How Digital Technology Shapes Us
How Digital Technology Shapes Us
How Digital Technology Shapes Us
Phone: 1-800-832-2412
www.thegreatcourses.com
LEADERSHIP
PAUL SUIJK President & CEO
BRUCE G. WILLIS Chief Financial Officer
JOSEPH PECKL SVP, Marketing
JASON SMIGEL VP, Product Development
CALE PRITCHETT VP, Marketing
MARK LEONARD VP, Technology Services
DEBRA STORMS VP, General Counsel
KEVIN MANZEL Sr. Director, Content Development
ANDREAS BURGSTALLER Sr. Director, Brand Marketing & Innovation
KEVIN BARNHILL Director of Creative
GAIL GLEESON Director, Business Operations & Planning
PRODUCTION TEAM
ALI FELIX Producer
JAMES BLANDFORD Post-Production Producer
GINA DALFONZO Content Developers
BRANDON HOPKINS
SAM BARDLEY Associate Producer
TRISA BARNHILL Graphic Artists
PETER DW YER
OWEN YOUNG Managing Editor
MILES MCNAMEE Sr. Editor
MAHER AHMED Editor
CHARLES GRAHAM Assistant Editor
CHRIS HOOTH Audio Engineer
RICK FLOWE Camera Operators
VALERIE WELCH
JIM M. ALLEN Production Assistants
RICK FLOWE
ROBERTO DE MORAES Director
PUBLICATIONS TEAM
FARHAD HOSSAIN Publications Manager
BLAKELY SWAIN Sr. Copywriter
RHOCHELLE MUNSAYAC Graphic Designer
JESSICA MULLINS Proofreader
ERIK A ROBERTS Publications Assistant
ELIZABETH BURNS Fact-Checker
WILLIAM DOMANSKI Transcript Editor
i
Professor Biography
Professor Viskontas has published more than 50 original papers and book
chapters related to the neural basis of memory, reasoning, and creativity in
top scientific journals, such as American Scientist, Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, The Journal of Neuroscience, Neuropsychologia, Current
Opinion in Neurology, and Nature Clinical Practice. Her work was featured
in Oliver Sacks’s book Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain as well
as in other publications, such as Nautilus, Nature, and Discover. She is a
sought-after science communicator who cocreated and hosts the popular
science podcast Inquiring Minds, which has been downloaded more than 8
million times. Professor Viskontas cohosted the 6-episode docuseries Miracle
Detectives on the Oprah Winfrey Network and has appeared on The Oprah
Winfrey Show, PBS NewsHour, NPR’s City Arts & Lectures, the TED Radio
Hour, and CBC Radio’s Sunday Edition. She also regularly gives keynote talks
for conferences and organizations as diverse as Ogilvy & Mather, Genentech,
TEDx, and the Dallas Symphony Orchestra.
ii
Table of Contents
Introduction
Professor Biography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i
Course Scope . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Lessons
1 How Experience Alters the Brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
iii
Table of Contents
Supplementary Material
Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 212
iv
How Digital
Technology
Shapes Us
T his course explores the myriad ways that our interactions with technology
are shaping or have the potential to shape our thoughts, feelings, and
social interactions. The course starts with topics directly related to how we
think, including whether our attention spans are dwindling, how reading on
screens is different from reading a physical book, and what the accessibility of
information is doing to our memory.
Next, you’ll learn about how current technologies are limiting our options
and pushing us toward similar content rather than encouraging us to explore
uncharted waters. You’ll discover what virtual reality can tell us about
ourselves and how screen time is affecting our children. And you’ll consider
the controversial question of whether playing violent video games leads to
aggressive behavior and what effect technology is having on our ability to
sleep. But sleep isn’t the only aspect of our health that might be affected by
technology; you’ll also examine how the internet is changing our relationships
with our physicians and therapists.
Then, the course will move away from a focus on our own selves and toward
a view of how technology is changing how we as a society behave. You’ll
consider the implications of selling our data to large tech companies, which
many of us do every day as we use “free” social media apps, among other
tools. You’ll also be prompted to think deeply about the issues of privacy that
these acts bring up.
1
Course Scope
The course will then move more directly to how technology is shaping our
relationships, from online dating to pornography and our political groupings.
You’ll consider how smart computers—artificial intelligence—might make
us more creative. Finally, you’ll look to the future, examining how emergent
technologies like blockchain, self-driving cars, and other innovations are likely
to affect how we spend our time and even what it will ultimately mean to be
human in the digital age.
2
ts
en
How
nt
o
Table of C
1
Experience
to
o
Click to g
Alters the
Brain
3
Lesson 1 How Experience Alters the Brain
● But we can change that activity, and even the very anatomy of the brain,
by our behavior—by our experience.
4
● Does technology shape how we think? Like the brain itself, the answer to
this question is complicated. Our brain changes our behavior—but our
behavior can also change our brain. The question is no longer whether
technology is shaping our minds, but how. And that depends on how we
use the technology.
5
Lesson 1 How Experience Alters the Brain
● This type of signaling can change how a cell expresses its genes. In your
gut, signaling between bacteria can lead them to work as a group—a
collective—rather than as individuals. Under the right circumstances,
bacteria in your gut express genes that make them stick to the mucus
lining more easily, helping them proliferate and protecting you from other,
less desirable microorganisms.
● And how easily these signals are sent or received, and what effect they have
on downstream cells, changes with use. Imagine a front door lock. When
you first get a new key, it’s often harder to open the door; the key gets a little
stuck and you need to wear it down a bit until it can slide into the lock more
smoothly. In your brain, a new signal is also harder for a cell to accept, and
with repeated signaling, the cascade of voltage changes becomes smoother.
In other words, cells that fire together wire together, to use an axiom that
summarizes Donald Hebb’s influential theory of neural networks.
● But imagine now that you’ve used that lock-and-key combination tens of
thousands of times. Eventually, the key or the locking mechanism gets
worn down. It doesn’t work as well. Now you have to add some jiggling or
force. Eventually, cells habituate to stimulation and are less sensitive to it.
6
● There are mammals who can grow new neurons in adulthood, in regions
of the brain that drive new learning. Whether adult humans grow new
neurons is still a scientific puzzle, but if we do grow new neurons, one
thing is clear: In order to keep those baby cells, you need to learn a new
skill. This is because neurons in the brain must communicate with each
other; socially isolated cells die more quickly, just like their human hosts.
And learning new skills forges new connections, incorporating the new
cells into the already-active mind.
● While the brain is just a collection of cells, albeit a highly organized one,
the mind emerges from the activity of those cells—what they do. We’re
still in the early stages of deciphering exactly how neuronal and other brain
cell activity enables thinking, but what we do know is that we can see
traces of experience and behavior at many different levels of analysis, from
the number and kind of receptors on a cell’s surface to the oscillations of
millions of cells across the brain.
● For example, experience can change the firing rates of cells: how quickly
and in what kind of pattern they send out their electrical messages. It can
also change the synchronicity of activity across millions of cells, what
we call brain waves. It can change which brain regions show a surge in
activity, what many functional MRI studies show, using blood flow as a
proxy for measuring brain activity.
● The results from these studies are often depicted in the media as images
of a brain lighting up in certain places, which is not an entirely accurate
representation, since most of these studies are showing where activity is
greater or more intense, not where it is present or absent. The truth is that
even when you’re just lying in the scanner, ostensibly doing nothing, your
brain is working—maybe thinking about your to-do list or feeling slightly
anxious about all the loud noises. And as long as you’re alive, your brain
is active.
● Experience can even change how these brain regions interact with each
other when we’re really not doing anything specific—what we call the
7
Lesson 1 How Experience Alters the Brain
default mode network, which has become a hot area of study as we’ve
realized that lying in the scanner, waiting to do something, is a fertile time
for thinking and therefore brain activity.
● That isn’t to say, though, that our brains are limitless in their ability to
adapt or change. There are strict biological limitations to neuroplasticity—
the term used to describe anatomical changes that happen with experience.
That’s why it’s harder to learn a new language, and to think in it, when
you’re 25 years old compared to when you’re 5. Neuroplasticity slows down
as our brains fully develop, and that’s good news, since we want to retain
what we’ve worked hard to learn.
● But our access to new experiences also slows down. When you’re 5, every
day you have the potential to discover something new: to taste a new food,
play a new game, feel a new emotion, or connect with a new person. Can
8
you say the same when you’re 55? Our lives become more routinized as we
get older, and many of us become less interested in, or even annoyed by,
new experiences. We gravitate toward the familiar, so our brains have fewer
opportunities to change.
● But then comes a revolutionary device like the smartphone. With the
internet and millions of apps at our fingertips, we no longer have to endure
long hours of waiting with no entertainment. It also means that we carry
temptation around with us all day long. Since the ways in which we use
our brains on a day-to-day basis are changing, our brains must change,
too. That’s both a blessing and a curse. The fact that our species has been
able to colonize much of this world, and even survive off of it, speaks to
the adaptability of our brains.
Questions to Consider
9
Are New
2
Media
Shortening
Attention
Spans?
10
T echnological advances have brought to our fingertips
an infinite number of ways to distract ourselves, and
what we pay attention to shapes how we live our lives.
But we often sacrifice our attention in exchange for
seemingly free entertainment in the form of social media,
streaming videos, and internet articles. Is technology
eroding our ability to focus? And if so, can we retrain our
brains to think deeply, or is there some bigger benefit to
skimming the surface?
11
Lesson 2 Are New Media Shortening Attention Spans?
● There’s also no guarantee that your brain is tracking all the important
things that you think it should. We do often miss the forest for the trees.
Just because you’re reading an impressive tome doesn’t mean that your
brain is doing the work necessary to store or synthesize that information
for the long term.
● Here’s where digital media comes into the picture. We often feel as though
we’re making deliberate decisions in terms of what we’re focusing on, but
we’ve also allowed technology companies to buy our attention. Instead of
paying for many apps and media services, we let them sell our data to show
us advertisements or influence us in subtle ways, capitalizing on the fact that
our attention is easily shifted. And many tech companies have shifted their
business models accordingly—creating content that is designed to keep us
scrolling or skimming, rather than losing ourselves in deep thought.
12
● Sustained attention is critical for what professor and author Cal Newport
calls deep work: the ability to focus without distraction on a cognitively
demanding task. And we needed this new term for it because our modern
lives have obliterated this ability for most of us.
● But what if this isn’t in our hands anymore? What if our brains are being
shaped by the internet and other technology to shift focus rather than
sustain it? What if it’s becoming harder and harder for us to control our
attention and easier and easier for tech companies to manipulate it? Are we
selling off too much of ourselves for the fleeting pleasures of social media
and streaming entertainment? The evidence that this is the case is mounting.
● Your brain is a creature of habit; how you think shapes how your brain
works. So if you spend a lot of time jumping from one website or idea to
the next, the pathways that enable this type of thinking will strengthen—
at the expense of the pathways that you’re no longer using as much, such as
those required for deep reading comprehension or sustained attention.
● What are we actually doing when we flit from one web link to the next? To
a large extent, we’re not directing our focus; we’re letting it be led by what
we’re exposed to. So it becomes harder and harder to keep it on track—
harder to return focus to the task at hand.
● And this exacerbates a problem that we face every minute of every day:
How can we navigate a world of such unfathomable complexity? We think
of ourselves as being fairly good observers of reality: We can see, hear,
smell, touch, taste, and consider our environment. But each of these senses,
and the cognitive skills we need to interpret them, represents just a sliver of
the available information.
● Our brains work hard to fill in the gaps, making sense of the impoverished
input and alerting our consciousness to what’s important. But we still need
to decide what to pay attention to, because even if our senses can register
the information, our brains can’t handle it all. Just think about what
happens when 2 people are trying to talk to you at the same time!
13
Lesson 2 Are New Media Shortening Attention Spans?
● The bottom line is that we are limited in terms of how many things we can
consciously attend to at the same time. And it takes work to keep focus
when our brain is busy tracking the environment for salient information.
When that environment includes the rapid-fire, eye candy–laden world of
digital media, paying attention for extended periods of time becomes next
to impossible.
Cognitive Multitasking
14
● We can’t consciously do 2 cognitively demanding tasks at the same time.
Instead, we quickly switch our attention between tasks, sometimes so
quickly we barely notice it. But this switching comes at a cost. It takes time
and energy—cognitive resources—to switch tasks.
● There are 2 costs, actually. One is the time it takes to switch, which can be
short or long, depending on how similar and how demanding the tasks are.
The other is the challenge of inhibiting your brain from continuing work
on the previous task, keeping intrusive thoughts at bay.
● Think about what happens when you check your email before delving
into a more cognitively demanding task, such as reading a technically
difficult article. If one of the emails you respond to requires some conflict
resolution or some real decision-making, it’s likely that you’ll find yourself
ruminating about the problem during a lull in your reading, instead of
synthesizing what you’re reading.
● These kinds of intrusive thoughts prevent you from devoting all of your
cognitive resources to the new task at hand. Now multiply this situation by
the hundreds of times you task-switch while using digital media and these
intrusions add up to a significant amount of lost time. Put another way, the
amount of time you spend focusing deeply on the difficult task becomes
astonishingly small, preventing you from making much headway.
● Most of us don’t realize what the effects of choosing how much time we
spend on different tasks might be on our ability to get things done. There’s
evidence that we tend to linger on the more-immediately-rewarding task,
such as checking email or online shopping, and to switch tasks only once
15
Lesson 2 Are New Media Shortening Attention Spans?
● And this is exactly what seems to be happening as we spend more and more
time on the internet or with digital media: We are getting more efficient
at scanning and skimming information but less skilled at reflecting on it,
thinking critically, and developing original lines of thought.
● One might argue, though, that this adaptation is inevitable and desirable, given
where society is headed: toward more and more screen time and information
overload. And you could say that people who multitask more often get better at
it, reducing switch costs and becoming more efficient thinkers.
● But evidence is mounting that that’s not necessarily the case. In a 2009
study from Stanford University, Anthony Wagner and his colleagues found
that heavy media multitaskers were actually more distractible—they had
more trouble ignoring irrelevant environmental distractors and intrusive
thoughts—compared to light media multitaskers. And they actually
performed less well on a task-switching test. It’s not entirely fair, though, to
take this one study and extrapolate the results across all of humanity.
● Our ability to focus attention changes across our life spans, too. Contrary
to popular belief, we are actually more distractible as we age. Our brains
slow down; the speed with which we can process thoughts diminishes.
The older we get, the longer we take to work through a problem. We make
more errors and fall prey to more distractions.
● Part of this age effect might be attenuated by the fact that the older we
are, the less time we presumably spend on digital media. This means
that this age effect might very well become more pronounced with every
generation, as younger people today are more likely to report multitasking
16
than previous generations.* And it’s not just kids in the US; it’s a worldwide
problem. But is it a problem? Or is it just a shift in how young people
spend their time?
● Media multitasking isn’t going away anytime soon. And it does seem to be
affecting how deeply we process information. We tend to allocate partial
attention to most tasks, instead of giving them our full attention for hours
at a time.
Questions to Consider
1 What is attention?
17
Does the
3
Internet Make
Us Shallow
Thinkers?
18
H as your brain already begun showing the effects
of long-term digital media use on your ability to
read? That’s a tough question to answer, given all the
unknowns. But we can use reading as an example of how
we’re often not very good at judging our own mental
strengths and weaknesses and how we develop complex
skills that become automatic.
How We Read
Not all reading is created equal, and how we develop the skill
can have lifelong implications that spill over to many different
aspects of our mental lives.
● Humans have only been reading for about 6000 years, a time period that
represents barely an eyeblink in the history of the world and the path of our
evolution. And in that short time, we have repurposed parts of our brains
to enable reading, which in turn supports other fundamentally human
cognitive abilities, such as critical thinking, imagination, and even empathy.
● We do have specific reading circuits in the brain, but they are developed
through learning and not written into our genetic code the way that other
aspects of language are. Because reading circuits are created as we learn to read,
how and what we learn to read will shape the way they are ultimately formed.
19
Lesson 3 Does the Internet Make Us Shallow Thinkers?
● We tend to think that everyone reads just like we do, but that’s not the
case. The more Proust we read, the easier it becomes. That’s because the
reading circuits are being optimized for long sentences and flowery prose.
Ultimately, our reading circuitry can encompass vast swaths of the brain,
involving cells in all the layers of our cortex, across both hemispheres
and in each of the 4 lobes—but only if we spend the time necessary to
adequately build and train these networks. Once they become entrenched,
it’s hard to turn them off; we automatically read text and it feels effortless.
However, if we only ever read comic books or short internet articles or
children’s books, then we won’t develop the networks necessary for long-
form reading.
● Deep reading can give everyone that gift, but it’s not the only way.
Immersive digital experiences, such as virtual reality, can arguably be an
even more powerful conduit to recreating yourself. But in virtual reality,
20
so much of the imagery is dictated by the programmers. During deep
reading, you’re as much a creator of that world as the author is. Film and
TV, being passive media, are not as effective at teaching us empathy as
media in which we are active participants.
● Digital media has the power to connect us, but the current trend is to
reinforce tribalism rather than to expand the tribe. In 2010, Sara Konrath
and her colleagues published a meta-analysis of studies of empathy
in college students over 4 decades, from 1979 to 2009. They found a
consistent decline in empathic concern and perspective taking—40%!—
with the steepest drop happening since 2000.
● We don’t know what is causing the decline, though mobile phones are likely
at least partly to blame. But what if another factor is shrinking the amount
of time that kids spend lost in novels, or worse, an inability to read deeply?
21
Lesson 3 Does the Internet Make Us Shallow Thinkers?
● There is some evidence that the neural networks involved in deep reading
overlap with those that support empathy. Specifically, after about age 3
or 4, most humans have developed a theory of mind, with which they
perceive, understand, and consider the thoughts, feelings, and beliefs of
others—a necessary step toward feeling empathy. A person’s theory of
mind continues to develop throughout his or her life span, with some
people becoming quite exceptional at reading another person’s thoughts by
observing his or her behavior.
