Abbas 1976
Abbas 1976
a stimulus-response relationship*
PaulJ.Abbas
t andMurray
B.Sachs
Departmentof Biomedical
Engineering,
JohnsHopkinsUniversity
Schoolof Medicine,
Baltimore, Maryland 21205
(Received 16 June 1975; revised16 September1975)
Average
discharge
rateofsingle
auditory-nerve
fibers
in catswasmeasured
in response
to one-andtwo-
tonestimuli.Onecomponent(the"suppressor
tone")of eachtwo-tone
stimuluswasat a frequency
(f2)
whichproduced two-tone
suppression
at some
stimulus
levels.Theothercomponent (excitor
tone)
producedanincrease
in rateabovethespontaneous
ratewhenpresented alone.Fractional
response
was
definedas the drivenrate to the two-tonestimulusdividedby the drivenrate to the excitorpresentedalone.
Fractional
response is thusa quantitative
measureof theamount of suppression
producedbya suppress6r
tone.A number of qualitative
differences
werefoundin thedependenceof fractional
response
forf2> CF
andf• < CF. Forsuppressor tonefrequencies
greater
thanCF,fractionalresponse depends
onlyontheratio
ofsuppressorto excitor levels
(P2/POfora rangeof excitor
levels
(P0. For P• largeenough
to drivea unit
intosaturation,
fractional
responseincreases
with P•. For f2< CF, however,
fractional
responseis a
monotonicdecreasingfunction
of P2and P2/P•;it is alsoa monotonic
decreasing
function
of P• for P2/P•
fixed.Consistentwith theseresultsif the tonelevelratio P2/P• is fixed,rateis a monotonic increasing
functionof the overalllevelof a two-tonestimulusfor f2> CF; for f• < CF, rateis typicallya nonmonotonic
functionof overalllevel.For f2> CF, slopesof (log)fractionalresponse versus(log) P2/P• curvesare a
fore the experiment. Voltage into the earphone could by adaptation effects.
then be set to provide a specified sound-pressure level,
Most of the rate-level plots were taken with level
independent of frequency.
steps of 2 dB; in some cases the steps were 5 dB. In
the following graphs of discharge rate versus level,
B. Experimental paradigm
each point has been replaced by an average of itself
For all results presented here, sound-pressure level and its two neighbors. The first and last points in the
at the eardrum can be expressed as graph are not averaged. Data for each graph were
saved on digital tape and plotted off-line on a Calcomp
p (t) =Pt sin27rftt
+P2sin2•rf•.t. (1) plotter. In plots made in this way, the individual data
points are sometimes connected with straight lines to
The frequency(f•) andamplitudet(P•)are always such form a continuous curve.
that this tone producesan increase in rate (abovethe
spontaneousrate) whenpresentedalone. This tone will It is convenient to consider the results as an exten-
be called the excitor tone. The secondtone (f•.), desig- sion of Sachs' (1969) earlier paper. He expressed dis-
nated the suppressor tone, is always chosen in a fre- charge rate as the sum of a spontaneous
rate (R,p)and
quency range that will produce two-tone supression; a stimulus-driven rate (Rd). The discharge rate to a
i.e., for some values of amplitude P•., the rate to the single-tonestimulus, r(f•, Pt), was expressedas:
two-tone stimulus is less than the rate to the excitor
tone presented alone. Supressor-tone frequencies were r(ft, Pt)=Rd(ft, Pt)+ R,p. . (2)
chosen both above and below CF in each experiment.
The discharge rate to a two-tone stimulus, r(ft, Pt, f•.,
p(t)= P•sin2nf•t * P2sin2nf2t f• = 125 kHz =CF rate was in saturation. Figure 2(b) plots the rate to
f= = 15 5 kHz the CF tone for four different levels; there is little
o Pe=50 dE}SPL change in rate, indicating that the levels are in the sat-
i.o ß
a
45
40
uration region. Fractional response is plotted versus
ß 35 P2/Pt in Fig. 2(a) for four different levels of Pt. The
curves do not superimpose upon one another as for low
-% • 25
levels. For any value of P•./Pt, as Pt increasesthe
-
_ p(t)=P,sin2wf,t +P2sin2wf2t
4/5/73
601 3 82 kHz = CF
5.00 kHz
,o J.0 -
P, dB SPL
ß o ß ß "' "' P,=70dBSPL
FIG. 1. (a) Fractional response plotted (log scale) as a func- o
ß o
tion of P2/P1 (dB)with P1 as a parameter. Fractional response o
rate to the CF tone as a function of P1. Data points are those ß55
ß
50 .
