tree and length of Mississippi River. To evaluate the effect of
age on the use of anchoring, we computed a repeated-measures ANCOVA, with item (redwood
height, Mississippi River length) as a within-subject factor and age (continuous) as a between-
subject factor. The main effect of age was non-significant, F(1, 305) = .034, p = .854, as was the
age by item interaction, F(1, 305) = .000002, p = .999, ηp2 < .001. In other words, the degree of
difference between estimates and their anchors did not vary based on age. The age effect
remained non-significant after controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, F(1, 301) = .297, p =
.586.
Gestation period of an African elephant. Evaluating age effects for a single item
eliminated the need for a categorical item factor, so we computed a simple linear regression.
There was a non-significant main effect of age, b = –.012, 95% CI = [–.027, .004], t = –1.48, p =
.140, which remained non-significant after controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, b = –
.008, 95% CI = [–.025, .009], t = –.951, p = .342. See Figure 1 for the age effects of each
15 75
20
10 50
10
5 25
0 0 0
20 40 60 80 20 40 60 80 20 40 60 80
Age Age Age
5 5 5
difference from anchor
Absolute proportional
3 3 3
2 2 2
1 1 1
0 0 0
20 40 60 80 20 40 60 80 20 40 60 80
Age Age Age
Figure 1. The absolute proportional difference between participants’ estimates and the
anchors they were given for height of the tallest redwood tree (left), length of the Mississippi
River (middle), and gestation period of an African elephant (right) did not differ across age
groups. The top row includes all data points. The bottom row shows that the pattern of results is
unchanged even after matching the ranges for absolute proportional difference by removing
extreme values. In all plots, zero indicates estimates that were equal to the anchors provided.
Plots depict simple regression lines with 95% confidence intervals (gray bands) and correlations
(r) between age and anchoring use; analyses controlling for covariates are reported in the text.
14
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
heuristic (i.e., selected that more men’s names were presented in the list, selected first letter in a
word). A score of 0 was assigned to responses that did not follow this heuristic. On average,
participants chose the more available option over half of the time (M = 59%, SD = 35%). To
ANCOVA, with item as a within-subject factor and age as a continuous between-subject factor.
We found a small item by age interaction, F(2, 612) = 3.17, p = .043, ηp2 = .01, driven by a
positive relationship between age and heuristic use in the R letter likelihood scenario, r(306) =
.12, p = .036. However, this interaction was not robust and did not remain significant after
controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, F(2, 604) = 1.18, p = .309, ηp2 = .004. The main
effect of age was non-significant, F(1, 306) = .389, p = .533 and remained non-significant after
controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, F(1, 302) = .0006, p = .981.
heuristic (e.g., selected Dublin—the more familiar option— even though Minsk has the higher
population). A score of 0 was assigned to responses that did not follow this heuristic. On
average, participants chose the more familiar option most of the time (M = 77%, SD = 19%). To
ANCOVA, with item as a within-subject factor and age as a continuous between-subject factor.
The main effect of age was non-significant, F(1, 306) = .434, p = .511, and there was no item-
level interaction with age, F(1, 306) = .018, p = .894, ηp2 < .001. The age effect remained non-
significant after controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, F(1, 302) = .013, p = .909.
Linda and marble problems require a binary choice and the lawyer vs. engineering problem
15
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
involves generating a percentage. To put all responses on the same scale, answers of 40% and
above in the lawyer vs. engineer problem were counted as heuristic responses. The correct
percentage is 30% as there were 30 engineers in the sample. Technically, a response of 31% or
higher suggests heuristic responding, but we chose 40% to select a value that is meaningfully
different from 30%. Using a stricter cutoff (i.e., 35%) did not change the results. Across tasks,
representative responses (i.e., selecting that Linda is a bank teller and feminist, selecting Marble
participants chose the more representative option most of the time (M = 76%, SD = 24%). To
ANCOVA, with item as a within-subject factor and age as a continuous between-subject factor.
The main effect of age was non-significant, F(1, 306) = .545, p = .461, and there was no item-
level interaction with age, F(2, 612) = 2.28, p = .103, ηp2 = .007. The age effect remained non-
significant after controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, F(1, 302) = .039, p = .843.
