Ed 010547
Ed 010547
Ed 010547
ED 010 517 48
TAGMEMIC AND MATRIX LINGUISTICS APPLIED TO SELECTED AFRICAN
LANGUAGES. FINAL REPORT.
BY- PIKE, KENNETH L.
MICHIGAN UNIV., ANN ARBOR
REPORT NUMBER BP. -5 -1301 PUB DATE NOV 66
CONTRACT 0Er-4-065
EDRS PRICE MF-$0.45 HC-$11.88 297P.
FINAL REPORT
Contract No. 0E-5-14-065
4f. 5-150 I
November 1966
Kenneth L. Pike
November 1966
Page
INTRODUCTION 1
CHAPTER
I: CLAUSES 12
CHAPTER Page
3.2.1. Narrative 84
Q
3.2.2. Indirect versus Direct Discourse 86
V: VERBS 114
-iv-
4
TABLE OF CONTENTS (continued)
CHAPTER Page
1
j fl
-- APPENDICES
-v-
TABLE OF CONTENTS (continued)
APPENDICES Page
was urged that all purposive human activity and perception were constrained
by in-b a elements of the human mechanism. Different languages--or sets of
nonverbal behavior patterns--might be learned from one's cultural surroundings,
but each such set was in turn restricted by the nature of man himself. I
-1-
L.
--2-
et
-3--
Data of the real world are notoriously more varied than the unaided imagina-
tion of man. The philosopher at his desk has never been able to cono.oct a
world as rich as that outside his window. Such a large block of the world as
Africa, therefore, could likely be concealing some surprises. These surprises
might be of several general kinds. One is the addition of new kinds of data
never seen before. Another brings forth data which are "new" in a subtler
sense: It consists of the discovery of an exaggerated form of a known problem- -
a problem which previously was known only in a form so simple that it did not
warrant the development of an elaborate apparatus to handle it--or so simple
that it was not complex enough to allow the researcher to predict the kind of
formula needed for which this known problem was but a special instance- A
third type of surprise is one for which there is less excuse, but which meets
us all: the forcing to attention of data already described by other research-
ers, but in a language in which he has had but little interest, so that the
special solutions already achieved have not had their deserved impact on him.
All of these were found in the African project being reported here, An
early search of the literature, with the help of graduate students who attempted
to restate in tagmemic terms data from various parts of Africa, guaranteed
broader coverage than could otherwise have been achieved. The written sources
included reference to West, East, and South Africa. Study of live data was
confined to West Africa--to Ghana, Nigeria, and Dahomey.
4
-S-
Of kinds of material new to me, perhaps the most interesting was on dis-
course structure. The intricate "ranking" matrices--or the ordered rules- -
required to describe Bariba use of direct versus indirect quotations were ele-
gant in beauty and totally unexpected. It was almost as if stage directions
for spotlighting characters were translated into language rules.
Equally surprising and elegant--though with less obvious semantic impli-
cations--were the elaborate matrix relations required to give insight into the
subtle traces of broken down concord in Abua verbs. When an independent o'::,ect
occurred in the clause, verb prefixes sometimes underwent a set of changes which
could be understood best as the fusion of forms of an old lost concord matrix.
A further surprise came with the concord elements in Etung noun classes.
could, by various historical stages, decay into lower-level verb phrases (with
one of the original free verbs becoming averbal auxiliary or particle), as in
Vagala; or, on the contrary, (3) the original series could remain a subcluster,
but become tagmemically specialized (filling a slot in a larger including cluster),
building up higher-level structures.
Probably tone structures, on the other hand, best illustrate the kind of
problem which had been described by other scholars, but not adequately worked
15
into my own thinking. In the middle of this century, William Welmers called
attention to "terrace tones." (This is a phenomenon where a tone which normally
is high after another high is sometimes lowered a bit after a high--but contin-
ues to have following it, at whatever level it then happens to be, other high
tones of the same system. Both the original high and the stepped-down high con-
trast with a low in that position after high. The step-down is occasioned,
historically, by the loss of a low tone, between the two original highs, which
lowered the second of the two by--then--non-phonemic conditioned variation.)
I have enjoyed experimenting with treating the "downsteppingness" as a "process"
phoneme--rather than as a segmental one.
The extensive morphophonemic changes, furthermore, require a special handling
16
of the relation of tone patterns to words as wholes rather than to stems or
affixes as more-or-less discrete parts. Work on Etung and Bimoba highlighted
this problem at the interface between lexicon, grammar, and phonology.
In phonetics proper, the most interesting item was the study of vowel quality
17
underlying the vowel harmony of Twi, a study undertaken withDr. John M. Stewart,
(who emphasized the linguistic reasons for needing a--Firthian--prosody for the
"close" subset) and Dr. Ruth M. Brend of the University of Michigan and Michigan
State University (who with Charles Peck of the University of Michigan provided
the instrumental analysis). Instead of treating /i/ and /I/ as differing by
either tense and lax, or close and open features, it appears best to treat the
'open It set as basic, with the other set modified by having the root of the tongue
thrust forward_.
Before reaching Africa I was able to consult with various scholars who very
kindly suggested areas of concern, warranting investigation, and gave me valu-
able bibliographical references. Among these scholars were Professor Jack Berry,
Nortliwestern University; Professors H. A. Gleason and W. Samarin, Hartford Semi-
nary Foundation; Earl W. Stevick, Foreign Service Institute; Prof. A. E. Meussen,
Musee Royal de L'Afrique Centrale, Tervuren, Belgium; Dra. Bertha Siertsema,
Free University of Amsterdam. From some old (1954) lecture tapes of Professor
William Welmers, Univ. of California at Los Angeles, I had access to data pre-
viously made available to members of the Summer Institute of Linguistics. Be-
fore reaching Africa, also, I had available some preliminary tagmemic restate-
ments of work by other scholars, made by graduate students Krohn (Shona), Sherman
(Grebo), Rensch (Bobangi), Kappler (Hausa), Nicklas (Tswana).
The time in Africa was divided into two parts. In Ghana, various members
of the Summer Institute of Linguistics gathered at Accra with their informants
from November, 1965, through the middle of January, 1966.
The personnel, and the languages involved were:
D John and Kathleen Callow, Kasem
Jack Kennedy, Dagaari
Gill Jacobs, Bimoba
Monica Cox, Basare
Marjorie Crouch, Vagala
Ron and Muriel Rowland, Sisala
From the middle of January until the end of March a second workshop was
held at the University of Nigeria, Nsukka.
Members of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, with the languages studied
at the workshop were:
Ian and Amelia Gardner, Abua
Paul and Inge Meier, Izi (and Dr. John Bendor-Samuel, Consultant)
Katherine Barnwell, and Patricia Revill, Mbembe
Richard and Nancy Bergman, Igede
Klaus and Janice Spreda, Agbo (and Dr. John Bendor-Samuel, Consultant)
Thomas and Eileen Edmondson, Etung
Elaine Thomas, Degema and Engenni
Member of the Sudan Interior Mission, Miss Jean Soutar, Bariba
-8-
Concordances made from texts in Vagala and Sisala were produced at the
University of Oklahoma by the Linguistic Retrieval Project for Aboriginal
Languages, partially supported by National Science Foundation grants GS-270
and GS-934. Input data on magnetic tape are archived at the University of
Oklahoma.
-10-
FOOTNOTES
1
An Introduction to Morphology and Syntax (Santa Ana, Calif.: Summer Insti-
Or
10
The Summer Institute of Ling-4.stics has work going on in more than 350
languages. The researchers of these languages have all been exposed, to some
degree, to tagmemic concepts. When a component of the theory would not work
in these contexts it was revised.
11For example, K. L. Pike, "Beyond the Sentence," College Composition and
Communication, 15.129-35 (1964); and Alton Becker, "A Tagmemic Approach to Par-
agraph Analysis;" College Composition anet Communication, 16.237-42 (1965).
12Cf. Philip Bock, The Social Structure of a Canadian Indian Reserve,
Harvard University Ph.D. Dissertation, 1962; and Philip Bock, "Social Structure
and Language Structure," Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 20.393-403 (1964).
13K. L. Pike, "Nucleation," The Modern Language Journal, 44.291-95 (1960).
14See now, however, Joy McCarthy, "Clause Chaining in Kanite," Anthropological
Linguistics, 7.5.59-70 (1965).
15In "Tonemics, Morphotonemics, and Tonal Morphemes, "General Linguistics,
4.1-9 (1959). I had not known of this phenomenon when I published my book on
tone languages in 1948.
16
Versus my skepticism concerning this possibility expressed in my Tone
Languages (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press) 1948, p.11 fn 22.
a
CHAPTER I: CLAUSES
In this chapter clauses are in view, and the central question is: Can
clause typology, structure, and dynamics for an area as a whole be conveniently
discussed in a tagmemic framework? Can we, that is to say, study the selected
basic likenesses--or differneces--in a substantial number of languages (whether
closely related, distantly related, or unrelated) without being overwhelmed by
detail? In this chapter we attempt to show, on preliminary African data, that
the answer to each of these questions is definitely yes. More detail must await
the publication of articles and monographs now in preparation.
1.1. Clause Typology 0
The data on which this chapter is based have been gathered only in a
preliminary form. One special caution grows out of this: The reader must not B
argue from silence. Whereas any particular datum reported is present in the
language concerned (although, even here, later revision may in some instances
yet cause changes), the absence of a clause type in the tentative description
has far less probability of continuing to be confirmed.
Precisely here a prime heuristic value of the preliminary cross-language
comparison is in evidence: Languages of a particular area, from the same lan-
guage family, often share many grammatical characteristics; an item reported
in one suggests a search in texts (or, with due care, with the help of an in-
formant) for the comparable type in the second. If the type does not occur in
a large body of text, and if the informant will not create such a form (as a
translation of an English eliciting model), or rejects suggested forms created
by the analyst on an analogical basis, the absence then appears to be more
significant, and may enter the permanent description as a null cell in the
clause matrix.
1.1.1. Basic (Kernel, Nuclear) Clause Types
1
The first generalization for the region of West Africa: within the
nucleus of their clause systems (i.e., within the kernel, and so basic or cen-
tral to the system) the languages have the clause set:
Intransitive ('without object)
-12-
......._
......,40- ..............
-13-
This at first seems either trivial, because expected human nature requires-
on occasion--reference to an action by someone without reference to another
dramatis persona (or element) involved; action by one person or element on
another; and action by a first party on a second party or item, in reference
to a third. These same data seem important if one is searching for language
universals, since the human necessity for the presence of these relationships
by no means requires that they be formalized in every language in the same way.
Precisely here some elegant formal surprises creep in: A kind of arbitrar-
iness, overriding universal probability,becomes formalized. Normally in Twi,
2
n for example, J. M. Stewart has shown (1) that when two pronouns are expected
after a verb, the first as indirect object and the second as direct (parallel-
ing the normal occurrence of two nouns in this position) the construction is
a not allowed to occur, because of a restriction which allows the pronoun to
come only directly after the verb, and not after another pronoun. The desired
Q communication effect, therefore, is achieved by replacing the construction with
a more complex form (which will be further discussed in §2.2).
.. ,
-14-
Here a universally expected etic relation, expected with a high degree of prob-
ability within simple clauses, is realized only through a different kind of
construction. The result is that the list of non-complex clauses will appear
to have an etic gap at this point; and complex constructions (clause clusters,
see §2.2) will be forced to occur more frequently and to be more varied in
kind, than one might have guessed.
The implications of the preceding data for an etics and an emics of
clause analysis are at least three:
(1) A compilation of known clause types is needed, classified in some con-
venient way. Preferably, experience shows us, this should not be a mere random
list of types, nor even a hierarchy of successively more inclusive types only,
but rather (a) some kind of dimensioned display (like a phonetic chart) with
intersecting categories--which allows the reader to abstract classes, at will,
from various viewpoints (from rows, or from columns, for example, rather than
forcing, by a tree structure, some One predigested set), and (b) some device
-15-
for cutting unwieldiness (when the chart gets too large), through 'multiplica-
tion' of basic matrices by simple elements or by other matrices (or by some
other kind of transformational device--e.g., by transformational commentary,
if the complexity is little enough to make the matrix devices appear to be
more cumbersome than needed).
(2) Emic analyses of clauses are needed, one language at a time. (a)
The sum of these emic clauses becomes the list for a general--universal--etics
under (1). (b) The specific emic clauses need to be studied for variation,
as seen for the Twi constructions containing pronoun forms and definite object
forms discussed above. (c) In order to differentiate these variants from the
emic clauses themselves, contrast between emic clause types must be specified.
(3) Since, (a), it is apparent in the Twi data that specific tagmemes or
even manifesting variants of tagmemes within a clause--e.g., the direct object- -
may affect the possibility of the occurrences of that clause, and since (b) a
concept such as instrumentality, which in other languages may be often found
manifested by a tagmeme within a clause but here is realized, rather, on a
different hierarchical level, it is important, before analysis is assumed to
be complete, that the analysis based on--for example--clauses abstracted from
text be supplemented by eliciting techniques (with the safeguards necessary and
appropriate to all eliciting techniques) one must see how a particular language
expresses those various kinds of concepts reported in other languages, expressed
in some of them within clauses, and assumed to represent human conceptual univ-
ersals.
On the other hard, it is important that the clauses of each language be
studied in reference to its own system, for their distinguishing, contrastive,
formal and informal features; for their kinds and ranges of variability; and
for their structural and transformational relations to, and distribution within,
higher-level or lower-level structures. If one then chooses to do so, comparison
of one area--e.g., West Africa--can be made with other areas of the world. This,
however, is a goal beyond the purview of our present study--but within the lar-
ger goal of which this study has been but one part.
With these elements now more explicitly in mind, we return to the listing
of some clause types observed in West Africa--a list begun, above, with mention
of the intransitive, transitive, and ditransitive.
-16-
'He-goes market'
or: o-ta sa Cpyg n
j
'He-goes to market'
See, also, Bimoba:
n saa daak nie
I am-going market in
II am going to market'
3 ,
Cinsiderably uncertainty has come when analysts have attempted to treat
the locational noun as a direct object of locational-directive verbs such as
these, since the locational clauses contrast with transitive clauses not only
by the potential expansion of locative noun to locative prepositional phrase,
but also in that they canner. undergo certain transformations allowed to the
transitives:
Occasionally this area of form and meaning is divided by the authors into
Equative (or Copulative) and Stative, in which the equative has a predicate nom-
inative, and the stative a special verb plus predicate adjective, or a verb which
carries both the affirmative and the quantitative (as in the Vagala example just
given). For Degema, compare:
51115-y5 ae5
'[The] child-emphatic is-there' (stative)
6m5-375 5-yin 5m5-m5e1
-18-
Less clear, but probably necessary to add to the list of numerous West
African languages is the
Impersonal.
In Kasem, some clauses with the subject ku 'it' differ in function from intran-
sitive clauses which superficially look like them. In a clause cluster (or
series, see §2.2), for example, the impersonal pronoun subject may occur within
the series which has a different pronoun subject, where Kasem otherwise would
require Uniformity. Note the 6...6 series with intervening kd:
...5 vim 5 nowri ku mgngi di sdwla fintb o gya ba
he go he beg it approx. with shillings thirty he take come
The perfect indicative contrasts with the imperfect by having the predicate
follow the object (but precede the locative margin); with object (or predicate)
preceded by clitic k'; with different tone patterns; and with verb-suffix -a.
From this the corresponding negative has predicate as clause final; negative
verb prefix k'; a further distinctive tone pattern; and a Group 2 vowel as
final in verb:
btg kidten bcg sa 6s3:m
`Father meat ate in house'
I
ote gten sa 5s5:m k 6ci
'Father meat in house hasn't-eaten'
Imperative and subjunctive, etc., have further changes for negative.
Some scholars feel that the extensive changes for aspect, as seen above
for Mbembe, make it desirable to treat these as transforms also. Kennedy, for
Dagaari, first sets up a matrix to show basic types (with possibility of trans-
forms to emphasis, mode, and dependency--see Dagaari Matrix I; note gaps- -
signalled by hyphens--for *imperative stative, and *imperative demonstrative,
as well as limitation of locative and demonstrative forms to positive qualities
only(Negative locative concept is expressed by a special negative verb--not
by a regular verb negated by a particle--and that negative verb must be followeu
by a locative word bgy 'there', as in a chg by 'He is-not-located there'.)
DAGAARI MATRIX I: Occurrence-Matrix of Basic Clause Types with Some
Permitted Transformations
Basic
Form (in Perfective
or Imperfective
Aspect)
Ditransitive x x x x x
Transitive x x x x x
Active x x x x x
(Intransitive)
Positive
Locative only x x x x
Stative x x x x
Descriptive x x x x x
Demonstrative Posi- Subj. x - x
tive only only
-21-
da 26w
"He did run'
u do bl zow
'He did not run'
u na 26w
'He will run'
kown 26w
"He will-not run'
DAGAARI MATRIX II: Permitted Tense and Quality Transform Types
Tense
+ Past + Future
Positive + da 'past' + na
zaa 'yesterday'
A. Intransitive:
Reciprocal-
Norm Benefactive Benefactive
Norm 1 3 5
Causal 2 4 6
B. Directive:
Numbers 1 to 4 in above chart
C. Transitive:
Norm Passive Reciprocal
Causal 2 4 6 8 10 12
D. Ditransitive:
Numbers 1-5 and 9-12 in above chart.
E. Stative:
F. Equative:
1. Subjective complement
2. Objective complement
II. Basic Clause Types -- Dependent
A. Introducer
-23-
These are now illustrated, giving first a detailed list of the lexical
items to be used, followed by a citation paradigm with examples numbered accord-
ing to the clause list, followed in turn by tagmemic formulas for these clause
types.
'we' bh - 'them'
ba 'they'
wi 'he' emphatic
Citation Paradigm
I. Independent
A. Intransitive
1. u kukba 'He hid'
9.
'They caused the words to be heard for me'
ba waa - n5 'They saw each other'
ai
10. na bu waa - na - sia 'I caused them to see each other'
11. ba yabanil dwi-a - na (tonal morpheme) 'They bought shirts for each other'
12. na nufi wim bibu waa - na - sia (tonal morpheme)
'I caused his children to see each other for him' a
'-'-'-'7-7-77T7V777777777--
-25-
D. Ditransitive:
1. u man gobi wT. 'He gave me money'
II. Dependent:
A. Introducer:
1. u na,.... 'He said,...'
2. u bh soowa 'He told them
This material from Bariba in Dahomey seems so different from Dagaari, Vagala,
and Kasem of Ghana and from the Mbembe and Degema of the lower part of Nigeria,
that I re-checked, after these pages were written, with Crouch concerning Vagala.
Somehow, the cultural universals of causation and benefaction would have to be
expressed in them also. Had the Bariba type of data been overlooked in these
other languages, or did it in fact not exist?
-26-
Here, once more, the semantic features found their expression in clause
clusters ( §2.2, or subclusters, §2.2.5)--rather than in single clauses with
extra tagmemes within the verb (i.e., as affixes) or outside theverb but
within the single clause.
Compare the following Vagala set for simple normal transitive, with the
corresponding causative transitive cluster and the corresponding benefactive
cluster:
U .k6yog 0
'He did this'
gil h k6yng
'He made him do this'
U 6 U t6 A
'He did it gave me'
'He did it for me'
!
U sd igyo to u 1)&71
+ + + + + +
Ditrans.: + Subject - Tense* + Dit.Pred. +(- I.Obj. - Object) - Manner - Location - Time
NP future, DitVP NP NP adverb locative temporal
1(all) la la,aa,d la(all)
time, NP loc. noun NP
NP l
NP subjunct. 2 NP 2a NP NP 2c
2a c
particles 3 NP2a NP NP NP 3a NP
3c
NP NP 3b NP
4a 5a,b
NPSa,b NP
NP
5a,b
5ab
, 5d NP
NP 5c
NP 6
6
+ + + + +
Trans.: +Subject - Tense + Trans.Predicate - Object - Manner - Location - Time
+ + + + +
Intrans.: + Subject - Tense + Intrans.Pred. - Loc.-Acc. - Manner - Location - Time
+ + + +
Locative: + Subject - Tense + Loc. Predicate + Loc.-Act. - Manner - Location - Time
+ + +
Stative 1: + Subject - Tense + St Predicate - Manner - Location
I ±- Time
same same SOT same same same
la
-3b
2
3
Stative 2:
+ + + +
+Subject - Tense + St Predicate + Adjective - Manner - Location - Time
2
Descr.: + + +
+ Subject - Tense + Des.Predicate + Complement - Manner ± + Location - Time
same same DesVP NP same
la la,aa,b same same
2 NP -3b
3
4a
NP
5a,b,c,d
NP
2a
Demons.: + +
+ Subject - Demonstrative - Tense
Emphatic NP NP restricted time
la(all) la,aa,b
1I
NP NP and adverbial
2a 4a,b particles,
NP NP subjunctive
4a 5a,b,d
NP NP 2a
5a,b
locative
la
r.-3 BM e7r",
-28-
(See, however, the footnote to the chart, indicating that the order of periph-
eral tagmemes cr nanner, location, and time is not completely fixed. Often,
in tagmemic formulas of this kind, arrows from one place in the string to
another show free variation of position.) Contrastive change, for emphasis,
has been discussed in 51.1.2. When a tagmeme is obligatory, it is preceded
by a plus sign: if optional, by +.
Note that, in each instance, any two of these emic clause formulas differ
both by their predicates (or absence of predicate in demonstrative) and by one
other tagmeme (presence or absence of object, adjective complement, etc.),
whether obligatory or optional. The demonstrative differs from other clauses
by its lack of marginal tagmemes.
Underneath each tagmeme is given in a small subcolumn a preliminary list
of the classes of items or constructions which can fill that tagmemic slot, and
which, along with the function named by the tagmemic label, comprises the tag-
meme as a whole--i.e., the function set. Abbreviations for the constructions
include:
Stat.1 IS DS DCSi
1 1
A A
u weyso ri weysg... di u weysc5...
He is good. When he is good... If he is good...
I= c=v t: saC) tz:= C:=1 t=3 c=1 c=) C:=1 am) t= r c=i) r E=3
-31-
may occur in a more extended sample of the one clause but which may not occur
in the other); or (b) in differences of potential for transformation to another
clause type (or in differences, that is, in their potential for coming in
same cells in matrix multiplication), or (c) in differences in occurrence of
the clauses, as wholes, in still higher-level slots--as, for example, in the
slot: for answer to a question versus reply to a statement, etc. Relevant emic
differences include not only the differences of tagmemes within a construction,
but also differences of the clauses, as wholes, comprising restrictions as to
higher units within which the clauses may themselves be found. (Or, one may
say, differences of internal and external distribution are both relevant; or
emic contrasts include features drawn from different hierarchical levels.)
Specifically,
to n kabila
'He gave me fufu-food'
contrasts with
t6-A ka.bila
'Give me fufu-food'
not only by the lack of subject of the imperative (and by tone) but also by the
fact that 'Give me fufu-food' but not 'He gave me fufu-food' can come in the
context:
Comment Reply
to A kg'bila mag to I
Other languages have different lists of optional tagmemes following the nucleus:
Dagaari is reported with location, time, degree, manner--and instrumental-accom-
paniment. (Contrast, for the latter, discussion of lack of basic-clause instru-
mental for Twi, §1.1.1.). For Igede: adverbial, onomatopoeic (ideophones),
time, and perhaps others. Degema: manner, location, time. Bariba (which,
above, was very different from these other languages) agrees in having manner,
location, and degree following the nucleus: but allows cause and time preced-
ing it, and instrument or accompaniment tagmeme within it.
1.33. Variation by Order of Tagmemes
The early placement of the time tagmeme before the nucleus occurs in
Degema and in Sisala, but as a variant from the postnuclear position. In
a ,:.376 wo
'He ran
or a zgnwo
'He jumped'
1.3.5. Variation by Substitution of Manifesting Constructions
a
a
ii
--------111,_-- -- .
-35-
--
-37-
pours
6-gww6
drinks
Onioa Neor
sits seat
B The wave of reference for subject would be the entire cluster; for the object,
the cluster minus the last serial clause.
8 Once it is clear that at least some tagmemes may be suppressed 10 --or
FI
shared--in a clause cluster, we want to know the limits of this sharing. Can
a all peripheral tagmemes, for example, be shared? And if so, in what position
do they come in the cluster?
Other variations of clause structure also occur, but I shall postpon,
discussion of them until §2.2.3 (for tagmeme limits in clause clusters),
§2.2.4. (for agreement factors in clause clusters), and §3 (for changes and
restrictions caused by discourse structure).
-38-
Even the gaps (due to his criteria for selection of somewhat isolated
illustrations for an article designed to illustrate theory) are instructive
in suggesting questions which an outsider would now like to ask of the au-
thor or his informants (whereas a professional scholar of Bantu would prob-
ably know these answers). Field investigation can move rapidly only when one
can ask questions to test a specific hunch about systemic structure. Wrong
divides into transitive and intransitive, and may be modified to become bene-
EI
factive or causative--see the labels on the left column and top row of Matrix I.
We see that from the simple types--transitive and intransitive--can be
derived either benefactive clauses or causative ones. Unanswered, however, is
the question whether both benefactive and causative may simultaneously occur.
Only negative samples are given. The positive contrastive forms are un-
available - -arid therefore the negative signal in the verb complex (which in-
cludes the function of ka at the end of the clause) cannot be identified here.
Dependent clauses are sometimes embedded within the listed clause types, with
a few samples to be discussed below.
