Gold Et Al-2006-Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
Gold Et Al-2006-Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
Gold Et Al-2006-Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
Library
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
www.cochranelibrary.com
TABLE OF CONTENTS
HEADER......................................................................................................................................................................................................... 1
ABSTRACT..................................................................................................................................................................................................... 1
PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY....................................................................................................................................................................... 2
BACKGROUND.............................................................................................................................................................................................. 3
OBJECTIVES.................................................................................................................................................................................................. 4
METHODS..................................................................................................................................................................................................... 4
RESULTS........................................................................................................................................................................................................ 6
DISCUSSION.................................................................................................................................................................................................. 7
AUTHORS' CONCLUSIONS........................................................................................................................................................................... 8
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS................................................................................................................................................................................ 9
REFERENCES................................................................................................................................................................................................ 10
CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES.................................................................................................................................................................. 14
DATA AND ANALYSES.................................................................................................................................................................................... 18
Analysis 1.1. Comparison 1 Music therapy vs. "placebo" therapy, Outcome 1 Communicative skills: gestural.............................. 18
Analysis 1.2. Comparison 1 Music therapy vs. "placebo" therapy, Outcome 2 Communicative skills: verbal................................. 18
Analysis 1.3. Comparison 1 Music therapy vs. "placebo" therapy, Outcome 3 Behavioural problems (end of therapy)................. 19
Analysis 1.4. Comparison 1 Music therapy vs. "placebo" therapy, Outcome 4 Behavioural problems (using all measurements 19
from 2nd day on)..................................................................................................................................................................................
WHAT'S NEW................................................................................................................................................................................................. 19
HISTORY........................................................................................................................................................................................................ 19
CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS................................................................................................................................................................... 20
DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST..................................................................................................................................................................... 20
SOURCES OF SUPPORT............................................................................................................................................................................... 20
INDEX TERMS............................................................................................................................................................................................... 20
[Intervention Review]
1Grieg Academy Music Therapy Research Centre (GAMUT), Unifob Health, Bergen, Norway. 2Institute of Music and Music Therapy,
University of Aalborg, Aalborg, Denmark. 3Grieg Academy Department of Music, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
Contact address: Christian Gold, Grieg Academy Music Therapy Research Centre (GAMUT), Unifob Health, Lars Hilles gate 3, Bergen,
5015, Norway. christian.gold@grieg.uib.no.
Citation: Gold C, Wigram T, Elefant C. Music therapy for autistic spectrum disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2006, Issue
2. Art. No.: CD004381. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD004381.pub2.
Copyright © 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
ABSTRACT
Background
The central impairments of people with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) include social interaction and communication. Music therapy
uses music and its elements to enable communication and expression, thus attempting to address some of the core problems of people
with ASD.
Objectives
To review the effects of music therapy for individuals with autistic spectrum disorders.
Search methods
The following databases were searched: CENTRAL, 2005, (Issue 3); Medline, (1966 to July 2004); Embase, (1980 to July 2004); LILACS, (1982
to July 2004); PsycINFO, (1872 to July 2004); CINAHL, (1982 to July 2004); ERIC, (1966 to July 2004); ASSIA, (1987 to July 2004); Sociofile,
(1963 to July 2004); Dissertation Abstracts International, (late 1960's to July 2004). These searches were supplemented by searching specific
sources for music therapy literature and manual searches of reference lists. Personal contacts to some investigators were made.
Selection criteria
All randomised controlled trials or controlled clinical trials comparing music therapy or music therapy added to standard care to "placebo"
therapy, no treatment or standard care.
Main results
Three small studies were included (total n = 24). These examined the short-term effect of brief music therapy interventions (daily sessions
over one week) for autistic children. Music therapy was superior to "placebo" therapy with respect to verbal and gestural communicative
skills (verbal: 2 RCTs, n = 20, SMD 0.36 CI 0.15 to 0.57; gestural: 2 RCTs, n = 20, SMD 0.50 CI 0.22 to 0.79). Effects on behavioural problems
were not significant.
Authors' conclusions
The included studies were of limited applicability to clinical practice. However, the findings indicate that music therapy may help children
with autistic spectrum disorder to improve their communicative skills. More research is needed to examine whether the effects of music
therapy are enduring, and to investigate the effects of music therapy in typical clinical practice.
People with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have difficulties with communication, behaviour and/or social interaction. Music therapy
uses music and its elements to enable people to communicate and to express their feelings. In this way music therapy addresses some
of the core problems of people with ASD. This review set out to assess the evidence for the effectiveness of music therapy for individuals
with ASD.