Use of Representativeness
heuristic (middle), and representativeness heuristic (right) across different scenarios did not
16
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
differ across age groups. Plots depict simple regression lines with 95% confidence intervals (gray
bands) and correlations (r) between age and heuristic use; analyses controlling for covariates are
hypotheses, we conducted simple Bayesian linear regressions. Age was included as a predictor of
anchoring, availability, recognition, and representativeness heuristic use. Consistent with our
frequentist results, we found moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (BF01 > 6),
indicating that the data is at least six times more representative of the null hypothesis (there are
no age-related differences in heuristic use) versus the alternate hypothesis (there are age-related
Sunk Cost Fallacy. Across scenarios, participants were slightly likely to cancel plans
(3.73 on a scale of 1 to 6; SD = 1.16). To evaluate the effect of age on the use of the sunk cost
age as a continuous between-subject factor. There was no item-level interaction with age, F(2,
612) = 1.44, p = .238, ηp2 = .005. However, there was a significant main effect of age, F(1, 306)
= 4.55, p = .034. Simple linear regression confirmed that age significantly predicted avoidance of
the sunk cost fallacy – as age increased, so did the likelihood that participants would forego their
plans, b = .007, 95% CI = [.0006, .014], SE = .003, t = 2.13, p = .034. The age effect was not
significant after controlling for sex, ethnicity, and education, b = .007, 95% CI = [–.0005, .014],
SE = .004, t = 1.84, p = .067. However, the point estimate was identical and the confidence
interval was highly overlapping with the simple age effect. This effect is similar in size to what
17
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
has been reported in previous studies that found age-related differences in the sunk cost bias
(Bruine de Bruin et al., 2014; Strough et al., 2016). Simple Bayesian linear regression found
evidence in favor of the alternate hypothesis (BF10 > 1), indicating that the data is relatively more
representative of the alternate hypothesis (there are age-related differences in the sunk cost bias)
versus the null hypothesis (there are no age-related differences in the sunk cost bias). See
6
Likelihood to cancel plans
r = .12
20 40 60 80
Age
1 (most likely to stick with plans) to 6 (most likely to cancel plans). Plot depicts simple
regression line with 95% confidence interval (gray band) and correlation (r) between age and
avoidance of the sunk cost fallacy; analyses controlling for covariates are reported in the text.
18
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
General Discussion
In this study, we found that classic heuristic use was common across adults of all ages.
Age did not significantly impact 1) how far participants’ estimates were from the anchors they
were given (anchoring heuristic), 2) how often participants chose the more accessible option of a
pair (availability heuristic), 3) how often participants chose the more familiar option of a pair
(recognition heuristic), and 4) how often participants chose the option that most closely
differences in the sunk cost bias. Older adults were more likely to avoid this fallacy and chose to
Although the age difference in the sunk cost fallacy is small, it is consistent with others in
the literature. One possible explanation for age-related differences in this heuristic and not the
others may be related to learning that occurs across the lifespan. The longer one lives, the more
opportunities one has to learn that persistence is not always worthwhile. In fact, several studies
have suggested that age-related improvements in the sunk cost bias are related to affect and
experience (Bruine de Bruin et al., 2014; Bruine de Bruin et al., 2007; Bruine de Bruin et al.,
2012). And while self-reports should be taken with caution, 78% of participants reported that
they relied on life experiences while rating their likelihood to cancel plans (see Supplemental
Table 1). These findings are in line with research that suggests that age-related differences in
certain decision making tasks may be related to age-related differences in fluid and crystalized
cognitive abilities. Similar to the sunk cost fallacy, some tasks (e.g., making internally consistent
probabilistic judgments of getting into a car accident vs. driving accident free) rely more on
crystallized capabilities that improve with age, while other tasks (e.g., applying designated
decision strategies when choosing between multiple products) rely more on fluid capabilities that
19
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
decrease with age (Bruine de Bruin et al., 2012; Li et al., 2013; Del Missier et al., 2013; Strough
et al., 2011a). Prior researchers have speculated about the role of age-related differences in
specific fluid and crystalized abilities (Agarwal et al., 2009) or cognitive and affective abilities
(Peters et al., 2007) in decision making across adulthood, yet relatively few studies (e.g., Li et
al., 2013; Li et al., 2015) have directly measured multiple domains of cognitive (fluid and
crystalized) or affective abilities across adulthood to confirm that they specifically account for
In contrast to life experience reducing susceptibility to the sunk cost fallacy, experience
might preserve use of the classic heuristics because they often lead to the correct answer
(Gigerenzer & Gaissmeier, 2011). Heuristics are shortcuts that reflect a common state of the
world: in most cases, more populous cities are mentioned in the news, learned about in schools,
and are common travel destinations. Thus, using the cue of familiarity would typically lead one
to choose the correct answer. Even experts use these heuristics, although sometimes to a lesser
degree. One review found that auditors tended to respond heuristically (e.g., use anchors, neglect
base rates) in neutral tasks, but this behavior was mitigated in familiar and job-related tasks
(Smith & Kida, 1991). Yet, some experts in other domains (e.g., real estate agents, legal
professionals, nurses) are still susceptible to heuristic responding even when making decisions in
their area of expertise (Northcraft & Neale, 1987; Englich et al., 2006; Brannon & Carson,
2003). Related work in finance reveals that the use of heuristic-like rules of thumb may increase
with age as investors gain more experience (Korniotis & Kumar, 2011). Together these studies
highlight the pervasive nature of heuristic use even after significant domain-specific experience.