ß 95
rs,o.s
sce,
P2/P• (dB)with P• as a parameter. The suppressor-tonefre-
quency•2) was chosenbelow fiber CF. f• = CF = 12.9 kHz. •
=1.00. • .2-
Fig•e
4shows
•at
•ebeha•or
creases
of
the
fractional
re-
sponsecurvesin the rangeof P[, whereR• •[, P[) in-
is similar to •e beha•or in •e saturation re-
I I
•on. •call thatquitedifferent
behavior
wasobserved .• 5 15 25 55 45
for f2 > CF (Figs. 1 and2) in the dynamicand saturation
P•/P• (dB)
regions. Results similar to those shownin Figs. 3 and
4 were obtained from 40 fibers. 200 -
o•e
We can summarize results presented above as fol- IO0 -
lows: For f•. >CF and at low levels of P•, when Px is in-
creased the level of P2 must be increased proportional- 5O
ly in order to producethe same amountof suppression. 5/17/73
However, when Px reaches levels close to those which 4.02
cause l:td (f•, P•) to saturate, increases in P• require
more than proportional increases in P2 in order to pro-
i i i i
duce the same amountof suppression(i.e., the same IO
65 75 85 95
fractional response). For f2 < CF, when Px is increased P!(dB SPL)
the level P2 must be increasedless thanproportionally
in order to produce the same amount of suppression.
FIG. 4. (a) Fractional response plotted (log scale) as a func-
B. Dependenceof rate on overall stimulus level; tion of P2/P! (dB)with P! level as a parameter. (b) Plot of
driven discharge rate to the CF tone alone as a function of P1..
fl, f2, P2/P1constant Data points are those usedto calculate the correspondingfrac-
Responsesfor bothf•. > CF andf•. < CF were measured tional response curves in (a). P1 levels have been chosento
underthe conditionP2/Px= constant. The stimulusin elicit rates well below and slso at the saturation discharge
this case can be written as rate of the fiber. Note that the levels used in this figure repre-
sent dB attenuation, i.e., dB attenuation from the maximum
p(t)=P(sin2•rf•
t +D sin2•rf2t)
, (5) voltage into the earphone (0 dB attenuation represents approxi-
mately 100 dB SPL at the tympanic membrane). fl = CF = 6.20
whereD= P2/P•. Thetone-amplitude
ratio (D) washeld kHz. f2 = 0.50.
12/27/73
,oo
[/ J iD=25
0 [ •
d8, I
•
I
p(t)
=P.
(sin
2wflt
+D-sin
2rr
f2t) P2/P[=D
200,•• I.Or-
11.2 $0 40 50 60 70 PI=P
/ i
i
I
I
i f,=8.10=CF zoo f•
f,= I1.0
I00 0.511- D=I4dB fz
,, ioo
i
• • io
I • 0
i
i
e D=30d8
i
500
1 D=ISdB
500
I D= 20dS
once more as the rate to the suppressor tone alone in-
creases. At high levels the two-tone rate function is
asymptotic to,that for the suppressor alone. Figures
6 and 7 are typical of results from 30 fibers.
The results of this section can be summarized as fol-
lows: For f,. > CF, P,./P• =D constant,bothtwo-tone
IOO IOO
discharge rate and fractional response are monotonical-
ly increasing functions of Pfor large P. For 3%.
< CF, frac-
tional response is always observed to be monotonically de-
o
I I ,I I
:,, l: :,",
'P
I
o
I I
6'o70' P
I , i
creasing and the two-tone rate functions are typically
nonmonotonic.
