Duncan(2011)_code_prefrontal
Duncan(2011)_code_prefrontal
SPEEDED RESPONSE CHOICE There is much evidence that the prefrontal cortex of disorganization in many different forms of behaviour
Tasks in which simple stimuli, makes a vital contribution to effective, organized (BOX 1). On the other hand, more general accounts —
such as lights or tones, call for behaviour. Patients with prefrontal lesions can show a including concepts such as EXECUTIVE FUNCTION9, temporal
speeded keypress or other
responses.
broad loosening in the structure of thought and action: structuring of behaviour2, control by cognitive context10
the normal picture, a coherent sequence of actions and or goal–subgoal selection11 — can be hard to apply in
EPISODIC MEMORY mental activities that allow the achievement of some detail to any specific problem.
The recollection of events in an selected goal, is distorted, sometimes bizarrely, by the Essentially, there are two approaches to understand-
autobiographical context.
omission of crucial components and by the intrusion ing broad deficits of the sort associated with prefrontal
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION
of irrelevant or interfering material1,2. According to the lesions. The first is to assume that damage to combina-
High-level processes that are circumstances, the patient might seem mentally passive tions of functionally specialized frontal regions under-
proposed to organize and or inert, or disinhibited and distracted. In formal test- lies the range of deficits seen after typical lesions.
control cognitive function. ing, the result is quantitative impairment in a broad Certainly, this is a plausible view, given the size of pre-
variety of tasks, including picture description1, SPEEDED frontal cortex, and its diversity in terms of cytoarchitec-
3 4 5
RESPONSE CHOICE , EPISODIC MEMORY , maze learning , prob- ture and patterns of connectivity with other brain
lem solving6 and many others. Similarly, functional structures12. The second approach is to assume that, in
imaging studies show activation of the prefrontal cortex some frontal regions at least, functions are sufficiently
in many different task contexts. A broad role in effec- broad to contribute to many different cognitive tasks.
tive cognition is also shown by prefrontal activation in This view, in turn, is given plausibility by the extensive
conventional tests of ‘general intelligence’7 (FIG. 1). interconnections between one frontal region and
Although of unquestioned importance, prefrontal another, indicating substantial sharing and exchange of
MRC Cognition and functions are particularly difficult to characterize and information12.
Brain Sciences Unit, understand. On the one hand, accounts based on highly In this review, the question is considered in the light
15 Chaucer Road,
Cambridge CB2 2EF, UK.
specific deficits — such as an impairment of ‘delayed of evidence from functional neuroimaging in humans
e-mail: john.duncan@ response’ after frontal lesions in the monkey8 — seem and single-unit electrophysiology in the behaving mon-
mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk too restricted to apply convincingly to the broad problem key. From imaging results, there is a strong indication
Evidence from single-unit physiology Figure 2 | Prefrontal activations associated with five
Of course, the imaging method has several restrictions. different cognitive demands. Green, response conflict;
Might different cognitive demands, for example, be pink, task novelty; yellow, number of elements in working
associated with distinct patterns of frontal recruitment memory; red, working memory delay; blue, perceptual
difficulty. Lateral (top) and medial (middle) views of each
on a scale that is too fine for imaging to resolve? hemisphere, together with whole brain views from above
CROSS-MODAL INTEGRATION
A requirement to combine
Assuming that different primate species are comparable, (bottom left) and below (bottom right). CC, corpus callosum;
information from different more detailed information comes from single-unit IFS, inferior frontal sulcus; SF, Sylvian fissure. Adapted with
sensory modalities. physiology in the behaving monkey. permission from REF. 21 © 2000 Elsevier Science.
a Object task Of course, many central questions are left open by this
o outline scheme. One of these is how task relevance itself is
l determined as the prefrontal cortex chooses which infor-
n2 mation to represent. In simple situations, this process can
o
l
be thought of in terms of rewards and their use58; more
n1 broadly, however, detailed world knowledge must be used
to establish how facts do or do not bear on the pursuit of
o particular goals — the central conceptual problem that is
l addressed in artificial intelligence systems for effective
n3
action planning in complex, real-world domains59 (BOX 3).
b Location task A second unresolved question concerns the mechanism
for achieving processing coherence; in particular, the
o
mechanism underlying frontal control of the processing
l
o n2 focus in sensory and other systems60.