Some views contend that we use heuristics to reduce effort, especially in the face of
capacity limitations (e.g., limited time, motivation; Shah & Oppenheimer, 2008). With even
20
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
fewer cognitive resources, older adults might be expected to be more likely to use heuristics.
However, many of the cognitive mechanisms proposed to underlie these heuristics do not show
declines with age. More specifically, accrued knowledge about the world plays a role in all of the
heuristics that did not show age effects. Availability effects are linked to the ease with which
information is retrieved from memory (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). To the extent that the task
is asking people to retrieve from semantic memory, we would not expect age-related differences.
Older adults generate category examples at the same rate as younger adults (Light & Albertson,
1989), so we would expect them to be similarly able to generate words starting with K or with K
as its third letter. The recognition heuristic also draws on stored knowledge. People rely on a
global sense of familiarity as a way of choosing their answer (Pachur & Hertwig, 2006). The
relative familiarity of two diseases or cities should not differ as a function of age given the
obscurity of the unfamiliar option (Pachur et al., 2009). Finally, the representativeness heuristic
draws on well-learned schemas and depends on the degree of similarity between the target
person/event/sample and the population from which it is drawn (Galavotti et al., 2021).
Schematic knowledge is preserved in healthy aging and is believed to be a major aid to memory
when episodic details are forgotten (Siegel et al., 2020). Informally, participants reported
drawing on their knowledge to solve all of these decision scenarios, especially the recognition
(65%) and representativeness heuristic scenarios (66%) (see Supplemental Table 1).
In contrast, it is less clear what role knowledge plays in the anchoring heuristic. Plausible
anchors are more likely to sway decisions (Sugden et al., 2013), but specific expertise in a
domain may reduce reliance on anchors altogether (Smith et al., 2013). In the present case, we do
not have any reason to believe there are age-related differences in specific knowledge of the
21
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
height and length of the tallest redwood tree or Mississippi River, respectively, thus it is not
These results are unlikely to be attributable to the online nature of the study. Paralleling
earlier papers showing similar results across laboratory and Amazon Mechanical Turk samples
(see Buhrmester et al., 2018 for review), research is now emerging showing similar adult age
effects across in-person and online samples from various data collection platforms (e.g., Greene
& Naveh-Benjamin, 2022; Seaman et al., 2021). Furthermore, we found an age difference in the
sunk cost fallacy using our online sample, replicating an effect observed numerous times in the
laboratory (Strough et al., 2008; Strough et al., 2011b). More generally, older adults have been
the fastest growing users of the internet for over a decade, with the gap between younger and
older adults’ smartphone, tablet, and internet usage steadily decreasing over time (Faverio,
2022). Thus, aging samples in online studies are likely becoming more representative of the
Future studies should address some of our limitations. While we attempted to recruit a
diverse set of participants, this sample was not nationally representative, especially in the oldest-
old group of 80-90 year olds, so our findings may differ when including people from other ethnic
and educational backgrounds (though we note we have no specific predictions about how
heuristic use might vary as a function of these factors). Additionally, Qualtrics Panels was unable
to provide us with the number of invited individuals who did not participate, thus underlying
differences may exist between those who participated and those who did not. This study was also
cross-sectional instead of longitudinal, so age differences may reflect cohort differences instead
of changes with age. However, it is not clear exactly what cohort difference might be predicted
to explain these effects. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, the heuristic tasks used
22
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
hypothetical scenarios; it is possible that participants’ responses to these scenarios do not reflect
their real-world decision making. This concern is especially valid for older adults; in other
domains (e.g., prospective memory), it has been shown that older adults behave differently in
laboratory studies than they would in everyday life (e.g., Rendell & Thomson, 1999; Henry et
al., 2004).