55 65 75 85 P• 60 70 80 90 P• C. Dependence
of rate on f2
The effects of changing suppressor-tone frequency
300 300 -
have been examined using the stimulus paradigm
I D=25dB 30 dB
described in Eq. 1. The excitor-tone frequency (f•)
was the CF and its level was held constant.
rate for a two-tone stimulus was measured
Discharge
as a function
•00 fl 200
f==3.80 kHz
• '/ •.""'
ol,;.';'"
, , , D=I3dB
200
49
62 72 82 92 102
59 69
I I
d9 SPL
79 89
I I I
P•'P
ß
IOO
f'•/'•'
p(t):
P.(sin
2•rf,
t+D.
sin2•rf2
P2/R=D .
f, = 4.50 kHz
FIG. 9. (a) Plots of discharge rate versus overall level P D = 25dB
(D=P•./P1 constant);eachgraphrepresentsresponsesfor a
different value of excitor-tone frequency (fl). The abscissae I I , I I I
are the same for all four graphs; the levels of each component
are indi.cated. Rates to the excitor tone alone ("fl") and to the ..
two-tonestimulus ("f! +f•.") are plottedfor valuesoff! chosen --.. 200 [u 1.0- /
below (2.00 kHz), at (2.40 kHz), and above (2.70, 3.00 kHz) o 1•.20
the CF. (b) Plot of fractional response versus overall level
(P) with excitor-tone frequency as a parameter. Data plotted • I ,'?
ß
'J .5 - 2.60I
in (a) were used to calculate the fractional
bols used correspondto the same values of fl as in (a).
response; the sym-
'.',oo
t !; z
_o
•• J,
01 ,[/ f,=2.60
'-'T'"'",, i i i
D. Dependence
of rateon fl dB
SPL
The effects of changing fz were explored with the FIG. 10. (a) Plots of discharge rate versus overall level P
stimulus in the form p(t) = P (sin2•ftt + D sin2•f•.t). Dis- (D=P2/.P{constant);eachgraph representsa different value
charge rate was measured as a function of overall level of excitor-tone frequency (fl). Excitor-tone rate (fl) and two-
P while P•./Pz= D was held constant. Rate-versus-level tonerate (fl +f2) are plottedfor values of j• chosenat (3.20
data were.generated for several different excitor-tone kHz) and below (2.60, 2.40 kHz) the CF. (b) Fractional re-
frequencie s . sponse versus overall level (P) with excitor-tone frequency
(fl) as a parameter. Data in (a) were used to calculate the
We considerfirst the casef•.> CF. Figure 9(a) illus- fractional response. CF = 3.20 kHz. f2 =4. 50.
p(t)= P-( sin2wf,t +D.sin2wfzt ) ing frequency. Results shown in Figs. 11 and 12 are
Pz/P, =D typical of those for 32 fibers studied for this stimulus
condition.
CF = ;5.20 kHz
D=25dB The effects of changingf• for f• < CF have been ex-
,,, I.O- 2.60 amined with the same paradigm. A typical examole is
z shown in Fig. 13. The two-tone and excitor-tone
o stimulus curves are generated for four values off•
n / I (includingthe CF); the sound-levelratio t>•/t>•is fixed
t¾ ..:' at 20 dB in all cases. The values off• have been chosen
JO.õ- X ........ so that there is a significant difference in threshold for
\ ,'....... ...oo kHz:CF responses to the excitor tones presented alone. As in
Figs. 6 and 7, the two-tone rate functions are nonmono-
i--
tonic. Note, however, that for each value of f• the two-
iz ß
tone rate function begins to decrease at approximately
U= I I
the same sound level, P = 50 dB SPL, even though there
o moo 200
are large differences in thresholds to the excitor-tone
DISCHARGE RATE
alone. Indeed, when the fractional response is plotted
{spikes/sec) $/20/74
1.0• as a functionof level I'Fig. 13(b)], the four curves
superimpose. At low levels the fractional response is
FIG. 11. Plot of fractional response versus discharge rate approximately one, indicating that there is little sup-
measure of suppression.