However, for some questions, the implications of
l the model are already fairly strong. In the next section,
n1
this point is illustrated with a discussion of regional
o prefrontal specializations.
l
n3 Adaptation and specialization
Figure 4 | The adaptive coding model. a,b | Selectivity of
Much attention has been given in both human and
three model neurons (n1–n3) for object (o) and location (l) is monkey studies to the question of prefrontal regional
indicated by schematic tuning curves; a sharper curve specialization. In imaging work, certainly, the common
reflects greater selectivity for the indicated dimension. aim is to attach specific cognitive interpretations —
Although neurons vary in relative selectivity for object and control of retrieval from episodic or working memory,
location, this variation is also modulated by task context; context setting, error monitoring and so on — to the
object selectivity becomes sharper in an object task (a) and
function of specific frontal regions. On the one hand,
location selectivity becomes sharper in a location task (b).
the adaptive coding model suggests clear limits to this
endeavour. As we have seen (FIG. 4), neurons carrying
knowledge, and the assessment of motivational signifi- very different types of information are closely inter-
cance. A good example is the suggested role of the pre- mingled in the prefrontal cortex, and, even at the level
frontal cortex in the control of visual attention56 (BOX 2): of the single neuron, adaptability is the converse of spe-
in multiple extrastriate systems, objects in the visual cialization. On the other hand, adaptability does not
input compete for representation, and the proposal is imply equipotentiality: neurons undoubtedly differ in
that frontal emphasis on a task-relevant object supports their relative potential for coding the many different
dominance of that object throughout the processing types of information that the prefrontal cortex can rep-
network. Subjectively, a selective prefrontal focus on resent (FIG. 4, compare n1 with n2). One obvious possi-
task-relevant information, with its accompanying domi- bility is that regional specializations within the pre-
nant representation in sensory, motor, memory, motiva- frontal cortex might be statistical rather than absolute
tional and other systems, would correspond to the state (REF. 61; for a more general proposal concerning statisti-
of controlled, active attention to this information, or, cal specializations in brain function, see REF. 62). Across
equivalently, to controlled, active maintenance in work- the prefrontal cortex, there might be a broad distribu-
ing memory. In this way, the prefrontal cortex carries tion of cells with the potential to carry any specific type
out a central function in configuring a flexible cognitive of information; for example, location information in
system to address specific, current concerns. working memory. For different kinds of information,
In FIG. 4, this general view of prefrontal representa- such as location and object, these distributions will
tion is illustrated for the case of working memory for overlap, even down to the level of the single neuron
location and object information30,39,57. Throughout (FIG. 4); but these overlapping distributions might have
much of the lateral prefrontal cortex, cells with variable different shapes and, in particular, different peaks or
potential for coding location and object information regions of maximal sensitivity.
are closely intermingled39,40. When object information This view is highly consistent with single-unit data. In
must be retained in working memory (upper panel), one important study, Ó Scalaidhe et al.63 measured face
object tuning across the population is enhanced, and object selectivity throughout a large region of the
whereas location tuning is weakened. When location monkey’s lateral and orbital frontal cortex. If analysis was
information must be retained, this situation is reversed. restricted only to the most highly selective cells, a strong
As a whole, the population produces a distributed rep- concentration was found on the ventrolateral surface, the
resentation, which selectively favours information of region that receives direct afferents from inferotemporal
current task relevance. As described earlier, exactly this cortex. However, as selection criteria were relaxed,
picture of adaptable location–object representation has increasing numbers of face- and object-selective cells
been shown experimentally across a large region of were found to be distributed elsewhere throughout the
dorsolateral and ventrolateral frontal cortex39. recording region. These data directly show a broad
distribution of neurons carrying some face and object the conclusion will favour strong regional specializa-
information, but with a focused, ventrolateral peak. In a tion. However, with greater demand or power, the full
study that compared location and object information in distribution might be made visible. In this case, the con-
working memory, Rainer et al.40 directly confirmed the clusion will favour overlapping frontal recruitment for
presence of overlapping regions of selectivity for these different types of demand.
two types of information, but found a higher frequency This pattern is clearly evident in imaging studies of
of location-selective cells in the posterior part of the working memory. There is some evidence that pre-
recording area, approaching the arcuate sulcus. In a suc- frontal recruitment is stronger in the left hemisphere if
cessive same–different matching task, with trial-by-trial materials are verbal70. However, such specialization is far
cues indicating whether the monkey should respond to from absolute; although weaker, activation is also seen
matches or mismatches, Wallis et al.28 showed similar in homologous right hemisphere regions, themselves
rule (match versus mismatch) and object selectivity strongly activated when materials are non-verbal70.
across dorsolateral, ventrolateral and orbital surfaces, but Results such as these are strongly consistent with a sta-
again with statistical differences (including higher inci- tistical view of hemispheric specialization, cells that
dence of object selectivity on the ventrolateral surface) code verbal information being most common on the
from one region to another. left, but also present to some extent on the right.