Moving forward, an interesting future direction for this research would be to explore
whether these results would extend to individuals with age-related diseases. To the extent that
cognitive impairment is mild and semantic knowledge is intact, we would not expect there to be
differences in heuristic use between healthy and clinical populations. However, if a breakdown
of semantic processing does occur, as is believed to be the case in Alzheimer’s Disease (AD;
Verma & Howard, 2012), then we would expect to see differences from healthy controls. AD
populations may have deficits in heuristic use because they are not readily able to rely on their
semantic knowledge. For example, Balthazar and colleagues found that patients with amnesiac
mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) performed normally on the Boston Naming Test (BNT), a
semantic assessment that asks participants to name line drawings of objects of increasing
difficulty (2008). Mild AD patients performed significantly worse on the BNT, but were able to
boost their performance to that of aMCI patients and healthy controls after they were given
semantic (i.e., a short explanation of the picture) and phonemic (i.e., the first phonemes of the
target word) cues upon making a naming error. Thus, we can speculate that people with MCI, but
not AD, would use heuristics to the same degree as healthy controls. People with AD may need
Overall, we believe it is important to highlight the skills and abilities that are preserved in
healthy aging. Null age effects should not automatically be relegated to the file drawer; they may
23
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
reveal something significant about the preservation of certain aspects of human cognition with
age (Lakens et al., 2020; Isaacowitz, 2020). For example, the current work suggests that adaptive
strategies like heuristics are used consistently throughout the adult lifespan; older adults are not
more likely to use them than younger adults. These results challenge some earlier speculation
that older adults might be more reliant on heuristic processing compared to their younger
counterparts (Peters et al., 2000) and identify yet another preserved aspect of cognition in healthy
aging. In fact, related prior research has shown that the simplifying decision strategies of older
adults produce high quality decisions in everyday life (Mata & Nunes, 2010). More generally,
knowledge about what is preserved across adulthood could be useful when developing tools and
Appendix
Anchoring
Availability
Note. *The list contained an equal number of male (Thomas Anderson, Richard Miller, David
Beckham, Michael Jordan, George Clooney, Brad Pitt, Chris Brown, Paul Jones) and female
names (Jennifer Moore, Elizabeth Clark, Barbara Smith, Linda Johnson, Britney Spears, Sarah
Wilson, Louise Martin, Sandra Murphy).
25
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Recognition
Representativeness
Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a
student, she was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also
participated in anti-nuclear demonstrations.
Jack is a 45-year-old man. He is married and has four children. He is generally conservative,
careful, and ambitious. He shows no interest in political and social issues and spends most of his
free time on his many hobbies which include home carpentry, sailing, and mathematical puzzles.
Five children (Alan, Ben, Carl, Dan, and Ed) are playing a game. On each round, 20 marbles are
distributed at random among them. Consider the following distributions:
A: 4, 4, 5, 4, 3
B: 4, 4, 4, 4, 4
In many rounds of the game, will there be more results of Distribution A or Distribution B?
Sunk Cost Fallacy (Adapted from Bruine de Bruin et al., 2014 and Strough et al., 2014)
You have driven halfway to a vacation destination. Your goal is to spend time by yourself. You
feel sick. You feel that you would have a much better weekend at home. You think that it is "too
bad" you already drove halfway, because you would much rather spend the time at home. Please
think about what you would do if you were in this situation. Would you be more likely to stick
with your plans (1) or to cancel your plans (6)?
After a large meal at a restaurant, you order a big dessert with chocolate and ice cream. After a
few bites, you find you are full and you would rather not eat any more of it. Would you be more
likely to eat more (1) or to stop eating it (6)?
You have been working on a project related to one of your hobbies. Lately, you have lost interest
in the project. Whenever you work on it, you are bored and wish that you were doing something
else. You must decide what to do. Would you be more likely remain committed to the project (1)
or stop working on it (6)?
