GROUP 8 Written Report
GROUP 8 Written Report
Group 8
WHAT IS TRAIT
A specific characteristic of an individual
Physical Traits
● Height
● Body somatotype
● Muscle composition
● Kinesthesis
● Handedness
● Perceptual abilities
● Motor abilities
● Personality Traits
● Interpersonal skills
● Self-awareness
● Motivation
● Behavior control
● Aggression
Emotional Traits
● Feelings
● Interests
● Curiosity
● Empathy
● Maturity
Individuals differ significantly in their capacity to perform and to learn motor skills. Some
people seem good at almost every new motor skill they attempt; others experience difficulty
when first attempting new physical skills. Most people appear average at the majority of
skills. Still others are good at one or a few skills but frustratingly unskilled at others. The
Encyclopedia of Social Psychology (Baumeister & Vohs, 2007) defines individual differences
in terms of enduring psychological characteristics. Individual differences are the more-or-less
enduring psychological characteristics that distinguish one person from another and thus
help to define each person's individuality. Among the most important kinds of individual
differences are intelligence, personality traits, and values.
Motor Abilities
● Are defined as genetically determined traits that are stable and enduring
and that underlie or support the performance of motor skills. There are four
features of this definition that are important to note.
● First, motor abilities are genetically determined. Like hair color or sex or
height, motor abilities are determined at conception. They are not
developed or acquired after birth. A person is born with all of the motor
abilities (and all of each of their abilities) that they will ever possess.
Everyone is also born with the same motor abilities—people do not have
different ones or a different number of abilities. People do, however, differ in
the relative strength of their inherited motor abilities. Just as with intellectual
traits, so also people possess motor abilities on a range from weak to
average to strong relative to the population as a whole (i.e., they are
normally distributed).
● Second, motor abilities are stable and enduring. They do not change over
the course of a person’s lifetime, and they cannot be improved with practice
(or lost from non-use). Motor abilities may vary as a result of developmental
factors, of course. An infant does not possess as fast a reaction time as an
adult, for example, and most adults will experience some reduction in
reaction time as they age beyond a certain point. But these changes
represent developmental processes rather than underlying alterations to
motor ability structures.
● Third, motor abilities underlie or support the performance of motor
skills. Motor abilities are considered the building blocks of motor skills. It is
important to distinguish here between abilities and skills. We learn skills
through practice, and they are directed toward the accomplishment of
specific environmental goals. Motor abilities are the “basic equipment” that
people are born with and from which they draw in order to learn and to
perform skills.
● Fourth, motor abilities are trans-situational. That is to say, a given motor
ability may be manifested in a wide variety of skill situations. Any given
motor ability may be an essential building block for a large number and
variety of different skills. Motor abilities are not specific to any particular
class or type of motor skill, but the same motor ability may cross many
classification lines in contributing to a large variety of actions. Conversely, a
motor ability may also play little or no role in the performance of other motor
skills.
● skilled performances of a motor ability like reaction time, for instance. The point
here is that motor abilities are the requirements for performing skills, as well as
the determinants for how well one can perform and learn particular skills, but not
all motor abilities are important, or as equally important, to all skills. In fact, most
motor skills probably depend on no more than a few motor abilities for their
performance, with three to five motor abilities most likely accounting for most of
the variance in skill observed between individual performers (Chaiken, Kyllonen,
and Tirre, 2000; Fleishman, 1972).
Depending on the particular cluster of motor abilities necessary for successful
performance in any given skill, people will be predisposed to certain limits on
their performance and ultimate learning of a skill based on the strength of their
relevant motor abilities. Motor abilities inform a ceiling or limit on both the
performance and learning of motor skills, with people manifesting differing limits
based upon their individual pattern and relative strength of motor abilities.
The only factor explaining these marked patterns of similar activity choices and
attained proficiency levels in identical twins is genetic determinism. Because
behavior choices do, at least to some extent, reflect an individual’s behavioral
strengths more than weaknesses, an argument can be sustained that the
similarity in genetic traits related to motor skill potential appears a likely
candidate for explaining similarity patterns in motor skill selection among twins.
Genetic predisposition to engage in and succeed at specific motor skills depends
on an individual’s underlying pattern of motor abilities .In contrast to twin studies
identifying interest and participation patterns, relatively few studies have
investigated physical and motor traits among twin samples. A number of
suggestive studies have been completed in recent years, however (Dowling,
2004; Segal, 1999). These studies involve actually bringing twins into
laboratories and measuring various physical and motor traits. Although research
in this area remains scant, some interesting conclusions are beginning to emerge
(Segal, 1999, p. 213). A general finding is that twins show considerable
similarities in some areas, but little similarity in others. Specifically, twin studies
have reported that twin pairs are extremely similar in reaction time and in
measures of agility and coordination, suggesting genetic influences on these
measures. They are no more similar than would be expected of non-twins in the
various physical measures of strength and endurance, however. An interesting
note is that similarities are observed in those traits classified as perceptual-motor
abilities in Fleishman’s Taxonomy, while those traits classified as physical
proficiency abilities show little or no genetic link among twin pairs
The most complete system for identifying and classifying motor abilities is one
developed in the mid-1960s by ergonomics engineer Edwin Fleishman.