8.10=CF
•./5.50
5.20// _
ß . =
In order to eliminate this effect of saturation at dif-
05
CF
_.1 ,5- .5-
<1: '
ferent levels, we can compare fractional response at z
••.,•8.70
discharge rate to f, alone, r (f•, P,). Except for very o o
low rates where the small number of spikes recorded DISCHARGE RATE DISCHARGE RATE
($pikes/sec): (spikes/sec)
gives rise to large statistical fluctuation, the fractional 12/•7/7• 3/28/74
response shows a clear tendency to increase with de- ii.2 I10
D=15dB f2= I1.0 D=20dB f•,= 750
creasing f• below the CF. Figure 12 illustrates exam-
ples from'several other fibers, for f• values both above FIG. 12. Plots of fractional response versus discharge rate
and below the CF. The fractional response is again to f! alone with exeitor-tone frequency as a parameter. Each
plotted as a function of excitor-tone discharge rate. of the four graphs plots data from a different fiber. The value
The trend is clear for values of f• both above and below off! (in kHz) is indicated adjacent to each curve; values were
the CF; the fractional response increases with decrems- in general chosen both above and below fiber CF.
,oo
r
/'f•=145 p(t)=P'(sin2Trf,t +D-sin2•rf2t)
responseincreaseswith Pt for fixed P•./Pt (Fig. 2).
For f•. < CF, fractional response is not a function of
Pz/P, =D:20dB P•./Pt over any rangeof levels; for fixed P•./Pt, frac-
tional response decreases as Pt increases (Figs. 3 and
4).
:,oo f• = 14kHz=CF
(2) Whencurvesof fractionalresponseversus
are plotted for several values of suppressor-tone fre-
qunecy, f•., the slope of the curves in log-log coordi-
nates is a decreasing function of f•. for f•.> CF (Fig. 8).
• • | ........14.5
For f•. < CF, no change in slope is observed with changes
"'i 14.O
=CF
z 13.0
• o
o
a. 12.0 in f•. (Fig. 8). Slopesfor f•. < CF are greater than those
UJ :•00
//
for f•.> CF; i.e., slope is a decreasing function of
.J ß
/ f• =15.0 for all f•..
I 0
zoo
[ / 1/24/74
3.01
CF = 14.0kHz
response increased at high overall level P.
%.< CF and RD (%.,P•.)= 0, fractional responsewas
alwaysa monotonically
decreasing
functionof P (Fig.
For
,oo
I /f':
la.o f2 = 1.00kHz
6). The two-tone rate functionsfor %.< CF were typical-
ly nonmonotonic, showing a decrease at high levels.
0l/*"i.........
"•.........
• , ,
26 50 40 50 60 P•=P
i i i l (4) The effects of changing excitor-tone frequency
4O5O6O;o ;o P2
were examinedwith P•./Pt constant. For f•.> CF, frac-
dE)
SPL (• tional'response
decreased
(indicating
moresuppression)
FIG. 13. (a) Plots of discharge rate versus overall level P as ft increased with f•. constant (Fig. 11). In constrast,
(D=P2/P 1 constant)with eachgraph representingresponses fractional response was independent of ft for f•. < CF
to a different excitor-tone frequency (fl). The abscissaeare (Fig. 13).
the same in all four graphs, the levels of each of the component
tones are indicated. Excitor-tone alone (fl) and two-tone rate
("fl +f2") are plottedat values of fl chosenbelow (12.0, 13.0 p(t) =P.(sin2wf,t +D.sin2wf=t)
kHz), at (14.0 kHz), and above (14.5 kHz) the CF. (b) Plot of P/P :D
fractional response versus overall level (P) with excitor-tone 1/24/74 2/19/74
frequency as a parameter. Data plotted in (a) were used to 1.04 1.07
0.5i'•"x•,
.....