27
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
References
Agarwal, S., Driscoll, J. C., Gabaix, X., Laibson, D., & Papers, B. (2009). The age of reason:
Financial decisions over the life cycle and implications for regulation. Brookings Papers of
Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and
Balthazar, M. L. F., Cendes, F., & Damasceno, B. P. (2008). Semantic error patterns on the
Boston Naming Test in normal aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and mild
https://doi.org/10.1037/A0012919
Braga, J. N., Ferreira, M. B., & Sherman, S. J. (2015). The effects of construal level on heuristic
https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000021
Brannon, L. A., & Carson, K. L. (2003). The representativeness heuristic: influence on nurses’
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0897-1897(03)00043-0
Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007). Individual differences in adult
956. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938
28
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2012). Explaining adult age differences in
https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.712
Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Strough, J. N. (2016). Choosing to be happy? Age
Bruine De Bruin, W., Strough, J. N., & Parker, A. M. (2014). Getting older isn’t all that bad:
Better decisions and coping when facing ’sunk costs’. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 642–
647. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0036308
Mechanical Turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological
Cire, B. (2016). World’s older population grows dramatically. National Institute on Aging.
https://www.nia.nih.gov/news/worlds-older-population-grows-dramatically#:~:text=The
Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2006). Cognition through the lifespan: Mechanisms of change.
Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., Hansson, P., De Bruin, W. B., Parker, A. M., & Nilsson, L. G.
(2013). The multifold relationship between memory and decision making: An individual-
29
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Englich, B., Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2006). Playing dice with criminal sentences: The
influence of irrelevant anchors on experts’ judicial decision making. Personality and Social
Faverio, M. (2022, January 13). Share of tech users among Americans 65 and older grew in past
those-65-and-older-who-are-tech-users-has-grown-in-the-past-decade/
Finucane, M. L., Mertz, C. K., Slovic, P., & Schmidt, E. S. (2005). Task complexity and older
https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.71
Finucane, M. L., Slovic, P., Hibbard, J. H., Peters, E., Mertz, C. K., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002).
skills in older versus younger adults considering health-plan options. Journal of Behavioral
Furnham, A., & Boo, H. C. (2011). A literature review of the anchoring effect. Journal of Socio-
Galavotti, I., Lippi, A., & Cerrato, D. (2021). The representativeness heuristic at work in
1464/FULL/PDF
Geurten, M., Willems, S., Germain, S., & Meulemans, T. (2015). Less is more: The availability
https://doi.org/10.1111/BJDP.12114
Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of
https://doi.org/10.1037/PAG0000655
Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review
https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27
Hess, T. M., Queen, T. L., & Ennis, G. E. (2013). Age and self-relevance effects on information
Isaacowitz, D. M. (2020). Doing more with null age effects: Introduction to the special section.
https://doi.org/10.1093/GERONB/GBZ123
31
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Jacobs, J. E., & Potenza, M. (1991). The use of judgement heuristics to make social and object
https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-8624.1991.TB01522.X
Jetter, M., & Walker, J. K. (2020). At what age does the anchoring heuristic emerge? Evidence
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JEBO.2019.08.017
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review,
Korniotis, G. M., & Kumar, A. (2011). Do older investors make better Investment decisions?
https://doi.org/10.1162/REST_A_00053
Lakens, D., McLatchie, N., Isager, P. M., Scheel, A. M., & Dienes, Z. (2020). Improving
inferences about null effects with Bayes factors and equivalence tests. The Journals of
Light, L. L., & Albertson, S. A. (1989). Direct and indirect tests of memory for category
exemplars in young and old adults. Psychology and Aging, 4(4), 487–492.
32
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
https://web.p.ebscohost.com/ehost/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?vid=0&sid=444d4aa3-7ca7-4094-
a2b7-9ab1f4120176%40redis
Li, Y., Baldassi, M., Johnson, E. J., & Weber, E. U. (2013). Complementary cognitive
capabilities, economic decision making, and aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 595–613.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034172
Li, Y., Gao, J., Enkavi, A. Z., Zaval, L., Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2015). Sound credit
scores and financial decisions despite cognitive aging. Proceedings of the National
Maley, J. E., Hunt, M., & Parr, W. (2000). Set-size and frequency-of-occurrence judgements in
young and older adults: The role of the availability heuristic. Quarterly Journal of
https://doi.org/10.1080/713755883
Mata, R. (2007). Understanding the aging decision maker. Human Development, 50(6), 359–366.
https://doi.org/10.1159/000109836
Mata, R., Josef, A. K., & Lemaire, P. (2015). Adaptive decision making and aging. In T. M.
Hess, J. Strough, & C. E. Lockenhoff (Eds.), Aging and Decision Making: Empirical and
417148-0.00006-6
33
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Mata, R., & Nunes, L. (2010). When less is enough: Cognitive aging, information search, and
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017927
Neth, H., & Gigerenzer, G. (2015). Heuristics: Tools for an uncertain world. In R. Scott & S.