Relative rather than absolute specialization between More generally, activation in several regions of frontal
frontal regions is also the conclusion most consistent and posterior cortex becomes stronger with increased
with lesion data. In human lesion studies, evidence for working memory demand, leading to increasing overlap
different patterns of deficit associated with different pre- of activation associated with different memory materi-
frontal regions is actually remarkably scant, being all but als71. Along related lines, it has been proposed that
restricted to a few cases of hemispheric differences64, and episodic memory retrieval is associated specifically with
differential deficits after orbital and lateral lesions65,66. right frontal recruitment when demands are low; how-
Potentially, monkey studies should be clearer, as lesion ever, with higher demands, a fuller pattern of bilateral
locations are better controlled; but here too, full-scale recruitment might emerge72.
double dissociations are the exception rather than the Such ideas might also help to explain evidence for
rule. For example, despite some evidence of stronger frontal adaptability after damage. As early as three days
spatial deficits after dorsolateral lesions, and stronger after a stroke affecting the left inferior frontal gyrus, a
object deficits after ventrolateral lesions67,68, the broad verbal task that usually recruits this region can instead
picture is of some deficit on both types of task after produce homologous activation on the right73,74. Such
either type of lesion67–69. adaptability is easily comprehensible if the full distribu-
The view that regional specializations are statistical tion of relevant cells extends to both hemispheres,
rather than absolute has strong implications for the but with a particular focus on the left. When the left
interpretation of imaging data. According to this view, hemisphere is intact, it might be the main focus seen in
results will depend jointly on the level of demand an imaging study. However, when it is damaged,
imposed by the task — and so the strength of neural task demands will not be satisfied without stronger
recruitment — and on the statistical power of the recruitment of further relevant cells on the right.
experiment. With low demand or low statistical power, Consistent with this interpretation, some control sub-
the active region observed might be restricted only to jects show weak right-sided recruitment in addition to
the peak of the underlying neuronal distribution. Here, dominant activity on the left74.
Of course, this general view permits some cases of itself. In monkey experiments, for example, we need to
relatively strong dissociation. In the monkey lesion know the relative roles of long-term learning versus
literature, some of the clearest double dissociations short-term task context in determining neural proper-
come from comparisons between large lesions of the ties. Do experiments show high proportions of task-
orbital and lateral surfaces75,76. In the imaging litera- related neurons because the same task has been
ture, as we have seen, simple cognitive demands pro- learned over many months, or because the monkey is
duce a pattern of strong lateral but only occasional carrying out this particular task at the moment the
orbital recruitment (FIG. 2); instead, orbital activations recordings are made? To answer such questions, stud-
might be associated with the processing of emo- ies need to be made of how prefrontal responses
tional77 and motivational78,79 materials. Single-unit develop through successive stages of training85, and
data are less clear: although reward-related activity is how they alter with a switch between tasks25 or atten-
certainly one prominent property of orbitofrontal tional sets44,45. Certainly, more data are needed to sup-
neurons 80–82, some direct comparisons between port the most basic prediction of the adaptive coding
orbital and lateral recordings have shown similar model — that single frontal neurons will vary widely
kinds of reward-related and other responses 28,82. in the information that they code from one task context
Further direct comparisons of this sort are needed; to another.
meanwhile, the data as a whole support relatively The statistical view of prefrontal regional special-
strong differentiation between lateral and orbital ization has strong implications for how such special-
functions. izations should be studied. In imaging, comparisons
One particularly significant question raised by the between tasks might profitably be made at various lev-
imaging data concerns comparative function in lat- els of demand; as we have seen, the strong possibility
eral, opercular and anterior cingulate regions. Do all is of focused peak activities at low demand, evolving
three of these show similar adaptive coding properties into a pattern of largely overlapping activity at higher
or, despite their common co-recruitment, do they demand. In electrophysiology, statistical specializa-
make essentially different contributions to cognitive tions can best be seen by a direct quantitative compar-
function? Hints of such differences already exist in the ison of population properties in different prefrontal
imaging literature83,84, and are likely to be developed in regions28,63.