Three small studies were included which examined the short-term effect of brief music therapy interventions for autistic children. Music
therapy was superior to "placebo" therapy with respect to verbal and gestural communicative skills, but it was uncertain whether there was
an effect on behavioural outcomes. The included studies were encouraging, but of limited applicability to clinical practice. More research
with better design, using larger samples, in more typical clinical settings is needed to strengthen the clinical applicability of the results and
to examine how enduring the effects of music therapy are. When applying the results of this review to practice, it is important to note that
the application of music therapy requires specialised academic and clinical training.
Data collection and analysis 3.2 Loss to follow-up and intention to treat analysis
The included studies had complete data for all participants,
1. Selection of trials and therefore an intention-to-treat analysis was straightforward.
Two reviewers independently inspected the titles and abstracts (If studies with drop-outs had been included, we would have
identified from the search. Potentially relevant papers were examined the impact of drop-out rates using sensitivity analyses
obtained. Any disagreement was resolved through discussion and/ and imputing the negative outcome as appropriate.)
or consultation with a third reviewer.
4. Data analysis
2. Assessment of quality Data from washout periods in crossover studies were excluded
The authors assessed methodological quality independently, and from the analysis.
any disagreements were resolved by discussion. Assessment was
made of all included studies, to consider the following questions: 4.1 Binary data
Was the assignment to treatment groups truly random? No binary data were available from the included studies. We had
Was allocation adequately concealed? planned to use risk ratio and number needed to treat statistics with
How complete was follow up? 95% confidence intervals for binary outcomes.
How were the outcomes considered for people who withdrew?
Were those assessing outcomes blind to the treatment allocation? 4.2 Continuous data
For studies where outcomes were measured at several
Randomisation occasions during each treatment condition, we used only the last
Randomisation was judged as 'adequate' when computer- measurement at the end of treatment.
generated random numbers, a random numbers table, or coin- Where raw data were available, the distributions of values were
tossing were used to allocate participants to treatment conditions. visually checked in an attempt to detect skewness. Where skewness
Randomised as well as quasi-randomised trials were included in was found, we attempted to remove it by log-transformation. We
the review, as noted above. then examined how log-transformation influenced the effect size
estimate and used the more conservative estimate. The alternative
Allocation concealment of using change data which tend to be less skewed was not
The trials were divided into quality categories as defined in the considered because using change data may be less conservative
Cochrane Reviewers' Handbook (Alderson 2004), where: than using endpoint data.
We used standardised mean differences (SMD) for the analysis of
A: adequate allocation concealment; participants and researchers all continuous outcomes. When combining different scales for the
were unaware of participants' future allocation to condition until same outcome, it was necessary to standardise the effects in order
after decisions about eligibility were made and informed consent to make them comparable. When combining results on the same
was obtained; scale, either non-standardised weighted mean differences (WMDs)
or SMDs could have been used. We decided to use SMDs even in this
B: unclear concealment; allocation concealment measures were
case in order to facilitate the interpretation of effect sizes as small,
not described in detail;
medium or large according to the guidelines that are commonly
C: inadequate allocation concealment; allocation was not used in the behavioural sciences (Cohen 1988). It is noted that the
concealed from either participants before informed consent or choice of SMD versus WMD does not usually affect the significance
from researchers before decisions about inclusion were level of the results, and the authors cautiously assessed whether
made (this will always be the case for quasi-randomised studies). such was the case.
As this review aimed to include randomised and quasi-randomised
All SMDs (regardless of whether the study was a parallel or
studies, all three categories were eligible for inclusion. The rating
a crossover design) were standardised by the pooled standard
was only used as a descriptive measure of study quality.
deviation between participants, rather than the standard deviation
of the difference within participants. This is the standard procedure
Blindness of assessors
which enables comparisons of different scales and facilitates
It is not possible to blind either those who deliver music therapy or
interpretation of the magnitude of effects (Cohen 1988, Gold 2004).
those who receive it to the nature of the intervention. Blindness of
The calculation of the standard error then depended on the study
assessors, however, can be used and was assessed as a part of this
design. For parallel designs the standard error was calculated
review. Quality of blindness was determined primarily by whether
using the standard formulae for SMDs as implemented in RevMan
those who assessed and coded outcome measures were blind to
and described in the RevMan handbook. For crossover studies
condition, and the quality of blindness was categorised where:
we took into account the correlations within the participants as
Adequate = assessor blind to condition; recommended and described in the literature on meta-analysis of
Unclear = blinding of assessor not reported and information not crossover studies (Elbourne 2002).
available from researchers; and
4.3 Meta-Analysis
Inadequate = assessor not blind to condition.