Kosslyn (Eds.), Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 1–18). John
Northcraft, G. B., & Neale, M. A. (1987). Experts, amateurs, and real estate: An anchoring-and-
Pachur, T., & Hertwig, R. (2006). On the psychology of the recognition heuristic: Retrieval
Pachur, T., Mata, R., & Schooler, L. J. (2009). Cognitive aging and the adaptive use of
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017211
Peters, E., Hess, T. M., Västfjäll, D., & Auman, C. (2007). Adult age differences in dual
Information processes: Implications for the role of affective and deliberative processes in
23. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1111/j.1745-
6916.2007.00025.x?casa_token=ABmjKA7ZWL8AAAAA%3AsrnOMwqjOY1EEBVnvii6
gF-Qqgci3AWzlMqa0TynQbm81Vnn7UvkGuV6Q68JAnisPfeXkEuIDu0yYw
34
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Peters, E., Finucane, M. L., MacGregor, D. G., & Slovic, P. (2000). The bearable lightness of
aging: Judgment and decision processes in older adults. In P. C. Stern & L. L. Carstensen
(Eds.), The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research (pp. 144–165). National
Queen, T. L., Hess, T. M., Ennis, G. E., Dowd, K., & Grühn, D. (2012). Information search and
decision making: Effects of age and complexity on strategy use. Psychology and Aging,
Reed, A. E., Mikels, J. A., & Löckenhoff, C. E. (2013). Preferences for choice across adulthood:
Age trajectories and potential mechanisms. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 625–632.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031399
Reed, A. E., Mikels, J. A., & Simon, K. I. (2008). Older adults prefer less choice than young
Rendell, P. G., & Thomson, D. M. (1999). Aging and prospective memory: Differences between
naturalistic and laboratory Tasks. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 54B(4), 256–269.
https://doi.org/10.1093/GERONB/54B.4.P256
Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Knutson, B. (2015). Decision making in the ageing brain: Changes in
affective and motivational circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2015 16:5, 16(5), 278–
289. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3917
Seaman, K. L., Juarez, E. J., Troutman, A., Salerno, J. M., Samanez-Larkin, S. P., & Samanez-
Larkin, G. R. (2021). Decision making across adulthood during physical distancing. Aging,
35
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2021.1962793/SUPPL_FILE/NANC_A_1962793_SM86
15.DOCX
2909.134.2.207
Siegel, A. L. M., Whatley, M. C., Hargis, M. B., & Castel, A. D. (2020). Changes in memory
and metacognition in older adulthood. In A. Drolet & C. Yoon (Eds.), The Aging
Consumer: Perspectives from Psychology and Marketing (2nd ed., pp. 26–46). Routledge.
Smith, A. R., Windschitl, P. D., & Bruchmann, K. (2013). Knowledge matters: Anchoring
effects are moderated by knowledge level. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(1),
97–108. https://doi.org/10.1002/EJSP.1921
Smith, J. F., & Kida, T. (1991). Heuristics and biases: Expertise and task realism in auditing.
Strack, F., & Mussweiler, T. (1997). Explaining the enigmatic anchoring effect: Mechanisms of
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.437
36
HEURISTIC DECISION MAKING
Strough, J. N., Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., Karns, T., Lemaster, P., Pichayayothin, N.,
Delaney, R., & Stoiko, R. (2016). What were they thinking? Reducing sunk-cost bias in a
https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000130
Strough, J. N., Mehta, C. M., McFall, J. P., & Schuller, K. L. (2008). Are older adults less
subject to the sunk-cost fallacy than younger adults?: Short report. Psychological Science,
Strough, J., Karns, T. E., & Schlosnagle, L. (2011a). Decision-making heuristics and biases
across the life span. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1235(1), 57–74.
https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1749-6632.2011.06208.X
Strough, J., Schlosnagle, L., & Didonato, L. (2011b). Understanding decisions about sunk costs
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr057
Strough, J., Schlosnagle, L., Karns, T., Lemaster, P., & Pichayayothin, N. (2014). No time to
waste: Restricting life-span temporal horizons decreases the sunk-cost fallacy. Journal of
Strough, J., Wilson, J., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (2020). Aging and financial decision making. In
Sugden, R., Zheng, J., & Zizzo, D. J. (2013). Not all anchors are created equal. Journal of
Taylor, M.K., Marsh, E.J., & Samanez-Larkin, G.R. (2022). Heuristic decision making across
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and
0285(73)90033-9
Verma, M., & Howard, R. J. (2012). Semantic memory and language dysfunction in early
1217. https://doi.org/10.1002/GPS.3766