Illustrations can now be brought together in a citation paradigm to illus-
trate the three transitive-intransitive pairs of Matrix I--(words involved are
clEngE 'youth', elike-nde '?', olinga 'to like, want', lotomo 'work', ka '?',
o-pim-a 'to go out', o-ten-ela 'to cut', mpomba 'elder', njete 'trees', o -'kon-
isa 'to plant', masangu 'maize'.)
e6
Negative Neutral
+ i
Intransitive (2) 8 + iP +h.q. *(4) -[c]Sac + biP(el) +
+13 -41:1 (6) - S + +
ciP(is) -0 +ka
[c] ac [c] .P-
(c] [c] cau [c] ac CD
I
BOBANOI MATRIX I
t=1 C.= t= =3 Cla =z11 C013 C=f CZ3 C21=1 c:= Crz CZ, C:74)
. -^
-41-
1. Transitive, Simple:
elenge eliki-nde olinga lomoto ka [see (2) pp. 13, 14] 'The youth
did not like the work.'
2. Intransitive, simple:
elenge eliki-nde opima ka [see (2b) p. 14] 'The youth did not go out.'
3. Transitive benefactive:
elenge eliki-nde otenela mpomba njete ka [see (2c) p. 14] 'The youth
did not cut down the trees for the elder.'
4. *Intransitive benefactive:
*(elenge eliki-nde opimela mpomba ka) [constructed, by analogy, from
information from the chart at the bottom of p. 15] *('The youth
did not go out for the elder')
5. Transitive causative:
mpomba eliki-nde okonisa elenge masangu ka [see (2d) p. 14] 'The
elder did not make the youth plant maize.'
6. Intransitive causative:
mpomba eliki -ride opimisa elenge ka [see (2e) p. 15] 'The elder did
not make the youth go out.'
Within the cells of Matrix I we have placed symbols (quite different from
those of Guthrie) to show the points of structural contrast between clauses:
The transitives differ from the intransitives (a) by a different list of
verbs (so far as available illustrations go) in the transitive versus intransi-
tive predicate slot (tP versus iP). In addition (b), the transitive clauses
each optionally(-) have a direct object (0 ) which functions semantically as
the goal of the action of the main verb (if simple or benefactive) or of the
secondary verb (if in a causative clause), whereas the intransitive clauses
have no nuclear object functioning as goal of the main verb.
The benefactive clauses differ from the simple ones (a) by the affix con-
tent of the verb (-el versus zero) and (b) by the specific obligatory (+) ad-
dition of the benefactive tagmeme (B).
The causatives differ from the simple and benefactive clauses (a) by the
affix content of the verb (-is versus zero or -61), (b) by addition of the
13
causal role (the causing of someone's action, not the performing of the action
itself) in the optional subject slot (S ) of the clause, and (c) by the
cau
transformation of the optional subject-as-actor tagmeme (of the simple and
benefactive clauses) into an optional object-as-goal tagmeme.
vrtcomm.
77.777.
--42-
The separate modifiers can themselves, however, fill the subject slot with-
out their head. On this basis Guthrie treats all three tagmemes as 'of equal
status' (p. 8, fn.), and as not comprising con_rastive tagmemes in the internal
chain of the nominal phrase. To us, however, the concord requirements, plus
difference of role (and--perhaps--the impossibility of arbitrary, meaningless
change in the ordering of the three, such as *[losiso lonya lotomo]?) imply the
presence of a zero manifestation of a dominating head in subject (or object)
, slot when the deictic or attributive appear to occur by themselves.
-43-
This modal nomino-verbal phrase (for terms, see p. 16) differs from the
noun phrase with attributive relative clause, in that this second dependent
clause type (a) has an included predicate which is not in concord with its
preceding object and (b) hence differs at least this much in internal structure
from the relative predicate; (c) has its goal optional, so that the predicate
may be itself fill the object slot of the independent clause; (d) is not accom-
panied by a dependent subject; (e) may not, in its form containing the object,
fill the subject slot of an independent clause. For this dependent clause in
context, note:
elenge eliki-nde olinga 0 :NP = (lotomo bomeki) ka (see [6], p.7)
nom-vb g
'The youth did not want to try [to] work'
Allo-Forms--Variants--of the Independent Clause
From the examples provided us, the meaning of neither eliki-nde nor of
ka can be determined. We assume that eliki-nde has some temporal significance,
however, since we are told (p. 13) that if it is omitted, the sentence as a
whole refers to the'time implied in the context'--but that in such an instance
the subject is obligatory, not optional as shown in Matrix I. If these inter-
pretations of the article happen to be correct, then as an allo-construction
of the independent simple transitive clause (1) we have:
(1) - [ + S - Time + tP 3 - 0 + ka]
ac
in which tP is tP of (1) minus the temporal element mentioned. This conclusion
3
is cast in doubt, however, by other data regarding temporal elements which
lead to expansion variants of (1). Preceding S in (1) we optionally find
ac
nambisa 'afterwards' (p. 12) as a Time tagmeme--or, instead, it may follow the
0 g, and in turn be followed by an Instrument-Cause tagmeme, e.g., by naepamba
so, how does this modify Matrix I, both as to general possibilities of sen-
tence types, and as to order of tagmemes within them? And can desideratives
be added to such hypothetical combinations, whether to main or dependent verb,
or both?
Stated in terms for checking with an informant, for example, we are curi-
ous to know how many--if any--of the following sentences are possible--or pos-
sible with change of order; or how comparable semantic components are handled
where these forms a7e incorrect:
(The asterisk before the parentheses means, here, that we have in-
vented these sentences. Their tentative justification or rejec-
tion can be obtained by informant elicitation. Firm judgment,
however, must rest upon finding analogous sentences in running
text. This checking is comparable to the standard checking,
against uncontrolled text, of morphological paradigms--but is
more essential for syntactic material since biased word order
in sentences is more likely to be introduced by elicitation or
by translation than is biased morpheme order, or occurrence,
within words.)
a. *(cicnge eliki-nde opimela mpomba ka) 'The youth did not
go out for the elder'
b. *(clengc eliki-nde opima bomaki ka) 'The youth did not go
out to try'
c. *(cicngc eliki-nde olinga opima ka) 'The youth did not want
to try'
d. *(cicngc eliki-nde olinga otenela mpomba njete ka) 'The
youth did not want to cut down the trees for the elder'
e. *(clengc eliki-nde olinga opimela mpomba k..) 'The youth
did not want tc. go out for the elder'
tt
-47-
_A
-48-
Here the initial optional temporal tagmeme (with noun phrase, or noun phrase
preceded by preposition and temporal marker), may alternatively. come at the
end of the clause (with same fillers, or with a temporal particle).
The independent subject tagmeme is in concord with the predicate per-
sonal-aspect pronoun of the predicate verbal phrase, agreeing with the pro-
noun as to person, number, and gender. fl
Pr Pr
10 o dep
-49-
. Pr
10
A second alternative for transitive requires that certain verb phrases
D (VP-2) be unaccompanied by indirect object, but obligatorily accompanied by
direct object with fillers of that form which would--in the basic transitive
type--be indirect object:
yg cfm ma zki
n 'He-has encountered lion'
.
za'ki 37. to sam masa
4
'[A] lion he-has suddenly-attacked him'
(Note ma in 'him'.)
-50-
woman is
horse is?
From this preliminary sample, inadequate though it may be, several like-
nesses and differences are seen between the 14froasiatic) Hausa and the Niger-
Congo languages of West Africa.
As for likeness, both have a quite similar order of tagmemes in transitive
clauses (e.g., subject, predicate, indirect object, direct object, manner, loca-
tion, time (see the Vagala, above). Both have intransitive, transitive, ditran-
sitive and equative types--though this; list of possibilities may be due as much
15
to some degree of universal constraints on human nature and language communi-
cation as to any other factor.
The complexity of the negative clause in its relation to the affirmative
is an interesting typological parallel.
On the other hand, some striking differences appear: (1) The Hausa seems
to have a much wider variety of transitive subtypes, with co-occurrence rela-
tions between a selected set of verbs and the manifesting forms of indirect and
direct object. (2) Hausa has an instrumental tagmeme in its basic transitive
clause. This would lessen the need for some of the special kinds--or frequency
of use--of serial verbs which are so characteristic of the West African Niger-
Congo languages. (3) Hausa allows two pronouns in sequence, in object posi-
tions; the lack of this freedom leads, in Niger-Congo types such as TwL, to
further forced serial constructions. (4) The obligatory, intricate aspect-
16
pronoun complex cf the Hausa verb phrase is quite different from the usual
simple verb (with pronominal prefix and tone changes) of many of the Niger-
Congo languages.
War
;Nr
-53-
FOOTNOTES
1
Based, of course, on our limited sample--a restriction which we shall
not repeat each time, but which may lead to modifications later. The Niger-
Congo languages of Ghana and Nigeria which we refer to as our primary sources
for clause data are largely Baraba, Dagaari, Kasem, Vagala, Degema, Igede,
Mbembe--and, from secondary sources, Twi.
2
In an article entitled 'Some Restrictions on Objects in Twi,' Journal
of African Languages, 2.145-49 (1963).
For Twi, cf. Stewart, op. cit. p. 149: ' "come, bring, send" ...can be
either intransitive or transitive, but which paradoxically never have a direct
object even when transitive.'
4
Imperfective is derived from perfective by a set of rules, phonologically
conditioned, involving added vowel length, sometimes with r or n, and with vowel
harmony.
5
See his Grammar Discovery Procedures (The Hague: Mouton and Co.) 1964.
Note some restrictions--such as in reference to concord, or agreement, where
two differences do not seem to justify an emic contrast. For concord rer.
ject' and 'object' in fn. 1, p. 16. Our use of labels--rather than Guthrie's
letters and Roman numerals--for tagmemes and tagmemic slots allows the reader
easier insight into the relations involved. Guthrie's work, however, is val-
uable in another fashion, in demonstrating the distributional validity of
groups and sequences, by using formal labels which lack semantic overtones.
14
R. C. Abraham, The Language of the Hausa People (London, 1959). She
uses some supplementary samples for C. T. Hodge, An Outline of Hausa Grammar,
Language Dissertation, #23 (1947); and Hodge and I. Umara, Hausa, Basic Course
(Washington, 1963); C. H. Kraft, op. cit. A Study of Hausa Syntax, Vol. 1-3,
Hartford Studies in Linguistic 8-10 (1963).
15
Precisely here is one need for wider sampling of languages--a sampling
forwarded a bit by this report. Once a large enough sample is available, more
certain generalizations can be made.
16
See chapter 5 for this data.
CHAPTER II! CLAUSE CLUSTERS IN SEITENCES
-55-
Kasem Chart 1
I I 1 I
I
+ + +
C [sequence]: + Primary - Secondary + Primary - Secondary Clause + Primary Secondary Clause
Clause Clause Clause (or Clauses) Clause (or Clauses)
I(or Clauses)
I
1 t
t
Nonterminal [+ Secondary]
CI
__Terminal [- Secondary]
Ditransitive Causative
ICS en =3 =I CMC3 C2111 Ce21 1:223 C=:3 CM CZ) CZ:3 C:23 C=3 C=3 CZZ C:=
IIITII
Kasem Chart 2
CONTRASTIVE NUCLEAR STRUCTURE OF PRIMARY CLAUSES
Final
Dependent +sl/pg +S:NP + Pred:VP
nr
0
Note, therefore, that these clauses differ internally by special particles,
and by the range of tense and mood which is allowed in the verb phrase.
0 Contrasts between the primary clauses of Row C of Chart 1, that is, are differ-
entiating features of elements of Row B.
2.2. Clause Clusters (Serial Clauses)
When, however, our focus shifts to the clause clusters themselves, we
are interested in the relationships between the p :imary and secondary clauses
which respectively comprise the nuclei and the serial members (or margins)
of the clusters. Row D of Kasem Chart 1 shows some of the constraints on this
relation.
2.2.1. General Restrictions on Sequence Types in Clusters
Specifically, certain Kesem clauses (shown in Row E of Kasem Chart 1 to
be the equative and locative clauses) cannot be followed by a secondary, serial
fl clause when they themselves are the nucleus of the cluster (i.e., when they
are primary). This set, therefore, is by the Callows called terminal, in rela-
tion to the cluster. On the contrary, however, nonterminal primary clauses
are required to have a secondary clause (or clauses) following them. This set,
seen in Row E, is made up of a special set of auxiliary, abilitative and causa-
tive clauses. The other type is neutral in respect to this requirement:
it may be followed by a secondary clause, or it may itself close the cluster.
Neutral clauses, as a class, include intransitives, transitives, and ditransi-
tives.
:
-58-
Abil. b na wAni 3 kytiw tii... 'when he had been able to jump down' 0
Caus. 6 na p-e- 3 kydw tu... 'when he had made him jump down'
Equat. 6 na yi bara tit.. 'as he is brave...'
Locat. O na w-i1 sOngo ni tti... 'when he was in the house...'
t.
f-'"",""7777-77=1111111M"-'""""'"7"'-`727-"'"' -
-61-
Secondary
Clause Intrans.2 Trans.2 Ditrans.2 Equat.2 Introducer2 Stat.2 Impers.
2
Primary
Clause
Intransitive x x x 1C x x
1
Transitive x x x x x x
1
Ditransitive x x x x x
1
Equativel x x x x x
Introducer
1
Stative -
1
Impersonal].
Secondary
Clause: Intran. Trans. Ditr. Loc. Stat. Des. Dem.
Primary
Clause:
Intransitive x x x x x x
Transitive x x x x x x
Ditransitive x x x x x x
Locative x x x x x x
Stative x x x x x x
Descriptive x x x x x x
Demonstrative - - - - -
AhI hu 610
we take load
plus ahI chi5 616
we put-on-head load
yields ahi hu 61i; chv
5 ka da 5mU hd TpWa
he will do start take thing to be house completive
On the other hand, the clauses may have mixed aspect, as perfect or imperfect:
ee nil di ra tá wii'zi hOplz1 bily maa
he perfective water and imperfective throw-to fetish day every
(did) (continuation)
'He took water and sprinkled it to the fetish daily'
1
be non-future.
In the Kasem occurrence of subject pronouns in secondary clauses, the
pronoun must agree with the subject of the primary clause in person, number, and
class: its tone is mid in non-continuous clauses, low in continuous ones.
Occasional irregular clusters occur in Kasem--as when the object (instead
of the subject) of the primary clause has the same referent as the pronominal
subject of the secondary clause--see 'mouse it' in the following sentence (which,
normally, would be broken into ?arallel regular clauses or sentences):
mu o ne tutwdy di gyey di soli gOli yi dl nabiyli D
focus he saw mouse it sitting it stirring porridge and its tail
sin zwin
washing calabashes
eft
'He saw a mouse sitting stirring porridge while its tail washed calabashesli
Here we have preserved, on the first level of symbolization, the special two-
tagmeme close-knit series, of an instrument set forth and then utilized. Within
each of the tagmemic slots (shown by vertical lines) is an included construction,
tagmemically symbolized, filling the slot. For each of its slots, in turn, the
fillers are given. Here note that the only verb which is allowed as a filler of
the instrumental slot is 'take'. Similarly, only the transitive verb is allowed
as the second of the series--and its object must be different from the object
of the instrumental member of the series.
In contrast with this, the regular series of a sequence type has various
allo-constructions which allow a great deal more flexibility. We give typical
samples here, involving transitive versus intransitive verb, and presence or ab-
sence of object:
Alloal: (+S) +P +0 +P +0
tr tr
IV NP IV NP
tr 1 tr 2
Allo : ( +S) +0 +0
a2 +Ptr +Ptr
IV INP IV I -NP
tr 1 tr 1
Allo : (+S) +P -0 +P
a3 tr tr
fl
V EJ
tr IVtr INP2 E-NP11
Allob: (+S) +P. +P
intr i ntr
vintr v
intr
Allot: (+S)
Vntr
+Ptr
tr
±0
+1)i
v
intr
Alio (+S) +P. +Ptr ±0
d. intr
vntr v
tr
These allo-constructions are designed to show certain co-occurrence restric-
tions. Allo-Set a begins with a regular transitive verb and is followed by a
direct object--with the direct object of the second transitive verb deleted
(Allo a-2) if it is the same as that for the first. The third allo of Set a
with transitive verb deletes the first direct object if it is in a sequence of
sentences, such that the context implies clearly what this object is. Allo b
includes intransitives only; whereas c and d show transitive with intransitive,
111715.7,11,1e, um,
-69-
9 and intransitive with transitive respectively. (Further allos occur when emphasis
leads to shift of certain tagmemes to the front of the construction--or when pe-
ripheral tagmemes are involved. These are not of concern to us at the moment.)
What we wish to point out is that ambiguity can occur with the instrumental
and the sequence series only when the sequence is of such a type that the first
verb of a sequence is kpa and the second verb is transitive with a noun-phrase
object which differs from that of the object of the first noun phrase. Such a
circumstance can occur with
kpa bikAwne di ee kylAl
he took round-stone and made [did]. blood
That is 'He "xid" and (he) "y'd".'
The second of these clauses, as indicated by the formula, can have the
first verb replaced without destroying the overall relationship of the parts-
for example, 'He saw a stone and made blood'.
On the other hand, the same morpheme sequence might conceivably manifest
the instrumental subcluster: 'He, with "x", "y-ed", 'He with a stone made blood'.
But in that case the kpa 'take' is replaceable by no other verb; and the second
noun cannot be replaced by the first (*'He take stone made stone') and then be
deleted(*'He take stone make') according to the formula for the regular cluster,
Allo a-2; nor can the second verb be replaced by an intransitive (*'He tike
stone ran') as for allo-c of the regular cluster.
The instrumental series is a close-knit subcluster of the grammatical hier-
archy coning between the simple clause and a regular--loose-knit--clause cluster,
and constituting a complex unit in its own right, with its own contrastive tagmemic
sequence.
In our view, furthermore, these unit subclusters with their special tagmemes
appear to be somehow "new" to the language. The language is in a state of
transition. Here the transition is toward a more complex structure of clause
clusters. Only a dynamic view of the total system can do descriptive justice
to such data.
Although we have used the instrumental subcluster for illustrative purposes,
Vagala seems to have various other types of subclusters currently relevant, or
in process of development:
I3enefactive:
In the benefactive, the verb to 'give' works with the main verb, which
precedes it. The object of to is the benefactee of the action of the main
-70-
The verbs kpa 'take' and laU 'take hold of are often used as accessory
verbs to a main verb. The main verb may occur alone and take an object, but some
verbs rarely occur without the accessory verb. The accessory verb precedes and
takes the object (if there is one) and the main verb follows, without an object,
AIN
since the object must be the same in this construction, and, therfore, not repeated: [I
a kpa 'nmag dU
he tocY rope put-in his bag
'He put the rope in his bag'
! .
compare: U du nmeng u huwr
he put rope his bag
Accessory motion:
Preliminary counts suggest that motion-verb combinations are more common
than any other type in the language. The verb iyzi 'get up', for example, in
one set of texts, occurred alone 45 times, and in combination with another verb
180 times. In many instances the meaning had become specialized or slightly changed.
-71-
Another verb kaall. 'to leave' occurred alone 45 times and in combination 60 Limes.
(It occurred alone more often in questions and commands.) When it occurred in
combination it often had a slightly different or specialized meaning.
U I kpglkeeli
he took went
'He went'
it. Note, for example, that the verb sow 'sit' has low stem tone and a suffix
when it occurs in the primary clause, but it is high and does not have the suffix
when it occurs in a cluster:
!
-73-
bk.
verb which follows them, however, will be marked not only with one of these suffixes
but all the other evidences of a main primary verb of a regular series.
In addition, four other characteristics make the wa and gA differ from main
verbs: (1) They cannot be preceded by the imperfective ra. (2) wa and ga are
not allowed to occur alone as verbs; they occur only in this position modifying
a main verb. (3) The tone rules which apply to wa and ga are a different set
from those which affect ordinary verbs: The tone of wa and ga may be raised by
a high tone on a preceding word in nonfuture constructions; in the future con-
structions (including purpose clauses), wa and ga are never raised by the preceding
high tone subject or particle, whereas other low verbs are. (4) Unlike other verbs
discussed, whether they are primary or secondary in a cluster, in a series these
auxiliaries are not allowed to have those complementing tagmemes such as location
or adverb which sometimes accompany ordinary verbs.
If this were the complete picture, one might feel that it was not worth-
while calling these morphemes verbs in any sense--but that it was preferable to
set them up as some kind of particle (even though in a neighboring dialect of
Vagaln the wa and 0 act as fully regular verbs the dynamic breakdown has not
gone as far 1n that dialect).
Our reasons for treating them as verbs: (1) In the same kind of 'auxiliary'
function there are three further stages of morphemes transitional between these
highly dependent morphemes and the regular independent verbs. It is convenient,
therefore, to treat wa and 0 as verbs--of a special auxiliary type--in the same
way as we want to treat the morphemes which are in a lesser stage of breakdown
in the same class, The morpheme kuari 'make, fix', for example, has a slightly
greater degree of freedom. It can, as a matter of fact, come alone as a verb or
as primary in a cluster. Note the following illustration:
U kugrd
'it is-fixed'
(2) In these circumstances it can have peripheral tagmemes, as can any regular
verb. When, however, kuari comes in the preposed auxiliary position where wa and
ga can occur, several changes occur. It takes on a special meaning of 'again'.
Note, for example:
U kuari la dig
he again went home
'He went home again'
-74--
(3) It is no longer allowed to have a perfective ending which was possible for
6
it earlier (see illustration under [1]).
(4) In contrast to verbs after wa and 0, a verb following kuari will be treated
like the secondary verb of a cluster.
One could, presumably, refuse to identify the auxiliary kuari with the
separate verb kuari. If one adopted this course, then one would merely move the
homophonous auxiliary kilari into the class with wa and ga. We, however, much prefer
for purposes of this article, to treat the two kuari forms as still the same
morpheme. We are attempting to bring forth the evidence which shows that the system
is in a state of transition--and it is precisely in terms of residues such as this
one that incompleteness of change can be observed. The form kuari, in our view,
is in the process of becoming a particle. If all the members of the class were
like wa and ga, on the other hand, we would assume that this change of the class
from verb to particle were already complete.
A third stage of transition is even closer to the regular verb than is
kuari. In this sub-set, also, we have just one member, bir 'to turn'. With
this same meaning, the ,=r1) occurs as a regular member of a cluster--either
primary or secondary. When it is the primary verb, the secondary verb following
has the serial form.
bitO ba
he turned came
'He came baa'
u izo di bir u har
he got up and turned his back
In these regular positions of the cluster, as main primary or main secondary
verb, it (like other verbs) can have the normal sets of peripheral tagmemes.
Note, for example, the following:
bir leng
verb--nor as a main verb in a cluster. The developing tagmemic slot has forced
on it a semantic change.
We come now to an even more difficult decision. Should weyr 'be able',
ktiti 'do purposely', ai.d futiri 'do unintentionally' be treated as auxiliaries
or as regular verbs? In favor of treating them as main verbs we note:
(1) They take the perfective endings:
.
weyto 16 ka-bila
he is-able pound fufu
'He is-able to pound fufu [mush]'
(2) The verb which follows them has the secondary form, not the primary form:
ktit6 bir
'He purposely turned'
(3) They can stand alone:
weyto
'He is-able'
On the other hand, we choose to treat these as auxiliariesor, more
accurately, as just moving into the auxiliary function--for the following reasons:
(1) If, in rare instances, these do stand alone, it is usually in a cultural
context in which a following main verb is understood, as for example in the fol-
lowing situation:
Question: i weyto ee gung '[Are] you able [to] do that?'
Response: ft weytii 'I am-able'
(2) When occurring with another verb following them (cien though the following
verb is in the secondary form as just indicated above) no peripheral tagmemes
are allowed to occur with them. (There can be no adverb, or locative, etc.
with them.) In this respect, therefore, they have already begun to receive the
specialized function which characterizes the restrictions on the preceding three
subsets.
shares any peripheral tagmemes with the preceding main verb (never has any which
refer only to it.) Example:
u kudr-O hunzi
he fix it fail
'He wasn't able to fix it'
Some other verbs, e.g., tOng 'finish', kiing 'tire', ping 'be satisfied' also
seem to share peripheral tagmemes with the preceding verb. Examples:
pato tung
he hoed finished
'He finished hoeing'
path kOng
he hoed tired
'He hoed until he was tired'
!
di ywo ping
'He ate until he was satisfied'
full
.0192--,I.
-77
(2) The verbs ha and ta, both meaning 'throw' frequently have a slightly different
meaning when occurring with other verbs:
gil u ha
he left him throw
'He deserted him'
U le u ha
he got him throw
'He saved him'
nmeng U ha
he cut-off it throw
'He cut it off'
(3) When used as post-auxiliaries, these verbs may have peripheral tagmemes only
if these tagmemes apply both to the auxiliary and to the main verb:
hUnzi deelA
he fix-it fail yesterday
'He wasn't ablei[yesterday] to fix it yesterday'
So, also, tung 'to finish', kung 'to become tired', ping 'to be satisfied':
U pato tUng creel:a
FOOTNOTES
1
Dependent clauses, whether initial ones which may optionally precede the
sentence nucleus, or final ones, have been but little studied for this report.
But see some earlier work: Structure of Subcrdinate Clause Groups in Kasem, a
paper presented by Kathleen Callow to the Fifth West African Languages Congress
(April, 1965).
2
I have changed the order in which he listed them to parallel more closely
that of Jacobs.