● The neural networks thought to underlie this ability include large regions
connected via the insula and cingulate cortex, where information from
different senses and association areas in the brain converge. There are large
cells here called von Economo neurons.* The size of these cells allows them
to transfer messages across large brain areas very quickly.
● Reading isn’t just about recognizing letters and words and stringing them
into meaningful ideas; it’s about predicting and anticipating what will
come next, something the human brain has been shaped by evolution to
become fairly proficient at.
* These neurons are seen in other animals who are highly social and who have
big brains, such as dolphins and elephants.
22
● Reading takes advantage of the brain’s ability to work in parallel: We can
consider what’s on the page—which constitutes bottom-up influences
on our thinking—while at the same time using our prior knowledge to
interpret what we’re seeing—so-called top-down influences.
● It’s the top-down processes that give deep reading its magic and that allow
us to find multiple meanings in the same words. But sifting through such
various meanings takes time, and much of digital media is designed to save
us time, encouraging us to speed through the reading, rather than linger
and let it blossom.
Digital Reading
● Some of the problems with early versions of e-readers have been almost
solved, with technology now available that reduces the eyestrain caused by
most screens. And they can be a boon for people with disabilities because
font sizes and contrast are easily adjusted.
● But there is another, perhaps more nefarious change that is building as the
e-book grows its market share: E-readers socialize reading, with their note-
taking, highlighting, and hyperlink functions. With these distractions, our
eyes can become restless; constant interruptions leave us craving more and
make it harder to lose ourselves in the content. And while we can ignore
the functions on our Kindles if we choose to, their mere presence is still
shifting our relationship with other readers, authors, and the text itself.
23
Lesson 3 Does the Internet Make Us Shallow Thinkers?
* No Friend but the Mountains won the Victorian Prize for Literature in 2019,
Australia’s highest-paying literary award.
24
They found that the more the families used the internet, the less they
communicated with each other and the lonelier and more depressed they
felt. Kraut dubbed this effect the internet paradox, since the internet was
invented to connect us, yet it seems more effective at isolating us.
● Then, in 2002, Kraut published a follow-up study with the same families,
plus a few more, who now had used the internet at home for about 3
years. It turns out that the people recognized the ill effects of internet use
and changed their habits. Now internet use was associated with better
communication and improved social lives. We don’t just passively accept
tools that make us miserable; we adapt our use.
The physical book is not quite dead yet. In fact, small bookstores
are on the rise.
Questions to Consider
25
Outsourcing
4
Our Memory
26
H uman memory is not, nor has it ever been, about the
past. It’s not stable, but rather biological. And like any
part of a biological organism, memory is differentiated
from nonbiological things by its ability to change—or,
more to the point, its inability to resist change.
Types of Memory
● Memory is not just one function. There are many ways that our
experiences can change us, which arguably is the broadest definition of
memory there is: how our past can affect our current behavior.
● At the molecular level, we can find traces of learning in the ways that our
brain cells send and receive signals. If you poke a sea slug, it will retract
its siphon, but if you keep poking it, eventually it will ignore you. And its
nervous system will have changed; it will have habituated to the stimulation.
● Your brain does this, too. When you move into a new home, you can often
spend a few restless nights woken up by the various new sounds your home
emits. But eventually, you tune them out. That’s a very basic form
of memory.
● Then there are more complex habits that we form, such as brushing
our teeth in the same pattern each day. These, too, leave a mark on our
nervous systems, as we automate sequences of actions through repetition.
In some ways, this kind of habit learning is more stable than our conscious
memories, because they are built into our automatic, inflexible, and slow-
learning brain system.
● But the memories that we wish would stay intact are the kind that we can
summon consciously from the deep recesses of our minds, such as the way
your grandmother smiled when she saw you. It’s these memories, though,
that are also the most dynamic.
27
Lesson 4 Outsourcing Our Memory
● When we evaluate our memory, it’s the ability to remember details of past
events or facts we’ve learned that we generally use as a measuring stick.
But this type of declarative remembering is only one part of the multiple
memory systems and processes of which our brains are capable. And
remembering is the last step in a long line of
memory-related activities.
● And given the ease with which we can often find the answer with a quick
Google search, we can’t help but compare our shortcomings with the
superior abilities of even the simplest computer. Of course, our ability to
28
retrieve information pales in comparison with what a search algorithm can
do. And since we’re smart and adaptable creatures, we’ve been outsourcing
this kind of remembering more and more.
● What’s more, if we think that we’ll always have access to the information,
we tend to remember where to find it rather than what it is. If we’re told
that the information we’re learning will be stored in a file on a computer,
we’re worse at remembering that information than if we think it will be
29
Lesson 4 Outsourcing Our Memory
● While this seems logical, this study yielded all kinds of alarming headlines,
suggesting that computer use and the internet were eroding our ability
to remember things. But instead of signaling the end of human memory,
this preliminary work underlined how our brains are adapting, arguably
appropriately, to the ubiquitous presence of the internet in our lives.
● Digital security companies like Kaspersky will point out that even in 2015,
people in the US could not remember the telephone numbers of their
loved ones or other personal information that used to be easily accessible
to them.
● Does this mean that the very ability to remember is fading or that we are
replacing the content of memory, moving from remembering information
to remembering how to find it? It’s the latter that the Google effect seems
to suggest: We are adapting to the new technologically rich environment,
limiting the expenditure of cognitive resources on things that we can
access anywhere, such as phone numbers, and saving these resources for
search strategies.
30
● Encoding is the first step in creating a memory,* but what items get included
depends on what we were paying attention to. In addition, emotion can
modulate memory by increasing the likelihood that we store experiences
that move us. And the internet encourages shallow thinking, or attending to
superficial features rather than taking a deep dive into the material.
● The internet discourages us from thinking deeply about what we’re reading
there, and as a result, our memory for what we do on the internet is
handicapped from the beginning.
● Given the adaptive nature of the brain and the pace at which technology
use is shifting, things are not as simple as they might seem. In 2018, the
Google effect could not be replicated in a large-scale effort to replicate
previously published psychological findings. Some researchers are
suggesting that we no longer see a decrease in memory retention for things
we think will be erased because we’ve become aware of the fact that erasing
something from the internet is remarkably difficult. So have our brains
already adapted?
* For facts and events, encoding is largely driven by a part of the brain called
the hippocampus, which gets information from all of our senses, creates a mini
index of co-occurring details, and makes associations between items that make
up the memory. Then, when it’s time to remember, the hippocampus drives
cortical reinstatement, essentially reactivating the brain networks that were
active when we first laid down the memory.
31
Lesson 4 Outsourcing Our Memory
● Our memory for the kinds of things that we store on the internet has never
been great. If we just stick to memory for facts and events—declarative
memory—which lends itself to conscious remembering, or explicit
processing, as opposed to implicit processing, then we can diagnose
memory failures in a number of ways, include encoding, storage, and
retrieval failures.
● And the internet has its own memory flaws. Information found there is
rewritten all the time. It can have multiple authors, and anyone can be an
author. There are also many distracting elements, in the form of hyperlinks
or ads.
● But in terms of a pure storage device, the internet easily has us beat. So
aren’t we better off outsourcing our memory storage to this nearly infinite
repository? There’s some evidence that when we save some information on
our computer, we’re more likely to remember what we’re about to work on
or learn.
32
● What we don’t always realize, though, is how our thought processes can
be shaped by habits.* The Google habit can have unintended consequences
on our repository of information. After all, testing yourself by practicing
retrieving information is a powerful form of improving your memory, by
both strengthening the ability to access that information and highlighting
what you don’t know.
● When it comes to what we don’t know, what we are often unaware of is how
much of our autobiography we rewrite, favoring some details over others,
which then become lost for good. For example, we tend to think that we
remember many details of salient life events, sometimes called flashbulb
memories,** such as when you first heard about 9/11. These events are often
negative, because emotions like fear are powerful memory enhancers.
In that group, they googled the easy answer 83% of the time. But in 2 other
groups, who either had to answer the hard questions from memory or weren’t
asked the hard questions at all, the googling rate was only about 63% to 65%.
This tendency to google was maintained even when checking the internet
required subjects to get up and cross the room to use a computer!
33
Lesson 4 Outsourcing Our Memory
● That’s true even for our most cherished memories. The fact that we usually
don’t get the details right doesn’t really matter in the grand scheme of
things. Whether the memory is positive or negative, what we remember
is how we felt, who treated us well, or what we should fear. This is useful
information when we’re trying to imagine what might happen when we
find ourselves again in similar circumstances.
34
explore and share our imagination. Depending on what we input, we
can construct models that give us clues to different futures, and these
in turn let us predict possible consequences with even more accuracy
than simply thinking about them.
Questions to Consider
35
Human
5
versus Digital
Content
Curators
36
W hat are the implications if we outsource all our
curation, of everything from videos to groceries
to friends, to the internet? How do recommendations
made by humans compare with those suggested by a
computer algorithm? And what are the consequences of
digital curation, if any, for how we think?
● Throughout the 1990s and early 2000s, Blockbuster video was slowly
eating up mom-and-pop video stores, making deals with studios to get
exclusive rights for new releases and managing huge inventories. They were
also notorious for their late fees, which they charged each day the rented
video was returned to the store late.
● Much of Netflix’s success was enhancing what small video stores used
to do so well: recommendations. The Netflix model was based on the
37
Lesson 5 Human versus Digital Content Curators
● By 2006, Netflix had realized that streaming video was a real threat
and that their recommendation system might be valuable enough to
retain customers as DVD rentals declined. So they announced a public
competition* to improve their algorithm. Their proprietary system, called
Cinematch, used prior ratings to predict which films their customers
might enjoy. And it was far from perfect, but it was still pretty good. The
challenge was to beat Cinematch by at least 10%.
● It’s worth understanding both the power and the problem facing algorithm
developers in this challenge, because a similar set of circumstances
is found in most digital curation domains, from Amazon’s shopping
recommendations to Google Scholar.
* Netflix provided access to a training data set that included more than 100
million ratings from almost 500,000 users about more than 17,000 movies.
38
recommendations really well. It took until 2017 for Vidiots to shutter their
flagship store, which was 7 years after Blockbuster went bankrupt.*
● Vidiots employees loved movies, and they passionately kept track of their
inventory. Their curatorial skills are what kept bringing customers back,
even when it was much more convenient to order a film on Netflix. These
employees could match tone and thematic content, along with other
factors, across different films—a difficult task for an algorithm.
● And while Netflix has gathered much more data, it’s still hard for artificial
intelligence to pull thematic content from films and TV shows in the same
way that a human can. But no human can watch and remember all the
50,000 films that Vidiots houses or the thousands of titles on Netflix.
* Vidiots has since reopened as a nonprofit, with the goal of bringing video
store culture deeper into the 21st century by serving as expert archivists.
39
Lesson 5 Human versus Digital Content Curators
Downton Abbey, or you might hate adult cartoons, taking away 1.9 points
from Rick and Morty.
● Think about what your goals are when you’re searching for something
online, whether it’s a product or information. Let’s say that you’re looking
for a coffee shop with Wi-Fi in a city you’re visiting. You type in “coffee
shop” and “Wi-Fi” into your search engine and assume that it will return
all the options nearby. In this example, unless you’re in San Francisco or
Taiwan, you’ll probably get a few hits, but the list won’t be overwhelming.
So you want it to be exhaustive.
40
● Now imagine you’re looking for a common object, such as a coffee mug.
The options are seemingly endless, so you don’t want to scroll through
thousands of items. You want a recommendation for the top 3 to 5 mugs,
and that’s enough choice.*
● The internet makes choice overload a real problem in many domains, from
how to spend your time to whom to date. If you come into a decision with a
clear view of what your perfect choice would be, and there’s a choice that is
clearly more in line with this vision than the others, and you’re pretty sure
that you know the set of available options, you can avoid choice overload.
● If you know that you want a mug that can travel without spilling the
coffee, can hold heat all day, is tall and thin rather than squat and wide,
and is navy blue, it’s pretty easy to make your choice and feel good about
it. But if you don’t have those preferences, you find yourself scrolling
through thousands of options, with different price points and different
features, and you no longer know whether size trumps insulation or vice
versa. A recommendation engine is what you need here.
● But the consequence is that these types of engines help the rich get richer
and don’t do what most of us think they do: introduce us to novel products
that we might not otherwise come across that are a good fit for our needs
or wants.
* Our brains function best when we have to choose between just a handful
of options. Otherwise, we get overwhelmed and end up less satisfied with
whatever choice we ultimately make.
41
Lesson 5 Human versus Digital Content Curators
● And it was also annoying the users. The algorithm would queue up videos
that were similar in some ways to the previously viewed video but not
similar to what the users were looking for. So the engineers revamped
the system to encourage longer viewing rather than just clicks. Instead of
measuring success by clicks, the engineers realized that if viewers stayed
on a video longer, that meant they enjoyed it and the recommendation had
been more successful.
42
● But watch-time optimization can lead to users going down potentially
dangerous rabbit holes. Human beings are naturally more likely to pay
attention to confirmatory, reinforcing, or positive feedback. This means
that the content they are more likely to engage with will confirm their
existing beliefs and will attract like-minded individuals, which sometimes
leads to a radicalization of views. It’s also easier to program an algorithm to
match similar content, because the set of options is limited, while there is
an infinite variety of conflicting or different content.
● For example, when positive posts in a user’s News Feed were reduced, the
user was more likely to include negative emotional words in subsequent
posts. When negative expressions were reduced, the user was more likely
to use positive statements. These results showed that emotions can spread
across massive online communities, with even small nudges by a digital
curation algorithm.
43
Lesson 5 Human versus Digital Content Curators
Digital curation can shape not only our minds, but our society
as well, as it can push us toward one emotional state or
another. In the face of this evidence, researchers are calling for
the inclusion of socially conscious rules that promote equality
and prosocial behaviors in algorithms.
Questions to Consider
44
Virtual
6
Realities and
Our Sense
of Self
45
Lesson 6 Virtual Realities and Our Sense of Self
● There is one simple starting point for the forces driving the evolution
of the nervous system: An organism needs to sense some part of its
surroundings in order to choose an action. Our brains, when distilled
down to their essence, sense things and then execute actions accordingly.
So our experience of our world—how we sense our surroundings and
translate that into perception—is a fundamental feature of our nervous
system and therefore of ourselves.
● So how does all of this play out in relation to virtual reality—a new form
of experience that is becoming increasingly common as the technology
becomes better and cheaper?
● If our experience of our current situation depends on both our past and
our current state, then spending a lot of time in a virtual environment will
change how we experience things in the future. Our brains are biological
organs, with adaptability being arguably their biggest strength. Experience
engenders change not only in how our brains are activated, but even in
46
their structural anatomy, if the experience is powerful enough or lasts for a
long time.
● Mary is a color scientist who is forced to live and work from the confines
of a black-and-white room. She only has access to the outside world via
a black-and-white TV set. But she has all the information known to us
about color, from the visible light spectrum to the 3 cones in our retinas
that have differential sensitivities to different wavelengths of light. In
other words, she knows everything there is to know concerning how our
brains turn light into color. But she has never actually seen color. What
will happen when she is released from her room? Will her experience—her
qualia of color—add anything to her knowledge?
● Most people will argue that without direct experience, her knowledge is
incomplete. There is something about qualia that defines our conscious
experience.
● At the very core of our identity lies our own sense of our physical bodies.
Anyone with a disfiguring injury will attest to the psychological pain that
accompanies unwanted changes to our bodies. And our sense of who we
are is remarkably malleable.
47
Lesson 6 Virtual Realities and Our Sense of Self
● When you’re first learning to drive a car, you can be a bit clumsy, not
entirely sure where the vehicle begins and ends. With time, you learn the
intricacies of the vehicle—its quirks and limitations as well as what it’s
capable of—so much so that if someone bumps into your car, you feel as
though you’ve been hit directly. Your proprioception, the sense of where
your body is in space, extends to include the boundaries of the car.*
● If you lose a limb, your sense of that limb’s position in space sometimes
paradoxically remains. Patients with phantom limbs, as they’re called, can
experience significant distress, as the body part that’s missing still hurts.
● While the actual causes of the pain remain controversial, one relatively
successful therapeutic technique shines a light on the relationship between
our sense of self, our experiences, and our proprioception.
● This is an example of how malleable our qualia are and how our sense of
self is tied to them.
* This is also true of other tools, such as a well-used tennis racket or skis.
48
The Science of Self-Awareness
● The anterior insular cortex (AIC), or insula, is the part of the brain that
receives input from all of our senses and is a candidate for the repository of
all subjective feelings.
● There are even specialized cells in your AIC called von Economo neurons,
which are long, thin cells that are found in some large animals, mainly in
species that are thought to have rich social lives and who have passed the
mirror test* of self-awareness.
● The AIC integrates information across modalities and has these special von
Economo cells that seem to play a role in our sense of self. But what this
work tells us is that our self-awareness and our identities are closely tied to
our physical bodies, our sensations, our qualia.
Mental Models
49
Lesson 6 Virtual Realities and Our Sense of Self
● Are out-of-body experiences proof that the mind transcends the physical
body? Probably not. When you’re coming out of sleep or anesthesia, which
is when the vast majority of out-of-body experiences happen, you might
not realize that you’re floating around your mental map of the scene rather
than actually floating around the room.
50
sense of self—of where we are in space, of our own consciousness—can be
manipulated by brain stimulation, by circumstances like waking up from
anesthesia, and even by immersive experiences.
● And this brings us back to virtual reality. What virtual reality promises is
an extension of our physical bodies into a digital space. Virtual reality is a
much more immersive experience than what we are used to from TV, video
games, and other media.
● Virtual reality puts you entirely into a virtual world, which may or may not
look or feel anything like the world you know. In fact, the more immersive
the experience, the less real it needs to be to capture our attention.
51
Lesson 6 Virtual Realities and Our Sense of Self
of their bodies—making them feel as though they were leaving their own
bodies in the digital realm, a bit like a virtual out-of-body experience.
● Say that instead of leaving your body, you get the sense of possessing a
different body—that of a child or someone of a different gender or race.