3.40
ft (Figs. 10-12); as excitor-tone frequncy increases,
the fractional response decreases. ;o 30 40 50 60 p,=P
4;::)
õ• 6•0/0 8•0
III. DISCUSSION
B. Comparison with previousstudies of dischargerate of a tone with increasing level corresponds to the de-
to two-tone stimuli crease in fractional response (increase in suppression)
with suppressor level (our Fig. 1). The attenuating
The observations made here are in general agree-
power of a tone is greater when its frequency is chosen
ment with those reported in previous studies using two-
closer to the CF; in our data, "threshold" for suppres-
tone stimuli. Two-tone suppression was observed in all
sion increases as the suppressor frequency is taken
fibers that were isolated long enough to permit a thor-
farther fromCF (Fig. 8). Rose et al. also observed
oughsearch. Suppressionwas observedfor suppressor that the increase in attenuating power for a given incre-
frequenciesaboveCF for fibers with CF's in the range ment in P•. dependsuponthe frequencyf•., at least for
0.10-29.5 kHz; suppression was seen for suppressor
f•. > CF; this observation probably corresponds to that
frequenciesless than CF for fibers with CF's in the made earlier (Sachs, 1969) and repeated here (Fig. 8)
range 0.42-28.7 kHz.
that the slope of fractional response versus P•. (in log-
Several portions of the study both repeated and ex- log coordinates) is a decreasing function of suppressor
tended earlier observations by Sachs (1969). His ob- frequency f•. for f•. > CF. Finally, Rose et al. consider
servation that the amount of suppression for f•. > CF is two-tone stimuli of the form of Eq. 5, where the tone-
dependent
onthe tone-levelratio (P•./P1)wasconfirmed level ratio is constant and the overall level is increased.
here for excitor-tone levels below saturation; Sachs They state that in such a situation the component which
did not present data for higher excitor-tone levels. Our is less effective at low overall levels dominates the re-
plots of fractional responseversus P•. for different sup- sponse at high levels. Forf•.< CF, our results would
pressor frequenciesalso agreedwith Sachs' earlier seem to be consistent with this statement; in our Fig.
dimensions
dyn/cm•'. In the figures, levels will be usedin- Membrane Motion," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54, 1517-1529.
stead of amplitudes. Levels are given in dB SPL. Legouix, J.P., Remond, M. Co, and Greenbaum, H. B.
Arthur, R. M., Pfeiffer, R. R., and Suga, N. (1971). "Prop- 0.973). "Interference and Two-Tone Inhibition," J. Acoust.
erties of Two-Tone Inhibition in Primary Auditory Neurons," Soc. Am. 53, 409-419.
J. Physiol. (London)212, 593-609. Rhode, W. S., and Robles, L. (1974). "Evidence from
Dallos,-P., and Cheatham, M. A. (1974). "Cochlear Micro- M•ssbauer Experiments for Nonlinear Vibration in the
phonic Correlates of Cubic Difference Tones," in Facts and Cochlea," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 55, 588-597.
Models in Hearing, E. Zwicker and E. Terhardt, Eds. Rose, J. E., Kitzes, L. M., Gibson, M. M., and Hind, J. E.
(Springer-Verlag, New-York). (1974). "Observations on Phase-Sensitive Neurons of Antero-
Goblick, T. J., and Pfeiffer, R. R. (1969). "Time-Domain ventral Cochlear Nucleus of the Cat: Nonlinearity of Coch-
Measurements of Cochlear Nonlinearities Using Combination lear Output," J. Neurophysiol. 37, 218-253.
Click Stimuli," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 46, 924-938. Sachs, M. B. (1969). "Stimulus-Response Relation for Audi-
Goldstein, J. L., and Kiang, N. Y-S. (1968). "Neural Cor- tory-Nerve Fibers: Two-Tone stimuli," J. Acoust. Soc.
relates of Aural CombinationTone 2f1-f2," Proc. IEEE 56, Am. 45, 1025-1036.
981-992. Sachs, M. B., and Abbas, P. J. (1974). "Rate Versus level
Kiang, N. Y-S., Watanabe, T., Thomas, E. C., and Clark, Functions for Auditory-Nerve Fibers in Cats: Tone Burst
L. F. (1965). "Discharge Patterns of Single Fibers in the Stimuli,',' J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 56, 1835-1847.
Cat's Auditory Nerve," MIT Monogr. No. 35. Sachs, M. B., and Kiang, N. Y-S. (1968). "Two-Tone Inhibi-
Kim, D. O., Molnar, C. E., and Pfeiffer, R. R. (1973). "A tion in Auditory-Nerve Fibers," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 43,
System of Nonlinear Differential Equations ModelUng Basilar- 1120-1128.