3
Kennedy suggests further restrictions of distribution in relation to
some special clause sequences, involving benefactive, desiderative, and comparative,
and following auxiliary. It is not clear to me from his data, however, how these
would be related to clause subclusters--see §2.2.5. A complete distributional
statement must await further research.
4
Kasem is the one exception listed in our materials, where a secondary
clause must begin with a pronominal subject. There the identification of a
clause sequence as a clause cluster is determined by other criteria, such as
relation to slots in the sentence ( §2.1) and agreement restrictions in the series
( §2.2.4).
5
Y. Callow states more generally, for Kasem, that in the secondary clause
no preverbal tagmeme ever occurs other than the [pronominal] subject--so that
in secondary clauses there is obligatory absence of the introductory and tem-
poral tagmemes, and of all preverbal particles, including negative ones. (Nega-
tion of secondary clauses must be accomplished by dropping the cluster form, adding
a conjunction, and producing a complex sentence.) Consequently, fifty percent of 13
their observed secondary clauses were minimal--pronoun plus verb (whereas many- Q E-
a
CHAPTER BEYOND THE SENTENCE
other than independent. Finally (4) we wish to suggest that the relationships
between the independent and the dependent elements in these paragraphs may lead
to sets with semantic significance.
1
(1) There is a clear formal marking, in at least some of the primary in-
dependent sentences versus the secondary dependent sentences. They are char-
acterized by a contrastive complicated pattern of tone differences, occasionally
supplemented by special suffixes. In some instances, for example, the dependent
verb will be mono-syllabic high--as for she 'carried [on head }' -- whereas the in-
dependent form has the first syllable in low tone, and an added suffix g6 with
high tone. Note the following contrastive pair:
Independent: u kylgo nif ba
she carried water came
Dependent: u kyf nif ba
she carried water came
The independent sentence of the paragraph can often be thought of as a
variety of 'topic' sentence--or one with the verb marked for focus of attention.
For a contrastive pair in context, note the high tone of ya 5 'buy' in the first
sentence but also in the second sentence the low tone yail 'buy' (the translation
could be in past tense, by implication of the context, not by formal marking):
-80-
order, however, to see the focus versus out-of-focus differences where the verb
changes, we use a pair of paragraphs. In the first paragraph, the primary sen-
tence will have the verb in focus, but in the secondary sentence a verb out-of-
focus; in the second paragraph the first sentence will now have the latter in
focus and the former first verb will become the second, but out of focus. Note
the verb kyag 'wash' in focus in the first paragraph, versus nyer 'sew' out of
focus; and nyer in focus, but kyag out of focus, in the second sentence pair:
haang la kyag wegyezi u gyaari gey nyer tagta
woman the washed clothes her husband contrastive sewed shirt
But now note neyr in focus, with kyag out of focus:
bal la nyer tagta u haang gey kyag wegyezi
man the sewed shirt His wife contrastive washed clothes
Sometimes, as in a clause cluster, tenses are given only in the primary sen-
tence, and not in the secondary sentence 'elaborating' the first:
6. ? 6. I 6. A A I 6. 6. I 6. .
n nu n haang ne de ra naa n haang kyag
I and my wife emph yesterday imperfect fight, my wife insult
(tense)
A kyage 11341.
me insult one
'Yesterday I and my wife had a fight (quarrel). She insulted me
(with a certain insult)'
(2) We next wish to show that the structure which we have called para-
graph is not, in fact, merely a clause cluster of types such as we have had
earlier. One might have assumed that all such paragraphs could be defined as
simple clause clusters with the following rule: In the clause clusters here
called paragraphs, the second clause has a subject different from the first,
-81-
and therefore the second subject is retained; but in the clause clusters pre-
viously defined ( §2.2) the second subject happens to be identical with the
first and is automatically deleted. This at first appears to be an attractive
rule, since relationships semantically between elements of a clause cluster
are often similar to the relationships between the clauses of the sentences
of a sentence cluster. Note the paralellism of structure in the following
two illustrations, in which the first has two sentences, but the second two
clauses of which the second has deleted subject:
dgglf dolzi di diy di ping
she cooked soup and ate and was satisfied
dagli do'I zi ba diy di ping
she cooked soup They ate and were satisfied.
How, we then ask ourselves, can one prove that the first set is indeed a se-
quence of sentences--rather than one of clauses?
It appeared to us that the crucial differential evidence to separate these
as belonging to different levels of the grammatical hierarchy would reside in
studying more complex sentences. We will recall that from the Callows' mate-
rial ( §2.1) a single complex sentence may have an optional dependent clause,
an obligatory independent clause, and a following optional dependent clause.
Furthermore, the Callows demonstrated that in each of these three positions
within a sentence there could be a clause cluster. Note, therefore, that
there is a crucial element in such a complex sentence--each component of the
sentence, each dependent and independent clause complex, can be either a single
clause or a clause cluster. It follows, therefore, that a complex sentence
with subordinate and independent clauses is not the same as a sequence of
clauses in a cluster (since it is essential that one be able to speak of sev-
eral clause clusters within a single sentence which is complex in this way).
If, therefore, one can show that, in the focus versus out-of-focus sequence of
independent versus dependent sentences the dependent sentence slot (or the in-
dependent slot) may be filled by a complex sentence which is itself composed of
dependent and independent elements which are clause clusters, it follows that
the sequence of independent dependent sentences must be on higher level than
that of the clause clusters included within one of the major parts of one of the
other of the sentences.
-82-
D
te u
h kyOwl
It fell,
Initiating action plus result of action:
u kpa daazi du nung nfyng lau daazi la
Various types of discourse differed from each other in ways barely investi-
gated. In narrative, for example, Rowland failed to find certain words of a type
common in conversation--words such as 'day before yesterday,' 'yesterday,' 'to-
day,' 'tomorrow,' 'day after tomorrow.' Compound time words and time phrases,
however, were found in narrative.
Within narrative structure a sequence of tagmemic slots seemed to occur:
± Preview ± Narrative Setting ± Sequential ± Focus change...±Climax
-Comment by Narrator Summary Application
In general, each slot in this structure has respective identifying formulas
II composed of the kinds of variants of transitive clauses occurring there.
Preliminary formula for transitive clause in slot for narrative setting:
ma
Here time phrases, or particles, are rare, and limited to clause final posi-
tion; time phrases exclude the types mentioned for conversational style, but
include those for narrative plus element T3 which is a particle fa meaning
'pre-present'--i.e., of historical relevance.
In the focus-change slot, only one sentence occurs at a time. Its struc-
ture is similar to that of narrative except that particles do not occur between
subject and predicate; and the clause may optionally end with wa/ya 'emphatic.'
The Climax slot has a formula like the setting slot, minus the initial
time tagmeme, but adding optional final emphatic particles.
The summary slot (and the initial preview slot) has the time particle fa --
and no other--occurring optionally between subject and predicate, or between
subject and particle + predicate.
Results from Vagala research--even in a most preliminary form--reinforce,
for West Africa, conviction that structures like that hinted at for Sisala may
be widely found. Special preliminary markers, in addition (like 'a long time
ago', for English) may open the story:
-86-
h mdr td kdyng
-
-87-
iy
-88--
Subject of Q.I.
2 ? I
1 A I/D I/D
3 I I/D 9 I/D
n 4 1 I/D ?
n is 'They would say [to you]' and the quotation is indirect in that cell of
Matrix A (i.e., M.A ):
32
b4r1 tii vet ba tunuma ba koo bikia amona mba ka mba
1 3 4 5 6 7' 8 9 10 11 112
'When2 they3 arrived4 they5 themselves]. would5 ask7 [you] how8 you12
Subject
1 A (D)
3 (D)
4 (D)
'They]. said2, "(You3) go4 and stay (drop off)8 there9." So itil
tabu ko a wi s-etnu ke
12 13, 14 15 16 17
FOOTNOTES
1
Others are ambiguous where the expected contrasts are neutralized.
2
Primary and secondary are used here in reference to relation to state-
ments. This use differs from that of Section 2.1, in reference to clauses
in a clause cluster.
3
First pointed out in matrix form in the submorphemic (or morphemic)
"formatives" of a complicated morphological structure. See K. L. Pike and
B. Erickson, 'Conflated Field Structures in Potawotomi and in Arabic, '
We have already had occasion to list some kinds of noun phrases, as fillers
of the subject slot of clauses (for Dagaari, in §1.3.5) and of other slots (for
Vagala, in §1.2). We now wish to see more detail of this type, as well as the
structure of the nouns themselves.
4.1 Types of Noun Phrases
For Mbembe, Barnwell gives as the most common types of nominal phrases the
following (symbolized in terms of their manifesting classes rather than by their
comprising tagmemes):
+ noun - (+ demonstrative particle): eten ndo sa
meat that there
!
+ noun + possessive: aten CE
meat his
+ noun
+ (-
+ min:a + numeral): anbr) min:a afa
Less common is a Mbembe time phrase (in appropriate clause slots) with a
time word as its head, with optional expansion by a relative clause:
awil 'tido c makwu ma
day that which they-came here
or a prepositional phrase, with preposition plus optional noun phrase or pronoun,
to tortoise to house
'to tortoise's house'
The most common Mbembe phrase composites are noun phrases in appositional
sequence (or pronoun in apposition to a phrase, or vice versa): or with a coor-
dinating link: or one subordinate (e.g., a relative phrase) to another.
In order to show in more detail the relation of noun phrase types to their
distribution in tagmemic slots, however, we return to the Vagala material of
Crouch. Classes of phrases are given here. Their distribution in clauseswith
some margin of error--was indicated in the chart in §1.2.
-93--
--94-
diini &Le la
adverbs same
A sal-1ga sanga la
the (that) time time the
1
a ha 'bgl a ha riga
a day one day that
'one day' 'that day'
d-- + Head
numeral
[a ga kg] gne
[they went left] two
'there were two left/they left two'
dig la n ky6w16 la
house the rel. fell the
+
b-- + Relative Head + Relator - Post-Demonstrative
directional nouns
NP la
NP directional-ni
lc
adjectives directional-ni
RI dd] sum ni ne
[it is] sweet emph.
-97-
NP NP NP
la laa laa
locative NP
laa
time noun NP
laa
time nouns (?)
kere kyciozi
tomorrow morning
adverb NP
laa
NP conn. NP conn. NP
la la la
NP NP NP
4a 4a 4a
NP3a
+
b-- + Coordinate Head + Conn. + Coordinate Head - Conn.... n
baniy di ban&
five and two
'seven people'
+
c-- + Coordinate Head + Conn. + Coordinate Head Conn.... n
NP NP
lc lc lc.
NP NP NP
2c 2c 2c
NP NP NP
3c 3c 3c
rilMormir, 7-
-_
-99-
CI
NP NP NP
4b 4b 4b
NP NP NP
laa laa laa laa
[deyni kpgwo ra
l la, di] wokozi nly kpownazi, vibazi...
then he took cont. go, and yams and yams yams...
'He went and came upon yams [type one], and yams [type two], and
yams [type three]...' (Here niy is optional, and no,t indicated in
of this section is quoted directly from the report of Mrs. Nancy Bergman, A Note
3
The Problem:
In Igede, certain nouns in sequence were seen to be fusing, with the fused
form sometimes having a specialized meaning. Were these fused forms simply fol-
lowing fusion rules which were normal to syntactic phrases, or were they a more
closely bound morphological unit? A practical problem was also involved: How
should these noun complexes be written--as two words or one?
Ube + utoji > iiblitTijI 'clinic'
room medicine
(Notice that the final vowel and tone of the first noun, Ube 'room',
completely elides.)
The Aim:
In view of the setting up of a new orthography, the aim, necessarily, was to
determine the difference (if any) between cwo nouns which are simply following
normal fusion rules and two nouns which are more closely bound together as a
single unit.
The Approach and Research:
The first step was to locate the different slots in which each noun and
each fused form could occur. In other words, in what slots can Ube 'room', titT5g.
'medicine', and Ubtitoji 'clinic' be distributed? Are they found in the same slots
*(anU, vturu, and -e-ntit-Jril are all time words so they have certain restrictions
Once again, notice that in noun, the final vowel and tone are dropped and are
completely overridden by the initial vowel and tone of the second noun.
The next step proved to be very interesting and quite helpful. A test of
internal expansion was tried for each of the fused forms. In a noun + noun re-
lationship, only nyd 'for, of (possessed by)' can occur between two nouns.
Not only do Igede speakers say:
fl
UbutOji
room-medicine
'clinic'
B
-102-
imenyi 'thirst x
storage'
iibUpT) 'school' x
611-61uhye 'God'
'character'
Tpweji 'village'
TigbOliihye 'airplane'
Entit17)rT) 'day'
i yenyi 'fish'
Now we ask: Why this difference? What is the controlling factor here?
It seemed apparent that there were differences in degrees of fusion in the
Igede noun + noun relationship. Following this hypothesis, four degrees of
noun fusion were set up: (a) obligatory absence of fusion, (b) optional fusion,
(c) obligatory fusion]. (with identifiable morphemes), and (d) obligatory fusion
2
a
(some parts unidentifiable).
(a) Obligatory absence of fusion. The loosest type of noun + noun rela-
tionship in Igede is found with certain nouns which cannot fuse and which have
the obligatory particle nya occurring, which usually denotes a genitive re-
lationship.
onyI nya Job ry oje ny(a) ;1e6
child of Job bicycle of man
'Job's child' 'the man's bicycle'
(b) Optional fusion. This type of noun + noun relationship is made up of
a set of nouns which fusr but which also can be expanded without changing the
meaning.
imenyl r"\J Imi ny(a) enyi
hunger-water hunger for water
'thirst' 'thirst'
Lam_ T
-103-
In fact, 'room for writing' occurs more frequently than 'room-writing', which
may be an indication that the form with nyd used to be the prevalent form,
but through the years fusion has become more dominant.
(c) Obligatory fusion,. In this type of noun + noun relationship there
is a very strong degree of fusion; obligatorily so, in fact. These fused
forms cannot be expanded, but the morphemes constituting the forms are still
recognizable and definable by informants (see also, the preceding list for
the full forms of the fusing nouns).
0 awT1A TPweji
hands-matter house-ground
'character' 'village'
There is wider divergence from the meaning of the source morphemes when there
is a tighter degree of fusion. That is, when two nouns fuse, the meaning of
the whole is often quite different from the meaning of the nouns as separate
forms.
- e
- e o- 40. x
-5 E_
-a 6-
-0 o-
-u 13-
-o e-
-u 5-
- u g-
-104-
Of the nouns considered in this Matrix, optional fusion occurs only when
the final vowel of the first noun is a front vowel. This occurs, as is seen
in the upper section of the matrix, in the fused forms, which can be expanded.
Obligatory fusion occurs when the final vowel of the first noun is a
central or back vowel knotice the one exception, which is front vowel plus
low, back vowel; it is shown between the two main sections of the matrix).
Also notice that these forms are the ones which can nct be expanded.
(d) Obligatory fusion2. Finally, the tightest degree of fusion in the
Igede noun + noun relationship is considered, where:
(i) there is no separate occurrence of the morphemes, and
(ii) the semantics of some morphemes cannot be determined by most
informants (but source in noun + noun pattern is assumed).
0,
ihwomu ihwcnyI
year-? year-?
'last year' 'this year'
The informant tried to attach a meaning to cnyI and omu but he was incon-
sistent, particularly with omu; and since we have not gotten any meanings from
previous informants, my conclusion is that no one is really sure of the mean-
ings any more. This could also be an indication that change is taking place
in Igede towards not only a dominance of fusion but also towards obligatory
fusion. In the first three types (obligatory absence of fusion, optional fu-
sion, and obligatory fusioni), the morphemes are still recognizable and de-
finable--but in the last type this becomes extremely difficult.
Conclusion.
My conclusion and solution to the problem, therefore, is:
(1) Types (a) and (b) are phrases and should be written as sequences of
separate words.
(2) Types (c) and (d) are compounds and should be written as single words.
4.3. Syntactic Markers Developed from Nouns
Just as independent verbs sometimes developed into restricted auxiliaries
or particles ( §2.2.6), furthermore, so regular freely-distributed nouns may
develop into particles or into special word classes marking other syntactic
functions. For illustrating the synchronic discovery and functional descrip-
tion of this dynamic state, we tux_ back to Vagala, with data from Crouch:
4
D -105-
Li
In Vagala a large list of nouns may occur with or without accompanying
modifiers or other compounds of noun phrases. Note, for example, dla 'house'
in the phrases:
dia la din{ ane la
house that house two those
'that house' 'those two houses'
...- .1
dia la u n ma'we la
house that he which built that
'the house which he built'
A locative element may optionally follow a noun phrase with locative-noun
head (e.g., dia 'house') in locative slot; but is required in lc-ative phrases
with nonlocative head (e.g., maa 'mother'). Compare the phrase after transi-
tive verb na 'see', il na il maa /a 'he saw his mother that', with a locative
phrase after a verb of motion la 'go: il la il mag la begy 'he went [to] his
1
mother's vicinity'. Note that the marking locative can also follow a fuller
noun phrase: i
1 %
n la dini Ana la seey
I went houses two those front
'I went to the front of those two houses' i
.
-106-
'my mouth hurts' or h 16 h nild 'it hit my mouth', buc one cannot use in these
positions either ni 'on, vicinity', or by 'to, presence'. Nevertheless one
finds n la diva 1a by 'I went house that presence (I went to the house)' with
bey, and u sowgo ka ra la ni 'he sat chair that vicinity (he sat on the chair)'
with ni. We conclude, then, that there is a specialized subset of nouns, iden-
tified by the fact that most of them occur as nouns in subject and object slot.
But they are a specialized subset in that they have a semantic central meaning,
in general (body part) with a tagmemic overlay of meaning (direction). As
members of this class, however, there are two elements to be analyzed as nouns,
on the basis of analogy with the other members of the class, even though they
occur here alone. ci
The resulting assumption is that these two morphemes by and
ni should be treated as nouns with restricted distribution; and that one should
be able to find them as regular nouns in some related dialzts.
4.4. Irregularities Within Noun Classes as Distortion of Field
Turning now to the structure of a nominal system, we find in a large num-
ber of Niger-Congo languages a difference in stems according to vowel harmony-
3
in which the vowels are paired into 'upper' (close) or 'lower' (open) sets.
The upper set, under certain circumstances, determines the selection of a mem-
ber of the comparable set in an accompanying word or affix.
The relations between the variant forms may be expressed by morphophonemic
rules or by prosodic formulas of a Firthian type, or by rules.
4
For this project, however, John Callow (who had previously explored three
alternatives) tested a presentation of Kasem data via matrix display. A small
sample of bis results are given here. (For further material, see Appendix.)
The first sample displays a simple relation between a chart of patterns
of Kasem noun stems, chosen because they comprise a class ending in d, but
arranged in two halves. The upper half of the chart includes stems which con-
tain upper vowels, placed in the order the vowels would come on a vowel chart.
The pattern of arrangement of the lower half of the chart as a whole is re-
peated, but with stems containing lower members of the paired vowels. The stem-
initial consonant is irrelevant to the structural relation, and is represented
by C. To the right is the word form, composed of the respective stem shape plus
the 'singular' morpheme, with allomorph /a/ after upper-harmony stems, but the
allomorph /a/ after lower harmonies.
'pee?.
77,
-107-
U
KASEM MATRIX I: Nominal-Group C, with d-final Stems
The 0 signifies systemic non-occurrence, i.e., /e/ and /e/ cannot occur
in stems of this general shape.
No startling results come from one simple chart of this kind. But it lays the
groundwork for compacting rules via matrix patterns. If, for example, the
upper left were to be written as Nu-d (noun of upper harmony, final consonant)
u u
versus NQ -d, the rule N -d + {e}N -d+a NZIld + a points toward a device in
6 5 6
By use of the braces, pairs of sub-groups can be shown: (1) the set of stems
ending in alveolar consonants; these have no variants in the plural; versus (2)
sterns ending in velar consonant; the consonant is lost in plural. (3) Stems
-108--
ending (a) in velar consonants and (b) containing rounded vowels; the vowel is
replaced by hal, versus (4) stems ending in front or central vowel; the vowel
is lost. (5) Stems (a) ending in velar consonants, (b) containing low vowels
a, o (central or back); the plural allomorph is the low front vowel (in open
or close harmony); versus (6) stems (a) ending in velar consonant but (b) with
f
high vowel; the plural allomorph is the high front vowel. (7) Stems (a) ending
in velar nasal consonant; (b) included in previously-mentioned set (6), (i.e.,
i
with high vowel); the suffix vowel alternates freely from oral to nasal quality;
versus (8) stems (a) ending in velar stop, and. (b) included in (5) and (6)
above; no further criterion; hence those paired members of (7) and (8) are am-
biguous except when (in 7) the freely-variant nasal vowel differentiates them.
Some Kasem examples:
bide 'wall' bidi 'walls'
jioa
u
'hand, arm' jLijr 'hands, arms'
digs 'room' di 'rooms' 1
B
-109-
When, now, we turn to the Etung, of the Bantoid5 group of West Africa, we
find not only singular-plural stem and affix differences, but two further exten-
sive phenomena. (1) The concord-agreement between the noun and other words in
the immediate noun phrase, and between the noun and some other tagmemes in the
clause; (2) a system--well-known for Bantu--in which (a) some particular prefix--
1 6,
or formative --(e.g., /en, used for singular, forms a pair with a correspond-
ing prefix (e.g., /bi/) used for plural with that same stem; whereas (b) on a
different stem, the same prefix used earlier for plural may now appear as marker
for the singular, now paired with a different prefix to mark the plural of this
second stem; etc. (c) The morphophonemic changes or suppletive replacements
(or correlative formatives of a concord set) undergone by any one prefix through-
out the concord patterns is the same whether it applies to its use as singular
or its use as plural, or whether paired with one or more shapes for different
stems respectively.
The following analysis and presentation of the Etung concord is prepared
in collaboration with Mrs. Eileen Edmonson. (For her extensive list of noun
stems according to overlapping singular-plural pairings of prefix shapes, see
Appendix.)
A striking insight of Edmonson added a further characteristic to the list
of those already mentioned: (d) There are patterned constraints (a ranking)
on the permitted pairings of prefix shapes: Some prefixes are allowed to occur
much more freely than others; and the occurrence-possibility forms a 'ranking'
series such that--to some degree--if the first prefix pairs with the second (in
singular to plural relation), or with the the third, or fourth, then the second
may be expected to pair with the third and fourth, while the third may be ex-
pected to pair with the fourth. The system is not as clean-cut and regular as
such a model implies, however, so the data are presented in Etung Figure 1.
Arrows connect the prefixes--with the arrow head indicating the plural form
of the pair. Arrows above the line refer to countable nouns, which may be found
with numerals in the plural. The dotted shaft shows movement form right to left
in the chart--with only two pairs, both involving o. An arrow completely over
a prefix indicates that certain stems use the same prefix in singular and in
plural. Arrows beneath the prefixes have nonmodified plurals as indicated--but
when accompanied by numerals, the plural words use the prefix which otherwise
1E1E11 OIOM 1E1E11 MEI OMNI. 11=1 111111111 11=1 IMOD - MIMI IMO 11111111
$
)
e N bi a Iba
Etung Figure 1
CZ3 C=3 co
ilrir"mr.tut MrtmgrA, loormropprommon
would be singular. Certain mass nouns do not occur in numerical plural contexts;
the contrast between singular and plural is neutralized; a prefix which occurs
with a set of these nouns is surrounded by a box in the figure.
A crucial question: What controls the ranking order--and the gaps? The
vowels from left to right in Figure 1 (from A to J) divide roughly into two
halves--the vowels /i/ and /e/ which are obviously front versus the vowels /o/
and /a/ which, in contrast to /i/ and /e/, may be called back. The ranking move-
ment from singular to plural is in general from front toward a position farther
back. There are some departures from this condition, as /e/ outranking /i/, and
as /o/ is involved in the only instance of a back vowel singular paired with a
front vowel plural. The pair {0} and /ba/ are outside the basic system, used only
with certain loans.
ETUNG: TABLE I
Concording I-have
Noun his that one which it-fell seen it
Sentence
'X'
Noun Class
,
/-..A - A..% . ;
D 'wife' fikge owe ano yot nno a-gboe n-yen 37&
v ,-1 .!, . ,-,
B 'box' akOp eye ani ylt nni a-gb.oe n-yen nni
H 'bucket pokit eye ani y ±t nni a-gboe n-yen nni
. .., I. . . -.,
A 'yam' eyu eje
''^ anji jlt nji e-gbO e n-yen nji
.01. .
C 'stick' biti ebe gmbi blt mbi bl-gbo e n-yen .nbi
.3, .0!
E 'broom' 8fak 8b& ambi blt mbi o-gbo e n-yen mbi
..v . i . . .
F 'pestle' ncok ame ama mat mma rii-gb(Se n-yen mma
. ..
G 'needle' aga ame ama mat mma g-gbOe n -yen mma
... . .. . I .. .. ...
Note, in the first row (D) that the vowel /o/ occurs in the second, third, fourth,
and fifth columns--and implies the /o/ color to the simple nasal AI/ in the first
column. Row F, on the other band, has /a/ in the comparable places of the same
columns, and in Column 8 as well, giving /a/ color to prefix /ri/ of F. In Row B,
/i/ occurs in Columns 3, 5, 8 (with front /e/ in 2.)
4.
-112-
a fuller paper.
This much, however, has alerted us to watch for source of these phenomena,
or reflexes of them, as we study related languages.
Q.
D.