future work. More broadly, quantitative comparisons might also
be crucial in comparing prefrontal and other
Future directions regions86,87. Although this review emphasizes adapt-
The lack of specificity in many models of prefrontal ability in prefrontal responses, such adaptability is
function reflects the breadth of cognitive deficits asso- also, to some extent, a property of many — perhaps
ciated with frontal lesions. Adaptability helps to most — other brain representations. Selective empha-
explain this; in different contexts, the prefrontal cortex sis of task-relevant information, for example, is seen in
might assist in many different cognitive operations. attentional modulations of neural response through-
The adaptive coding model, for example, cannot gen- out the visual system56, at least as early as the primary
erate detailed predictions about exact error types to be visual cortex88. What then is the special contribution of
expected after prefrontal damage. At the physiological the prefrontal representation, leading to such a key
level, however, it is more specific, with fairly direct im- role in overall behavioural coherence? Several possibil-
plications for how future questions can be formulated ities immediately come to mind. In early visual areas,
and addressed. for example, suppression of irrelevant information is
As we have seen, imaging studies show much the predominantly local89, being strongest when relevant
same patterns of prefrontal recruitment in association and irrelevant inputs fall within one cell’s receptive
with many different kinds of cognitive demand. When field. In the prefrontal cortex, such suppression could
this pattern is seen in any individual study, it would be more global, extending to inputs in different hemi-
seem to be inappropriate to interpret this in terms of fields, or perhaps even in different sensory modalities.
that particular study’s demand. Instead, the results Other possibilities are that the prefrontal representa-
probably reflect an adaptation of general prefrontal tion of relevant information is particularly stable in
mechanisms to the specific task context. Similarly, any the face of irrelevant, distracting inputs90; or that it
single-unit study is likely to find large proportions of can adapt more quickly as task context changes.
lateral prefrontal units showing task-related responses. Formulating and testing such hypotheses will be a par-
Again, the interpretation should not be in terms of ticularly important step in developing the adaptive
specific frontal involvement in these particular cogni- coding idea.
tive operations; rather, these data should be viewed as Interestingly, such approaches point to a view of
an adaptation of neural properties to this particular the prefrontal cortex not so much as the seat of partic-
task’s requirements. On the whole, the adaptive cod- ular cognitive operations, but as a resource that gives
ing model argues against simply extending these such operations greater focus, power or flexibility. In
common findings to an ever-expanding list of further any given experiment, the challenge is then to separate
task domains. the general from the specific — to deduce general
Instead, the model emphasizes other research ques- principles of prefrontal function from its contribution
tions. One, as we have seen, is the nature of adaptability to one particular sample of effective behaviour.
1. Luria, A. R. Higher Cortical Functions in Man (Tavistock, 29. Fuster, J. M., Bauer, R. H. & Jervey, J. P. Functional 55. Duncan, J., Humphreys, G. & Ward, R. Competitive brain
London, 1966). interactions between inferotemporal and prefrontal cortex in activity in visual attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 255–261
A rich, indispensable, clinical description of the a cognitive task. Brain Res. 330, 299–307 (1985). (1997).
loosened structure of thought and behaviour that can 30. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. 56. Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective
follow frontal lobe lesions. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222 (1995).
2. Fuster, J. M. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349 57. Wilson, F. A. W., Ó Scalaidhe, S. P. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S.
and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe 2nd edn (Raven, (1989). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in
New York, 1989). 31. Watanabe, M. Prefrontal unit activity during delayed primate prefrontal cortex. Science 260, 1955–1958 (1993).
3. Drewe, E. A. Go–no go learning after frontal lobe lesions in conditional go/no-go discrimination in the monkey. I. 58. Braver, T. S. & Cohen, J. D. in Control of Cognitive
humans. Cortex 11, 8–16 (1975). Relation to the stimulus. Brain Res. 382, 1–14 (1986). Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (eds Monsell,
4. Shimamura, A. P. in The Cognitive Neurosciences (ed. 32. Sakagami, M. & Niki, H. Encoding of behavioral significance S. & Driver, J.) 713–737 (MIT Press, Cambridge,
Gazzaniga, M. S.) 803–813 (MIT Press, Cambridge, of visual stimuli by primate prefrontal neurons: relation to Massachusetts, 2000).
Massachusetts, 1995). relevant task conditions. Exp. Brain Res. 97, 423–436 59. Newell, A. Unified Theories of Cognition (Harvard Univ.