A fixed effect model was used in all analyses. Fixed effect models
All of the above were included in the review.
have the advantage of being simpler and more easily interpretable.
3. Data management If a common effect size had not been tenable due to heterogeneity,
3.1 Data extraction a random effects model would have been considered. In addition
This was performed independently by two reviewers (CG, TW). to the fixed effect analyses, the authors also examined whether
When necessary the reviewers contacted the authors of trials to random effects analyses would have altered the results, and any
provide missing data. such difference would have been reported.
Music therapy for autistic spectrum disorder (Review) 5
Copyright © 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Cochrane Trusted evidence.
Informed decisions.
Library Better health. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
The length of all included studies was extremely short, varying from
5. Assessing heterogeneity one (Farmer 2003) to four weeks (Brownell 2002). The duration of
Consistency of results was assessed visually in the forest plot each treatment condition was only one week in each study. No later
and by examining I2 (Higgins 2002), a quantity which describes follow-up assessments were included in any of the studies.
approximately the proportion of variation in point estimates
that is due to heterogeneity rather than sampling error. We 4.2 Participants
supplemented this with a test of homogeneity to determine the The participants in the included studies were between 2 and 9
strength of evidence that the heterogeneity is genuine. In case of years of age, with 80-100% boys in each study. All participants had
heterogeneity, possible sources would have been investigated. received a diagnosis of autism. In one study (Buday 1995), levels
of retardation and severity of autistic traits were also specified.
6. Assessing bias Participants in this study ranged from mildly to severely mentally
Funnel plots were planned to be used to investigate any retarded (according to DSM III-R) and from mildly to moderately
relationship between effect size and study precision, but were not autistic (according to the Childhood Autism Rating Scale, CARS).
used in this review due to the small number of included studies.
4.3 Setting
7. Sensitivity analyses The participants received therapy either at home, at school, or at
Sensitivity analysis were planned to be conducted to determine the an outpatient therapy centre.
impact of study quality on outcome if studies of different quality
had been identified and included (for example studies with high 4.4 Study size
attrition rates). All three studies had extremely small sample sizes, varying from
four to ten participants per study. Crossover designs were used
8. Subgroup analyses in two of the studies (Brownell 2002 and Buday 1995) to partly
The impact of clients' age and intensity of therapy (number and compensate for the small sample size.
frequency of music therapy sessions) would have been examined in
subgroup analyses had heterogeneity been found. 4.5 Interventions
elements similar to the music therapy conditions, including some 3. Loss to follow-up
potentially active ingredients (but none that are specific to music No dropouts were reported in any of the studies.
therapy). The concept of a "placebo" therapy designed to control
for the therapist's attention has been widely used in psychotherapy 4. Overall
research (Kendall 2004), but has also been criticised (Lambert 2004;
4.1 Performance bias
see the discussion section of this review).
Medication levels were not monitored in the included studies.
4.5.3 Other conditions However, due to the short duration of the studies, it appears
One study (Brownell 2002) also reported the outcomes during a unlikely to assume significant differential change in medication
baseline and a washout period with no intervention. These data might distort the results. All participants received the full therapy
were not used in this review. intervention as intended.
When applying the results of this review to practice, it is important had very small sample sizes and included no discussion of test
to note that the application of music therapy requires an academic power, possibly indicating that the issue has been ignored (see
and clinical training in music therapy. Trained music therapists Gold 2004 for a discussion of the issue of test power in music
are available in many countries. Training courses in music therapy therapy research). Limited sample size is common in research
teach not only the clinical music therapy techniques as described on ASD, and crossover designs seem an obvious choice because
in the background of this review, but also aim at developing the they help to improve test power. However, crossover designs
therapist's personality and clinical sensitivity, which is necessary entail other problems. First, longer-term follow-up assessments
to apply music therapy responsibly. Academic training courses in are not possible with such designs. Second, crossover designs
music therapy exist in many countries, and information is usually are only adequate for interventions whose effects are thought to
available through the professional associations. be short-acting (Elbourne 2002), but for music therapy it is not
unreasonable to assume lasting effects if children learn new modes
Implications for research of behaviour as a result of therapy. In this case, crossover designs
would be inappropriate due to carryover effects which would lead
Relation of studies to clinical practice
to the effect being underestimated. Large parallel group trials
More research is needed to examine the effects of music
are necessary to resolve this dilemma. An informed discussion is
therapy over a longer term. Future studies should involve
needed about what designs are considered appropriate in research
therapy conditions that are close to clinical practice, especially in
on music therapy for ASD.