I
-113-
Footnotes
1
Unless one knows whether a verb is trasitive or intransitive, certain
ambiguities may develop. Certain verbs of motion may be followed by an unmarked
locative, as u lA di 'he went house'. The subset of unmarked locative phrases
[1
does not concern us further here.
2
A very similar typological situation has long been known elsewhere.
rote, from Mixtec, a similar set ('Analysis of a Mixteco Text,' International
Journal of American Linguistics', 1944, 10, 113-138). As in Vagala, the Mixtec
[1 data includes a couple of items ('road' for 'direction toward' which do not
fit the body part class.
3
See §7.7 for discussion of phonetic analysis of this difference.
4
In 117.asem Yominals, A Study in Analysis,' The Journal of West African
fl Languages, 1963, 2, 29-35.
Ci
5
See Greenberg's Class IA5d, as in Introduction, above. David W. Crabb,
in Ekoid Bantu Languages of Ogoja, Part I (Cambridge, 1965), asserts with
more detailed data that the Etoid group is part of the Bantu family.
6
I use the term formative when I wish to emphasize that there is identity
of phonemic shape, but when I do not wish to commit myself to morphemic identity
in the - lassical sense of morpheme as a relatively stable form-meaning composite.
Thus, the same formative can be either singular or plural.
7
Note that the phonologically-alternate manifestations of the formative
must be treated in the concord series, whether or not the formative is considered
as the same morpheme both as singular and as plural.
0
CHAPTER V: VERBS
A crucial problem in describing verb structures of some of the Niger-
Congo languages of West Africa lies in the control which the prefix complex
exerts over the shape of the verb as a whole, combining with an intricacy of
fusion of elements within the prefix itself. Into a monosyllabic prefix are
jammed clues to a variety of tenses (or aspects), modes, persons, and contrasts
for positive versus negative. I shall choose two sets of data from the work-
shop in order to highlight, in turn, problems of vowel fusion (in Abua), and
tone spans (in Etung). Then I shall add discussion of some comparable problems
of a Semitic language (Hausa) of this general geographical area.
some fifteen prefix forms of Abua. Matrix I lines up the data in order to
bring together into single blocks, insofar as is feasible, like vowels of the
respective prefix complexes.
Note that the vowels, in contrastive columns, differentiate fl
B
MATRIX I: ABUA VERB PREFIXES
(when no independent pronominal object
follows the verb)
Nonhuman
Qual- 1)14 yoor yira
rung idi idi na mi
Tense ity4,
B Future e e e/a a a a 0
C Future e e e/a a a a
D Present e e e/a a a a o 0 0 t-
F Present e e e/a a a a O 0 0 r
G Completive e e e/a a a a 0 0
H Perfect E E E/A A A A O 0 0
J Impezative E E/A A u- 0 0
K Participial r. E E E/O O 0 0 O 0 0
L Stative E E E/O 0 0 O 0 0 0
M Stat./Perfect k E E/O O 0 O 0 0 0:
P Conditional + m i i/ui U u u u u u
Q Conditional m i i i/u: u u u u u u
R Imperative k e i ,i/u u a I u u u u
-114-
-115-
some of the persons: e characterizes second plural and third plural nonhuman,
in "tenses" (rows) B-M; the vowel i in tenses N-S (but with i, e, reversed
in second plural J and R). The vowel a is found in tenses B-H, versus o in
K-M, u in P-0 (and mixture in J, N, R, S) for third singular human, and for
first and second singular. Similarly, o and u occur with 3 plural human,
and first plural inclusive and exclusive:, whereas third singular nonhuman
has alternate forms for e/a, e/o, i/u, and i/a.
Several other elements must be understood on the matrix before illustra-
tions can be matched against it: 'Quality' refers to positive (+) (i.e.,
affirmative) and negative (-); this affects the consonant and tone frequently,
but the vowels (as in J, R) less often. The consonants of the prefix are
often contrastive for tense (e.g., B versus D). The vowels sometimes differ-
1
entiate "tense" (e.g., N-S versus B-M), as well as simultaneously differenti-
ating person (e.g., the vowel i signals rows N-S, and columns 2p1 and 3p1 non-
human). All vowel letters are morphophonemic symbols, representing vowel har-
mony with vowels of the verb stem. Vowels in capital letters are predictive
morphophonemic symbols indicating that the following consonant of the stems
of Class 2 verbs--but not the vowel itself--will be changed. In a Class 2 (but
not Class 1) verb, the stem-consonant will be replaced by w after I, U, but by
zero and vowel length after E, A, 0; p replaced by t by r, k by g,
We leave for the moment the tone contribution to the verb so as to continue
studying the vowel patterning. Abua Matrix 2 shows what happens to any one
vowel quality of Matrix 1 when the verb is accompanied by an independent object
pronoun. (In positive sentences, the pronoun object usually follows the verb;
in negative sentences, it usually precedes it.) Vowel harmony continues to
operate between verb prefix and stem. Capital letters, in Matrix 2, again pre-
dict consonantal morp!ophonemics.
There is an astonishing degree of regularity in the fusion of the old in-
cluded object element to these non-object vowel forms. There are few exceptions
to the implications of Matrix 2:
DP
-116-
Object Pronoun 2 pl 1 pl 1 pl 1 sg 2 sg 3 sg 3 pl
I I I I r E
u I U U U u u u
0 I U U U 0 0 0
a U U U A a a
Note the i- prefix in the independent object pronouns 2p1, 1pl incl, 1pl
excl, at the top of Matrix 2: 1-fan .a '2 pl', etc. Only when the independent
DI
object pronoun has this high front prefix does change take place in the fusion
matrix: no change occurs with 2 sg, 3 sg, 3 pl. A similar *i- element seems
to have occurred within the verb itself. There i + i > i; i + e>i; and i +
u,o,a > u except in 2 pl when i + u,o,a > i. The *i-, that is, raises front
and back vowels, and also fronts the central and back vowels except in 2 pl.
This in turn leads in Matrix 2 to the display which shows i outranking
u, and u outranking o and a, resulting in the characteristic L shapes. Re-
turning to Matrix 1, an included ranking structure is seen in the relation of
o to a in Rows B-M of the columns to the right. The source of such ranking
structures, in instances of this kind, seems to be priorities in fusion. The
The tone of the Abua verb as a whole is determined by (1) the inherent
tone of the last syllable of verb stems, (2) by the le.gth of vowel in the
prefix (not discussed here), df'cermined by the stem, (3) by the number of
2
syllables in the stem-plus-suffix complex ; (4) by the tone of the prefix
complex (see in Column CT of Matrix 1), (5) by presence of pronominal object
in the context, (6) by position of verb in primary or secondary place in a
cluster:
ci
-117-
(1) Although stems in Abua verbs differ contrastively by tone, these in-
herent differences are relevant only to the present positive- -Row D--and only
to the last syllable of the verb; e.g., for bula 'to forget' versus tusemi
'to teach'. (All other rows of Matrix 1 of Abua have inherent stem tone com-
pletely overridden by patterns determined by the following features.)
(2) Some stems require a long vowel in the prefix, and a different set
of tone patterns on stem-plus-suffix, conditioned by the prefix,
(3) The tone pattern of polysyllabic stem-plus-suffix is in two parts:
(a) the final syllable and (b) all other syllables. The final syllable car-
ries one tone, and the balance a second tone (with each syllable of the bal-
ance having that second tone repeated). In monosyllabic elements of a com-
parable type, the two tone parts are often fused on the one syllable, often
with a resultant glide, but occasionally the fused form is not the same as
the longer form compressed.
(4) The prefix complex carries its own contrastive tone, as well as
determining the contrastive tone of the verb pattern as a whole.
Some typical patterns of the total Abua complex can be given, keyed into
the rows of Matrix 1. Tones-will be labeled T (top, special tone-in negative
only), H (high), L (low):
D L varied varied
F T ...L H LH
H H .H L L
M HL ...L H H
S H-L ,..L H H
Stems requiring a long-vowel prefix may have contrastive patterns:
F TL L H H
H H L L L
S HL L
-118-
Sample verb forms are now given to illustrate both the Abuan tone ele-
ments mentioned, and the vocalic and consonantal elements of Matrix 1:
Tense D, present positive:
nines re-b61 'You are-forgetting'
aniin re-bUla 'The-bird is-forgetting'
odi 'He is-forgetting'
bidi 'They are-forgetting
Tense F, present negative (with " as top tone): 1.;
nina
.
re-bula 'You are not-forgetting'
Tense G, completive positive:
{.
nina e-
e, legheri ni 'You
Tense H, perfect positive:
- .
nina me-ghi 'You have-gone'
Tense Q, conditional negative:
e .
nina mi-ki 'You would-not-have-gone'
suffixes affect the tone of the verb stem; the stem-suffix combine is closely
n
U enough integrated to warrant a special term--the verb 'core'. Covert (fused)
tense suffixes are actualized as tone changes of verb stems. (Tone patterns
of the core are distributed over the core, with the last syllable of a core
carrying one tone or one tone complex, and the other core syllables all carry-
ing the other tone of the pattern.) The tone class of a suffix, mediated by
the tone class of the core, may affect the tone of the prefix. The tone of a
prefix occasionally affects the tone of a suffix. The whole verb therefore,
comprises a close-knit but contrastive chain of tones. We can only give brief
illustrations here. Elm: patterned details, see the Appendix.
(Indicative) Prefixes affecting other (pronominal) prefixes:
a-k{ -gure 'He is selling'
! .
a- mo-gure 'He won't sell'
Here a 'he' is raised before the future negative .mo.
(Indicative) Prefixes affecting stems:
a- mo-gure 'He won't sell'
. . . . .
a- mo- bome 'He won't put on'
.
Here the Class A stem gure is
i low-high in tone, whereas the Class B stem has
lowering influence plus high high. All other Class A and Class B stems would
! -
13 act like these, after mo. The indicative prefix may affect a following re-
petitive prefix (as if it were a Class B stem) which in turn affects the stem.
(For this, and patterns after other indicative prefixes, see Appendix.) 1.
Overt suffixes affect the tone of the stem, leading to an over-all pattern
for the core:
a-bome 'He put on'
. , .
a-bom-a 'He puts on habitually'
The CVCV stem b6me loses its final vowel, replaced by the suffix -a; and the
high-low pattern is reversed.
Suffix affecting prefix:
n-su-k 'I have been washing'
. v
n-su-k 'I wash'
The tense labelled by Edmonson past habitual -k contrasts, over the total
verb (seen here in the low prefix), with the present habitual (seen with
high prefix). The tone classes of these suffixes differ arbitrarily but
morphophonemically, at this point, in their effect on the prefix.
-120-
n
U
Prefix affecting suffix:
A-su-k 'He washes'
0
desd '-k 'They wash'
v
a-su-k 'He has been washing'
The first two illustrations share the present habitual tense, but differ in
9
their pronominal prefix. The phonological contrast arising from change of
prefix, however, is manifested in the core. The third illustration, chang-
ing tense to past habitual, again shows contrast of total word, but with the
pitch of the pronoun actualizing the difference; in the first illustration,
the fused suffix raises the tone of 'a to a.
The interlocking of various parts of the verb with one another, in tonal
relations, raises a question: How can one visualize the mechanism by which
this has arisen? And can an alternate descriptive device aid in understand- n
ing of the synchronic system? I wish to suggest one kind of answer, but
this must be delayed until after certain tone matters have been discussed from
a different viewpoint in the phonology chapter. (See §6.4 where I shall
suggest a left-to-right ordering of rules of fusion, with a morphophonemic
raising influence--as well as down-step phoneme--to handle some of these de-
tails.)
5.3. Hausa (Afroasiatic) Formatives in Person-Aspect Markers
In §1.6 certain clause characteristics of Hausa, a Chad language of
West Africa, were given in order to show how properties of syntax can be
compared. LikeneSses and differences within a region can thereby be discussed,
as over against the family likenesses. Similarly, for the same Chad lan-
guage, I now wish to point out that techniques of matrix arrangement, with
permutation of rows and columns, allow interesting possibilities of abstract-
ing formatives, for contrast or comparison with the Niger-Congo materials.
Here, again, I use the work done for me by Gisela Kappler, based on data of
Abrahams and others (see fn. 14, Chapter 1). Kappler first points out that
the aspect-pronoun complex (seen in syntactic context in §1.6 above) is very
intricately structured, Within it, a bewildering interlocking of components
fuse into irregular patterns of contrastive prefix, suffix, tone, length,
consonants and vowel to signal negation, time, aspect, syntactic function, per-
son, number, gender. In order to show some of the near-regularities buried
within the larger mass, she abstracts a chart (see Hausa matrix of pronoun-
formatives) which gives only the nuclear syllable of the aspect-pronoun
-121-
complex of the verb phrase (eliminating prefixes and suffixes from the com-
plex, and deleting also length and tone from it, and omitting independent
pronominal forms).
Future MU su ku ki : ka ta a i n
Neg. Future MU su ku ki : ka 1
ta a i n
Neg. Perfective MU su I ku ki : ka ta a i n
Progressive mu su ku ki : ka 1 ta a ya : n
Optative mu su ku ki : ka ta a ya : n
Neg. Progr. 2 mu su i
ku ki : ka to a ya
i .. 1.11-0.
.
Perfective mu su ku ki : ka ta a ya na .
.
.
Relative Perf. mu su ku ki : ka 1
ta a ya na .
Rel. Progr. mu su ku ki : ka to a ya na .
.
.
Habitual mu su i
ku ki : ka ta a ya na .
Neg. Habitual mu su f
ku ki ka ta a ya na :
w .
Neg. Progr. 1 m a
: swa I
k a kYa ka ta a ya na .
w IV
Neg. Indefinite: m a swa k a kYa ka 1 ta a ya na
Future
w .
Indef. Future : m a swa kwa kYa ka to a ya na .
1.
*ku-gave kwa (although here I seem to have two non-identical sets of data, in
D
one of which the w occurs only after the k in these rows); and *ta- plus the
same *-a gave ta. In the early rows of the last column, similarly, the synchro-
nic anti-redundancy is due to n + a> n, and so on.
With the full, non-abstracted data, further layering of nonfusion, partial
fusion, and complete fusion need studying. Note, from Kappler's listing, the
following full forms of the eleventh row (negative habitual), in the order given
6. of.
on the matrix from left to right: bamukan, basukan, bakukan, bakikan, bakakan,
batakan, baakan, bayakan, banakan. Here the addition of ba- 'negative' and -kan
'habitual' (compare takan, 'positive habitual third feminine') seems straight-
forward. But even here the tones are not so easily treated as this would make
a
them appear. The tone of the nuclear element of the complex is high before a
suffix like -kan (hence banakan); but low after a prefix (ba-), when not followed
.
by a suffix, the ba- > ba and the nucleus becomes low, as in ba'su 'negative
progressive2 plural third person'. Yet elsewhere, tone may be contrastive: ka'
'perfective second person singular masculine' versus ka . 'indefinite future
second person singular masculine'. Part of the range of such problems can be
seen by illustrating one column of the matrix, the third plural, in the order
given there: 'future', baz'sil 'negative future', basil 'negative perfect',
sunr progressive , su
'progressive', optative'., ba su 'negative progressive2', sun 'per-
fective', suka 'relative perfective', suke 'relative progressive', sukan
'habitual', basukan 'negative habitual', ba su 'negative progressive', ba so
'negative indefinite future', sa' 'indefinite future'.
In comparison, now, with the Niger-Congo languages Etung and Abua, this
Chad language shares certain typological characteristics: (1) The tone
pattern of the aspect-pronoun complex must be treated as a whole, just as the
9
Etung verb tone (§5.2) had interlocking relations from prefix through suffix.
(2) The complex of elements, with irregular blocks of formatives, is typolog-
B
ically reminiscent of the Abua verb (§5.1).
4
-123-
D
On the other hand, repeated warning must be kept in mind that general
typological similarity is not evidence for genetic relationship. Details
must be reconstructed.
In thinking of utilizing matrix block-shapes for reconstruction purposes,
one notes (and see discussion in last reference given in fn. 2 of Chapter 1)
that the specific arbitrary shapes in relation to specific category sets must
be compared. The basis of classical comparison is the patterned relationships
across languages of the arbitrary relations of morphemic form (phonological
content) to semantic content (lexical meaning) within a language. An analogous
basis for reconstruction of certain morphological patterns would appear to
B be an equally rigorous application of the comparative method to an arbitrary
U relation of matrix form (that is, shape of its formative blocks) to matrix
semantics (its intersecting categories). General relationship will not do.
It is the highly arbitrary detailed relation of form to meaning which, when
seen as patterns across languages, should be able (1) to carry conviction of
relationship and (2) to be reconstructable.
:1
ta
1
,...--=rai=1=1311==io.
-124-
FOOTNOTES
1
The dual semantic role of such a formative I have elsewhere called
anti-redundant. See my 'Non-linear Order and Anti-redundancy in German
Morphological Matrides' Zeitschrift fur Mundartforschung, 32.193-221 (1965).
2
The suffixes of Abua are not otherwise relevant to our discussion
here. Several orders of suffixes--and possibly enclitics--occur.
CHAPTER VI: PHONOLOGY
laboration with Monica Cox led to the postulation of quasi-isochronic feet which
were in some sense simultaneously accentual units, and an accentual hierarchy
which interlocked with the grammatical_ hierarchy but was not identical with it.
Within a unit substantially larger than a word--a clause, for example- -
replacement of one word by others with respectively different numbers of
syllables left the basic timing and the accentual dynamics of the clause
perceptually unaffected. The stressed syllables (which I shall now call
"accented syllables" in order to avoid the implication of intensity in the
1
term "stress") remained the same in number. A unit of timing was clearly
perceived under these conditions--and may be called a "foot".
Compare:
[1] a "ni It'Ll'wanl tum-,mi-nee 'You know 'thing' which-is-here'
1
... ... . .
[2] a "batilt'Ll'wan I tum- mi-nee 'You carve-up 'thing' which-is-here'
i
In this set, the units 'ni, fbati,baa, and liakafi ('know', 'carve-up',
'have', 'taste') are perceived as astonishingly similar in length, except
that the length of ni (but not of caa) is partially carried by the condi-
tioned length of the following t (intutwan). The rhythm--clear, and easy
to hear within this arrangement of examples--is unaffected. Accents come
at the same perceived intervals; the accent of tt-Pwan is not delayed, that is,
by the added syllables of the preceding 'dakafi. The short ni versus long .
-125-
-126-
.24 .3 .38
.34 .2 .38
[2]:
.4 .36 .4
.38 .3 .42
[3]:
.32 .3 .4
[4]:
.48 .34 .4
.4 .34 .42
Brend considers that the [t] probably belongs to a shared border between the
first and second foot in utterance [1] and that, taking this into account, the
measured differences between the length of the various utterances of the first
foot in [1] to [4] to be no greater a variation than could be compatible with
perceptual isochrony. (This is especially true in view of the second utterance
of [4].) Other utterances of these same sentences measure as follows:
[1] (with [t] as part of second foot):
.3 .33 .4
.39 .3 .42
.44 .32 .4
--127-
0
If the calculation of the utterances of [1], in all four cases above, puts
the juncture in the middle of the [t], then the measurements of the first two
0 feet almost invariably come out as nearly identical.
The final foot proved to be longer than the first two, in some of the
utterances. This was unexpected; perceptually it had not appeared longer. My
assumption, based on prior work with accentual systems, was that perhaps the
lengthening before pause was accompanied by a weakening, which lessened its per-
ceptual impact, and went unnoticed by us. I asked from the laboratory further
measurements which confirmed this hunch. In observing amplitude sections of
these utterances, Brend reported that the over-all amplitude of the final foot,
in each utterance, consistently measures two to three db lower than the ampli-
U tude of the first two feet, and there is, typically, a gradual decrease in
amplitude in the final foot which is not seen in the first two feet.
I had long used whistling by informants to help me perceive tone changes,
and wondered whether it might be helpful in studying dynamic features. On
the basis of laboratory measurements, however, it has since become evident that
informants, whistling forms like these, did not consistently whistle in chunks
a which corresponded either with feet or syllables, but rather seemed to mix
these borders indiscriminately. Nevertheless, they consigtently whistled the
highest tone at places we have marked with double-stress (") to indicate the
peak of the intonation contour as well as the heaviest accented syllable in
the utterance. The frequency measurements here, therefore, would seem to con-
firm the hypothesis that the nucleus of these utterances falls at the places
marked with double stress.
It seemed possible, furthermore, in an area where drums were widely used,
some kind of tapping might reveal or support the analysis of some of the dy-
namic characteristics. We were unable to explore this possibility adequately- -
but tapping on a table or a tin did not appear to give as satisfactory a per-
ceptual relation to speech as did the whistling. Brend, in measurements of the
tapping ,feels that the tapping units correspond more closely to syllable units
than to feet. In instances where several syllables in speech are collapsed into
one spoken foot, for example, the measurements of analogous tapped feet do not
coincide with the groupings of the speech measurements. When, however, there
is a one-to-one correspondence of number of syllables within two feet, these
feet do measure as being very close to identical in length, and to this degree
-128-
In [5], vowel qualities and tone heights are kept constant. Here, under con-
trolled conditions, a slight accent carried by half length of vowel seemed
to be perceived.
B
iii
[5] ki ki
.02 .06 .1
.1 .16 .16
.06 .2 .16
[6] ki 'no ko
.04 .08 .12
t
.11 .18 .2
.1 .18 .21
II [7] ki 'IA' ki
.08 .16 .16
0Li .06 .1 .11
.1 .16 .2
These data confirm clearly that the pre-stress syllable, as expected, is espe-
II
cially short. On the other hand, the final syllable was often as long as--o-
even longer than--the accented syllable, which we had not predicted from our
0 field observations. Here, as above in reference to the foot as a unit, we
would now assume that a final drawled syllable would be heard as part of the
g prefinal phenomena and that this particular length had served as a cue to pre-
pausal position rather than as a signal of placement relative to the nucleus of
El the foot. This necessitated, however, searching for further cues of accent
within the acoustic data. A simple measurement of amplitude, however, does
Here the intonational pitch seems to drop progressively lower from the begin-
ning to the end, but with major upstep after the first double bar (not, however,
as high as the initial start) and with minor ups and downs caused by the separ-
, ate tones and by the included feet.
Brend suggests that perhaps the most crucial organizing principle for sev-
eral of the levels of the hierarchy arises from length. Perceptually accented
and unaccented syllables within a foot measure long and short respectively (with
the exception of utterance-final syllables, for which see discussion, above);
sequences of feet within an utterance show isochrony, to a remarkable degree
(with the exception of utterance-final feet, for which see above discussion);
while a foot occurring at the peak of the intonation contour turns out to be
slightly lengthened.
Grammatico-lexical considerations affect the hierarchical organization in
various ways. The phonology is partially but not completely independent of
lexicon and grammar, and vice versa.
A pronominal subject, as in [1-4], is often treated as part of the foot
which has its nucleus in the verb. A longer, independent noun subject may com-
prise a separate foot, as in:
[9] .a"liis.an I 'giff-bi 'Alasan chops-them'
The object of a verb joins the preceding foot with verb as nucleus, if the
verb is short--but may comprise a subsidiary foot if the verb is long:
[10] 'doo "yee 1 bl.'nib nin I 'ka fataku 'Formerly I people remote-time I
saw blood'
[11] 'doo"yee Ibi'nib nin I 'dakafi fataku 'Formerlylpeople remote-time I
thought-of blood'
-1
-131-
would leave room for some type of compensatory lengthening such as we had with
the long [e] in the first of the Basare illustrations.) Note the following
measurements, therefore:
[1] kO8'agL -- 1.0
[2] k99eygolgL -- 1.08
[3] kookpogorongan -- 1.18
As we compare these we notice once more that in spite of the difference of
two syllables the respective feet are similar in length. I would conclude, there-
fore, that it is the foot length as a whole that must be studied if one is to
correlate measurements with perception.
One must, if these data prove as a guide, however, not expect that the measure-
ments be identical where the feet contain different number of syllables, but rath-
er that a foot with four syllables will not be twice as long as two, nor one with
five be five thirds longer than one with three. The partial shortening of the
-132-
whole foot (relative to what it would be if each syllable took the same time)
impresses the hearer as somehow 'rushing to a conclusion' which, in turn, is
interpreted as in some sense 'the same'.
6.2. Tone
Many problems of tone analysis interlock in a fascinating complex: The
placement of tone, as we have already seen, can be viewed as affected by the
morphophonemics of a unit which is prefix-plus-stem-plus-suffix-plus-variant-
D
number-of-E'..eL-syllables ( §5.2); or some of the same phenomena (e.g., a medial
series of like-tone rapid syllables) can be related to accentual dynamics (6.1).
The number of levels of tone in a language can for some purposes be viewed as fl
a.set of classical static contrastive tone phonemes; but for other purposes is
better viewed as a basic set plus a special level limited to negative contexts;
and in other instances of a different kind as a basic set with a special con-
trastive level developed through the loss of low syllables which leave a dynamic
influence behind them. Allotones, under these conditions, bring problems of
recognition and description. Both the theory and practice of treating tone
languages are affected. New phonemes of tone seem to be visible in the process
of development; West Africa seems to be an enormously interesting "laboratory"
for the study of the dynamics of changing tone systems.
6.2.1. Basic Tone Heights
Niger-Congo languages of West Africa vary in the number of their basic
or "normal" tones; e.g., Igede has four, Bette three, Degema two.