The illusion can be incredibly powerful. Just like out-of-body experiences,
virtual immersion can profoundly change one’s sense of self.
52
off, but we can’t know for sure. We assume we’re always conscious, but the
truth is that when we query our consciousness, we change it.
● Virtual reality, however, can help us see our mental models, including
those of the self, by the ease and methods by which they are changed.
Questions to Consider
53
Screen Time’s
7
Impact on
Kids
54
O ne of the main arguments against screen time for
kids is that it turns them into passive viewers rather
than engaged, active participants in an activity. We know
that an enriched environment can foster development,
even in terms of neuroanatomy.* The question is, then,
do screens enrich or impoverish a child’s environment?
Can screens themselves be detrimental? How does a
screen fare in comparison with live interactions?
● The problem is that whether they are watching TV shows or engaging with
their devices, many kids spend more time in front of a screen than they do
* A rat raised in a cage with other rats and some toys, such as a running wheel,
will show bushier neurons—those capable of making many more connections—
than a rat raised alone or in an impoverished environment.
55
Lesson 7 Screen Time’s Impact on Kids
interacting with their parents. And when screen time is more rewarding
that face-to-face interactions, we can see why they make these choices.
● The main tenets of the plan include waiting until at least 18 months of age
to introduce any screen time. Then, until the age of 2, toddlers should only
be exposed to educational media programs and only as their caregivers
interact with them and help them understand what they are seeing.
● Then, between the ages of 2 and 5, kids shouldn’t have more than an hour a
day of screen time, and it’s best if parents or caregivers watch with them. After
that, screen time should not interfere with other beneficial activities, such as
playing outside, sleeping, or eating. And parents should designate screen-free
times, such as dinner, and screen-free environments, such as the bedroom.
● One of the reasons why the AAP suggests no screen time for babies and
toddlers up to about 18 months is because kids that young don’t seem
to understand what they are seeing, even if it’s pretty close to what
they would experience in real life. For example, watching a video of
someone talking to the infant has little to no effect on his or her language
development. But interacting with a live person has a big effect and is
critical for the baby to learn to speak.
● Research has shown only negative effects on language and executive function
development in babies under 2 with exposure to TV. That’s thought to be
largely because most of the content is programmed for adults, and because
kids this young don’t really understand what they are seeing, any TV is
treated pretty much as background noise by their little brains.
56
working to process it. What’s more, interactions between parent and child
are much richer when there is no TV on in the background, since the
parent is more engaged with and attentive to the child.
● Both in terms of the quality and the number of words spoken, talking
by the parent is more effective as a learning tool when it’s not set against
the backdrop of a TV. But when parents and infants or toddlers watch
age-appropriate shows together, the quality of the vocabulary spoken after
co-viewing actually increases.
● While the overall time during which kids use media hasn’t changed much,
remaining steady at a little more than 2 hours a day, the proportion of
screen time that kids aged 0 to 8 spend on a mobile device increased from
4% in 2011 to 35% only 6 years later. The actual number of minutes on
these devices has increased almost tenfold, from 5 minutes in 2011 to 48
minutes in 2017.
● So while kids’ total passive viewing time might not have dramatically
increased, kids are spending much more of that time looking at handheld
devices. This shift has implications both in terms of the physical
differences of the viewing experience (for example, a screen held closer to
the eyes might affect the development of the visual system) and in terms of
content, since users of handheld devices have more control over the media,
are able to switch between videos and games, don’t have to sit through
57
Lesson 7 Screen Time’s Impact on Kids
long periods of ads, etc. This means that they might find the experience
more compelling or more rewarding, making it more difficult to put the
screen away.
58
Negative Consequences of Media Use
● But it turns out that the cause of obesity in heavy screen users is not
necessarily because turning off screens leads to more physical activity.
Studies measuring physical activity using accelerometers have found little
to no change in activity with media-use reduction.
● These studies have found, though, that media-use reduction leads to the
consumption of fewer calories. Kids who spend more time in front of
screens also eat fewer fruits and vegetables and more energy-dense snacks.
Eating while watching TV or videos on a tablet is common in many
households and may be partially responsible for the increased incidence of
childhood obesity.
● Two other factors might account for the causal relationship between
increases in body mass index and media use: food advertising, which leads
kids to make poor dietary choices; and sleep deprivation, which changes
their appetite-regulating hormone levels and leaves them craving less-
nutritious foods.
59
Lesson 7 Screen Time’s Impact on Kids
● Increased risk for anxiety and depression has also been attributed to
heavier media use in children.* However, this is just a correlation and
therefore does not necessarily mean that the increased rates were caused by
the media use.
● At the same time, digital media can actually be used to help kids with
anxiety and depression. It can also be used to help bullying victims find
support. Social media can spread word of support groups, help lines, chat
forums and other resources designed to help kids who are being bullied. So
if they do find themselves on the receiving end of abusive behavior online,
chances are that they will stumble across or easily find help.
Media Multitasking
● The key is to moderate and examine kids’ digital media use, but far too
often, parents, teachers, and caregivers don’t pay attention to usage. But
in addition to monitoring the amount and quality of screen time, parents
and caregivers should also be made aware that digital media lends itself to
multitasking, and many young people, just like their parents, try to do more
than one thing at a time: watching TV while doing homework, for example.
● But humans are not built to multitask in the truest sense of the word.
We can’t focus our full attention on many activities at once. Instead, we
* Kids are very prone to making social comparisons, and modern digital media
abound with images and videos of attractive, wealthy, successful people and
can engender negative feelings when a child compares his or her lot with theirs.
Social media apps exacerbate this effect, as kids can more directly compare
themselves in terms of numbers of likes, friends, and other characteristics.
60
quickly switch focus between tasks, and we pay a cost. It can be pretty
minimal, just a matter of shifting attention, or it can be detrimental if we
can’t suppress intrusive thoughts generated by the previous activity.
● Doing homework while engaging with digital media also makes the
homework take longer. Checking social media or text messaging more
often is correlated with a lower grade point average.
● Perhaps more troubling is evidence that people who multitask with media
often show some personality traits that might make it difficult for them to
succeed. They tend to be more impulsive, seek sensation or take risks, and
suffer from social anxiety and depression.
● They are also less likely to endorse a growth mindset; that is, they are more
likely to report believing that traits like intelligence or mathematical ability
are fixed and can’t be improved by trying harder or working more. This
consequence, if indeed it holds true, could be detrimental to academic and
other measures of success.
A Silver Lining
* The majority of research suggests that people who multitask a lot using media
devices are either no better at multitasking or maybe even a bit worse at it than
people who are less prone to subjecting themselves to more than one medium
at a time.
61
Lesson 7 Screen Time’s Impact on Kids
Questions to Consider
62
Video Games
8
and Violence
63
Lesson 8 Video Games and Violence
Sometimes, in the same breath that they claim violent games are
of no concern, game proponents argue that games can even make
people better. And indeed, there is some evidence that prosocial
games can lead to more prosocial behavior.
● Among decades of social science research, they cited 5 studies that they
concluded show that exposure to violent media increases the incidence
of aggressive behavior and angry thoughts and feelings, decreases helpful
behavior, and increases physiological arousal. Four out of 5 of the studies
were coauthored by Craig Anderson, a longtime researcher of violent
video games, and one of his coauthors, Brad Bushman, has also published
extensively on the topic.
64
aggression-related variables, while prosocial video games had the opposite
effect. Their meta-analysis found these effects to be reliable whether the
studies were experimental, correlational, or longitudinal.
65
Lesson 8 Video Games and Violence
Bushman, and colleagues. Ferguson has argued that since the 1950s, the
correlation between media violence and homicides has broken down,
and while video games have continued to obsess America’s youth, rates of
violence are at 40-year lows.
● Why might one set of authors find such reliable results when another
group fails to find them? This discrepancy can happen for a number of
reasons, including the sensitivity and type of social outcome measures and
the control group.
● If you compare kids playing video games with kids doing nothing, you’ll
find an effect. That’s called a passive control group. The best control group
might be one in which the kids either watch the game but don’t play it or
play a nonviolent video game. And in those cases, the effect is often still
there. But if you have a nonviolent video game that is very frustrating,
leaving the players angry, you might find that they’re just as aggressive as
kids playing a violent video game.
● So it’s not always the content of the game that we need to consider, but
how it makes the children feel. And whether it’s good or bad, there’s plenty
of evidence that video game playing manipulates kids’ emotions.
66
● It’s no wonder, then, that the question of whether exposure to violent
video games leads inevitably to, or even nudges people toward, violent acts
remains open.
● In the 2015 report, the APA describes an additional 170 studies published
since 2009 that attempted to address this question. Taking these new
research findings into account, the task force concluded that the link
between aggressive behavior and violent video game exposure is not only well
established but also well studied, with 14 studies reporting significant effects.
● The most basic and perhaps oldest theory of how exposure influences
behavior is based on a series of studies conducted in the 1960s by social
psychologist Albert Bandura, widely considered one of the most influential
and greatest psychologists of all time. Bandura’s experiments were designed
to test a theory that kids learn behaviors through observation and imitation.
67
Lesson 8 Video Games and Violence
his social learning theory, which suggests that people can learn through
observation and imitation, even without direct reinforcement.
● He pointed out that learning occurs in a social context and that the more
we relate to the teacher, the more easily we learn. In particular, Bandura
proposed this theory to account for the fact that humans and other animals
can produce new behaviors seemingly spontaneously, without having to go
through a careful and slow process of shaping and reinforcement.
● One of the arguments against the idea that playing violent video games can
directly cause us to behave more violently highlights the fact that watching
someone do something in person is much more powerful than watching
it on TV. The screen tells us that what we’re watching is not real. But can
kids make the same distinction?
● But there are a few more theoretical frameworks that could explain how
playing violent video games might lead to more frequent aggressive acts,
even in older kids and adults.
68
● The next one to come on the scene chronologically was the excitation-
transfer theory, proposed by Dolf Zillmann in 1983. Zillmann was
building his theory on the assumption that emotions are accompanied
by physiological arousal, or excitation, but that the arousal is nonspecific.
In other words, you might find that your heart is racing in a number of
different situations: when you’re angry because someone cut you off or
because your favorite character in a TV show was just killed; or when
you’ve just come back from a run.
● In each case, you have the same physiological response: a racing heart. But
the causes differ: One is because something made you angry while the
other was because you were exerting yourself physically. And in the case
of anger, we can further separate the causes: because you were cut off or
because your favorite character was killed. Sometimes the physiological
reaction lasts longer than the emotion, such that your heart might still be
racing even though you don’t really feel angry anymore.
● Zillmann noted that this residual excitation, or arousal, can then influence
your behavior, and you might misattribute this change because you
didn’t realize that you were still in an excited state. There’s even a clichéd
metaphor to describe this bleeding of stress from one situation into
another: kicking the cat, or the dog.
● So maybe watching violent video games entails the danger of getting you
into an excited state, which, when paired with an activity in which you
might become aggressive makes you more likely to metaphorically kick the
cat. And there have been studies where such behaviors have been observed.
● But Zillmann noted that the effects of arousal dissipate quickly. So maybe
the long-term effect of playing violent video games on behavior is pretty
negligible. That’s indeed an argument that is often levied against those
who caution us about the negative effects of video game playing. And to be
fair, the effect sizes in many of the studies are fairly small.
69
Lesson 8 Video Games and Violence
● The thinking is that violent video games pair frustration with acts
of aggression, and once that link is well established, individuals are
more likely to behave aggressively when frustrated in other situations.
Neuroscientists call this type of model spreading activation—as
associations laid down in one context create a network of linked thoughts,
emotions, and behaviors such that when one component is activated, it
automatically actives the rest of the network.
● Perhaps the most prolific and vocal proponents of the positive relationship
between violent video game playing and aggressive behavior, Anderson
and Bushman have suggested a general aggression model that incorporates
aspects of all 3 previously outlined theories and that also distinguishes the
short- from the long-term effects of media exposure.
● Already in 2002, Bushman and Anderson felt that the case for violent
media leading to more violence had been adequately made. So they
70
advocated for a move toward understanding why that might be the case
and testing their general aggression model. And a good test of the model is
to assess how people interpret an ambiguous situation: Do they read it as
aggressive or nonthreatening?
● In the short term, the model suggests that a person’s internal state can be
impacted by exposure to violent media, priming aggressive cognitions like
scripts and schemas; that is, in this situation, a person will be hostile and
react violently, increasing physical arousal and triggering angry feelings.
● With video games, they also learn that aggressive acts are effective in
neutralizing the situation or are rewarded in some way. In the extreme
case, a person might be consistently activating these knowledge structures,
such that they become aggressive under many different circumstances.
● Is there any evidence that such knowledge structures are indeed developed and
that they can influence how a person interprets an ambiguous situation?
● Studies have suggested that at least in the short term, violent video game
playing does influence how a person sees the world. Specifically, research
has shown that violent video games can increase aggressive thoughts,
feelings, and to some extent behaviors in adults. But most of these effects
are short-lived and are thought to be the result of activating hostility
association networks and increasing physiological arousal.
● Adults seem less affected in the long term, but that’s not the case for
children, who show the opposite pattern. While we do see short-term
effects in kids, these effects aren’t as robust in lab settings as they are
for adults. But when it comes to long-term effects, or the results of
longitudinal studies, kids do seem to be more affected.
71
Lesson 8 Video Games and Violence
Questions to Consider
72
Is Digital
9
Technology
Ruining
Sleep?
73
Lesson 9 Is Digital Technology Ruining Sleep?
A wealth of evidence
Stages of Sleep is showing that of the 3
pillars of health—sleep, diet,
● Our eyes have an outsize influence and exercise—sleep is the
on how our brains fall and stay
asleep. There are receptors in your
most important in terms of
retinas that send their projections your optimal cognitive and
directly to parts of the brain that physical function.
regulate the hormones that make you
feel sleepy. They bypass your visual cortex
and track the slow changes in ambient light
that characterize the setting and rising of the Sun. And if you trick these
receptors into thinking it’s not yet dark, you’ll also find it more difficult to
fall asleep.
Sleep is not a time during which your brain is resting. It is a time for
cycling through different stages of brain activity, some quite distinct
from our waking minds and some quite similar, but each with an
important role to play in keeping our brains functioning at their best.
● We are the only species on Earth that has managed to override the strong
signal of the Sun by inventing ways to bring light into our homes even
when our part of the world has turned its back on our solar system’s core.
* The impact might be greatest on young people, who have more trouble
understanding the importance of sleep and regulating their sleep duration.
74
● Falling asleep doesn’t just happen because we’re tired. There are external
signals that it’s time for sleep, such as the slow dimming of environmental
light, as well as internal signals, such as the buildup of a metabolic by-
product of your nervous system’s activity called adenosine. But the actual
falling-asleep process is a dynamic interplay between the activity of different
brain regions and the neurotransmitters and hormones they modulate.
● Falling asleep happens when cells in 2 parts of our brain, the ventrolateral
preoptic nucleus of the hypothalamus and the parafacial zone in the
brain stem, increase their activity and send us into a different state of
consciousness, one in which the thalamus, at the geographical center of the
brain, blocks access to the outside world.
● After all, we see by turning photons of light into voltage changes across cell
membranes in a 2-dimensional retina. Yet somehow we recognize objects,
interpret color, and predict depth.
● So the first thing our brains need to do in order to sleep is to shut down
this illusion and prevent our minds from being distracted by what’s
happening around us and then instead focus on the task of generating the
rhythms of sleep.
● We then begin the slow descent into ever-deeper stages of sleep, each of
which is characterized by distinct patterns of neural activity. With respect
to brain waves, the first stages of sleep look like slightly lazier versions
of resting wakefulness, with stage 2 distinguished from stage 1 by the
presence of 2 specific markers, called K complexes and sleep spindles.*
75
Lesson 9 Is Digital Technology Ruining Sleep?
● The deepest stages of sleep are called slow-wave sleep, because in aggregate
the brain waves that represent the activity of thousands or even millions of
cells look like synchronized slow waves.
● After your brain passes through the deepest stage, it begins its ascent back
through the lighter stages and, if all is going well, enters rapid-eye-movement
(REM) sleep instead of waking up. REM sleep is also called paradoxical
sleep because the brain’s activity looks a lot like it does when we’re awake,
but our bodies are paralyzed, so the only movement, in a healthy individual,
is that of our eyes, which dart back and forth in synchrony.
● During the second stage of sleep, called light non-REM sleep, our brain waves
are decorated with short bursts of a signature electrical signal: a sleep spindle.
They are generated by the reticular nucleus in the thalamus, a structure that
plays a significant role in generating our sleep-wake cycle and modulating
consciousness. It seems that sleep spindles aid in the freeing up of space for
what we need to learn next. The more of them you generate through the night,
the better able you will be to learn new things the following day.
● In the next stages, the deep non-REM sleep happens, characterized by those
slow, synchronized waves traveling from the front to the back of the brain.
● While sleep spindles clear the hippocampus whereas deep sleep ensures
proper storage of that information elsewhere, both stages are important for
learning and remembering.
● The last stage, which we see more and more of as the night progresses, is
REM sleep, which is when we’re most likely to experience vivid dreams. It’s
76
also when neurotransmitters like serotonin and norepinephrine are at their
lowest concentrations, while acetylcholine levels are high.
● Given that our brains are looking for signals from the environment that
the Sun is setting in order to begin the cascade of events that puts us into
sleep cycles, artificial light can block these signals very effectively.
● You might have heard that blue light from screens is particularly nefarious.
And indeed it is, largely because of how our vision evolved. We can trace
the emergence of vision to our ancient aquatic ancestors, who evolved the
ability to see light in a certain part of the electromagnetic spectrum—
specifically, 380 to 720 nanometers. And we’re highly sensitive to light
in the blue part of that sliver of the spectrum; in other words, melatonin
suppression is most potent when we are exposed to light whose wavelengths
are on the shorter side, closer to 400 nanometers than to 700.