terms of frequency, duration, and therapy techniques. This may
include further efficacy studies as well as effectiveness studies.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Standardised and published tools should be used to evaluate the
outcome of music therapy. We would like to thank Jane Dennis and Jo Abbott of the Cochrane
Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group for
Sample size, power, and the choice of research design their help throughout the process, and Lisa Tjosvold of the
Future research on music therapy for people with ASD will need to Cochrane Child Health Field for her help in retrieving additional
pay close attention to sample size and power. All included studies unpublished studies.
REFERENCES
References to studies included in this review Bruscia 1982 {published data only}
Brownell 2002 {published data only} Bruscia KE. Music in the assessment and treatment of echolalia.
Music Therapy 1982;2(1):25-41.
Brownell MD. Musically adapted social stories to modify
behaviors in students with autism: four case studies. Journal of Carroll 1983 {published data only}
Music Therapy 2002;39(2):117-144. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 280]
Carroll JG. The use of musical verbal stimuli in teaching low-
Buday 1995 {published data only} functioning autistic children (doctoral thesis). University of
Mississippi, 1983.
Buday EM. The effects of signed and spoken words taught with
music on sign and speech imitation by children with autism.. Chilcote-Doner 1982 {published data only}
Journal of Music Therapy 1995;32(3):189-202. [MEDLINE: PBS
Chilcote-Doner SE. The effect of contingent vs. non-contingent
Record: 1220; 0022-2917]
presentation of rhythmic asynchonous stimulation on the
Farmer 2003 {published data only} stereotyped behavior of children with autism (doctoral thesis).
Vanderbilt University, 1982.
Farmer KJ. The effect of music vs. nonmusic paired with
gestures on spontaneous verbal and nonverbal communication Clauss 1994 {published data only}
skills of children with autism between the ages 1-5 (Master's
Clauss EL. Effects of music on attention and self-stimulatory
thesis). Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University (School of
behaviors in autistic people (doctoral thesis). Hofstra University,
Music), 2003.
1994. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 1340; 0419-4217]
Blackstock 1978 {published data only} Edelson 1999 {published data only}
Blackstock EG. Cerebral asymmetry and the development Edelson SM, Arin D, Bauman M, Lukas SE, Rudy JH, Sholar M,
of early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Rimland B. Auditory integration training: A double-blind study
Schizophrenia 1978;8(3):339-353. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 2190; of behavioral and electrophysiological effects in people with
0021-9185] autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities
1999;14(2):73-81. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 880; 1088-3576]
Bonnel 2003 {published data only}
Bonnel A, Mottron L, Peretz I, Trudel M, Gallun E, Bonnel AM. Edgerton 1994 {published data only}
Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: a Edgerton CL. The effect of improvisational music therapy on
signal detection analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience the communicative behaviors of autistic children. Journal of
2003;15(2):226-35. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 270] Music Therapy 1994;31(1):31-62. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 1420;
0022-2917]
Brown 1994 {published data only}
Brown SM. Autism and music therapy - is change possible, and Frissell 2001 {published data only}
why music?. Journal of British Music Therapy 1994;8:15-25. Frissell JK. Musical preferences and reproduction abilities
in children with autism. Dissertation AbstractsInternational:
Brown 2003 {published data only} Section B: The Sciences and Engineering 2001;62(3-B):1574.
Brown WA, Cammuso K, Sachs H, Winklosky B, Mullane J, [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 760; 0419-4217]
Bernier R, Svenson S, Arin D, Rosen-Sheidley B, Folstein SE.
Autism-related language, personality, and cognition in people Goldstein 1964 {published data only}
with absolute pitch: results of a preliminary study. Journal of Goldstein C. Music and creative arts therapy for an autistic child.
Autism & Developmental Disorders 2003;33(2):163-7; discussion Journal of Music Therapy 1964;1(4):135-138. [MEDLINE: PBS
169. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 250] Record: 2460; 0022-2917]
Therapy 1973;10(4):184-188. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 2360; characteristics and other related pervasive developmental
0022-2917] disorders (master's thesis). Tallahassee: Florida State University,
1997.