In Igede, the four levels of contrast were extraordinarily clear, with the
particular informant brought to the workshop by Richard Bergman. (Tone data are
from him and from Martin Leigh.) Instrumental analysis of utterances--which
were spoken quite normally--gave displays which left the levels so clear cut
that they appeared to Brend almost as if they had been "sung". The whistling
of the patterns by the informant gave a similar result. Very little influence
is seen where one tone affects another.
In the following illustrative set, note Igede contrasts within the frame
5...1E. Symbols are high to low respectively, /'/, //, /'/, /'/ (where /^/
represents a lower-mid level tone, not a glide).
o hu lc 'He has washed'
hu lc 'He has stayed'
_
o hu lc 'He has scattered'
o hu lc 'He has flown'
-133--
o I
Note also, la 'to miss', li 'to have', la 'to chew a stick', la 'to bulge out'.
For Bette, note the following sets, contrasting both before low and after
low respectively (with some fusion in the second context):
undi ken 'one person'
kuno ken 'one soul'
Q
usi ken 'one kidney'
leta 'stone'
wadum 'man'
eten 'animal'
On numerous short utterances, frame techniques can be used which compare levels
of preceding and following pitches, without too much interference from 5ntona-
tional downdrift or conditioned variation of tones.
In many of the languages of West Africa, however, two basic tones are in-
volved, plus added phenomena of great complexity--growing out of intonational
downdrift, conditioned variation, and fusion with special residual phenomena
(made up of morphophonemic replacements, newly developed tones, and levels of
pitch which fit easily neither of these two categories). Illustrations will
be found in the next few sections.
6.2,2. Extra-High Tone Developed from Lost High
The Degema and Engenni are very closely related Kwa languages (Delta Edo
group). Yet, of the relatively few differences between them, two are of great
interest in the study of the dynamics of the change of tone systems for West
Africa as a whole. (Elaine Thomas supplied me with these materials.)
In the Degema, the personal prefix carries a regular high tone for sub-
junctive. In Engenni, however, the prefix is lost and the tone of the preced-
ing word is replaced by a new tone level /"/ which is higher than either of
the two normal basic tones and in contrast with them. In addition, all tones
(both in Degema and in Engenni) following the extra-high are a bit lower than
they would be otherwise--the general "key" is depressed. Not only single low
or high, but the entire following sequence is lowered somewhat.
4011111.11wr
-134-
Degema:
imo eve e-kpein ak,yo
children two should-wash put-the
imo ive kpei akLna
children two-should wash pot-the
'the two children should wash the pot'
and
Degema:
*ploy° o-ta nuvaL
child go to-house
Engenni:
amona to mu
child go house
'the child should go to the house'
I
The negative verbal prefix /o- in Degema already carries the extra-
high tone--from unknown origin - -but also passes it back to the preceding word
in Engenni:
Degema:
omo noonayo o-kpein akLyo
child this not-wash pot-the
Engenni:
amo anona kpei akLna
child this-not wash pot-the
'This child did not wash the pot'
In various other languages an extra-high tone occurs, and may be limited to the
negative. Note, for Abua:
nina ke-ki 'You will go'
nina ke-ki 'You will not go'
nina re-bula 'You are-forgetting'
pog 'Look!'
Gardner and I seem also to hear, for this special Abua tone, an added intensity,
or "tight" voice quality. Note, further, the high replaced by low following it,
in the first two illustrative pair's. Brend's laboratory findings clearly con-
firm the special height with regard to measured frequencies. The extra-high
tone is consistently and regularly higher than the normal high tone.
6.2.3. Lowered Key (Terrace) Developed from Lost Low
2
It has been less than a decade since Welmers called attention to the lin-
guistsof this century the importance of ' downstep' or 'terrace' tone, to tonal
3
studies. Much earlier Cristaler (who had studied with Lepsius) had devised
an elegantly simple orthography to cover accurately the transcription of Twi
tone with its downstep; and--as Gleason pointed out to me--some published ma-
terials showed, in retrospect, that a downstep of a high tone could be corre-
4
lated with evidence of a lost low tone. The reconstructed low (sometimes
optionally present) by conditioned variation lowered a preceding high; with
the loss of the low, the lowering effect on the high remained,-with three fur-
ther results: (1) The key (general height of all following tones in the phrase,
including high tones) was lowered accordingly. (2) Contrast developed between
a normal-high (i.e., one not thus conditioned downward) after high, and a step-
ped-down-high after high and both contrast with low after high. (3) The result-
ant system of contrasts contained an imbalance: After any one morpheme with
final low tone there could follow it only two contrastive phonetic levels--low
and high; after any one morpheme with' final high tone, a three-way contrast of
phonetic levels could occur between high plus high, high plus stepped-down
high (from a reconstructed low plus high morpheme), and high plus low. Thus
(with /*/ representing the downstepping of the tone immediately following that
symbol; and with *LH having high slightly lower phonetically than in *H):
(1) i) H + H
ii) H + 'H < *H + LH
iii) H + L
but
(2)- i) L + H
ii) L + L
U
In phonetic graph (with hyphens representing relative height in the pitch
envelope):
*
(3)
H L H H ! H
-136-
The clearest data from the workshop, for these purposes, comes from Elaine
Thomas, in a comparison of two closely-related languages--the Engenni and the
Degema of Nigeria.
In Degema, three of the major tone classes of nouns have a downstepped '4
B
o
-139-
The system described here (three regular phonemic levels plus a nonphonemic
downstep after a stressed high) could very easily develop into a typical phonemic
downstep, plus three phonemic levels. If, for instance, fusions as in (4c) were
to lead to a different (nanfinal-in-word) placement of stress while retaining
placement of downstep, the downstep would probably have become phonemic.
On the other hand, if other investigators (a) did not on these same data re-
cord stress as in the way we have done, and (b) if word boundaries were ignored,
their analysis would probably show downstep as already phonemic. Inasmuch as
stress analysis in this language is by no means obvious--or certain--the alternate
analysis should be kept in view.
After this analysis in the field, laboratory work by Peck and Brend gives
general confirmation of the pitch contrasts of instances such as (la-c). Simi-
larly, the instrumental measurements add no major difficulties to the analysis
of pitch sequences in the downstep series--nor are they able at the moment to
help very much (because of limits to current acoustic techniques) on the important
problem of stress.
6.2.6. Overlap of Conditioned Allotones
In the Igede, of §6.2.1, levels were clean-cut, and fairly uniform in their
respective heights throughout the phrase.
In downstep systems, however, conditioned variation of tone levels leads to
extensive problems. This statement, however, is historically stated backwards:
A system of conditioned variants of tone has led to the particular tone feature
known as phonemic downstep. The sources may be the' conditioned lowering of a high
by A preceding low ( §6.2.3); by conditioned lowering of high in its morphophonemic
source in a low ( §6.2.4); by a lowering of high after stress ( §6.2.5). Or an extra
high may take its source from a lost high ( §6.2.2) which was raised before a low.
Many morphophonemic changes ( §5.2', ,and Etung Appendix data )..reveal conditioned-
replacement of tones without observable current allophonic traces.
The extensive variety of such effects implies some general dynamic character
LI (see also §6.1) for these systems: A general tendency for the pitch level of a
6
phrase to 'drift' downward, rather than stay on an even key, in steady pitch
"bands".
El
77-111111101.1P-
-140-
L L
in which each syllable can carry any one of three tone phonemes (H or M or L),
but the next syllable in the series (with the same set of possible contrasts,
no more and no less) would by conditioned variation (in the down-drifting con-
tour), have each tone phoneme suppressed in phonetic (not phonemic) height one
notch. The result would be : H + H would not be level (the second would be
lower); M + H would be level with each other (but in phonemic contrast, relative
to other potentially substituted elements at that point in that frame); L + M
7
would also be on a single level, etc.
In Degema, Thomas shows some of these phenomena. Between words (but not
within them, under penalty of collapsing some of the system) a word-final low
depresses a following word-initial low to its own low phonetic pitch, but a
following word-initial low to lower than the first low. Note:
L H H L HL
but
. , .
(2) /mtmon spa kLkre/ 'I-saw frogs all'
L H HL L L
I 4\ ,.. 4..
In (1), note that L and ', in the. middle of ikpe meki are on the same level;
and that -ki is lower than the first syllable of mt,mon.
In (2), on the other hand, note that L plus L between tpa and kAre drops
sharply; the L of I- contrasts with (is lower than) the me- of meki in (1).
The phenomena, furthermore, are in addition to those for phonemic downsteps
!
/ / discussed for the same language in §6.2.4; the /1/ adds a nonpredictable con-
trastive drop outside the major system of two levels or the special high contrast-
ive level ( §6.2.2).
-141-
In Izi, data from Paul Meier illustrate one of the most pervasive z-ld, at
first, disturbing--of these situations: A sequence of L H H has the low and
the downstepped high on the same pitch:
! . t ,
/ma re ji/ 'I-present sell yams' (with e and 'ji affirmed
to be on the same level; a statement clearly
confirmed, like many other crucial parts of
the tone phonetics of Meier's material, by
laboratory tests by Peck and Brend)
AA NI Ay/ ,v,
/muturogbonosi/ 'I threw a stick at Ogbona' (with -ro- and nosi
. .
. . . " . .v.
on same pitch; from *[mu turu ogbona osi])
!
But there is a contrast of LL with the HH, in the same environment:
. . . .
/muturogboniphe/
. .
'I threw something at Ogbona' (with -ni- lower
than ro--and - no--; and with -phe still
lower, since prefinal low-low drifts down
a bit; from *[mu turn ogbona iphe] where
the nouns belong to different morp5Ophonemic
classes)
It is the presence of contrasts in such comparable environments which justifies
the contrastive phonemic status of the elements; and it is the lack of more
than the topologically-equatable members of the sets which forces the analysis
of like-le 7els as phonetically-overlapping but phonemically contrasting tones
in the different environments. (Emphasis on different environments is main-
tained, lest that kind of intersection of phonemes be postulated which is un-
acceptable within postulates of classical phonemics.)
Many other intricate problems of contrast arise in the Izi data, but they
cannot be illustrated here in detail.
6.2.7. Towards Internal Reconstruction of Tone Chains
In scattered instances in preceding sections there have been given starred
forms, implying some degree of internal reconstruction. Can such tentative, in-
ternal reconstruction be carried substantially further? If so, could it illumi-
nate some of the deepest puzzles of Niger-Congo languages in which--for example--
the negation of clause involves pitch changes at discontinuous places? And
could it, furthermore, suggest sources for some of the most pervasive of tone
rules--such as a "flip" of high-low forms to low-high, etc.?
---
-142-
(already in the system phonemically) and neither adds a new level, nor a condi-
tioned variant of a level. It is a morphophonemic symbol, not a phonemic one.
(In some languages of the area there is the suspicion that a rising influence
/i/, analogous to the lowering /!/, des achieve phonemic status. Meier is
chocking on this possibility in Izi; so far, however, I have not seen evidence
which for me completely eliminates the nonphonemic interpretation. In Etung,
the kind of data giving rise to this possibility have not been found.)
The + before ba leaves it unaffected, since:
(5) t + >
I
!
(whereas the 1:, after of (2) develops an audible downstep of the 'ba complex).
Therefore morphonemic rising influence residing in kat is not manifested--not
detectable--in (2). Note, however:
. .
(6) i) a k ruse gile (did) not go'
.. .. I
ii) * (t + a + kat + rue)
(7) i) a ic. rile 'They (did) not go'
ii) *(t + afi + kat + rile)
-143-
a chain of elements affect the ones directly next to them. This would save
some of the problems of explanation of tone replacements (which appear to be
largely composed of assimilation phenomena) without requiring action at a dis-
tance. Explanation would then be in terms of the kinds of assimilative changes
actually observable, currently, in fast versus slow forms, or across closely-
related dialects.
Two further problems, however, would remain: (a) The origin of + would
itself have to be explained. This would now seem to comprise a reasonable re-
search task. Can it, for example, by comparative techniques be traced to a
lost high tone? Or does it suggest an easier way of searching for (or testing
present hypothesis of) a phoneme /i/ of rising influence analogous to /!/?
(b) The second fr'ature to be noted carefully is that such influences perhaps
do not alter the relevant domains of level pitches in those languages in which
several syllables in a row hang together (all up, or all down) in rules; and
several syllables of a domain operate in the rules like a single-syllable domain
in the same rules.
Returning now to (1) and (2) we note that + at the beginning of the formulas
has not been justified. The added basic assumption here is that the transformation
of a positive clause to a negative requires changes at more than one place in the
clause. This, however, is amply demonstrated in clause materials (see Chapter 1,
§§1.1.2). And if this is granted, the assignment of a second morpheme (or part
of a morpheme, in an alternate analysis) to the clause complex (+ before the
pronoun, in addition to the ka) seems no longer surprising. Yet it is precisely
this (or other?) morpheme with zero phoneineb but with significant morphophonemic
8
impact which in part makes the negative appear so mysterious. Here, however, the
4' plus third person a gives the a of (1).
Note, howeVer, that we would have expected + + to give AI, as in (4). How
can this absence be explained? Note that the downstepping influence would then
have preceded the low of ka, which it cannot affect since:
-144--
(8) ! + >
do hope, however, that this kind of approach will open the door to a new type
of fruitful work in this area.
6.2.8. Theory of Phoneme Types as Item, Process, and Relation.
I now wish to suggest that these studies of tone (differentiating tone
phonemes-of-level, from tone phoneme-of-downward-influence, from morphophonemic
non-phonemic symbols of raising influence) can be fitted into a deeper theoret-
ical perspective and at the same time help to solve an old problem.
The general thesis: That _phonemes may be of three types--item, process,
and relation; that these in some sense are related, in turn, to tagmemic per-
spectives of particle, wave, and field. The specific claim: That "segmental"
elements such as /o, s, t/ are particle phonemes, as items; "suprasegmental"
elements of tone level, and of length, are field phonemes, as purely relational
elements; that the /!/ of Africa is neither a segment nor a relational pole of
a sector of a field, but a wave (or process) phoneme.
Note that, in general, the particles--once the system is known--can in
general be recognized in isolation, as 'things' in themselves. The field
phonemes are recognizable by paradigmatic contrast in a frame; the frame shows
contrastive relations within the subset of that field. The process phonemes
are recognizable only in a sequence--in a phonological construction, or wave- -
in reference to what has happened to an item previously identified in the
paradigmatic field.
The development of further implications of this view--and problems with
the well-known relational characteristics of segmental phonemes in a phonological
matrix--I must leave to another publication (now in process). This much, how-
ever, should indicate to the reader the deep theoretical significance of a
special phoneme such as the downward influence /!/.
-145-
with obiim 'frog' versus oyil 'priest'. Such elements, it should be clear,
imply the need of careful handling of rhythm statements where--as in Abua --
some degree of evenness in length is found for feet with differing number of
syllables, or for syllables (in relation to feet) with differing number of
rowels (see §6.1).
A problem of certain consonantal contrasts warrants continuing study:
In Abua, the Gardners provide samples where I seem to hear phonemic contrasts
between flapped versus nonflapped [1]. The flapped one appears to be shorter,
weak, possibly shortening the preceding syllable; the nonflapped one appears
to be longer, perhaps fortis, and affecting (lengthening), in its turn, the
syllable before it. Note (with [1 as the longer, fortis, nonflap types,
[1] as flapped):
[O1.] 'bangle', [ola] 'intestines'.
For Agbo, Dr. Bendor-Samuel has in preparation, with Klaus Spreda, an
article treating in more detail the problem of fortis consonants there.
6.3.3. On Data Collection Preparatory to Workshop Analysis
For any report such as that of the preceding section, the experienced
analyst may retain a certain amount of reserve. Sample, isolated illustrations-
no matter how contrastive and relevant they may appear--sometimes disintegrate
under closer scrutiny. A supposed minimal pair, for example, may in fact con-
tain a factor unnoticed, which makes the pair nonminimal and cuts all support
from the analysis.
The reader may very well wonder, therefore, what kind of basic, solid
tests are made in workshops such as those reported here, to minimize such
sources of error in the phonemic work of some of the analysts who are in the
early stages of investigation. And how, furthermore, can the data be checked
rapidly--since time is limited--and problems isolated which warrant study in
depth.
The approach--which I first applied extensively, in this particular form,
in workshops in Peru in 1955-56--calls for the lining up of data in a specific
format: Sounds which are similar, and subject to confusion in analysis (giving
'suspect pairs' of sounds) are in each instance, for example, lined up with
hypotheses as to their contrast or as to their complementation, and accompanied
by contrastive word lists--and preferably with tape recordings. Segment se-
quences (like ts) which in general experience are known to be unit phonemes in
some languages, but sequences of phonemes in others, are required to be listed
-147-
with arguments pro and con, supported in turn by data. Distributional patterns
and lists--necessary for decision as to consistency or proof of statements of
complementation or contrast are added.
Charts of phones and of phonemes show the patterned development of the
analysis. Frames, for tone contrast, also lead to illustrated statements of
morphophonemic rules and patterns of tone behavior.
12
With data before him in this form, the consultant is able to check and
utilize the underlying data rapidly--far more rapidly than he can do so from
the standard professional article on the same data--and suggest further work.
The body of this report does not allow a full illustration of this type
of data collection. But, since it is crucial to understand the technology of
13
the workshop, I have included in an Appendix a section on Agbo, by Klaus
ci
and Janice Spreda.
6.4. Intersection of Voice Quality with Gesture
Miss P. M. Revill attempted to go beyond routine linguistic analysis
in Ilbembe, by studying features of voice quality, speed, and rhythm with the
expression of emotion.
To her surprise, however, it became evident that the contrast between cer-
tain strong emotional states was in part carried by verbal cues and in part by
nonverbal ones. It was only by the intersection of vocal and gestural systems
thatsome of the emotional states could be recognized and differentiated.
Specifically, for example, both anger and surprise were breathy, fast,
and with low pitch on the final phrase. Yet portrayal of anger included fast
nods of the head, with eyes rolling and flashing--characteristics not present
with surprise. On the other hand, a surprised exclamation was accompanied
by a quick movement of the hands, with palms upwards, whereas anger was accom-
14
panied (contrastively) by fast pointing gestures.
For the details of this report--still in its preliminary stages--see the
Appendix.
-148--
FOOTNOTES
1Kay Williamson, in A Grammar of Tjo Cambridge, 1965, p. 26, suggests, for
the Kwa language, a similar unit of timing. Within these 'tone groups' morpho-
,
phonemic changes of tone may occur (p.7), determined by the syntax. Tone groups
become part of tone phrases, and of breath groups (pp. 7, 11-12).
2"Tonemics, Morphotonemics, and Tonal Morphemes" General Linguistics, 4,
1959, pp. 1-9.
3
In A Grammar of the Asante and Fante Languages Called Tshi [Chwee, Twi], U
Basel, 1875 (republished in 1964 by Gregg Press Incorporated, Ridgewood, New
Jersey).
4
E. g., in A. N. Tucker and J. Tompo Ole Mpaayei, A Maasai Grammar, London,
Longman's Green and Co., 1955, p. 172.
5
In J. M. Stewart, "The Typology of the Twi Tone System," with comments by
P. Schacter and W. E. Welmers, preprint from the Bulletin of the Institute
of African Studies I, Institute of African Studies, University of Ghana, n.d.
See also,--amongst others--D. W. Arnott "Downstep in the Tiv Verbal System,"
African Language Studies, 5. 34-51, 1964, and F. Winston, "The 'Mid Tone of Efih,"
African Language Studies, 1. 185-92, 1960. Winston's phonemic treatment of the .
r
a
-149--
-150-
-151-
11.0
0
-152-
the semantic component of a tagmeme, and with the vectors of a clause matrix
of the tagmemicist.) Some plot relations will be found expressed as simple
expansions of (optional tagmeme additions to) the basic clause patterns. Others
(like the instrumental in Twi and Vagala, §1.1.1 and §2.2.4) may only be ex-
pressed by moving to structures on a different level.
B
(2) The learner will also attempt to set up matrices of clause types
(and of other constructional levels). Irregularities here--holes in the
pattern, or dangling, unintegrated bits--suggest search for the possibility of
regularity--that is, for example, if a declarative statement is found both
independent and dependent, but interrogative is listed only as independent, the
analyst might well search for a possible dependent interrogative.
Lesson plans, then, can be built from the beginning of a knowledge of how
to play-act and word-act. Discourse-in-play-act-context can be memorized (in
standard fashion) before analysis is finished. Drills (also in standard fashion)
can be set up using sample clauses, phrases, sentences from the discourse as
frames for substitution drills. Expansion drills can exploit the addition of
optional tagmemes of a construction. Discourse drills can be patterned on
minimum speaker exchange (e.g., standard question-answer techniques). Deriva-
tion of complex from simple matrices, when known, show transfer of dramatis
2
personae plot relations from one structure or level to another (or, in other
theoretical terms, transformation drills can be set up).
The advantage in pedagogical presentation, here, is the potential for a
linguistic bookkeeping which allows one more easily to see just how much has
been presented, relative to the partially-known system in each lesson, and in
the sequence of lessons. If one keeps in an appendix a set of syntactic para-
digms plus a set of matrices, each paragraph of the lesson can be keyed to
the paradigms and matrices, and--in turn--these latter may have for each tagmeme
and for each matrix cell a section number showing where in the text each item
has been presented, drilled, expanded, and related to other structures from dis-
course to word.
Such an index meets the requirement raised at the first of this section.
3
It allows the beginning analyst-learner to know, in principle, what he knows;
to guess at gaps; and to keep track of his assimilation responsibility toward
what he knows analytically. In addition, it would vastly simplify the task of
a textbook writer of a 'second-level' text by letting him see, directly, some-
thing of the structural rationale and developmental sequence of the data inherited
from the writer of the first level.
o -153-
Q FOOTNOTES
1
Noam Chomsky, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Cambridge: The M. I. T.
o Press, 1965.
2
See Kenneth L. Pike, "Discourse Analysis and Tagmeme Matrices," Oceanic
Linguistics, 3, 1964, pp. 5-25.
D
3
Herbert Stahlke, of the Evangelical Lutheran Mission, attempted to apply
these principles in carrying on his learning of Yachi. He made available to
D
me his report of the experiment, which confirms the usefulness of the approach
under field conditions.
D
a
D
0
a
CHAPTER VIII: SUMMARY
I
Chapter 1; Studies are reported for certain Niger-Congo West African languages:
Kasem, Vagala, Sisala, Dagaari, Bimoba, Basare, Twi, Bariba, Degema, Engenni,
Igede, Izi, Abua, Mbembe, Agbo, Bette, Etung, Yachi. (Secondary sources are
used, also, for Bobangi [Bantu] and Hausa [Chad, Afroasiatic],).
Clauses may be basic, or derived, described in reference to tagmemic con-
trasts, variation, and distribution.
Chapter 2: Clauses differ according to the place they occur in the sentence.
In addition, clauses enter into specialized, characteristic clusters (serial
clauses), within these sentence parts. Clauses in clusters exhibit special
variants, loss of tagmemes, and co-occurrent restrictions. These processes
lead to semantic specialization of subclusters; development of verb phrases;
and classes of verbs seen as in a state of change. Some tagmemes and some
clause variants expected by the English speaker to occur within the separate
clause are expressed only within the clause cluster or subcluster. (See,
also, §1.)
Chapter 3: Sentence clusters (= paragraphs) show, on a higher level, structural
restrictions and relations analogous to those on lower level clusters. Nuclei
of paragraphs (topic sentences) are structurally analogous to the independent
clause of a sentence. Detailed study of discourse will apparently show similar
kinds of structure, of which only the first bits are reported here.
Further kinds of discourse-tie involve the intricate relation of prescrip-
tions for the choice of direct versus indirect quotation according--for example- -
to "on-stage" versus "off-stage" focus, status of speaker (chief versus commoner),
involvement of dramatis personae (as speaker versus addressee, for example).
Chapter 4: Noun phrases, like clauses, can be described and compared in type
by techniques of presentation in syntactic paradigm. Some noun phrases, like
clauses, can be seen in process of change to a lower-level construction.
Matrix treatment of nouns allows irregularity to be seen as distortion of
a simple field structure, and lays the ground for comparison across languages.
Concord of noun to other tagmemes of a clause, seen via matrix, sometimes
shows ordered regularities of a ranking type.
Chapter 5: Matrix presentation of verb structures allows the clear exhibition
of highly complex rules of fusion by ordered change from simple matrix to de-
rived matrix.
-154-
ri
B -155--
many of them a new, extra-high tone can be seen in a limited set of special
contexts (e,g., the negative). Otherwise, basic, contrastive tone heights
vary in number from two to four.
A special phoneme of process--a lowering (or "dowlstepping") influence- -
is characteristic of the region, and leads to a modification of phoneme theory
to accommodate phonemes of particle (or item), of wave (or process) and of
field (or relation). Segments like s, o, 1 would be phonemes of a particle
type; high versus low tone would be relational elements of field; and the /!/
lowering influence would be a phoneme of process (distinct in phonological
result from a hidden morphophonemic influence). Combinations of the three
types, plus morphophonemic symbols, suggest special results for internal re-
construction of tone chains.
Segmental phonemes involve various problems--including the role of the
throat in vowel harmony.
Phonological data can be presented, for testing a preliminary analysis,
in a convenient summarizing format.
Vocal quality comprises one component of certain cues to emotional
states. These ambiguities can be resolved only by the intersection of
these cues with gestural ones.
Chapter 7: For the learning of a language before the analysis of that lan-
guage is well along, tagmeme and matrix techniques allow for a bootstrap
operation in which learning and analysis proceed simultaneously. Language
is treated as but one component of communicative behavior. Discourse oper-
ates in a setting of action, and standard drill types exploit data from dia-
logue. But preliminary syntax paradigms allow the early use of substitution
drills, while suggesting search-leads for expansion of patterns of constructions.