● And that seems to be because the ocean filters out the longer-wavelength
light, letting through shorter wavelengths, which our brains interpret as
blue. For a fish, the setting of the Sun is most easily tracked by the amount
77
Lesson 9 Is Digital Technology Ruining Sleep?
● While we can thank our fishy ancestors for our disproportionate sensitivity
to environmental changes in blue light concentrations, we can blame the
inventors of light-emitting diode (LED) light for its outsize effects on our
ability to fall asleep. Blue LEDs use much less energy than incandescent
light bulbs and have longer life spans, which is great for our pocketbooks
and our environment. But they’re not so good for our health if they are
disrupting our sleep. Light from blue LEDs has a powerful melatonin-
suppression effect, and it just so happens that many of our screens, from
smartphones to iPads and TVs, are enriched with blue LED light.
78
● So sleep studies have shown that compared with reading a printed book
illuminated by an incandescent light bulb for a few hours before bed,
reading on an iPad delays the rising of melatonin levels—the signal for
sleep—by up to 3 hours. Peak melatonin levels aren’t reached until the
early-morning hours, compared with around midnight for traditional-book
readers. That’s the equivalent of crossing 3 time zones.
● Research shows that tablet readers also lose significant REM time; they
feel less rested and sleepier the following day. And in the days following,
when they no longer use an iPad before bed, they still show a significant
lag in melatonin rise.
● While interacting with technology can delay or disrupt your sleep, there
are ways that you can use technology to hack it.
● There is now a cottage industry of apps and tools designed to help you
undo the damage of melatonin-suppressing blue light. The first tool is just
the opposite of blue light: Night-lights that emit primarily red light do
not seem to have the same effect on melatonin suppression and might be a
great alternative.
● But there are other options that use visual stimulation to put you to sleep.
Perhaps the most extreme and controversial form of sensory-stimulation
technology is called audiovisual stimulation (AVS), which is designed to
entrain your brain to synchronize its activity and set you into the sleep
wave states.
79
Lesson 9 Is Digital Technology Ruining Sleep?
images at a certain frequency and assume that it gets your brain waves
entrained, or you can include a measure of brain wave activity in the form
of an electroencephalogram (EEG) that feeds into the system. The EEG
data are then used to modulate the audiovisual stimulation, giving it real-
time neurofeedback.
● When it comes to AVS, the open-loop devices have a series of set programs
that delivers sensory pulses of a particular frequency. EEG studies show
that the right type of stimulation pretty reliably generates a response in
sensory regions and the thalamus. So if you can trigger these responses to
come at a certain frequency, you can presumably trick the thalamocortical
loops to fire in sync at the frequency that we observe when you’re falling
asleep. In 20- to 60-minute sessions, the AVS system is fairly good at
generating the brain wave frequency in the desired frequency band.
80
through sensory channels, to aid sleep are likely coming down the pike, the
current offerings aren’t life-altering just yet.
Questions to Consider
81
How “Dr.
10
Google” Is
Changing
Medicine
82
F rom email interactions with your physician to drug
therapies or interventions crafted to your genetic
profile, technological advances are making personalized
medicine possible. Technological advances are also
improving early detection, even at the molecular
level of disease processes. And once we do get sick,
personalized medicine is improving diagnosis, choice
of treatment, and management of care. However, as
medicine becomes more personalized, traditional
models of funding have to be reconsidered, and the
gap in health care between those with means and those
without will widen. And cost is hardly the only potential
problem that technology is bringing to medicine.
● One big problem that technology is bringing to medicine is the fact that
finding information about any number of health problems has never been
easier. With “Dr. Google” at our fingertips, we’re all prone to suffering
from medical student syndrome—the disease in which you read about
symptoms and suddenly start suffering from them.
Personalized medicine can help us figure out our risk profiles for
certain diseases based on our genes and history. Preventative
treatments and recommendations can help us tailor our diet,
exercise, sleep, and other lifestyle factors to our genome,
microbiome, and other personal information.
83
Lesson 10 How “Dr. Google” Is Changing Medicine
sounds the alarm of googling about medical issues. Fake news threatens
our democracy, he says, but fake medical information threatens our lives.
● Warraich cited a 2017 study that found that people who turn to alternative
treatments for cancer, such as diets, herbs, and supplements, and eschew
traditional treatments are 2.5 times more likely to die than those who stick
to doctors’ orders.
● Almost as soon as the internet became widely available, the public began
to use it to learn about their health. A study published in 1999 found
that medical information was among the most-sought-after treasure on
the fledgling internet. But back then, it was difficult to find and parse
information, and it was clear that medical degrees had their usefulness. In
fact, access to all of that information at least initially increased patients’
trust in their doctors, as reported in a 2001 Health Information National
Trends Survey.
● Over the past few decades, however, the information has become
much more accessible, both in terms of technology and digestibility.
Large hospitals like the Mayo Clinic now have their own vast and well-
maintained websites that are dedicated to the dissemination of medical
information. And internet users are much more diligent in checking
sources and maintaining skepticism when it’s warranted. Yet our ability to
access nearly endless medical information sometimes has made us feel as
though we know just as much as our physicians. But we don’t.
84
expand the harms that alternative, unregulated “treatments” can wreak on
desperate patients.
● Beyond causing stress and anxiety where it might not be warranted, “Dr.
Google” also can erode the relationship between doctor and patient. In
the 1960s, most Americans—73%—felt confident that their doctors knew
what they were doing. By 2012, that number had dropped to 34%, as
published in The New England Journal of Medicine. And this drop did not
go unnoticed: A 2017 survey found that 87% of physicians felt that their
patients trusted them less than they did a decade earlier.
85
Lesson 10 How “Dr. Google” Is Changing Medicine
● If a patient doesn’t trust his or her doctor, then he or she will be less likely
to comply with medical advice and then quick to blame his or her health-
care professional when things don’t go as expected. There’s even data to
show that patients who trust their doctors more also have better health
outcomes. In other words, the erosion of trust can keep you sicker longer.
● Searching online has another potential pitfall: the loss of privacy, though
arguably simple searches are less problematic when it comes to this issue
than many of the apps people now use to track their health.
● There are apps that help women track fertility and become pregnant, help
people with diabetes manage their glucose levels, and track blood pressure
and heart rate. The latest version of the Apple Watch even has a built-in
echocardiogram app. And the vast majority of these apps are not subject to
the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA), a US
law that was enacted specifically to address the threat of technology on the
privacy of our health information.
● In accordance with HIPAA, if you tell your doctor that you’ve been feeling
a little depressed, he or she can’t tell your employer, for example. But if you
report your mood on an app that doesn’t interact with your health-care
team, the company that developed the app can sell your information to
some third party, including a health insurance company or a recruiter.
● There are, of course, many more ways in which technology has improved
medicine. And diagnosis is one area where doctors working alongside
artificial intelligence (AI) can really make a difference. Computers are,
after all, really good at finding patterns. And that’s essentially what
radiologists and pathologists do.
86
● In a research paper from 2015, scientists trained a group of novices to
pick out cancerous cells in breast tissue slides. Without the help of any
physicians, the novices were shown cancerous and noncancerous tissue and
had to indicate which was which.
● But there are also case studies in which computers just haven’t measured
up to their potential. One such case is IBM’s Watson, which became
famous by beating 2 of the greatest champions of the game show Jeopardy!
in 2011 and winning a $1 million prize. Two years later, IBM announced
that Watson’s genius would be applied to aiding lung cancer treatment
decisions at Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center in New York, among
other projects. By 2019, Watson became a cautionary tale of hubris and
hype, with several projects being discontinued despite the investment of
tens of millions of dollars.
● Part of the problem in training AI doctors is that the data are so poorly
managed in health care in general. If Watson doesn’t have access to
clean and thorough data sets, we can’t expect it to be much better than
a conglomeration of human physicians, so it won’t be replacing them
anytime soon.
87
Lesson 10 How “Dr. Google” Is Changing Medicine
● Whether it’s socialized medicine like the national health-care system in the
UK or private institutions as in the US, health-care information is often
dispersed among different providers and hospitals, with little or no cross
talk. Exchanging records is cumbersome and difficult, so doctors don’t
have a comprehensive view of a patient’s full medical history.
● Plus, patients are treated mostly for acute problems, especially in the US, where
regular doctors’ visits might be a cost that patients with high-deductible health
insurance or no health insurance are unable or unwilling to cover. So no one
gets a chance to evaluate the forest rather than just the trees.
88
symptoms through a diagnostic algorithm, without the doctor even
touching a keyboard or staring at the screen for too long.
● One of the biggest reasons why we see a primary care physician in the
first place is so that there’s at least a chance that someone is keeping a full
record of our medical history. But most of us don’t see the same physician
for decades, as was more common in the past.
Questions to Consider
89
The Virtual
11
Therapist
90
T oday, it’s easier than ever to find and talk to a mental
health professional from the comfort of your home.
And therapies that can transport patients into virtual
spaces, where experiences can be carefully curated and
personalized to help them overcome their psychological
issues, are growing in number and proving to be quite
effective.
91
Lesson 11 The Virtual Therapist
symptoms improve, even though the levels of the neurotransmitter that the
drug acts on are “balanced” almost immediately. The negative side effects
show up before the psychological benefits become clear.
● For the most part, we don’t know exactly what causes depression and other
psychiatric disorders. But we do know that it’s a complex interplay between
neurotransmitters, brain development, neuronal growth, survival, and
psychological factors like exposure to trauma, rumination, and so on.
● It’s just not as simple as recalibrating brain chemistry and therefore curing
mental illness. The complexity of the relationship between neurochemistry
and psychiatric disorders is one of the reasons why we’ve been essentially
stuck with the same mediocre drugs for the past few decades.
92
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
93
Lesson 11 The Virtual Therapist
● The long-term outcomes for CBT are often superior to those of a drug
regimen because once the patient stops taking the drug, the symptoms
often return. But CBT gives the patient cognitive tools that he or she can
apply when the next episode hits.
Virtual Therapy
* This finding was described in a paper by Robert DeRubeis, Greg Siegle, and
Steven Hollon published in Nature Reviews Neuroscience in 2008.
94
If the treatment outcomes are no different, and it’s more
convenient for both the therapist and the patient, and it’s
cheaper, it seems inevitable that virtual therapy is part of the
future of medicine.
● Fear is a very effective learning tool. It taps into powerful neural circuitry
that evolved to protect us from life-threatening situations. So it can shape
our behavior even with one exposure. If you lived through an experience
that set off your full-blown fight-or-flight nervous system and left you
physically and psychologically scarred, you don’t want to put yourself
in those circumstances again in case the outcome is more final. So your
brain is very good at tracking things that might lead to such an aversive
event and triggering your nervous system to behave such that you’re out of
harm’s way.
● People with a specific phobia need to learn that the object of their fear
is not as dangerous as they think. In the case of the boy, he needs to
distinguish dogs that are not vicious from the one that was. One effective
way to eliminate the fear is to give him opportunities to interact with
dogs and learn that nothing bad will happen. This process is called
extinction because it essentially extinguishes the conditioning between the
stimulus—in this case, the dog—and the aversive outcome, such as pain
from a dog bite.
● The problem is that you can’t just force him to interact with dogs. While
this might turn out OK, much more likely it will cause the person to
reengage the sympathetic nervous system, inducing a panic attack, which
is a very aversive experience. So even if the dog doesn’t bite, he has still
95
Lesson 11 The Virtual Therapist
learned that the fear he holds for dogs is justified. After all, something
bad—a panic attack—happened when he was exposed to the stimulus.
● But some fears are harder to bring into a therapist’s office, such as the fear
of flying or public speaking. That’s where VR comes in. What’s more,
some people with phobias have a hard time imagining interactions with
the objects of their fear and are reluctant to engage in real versions. VR can
overcome both of these obstacles.
● In situations where a patient’s anxiety begins to spiral, it’s easier to turn off
a VR program than to extract the person from a live situation, reducing
the risks of exposure therapy. This is an important benefit when exposure
therapy is applied to an arguably more extreme or debilitating anxiety
disorder: post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
96
● But prolonged exposure therapy remains one of the most effective and
empirically validated treatments for PTSD. Yet a 2004 survey found
that only about 17% of clinicians use it to treat their patients. Barriers to
treatment include patients’ aversion to in vivo exposure and their inability
to effectively use their imagination to relive the trauma. VR exposure-
based therapy (VR-EBT) can again overcome these barriers, and 76% of
PTSD patients reportedly are more accepting of it than of traditional EBT.
And VR-EBT has been shown to be as effective as in vivo therapy.
● For some burn patients, these daily treatments are even more painful than
the original burn, as the nurses have to clean the wound, remove dead
tissue, and stretch the skin. Even the strongest pain medications, such as
opioids, aren’t enough to numb the pain.
● Just like general health apps, those for mental health are a growing set of
tools designed to enhance more traditional forms of therapy. One of the
major problems in the mental illness field is the fact that a large proportion
of people with mental health issues don’t seek treatment. Stigma, cost,
time, and accessibility are all contributing factors. But each of these
barriers can be alleviated or eliminated by apps at our fingertips.
97
Lesson 11 The Virtual Therapist
● A person with suicidal ideation can text a crisis center in the middle of the
night without disturbing anyone else in the household. Patients can enjoy
total anonymity, though privacy issues should be considered.* Patients who
are reluctant to seek help can take the first microsteps toward treatment
using an app. The cost is much lower, and mental health providers can
reach a larger population of patients. Apps hold tremendous promise,
which is why there are already thousands of them, with new ones being
created all the time.
Questions to Consider
2 What are some ways that virtual reality might be used to enhance
therapy?
* Most apps aren’t governed by the FDA yet and therefore aren’t subject to the
Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act, better known as HIPAA.
98
How Big Data
12
Can Predict
the Future
99
Lesson 12 How Big Data Can Predict the Future
● But a big issue is also the fact that before we had the ability to collect and
analyze massive data sets, we had to rely on samples of a population, such
as 10 to 20 mice or 15 human participants. Sampling can provide a decent
proxy for the population provided that it’s random—so that any individual
factors wash out across the group—and that it’s large enough to uncover
the effect in question.
● Of course, the smaller the effect that you’re searching for, the larger the
sample size must be. But even large effects, when limited to 15 human
brains in a neuroimaging scanner, can be caused by factors that aren’t
obvious in the initial study design and that wash out when the sample size
is orders of magnitude larger.
● When you’re trying to figure out how large of a sample you need to find a
given effect, you must consider the estimated effect size and the diversity
100
of your population—and then how generalizable your results will be to the
larger population of interest.
● For some scientific questions, such as how the visual system works,
diversity in terms of education and income are less important. But for
others, such as whether self-control is a limited resource, these variables
matter a lot. So when choosing your sample, you need to be mindful of
what you’re looking for and what factors to control for so that your sample
is representative and random.
● But what happens when the sample size gets so large that it includes almost
the entire population? That’s what technological advances are currently giving
science and other domains that rely on data collection. It’s called big data.*
● The ability to collect and analyze large data sets is changing how science
is being conducted and what it takes to be a successful scientist. Even in
fields like biology, the emphasis is shifting away from manual skills like
pipetting and preparing slides for the microscope and toward complex
statistics and coding algorithms.
* The phrase big data doesn’t show up in publications until around 1956
(though there’s a curious blip in 1931), but it rises exponentially in popularity
around 1995.
101
Lesson 12 How Big Data Can Predict the Future
fields are built. The very way that we devise, test, and confirm or overturn
hypotheses is changing. What we consider strong evidence has shifted.
● Big data allows scientists to let the data drive the process of hypothesis
generation. When you have information from the (almost) entire
population, you can just ask the data.
● Today, you can sequence the entire genomes of 1000 people with the
disease or trait, find the common markers, and use an algorithm to
identify causes—resulting in a bunch of candidate genes to consider.
102
● Now the hard work is not in careful lab work but in mindful analysis of
multidimensional and massive data sets. When your algorithm spits out
500 candidate genes, how do you figure out which are important and
which are coincidental? You can’t just reason your way through; you need
to devise a mathematical model of how you’ll rank and order the results.
● But big data’s promise is not just about size; it’s also about the ability to
quantify previously unquantifiable things, such as which routes you take
to work each day and how much traffic you have to fight to get there. Your
ability to predict traffic patterns at different times of day used to be built
on your memory for all your previous commutes or the current conditions
that you heard about on your local news station. But now mapping apps
103
Lesson 12 How Big Data Can Predict the Future
can crunch millions of data points from many different sources, giving you
real-time updates and predictions regarding your expected travel time.
● As small sample sizes become large populations, data sets don’t need to
be cleaned up for missing data points or outliers, as used to be the case
in scientific studies. Messy data is OK now because the samples are so
massive. You can keep that one participant who wasn’t really paying
attention to your task because his or her data represent a miniscule
proportion of your overall set.
● Big data analysis is allowing us to solve problems like how to teach a driverless
car to navigate city streets. In fact, we technically don’t need to teach the car; it
teaches itself by collecting and crunching massive amounts of data. That’s why
we see them driving around seemingly aimlessly around our cities.
● The previous model would have been to have engineers working on the
problem for years in a computer simulation or a parking lot. It was a
very difficult problem and progress was very slow. But then our ability to
crunch large data sets became much easier, and now the cars are gaining
real-world experience to teach themselves.
Prediction Models
104
● Part of the reason we make this error is that we attribute the trend to some
cause. Study after study in behavioral economics has shown that we’re
primed to infer causation when 2 things co-occur. If you’re thinking about
your aunt and suddenly the phone rings and it’s her, you can’t help but
wonder if there’s some force in the world that led to this turn of events—
that somehow your thinking about her caused her to call you.
● Part of the reason that health-care professionals record your vital signs at
virtually every opportunity is so that they can spot any changes that might
signal a negative outcome. They care less about the immediate cause of a racing
heart rate or higher blood pressure and more about what those signals portend.
● They called it Google Flu Trends, and they showed that search data can
provide accurate estimates of the prevalence of the flu in certain regions
up to 2 weeks before the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention could
using their more traditional tracking tools. The results were published in
the prestigious journal Nature.