5. Milner, B. Visually-guided maze learning in man: effects of (1994). Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1990).
bilateral hippocampal, bilateral frontal and unilateral cerebral 33. Romo, R., Brody, C. D., Hernández, A. & Lemus, L. 60. Tomita, H., Ohbayashi, M., Nakahara, K., Hasegawa, I. &
lesions. Neuropsychologia 3, 317–338 (1965). Neuronal correlates of parametric working memory in the Miyashita, Y. Top–down signal from prefrontal cortex in
6. Shallice, T. in The Neuropsychology of Cognitive Function prefrontal cortex. Nature 399, 470–473 (1999). executive control of memory retrieval. Nature 401, 699–703
(eds Broadbent, D. E. & Weiskrantz, L.) 199–209 (The Royal 34. Watanabe, M. Prefrontal unit activity during delayed (1999).
Society, London, 1982). conditional go/no-go discrimination in the monkey. II. 61. Haxby, J. V., Petit, L., Ungerleider, L. G. & Courtney, S. M.
7. Duncan, J. et al. A neural basis for general intelligence. Relation to go and no-go responses. Brain Res. 382, 15–27 Distinguishing the functional roles of multiple regions in
Science 289, 457–460 (2000). (1986). distributed neural systems for visual working memory.
8. Goldman-Rakic, P. Topography of cognition: parallel 35. Di Pellegrino, G. & Wise, S. P. Visuospatial versus Neuroimage 11, 380–391 (2000).
distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu. visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of Strong fMRI evidence that regional specializations in
Rev. Neurosci. 11, 137–156 (1988). a primate. J. Neurosci. 13, 1227–1243 (1993). a working memory network are relative rather than
9. Baddeley, A. D. Working Memory (Oxford Univ. Press, 36. Fuster, J. M. & Alexander, G. E. Neuron activity related to absolute.
Oxford, UK, 1986). short-term memory. Science 173, 652–654 (1971). 62. Requin, J., Riehle, A. & Seal, J. in Attention and
10. Cohen, J. D. & Servan-Schreiber, D. Context, cortex, and 37. Watanabe, M. Reward expectancy in primate prefrontal Performance XIV (eds Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S.)
dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and neurons. Nature 382, 629–632 (1996). 745–769 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1993).
biology in schizophrenia. Psychol. Rev. 99, 45–77 (1992). A demonstration of reward-related activity in neurons 63. Ó Scalaidhe, P., Wilson, F. A. W. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S.
11. Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R. & Freer, C. of the lateral prefrontal cortex. Face-selective neurons during passive viewing and working
Intelligence and the frontal lobe: the organization of goal- 38. Niki, H. & Watanabe, M. Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity memory performance of rhesus monkeys: evidence for
directed behavior. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 257–303 (1996). during timing behavior in the monkey. Brain Res. 171, intrinsic specialization of neuronal coding. Cereb. Cortex 9,
12. Pandya, D. N. & Yeterian, E. H. Comparison of prefrontal 213–224 (1979). 459–475 (1999).
architecture and connections. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B One of the few direct comparisons of neuronal An important demonstration that regional specificity
351, 1423–1432 (1996). properties in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and within prefrontal cortex depends on the criterion for
13. Miller, E. K. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. anterior cingulate, revealing highly similar response cell classification; although the most highly face-
Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1, 59–65 (2000). types in these two regions. selective cells are clustered on the ventrolateral
14. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of prefrontal 39. Rao, S. C., Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Integration of what and surface, more weakly selective cells are more broadly
function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 (2001). where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 276, distributed.
An authoritative recent review and theoretical 821–824 (1997). 64. Milner, B. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of
synthesis of physiological and cognitive work on 40. Rainer, G., Asaad, W. F. & Miller, E. K. Memory fields of psychological processes in man. Br. Med. Bull. 27,
prefrontal function. neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. 272–277 (1971).
15. Duncan, J. & Miller, E. K. in Principles of Frontal Lobe Sci. USA 95, 15008–15013 (1998). 65. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Anderson, S. W.
Function (eds Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T.) (Oxford Univ. 41. White, I. M. & Wise, S. P. Rule-dependent neuronal activity Dissociation of working memory from decision making
Press, Oxford, UK, in the press). in the prefrontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 315–335 (1999). within the human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 18,
16. Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M. & 42. Freedman, D. J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T. & Miller, E. K. 428–437 (1998).