Preliminary matrices of basic and derived constructions, on the other hand,
allow search for analogous constructions implied by the presence of holes in
the pattern. In addition, the combination of the two allows the construction
of an index matrix which permits the textbook writer to keep track of the
areas taught and those still to be taught--whether in a first text or on an
advanced level. Thereby the learner can answer the question 'What don't you
know?'--and set the stage to learn; while the textbook writer can discuss, in
reference to his own efforts or those of a predecessor, 'What remains to be
added before my task, as I define it, is done?'.
or
Appendix I
Kathleen Barnwell
Basic Theory:
(1) Any unit of purposive human behavior is well-defined if and only if one
describes it in reference to
a) contrast (and resulting identification)
b) range of variation (with its essential physical manifestation)
c) distribution (in class, in hierarchical sequence and in systemic
matrix) "
(3) Longacre's 'dual criteria'principle has been applied for the separation of
clause types. Clause types are separated if they contrast in two respects either
-156-
-157-
B Note: Thit description is limited to major clause types, which comprise about
95 per cent of the text examined, so far.
General Outline:
{Serial constructions
VII Close-knit clause
Result-clause composite
composites
Introductory. clause composite
IX Illustrations of negative
formations
Bibliography
I
-158-
Nine Independent and two Dependent clause types are set up. These show
structural contrasts in:
a) the class of verb-roots which may function in the verbal phrase filling U
the Predicate place
b) the relationship of the fillers of the Complementation places (charted
under the labels Object, Indirect Object, Complement and Goal) to
the other components of the clause
c) the restrictions on the classes of fillers which may occur in various
clause slots.
The Intransitive, Transitive, Ditransitive, Directive Motive, Stative and
Independent Introductory (1) clause types have an optional extension of the verbal
phrase which occurs after the Complementation places (or, if no Complementation
places occur, immediately following the Predicate) and which consists of a redup-
lication of the verb functioning as head of the verbal phrase. e.g.
CHART I shows the structural contrasts of the Nucleus of the Basic Indicative
clause types.
Complementation
U
SUBJECT I PREDICATE I.O. ORJrCT comPumm COAL
+
- n.p. Intransitive v.p. - - -
I n.p.co
pro.
N
+
n.p.(p) Transitive v.p. - n.p. - -
n.p.co(p) n.p.co
pro. pro.
+ + +
E - n.p.(p) Ditransitive v.p. - n.p.(p) - n.p. - -
n.p.co(p) pro. n.p.co
pro. n.p.co(p) (pro.)
P
+
- n.p. Copulative v.p. - n.p.(p) - + n.p. -
E n.p.co n.p.co(p) n.p.co
pro. pro.
N
+ +
- n.p.(p) Directive v.p. - - - - n.p.
n.p.co(p) n.p.c.
D pro.
+ +
- n.p. Initiative v.p. - Complementation + verbal noun
n.p.co M. /
pro.
- n.p.(p)
+
Introductory (1) - n.p.(p) + n.p.(c) - -
n.p.co(p) n.p.co(p)
pro. pro.
D +
- n.p.(p) + +
E Introductory (1) - n.p.(p) - n.p.(c)
P n.p.co(p) n.p.co(p) - bt + speech/
E pro. pro. clause
N
D +
- n.p.(p)
+
Introductory (2) - n.p.(p)
N n.p.co(p) n.p.co(p) -
T pro. pro.
n.p.(p) and n.p.co(p) indicates that the head of the construction filling this
place must be a personal noun. However in fable style a non-personal noun may
also function here.
n.p.(c) Indicates that only a very limited group of nouns, which are semantically
cognate with the preceding verb, nay fill this place. e.g. gbA:ga 6d1 'speak word'
-160--
A full description would include notes on each of the clause types giving
further details of the classes of the fillers and any restrictions in their occur-
rence not shown on Chart I, and transformations or details of distribution relevant
to the separation of clause types, notes on frequency, and examples from text ma-
terial to illustrate the clause types and also to prove their validity from unelic-
ited forms. In order to restrict the length of this paper only the Transitive and
Directive clause types are here so described.*
CHART II Citation paradigm showing contrasts in the nuclear places of the basic
clause types
Clause type Subject Predicate Complementation
Intransitive Cite 6460"
father lingers
.t
Transitive Cite oti eten
eats meat
Ditransitive Cite Ago twa eten
gives child meat
Copulative Cite 6he 644:n50
is chief
.1.
Directive 'ate °yin eten
father goes meat (to fetch meat)
.1
or Cite °yin gPYg
goes market
t,
Motive ate 5to:ma jwa eten (to fetch meat)
sends child meat
Stative Cite 6311, 63111
is good rich
Initiative Cite 45m eten Oc=ci (- indicates open
begins meat to-eat transition)
Ind. Introductory 1
Cite 5ba:ga jwa Odig
talks child word
says
Dep. Introductory 1
.
ote ogba:ga 6we (±be)
II
says child that "
Dep. Introductory 2
4
Cite 6n
ob.! jwa
II
says child that "
t.
-162-
El
-163-
iv) Either the Goal place or Ex.l. place is almost always (perhaps) filled or is
explicit in the linguistic context. An exception to this rule is the common use
of the verb yin to mean 'to go to farm'. In this use the Goal place is not filled.
Examples from text
oyin Ar5b Irts5:0 (1.4)
she-goes mushrooms in evening (she goes mushrooming...)
P Goal Ex.l
dir n.p. prep.p.
..6yin 1357'd (19.1)
he-goes market
P Goal
dir n.p.
egbaji (18.3)
they-go hunting
P Goal
dir n.p.
MT'
-164 -
CHART III shows the possible transformations of the basic clause types into
contrastive moods
Trans. X X X X X X
Ditrans. X X (X) X X X
Copul. X (X) X X X X
Ind.
Intro.l X X X X X (X)
Dep.
Intro.l X X (X) X X X
Dep.
Intro.2 X X (X) X X (X)
. -
(X) indicates that this form has not yet been found in text material, only
in elicited forms.
-165-
Rules for the transformation of basic clause types into different moods
The points in which the Indicative mood contrasts with other moods are summarized.
Transformations of other moods are described in relation to contrast with the
B
indicative.
Indicative i) Has the order Ex.3 S P Complementation Ex.l Ex.2
ii) Obligatory presence of order 1 verb-prefixes (person)
iii) Optional occurrence of order 2 verb-prefixes (tense) and of
verb-auxiliaries within the verbal phrase
iv) Occurrence of indicative tone patterns
Imperative i) Obligatory absence of Subject place
ii) Obligatory absence of person and tense verb- prefixes
iii) Occurrence of imperative verb-prefixes zero 'sing'
ma- 'plu.'
iv) Occurrence of imperative tone patterns
Subjunctive i) Optional presence of particles c' or bg preceding the Subject
or Predicate place
ii) Occurrence of distinctive tone patterns
Interrogative i) Occurrence of an interrogative class word in the Subject place,
or in any one of the Complementation places, or in Ex.l place
Subordinate i) Obligatory occurrence of clause conjunction Examples of clause
conjunctions are Ode 'if' gra 'before' mink kw' 'like'
ii) Distinctive tone patterns (= same as subjunctive)
Relative i) Obligatory presence of a relative particle (in concord with the
noun which the clause qualifies)
Interrogative, Subordinate and Relative clauses are further distinguished by
differences of distribution. These are described in section IV.
intro 2
Pronoun
1
P Ex.2
intr adv.
Interrogative c' 51'150 kw' ape 6Bekwe 6niog men -o (126.5)
grave of person who died newly it-is where ?
- - S. - - - - P Ex.1
n.p.co intr interrogative
...5gben5ke' kwg 6kpeng yen (128.3)
brother his is-called what?
S P Comp
iQ
n.p. cop interrogative
6w6 ma 6kel, áîíì -o (85.1)
this here meets whom ?
S P 0
n.p. tr interrogative
Subordinate 6de aben bg/ Ayin 5t5g ( 4 iken yin (2.1)
if you-want that you-go thing early go
conj P P Goal Ex. P
intro2 dir n.p. prep.p. dir
isa 6yog Okwil / 5ta e / 545 ) (20.2)
if friend comes he-will he-kill
-shoot him
conj S P P 0 P
n.p. dir tr pro tr
-168-
S: n.p. co P IO 0
imper ective x
SUBORDINATE h
perfe tive j
imper 'ective x
RELATIVE k
perfe :tive m
-169-
Positive Negative
Positive Negative
Indic. imperf. Cite eten
aci a 5s5:m Cite eten sa 5s5:m mOci
tather eats meat in house father meat in house won't-eat
x a
Indic. perf. Cite Veten 5cg qa 5s5:m Cite eten sa 5s5:m koci
father meat ate in house father meats in house hasn't eaten
past b c
us he-will-not-see
b) tata 61M. k' ij5:g 5d 5b50 sa q'gbg (14.3)
I English can't-hear
n
iv) as 1st element in a result clause composite (see result clause composite
diagram section VII)
i) x x x x
U ii) x
iii) x
iv) x x
v) x
vi) x x x
vii) x x x
viii) x
ix)
clause qualifies.
-172-
Ex.2
* [In the report, changes caused by shifting tagmemes for emphasis are treated
as leading to sets of emphatic clauses, one set for each basic type. K. L. P.]
-173-
ci
-174-
0-
Clause Groups reflect certain close-knit relationships within the serial con-
struction. They are set up on the basis of:
(a) frequency of occurrence of certain patterns
(b) restrictions of the fillers of certain Predicate places
(c) potentiality of transform
The Transitive group may be diagrammed:
+P:t6ga + 0 P:cga + P:tr - Ex.1
Lima tima
This may be transformed into a single clause P:tr + 0 - Ex.'. There is a tendency
to avoid the occurrence of two non-Predicate places explicit in one clause within
a serial construction.
The Ditransitive group may be diagrammed:
+P:t6ga + 0 + P:ditr +10
Vina
0-
+ P:ditr + Goal /
0
-175-
The Object referent of the two obligatory clauses must not be the same. This
group will not transform to a single clause.
Groups may be, but are not commonly, interrupted by other serial clauses
(occurring before the second predicate).
If the first clause of the group occurs within an Object wave of reference
the Object will not be explicit.
'Clause Auxiliary Verbs'
never occurs in an unelided form but may be prefixed by the full range of verb
prefixes.
I
-176-
fl
1;
E.g. oyog ofona Oyfn gpyg (19.1)
kg' ose oyog sg / obina Ota oyog / ayOg oto / ape (20.4)
Note:- Paux place would be filled by a verb phrase with one of these verbs as head.
S P:toga 0 / P:ditr I0
0 >
S
Accompaniment:
all baba 5116:0E / abg sa' Etakpa min:a / ose b / c'fid aseig 610" iby6
7 1 7
S
-177 -
E] Accompaniment; Tr-n.
El btg mgkpgr / (5t5ga OkorO / Ope oyog es6) / (50ga s / ole fia iqeg)
he-went at-once took knife cut friend head took him put in ground
P:dir Ex.2 P:toga 0 P:tr 0 P:toga 0 P:tr Ex.1
0
(S)
On5:0a
(7.5)
he-buried him
P:tr 0
Tran.
(obina 515ga gri5risti'AdO//Ottim epe mApyir // OyigA) (102.4)
he-got- took bird's that dug hole all put-in
up head
Paux P:toga 0 // P:tr 0 // P:tr
Tran.
(gOga aik5mgirido c' wura k' gkima / Agog sg) / (At5ga // gwOria //
you-take hot-water that which you put keep there take pour
P:toga 0 P:tr Ex.1 P:toga P:tr
0
(5)
Accompaniment
Arg:ita ab,A)
(56.1)
you-cook soup
P:tr 0
13. Osood
knows/recognizes
14. 5targ
adds
All these sentences can be formed which can be made by interchanging substitution
items.
Structure of verb is verb-prefix (o/o 3rd person .7ing.; 3 before low vowels,
a o before high vowels.) + verb-root.
Subject and Location places are optional. Verb and Object obligatory.
Future Transformation
The above sentences can be turned into the future tense by changing the tone
on the verb-prefix to high, and further prefixing the future prefix m-.
In addition, in sentences 8-14 the first (high) tone of the Object noun is
slightly lower than the preceding high tone (downstepped). This is shown by ',
e.g. 1. 1,7A mode) dwilr5 snpyg 'the child will buy a dress in the market'
2. moror'ewiir5 snpyg 'she will want the dress in the market'
In addition:
In sentences 1-7 the verb prefix o- becomes high tone with a downstep onto the
verb root. All syllables of the verb root are high tone.
In sentences 8-14 the verb prefix o- becomes high tone. The pattern on the
verb root is low tone followed by high tone. Where the verb root has two syllables,
the first will be low, the second high. Where the verb root has only one syllable
a low-high glide occurs on this syllable. (Glides are not normally a part of the
tone system--this represents a compression of a word tone pattern onto one syllable):
E.g. 1. Wa gwur3 sglpya ktideb6 'The child didn't buy the dress in the
market'
2. Wa 6115:ma k56am 'The child didn't forget the dress'
8. Wa gwiirb koror6 'The child didn't want the dress'
13. Wa gwiir5 k6s60 'The child didn't recognize the dress'
Where the basic initial tone of the noun functioning as object is low it becomes
high after a Class 1 verb in the future tense, but remains low after a Class 2 verb,
but this is not illustrated here. In the perfective negative Verb-roots of Class 1
have high tone on all syllables, Verb-roots of Class 2 have the pattern low-high
spread over one or two syllables.
The same illustration can be amplified to include the plural distinction:
The verb-prefix for 3rd person plural is a- with same tones as in singular. In
addition the clitic m- is prefixed to the verb in positive tenses: but to the
object (in other clause types to the first complementation item- not illustrated
here) in negative tenses. In the future plural, where it precedes the consonant
m it carries low tone.
n Examples (plural prefix m- underlined):
Present plural
Iva madeb ewiirb 'The children are buying the dress'
iva makerE ewiirb 'The children are looking at the dress'
Future plural
iva ilmagdeb ewiirb 'The children will buy the dress'
Iva filmakerc'ewilrb 'The children will look at the dress'
Perfective negative plural
iva mewilrb kadebg 'The children didn't buy the dress'
iva mewilrb kaker6 'The children didn't look at the ''-ress'
Non-perfective negative plural
Iva mgwilrO madeb 'The children won't buy the dress'
lva mewilrb mAkerg 'The children won't look at the dress'
In all examples where two vowels occur together elision occurs; the first vowel
is lost, the second retained. In the examples given, elided vowels have no tone
mark on top of them, signalling that they do not in fact occur in normal speech,
but would occur in very slow speech.
Appendix II
PRELIMINARY NOTES ON SERIAL
CONSTRUCTIONS IN KASEM
Kathleen Callow
CORPUS AND METHOD
The evidence for this preliminary survey is based on 100 serial construc-
tions taken from running text, and all percentages and frequency counts are
taken from this corpus. Conclusions were then checked against a larger body
of material, and a few examples of features which did not appear in the counted
corpus are taken from this extended corpus. The original 100 serial construc-
tions were taken from three different texts covering a total of about 400
clauses (150 sentences), and the further corpus used for checking was probably
slightly smaller than this.
REGULAR SERIAL CONSTRUCTIONS
Regular serial constructions are defined as those that consist of a pri-
mary clause followed by one or more secondary clauses in the same aspect. That
is to say, a regular serial construction is either entirely in the non-continu-
ous aspect (alias perfective, alias aorist), or entirely in the continuous
aspect (alias imperfective, alias stative). In the counted corpus, 79 percent
were non-continuous, 8 percent were continuous, making a total of 87 percent of
the serial constructions which were regular.
Certain statements can be made concerning the structure of the clauses in
a regular serial construction.
The primary clause may be any one of the neutral or non-terminal types de-
scribed in connection with the clause matrix, below.
The secondary clause subject. All secondary clauses must have a pronoun
subject agreeing in person number and class with the subject of the primary
clause. Its tone is mid in non-continuous clauses, low in continuous clauses.
The secondary clause verb mood. In a non-continuous series, the verb is
always in the consecutive mood; in a continuous series it is always in the non-
future mood.
The secondary clause Dreverbal tagmemes. No preverbal tagmeme ever occurs
other than the subject, which means that there is obligatory, absence of the in-
troductory and temporal tagmemes, and of all preverbal particles, including the
negative ones. Consequently, 50 percent of the counted secondary clauses occurred
in the minimum form, viz., S:Pn + Pred:V.
-182-
VW'
-183-
U occurs, it may be of any length (one object was 14 words long) and may include a
rankshifted clause.
All attempts have failed* to state restrictions in terms of verb occurring
in specified sequence. There is a tendency for the following verbs to occur in
fl
the final position in the series: dwey 'to exceed', gaali 'to exceed', gara
'to be better', gan 'to fail', pa (+ 0) 'benefactive', by 'to finish'. These
occur final in the series very regularly, but the non-continuous series consists
essentially of a sequence of successive actions, and it is only necessary to
add another action to reduce these verbs to medial position, e. g.,
awia duwri o daali "pilwri 3 yiy songo yiyga 3 dlari
Awia ran he exceed Apuwri he reach house first he leave him
'Awia ran faster than Apuwri and reached the house before him.'
It is probable, however, that these verbs would always be final in a continuous
construction, which describes simultaneous actions or states.
(a) they may have a particle between the subject and the predicate;
(b) there is an alternative construction in which the series is termi-
nated before the continuous clause and the conjunction yi 'and' inserted,
thus making a complex sentence. It is normal to form a complex sentence
using yi whenever a construction is required which would break the series,
by changing the aspect or by introducing a new subject in the same sentence.
(Query: Does this imply that yi introduces a change of subject which is
out of 'focus'?) E.g.,
'He saw a mouse sitting stirring porridge while its tai) washed calabashes'
.
3. o to kOwri o yi babia
he still contin. he is brave
'He was still as brave as before'
4. pe ma gya u 3 pa 3 diyga
chief took him he give him enter room
'The chief made him go into a room'
5. zimbaaru wpm ma zangi o swe o yira...
brother the - rose he wash his body...
'The older brother got up, took his bath...'
(b) Causative. This is the only clause type which must be followed by a
different subject in the following secondary clause. Where this subject is a
pronoun there is no other contrast with a regular series, but the subject of
the secondary clause may also be a noun. This may point to the setting up of
a complex series to handle this.
-186-
o pE a niingi
he gave I go-out
'he made me go out'
Impersonal verbs
There are certain clauses with the subject ku 'it' which have so far been
handled as intransitive clauses, as they do not contrast with them in the prim-
ary clause place. However, they would'be better handled separately as they have
unusual functions elsewhere.
(a) They may occur as included (rankshifted) clauses, filling an object,
time, or adverb place without breaking the series. E.g.,
fl
...5 viiw 5 nowri mangi di siwla finto o gya ba Li
(c) There is a further construction, without any known allos, which also
permits a following clause which is not possible in a regular series, as it r
may change the aspect (first example) and the subject. E. g.,
ku da dibam sangi
it is-not us cooked food
'it wasn't us who cooked the food'
INDEPENDENT CLAUSES
D
Elaine Thomas
n 4. Stative
5. Equative
n
6. Benef active
tj
7. Directive
ri
LI
2. Citation paradigm
1. omo- yo m3- de isen
D 2. &16-37 rri-ue
1
u 6a. 6m6-37
, ,
m3-d? -ke Oyt isen
6b. omo-yo mo,a-k isen my 6n6m6 1-161)
0
U 3. List of lexical items
isen
sen fish -y emphatic
D (t6Ln wood mo- nonpast 3rd person
,!, singular, subject
omo child
. . o- 3rd person singular,
-most. male
Pi subject equative
. .
oyo him/her
-kc benefactive
6n6m6 mother
D .%
mu introductory particle
nou his for NP benefactive
t, V.
aido be there
D A buy
ki. give
LI to go
v(6 cry
-187-
o yin be
i S.'
-188-
4. Translation of clauses
1. The child buys fish.
2. The child cries.
3. The child gives him fish.
4. The child is there.
5. The child is a male.
6.a. The child buys fish for him.
The child buys fish for his mother.
The child buys it for his mother.
7. The child goes for firewood.
Intrans ± NP +Intrans VP
pro
1 4.
Ditrans NP +Ditrans VP +pro - NP
pro
Stative
+
- NP +Stative VP
pro
+
Equative NP +Equative VP +NP
pro pro
+ + NP ± NP
Benefac. ± NP +Benefac. VP -pro
K.....,
pro o-- >Ben
Directive +
- NP +Direct. VP +
NP
pro
Note
1. If the I.O. is a personal noun a transformation into a serial clause is re-
quired.
Marginal places
All basic clause types may be expanded by optional marginal places which may be
diagrammed as follows:
Ex.l: time phrase +Nucleus -Ex.2:manner phrase - Ex.3:loc. phrase
Ex.4: time phrase / temporal
Fillers of places
(a) Subject, Object and Complement places.
i. NP +Head ± Mod ± Num ± Poss. ± Dem.
So far only 2 attributes have been found in one phrase.
iI \ I
Examples sen ive noir 'his 2 fish'
Co-ordinative omoyo nv. onoma nov. 'the boy and his mother'
Relative omo nootdn mekiyo
Note
1. 1st and 2nd person singular. object = I.O. are expressed by a
verb suffix.
mo-mon-me 'he sees me'
'he buys (it) for me'
(b) Predicate place
The filler of the predicate place is a verbal phrase with a verb of the
appropriate class as its head. The VP may be diagrammed as follows:
± Aux 2 ± Aux 1 + Verb
Examples oda 'so) he still buys '
A.2. Al.vb
ti
7. Verb chart
a
Singular Plural
0 2 3 1 2 3
Past
mi- s u- o- e- a- e-
Q
Nonpast me- mu- mo- me- me- me-
o
4. There is vowel harmony throughout the verb word between the followiilig 2
sets of vowels; the vowel of the root determines the set used.
11
tense set i e e o u
lax set L
e a 0 I)
D
Appendix IV
GRAMMATICAL PROSODIES ??
John T. Bendor-Samuel
Each phrase consists of two words and each word includes a prefix or suffix.
The type of analysis proposed here would state that such phrases consist of two
main elements (the two words), together with one of a series of concord prosodies.
These concord prosodies are realized by the affixes. The selection of one out of
the set of concord prosodies is determined by the class of the noun head of the
nominal phrase.
Concord in Etung extends beyond the single phrase to include nominal and
verbal phrases. Clauses such as the following occur:
e yu. ji - t - man
yam one is finished
bi yu bi bae bi - man
yams two are finished
n - kop yi - t a man
box one is finished
o fak bi - t o man
broom one is finished
-194-
The concord prosodies extend over the subject nominal phrase and verbal
phrase. Thus the clause as a whole is marked by the concord not just the
phrase. In a full treatment of Etung it would be necessary to state these con-
cord features as operating as a feature of certain clauses and phrases.
4
Another type of extension occurs in clauses in Bimoba . A conditional
clause is identical in structure to an independent clause except that it is
marked by the particle i and/or tii which occurs after the nominal phrase sub-
ject and before the verbal phrase, together with a low tone on the final syllable
of the clause. Sometimes the particle i occurs more than once in the clause,
occurring as the final syllable of some of the verbal particles. The combina-
tion of particle(s) and low tone must be regarded as the exponent of the sin-
gle grammatic category of "conditional". Instead of considering such a parti-
cle as a unit of the structure of the clause, which would raise problems since
it is clearly not a phrase, it is simpler to regard it as a part of a type of
grammatical prosody. The prosody would include the repeated occurrences of
the i particle and the final low tone.
Examples:'
kojok i dii napayon gbanl not saa mok n bik na
cock if cat stone one fidish he future marry my daughter this
"If the cock finishes eating one stone, he will marry my-daughter."
What of the other type of prosody - that which though not extending over
more than one segment has implications for the structure as a whole.
In "maay West African languages there are particles which can be regarded
as playing just such a role. To take another Bimoba example, the general sub-
ordinative clause is marked by the particle a (after pronouns) or mba (after
nouns). It is difficult to describe these particles as being either themselves
phrases or as being a constituent part of any other phrase in the clause. To
assign them to the nominal phrase or verbal phrase seems quite arbitrary and
their function is to mark the clause as a whole. This point is further illus-
trated since when a series of verbal phrases combine to make a serial clause
the particle only occurs once. These particles may then be:said to have an
-195 -
Examples
o a won wuur gben ti won joob ti sanjok
he yesterday washed finished we yesterday took our road
"Yesterday as he finished washing, we started on our way."
FOOTNOTES
II
1
This note arises in part from the two workshops conducted by DI. K. L. Pike
under contract 5-14-065 of the U. S. Office of Education, through the Center
for Research on Language and Language Behavior, of the University of Michigan.
The underlying research on West African language has been carried out as a
member of the Summer Institute of Linguistics in association with the Univer-
sities of Ghana and Nigeria.
2
For an introduction to the theory of prosodic analysis see J. R. Firth,
"Sounds and Prosodies", Transactions of the Philological Society, 1948, pp.
127-152; and R. H. Robins, "Aspects of Prosodic Analysis", Proceedings of the
University of Durham Philosophical Society, Volume I, Series B, No. 1 (1957).
For an example of this type of analysis applied to a language see my "Some
problems of segmentation in the phonological analysis of Tereno", Word, Vol.
16, No. 3, pp. 348-355 (1960).