● But in 2013, Google Flu Trends overestimated the prevalence of flu at its
peak by 140%. And this epic failure of big data made the headlines. The
project was quietly shut down shortly thereafter.
● One of the problems that this failed project highlights is that often
the people writing the algorithms to analyze big data don’t have the
background knowledge that experts in that domain do. In the case of
Google Flu Trends, clinicians argue that Google mistook searches for
symptoms of other ailments for those of the flu. And most people make
the same mistake: The vast majority of doctors’ visits for flu-like symptoms
are actually caused by some other virus. That’s why Google Flu Trends was
chronically overestimating the prevalence of the flu.
105
Lesson 12 How Big Data Can Predict the Future
● The failure of Google Flu Trends represents a pitfall and temptation of big
data: jumping from finding to finding without processing the implications
of the findings or even considering alternative causes. It can encourage
shallow thinking. And governments are in danger of making these same
mistakes; using big data to predict crime, for example, can increase bias
and alienate communities unfairly.
● The question facing us now is how to turn big data into knowledge—
shifting from a view of knowledge as an understanding of the past to a
prediction about the future, which may not include a satisfying explanation
of why the predicted event might occur. Many of the statistical tools we’ve
relied on so heavily, such as p values and control groups, might be traded in
favor of data analytics, such as discovering trends in large data sets.
Questions to Consider
106
Is Privacy
13
Dead in the
Information
Age?
107
Lesson 13 Is Privacy Dead in the Information Age?
● Our increasingly lax attitude toward our own privacy is known as the
privacy paradox. Doomsday prophets point to the paradox as the harbinger
of the ultimate demise of our private lives.
108
● Yet in 2018 and continuing into 2019, Facebook continued its rapid
growth, adding users at an even faster pace than before the breach came to
light. Despite the knowledge that their personal information might have
been misused, users continued to upload it to the site. What’s more, while
we all know that we can adjust the privacy settings on our apps, phones,
and other devices, many of us just don’t bother.
● Does the fact that we’re willing to trade privacy for what social media has
to offer mean that we no longer care about our privacy? How important
can privacy be if we won’t spend 2 minutes protecting it by adjusting the
factory settings on our devices?
● Most of us, it turns out, readily log in via social media or hand over our
email address when asked.
● But what if the box gave you these options: allow them to access your
personal information and give you $1 for your trouble, or let you browse
the web in complete privacy? In this case, most of us would probably
choose privacy. After all, in that direct setup, we recognize that our privacy
is more valuable than a measly dollar.
109
Lesson 13 Is Privacy Dead in the Information Age?
logging in via Facebook was the “low-privacy” condition and that they
could opt for a “high-privacy” condition if desired.
● Under these conditions, most people chose to be paid for the survey but
did not click the button to reveal whether or not they were selling their
privacy. They preferred to hide. This behavior persisted even well after the
Cambridge Analytica scandal broke, in which people’s trust in Facebook
keeping their information private was tested. Interestingly, though,
immediately after the scandal was made public, people were more likely
to click to reveal the privacy settings. But 45 days later, they reverted to
ignoring the extra button.
● In the case of the café offering Wi-Fi access, your choice to sell your
privacy is veiled. Though we don’t generally think of logging in with
Facebook as giving away our private information, that’s exactly what
we’re doing.
● So what’s going on? Svirsky suggests that we’re avoiding details about the
trade-offs we make between convenience and privacy. If that’s true, then
simply providing people with more details isn’t going to change their
behavior in a lasting way.
110
Institutional Trust
● Over the past few decades, the information that companies collect has
widened enormously to include all kinds of personal data. In the 2012
Target pregnancy scandal, the American retailer began predicting whether
shoppers had recently become pregnant and offering specials on baby
items. Enter a teenage girl’s angry father, who admonished the company
for normalizing teen pregnancy by sending pregnancy coupons to his
daughter. But he soon found himself apologizing, as the company had
accurately predicted that she was indeed pregnant.
● One of the reasons why we keep things private is because we don’t trust the
world with that information. And trust is certainly changing as we fill our
lives with technology.
● One of the great leaps forward that human society has made was to
develop institutions that foster trust. In her book Who Can You Trust?,
Rachel Botsman describes the evolution of trust as starting from the point
at which we trust only those closest to us, whom we have personally vetted,
to the current age, in which trust is placed in many different institutions.
And she points out that trust in contracts, courts, and corporations created
the foundation for an organized industrial society.
● This trust revolution was built within the social structure of institutions
whose laws and practices are laid out and then agreed upon by their
members. And trust is key: When we belong to or operate within an
institution, we trust that these laws will be adhered to. And we punish
those who break them.
111
Lesson 13 Is Privacy Dead in the Information Age?
● But what happens when institutions break our trust and are not punished?
Trust is eroded much more easily than it is built, and once broken, it can
be very hard to reestablish because it’s based on a belief that has been
proven false.
Distributed Trust
● Just as we receive news from multiple sources, often from people we know
personally or from people who are experiencing the newsworthy event
in the moment, we now rely on multiple individual experiences to tell us
whom to trust.
● Many applications that enable people to connect with and share resources
with strangers, such as Airbnb and Uber, are changing the very nature by
which we evaluate trustworthiness. What we’re learning is that given the
right motivation, strangers aren’t likely to betray our trust.
● The internet was invented to help us connect, and the value of a person’s
connections can be broadly defined as social capital. While our obsession
with devices and online communication can come at the cost of in-person
interactions, the internet also affords many more ways of earning social
capital. A person’s reputation remains a valuable commodity, as it can give
him or her access to all of the conveniences that a digital world offers.
112
● When you book a home on Airbnb, for example, you’re well aware that
a company is learning a lot of information about you and your personal
preferences. You sacrifice that privacy for the convenience of living in a
home away from home, complete with a kitchen and other amenities, while
on a trip. And because you have a sense that somebody is watching—that
your behavior will be reviewed—you behave better overall than you might
in a hotel room or other accommodation in which the provider doesn’t
have the ability to give you a bad rating.
● The idea that people who log in to other sites via Facebook don’t care
about their privacy is patently untrue, as Dan Svirsky’s study and others
have shown. In fact, what they are exchanging their privacy for can
sometimes be a path toward demonstrating their trustworthiness.
● Social capital has value and can be bought. This means that people who
have more money can increase their social capital by bypassing traditional
channels. You can buy followers on social media platforms, pay people to
leave reviews for your products on Amazon or your restaurant on Yelp,
and so on. And by the same token, you can pay more to ensure that your
privacy remains intact.
113
Lesson 13 Is Privacy Dead in the Information Age?
● As Ramesh Srinivasan suggests in his book Beyond the Valley, we’re moving
into an era in which we’re not just buying technological innovations; we’re
entering into agreements with companies that provide services in exchange
for our personal data.
114
allowing people to pay a premium for greater control over their data. She
points out that this state of affairs will establish a new divide between the
privacy-rich and the privacy-poor.
● Instead of us dictating how and what we share with others, the connected
digital world will establish privacy norms. Once we take for granted that
our lives are no longer private, how will that change the very essence of
who we think we are?
Questions to Consider
3 Will privacy become a luxury available only to those who are willing
to pay for it?
115
The Emotional
14
Effects of
Social Media
116
P lenty of research has shown the negative effects of
social media, such as inducing sadness and loneliness
in users. But if the internet makes us sad and lonely, then
why are we so interested in it? The runaway success of
social media suggests that there must be net positive
benefits for users.
● The authors sought to answer 2 questions: Does Facebook make you feel
worse in the moments after use? And does it affect life satisfaction in the
longer term? These authors found that the more people reported using
Facebook, the worse they felt. But the association didn’t go in reverse—
that is, people weren’t using Facebook only when they were feeling sad.
● Over the course of the 2 weeks, the more they used the site, the more their
overall well-being declined. Direct interactions with people showed the
opposite effect: These interactions were more likely to improve the person’s
mood, and the more he or she socialized with people in real life, the better
off he or she was at the end of the 2-week period.
● You might think, then, that lonely people are more likely to turn to
Facebook—and that was found to be true. But when the authors controlled
117
Lesson 14 The Emotional Effects of Social Media
118
The Desire to Belong to a Group
● Social media platforms tap into another basic human desire: to belong
to a group. Experimental psychologist Robin Dunbar has put forth the
social brain hypothesis, which suggests that modern human brains were
shaped by evolution specifically to enable us to get along with others and
live in larger social groups. During the great expansion of brain size among
hominids around 1.5 million years ago, our ancestors also began living in
larger and larger groups, and those who were better equipped to navigate
social interactions were more likely to produce offspring that survived.
● Looking at social group size and neocortex volume, Dunbar noted that the
more neocortex a primate species has, the larger its social community is.
Extrapolating from this relationship, Dunbar has calculated ideal social
group sizes for different primate species, including humans.
● Dunbar has also suggested that our social networks are hierarchically
structured in layers. We have about 5 people with whom we are
exceptionally close: our spouses and our kids or perhaps a sibling and a
best friend. He calls this the support clique.
* Dunbar has found evidence for the accuracy of this number in a variety of
sources, including the estimated size of Neolithic farming villages, the basic
unit of an army in Roman times, and the number of employees in a company
building. In fact, one study found that companies with more than 150 employees
in a single location tend to have communication problems that slow down
productivity.
119
Lesson 14 The Emotional Effects of Social Media
● Finally, the outer layers of 500 or 1500 match up with the number of
acquaintances that we’d recognize enough to say hello to at the airport or
the number of faces we can name, including celebrities and people we don’t
know personally.
● Dunbar has argued that we have a basic need to keep track of our social
network and maintain social bonds, which he calls social grooming. Much
like physical grooming in nonhuman primates, checking in with friends,
liking their status updates, and generally staying in touch helps promote
the stability of the social group. Facebook and other social media platforms
tap into this desire and make grooming our network feel simpler.
● Dunbar argues that the same constraints that limit our social network
size in real life also apply to our online lives: the cognitive constraint of
maintaining and remembering the relationships and the time constraint of
interacting with them. Understanding these constraints also gives us hints
as to what kinds of social media use might benefit us versus what might
harm us.
● Several studies of Facebook use have shown that active interaction with
a small number of users—posting on walls and commenting on posts—
can lower feelings of loneliness and promote well-being, while passively
reading feeds and viewing content has the opposite effect. Strong ties can
be maintained with active interaction, and that can increase your social
capital. But active interaction requires time and effort. Passive scrolling can
make you feel less satisfied both as a result of making social comparisons
and wasting time that you could be spending developing meaningful
connections with others.
120
Benefits of Social Media Use
● Despite all the negative effects of social media that have been found, there
are studies showing that social media use can make you happier, as in
the work concerning active interactions. Studies have shown that using
platforms like Facebook can make us more trusting and even encourage
political participation.
● One of the ways in which social media platforms are changing social circles
is by combining different social spheres and blurring the lines between
categories of relationships, such as those between business colleagues.
Many Facebook users, for example, have friend networks that encompass
work colleagues in addition to friends from other spheres.
● And while there are many instances in which individuals have lost jobs
after posting inappropriate content, there’s also evidence that users are
learning to implement strategies that minimize this type of friction, such
as choosing more specific privacy settings, messaging directly rather than
posting on walls, and self-censoring content.
* This finding comes from a study out of Matthew Lieberman’s lab at UCLA,
published in Psychological Science in 2013.
121
Lesson 14 The Emotional Effects of Social Media
● In one way at least, social media’s success might also be its downside:
Because of the ease with which we can now access our social circles and the
proliferation of ways by which we can connect, our attention is constantly
being pulled in many different directions at once. That makes it difficult
to interact actively, and in fact, studies of active versus passive social media
engagement show that while the increases in social capital and well-being
are only possible through active interaction, most of us spend more time
passively scrolling.
● Indeed, teenagers who use 7 or more social media platforms are more likely
to report feeling anxiety and depression than those who are active on 2
or fewer. With 7 different platforms, it’s much more likely that these kids
spend most of their time scrolling rather than interacting. And there are
implications for how they learn to handle or avoid boredom.
● Access to smartphones and all of the distractions they offer has eliminated
many of the situations in which we learn to overcome boredom. People in
waiting rooms, commuting on the train or bus, or even waiting for food
at restaurants now have the option of checking email, playing games,
watching videos, listening to podcasts, or interacting with social media
instead of reading a magazine or simply being alone with their thoughts.
● John Eastwood, who runs York University’s Boredom Lab, defines boredom
as an unfulfilled desire for satisfying activity. We’re often bored in situations
in which we feel as though we’re not in control of our attention; we feel
disconnected from our inner thoughts or the external world.
● The problem with social media is that while it can seem to stave off
boredom in the very short term—for example, while you’re waiting in line
at the DMV—it can make you more susceptible to boredom in the long
term. That’s because you become disconnected from your inner mental
life. Your thinking is shallower as you skim over material like images
and short posts, and rapidly switching between nuggets of information
becomes a mental habit. As a result, it feels less natural to delve deeply and
lose yourself in your thoughts.
122
● Allowing yourself to get bored can actually be really helpful, especially
if you’re a teenager learning to navigate the new ways of thinking you’ve
developed now that your prefrontal cortex is becoming more efficient
through the process of myelination: the stage during which connections
between neurons get wrapped up in an insulating fatty sheath, increasing
the speed and efficiency with which messages are exchanged. But if you
never have the opportunity to do this kind of mental work because you’re
using social media to fill up every spare minute, then your ability to
tolerate boredom and to benefit from it diminishes.
● Boredom can also serve as a motivator. Since it’s an aversive state, we work
to avoid it, and that might lead us to make positive changes in our lives.
We might then find the courage to find more meaningful and engaging
goals and projects.
● Because boredom feels like an emotional trap, we try to find ways to escape
from it. If pursuing meaningful goals and developing strong and lasting
personal relationships is the equivalent of a healthy mental and emotional
diet, we need to limit the number of snacks that we consume before meals.
And if our escape from boredom too often involves simple snacking on
social media, we’ll never be fully satisfied.
Questions to Consider
1 How has our use of social media evolved in response to our feelings
when we use it?
3 What are ways in which we can minimize the effects of social media
on our emotions and well-being?
123
How Online
15
Dating
Transforms
Relationships
124
O ver the past few decades, how Westerners meet
each other and end up in relationships has changed
dramatically. According to a study published in 2017, the
percentage of married couples meeting online has gone
from essentially 0% to 25% if you’re heterosexual and nearly
70% if you’re in a same-sex partnership. Meeting through
friends is still the most popular way to find your lifelong
mate if you’re heterosexual, but just barely, accounting
for almost 30% of couplings. If you’re homosexual, you’re
now slightly more likely to meet your partner at a bar or
restaurant than from among your friends.
● Today, there are so many dating services that it’s helpful to group them
into categories.
— Some are open to anyone, while others cater to a particular niche, such
as religious affiliation, social class, age, or hobby.
— And they can be divided up into traditional websites and services that
are more app-based, including those that use your smartphone’s GPS
function to match you with people who are geographically close to you.
125
Lesson 15 How Online Dating Transforms Relationships
● The wide proliferation of online dating services speaks to the fact that one
major obstacle to active partner searching has been largely removed: the
stigma associated with advertising yourself. Personal ads in newspapers and
other media remained on the fringes of the dating world because it was
considered a social taboo to seek help in finding a partner in such an overt
fashion. This stigma bled into the online dating scene initially but has
since been somewhat attenuated, at least among digital natives.
● Are there significant differences between people who find each other
online and those who use more traditional means? Recent research of
personality traits among these 2 groups suggests that there aren’t many
differences between the populations anymore, though online daters might
be slightly less likely to be religious and slightly more likely to reject
traditional gender roles and be open to new experiences than those who
eschew the practice.
● Once a person has decided to seek love online, one of the first steps is to
create a profile and a list of traits that would be desirable in a partner.
This step can be incredibly impactful when we’re considering how dating
services are shaping relationship building.
126
● While women are more likely to lie about physical characteristics, men
have been shown to be more likely to lie in their profiles in general—as
a 2012 study by Rosanna Gaudagno, Bradley Okdie, and Sara Kruse
reported—with a tendency to appear to be more dominant, have more
resources, and be kinder than they actually are.
127
Lesson 15 How Online Dating Transforms Relationships
● Teenagers, who represent some of the most active and prolific social
network users, explore and build their identities with the help of these
tools. And single people, or those looking for romantic or sexual partners,
can also benefit from the questionnaires they need to fill out and the
psychological work they need to do in order to present their best selves to
potential mates.
● Profile creation and other social media habits affect how we think about
ourselves and how we craft our identity. There’s now growing evidence
that how we behave online can have implications for our personalities
offline. For example, because social media encourages us to focus on
ourselves, creating, tweaking, and refining our online identities can push
us toward behavior that builds our self-esteem. But it can also make us
more narcissistic.
Evaluation
128
Comparing Tinder users to those using other online dating apps or
none at all, the authors of a 2016 study, Karoline Gatter and Kathleen
Hodkinson, found that Tinder users were slightly younger and that
the men were more sexually permissive than the women, but there
were no differences in sociability or self-esteem between groups.
● This also means that you can glean a lot of information about someone at
one time, rather than slowly learning through conversation. And instead of
considering one or 2 potential partners at a given time, daters are offered
dozens, hundreds, or even thousands of options simultaneously.
● The problem is that once you’re in a relationship, you’re stuck with that
person in isolation. And if you chose that person by comparing him or her
to others, rather than evaluating his or her compatibility, you might have
made the wrong choice.
129
Lesson 15 How Online Dating Transforms Relationships
● You can see how this evaluation mode difference might apply to online
dating: If you’re faced with choosing between several attractive candidates,
joint evaluation diminishes your appreciation for any one of them. If,
by contrast, you’re going to have to consider only relatively unattractive
options, then comparing them will make you feel better about your choice,
as you’ll focus on the positive aspects of what makes your particular choice
superior to the others.
● The joint evaluation mode in dating, then, might push you to focus
on differences between dates that are most salient but perhaps not
most important in terms of long-term relationships. Attributes that are
easily compared via online dating sites are things like income, physical
attractiveness, and education. Attributes that are likely more important
but require face-to-face contact are sense of humor, conversational style,
kindness, and generosity.