Raichle, M. E. Positron emission tomographic studies of the Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate 66. Stuss, D. T. et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance
cortical anatomy of single word processing. Nature 331, prefrontal cortex. Science 291, 312–316 (2001). in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage:
585–589 (1988). A key experiment showing that neurons in a wide effects of lesion location and test structure on separable
17. Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G. L. & region of the lateral prefrontal cortex adapt to task cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia 38, 388–402 (2000).
Petersen, S. E. Selective and divided attention during visual context, coding just those stimulus distinctions or A comparison of cognitive deficits after lesions in
discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional categorizations of current behavioural significance. different regions of prefrontal cortex, showing
anatomy by positron emission tomography. J. Neurosci. 11, 43. Schall, J. D., Hanes, D. P., Thompson, K. G. & King, D. J. comparative preservation of function in orbitofrontal
2383–2402 (1991). Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. patients.
18. Pardo, J. V., Pardo, P. J., Janer, K. W. & Raichle, M. E. The Visual and premovement activation. J. Neurosci. 15, 67. Passingham, R. Delayed matching after selective prefrontal
anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in 6905–6918 (1995). lesions in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Brain Res. 92,
the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proc. Natl Acad. 44. Rainer, G., Asaad, W. F. & Miller, E. K. Selective 89–102 (1975).
Sci. USA 87, 256–259 (1990). representation of relevant information by neurons in the 68. Mishkin, M. & Manning, F. J. Nonspatial memory after
19. Goel, V., Grafman, J., Sadato, N. & Hallett, M. Modeling primate prefrontal cortex. Nature 393, 577–579 (1998). selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys. Brain Res. 143,
other minds. Neuroreport 6, 1741–1746 (1995). 45. Everling, S., Tinsley, C. J., Gaffan, D. & Duncan, J. Neural 313–323 (1978).
20. Fletcher, P. C. et al. Other minds in the brain: a functional activity in a focused attention task in monkey prefrontal 69. Petrides, M. Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and
imaging study of ‘theory of mind’ in story comprehension. cortex. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 30, 2227 (2000). externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of
Cognition 57, 109–128 (1995). 46. Shima, K. & Tanji, J. Role for cingulate motor area cells in the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex of the
21. Duncan, J. & Owen, A. M. Common regions of the human voluntary movement selection based on reward. Science monkey. J. Neurosci. 15, 359–375 (1995).
frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends 282, 1335–1338 (1998). 70. Postle, B. R. & D’Esposito, M. Evaluating models of the
Neurosci. 23, 475–483 (2000). 47. Procyk, E., Tanaka, Y. L. & Joseph, J. P. Anterior cingulate topographical organization of working memory function in
A systematic analysis of published studies showing activity during routine and non-routine sequential behaviors frontal cortex with event-related fMRI. Psychobiology 28,
common patterns of prefrontalrecruitment for a in macaques. Nature Neurosci. 3, 502–508 (2000). 146–155 (2000).
broad range of different cognitive demands. 48. Jacobsen, C. E. Functions of the frontal association area in 71. Nystrom, L. E. et al. Working memory for letters, shapes,
22. Wagner, A. D., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H. & Gabrieli, J. primates. Arch. Neurol. Psych. 33, 558–569 (1935). and locations: fMRI evidence against stimulus-based
D. E. Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory. Functional- 49. Chao, L. L. & Knight, R. T. Contribution of human prefrontal regional organization in human prefrontal cortex.
MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes. cortex to delay performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, Neuroimage 11, 424–446 (2000).
Brain 121, 1985–2002 (1998). 167–177 (1998). 72. Nolde, S. F., Johnson, M. K. & Raye, C. L. The role of
23. Stroop, J. R. Studies of interference in serial verbal 50. Malmo, R. R. Interference factors in delayed response in prefrontal cortex during tests of episodic memory. Trends
reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662 (1935). monkeys after removal of frontal lobes. J. Neurophysiol. 5, Cogn. Sci. 2, 399–406 (1998).
24. Asaad W. F., Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Neural activity in the 295–308 (1942). 73. Thulborn, K. R., Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. Plasticity of
primate prefrontal cortex during associative learning. 51. Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J. & Tuholski, S. W. in Models of language-related brain function during recovery from
Neuron 21, 1399–1407 (1998). Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and stroke. Stroke 30, 749–754 (1999).