3
I am indebted to my S.I.L. colleagues, Mr. and Mrs. T. Edmondson, for
the Etung material used in this illustration.
4
For further details of this and the other Bimoba examples quoted in
this note, see my 'Problems in the analysis of sentences and Clauses in Bimoba'
to be published shortly in Word. I am indebted to my colleagues, Miss G. Jacobs,
for the Bimoba material.
5
Compare R. S. Pittman's treatment of conjunctions in "A grammar of
Tetelcingo (Morelos) Nahuatl", Language Dissertation No. 50, 1954. In setting
up the category of 'valence', in contrast to morpheme, he regards conjunctions
as 'valence-carrying' morphemes. Valence is used particularly for the relation-
ship between two immediate constituents, whether overtly or covertly expressed.
Conjunctions would exemplify overt valences carried by morphemes.
Appendix V
Consonant-final stems
a Stems with a final /d/ are taken for illustrations in the singular below.
On the left is a matrix of all such stems, and on the right is the matrix of
the ci-14;ular forms, arrived at by adding the central vowel suffix to the stem.
+1ICen. V
sg.
5 6
Lower C id- Cod - Cida Coda
Harmony 7 8
0 Cad- Cod- 0 Cada Coda
The Osignifies systemic non-occurrence, i.e., /e/ and let cannot occur in
stems of this general shape; the numbers refer to the examples below.
1. bide 'wall' 5. fida 'cane'
Now note: (1) The topology (overall pattern) of the harmonies is the same,
1
and this is true throughout the language. So, replace i and I by the cover
symbol I; a and a by A; o and o by 0; u ando by U. (2) To repeat the above
matrix formula for each of the 5 final consonants (d, 1, n, 0, g) would be
redundant and inelegant, so reduce to one matrix formula by having one final
consonant per row, one stem vowel per column.
-197-
-198-
Having established this general pattern of matrix, the plural, which is more
complicated, can be exhibited in the same way.
Consonant-final stems, sg. form (N.G. The examples will follow the columns
of the matrix, and then the rows)
bide 'wall bakada 'boy' koda 'voice' boda 'fishing-net'
kikila 'round' fele 'white man' gola 'fish hook' fula 'granary'
di YE jwc bwi
-199-
Group D
to For this group, the singular suffix is a back vowel, and the plural suffix
one of the three alveolar medial consonants (d, 1, n) followed by a close back
vowel. As before, the phonemic shape of the suffixes is determined by the
phonological shape of the nominal stem.
Singular and plural matrix for Nominal Group D, consonant-final stems
NOMINAL GROUP Bl
Stems, Nominal Group Bl
(For matrix see following composite chart; group B2, not illustrated here,
includes just a few irregular forms)
sg. (-I) pl. (-A)
tidi tide 'forehead'
wad'. wada 'mud brick'
pori pore 'okro' (sometimes spelled 'okra')
kuri kure 'stone for cooking on'
it
nabili nabila 'tail'
Fa
p L pLa 'yam'
de da 'day'
Further, all the morphological forms (which function verbally, i.e., not
nominalizers) exhibited by any verb can be exhibited on a matrix of the follow-
ing pattern:
MOOD
Past Future
Abs.
Perf.
NAbs.
ASPECT
Abs.
Imperf.
NAbs.
Each of the verb groups, then, will be displayed in this matrix to show
the considerable variety of pattern that results, restricting the material,
however, to consonant-final stems.
NOMINAL GROUFS
STEM
SHAPES Bl C D
-I -A -A -I -U -TU
sg. pl. sg. pl. sg. pl.
CAD- 0 0 CADA CE 0 0
CIg- 0 0 CIgA CI 0 0
CAg- 0 0 CAgA CE 0 0
COg- 0 0 COgA CwE COgO CwAdU/COdU
CUg- 0 0 CUgA CwI CUgU CUdU
forms are needed to separate the words into classes--but many further
forms are needed to describe concomitant variants--KLP
**The absolute form usually precedes zero or some variety of juncture;
it is not followed by forms in closest-knit relation to it.
-203-
VERB GROUP A
Verb Group A is divided into four subgroups according to the pairing of
the stem-final consonants in the Perfect and Imperfect as follows:
g-d, g-n, g-zero, n-n.
Verb Subgroup Al
Past Fut. East Fut.
Verb Subf,roup A2
Verb Subu2uE_A3
Yf7-EIatgLTIPA
Past Fut. Past Fut.
-204-
VERB GROUP B
Past Fut. Past Fut.
VERB GROUP C
Past Fut. Past Fut.
fl
VERB GROUP D [The most commonly-occurring class--though with fewest contrasts
within the system]
no
-205-
VERB GROUPS
All l A2 j
A3 A4 B C D
Past Abs. CVgA CVgA CVgA CVnA CVgA CVn/oI CVCI
[The stems are first subdivided according to the prefixes which mark their singular
versus plural forms. Within this grouping, stems are subdivided according to the
tone patterns which cover the word as a whole. The tone pattern is given only once,
for the class, (determined by the stems), since the tone of the word in singular
form is the same as in the plural form (if /6/ is low, /bi/ is low: if /g/ is high,
/bi/ is high).
Loans--0 to /ba/--are not included.]
Abang E'ta.
-207-
ile in fable.
v
ebat ' light. ecabe ' disgrace.
ebibe ' dust. egbut ' swelling disease on the hand.
ebimim hiding oneself. ekap mark of ring worm or leprosy.
U ' '
esebe sand.
-208-
or Others.
ekpin life. ^"ebek sweet alligator.
1
epit superciliousness. eRagasi 1
that which is quiet, gentle
1
eriri deceitfulness. and polite person.
1
eyuk cold. eYCitiikibi
1
that which is cut into particles.
eirie e grass land.
or
esome comb.
ebam' compound.
etim hut, shelter.
ebaoe iron-thing.
eyom hoe (alt. plural in bi-).
e6oe load.
AA
efuk axe. or
ejio pile, heap (alternate ebuge tray for loads.
plural in bi-). ebune mud-bed.
er4m pod. ekoo spine (fruit edible).
eroo line. ekp4me bottle.
es4n trunk, stalk. etage flat basket for drying pepper.
esum clearing.
etek or
village, region.
etim ebln dance (alt. plural in bi-).
bush.
eb i palmnut.
or ebum heap (alt. plural in a-).
eCan electric fish. efit definite length of cloth.
efo cloth. ekim half: part which is short.
ekun spiny fish. ekon song.
ekpao hamper. etum work.
eya year.
Others
esome worm.
-209-
eyu yam.
DIALECT OF BENDEGHE AYUK, or /Bindege/,
erio sign.
in Ogoja Province, Eastern Region,
esore clay plate.
Nigeria.
esup court.
INFORMANT: MOSES ABANG ETA
ewao implement for plucking cocoa.
eyak rubber.
-210 -
e- to bi- con't.
Tone class Tone class
44. ,.
or or
ebe escape. ebaoe squirrel.
eblk path; something partly filled. eblge flying squirrel.
ebln dance. ebit ceremony after death.
ebop parcel. eboge second occasion.
V.
eciri sneeze. ebuoe stack of yams.
ef une red fly. efup fear.
efiffam kind of tree. efloe key.
eflO stomach. efige fan (alt. plural in n-).
efap floor, brooklet. efiri screw.
eflp polygyny. egome plantain.
egbuk ravine. egan story, parable.
ejimi custom. ejlp lake.
ejo little brush. ejo civet cat.
ekon song. ekebe small sickle.
ekop ladle. ekime small basket.
ekii cry, death. ekun adultery.
eribe tongue. . emaoe sickness.
eru manner, behavir:-.7. emege ankle, wrist.
esikb0 ' mud or wooden pipe. etan ant-hill.
eyat chin. etiin hair left after barbering.
eyem wound. eyom hoe.
eyilk wooden drum. eyu ray.
eyum voice.
Tone class
or
Tone class
ebore yellow scum. or
**.
* A's indicate that I think they are the Etung way of handling Adjectives; they
need a following noun to make sense, but they act like nouns themselves in that they
perturb the following noun according to normal N + N rules.
-211-
e- to bi- con't.
Tone class
.V
or
edimba narrow necked pot.
eginlm something or (someone) bent over.
eruoa water-yam.
-212- a
e- to a- (85 items)
or
n. n
or B
, , ,
ebi fox (alt. pl. in bi-). ebe breast.
, , ,
edil maggot. eb un heap (alt. pl. in N-). I]
, ,
efabe wing. A? ' eCak something torn.
, ,
a
,
efu marble. efao shoulder.
, ,
eglre ,
ring, girdle. ekim half, part which is short.
egut ,
parrot (alt. pl. in bi). (alt. plural in n-).
egbaogbao ,
big vessel. zinc. ,
eko ,
stem (of leaf).
n
, , ,
ej ik kind of maggot. ekup tick.
ejooe ,
thorn. ,
ekiik
,
cooking-stone. o
, ,
ejti
,
native pear. emao palm-leaf.
, , ,
ekek molar, black tooth. enyin dot.
, ,
ebare ,
finger-nail. eoan bat.
, , ,
A? etlm old thing. erok sore.
, ,
eyioi ,
root. esikoo wooden or mud pipe.
, ,
eyem wound (alt. plural in bi-).
.. A , ,
or eyio tooth (plural am*0).
, , ,
A? ebage something divided, e.g. yam. eyit eye (plural amit).
, ,
A? ebare ,
something growing wild. eyoo drop (e.g. of water) eczema.
,
ebi pot: placenta.
, V
ekae basket. or
, , ,
ekat leg, foot. ebat side, part.
,
ekm ,
stump of tree. ebip weal.
, ,
A? ekpage ,
empty thing. efut feather (alt. pl. in m-).
, ,
ekpage ,
skin (of tree). egbem hollow place (not filled up).
, ,
emage ,
reed. ej are foot-print, foot step.
; , ,
ej i egg.
,
emap ,
dint, dent. B
, ,
emage ankle, wrist (alt. pl. in bi-).
, ,
enyot hair.
El
,..
-213--
e- to a- con't.
IA
or or
ebare hollow rock. enyio ' guinea fowl.
A? ebare white thing.
...v
ebare clay pot. or
A? ebi red thing. egbo ' kind of pin used in decorating
ebiri hole, pit. gourds and calabashes.
eftoi broken crockery.
efare shoulder blade. ' Others.
"
A? efebe that which is light. ' A? ebege ' something broken in
efoge empty space. pieces.
efu branch of plant with efae wooden spatula (for fufu).
1 .......
leaves. efue ' yellow fly (not harmful).
......
efi ' boil. I
egbeme ' hollow place (not filled ill
A? egbogbok ' something old e.g. I
up) term of an abuse. i
An..
machine, (alt. p1. in bi-). ' ejagba ' big needle for sewing
I
A? egbligi someone moving sluggishly ' sacks (alt. pl. in bi-).
as a leper with no toes ' A? "" ejoge ' that which is broken
(alt. pl. in bi-). I
into pieces.
.......
ejak I
a laughing bird with ' A? enyage ' black thing.
......
long beak. ,
etugi ' piece of burning wood.
ekap ' mark made by ringworm. I
of ant. I
or .V
or
nabe hopping.
mbume rafters.
ndlm beans game.
ngun world.
nduge beni-seed.
nnyilo vegetables.
mf an number.
nsi mud.
mf em flower.
ngom fine.
or
mgbeyuk severe fever.
mbim ' fruit with black paint.
nkoge a tree fallen across road.
nnycre shea-nut.
Others
nntik porridge water. .0 1
mfiri ground-nut.
nnyage black juice leaf.
or
nciri gunpowder.
mf a green leaves.
mfune shredded leaves.
njue sun.
or
fl
robot pulp of fruit. LI
mbuta rain.
ncen noon.
mfam kind of juju.
I
mfep wind.
4
mfili scum.
ngum places.
nnti hair.
(8 items)
.14. 414.
or
nkoge f
tree fallen cross road.
nnyore f
shea-nut.
ntip ' sticks used for mud houses.
414. do., 0. .0 .
i
or
nkap ' kind of weaving.
nkat ' kind of fruit (edible).
nkik4t ' small mortar.
or
nsa ' draughts.
Others
mbanamba""' f
swallow (bird).
fl
-216-
N (i) to o-(66 items)
*a. S.
or or
U
mbae two spotted palm civet. mbue goat.
(i)
N - to 0- con't
or
T
mbim mosquito.
mboe T
heart.
T
robot sandfly.
T
nce small snail.
T
ncit garden-egg.
T
ndoo jigger (pl. oroo).
mfem T
cockroach.
7
ntiir) navel.
Others
I
mbiantuo-"" T
spitting cobra.
I
mfiri "" /
ground-nut.
nkupeyit-"" T
eyebrow.
-218-
palm-nuts). V
or V
or
ndap ' branch. 1
I
.V ...
or
mbue cover, lid.
ncot trouble.
nsure ripe plantain.
or
nje ' leaf.
Others
V
.^..
ndumoto ' kind of gourds
(pl. arumoto) .
.,../1116,,
-219-
or
bia'ao visit.
biji body.
bika0 wild nut with strong
smell.
birimtim violent.
bisi0 wrestling.
bitop muddy lane.
or
or
1
biC'oe bad luck.
I
bikep spy.
1
binot evening visit.
0 bisuge 1
kind of fruit with
U
sticky juice (edible)
or
...v
or
a
bi- to N (35 items)
... .0...
or or
I I
bisoo ' hip. biCage toe-sore.
i
bisu ' dove. t
biome I
calf of leg.
t
bit4me ' knife, machete. bifage ' piece of wood with fire.
t
bifure ' cane rope.
...
or bifio ' space between toes and
t
D
bibore ' kind of bird. finger.
t I
bifam ' wedge (for breaking bikebe rod for fishing.
wood). t
bikon ' wasp, or mason fly.
D
I t
bifige vampire-bat. bikpa ' whip.
bikae I
pangolin. I
bisuge I
weaver bird. o
t
bikoe ' curve. bisao ' frog with long legs which
biru ' defective pupil of the eye. ' jumps very far.
t
bise ' plate, basin.
i
bisooe ' spoon, fork. or
1 I
bitin ' calabash. biCit sunbird.
t t
bikpae ' brown bird with yellow bifet mongoose.
t
feet.
...V
or
biter) ' plait.
biton ' hair on woman's pelvis.
-221-
bi- to a- (4 items)
or
or
or
bibar) ' horn.
vein.
(o)
N to a- (20 items)
or or
nne person.
nsop girl, lady.
ntem- friend.
I
nCok fool.
0 ....
or
ndum husband.
mf4p co-wife.
nje witch.
*. V
or
mfone owner.
mgban powerful man.
nnyen mother.
-222-
0- (no plural)
or or
II
or
obi evil.
oke marriage.
osam diarrhoea.
osi chicken-pox, measles, fish.
6. A
or
V
obibe tear (from the eyes).
ob im group hunters.
V
ofi selfishness.
V
otue chieftaincy.
.11
or
obp mushroom.
ogm ' backbiting.
III
or
o- to N (1) - (3 items)
S. A
or
okun ' fire-wood.
osio ' mango tree.
Others.
o- to a- (44 items)
. v
or or
obukpoO trumpet. okpi ' boat, canoe.
ofun leprosy. osep soup.
ogm market, week. osim web, (spider's).
1
ogbe special dance.
or
ojU native pear tree.
okoro
okut wooden barrel.
down-wine.
okpa first born.
okpere gourd used as plate.
9 okpuga six pence, (pl.- a-
osere down, under.
which is money). osogo stack (of yams).
omone monitor, (kind of lizard).
A%
or
oraga bridge.
orer
oda ' drying platform.
sponge.
otere vulture. A^
or "'
otun funnel.
ogbiri chair.
otU jar.
oya stomach, pregnancy. Others.
o54m grave. okuOa chameleon.
oyUk arrow. okpiton umbrella.
A
or
ft.e %A%
or or
obi palm-tree. okik cheek.
obo arm. okoe cough.
.111,4
-224-
.(a)
- to o- (3 items)
or
mouth.
or
mbuk ' forehead.
mmi nose.
N(a)- to a- (9 items)
or
ndini ' boundary.
or
mbum ' new palm-leaf. v
or
t
n6ok pestle. 1
mf en ' shaft v
1
ngun gun, fire.
njan ' medicine. v
V ..
or
z-77"W77-7'77=n,.-, . '
-225--
%,, %%,
or Others
I ,%A I
aam palm-wine. abii race.
I AA I
adagat kind of yam. ace cutting sticks.
I %%A I
agun rust. aceme one who tramps heavily.
I %%A I
ajiri round beans. ac ±be twister.
I
aron brain. afere dust of roasted yam.
%%A I
asabe ' itching leaf with dangerous anabe repairer.
hairy fruit.
ayu willful accusation.
i
1
-226-
a- to a- (14 items)
or '"
y
aga needle.
y
akaCak rattle.
atma y
old farm ground.
y
atore mahogany.
y
ad" night.
y
aya river.
y
ayip water.
or
adiidii ' long haired goat.
afat ' set of twins.
akpakak ' chair.
.... I...
or
or
Banao ' deceit.
Batm ' beating.
Bafao untruthfulness.
II III
or
Bakpii ' wickedness.
Appendix VII
PRELIMINARY DESCRIPTION OF
SOME VERB STRUCTURES IN ETUNG1
Tom Edmondson
0
Introduction
The focus of this paper is upon some segmental and tonal structures of
2
verbs in Etung , a Cross-River Bantu language of Southeastern Nigeria.
The verb structures handled here are those found in the predicates of
independent and dependent clauses (i.e. not subordinate clauses) with the
exception of (independent) imperative clauses. These clauses are so decribed
because of their distribution:in the sentence.
Verb structures are discussed here under two heads: basic forms (section
1.) and amplifications of basic forms (section 2.). On the basis of structural
differences two sub-groups of basic forms are set up: (a) indicative-prefixed
forms (section 1.1) and (b) minimal and suffixed forms (section 1.2).
Features relevant to each section, though not necessarily treated in this
order are: (1). Principal classes of verb stems; (ii). the segmental and CV
structures of basic forms of verb structures and their amplifications; (iii).
The relations of these structures to corresponding tonal structures, and (iv).
the varying nature of these tonal structures in terms of predictability and
unpredictability.
1. Basic Forms of Verb Structures.
1.1. Indicative-prefixed Forms.
1.1.1. The structure of any indicative-prefixed form consists of an obliga-
1
-227-
-228-
C CV
CV V CV CVC
Structures CV CVV
CVCV
Tone 3 2 1
Patterns Syllable Syllable Syllable
1 LLL LL L
2a LHH LH LH
I I
2b 'HH 'H
3 HLL HL HL
4 HHH HH H
5 LHL La -
6 LLH LLH -
7 HHL HHL -
t fil
8 HIM H't H H:H
r\
9 HLH HLH -
1 'TN i
10 LH'H LH'H 1 -
1
0 i eTh
11 ' HH'H 1-1Hi li
wr-
-229-
Tone Patterns of 1
1
Tone Patterns of ! Tone Patterns of Tone Patterns
Pronominal Prefixes ! Indicative Prefixes 1 Verb Stems of 1 of Verb stems
Class A of Class B
t
H L (kA) H.H HH
L HL (kk.) HL HL
i
III sg. a-
1 pl. e-
II pl. o-
III pl. a-
Third person singular and plural indicative-prefixed forms are nearly
always distinguished by tonal differences over the pronominal prefix segment
or over the indicative prefix segment where the latter requires a special
tone in the 3rd person plural.
-230-
Supplementary Examples:-
'he-is-washing'
. .!
a-ka-su
'he-did not-wash'
'he-should not-wash'
Examples for
TABLE II: Class B Verbs.
!
(6) a -ki bome 'he-is-putting on D
(7) a- mo- bome 'he-won't-put on'
.
(8) a -ka-bome 'he-has not-put on'
(9) 'he-will not again-put on
(10) a -ka 'he-should not-put on'
Supplementary Examples:-
.
a -ki ka 'he-is-staying'
-ka -k 'he-did not-stay'
a -ka -ka 'he-should not-stay'
The 3rd person singular forms cited above represent tones of all other
persons except 3rd person plural. Under section 1.1.2. above, it was observed
that 3rd person singular and plural are distinguished nearly always by tone,
either over the pronominal prefix segment (compare examples 1 and 5 above with
examples 11 and 15 respectively below: similarly, compare examples 6 and 10
above with examples 16 and 20 below.) or over the indicative prefix segment
(compare examples 2 and 3 above with examples 12 and 13 below: similarly, ex-
amples 7 and 8 above with examples 17 and 18 below.). Examples 4, 9, 14 and 19
show the exception to the general tonal differentiation of 3rd person singular
-231-
and plural, it will be noticed that while in 3rd person plural forms,
the relations of pronominal prefix and indicative prefix tone patterns do
not generally correspond to Table II, then relations between the indicative
prefix tone patterns and verb stem tone patterns are perfectly regular.
1.2.2. In the phonological analysis of Etung, pitch has been treated at the
level of the phonological word in terms of tone patterns, so that each phono-
logical word is marked by a tone pattern. The phonological word is not, how-
ever, necessarily congruent with the grammatical word and in fact the constitu-
ent parts of the verb are themselves regarded as phonological words. For example,
the pronominal prefix is regarded phonologically as a word and therefore carries
a tone pattern. The same is true of other verb prefixes and of the verb stem.
r
-232-
Upon the same principle the suffix too is a phonological word except when it
ii
is not a vowel. Li
The suffixed verb stem might then be treated as a complex unit bearing
two tone patterns, i.e., the tone pattern of the verb stem plus the tone
pattern of the suffix. But it facilitates description to regard the verb
plus suffix as a complex unit, both phonologically and grammatically. As a
complex phonological wor.! comprising two such words, it is regarded as being
marked by one tone- pattern. As a grammatically complex unit, here termed a
verb core, its tonal structure generally is predictable, being determined by
the tone pattern of the suffix morpheme and the class to which the verb stem
belongs. Table III illustrates the predictability of tone patterns over verb CJ
cores.
The core whose tone pattern is LH!H is confined to Class A verbs. Some a
exceptions to these rules are stated below under Suffix Morphemes.
Suffix-Morphemes.
These suffix morphemes and the tenses they indicate are now listed to-
4
gether with the tense abbreviations employed in Table IV below.
a
-á Past habitual (PH) [also -k for certain verbs with CV stems]
f,
-a -'a Present habitual (PrH) [also -k for certain verbs
with CV stems.]
_!A
occurs with third person plural
-a occurs elsewhere
(in class B verbs, the suffix -a produces tone
pattern LH over the verb core, an exception to
the rules above.)
D[
-233-
o
vectors IP, the dotted ring shows that in the third person singular of the
immediate past /future tense (with both class A and class B verb stems), the
, G
expected tone pattern LL does not occur over the verb stem segment. Similarly,
in vectors 0 (with both classes of verb stems) it might be expected that
third person plural would exhibit tone pattern HL over the verb stem following
o
tone pattern L over the pronominal prefix. But this is not the case and the
dotted rings show this. Second, complete rings at interstices indicate either, o
(i) that the expected tone pattern over the verb stem or core occurs in an un-
expected position, e.g., verb stem tone pattern in third person plural of vector
0 (both classes of verbs), or (ii) that an unexpected tone pattern appears in
an unexpected position, e.g., in vector OP (class A verb stems) the verb stem
fl
U
tone pattern in third person singular is LH following pronominal prefix tone
pattern L. Similarly in vector PPv (class A verb stems) the verb stem tone
D
pattern in third person plural is HH following pronominal prefix tone pattern H.
It will be observed that all these irregularities only affect third persons
singular and plural which are thus always distinguished tonally. There are three a
types of distinctions:
(i) distinctions dependent upon dissimilar tone patterns n
over both pronominal segments and verb stems or cores
(see Table IV: Class A verbs, vectors IP, OP, PPv,
II
and PP; Class B verbs, vector IP);
(ii) distinctions dependent upon dissimilar tone patterns
of the pronominal prefixes only, while tune patterns
a
over verb stems and verb cores remain identical (see Table
IV: Class A verb stems, vectors PH and 0; Class Bverb stems,
0
vectors OP, PH, PPv, 0 and PP), and
(iii) distinctions dependent upon dissimilar tone patterns a
over the verb cores only (see Table IV: Class A verbs,
vector PrH; Class B verbs, vector PrH). These differences
of differentiation between third persons singular and plural
are supporting criteria for distinguishing between the two
classes of verb stems.
It has been noted above under 1.1.2. that second persons singular and
plural are distinguished by the plural verb-suffix -wlin rl, -tin. This suffix
is secondary in position to the tense suffixes when they occur.
-235-
TABLE IV:-
FORMATION OF MINIMAL AND SUFFIXED FORMS OF THE ETUNG.VERB
I sg L - - LH LH - HL EETTIT - HH HH - HL H'1 H
1H
H LL H' LH - LL HH - - LH - -
IIsg o- L - LH LH - HL LH .H HH HH - HL H't il
...,
t
H LL H. H - LH - - LL HH - - LH -
n -- 1
U 1-
I pl e- L - - LH LH - HL LOH - HH HH - HL H' H
1
1
H LL H.H - LH - - LL HH - - LH - -
I
4- /-
IIpl o- L - 1- - LH LH - HL LH.H
t
%....
- HH HH - IHL H'H
t
H LL HIH - LH - L HH - - LH - -
IIIsg a- L E) (D LH LH - L LHIH
..-
40 Ci HH HH 1 - HL H'I H
H (2eN -1 - LH - CI, :) LH
.._/ k.....1
H
L -
LL
-
H1H
1/4-.i
LL HH
t_)
...)