130
Mindset
● Many online dating services market the size of their user base, touting the
millions of available singles a person will be able to choose from. But too
many options can lead to choice overload, exhausting us and leaving us less
happy with our ultimate choice.
● Perhaps the most worrisome psychological shift that online dating services
push us toward is a move from a locomotion mindset to a deliberative or
assessment one.
● The problem is that relationships aren’t static; they are dynamic and
require effort to maintain, just as a garden does. To nurture and maintain
a satisfying long-term relationship, we need to have a locomotion mindset,
one that continues to move and change as time goes on, which emphasizes
the psychological resources we’ll need to attain our desired goals as
conditions shift. Online dating services make us feel as though we’ve put
all the work in up front and once we’ve made our choice, we just need to sit
back and enjoy the fruits of our labor. But love doesn’t work that way.
● And research has shown that people who have a strong assessment
mindset coupled with a weak locomotion mindset tend to become overly
critical of their partners as well as more pessimistic about the future of
131
Lesson 15 How Online Dating Transforms Relationships
Questions to Consider
132
Technology
16
and Addiction
133
Lesson 16 Technology and Addiction
● For many years, addiction researchers and clinicians have argued whether
the definition of addiction should include types of behavior rather than
remain specific to psychoactive substances like heroin, alcohol, and cocaine
that target our reward pathways. Psychoactive drugs that don’t directly
affect the brain regions and neurotransmitters involved in reward are
generally not considered addictive.
● But what about drugs that work on dopamine? Isn’t that the reward
chemical in the brain?
134
● Dopamine plays a number of different roles in the brain, including
facilitating movement, some types of memory, and motivation and
pleasure. But its role in reward is not simple.
● They then observed how the rats compulsively pushed the lever. And it
turns out that the rats would do so even in the presence of a receptive
female. They’d run over an electrified grid to get to the lever, even though
they would not do so for food if they were starving.
● But did the rats enjoy the stimulation? Or did it simply cause them to want
it? We can’t ask the rat, but we can ask human addicts, who often report
not enjoying the object of their addiction but still being highly motivated
to pursue it.
135
Lesson 16 Technology and Addiction
● Addictive drugs change the brain in a number of ways, but there are 2
salient mechanisms by which they can induce long-term effects: one is
through changes in gene expression, which make the individual seek out
the rewarding effects of the addictive substance, and the other is through
changes in the brain’s reward pathways, which make previously pleasurable
activities less satisfying and encourage riskier reward-seeking behaviors.
● What puzzled scientists for a long time was the fact that some people could
take addictive drugs like cocaine for a while, seemingly without becoming
addicted, but then at some point, prolonged drug use would seem to flip a
switch, leading to compulsive behaviors characteristic of addiction.
136
● ∆FosB gene expression may be the mechanism by which this happens.
When it’s turned on, it changes the anatomy of cells in critical regions of
the reward pathway, making the individual more sensitive to the addictive
drug or behavior, increasing that person’s desire for the specific stimulus.
This is called reward sensitization.
● So, through the accumulation of ∆FosB, the wiring in the brain’s reward
pathway changes such that the person experiences strong cravings for
the object of their addiction: He or she becomes sensitized to cues that
anticipate the reward. But paradoxically, the drug or activity itself might
have become less pleasurable.
● Repeated use also raises tolerance, which means that more of the drug or
activity is needed to induce the same effect. But this seems contradictory:
How can you have both reward sensitization and tolerance? The answer is
that tolerance involves a different set of mechanisms, but it can be roughly
thought of as the brain’s attempt to return to normalcy.
● If you stop taking an addictive drug, then you also experience symptoms
of withdrawal—physical symptoms that are essentially the opposite of
the drug’s effects. So both tolerance and withdrawal can make a drug
addictive.
137
Lesson 16 Technology and Addiction
Internet Pornography
* If you offer a male rat a female in heat, the rat will copulate with gusto for a
while but eventually will be less interested. Replace the old female with a new
one and the rat finds his mojo again. This is called the Coolidge effect, and it is
attributed to a gradual decline in dopamine levels with the same old thing and a
surge when there’s something new.
138
● But the internet has made it trivial to find novelty. Individuals who
feel that their use of pornography has become an addiction describe the
cravings for new clips and images as insatiable, sometimes spending hours
clicking through videos in an endless stream of diversity, eschewing the
supposed goal of climaxing in the process.
● But it’s not just novelty that makes internet pornography potentially
addictive: It’s also filled with supranormal stimuli.
● In the past, a Playboy magazine or a sexy poster was no match for a real
live person. But pornography online is rife with supranormal stimuli:
artificially enlarged breasts and penises and exaggerated sounds and
actions. So some users of porn find themselves preferring the fake stuff
over reality. And however good internet porn is today, virtual reality is
poised to give people the perfect sexual experience, on demand, with
unlimited variability.
139
Lesson 16 Technology and Addiction
● There is now a growing literature mapping the brain changes that occur
in people who show signs of internet porn addiction onto changes that are
seen in individuals with substance abuse disorder. Of course, these diseases
are not equivalent, but the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, which is used by psychologists around the
world, includes hypersexual disorder, whose risk factors include the easy
access of sexual content.
140
Addictive behaviors are not new, nor have they been created by
the internet or other modern technological advancements. But
technology has made it easier to acquire stimulation that can lead
to the brain changes signaling an addiction.
Questions to Consider
141
Is the Internet
17
Hurting
Democracy?
142
T here is a strong interest in merging neuroscience
with politics. Indeed, there are measurable and
documented differences in brain function in people
who ascribe to quite different political views. And the
internet and other technologies can shape our behavior
and ultimately give one candidate an advantage over
the others. In the aggregate, these changes can affect
the ways in which we make voting decisions and how we
build and craft our communities in turn.
Political Affiliations
143
Lesson 17 Is the Internet Hurting Democracy?
Tribal Mentality
● That means that well before we become conscious of it, our political or
social affiliations influence what we see, hear, and experience. We can
see this effect in both behavior and brain activity. What’s more, these
affiliations are easily manipulated.
● Studies in the lab suggest that we’re pretty quick to realign ourselves when
alliances shift, even if they’re arbitrary. For example, say a group of people
were just assigned to wear different-colored name tags. Within seconds,
they’ll find themselves perceiving the situation differently now that their
vision of their tribe has shifted.
● That’s because there are many different ways with which we can categorize
people: race, socioeconomic status, language, gender, interests, education,
144
marital status, age, fandom, dietary preferences, etc. And maybe you don’t
have a clear view of your preferred name tag color, but you probably have a
preferred political party, or at least an ideology. And while it might be easy
to nudge you to think of people wearing your same-colored name tag as
part of your tribe, it is very difficult to overcome a strong political or other
identification that already is entrenched.
● Robin Dunbar calls this the social brain hypothesis. We are motivated to
seek connections with others—to belong to a tribe.
● These intuitions are part of what Daniel Kahneman terms System 1 thinking.
Categories of Cognition
● Our fast System 1 quickly appraises the world and makes suggestions to
System 2 concerning what we should do next. It’s automatic in that it
doesn’t require conscious deliberation or searching. In fact, much of what it
does is not available to us consciously.
● We often think that most of our actions are driven by our slow System 2,
but that’s generally not the case, because, as Kahneman points out, System
2 is lazy. It will choose the easy solution after giving it a quick once-over.
145
Lesson 17 Is the Internet Hurting Democracy?
be so frustrating. Not only is it highly unlikely that you’ll change the other
person’s mind, but it’s actually more likely that both of you will become
even more convinced that your own view is correct.
● There have been many studies that have demonstrated this effect, showing
that when presented with the same information, people with opposing
beliefs, regardless of whether those beliefs tend toward liberalism or
conservatism, will each come away feeling as though the evidence was in
line with their beliefs. It’s the confirmation bias: the fact that we often look
for and remember evidence that confirms or supports our beliefs rather
than evidence that would disconfirm it.
● While we can’t blame the internet for our tendency to categorize people
into in-groups and out-groups or for succumbing to the confirmation bias,
many features of the internet exploit these human tendencies and make it
harder for us to remain objective and rational.
146
● One of the big changes that the internet has brought is a shift from a
relative scarcity of information—a problem that the internet was explicitly
designed to solve—to information overload. What happens when we’re
bombarded with information? We need to make choices about what we pay
attention to, and those choices are often dictated by System 1.
● But when the participants were asked to reflect on what they just saw,
they showed activation of a region called the ventral striatum, which is
part of our reward and appraisal pathway. From other studies, we know
that what’s likely happening, then, is that the participants are taking the
opportunity to explain away or justify their beliefs that this is still their
preferred candidate.
Threatening Democracy
● The success of tech giants like Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, YouTube, and
other social media companies rests on their ability to capture our attention
147
Lesson 17 Is the Internet Hurting Democracy?
● And more importantly, we’ll behave in ways that benefit their advertisers
and, by proxy, their share price. They want us to click on things and like
things, so they tweak their content to maximize these behaviors.
● If you look at the history of tweaks that tech giants have implemented in
their home pages and apps, you’ll notice an increase in photographic and
video content as they realized the power of the image over words to engage
us. You’ll notice the increased prominence of numbers of likes, retweets,
shares, and so on, to show us that we’re part of a larger crowd—and if 10.6
million people liked this content, won’t you?
* As any casino manager knows, people will keep playing the slots if the rewards
are randomized and unpredictable.
148
effect on the elections they were involved in remains an open question. But
their methods reveal a threat to democracy that is very real.
● How might this work, given the neuroscience of politics? Threats and
the perception of danger tend to be associated with a greater desire for
order and a resistance to change, which pushes people toward a more
conservative candidate. Bombarding a so-called persuadable with fear-
mongering messaging might trigger his or her alarm response, activating
the amygdala and inciting feelings of anger and hate.
149
Lesson 17 Is the Internet Hurting Democracy?
● But what’s most nefarious is that we’re often not privy to all the factors that
influence our decision-making. That’s why large social media companies
like Facebook and YouTube need to be watched carefully: Changes in their
algorithms can create outsize effects, pushing society in directions that
leave us more polarized and insulated—and ultimately less democratic.
Questions to Consider
150
The Arts in
18
the Digital Era
151
Lesson 18 The Arts in the Digital Era
● With access comes excess. Whereas there was a real physical limitation to
how many albums you could purchase and store, thereby limiting the set of
available options when you wanted to listen to music, Spotify and other cloud-
storage options have brought the world’s musical catalog to our fingertips.
* Laugh tracks, a common feature of TV sitcoms, make us think that what we’re
watching is funnier than it is, even when we find them annoying and know
they’re fake.
152
● But have you ever felt overwhelmed by all the choices? And if you have,
what influenced your decision on what to play or watch or experience
next? Probably some kind of recommendation. So here we go again
with the gatekeepers, except this time, we’re letting the companies that
provide us with access to the digital materials decide how they’ll make
the recommendations, whether it’s crowdsourcing or an algorithm or,
increasingly more commonly, the highest bidder. And with a few gentle (or
not-so-gentle) nudges, companies like Facebook that can access hundreds
of millions of users have the power to make or break artists and their art.
● But, you might be thinking, surely the cream rises to the top, especially
when the playing field is so level, as the internet was supposed to be the
great equalizer, providing access to global audiences virtually for free. And
to a certain extent, you’d be right: The best artists generally do well under
any circumstances. That’s because they’re pretty easy to spot. The music of
153
Lesson 18 The Arts in the Digital Era
● The exceptionally good tracks slowly rose in the charts in all of the
versions, and the exceptionally bad ones didn’t. But it’s what happened to
the ones in the middle that’s really interesting.
● The listeners were much more likely to download a track if they knew that
other people had listened to it and liked it. And the effect snowballed. This
same effect explains why authors are so keen on having their books reach
the New York Times best-seller lists early. By some counts, being on the list
can boost sales for first-time authors by more than 50%.
● To test just how powerful social acceptance can be, Salganik and his
colleagues conducted a follow-up experiment in which they flipped the
charts upside down: The hits became the worst ranked and the bombs were
at the top. This time, the previously top-performing tracks didn’t do as well,
but they still were more frequently downloaded than the ones that were now
at the top. And the bad tracks actually caused the listeners to give up on the
players entirely, dropping out of the study. So good press and social scores
can enhance good content, but they can’t save the bad stuff from failure.
154
allow us to access ever larger amounts of content, make this problem even
harder, as the vast majority of the stuff is in the middle.
● In 2013, Banksy set up a stall in New York’s Central Park, alongside others
laden with touristy wares, and hired a nondescript 60-year-old man to
sell authentic, signed, original canvases for $60 each. His total sales for
the day were $420—not bad for an unknown artist or someone selling
knockoffs, like most of the other stalls. But the artwork he sold for $420
was estimated at the time to be worth $225,000.
* An example is his piece The Mild Mild West, which shows a cute teddy bear
throwing a Molotov cocktail at police in riot gear.
155
Lesson 18 The Arts in the Digital Era
● This saturation can also make it daunting for young artists to take the time
to hone their craft. Why spend 10 years learning to play the violin when
you can listen to Itzhak Perlman (or any other great violinist) whenever
you want—or better yet, play a digital violin just as beautifully yourself in
a virtual world? And the chances of your standing out from the crowd and
being able to make a living as a musician in the real world are dwindling.
● That’s because streaming services pay their artists a pittance. And the
public is becoming accustomed to being able to enjoy art—music, film,
and so on—without paying for it, or paying a small monthly subscription
fee that gives them access to massive catalogs of artwork.
● While streaming music services are often touted as solving some of the access
problems that the record label monopolies created, such services are not artist-
friendly. And the shift from owning music—such as buying a track, digital
or analog—to renting music—paying for access to a catalog as one does on
Spotify—is a big deal. It’s changing how we consume and value music.
● For one thing, we’re much more likely to pay more to own rather than rent,
naturally. Illustrating this point, Sam Rosenthal, who founded the label
Projekt, the first to withdraw its catalog from Spotify, wrote that 5000 plays
of music from his label yielded $6.50 in royalties from Spotify. If those same
tracks had been downloaded via iTunes, his income would have been $3,487.
● Some labels blame Spotify explicitly for the cratering of physical album
sales in countries where the app is available. In the last few years, though,
Spotify has continued to dominate the music industry, and some labels
that initially withdrew have come back, hat in hand. Labels representing
very popular artists have more leverage, of course, to negotiate better
royalties. But bands that hope to build an audience and make a living
doing so are out of luck.
● It’s not just musicians who suffer. All kinds of artists, writers, and small
business owners are feeling the pinch.
156
● Almost outside of our awareness, how we value everything that can be
accessed online is changing. This shift in the value of art leaves little room
for amateurs and community theaters, smaller arts organizations, and places
where amateurs can train and perform. With the limited time we have, why
waste any of it on mediocre art when we can experience the greats?
● In order to reap the benefits of musical training—or any learning, for that
matter—we need to be willing to step outside our comfort zone, to try and
fail, and to take risks and get frustrated. Technological innovations often
make our lives easier, and we become less able to tolerate the challenges
inherent in learning something hard.
157
Lesson 18 The Arts in the Digital Era
● Making things easy is not good for learning. The easier something is, the
less likely it is to leave a permanent trace in your brain. When learning
feels easy, we get illusions of competence. When it’s hard, we induce lasting
neuroplastic changes in our brains.
When we get something right on the first try, we think, Wow, I’ve
learned that well! But the truth is that performance in the moment
is not a good measure of long-term retention.
● When you love something so much that you’re willing to toil at it without
pay, then it will certainly change you. And like so many amateurs
throughout history, you might even end up changing it. But this won’t
happen if we allow technology to lure us into a world where we no longer
pay for art or entertainment and there’s no more incentive for amateurs to
toil away, as their flawed performances are perceived to have no value.
Questions to Consider
3 How does musical training shape the brain, and does listening to
music have the same effect?
158
How AI Can
19
Enhance
Creativity
159
Lesson 19 How AI Can Enhance Creativity
160
● Novices need to memorize each piece and its location one by one, and
their working memory, which enables them to keep this information in
mind and to manipulate it if necessary, is quickly taxed. An expert sees the
same board and assigns a meaningful chunk (oh, it’s the classic Budapest
Gambit!). The expert needs only to keep this one piece of information in
mind to re-create the entire board.
161
Lesson 19 How AI Can Enhance Creativity
toddlers can still best supercomputers at things we find easy, such as seeing
and walking. But machines are catching up.
● When Deep Blue beat Kasparov in 1997, there was still a feeling that AI
could never really mimic human intelligence in any meaningful way.
After all, even Deep Blue used brute force rather than pattern recognition,
chunked knowledge, or other “human” methods, such as reading body
language, overcoming exhaustion, and avoiding mistakes. It just so
happens that chess can be won by brute force. But there’s another game
that cannot.
● The Chinese game Go, which is much more complex than chess, is played
on a 19-by-19 board of squares with 361 black and white stones. It’s too
big a matrix to allow brute-force methods like exhaustive search to thrive
and, as Kasparov writes, “too subtle to be decided by the tactical blunders
that define human losses to computers at chess.” Instead, Go* is revered as
a uniquely human game, requiring creativity, intuition, and perseverance.
It’s a game of 9 simple rules but many complex strategies. The number
of possible positions on the board exceeds the number of atoms in the
observable universe.
● That’s why it was so shocking to Musk and many others tracking the
ascent of AI when, in 2016, DeepMind’s AlphaGo beat one of the world’s
best Go players: 33-year-old Lee Sedol, who had 18 world championships
under his belt at the time. AI expert and Taiwanese venture capitalist Kai-
Fu Lee calls it China’s Sputnik moment, as it launched a frenzy of research
and work in AI in the East.
● There was a time when computer experts thought that AI would never
beat a human at Go. Sedol himself confidently predicted a 5-to-0 sweep
against the machine. He lost 4 games and only won 1. But what is most
remarkable about AlphaGo is not so much that it beat arguably the best
human player but that it learned to do so on its own (sort of).