Fleishman’s taxonomy of motor abilities is based on a tremendous amount of
research and resulted in the identification of 21 separate motor abilities. In his
classification system, Fleishman identified two broad categories of motor abilities
He labeled these categories perceptual-motor and physical proficiency abilities.
● The first, perceptual-motor abilities, is comprised of motor abilities for
which the central nervous system is the primary determinant of the
ability’s relative strength.
● The second, physical proficiency motor abilities, depends upon, in
addition to neurological factors, such underlying physiological factors as
muscle composition, the mechanical properties of muscle based on length
and origin and insertion locations, the efficiency of sensory receptors
associated with movement, and other physiological factors such as lung
volume and body somatotype.
Perceptual-Motor Abilities
● Multi Limb Coordination - Ability to coordinate the movement of a
number of limbs simultaneously. A high level of this ability is probably
important when serving a tennis ball or playing the piano.
● Wrist Finger Speed - Ability to rapidly move the wrist and fingers with
little or no accuracy demands. An example is playing the bongo drums or
keyboard entry tasks.
● Balance With Visual Cues - Ability to maintain total body balance when
visual cues are available. This ability is important for gymnasts who
perform on the balance beam.
● Speed Of Limb Movement - Ability to move the arms or legs quickly, but
without a reaction-time stimulus, to minimize movement time. Examples
include throwing a fast pitch in baseball or cricket or rapidly moving the
legs when tap dancing or clogging.
● Stamina - Ability to exert the entire body for a prolonged period of time; a
kind of cardiovascular endurance. Individuals requiring high levels of
stamina include distance runners and cyclists.
The Correlation among Motor Abilities: Testing the GMA and SMA Hypotheses The
search to confirm the GMA hypothesis was extensive in the 1950s and 1960s. Typically,
researchers would test individuals on different types of motor ability tests or actual motor
skills and determine the correlation between the various pairs of abilities or skills. It soon
became apparent, however, that even similar skills typically exhibited low correlations
between groups of subjects performing both skills. In fact, the correlation coefficients
observed in the many research investigations completed typically ranged in the very low
numbers.
Eventually, the inability to confirm the GMA hypothesis resulted in accepting its
logical alternative. The SMA hypothesis. A classic example is a study conducted by
Drowatzky and Zuccato in 1967. Using a large sample of Air Force recruits, they examined
scores on six different balance tests, three static balance tests, and three dynamic balance
tests. The assumption was that the same subset of motor abilities would underlie all three
tests for static balance and that a closely similar subset of abilities would underlie
performance for the dynamic balance tests. The researchers expected to discover how
similar the two groupings of abilities were that were responsible for static and dynamic
balance.
As a result of these many research investigations into the correlation among skills
and motor abilities, the validity of the SMA hypothesis has become one of the most strongly
accepted concepts in the field of skill acquisition. This acceptance leads to several important
conclusions about making predictions relative to skill level in one skill by making
observations of another skill as well as predicting future success in a skill by observing early
performance when learning the skill.
Motor educability
First proposed in the 1920s, argues that a single factor is responsible for all motor abilities
and that individuals can be determined to possess some general ability (ranging from weak
to strong) underlying their capacity to learn any motor skill.
Regression toward the mean
A summary of some of the other factors important in explaining the individual differences
observed among people in relation to motor skill learning and performance is presented in
Table 8.6.
EVERYONE IS GOOD AT SOMETHING!
It might be tempting to conclude that because some individuals are lucky at the genetic draw
of motor abilities, then some people are also unlucky and destined—not by nature but by
chance—to be poor at every motor skill.
People who appear good at everything are really only good at many things—at least the
things they attempt (and they are probably not interested in attempting skills in which they
would not prove successful).
REFERENCES
Boyle, MO, and Ackerman, PL. (2004). “Individual differences in skill acquisition,” In AM
Williams and NJ Hodges (eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory, and practice.
London: Routledge
Buss, AR, and Poley, W. (1976). Individual differences: Traits and factors. New York:
Gardner Press.
Ericsson, KA, Krampe, RT, and Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). “The role of deliberate practice in
the acquisition of expert performance.” Psychological Review,
Fleishman, EA. (1972). “On the relationship between abilities, learning, and human
performance.” American Psychologist,
Keele, SW, and Hawkins, HI. (1982). “Explorations of individual differences relevant to high
level skill.” Journal of Motor Behavior,