Schmidt 1976 {published data only}
Schmidt DC, Franklin R, Edwards JS. Reinforcement of O'Dell 1998 {published data only}
autistic children's responses to music. Psychological Reports O'Dell AW. Effects of paired auditory and deep pressure
1976;39(2):571-577. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 2290; 0033-2941] stimulation on the stereotypical behaviors of children with
autism (master's thesis). Tallahassee: Florida State University,
Starr 1998 {published data only} 1998.
Starr E, Zenker K. Understanding autism in the context of music
therapy: Bridging theory and practice. Canadian Journal of Wood 1991 {published data only}
Music Therapy 1998;6(1):1-19. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 970; Wood SR. A study of the effects of music on attending behavior
1199-1054] of children with autistic-like syndrome (master's thesis). San
Jose State University, 1991.
Staum 1984 {published data only}
Staum MJ, Flowers PJ. The use of simulated training and music
lessons in teaching appropriate shopping skills to an autistic References to ongoing studies
child. Music Therapy Perspectives 1984;1(3):14-17. Kim 2005 {unpublished data only}
Stevens 1969 {published data only} * Kim, J. Joint attention and attunement in improvisational
music therapy with the autistic child. Aalborg University 2005.
Stevens E, Clark F. Music therapy in the treatment of autistic
children. Journal of Music Therapy 1969;6(4):98-104. [MEDLINE:
PBS Record: 2430; 0022-2917] Additional references
Thaut 1987 {published data only} Alderson 2004
Thaut MH. Visual versus auditory (musical) stimulus preferences Alderson P, Green S, Higgins JPT. Cochrane Reviewers'
in autistic children: a pilot study. Journal of Autism and Handbook 4.2.2 [updated December 2003]. The Cochrane
Developmental Disorders 1987;17(3):425-32. [MEDLINE: PBS Library. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, 2004.
Record: 390]
Aldridge 2002
Thaut 1988 {published data only} Aldridge D, Fachner J. Music Therapy World Info-CD ROM IV.
Thaut MH. Measuring musical responsiveness in autistic Witten-Herdecke: University Witten-Herdecke, 2002.
children: a comparative analysis of improvised musical
tone sequences of autistic, normal, and mentally retarded Alvin 1991
individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Alvin J, Warwick A. Music therapy for the autistic child. 2nd
1988;18(4):561-71. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 380] Edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1991.
Toolan 1994 {published data only} AMTA 1999
Toolan PG, Coleman SY. Music therapy, a description of American Music Therapy Association. Music therapy research:
process: Engagement and avoidance in five people with Quantitative and qualitative foundations CD-ROM I. 1964-1998.
learning disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research Silver Spring, MD: American Music Therapy Association, 1999.
1994;38(4):433-444. [MEDLINE: PBS Record: 1400; 0964-2633]
APA 1994
Watson 1979 {published data only}
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical
Watson D. Music as reinforcement in increasing spontaneous Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). 4th Edition. Washington,
speech among autistic children. Missouri Journal of Research in DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
Music Education 1979;4(3):8-20.
APA 2000
Wimpory 1995 {published data only}
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical
Wimpory D, Chadwick P, Nash S. Brief report: Musical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). 4th Edition.
interaction therapy for children with autism: An evaluative Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
case study with two-year follow-up. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders 1995;25(5):541-552. [MEDLINE: PBS Asperger 1979
Record: 1200; 0162-3257] Asperger H. Problems of infantile autism. Communication
1979;13(3):45-52.
References to studies awaiting assessment Ball 2004
Laird 1997 {published data only} Ball CM. Music therapy for children with autistic spectrum
Laird PD. The effect of music on cognitive/communicative disorder. Bazian Ltd 2004, issue November 11.
skills with students diagnosed with autism, autistic like
Wigram 2006 developmental disorders. 2nd Edition. New York: John Wiley,
Wigram T, Gold C. Music therapy in the assessment and 1997:148-172.
treatment of autistic spectrum disorder: Clinical application
Wing 2002
and research evidence. Child: Care, Health and Development In
press. Wing L, Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Gould J, Larcombe M. The
diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders:
Wing 1997 background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. Journal of
Wing L. Syndromes of autism and atypical development. In: Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2002;43:307-325.