162
to play against versions of itself, using a technique called reinforcement
learning. This is the key difference: Programmers can’t predict the moves
that the AI will make; they emerge from the extensive training.
● As Kasparov noted, Deep Blue was the end of human mastery of chess;
AlphaGo is the beginning of truly intelligent AI. Deep Blue represents a
form of AI that capitalizes on the strength of machines to crank through
permutations—what we’ve been calling brute force. The reason your free
chess app can best a grandmaster is because in chess, there are a finite
number of moves and a finite number of possible games. The computer
just has to cycle through all of them to figure out which one will lead to a
checkmate. In a sense, the chess app isn’t thinking; it’s just searching.
● What’s fascinating about this matchup is how the computers, whether it’s
Deep Blue in chess or AlphaGo in Go, affected their human counterparts.
After losing to Deep Blue, Kasparov became obsessed with computer chess,
and it had a profound influence on how he’s spent the rest of his career.
163
Lesson 19 How AI Can Enhance Creativity
● At Yale, Donya Quick has a computer program that can emulate Bach
and fool even the most sophisticated experts. But it can go a step further,
creating completely novel sounds and compositions. The program, called
Kulitta, can expertly meld styles, incorporating rules from classical and
jazz genres or composing music that doesn’t adhere to any of those rules.
Listening to Kulitta’s work makes you wonder what Bach might have
composed had he lived through the golden age of jazz.
● There are now robot reporters, chefs, painters, poets, and DJs. Granted,
many of them aren’t fooling the experts yet, though Forbes.com has even
outsourced the writing of corporate earnings previews to a company
called Narrative Science that generates them using an AI. They’re hard to
distinguish from what a human would write.
● Even if these artistic robots aren’t quite at the level of creative humans,
there are plenty of ways we can learn from them, both in terms of the
content they produce, which can lead us to think outside the box, and how
they learn to do it.
● But that’s true, too, of the vast majority of the decisions that we make,
including those of judges, mortgage brokers, and doctors. The difference is
that we can force makers of AI to find out the cause of the decision—but
we can’t force it out of other humans.
164
machine would have to explain how it came to make that choice: Was is a
particular feature, a relationship between features, a probabilistic map of
features? And in this, we’re bound to gain a deeper understanding of our
own metacognition and ultimately even our own consciousness.
● Perhaps the best use of AI, and the least scary, is as a partner. Since
computers are much better than we are at brute-force calculations and
search, we can outsource those tasks and benefit greatly.
● Say you want to build a new car. There are many different parts and
options to consider, each with its own pros and cons. By building a digital
replica of your design, you can simultaneously test many different versions
using digital simulations.
● Before AI, engineers had to do much of this work themselves, testing and
retesting prototypes. But now, they can outsource it to machines and get
answers much more quickly. This means that they can spend the extra
time on the more creative tasks—innovating and connecting—which is
what we humans do better (so far) than our computer counterparts.
● So which part of the process, exactly, is the most human—the least amenable
to outsourcing to a computer? Probably the spark. But even this stage doesn’t
come out of a vacuum. And there are many ways that AI and other types of
computer programs can help us with each of the other stages.
165
Lesson 19 How AI Can Enhance Creativity
● What if that foreign friend or partner was, in fact, an AI? Kasparov, Sedol,
and others who have interacted closely with AI report that their creative
work has benefitted from the interactions. Diversity helps us think outside
the box, and what’s more diverse than a machine that solves complex
human problems in a distinctly inhuman way?
Questions to Consider
1 What did Garry Kasparov and Lee Sedol learn from being bested by
computers?
166
Do We Trust
20
Algorithms
over Humans?
167
Lesson 20 Do We Trust Algorithms over Humans?
Machine Learning
● Consider mortgage lending decisions. Who gets a mortgage loan, and for
how much, should be a computational decision in which the risks and
potential revenues are weighed and assessed. A mortgage broker should
not be swayed by the desperate pleas of a single mother or the flashy car a
client shows up in. Mortgage brokers should be able to plug in the relevant
numbers, such as measures of employment security and income potential,
assets, and liabilities, and come up with a loan amount that the applicant
can afford.
168
a series of logical rules that the programmers code in. If an applicant has
more assets than liabilities, then a mortgage might be a good fit.
● But then you still just get computers that are as smart as, but not smarter
than, humans. What if you could teach the algorithm to teach itself rather
than telling it what to do?
● Computer scientists who rejected the expert systems approach had the
goal of making the algorithm smarter than them. So they turned to the
most intelligent system they could find: the human brain. This strategy is
called the neural networks approach because it more or less mimics certain
aspects of human brain function. Layers of artificial neurons are built on
top of each other, and they send and receive information much like our
own brain cells.
● Babies are incredibly effective learners, even though they aren’t given
explicit instructions. So why not try the same approach to train an AI?
● Neural networks do just that. They extract regularities in the data that
they are fed and find the patterns and write their own rules. And they’re
not new. AI pioneers in the 1950s and 1960s were already experimenting
with neural networks. But they found them to be fairly limited, so they
were relegated to the fringes of computer science.
* According to a study in which the scientists recorded all the sounds that
babies were exposed to over the course of many days, babies hear an average
of about 12,000 adult words a day. So by the time they’re 3 and speaking for the
most part in full sentences, they’ve heard some 15 million words.
169
Lesson 20 Do We Trust Algorithms over Humans?
● By the early 2000s, the availability of computing power and data had made
neural networks much more powerful. But there was still the problem of how
well they were trained, or what algorithms they used to make decisions.
Deep Learning
● We don’t teach babies how to recognize faces, yet almost as soon as their
visual acuity is sufficiently precise to distinguish facial features, they begin
to stare intently at faces and can distinguish their caregivers from, say, the
dog. This sounds trivial, but anyone who has tried to program a computer
to do the same thing will tell you that it’s hard.
● Real faces are hard, but they’re not as hard as pictures of faces or other
objects, which are 2-dimensional. A baby can recognize its primary
170
caregiver’s face from photographs. Once babies learn a concept like dog,
they can apply it to many different types of dogs, even ones they’ve never
seen before. Think about how hard that is. How do you decide what’s a
dog and what isn’t?
● How we learn concepts like dog has puzzled psychologists for decades.
We still don’t really know. If we don’t know how we do it, how can we
program a computer to do it?
● That’s where deep learning comes in. We just give the program the same
experience and the same (or a similar) neural architecture, and it figures
it out!
171
Lesson 20 Do We Trust Algorithms over Humans?
features (wagging tails, 4 legs, snout, etc.), and it spits out a decision: dog
or not-dog.
● Most people will accept decisions made by algorithms more readily than
those made by fellow, flawed humans. The belief that automated decisions
are better than human-generated ones, even when presented with evidence
that the decisions are incorrect, is called the automation bias. It’s been
observed in many different situations, including in ones in which the
consequences of a bad decision are truly catastrophic.
172
such as the fact that we tend to overestimate the frequency of an event that
we just heard or read about or that pops easily into mind.
● People tend to think that flying is more dangerous than driving because
when there is a plane crash, the media coverage is extensive. It’s easy to
forget that car crashes are much more common because we don’t think
about them very often.
● It’s pretty easy to imagine how an error of omission might occur in the
presence of an automated aid; you assume that the computer is paying
attention so that you don’t have to. For example, in 1972, the crew of an
Eastern Airlines plane set the autopilot to keep the plane at an altitude of
2000 feet while they tried to figure out why the landing gear indicator
light hadn’t turned on. But they failed to monitor their altitude, and
when air traffic control warned them that their landing gear had not, in
fact, dropped, they were only 30 feet above ground, and it was too late to
prevent the crash.
● But what about errors of commission? These are errors made when a person
follows the lead of an automated aid—even though the choice being made
is incorrect and even goes against many hours of training.
173
Lesson 20 Do We Trust Algorithms over Humans?
● But there was also one opportunity to make an error of commission. The
pilots received a message mid-flight that one of their engines had caught
fire. This message was contradicted by normal engine parameters and the
absence of 5 other indicators that normally would be present if there were
indeed a fire. They were even reminded of these indicators during the
simulation training.
● But 100% of them chose to shut down the engine in response to the false
fire message. In a post-experimental questionnaire, though, they said that
the one message would not be sufficient to diagnose a fire in the absence
of other indicators and that in that situation, it would be safer not to shut
down the engine.*
* Additionally, 67% of them reported a false memory that there was another
indicator present during their simulation.
174
● In many aspects of our society, there is bias baked into our history, so if we
use what happened before to predict what will happen, bias will come with
it. But if we can systematically program algorithms to become less biased
over time, could we use them to better society and eliminate bias?
● While we’re still learning how they work, it’s important to have regulatory
bodies checking algorithms for unintended consequences and looking
under the hood whenever possible.
Questions to Consider
175
Could
21
Blockchain
Revolutionize
Society?
176
B lockchain is a social technology that has the power
to reknit the fabric of society by changing our
approach to currency and exchange. And it’s already
affecting many different aspects of our lives, without
our even knowing it.
● The internet changed the way data and knowledge flow between people
and organizations. This information has become vast, but it’s also easily
changed and easily lost. So that means that it’s not particularly reliable. It’s
not trustworthy.
● That’s what blockchain was meant to do. You might call it in the internet
of value, as Don and Alex Tapscott do in their book Blockchain Revolution.
177
Lesson 21 Could Blockchain Revolutionize Society?
● It’s been immensely successful because customers and sellers can interact
through the site and still be reasonably sure that they won’t be defrauded
because there’s this added layer of security through Alipay.
● Engaging with the internet requires a leap of faith, since it also affords
some anonymity, which Ma realized as he founded Alibaba. We rely on
intermediaries like Alipay, PayPal, and other services to keep us all honest
and happy. In this way, the internet is shaping who and how we trust.
178
● When we exchange value, we want to make sure that we don’t duplicate
the transaction. If you give someone $100 for a bike, you can’t give that
same $100 to someone else. And you don’t want the seller to sell that bike
to someone else at the same time, either. You both want the originals.
● But how do you know that the seller hasn’t sold the bike to anyone
else? And how does the seller know that you haven’t given that $100 to
anyone else?
● We could write it down in a ledger, which anyone can access—but it’s the
21st century, so let’s digitize it. That’s essentially what the blockchain is: a
digital diary of value exchanges.
● But even banks, with their expensive security systems, can get hacked.
Can’t someone just change the ledger to benefit themselves?
● That’s the core genius of the blockchain. It’s really, really hard—some use
the word impossible—to fake or hack.
● Each entry in the diary is tagged with something called a hash, basically
a string of letters and numbers. But each new entry is also tagged with
all the previous entries, so it’s like a hash plus a record of all the other
transactions. So if you want to change an entry, you have to change all the
entries that came before it, too.
● But that still sounds hackable. After all, computer algorithms can batch-
change things.
● In any case, the right nonce is hard to find, and finding it is proof that a lot
of work was done. That means that changing it, and all the other nonces,
would be even more work—so much so that it can’t be done very quickly.
179
Lesson 21 Could Blockchain Revolutionize Society?
● Now if the ledger were only stored in one location, presumably you
could take the necessary time and computing power to figure out each
puzzle and change every nonce. But what if it’s simultaneously stored in
its entirety on 5000 computers, distributed across the globe? Now this is
getting harder to hack.
● Additionally, every time there’s a new entry, or nonce, the set of computers,
or nodes, that each contains a copy of the original record has to approve of
the entry, validating it by checking it against the versions of the blockchain
that it has. If a majority of these nodes approves the entry, it gets added to
the chain.
● The blocks in the blockchain are essentially groups of entries (say, one
spreadsheet in a ledger). Then, they are chained together because each
block also refers to all the previous blocks—hence the chain. It’s spread
over many computers, and the entries are constantly checked by people
who earn or have value embedded in the blockchain. Finally, it’s time-
stamped and updated every 10 minutes.
● It’s the Fort Knox of ledgers. Note that there’s no centralized node; it’s
completely decentralized and works on consensus. That’s why it has the
power to change the world.
● Mt. Gox now serves as a cautionary tale, and individuals and exchanges
are much more careful about protecting their private keys. And this also
180
illustrates that the hacking of blockchains happens at its edges—the entry
and exit points—rather than in the ledger itself.
● What’s more, there are plenty of companies that use blockchain, more or
less successfully. Just as a company is responsible for its own IT, which can
be good or bad (secure internet connectivity or an easily hackable Wi-Fi
network), different uses of the basic idea behind blockchain have different
strengths and vulnerabilities.
● There is also the problem of how the anonymity that blockchain technology
provides lends itself to supporting shady activities. For example, the Silk
Road* was a marketplace on the dark web, the part of the internet that is
only accessible if you have specific kinds of software that allow users to
* The Silk Road marketplace was founded and run by Ross Ulbricht, who called
himself Dread Pirate Roberts, or DPR, online.
181
Lesson 21 Could Blockchain Revolutionize Society?
● People talk about how the gig economy, fueled by tech companies like
Airbnb and Uber, has disrupted the workforce by decimating the value of
taxi medallions and hotel rooms. Financial industries have been relatively
unaffected in comparison, but blockchain has the power to change that.
● But a blockchain Airbnb could still give users access to inventory and
reviews. And just as Uber destroyed the jobs of many taxi drivers, a ride-
sharing blockchain could destroy Uber’s headquarters. Blockchain presents
an opportunity for the creation of a truly decentralized economy.
● Because it is so easy to lie online, we don’t really trust what we read and
whom we encounter on the internet. But with blockchain technology,
we could become much more confident in the truthfulness of what the
internet provides. Imagine a kind of ledger of honest acts or integrity: A
person’s reputation could follow him or her to a new destination, opening
doors more easily. A person from a developing country in Africa could
prove his or her trustworthiness with a digital wallet and begin transacting
business in a new place immediately.
182
A New Model of Identity
● In the developed world, we have to protect our identity, and our actions
online and off are surveilled by any number of organizations, from tech
giants like Facebook to the government. But if you’re living in a developing
country, you might have the opposite problem. Even in the US, 7% of
people don’t have any identification, making it impossible for them to vote
or open a bank account.
* Even refugee camps, set up to help the poorest of the poor and the
disenfranchised, are exploited by traffickers who recognize this vulnerability.
183
Lesson 21 Could Blockchain Revolutionize Society?
● Blockchain could also change how we define our identity. The internet has
allowed us to craft our digital personas to some extent, and now we could
extend that control over our identities offline, too.
● Blockchain could disrupt our economy in ways that could help eliminate
poverty and inequality. It could bring the billions of poor people without
access to a bank into the world’s exchanges.
● Michael Casey, coauthor of The Truth Machine: The Blockchain and the
Future of Everything, thinks that this technology has the potential to make
the world “flatter”: to take the power away from the major value holders on
the internet, such as Google and Apple, and hand it back to the people.
● The core idea is that if you incentivize people to take care of a ledger by
giving them a piece of the value of the ledger and then you make it really
hard to cheat, you’re building trust on the backs of many, many individuals
rather than on an institution. Perhaps in the future the very nature of how
and who we trust will be fundamentally changed by this new technology.
Questions to Consider
2 What are the ways in which blockchain can help poor people?
3 What are the benefits and costs of putting our identity into digital
formats?
184
Effects of
22
Technological
Metaphors on
Science
185
Lesson 22 Effects of Technological Metaphors on Science
● From about the late 1940s up until the last decade, the computer has been
the dominant metaphor used by scientists to understand the brain.
186
● Back then, as Paul Cisek describes in an influential paper published
in 1999 in the Journal of Consciousness Studies, the computer metaphor
provided mechanisms for 4 unanswered questions puzzling neuroscientists.
● This computer metaphor was and remains so compelling that it’s hard
to talk about the brain without invoking it. We talk about processes,
representations, wiring, programming, encoding, storage, etc.
● But it’s in part because metaphors are so influential that they also can
limit understanding. After all, they are not wholly correct. And they can
influence our thinking in ways that we’re not fully aware of. Research has
shown that metaphors can nudge our thinking in one direction or another
and can lead to confusion when they’re not quite mapping on to the
concept that we’re trying to understand—and that we’re not very good at
noticing their influence.
● Some scientists have argued that the computer metaphor has become
so pervasive when talking about the brain that we almost can’t describe
cognition or brain function without referring to it. We talk about
cognitive functions like working memory, decision-making, and analogical
reasoning as distinguishable components with specific hardware—or
neuroanatomical underpinnings.
187
Lesson 22 Effects of Technological Metaphors on Science
● Brains also evolve, following the rules of evolution, just like any other
organ or organism. Computers don’t. They are engineered for specific
environments; they don’t adapt, for the most part. There are 2 main
corollaries of this fact that are often ignored because they are not part of
the computer metaphor.
● First, Mother Nature is a tinkerer, which means that brains reuse, recycle,
and refine existing functions or traits, and studying their building blocks
in other animals can provide many insights into how they work in humans.
Consciousness is a great example. We tend to think of consciousness as an
emergent property of the human brain, often like an on-off switch. But
consciousness is by no means binary. It appears in more primitive forms
in animals who feel pain or other emotion analogs, or who can remember
both what and where something happened, or who behave as though they
understand justice.
● The second problem with ignoring the evolutionary trajectory of the brain is
that while computers are designed, hopefully optimally, to perform certain
tasks, brains are not. So even if we can do those tasks, we probably don’t do
them as effectively or efficiently as we could if they had been engineered.
● Memory is a great example of this problem. It’s hard not to think of memory
as a 3-stage process that involves encoding some information, then storing it,
and finally retrieving it, with little to no alteration between steps.
● But that’s far from how human memory actually works. We can encode
and store information that we are not even aware of, and it can influence
188
our behavior outside of our explicit knowledge. We also alter memories
whenever we retrieve them; instead of pulling out a file and reading it as it
was written, we rewrite it as we retrieve it! Memory is constructive, rather
than veridical or objective.
● In fact, memory isn’t even about the past at all; instead, it’s a tool we use
to help us predict the future. And this is not at all what memory is in
a computer. So when we compare our fallible memories with what we
can store in bits on a silicon chip, we can’t help but feel inadequate. But
if we consider memory a way of harnessing the past to imagine future
consequences, then we realize that what we’re capable of exceeds any
computer ever built (so far).