-
C._
...,
HH
_
rt---
.1 `J
100 EHL e
_
el
_ _ _ _
3 HL r"--- - _
I
Notes: (1) In vector PP (Class A verbs) the ligature linking H with 'H
indicates that these two tones occur over the same syllable.
(2) Tenses constituted by minimal forms [by change of stem tone]
are indicated at the heads of relevant columns by abbreviations
in italics.
io
-236-
u
-237-
Supplementary Example:
mI I
-bo m-a 'I put on'
Supplementary Example:
U
a -bom -a 'He puts on'
-238- 0
Basic Forms
IP OP PH PPv PrH f) PP Indic-Prefix
Repetitive
Optional x x - x - x x i x
Prefix
kpo
now the optional-prefix, not the indicative-prefix that determines the tone
pattern of the verb stem. The relations involved here are stated in Table
VI.
H LH 'H
'H LH 'H
L LH H
HL LH H
Under 2.1 it was noted that the repetitive optional prefix is tonally affected
by the tense whose form it amplifies. In minimal and suffixed forms this means
that the tone pattern normally found over a Class B verb stem or verb core in
the tense to be amplified is now found over the repetitive-optional prefix ex-
cept in the amplifications of pluperfect variant (PPv) and Pluperfect (PP),
where the tone pattern of the optional prefix becomes that of Zero tense (0).
For example, in the optative tense (OP), whether the form to be amplified in-
volves a Class A or B verb stem, the repetitive optional-prefix tone pattern
is HH, while the verb stem tone pattern is LH in the case of a Class A verb
stem or 'HH in the case of a Class B verb stem.
In the partial citation paradigm which follows, both basic and amplified
forms are given to facilitate comparison. The paradigm is confined to first
person singular and to verbs with Class A stems. Also included are the tone
Patterns of Class B verb stems and cores in basic tenses.
-240-
2.3 Initial optional prefixes occur before the pronominal prefix. Not only
do they have little effect upon the tonal structure of basic forms, but with
the exception of kln they are severely restricted in distribution. Table
VII displays their distribution.
-241-
Cl
(Partial Citation Paradigm, cont'd)
Indicative-Prefixed Forms. 0
. ... . V
ki k4n-n-ki-su 'I should be about to wash'
!
. . .... ,
mo kin-m-!mo-su 'I would not have washed' (meaning difficult
I A
. . .1.. to ascertain.)
ka Icri-n-ka-su. 'I should not have washed'
! ' . . . .1.
ro kin-n-do-su. 'I would not have washed again'
(if it hadn't been necessary)
ka kin-n-ka-su 'Perhaps I should not wash'
(I'm not certain).
Reading along the second row of Table VII.
tik-n-suk 'I will be washing'
tik-n-su 'I will wash'
. v
kin-tik-n-suk 'Perhaps I will be washing-I have actually
planned to do so'
'Perhaps I will wash (if plans work out)'
Reading along the third row of Table VII.
v. v
cap -n-suk 'I don't wash (never do)'
.
kin-card -n-suk 'I should not have been washing'
k4n x x x x x I x
+ , i
- k±n + tik x
+ 4, VP
- k±n + cap x
I
1
I
Indicative-prefixed forms:
k4n-a-ki-I V
kI l(po-su 'I should have been about to wash again'
2.5. Finally, it should be noted that amplifications of basic tenses are rare
in text data, those found being the repetitive optional prefix, and the initial
optional prefixes tik and CaO. The repetitive optional prefix is commonly used
in speech with tenses Immediate Past/Future (IP) and Optative (OP), while_ the
initial optional prefix tik is usually used in speech with the Zero (0) tense.
The other initial optional prefix,C.a.0 is commonly employed in speech with the
Past Habitual (PH) tense. Other amplifications than these are nearly all
6
elicited forms.
-244-
FOOTNOTES
1
This paper presents a preliminary analysis only The author is currently
preparing a further paper in which it is intended to state a more comprehensive
analysis in terms of rules.
2This study is based upon the dialect of Bendeghe Ayuk in Ikom division
of Ogoja Province. This dialect is almost identical with those of Etome and
Abia, the nearest Etung villages to Bendeghe Ayuk and constituting with
Bendeghe Ayuk the Mbuma Clan. Some differences in the verb structures have
been noted between the dialect of Bendeghe Ayuk and other Etung dialects (than
those mentioned above) in Ikom Division, These differences involved tone, pre-
fixation and suffixation.
3
In Etung, each phonological word is marked by a tone pattern "consisting
of a sequence of three level tones. With three syllable words, these patterns
are realized as a series of three level tones spread out over the three syllables.
With two syllable words, which are marked by tone patterns involving dissimilar
tones for second and third tones, the pattern is realized with the final two
tones compressed as a glide. With one syllable words, when the patterns involve
any dissimilar tones, the tone pattern is realized as a glide." This statement
is taken from T. Edmondson and J. T. Bendor-Samuel "The Tone Patterns of Etung",
To be published in JAL vol. iv, 1966. In that article, it is stated t'iat there
are 12 tone patterns.
4
A list of the tenses (and abbreviations used in this paper) constituted
by minimal forms and suffixed forms is given here. Minimal Forms in Immediate
past/future (IP), Optative (OP) and Zero (0). Suffixed-Forms:- Past Habitual
(PH), Pluperfect-variant (PPv), Present Habitual (PrH) and Pluperfect (PP).
5
The initial transform prefix tik has two allomorphs: -
tik employed with 3rd person plural
tik elsewhere
6However since this writing the following example of a double amplification
has been found in text data:
kin n kl ikpO
-245-
-246-
.
Unelided form Okwii &gm 5vg:n5Q pira o. ci II
1
b) Angry speech.
Okwiittim5g:n5" plrOci
1
Breathiness
13
13
U
-247-
1.23 RESPECT Situation. The informant was asked to make the words of
the example in 1.22 a request to a chief or older man.
Linguistic signals. The respectful utterance is always
spoken slowly and quietly. There is a tendency to
glide the phonemic tones giving an undulating effect
over the whole utterance. The final syllables will
bear low pitch and may be preceded by juncture.
B
Non-linguistic signals. A slow nod of the head and a
movement of both hands away from the body, the wrists
turning and finishing with the palms of the hands
facing upwards. The beckoning gesture with kwe:
2
'Come!' will be slight and slow.
phonemic tones.
I I / I I
Unelided. form okwu otum ova:noo pira oci
Example.
Elided form with
intonation and wurobenbokwu kokwu cya
+ +
phonemic tones.
phonemic tones.
Meaning child
r small thus he-came
.f.
m
makwube
obir;;;LT
obira okwu
ma wube
makwube matete ma
,,
maitet_ma piIan conina
oo
pira nnabene be
I/
conina
they-drove-him he-came- they-drove- plenty before they- let-him
(away) again him say-that stay
/
o ioa
0
o nioa
he-stayed.
Situation 2. The speaker expresses surprise at
someone's action.
Linguistic signals. There may be wide relative
pitch intervals, and it is usual for one word or
syllable to be extra high. This word may also be
lengthened and stressed. Certain lexical items
B
appear frequently in this climactic position.
e.g. mapyir 'all', matete ma 'plenty'. (When
mapyir is used indicating surprise it will be
lengthened on the first syllable - ma:pyir 'all',
when it is used in a story it may be lengthened
stylistically on the second syllable-mapyi:r).
-250-
Example
Elided form with
intonation and
. . , ., .
ono° febeba
01
L
,F_1
ici.ma:pyir
....
(gesture)
phonemic tones.
sr
-251-
chief was
There was once a chief...
B e.g. ino:ni
Bird (it is)
(iii) The use of this form for emphasis may be associated
with distance from the speaker to the hearer. e.g. The
form i: 'yes' will be used when speaker and hearer are
I close to one another. At a distance of about 12 yards
or more the form iye: will be used for 'yes'.
(iv) The final feature also has a polite connotation and
will be used for instance in summoning a chief with the
word kw4 'come!' (cf. kwu 'come!' familiar, non-emphatic
form)
p
ti
0
AJ - Accent preceding juncture
JF - Juncture before final low syllables
S Slow speech
G Gliding pitches
SS - Slow soft speech Area of linguistic
ambiguity
T Tense
L - Lengthening of all syllables
Non-linguistic signals
E Eyes open wide and rolling
QP Quick gestures, pointing
QN Quick nod
S Shrug shoulders with ci:a (Exclam.)
I - Intake or sigh before speech
B Beating fist of one hand into palm of other
H Gesture of both hands with palms turning upwards
SN Slow nod
IG Gasping intake of breath
E3
00 FOOTNOTES
1
Quotations are from informant's comments.
2
See section 1.26 a) iv
io
t
LINGUISTIC SIGNALS EMOTION NON-LINGUISTIC SIGNALS
+ B + LF + Q ANGER + E +Qp + ON
+ B + LF + Q + ci:a DISTASTE +S
+ B + WI + LC + Q + w9 + mapyir EXCITE-
+ AC ca maaama MENT
+ B + LF + JF + S +-1
ci:a SORROW + I
+ B + LF + JF + S + fisb: + B
w6,
kwa DISTRESS
+ G + JF + SS RESPECT + H +SN
+ T + L + S shortened SOCIAL
forms. FEAR + IG
SITUATION
+ -e: + Eli Emphasis
Repetition
+ -e: Emphasis
Clarity
+ unelided + EH +M Foreigner
forms
le111
-e: + EH Child
4
Appendix IX
AN INTERIM WORKSHOP REPORT ON
THE PHONOLOGICAL DATA OF AGBO
Klaus and Janice Spreda
Table of Contents
1. Introduction.
2. Phonological Hierarchy.
3. Non-suspect CV Patterns.
4. Interpretation.
5. Phonetic Workchart.
6. Evidence for Uniting or Separating Phones.
7. Chart of Phonemes.
8. Formational Statement of Phonemes.
9. Distribution of Phonemes.
10. Suprasegmental Features.
11. Unsolved Problems.
12. Orthography.
13. Text.
I. Introduction
Non-linguistic
The Legb6 language is spoken by the people of the Agb6 clan which inhabits
the banks of the Cross River. About two thirds are living on the western bank
in Afikpo Division, the rest live on the eastern bank in Obubra Division. Both
-255-
.kospofF-
-256- 13
sp 2
The clan consists of four townships with roughly the following numbers
LI
of inhabitants:
Igbo Ekureku 12.000 +
Igbo Imabana 8.000 +
Adadama 6.000 +
Itigidi 2.000 +
The figures are approximations based on the unpublished census results
of 1964. Igbo Imabana is the part situated in Obubra Division.
ED
All townships are accessible by road all the year round and have a daily
lorry service. The markets are frequented by members of the surrounding clans
and Igbo traders. However the outside influence does not seem to affect the
n
clan too strongly.
a
Linguistic
No mention of the language in linguistic literature has been discovered. U
However observations made in passing seem to indicate a certain relationship
with the neighboring languages Yakur and Mbembe (in lexical items).
Efik used to be the trade language and the majority of men have some
knowledge of it, however the use of it is decreasing in favor of English.
U
The women are mostly monolingual.
Each of the townships speaks a different dialect but the dialects are
mutually intelligible. This statement presents the dialect of Adadama.
The language has been studied in Adadama during three periods totalling
1
thirteen months between August 1964 and January 1966 . The principal informant U
was Mr. John Ebong, but about a dozen further informants of both sexes have
been used to various degrees.
This preliminary statement follows the outline suggested in "Format for
a Routine Phonological Statement", West Africa Technical Helps No. 4 of the
Institute of Linguistics.
We are greatly indebted to Mr. R. Stanford and Dr. J. T. Bendor-Samuel
for their advice during the studies and in producing this statement.
Symbolization
Mid tone and stress are not marked.
N stands for syllabic nasal. Or
-257-
II. Phonological Hierarchy
The Syllable
The syllable is defined as the tone bearing unit. Its structure can be
summarized in the following formula:
+ Marg + Nuc + Marg
1 2
C N/V
Restriction: If the nucleus is manifested by N the margins are obliga-
torily absent.
Within the syllable the nucleus is considered the tone bearing unit.
For distribution of phonemes in these three slots see section 9.
The syllable functions as a unit on the next higher level, the word level.
The Word
The word can be defined as the stress bearing unit (stress group). As
to distribution of the stress within the word see section 10.
The second criterion for the word is potential pause on either side of
it.
The word consists of one to six syllables. For details about distribution
of syllables within the word see section 9. There,details about the three
E11
types of phonological words set up according to distributional criteria can be
U
found as well.
The word has a suprasegmental feature of tenseness which is discussed in
section 10. Another feature of the word is a limited vowel harmony, for details
see section 9.
The phonological word largely corresponds to the word unit in the gramma-
tical hierarchy. It functions as a unit on the next higher level in the
phonological hierarchy, the utterance.
The Utterance
The utterance is made up of phonological words. Its criteria are pause
before and aftel it and secondly a slightly raised pitch on the first syllable
-258--
High
Mid
Low SOS
A feature of the utterance is elision. For elision rules see section 11.
Tentatively one can say that the phonological utterance corresponds
roughly to the sentence unit in the grammatical hierarchy. It is however likely
that one or more phonological levels between the word and the utterance must
be constituted (see pause [I] in the examples above), but the necessary inves-
tigations have not yet been carried out.
[e.lo.lon] 'snail'
IV. Interpretation
It proved convenient to base all the interpretation on the non suspect
syllable patterns and also to lean heavily on tone as being a feature of the
syllable and marking the syllable nucleus.
A
-259-
Syllabic
Long V or VV
[dyaakpu] 1 cassava' [essii] 'he is doing'
[atgemi] 'farm' [doom] 'burn!'
[zee] 'see!' [ewitu] 'he has already grated'
[ea55gi] 'he did not
vomit'
These are interpreted as vowel clusters because each vowel bears its
own tone though they are very closely knit together and because there are
vowel clusters in the language according to interpretation in paragraph C.
-260-
[I]
22(palatalized C followed by or c followed \T-clus ter :
[lidy6go] 'cluster'
w
C V (labialized C followed by V) or CuV (C followed by V-cluster):
MouTLn't] 'book'
w
C V or CuV or CuwV.
-7777-77711WPRr7777-7=-.. r-
, .
.1
-262-
CVi or CVyi
[ekpei] 'dove' [Ilya] 'wing'
V. Phonetic Workchart
vl. f
Fricatives
vd. v
vl. s
Sibilants
vd. z
Affricates vd. dz
Nasals vd. m ow
Laterals
Semivowels
vd.
vd. w
dillillMil
\
Y
Syllabics vd. \J ni
1111111111N
,_.
Vowels:
Front Central Back
close
High
open
Mid
Low
a
Features on higher level: fortis as feature of the word final nasalization as
feature of the utterance.
I
-263-
Word initially
[gabi] 'entwine!'
[gali] 'lift'
Generally women and softly spoken men prefer [y] while older men and force-
fully speaking people prefer [g].
Word medially
[eg5] 'cloth'
[ogg] 'bullet'
[alga] 'needle'
- .
-264-
[N] carries its own tone and stands in the nucleus of the syllable
1
1
[1E60] 'mosquito' [mama] catch!'
[dzellima] 'today' [fizam] 'back'
[Ogg] 'bullet'
1
[amin] [amen]
[enilo] 'salt'
[fiztio] 'nose'
[num] 'take!'
Sometimes older people will give [L] and [u] word medially before a syl-
lable beginning with N or /1/, while young people almost never do this:
265-
_
[nnina] ti [nnLna] 'I placed'
1 1
eko 'he hated' egb 'cloth' ckpo 'drawnet' cgbo 'he jumped'
Azi 'blood' adzi 'you ate' Aayi 'a feast' gdi 'you will say'
[s] [z]
su 'steal!' zu 'rot!'
-267-
[1] [y]
. ..:.. .
aviya 'you wedged' aviiya 'brains'
[a] [0]
fa 'rub!' fo 'return'
[o] [o]
vl. p t ty kp k kw
Stops
vd. b d dy gb g gw
vl. f i
Fricatives
vd. v
4 r
vl. s
Sibilants $
vd. z
n
a Affricates vd. dz
.. _ _
Nasals vd. m ii 0
Laterals vd. I
Semivowels vd. w y
:
High i u
Mid e o
Low 6 a o
Consonants
in /p/ [p] voiceless bilabial stop
swag...mar
-270
Vowels
/i/ [i] voiced closed unrounded front vocoid
[0 voiced half closed unrounded front vowels
[i] and [L] fluctuate in closed syllables and word medially
before a syllable beginning with [1] or a nasal, but not
in other positions:
[Amin] -[AmLn] /Amin/
[lebilA] -[lebila] /lebila/ 'bundle'
-,I=N=7:13====2.0,21=r5eGir,
-273--
but:
but:
/gitA/ 'lizard'
Any vowel may occur alone as a syllable. Front vowels are more frequent in
occurrence than back vowels. Examples are given below.
moon 'hen' AkpAikon 'shoe' ekpei 'dove'
'fish trap' ipftnAn 'vessel for g& 'a belch'
oil and pepper'
esi 'bag' gezee 'thorn'
Akpan 'plate' enaanaA 'sediment of oil' epia 'market'
iboOkolO 'dormouse' eyoo 'civet'
ittlutu 'baby' esuu 'he has
filled'
etopsi 'he threw' too 'throw!'
VC
el31-11 'hornbill'
gepite 'forenoon'
the phonemes least frequently found in the language which no doubt accounts for the
gaps. Combinations of vowels with these phonemes are illustrated in the chart below:
ty 1
dy
d +1 + ! + + +
+ + 1 + + i +
Ow 1 .
+
kw 1 + +1 +
FY +_, + + I +
Examples of CV Syllables
bUge 'children' mMoo 'answer!'
dee 'buy' nana 'run away!'
dyaakpu 'cassava' nanaoili 'fowl with rough feathers'
dze 'alligator' esuaa 'he filled'
fa 'rub!' eowA 'flood'
rt ibozu 'calabash'
U
Examples of CVC Syllables
a num 'take!' din eye
'eye' 'jigger' lOcal 'fishtrap'
fl The Syllable N
N is a syllabic nasal m n o as illustrated iZ the following words.
mbu 'coco yam'
ndegba 'road junction'
agwati 'fence'
-276--
or across morpheme borders as in the verb where the first person singular, Is
indicated by a syllabic nasal.
E.g. mbaagi 'I tied' ndaka 'I dreamt' alsele 'I chose'
Vowel clusters occur across syllable borders both within the morpheme and
across morpheme borders. They may be of the following combinations.
1. Each vowel followed by i
2. Each vowel followed by a reduplication of itself.,
3. i followed by a o o
4. o or u followed by a
2nd. member
OOOOO
ieeaoou
i + + ++
e +
1st. member e + +
a +
+
o +
u +
eploo 'hornbill'
gepiOn 'forenoon'
ekpei 'dove'
gezee 'thorn'
liveil 'wing'
gezft 'journey'
AkpAikon 'shoe'
vaal 'chief'
-277-
Vowel Harmony
Syllables are restricted in their distribution in the word to accord with a
limited system of vowel harmony.
If the vowel of the first syllable is a low front vowel, the vowel of the
second syllable is also low.
There is no vowel harthony after high or central vowels.
If the vowel of the first syllable is a front mid vowel, the vowel of the
second syllable is high or mid.
This harmony is restricted torthe first two syllables of a word except in
the verb where three harmonizing V syllables may precede the' verb root.
In the verb root only, the vowel of an initial CV syllable may be a back
vowel. The vowel of the second syllable may then be:
1. i
2nd. syllable
Lee a o o
i + + + + + + +
B .e + + + +
1st. syllable E + + +
ai + + + + + + +
0 + +
;13 + o
us +
-278-
The Noun
The syllable pattern of the noun is
1-4 + + 1-2
1. + (N/V/CV) + CV VC V
2. + CVC
3. + CV
The minimum forms are CV dze 'alligator'
Out of 750 nouns the first syllable of 380 begins with a consonant, of these:
145 have an initial g
ft
132 " 1
tt ft
85 " N
37 " a
All nouns with an initial 1 also have a final 1. Many nouns beginning with
ge- or ge- have alternate forms beginning with e- or e-.
Some of the nouns pith an initial N have an alternate form with an initial
e- or e-.
This would seem to give validity to the hypothesis that there was a noun class
system in Agbo. A further pointer in this direction seems to be that certain nouns,
especially names for relatives, change the e- or e- to a in the plural, while
most of the nouns do not have a sg. vs. pl. distinction. The related language,
MBEMBE, retained a noun class system.
The Verb
The syllable pattern of the verb is:
+ +
[- CV.CV (t CV N/V1-2)] + CV [ ±(V CV) + VC]
CVC
Prefix Root
following:
b g k m n 0 1
gali 'lift!'
Particles
Under this everything that is not noun or verb is collected. The syllable
1-3
CV + V + CVC
1-2 1-2
+ N /V
CVC
X. Suprasegmental Features
Tone
Legb6 has three tonemes as can be shown for nouns as follows:
s h h 1 akpa0 'plate' 1 1 s h H M
s h h 2 waka 'brother' 1 1 1 s H L
1 s h 3 ekpon 'hill' h s h h M H
1 s h 4 kpakpa 'gr.nut' s 1 s h M M
1 1 s 6 leta 'stone' h s h h L H
1 1 s 7 wadum 'man' h 1 s h L M
1 1 s 8 eten 'animal' s 1 1 s L L
-280- 0 4r
The frames are:
Preceding: Following:
A AdA 'where is?' = H A s6 'the' = H
B aye ezee 'he saw' = M B aaman 'our' = M
C aye Lee 'he will see' = L C w3ni 'one' = L
There are so far no disyllabic nouns of the tone pattern H H, although the tone
sequence H H does occur in nouns of more than two syllables. A strong preference
for the lower tones is evident in the language and can be shown in the following
statistical table on disyllabic nouns:
H L: 12 M L: 26 L L: 97
16 69 198
No perturbation has been observed, however tone changes are widely used to
mark grammatical distinctions like tense/aspect in the verb on the word level,
relationships between nouns on the phrase level, and certain dependent clauses
on the clause level.
Stress
Stress is not phonemic. Two factors control the distribution of stress within
the word:
(1) Tone: the stress is born by the syllable with the highest tone.
(2) Linear position in the word: out of several syllables with the
same height of tone the one nearest to the end of the word bears
the stress.
12. dy
16. z
Out of these examples the Nos. 2-6, 14, 18, 19, 21, 23, 24 are minimal
it takes place between various word classes. The elision rules for all these
combinations have not been fully investigated. However some work has been done
on the most frequent combination of verb followed by noun with the following
result:
Verb + Noun
(Nouns can begin only with a front or central vowel)
The verbs of the pattern CV elide differently from those of the pattern CVCV.
CVCV Verbs
These elide quite regularly: the final vowel of the verb assimilates
3 completely to the vowel of the noun.
CV Verbs
Verb final front or central vowels assimilate completely to the following
vowel.
Final back vowels remain unchanged except in the following cases:
+i = uu o + i = uu
+ e = oo u + i = uu
+ e = oo
Tone
It seems very difficult to determine what happens to the tone on the inter-
acting vowels as there seems to be much fluctuation according to speed of speech.
We are not certain that one of the two tones ever disappears, if so this happens
only at extreme rapidity, in which case the lower of the tones disappears.
Consonants
The final consonants 1 m n 0 are dropped when the following word begins
with a consonant. However here too the details have still to be investigated,
as there are instances, when the final nasal is not dropped:
The conditioning factor may be the vowel, the consonant, or the tone.
-284-
XII, Orthography
The following orthographical symbols have been proposed:
Consonants
Phoneme Orthogr. Symbol Phoneme Orthogr. Symbol
p p Ow nw
t t 1 1
kp kp
k k y y
b b
d d Vowels
gb gb
g g u u
ty
dy
kw kw E er
gw gw 3 or
f f a a
dz dz
z z
m m
n n
ny
Vowel Clusters
Vowel clusters which involve the half open vowels er and or are writ-
ten with the "silent letter" r at the end of the cluster to avoid the appear-
ance of the r between vowels, where readers who are familiar with English
may pronounce it instead of recognizing it as a "silent letter" the function of
which is to mark the half-openess of the vowel:
/livei/ liveir 'wing'
Fortisness
This is written by reduplicating the consonant on which it focuses. In
Tone
It will certainly be a help for reading if tone is marked in the ortho-
graphy. The suggested symbols are:
over the V or N for high tone
over the V or N for low tone
no mark for mid tone.
XIII. Text
The following few lines of text are meant as a sample of the language
written in the proposed orthography. The first line is a phonemic transcription,
the second is in the crthography, and the third is a word by word translation.
/ means short pause, // means a more definite pause with difinite drop of voice
before and definite rise of voice after it.
[hw] occurs only before [u] see 41, 64, 80, 91, 152
[h] elsewhere
a
B
-287-
cow ? 4A gvaAm ?
57 see! he he zee ze
64 fly hwuoa hwuoa w610 wuoa
65 walk he he zee ze
XV. Conclusion
More investigation needs to be done on the higher level phonological
phenomena, which will no doubt shed light on the grammatical structure of
the language.
7 .79qpiw
-288 -
FOOTNOTE
1
The final write-up of this material was undertaken at a workshop led by
Dr. K. L. Pike and financed in part by a grant from the United States Office
of Education, under contract 5-14-065, through the Center for Research on Lan-
guage and Language Behavior, of the University of Michigan.
Appendix X
-289-