Cohen D, Volkmar F editor(s). Handook of autism and pervasive
* Indicates the major publication for the study
CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES
Brownell 2002
Methods Allocation: quasi-randomised, possibly randomised ('counterbalanced')
Blindness: independent assessor (teacher), blinding not reported.
Duration: 4 weeks.
Design: crossover.
Interventions 1. structured receptive MT (songs with social stories). 5 individual daily sessions. N=4.
2. structured receptive "story therapy" (reading of social stories). 5 individual daily sessions. N=4.
3. no intervention. 2 x 5 days. N=4.
Outcomes Repetitive behaviours outside therapy sessions (in classroom). Inter-rater reliability 0.86 to 0.94
Notes
Risk of bias
Buday 1995
Methods Allocation: randomised.
Blindness: assessor blinded to the nature of the hypothesis and to treatment condition.
Duration: 2 weeks.
Design: crossover.
Interventions 1. structured receptive MT (songs used to teach signs). 5 individual sessions. N=10.
Outcomes Imitating behaviour in sessions (rating of a video recording with sound turned off to ensure blinding of
raters; inter-rater agreement 98%).
a) sign imitation
b) speech imitation
Notes
Risk of bias
Farmer 2003
Methods Allocation: randomised.
Blindness: not known.
Duration: 5 days.
Design: parallel group.
Interventions 1. MT sessions (combined active and receptive: guitar playing, songs. N=5
2. Placebo (no music) sessions. N=5.
Mostly individual sessions of 20 minutes
Notes
Risk of bias
Griggs 1997 not RCT/CCT (case study), not intervention study (assessment)
Hadsell 1988 not RCT/CCT (case series), not ASD (Rett syndrome)
Krauss 1982 not RCT/CCT (case series), not ASD (apraxia, language delay)
O'Loughlin 2000 not RCT/CCT - includes 3 case series where all received the same treatment (no. 1, 3, 4) and 1 case
series with an ABA design (no. 2).
Kim 2005
Trial name or title Joint attention and attunement in improvisational music therapy with the autistic child
Methods
Notes
1 Communicative skills: gestural 2 SMD (Fixed, 95% CI) 0.50 [0.22, 0.79]
2 Communicative skills: verbal 2 SMD (Fixed, 95% CI) 0.36 [0.15, 0.57]
3 Behavioural problems (end of therapy) 1 SMD (Fixed, 95% CI) -0.14 [-0.42, 0.14]
4 Behavioural problems (using all measure- 1 SMD (Fixed, 95% CI) -0.24 [-0.45, -0.03]
ments from 2nd day on)
Analysis 1.1. Comparison 1 Music therapy vs. "placebo" therapy, Outcome 1 Communicative skills: gestural.
Study or subgroup Treatment Control SMD Std. Mean Difference Weight Std. Mean Difference
N N (SE) IV, Fixed, 95% CI IV, Fixed, 95% CI
Buday 1995 10 10 0.5 (0.15) 95.77% 0.48[0.18,0.77]
Farmer 2003 5 5 1.2 (0.716) 4.23% 1.17[-0.24,2.57]
Analysis 1.2. Comparison 1 Music therapy vs. "placebo" therapy, Outcome 2 Communicative skills: verbal.
Study or subgroup Treatment Control SMD Std. Mean Difference Weight Std. Mean Difference
N N (SE) IV, Fixed, 95% CI IV, Fixed, 95% CI
Buday 1995 10 10 0.3 (0.11) 97.42% 0.35[0.13,0.56]
Farmer 2003 5 5 0.8 (0.674) 2.58% 0.81[-0.51,2.13]
Study or subgroup Treatment Control SMD Std. Mean Difference Weight Std. Mean Difference
N N (SE) IV, Fixed, 95% CI IV, Fixed, 95% CI
Heterogeneity: Tau2=0; Chi2=0.45, df=1(P=0.5); I2=0%
Test for overall effect: Z=3.32(P=0)
WHAT'S NEW
HISTORY
Protocol first published: Issue 3, 2003
Review first published: Issue 2, 2006
CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS
CG - designed the protocol, co-ordinated the reviewing, searched for studies, extracted and analysed data, and wrote the report.
TW - extracted and analysed data, helped with writing the protocol and the report.
CE - helped with data extraction and analysis and with writing the report.
DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST
The authors of this review are clinically trained music therapists.
SOURCES OF SUPPORT
Internal sources
• Sogn og Fjordane University College, Norway.
• Aalborg University, Denmark.
External sources
• The Research Council of Norway, Norway.
INDEX TERMS