● And that’s the problem with how powerful the computer metaphor has
become: It’s influenced scientific thinking to the extent that it’s now
starting to hold us back.
● But unlike in computers, function also dictates anatomy in the brain. With
training, you can repurpose another part of your brain to take on speech
function or even regain some function in the part that was damaged.
189
Lesson 22 Effects of Technological Metaphors on Science
● Some cognitive scientists have argued that the mind and brain are so
distinct conceptually that studying the brain tells us nothing about the
mind. There’s some truth to that: Many neuroimaging studies have been
overinterpreted by either their own authors or the media.
● Just because listening to music activates your reward system, that doesn’t
mean it’s just like an addictive drug, which also activates overlapping
circuits. If your amygdala lights up when you see an ad for a political party
whose policies you disagree with, it doesn’t necessarily mean that you’re
afraid, even though we know that the amygdala plays a role in learned fears
and is active when we are exposed to something we’ve learned to fear. But
it does much more than that.
● If you feel scared when you see a rival political ad and your amygdala lights
up, then we might be able to say that the amygdala activation underlies the
fear you feel. But so what? We know you’re scared because you said so or
because we saw other types of behavioral changes indicative of fear, such
as sweaty palms or nervous fidgeting. What does the amygdala activation
information add?
● Cognitive scientists have even set out to prove this particular point. One
group took a microchip and threw a number of cutting-edge neuroscience
tools at it. They mapped its connectome, tracking all the connections
between different parts. They lesioned parts of it at a time to observe the
effects of damage to different parts. They measured local field potentials,
the electrical activity of the circuit. They learned a lot. But they didn’t get
anywhere near understanding what it was actually for—or what it does
and how it works. It turns out that it was a chip made by Atari with which
people could play video games.
190
Memory
● Even if we mapped out every cell, every connection, and every chemical
and electrical change in a massively complicated human brain, would we
understand consciousness? The argument here is no.
● But here again is where the computer metaphor fails and even leads us
astray. Take memory as an example. Computer memory fails only when
the hardware fails. But if everything is in working order, you’ll get the
same answer to a query every time.
● But over time, some memories are so ingrained that the pattern of
activation that represents them is not the same as when they were first
encoded. So you can damage the part of the brain where they were initially
laid down and still extract information about what happened because there
are intact parts of the brain where the information now resides.
191
Lesson 22 Effects of Technological Metaphors on Science
● We can’t talk about memory without referring to terms like encoding (the
process by which information or experience gets into the brain), storage
(how it’s represented, whether in a pattern of neural firing or the strength
of connections between neurons or some molecular changes), and retrieval
(how the activity of the brain yields the experience of pulling up the
memory or information).
192
Questions to Consider
193
Robots and
23
the Future of
Work
194
D uring the information age, we designed computers—
machines that can do things for us. Initially that meant
things like counting and monotonous computations, but
now they can play chess, compose music, and learn. In
fact, we’ve built machines that are so good at humanlike
tasks that they are putting many humans out of work.
Now people are talking about the coming of the second
machine age, this time with machines that are smarter
than we are. What will the future human workforce look
like, and how should we prepare ourselves and the next
generation for the robot revolution?
Smart AI
● But a machine can’t have goals, right? Tell that to a heat-seeking missile or
even a Roomba. Conscious, evil robots are not what artificial intelligence (AI)
researchers have nightmares about, but they do worry about the unintended
consequences of smart AI and that someday AI might become so smart that it
prevents us from tweaking its settings or changing its programming.
195
Lesson 23 Robots and the Future of Work
● But programmers being human, and humans being unable to consider all
the myriad consequences of complex actions, there is plenty of room for
error and misalignment of goals.
● Tesla and Uber want to build self-driving trucks, which will save the
trucking industry money and presumably be easier on the environment,
196
as they can be powered by electricity. In order to even be allowed on the
roads, they’ll be safer than human-driven trucks, which kill 4000 people
a year in the United States alone. And they will address the shortage of
drivers in the trucking industry, which is estimated to increase to 175,000
jobs by 2024.
● Sure, some truck drivers might be out a job, but other jobs will be created,
industry insiders say. And the self-driving truck innovation illustrates a
scenario that many techno-optimists hope for across all industries: We
leave the things that humans find challenging or boring to the computers
and free up time to do the things we really love to do. Would a trucker
prefer to have to focus on the road for long stretches of highway or take
that time to FaceTime with his or her kids or even exercise in the back of
the truck?
● Since 2000, the situation has gotten worse for workers, as owners have
been taking home more of the corporate profit, leaving employees with less
and less. This trend will not be improved with automation. In fact, unless
something changes, those who own the machines will get an even bigger
proportion of profits than the workers.
197
Lesson 23 Robots and the Future of Work
and Apple and streaming media producers like Netflix can afford to hire
fewer employees and still be among the world’s most valuable companies.
● Given the growth of the tech industry, it would make sense for parents to
encourage their kids to learn to code and become proficient at computing.
And having a general understanding of programming and other ways of
utilizing digital tools is likely essential for most workers moving forward.
● But among AI experts and futurists like Kai-Fu Lee and Max Tegmark,
there’s an interesting trend: They are not advising their kids to follow
in their footsteps, instead telling them to find jobs in industries that
are not predicted to be in imminent danger of being fully automated or
revolutionized by AI.
● Lee suggests that nursing and other caregiving professions will grow
and that jobs in accounting will shrink. Tegmark advises kids to ask 3
questions about potential careers:
— Does the job require interacting with people and using social
intelligence?
198
— Does it require working in an unpredictable environment?
● These questions are telling in that they capture what Tegmark and others
see as job characteristics that will be hard for computers to master.
● That’s why teaching coding to everyone might not be the best way to
prepare the majority of students for the robot revolution. You don’t need to
be an expert in French literature to speak French.
● Daron Acemoglu and David Autor at MIT suggest dividing work into
a 2-by-2 matrix: On one side are tasks that are cognitive versus manual,
and on the other are tasks that are routine versus nonroutine. Routine
manual tasks are ones that were most adversely affected by the industrial
revolution. They also won’t fare well in the coming robot revolution.
● But the same goes for routine cognitive jobs like tax preparation
and dressmaking. Nonroutine tasks—whether they are manual, like
hairdressing, or cognitive, like science—will be most likely to weather the
coming changes, and even grow.
199
Lesson 23 Robots and the Future of Work
● What machines are good at—and will only get better at—includes
computation and programming. Soon, robots and other AI will become
too complex for the majority of us to understand, even with extensive
training and education. So teaching all kids to code as a way of preparing
them for the future might be misguided.
● What every kid will most likely have to do is work alongside machines in
some capacity. That’s why a little coding is important; they need to be able
to run and maybe fix their coworking robots.
200
would not ride in a self-driving vehicle. Add a human supervisor and the
majority of people in the survey say they would feel better about it.
● The question, then, is what does the human supervisor add? When it
comes to road safety, humans are generally poor decision-makers, choosing
to drive under the influence or without seat belts or at illegally high speeds.
AI wouldn’t do any of these things.
● Most people who say that they wouldn’t want to ride in a driverless car
express not wanting to cede control to a machine that might be making life-
and-death decisions. But we cede that control to a fellow human every time
we ride with one and to many humans when we join them on busy roads.
● Thinking that a machine would make a less ethical choice than a human
is a great example of how poorly we understand ourselves. After all, there’s
plenty of evidence that humans make poor choices behind the wheel, ones
that are self-serving or simply reckless, such as texting while driving. The
self-driving car in the future will never make that mistake.
● There are 6 million car accidents in the US every year, and 90 people die
every day, despite the fact that we’ve had many decades to perfect this
particular skill. Clearly, we’re just not that good at driving.
201
Lesson 23 Robots and the Future of Work
● Ultimately, what this and other research shows is that our understanding
of what computers are good and bad at, and what are our own strengths
and weaknesses as humans are, is flawed. And we’ll continue to be
surprised by what we can and cannot outsource to AI and robots.
● But what’s clear is that the second machine age—the robot revolution or
the automation of the workforce—is already disrupting not only what
we do for work but how we should educate our kids. And while we need
to heed Elon Musk’s call to be thoughtful about the consequences of
continuing to develop ever smarter AI, we also need to recognize that AI is
already shaping us and our opportunities.
Questions to Consider
202
Redefining
24
What It Means
to Be Human
203
Lesson 24 Redefining What It Means to Be Human
● The fact that we aren’t perfect is, of course, not the only thing we learn
from studying robots, AI, and other technological innovations.
● In one experiment, Nicholas Christakis, who runs the Human Nature Lab
at Yale, and his colleagues had participants work on tablets alongside a cute
humanoid robot to lay railroad tracks in a virtual world. In one condition,
the robot made fairly bland comments, ones that we expect robots to
make. In the other condition, the robot made mistakes and owned up to
them. “Sorry, guys,” it would say in a perky voice. “I know it might be hard
to believe but robots make mistakes too.”
● It turns out that the human participants not only liked that robot more,
but they also worked better together: The confessional robot, as Christakis
calls it, helped the humans communicate with each other and collaborate
more effectively.
204
connect to our devices instead. But what Christakis and others have shown
is that technology can both nudge us toward more prosocial behavior and
away from it.
● Early in the game, human players acted generously about 2/3 of the time,
expecting that if they made the donation in one round, their altruism
might be reciprocated in subsequent rounds. But when the experimenters
added a few bots that posed as humans but behaved selfishly, keeping all
the money themselves round after round, the human players eventually
stopped cooperating. The actions of these bots altered the behavior
of thousands of humans, changing them from, in Christakis’s words,
“generous people to selfish jerks.”
Among all the Twitter users included in the study, there was about
an equal number of liberal- and conservative-leaning bots (basically
nonhuman trolls). The study found that even a small number
of bots—about 6% of users studied—can influence the national
conversation if we let it, at least as it’s measured by tweets.
● In better news, Kevin Munger at New York University ran a study to see
whether a gentle reminder from a compassionate bot might be able to make
Twitter users nicer. His study showed that under the right circumstances,
205
Lesson 24 Redefining What It Means to Be Human
some humans can behave better after being chastened by a bot, as long and
they respect or relate to the type of human being that the bot is imitating.
● There’s little doubt that as long as we don’t know it’s not human, AI in its
various forms can affect even our most humanlike behavior: how we treat
each other.
● It must be the brain, then. But here, too, there is controversy as to what is
considered alive or dead. Every year or so there’s some story in the news
about family members who disagree with doctors or each other as to
whether their comatose loved one is still technically alive or if, with limited
brain function, the loved one is past the point of no return.
206
● This definition harkens back to what the nervous system in any animal is for:
to sense the environment and act accordingly. Without the ability to sense, or
to react, the animal, human or otherwise, is no longer considered alive.
● If it were possible to keep the brain sensing and reacting with the aid of
implants or medications—what cardiology has done for the heart—would
that brain still be human?
● One way to think about this problem is to ask how much of a biological
brain we need to have in order to still be human. If we replace a few cells
with a chip, then it seems obvious that we’re still human. But what about
replacing an entire region? Or an entire hemisphere? What about 80% of
cells? Or 90%? What is the cutoff?
● Turing himself did not use the word consciousness. He was only concerned
with whether the machine was capable of exhibiting intelligent behavior.
His view was that in order to assess whether a computer can think,
we need to define thinking and that we can only objectively do so by
evaluating actions. And there have since been machines that have passed
the Turing test by brute force and following rules, rather than thinking on
their own.
● Perhaps the Turing test is no longer sufficient or needs an update. But what
if we had a biomarker for consciousness—an objective signal that we could
measure? We do have something like this for human consciousness in the
form of brain imaging.
207
Lesson 24 Redefining What It Means to Be Human
208
along our visual processing stream first pull apart the visual world and
then put it back together again, seemingly instantaneously and in parallel.
● There’s another problem with laying out the neural correlates of consciousness:
Different subjective experiences have different neural signatures. Your
brain activity will look one way when you’re imagining playing tennis and
another way when you’re contemplating what it means to be human. So far,
neuroscientists have failed to find the exact signature of subjective awareness or
any universally-agreed-upon definition of consciousness.
209
Lesson 24 Redefining What It Means to Be Human
unable to store new information for more than a few seconds, still
experience consciousness, so it doesn’t need to last very long.
— The system needs to be fairly independent from the rest of the world
so that it can have a sense of its subjective awareness as being different
from what surrounds it. Though there is still room for embodiment, as
long as the embodiment is separable from the environment.
● We’re a long way off from building an AI that has all of these
characteristics, but what Tegmark is arguing is that we don’t need the
biology of the brain to support consciousness. So the human with 99%
of brain matter replaced by computer chips remains conscious as long as
that human meets these conditions. This gradual-replacement thought
experiment has been used in debates between AI experts and among
neuroscientists, philosophers, and theoreticians of consciousness without a
compelling solution.
● Tegmark also argues that much of what feels to us like subjective awareness,
free will, or consciousness is actually the result of activity in the brain—
call it computations, processing, or whatever new metaphor the next
technological innovation will hand us—that occurs outside of our awareness.
● When you decide whether to buy a house, your decision is not a simple
analysis of pros and cons, the product of considered, rational, conscious,
deliberate thinking. It’s the end result of a whole series of prior events,
including emotional reactions and fast, automatic, intuitive thinking
influencing your deliberate thinking. You aren’t aware of many of these
influences. You come to a decision and then you have the subjective
experience of having gotten there entirely of your own free will.
● Sure, it’s your brain doing the computations (or whatever you want to call
them), so in that sense, you do own the decision. But it’s not entirely free.
In the most sophisticated AI systems, we don’t know the outcome until
we let AI system run through the computations. In Tegmark’s argument,
the computation is the decision and subjective experience is how the
210
computing feels from the inside. So perhaps AI already knows what it’s like
to be an AI.
Questions to Consider
211
Bibliography
Recommended Books
Botsman, Rachel. Who Can You Trust? How Technology Brought Us Together
and Why It Might Drive Us Apart. PublicAffairs, 2017.
Carr, Nicholas. The Shallows: What the Internet Is Doing to Our Brains. W. W.
Norton, 2010.
Greenfield, Susan. Mind Change: How Digital Technologies Are Leaving Their
Mark on Our Brains. Random House, 2015.
Kasparov, Garry. Deep Thinking: Where Machine Intelligence Ends and Human
Creativity Begins. PublicAffairs, 2017.
Lee, Kai-Fu. AI Superpowers: China, Silicon Valley, and the New World Order.
Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2018.
Newport, Cal. Deep Work: Rules for Focused Success in a Distracted World.
Grand Central, 2016.
Tapscott, Don, and Alex Tapscott. Blockchain Revolution: How the Technology
behind Bitcoin Is Changing Money, Business, and the World. Reprint ed.
Portfolio, 2018.
Tegmark, Max. Life 3.0: Being Human in the Age of Artificial Intelligence.
Vintage, 2017.
Wolfe, Maryanne. Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading
Brain. Reprint ed. Harper Perennial, 2008.
212
———. Reader, Come Home: The Reading Brain in a Digital World.
Harper, 2018.
Wu, Tim. The Attention Merchants: The Epic Scramble to Get Inside Our
Heads. Knopf, 2016.
Recommended Articles
Allcott, H., and M. Gentzkow. “Social Media and Fake News in the 2016
Election.” Journal of Economic perspectives 31, no. 2 (2017): 211–236.
APA Task Force on Violent Media. Technical Report on the Review of the
Violent Video Game Literature. August 2015. https://www.apa.org/pi/
families/review-video-games.pdf.
213
Bibliography
Cukier, K., and V. Mayer-Schoenberger. “The Rise of Big Data: How It’s
Changing the Way We Think about the World.” Foreign Affairs 92, no. 3
(2013): 28–40.
214
Ekeland, A. G., A. Bowes, and S. Flottorp. “Effectiveness of Telemedicine: A
Systematic Review of Reviews.” International Journal of Medical Informatics
79, no. 11 (2010): 736–771.
215
Bibliography
Hesse, B. W. “The Patient, the Physician, and Dr. Google.” AMA Journal of
Ethics 14, no. 5 (2012): 398–402.
Hsee, C. K., and F. Leclerc. “Products Look More Attractive When Presented
Separately or Together?” Journal of Consumer Research 25, no. 2 (1998):
175–186.
216
Hyler, S. E., D. P. Gangure, and S. T. Batchelder. “Can Telepsychiatry
Replace In-Person Psychiatrics Assessments? A Review and Meta-Analysis
of Comparison Studies.” CNS Spectrum 10, no. 5 (2005): 403–413.
217
Bibliography
Landripet, I., and A. Štulhofer. “Is Pornography Use Associated with Sexual
Difficulties and Dysfunctions among Younger Heterosexual Men?” The
Journal of Sexual Medicine 12 (2015): 1136–1139.
218
com/~/media/McKinsey/Featured%20Insights/Digital%20Disruption/
Harnessing%20automation%20for%20a%20future%20that%20works/
MGI-A-future-that-works_Full-report.ashx.
219
Bibliography
220
Rideout, V. “The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to
Eight.” Common Sense Media, 2017, https://www.commonsensemedia.
org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-kids-age-zero-to-
eight-2017.
Romer, D., and M. Moreno. “Digital Media and Risks for Adolescent
Substance Abuse and Problematic Gambling.” Pediatrics 140, no. 2 (2017).
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758L.
Roose, K. “The Making of a YouTube Radical.” The New York Times, 2019,
https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2019/06/08/technology/youtube-
radical.html?auth=login-email.
221
Bibliography
Taylor, C., and B. M. Dewsbury. “On the Problem and Promise of Metaphor
Use in Science and Science Communication.” Journal of Microbiology and
Biology Education 19, no. 1 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1128/jmbe.v19i1.1538.
222
Warraich, H. “Dr. Google Is a Liar.” The New York Times, 2018, https://www.
nytimes.com/2018/12/16/opinion/statin-side-effects-cancer.html.
223
Image Credits
224