25. Asaad, W. F., Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. Task-specific neural Executive Control (eds Miyake, A. & Shah, P.) 102–134 74. Rosen, H. J. et al. Neural correlates of recovery from
activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 84, (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK, 1999). aphasia after damage to left inferior frontal cortex.
451–459 (2000). 52. Norman, D. A. & Shallice, T. Attention to Action: Willed and Neurology 55, 1883–1894 (2000).
26. Funahashi, S. & Inoue, M. Neuronal interactions related to Automatic Control of Behavior. Report No. 8006, Univ. 75. Butter, C. M. Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination
working memory processes in the primate prefrontal cortex California, Cent. Hum. Inf. Process. (1980). reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca
revealed by cross-correlation analysis. Cereb. Cortex 10, 53. Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M. & Changeux, J.-P. A neuronal mulatta. Physiol. Behav. 4, 163–171 (1969).
535–551 (2000). model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. 76. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W. & Roberts, A. C. Dissociation in
27. Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M. & Kroger, J. K. Cross-modal and Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 14529–14534 (1998). prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature
cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex. A computational model sharing many central aspects 380, 69–72 (1996).
Nature 405, 347–351 (2000). with the adaptive coding model. 77. Drevets, W. C. Neuroimaging and neuropathological
28. Wallis, J. D., Anderson, K. C. & Miller, E. K. Single neurons 54. Duncan, J. in Attention and Performance XVI (eds Inui, T. & studies of depression: implications for the cognitive-
in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature 411, McClelland, J. L.) 549–578 (MIT Press, Cambridge, emotional features of mood disorders. Curr. Opin.
953–956 (2001). Massachusetts, 1996). Neurobiol. 11, 240–249 (2001).
78. Elliott, R., Frith, C. D. & Dolan, R. J. Differential neural 86. Chafee, M. W. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Matching patterns of 94. Desimone, R. & Ungerleider, L. G. in Handbook of
response to positive and negative feedback in planning and activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip Neuropsychology Vol. 2 (eds Boller, F. & Grafman, J.)
guessing tasks. Neuropsychologia 35, 1395–1404 (1997). neurons during a spatial working memory task. 267–299 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1989).
79. O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J. & J. Neurophysiol. 79, 2919–2940 (1998). 95. Duncan, J. Selective attention and the organization of visual
Andrews, C. Abstract reward and punishment 87. Anderson, K. C. & Miller, E. K. Neural activity in the prefrontal information. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501–517 (1984).
representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature and posterior parietal cortices during a what-then-where 96. Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S. & Funnell, E.
Neurosci. 4, 95–102 (2001). memory task. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 30, 975 (2000). Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia with
80. Rolls, E. T. The orbitofrontal cortex. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. 88. Roelfsema, P. R., Lamme, V. A. F. & Spekreijse, H. Object- temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 115, 1783–1806 (1992).
Lond. B 351, 1433–1444 (1996). based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque 97. Spearman, C. General intelligence, objectively determined
81. Tremblay, L. & Schultz, W. Relative reward preference in monkey. Nature 395, 376–381 (1998). and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 15, 201–293 (1904).
primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature 398, 704–708 (1999). 89. Moran, J. & Desimone, R. Selective attention gates visual
82. Hikosaka, K. & Watanabe, M. Delay activity of orbital and processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 229, 782–784 Acknowledgements
lateral prefrontal neurons of the monkey varying with (1985). I am grateful to E. Miller for his contribution to many of the ideas
different rewards. Cereb. Cortex 10, 263–271 (2000). 90. Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A. & Desimone, R. Neural presented in this paper.
83. MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A. & Carter, mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex
C. S. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 16, 5154–5167 (1996).
anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 280, 91. Williams, G. V. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Modulation of Online links
1835–1838 (2000). memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal
84. Owen, A. M. The functional organization of working memory cortex. Nature 376, 572–575 (1995). DATABASES
processes within human lateral frontal cortex: the 92. Diamond, A. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Comparison of human The following terms in this article are linked online to:
contribution of functional neuroimaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9, infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s A-not-B task: MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences:
1329–1339 (1997). evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. http://cognet.mit.edu/MITECS/
85. Chen, L. L. & Wise, S. P. Neuronal activity in the Exp. Brain Res. 74, 24–40 (1989). attention | hemispheric specialization | magnetic
supplementary eye field during acquisition of conditional 93. Gaffan D. & Harrison, S. A comparison of the effects of resonance imaging | positron emission tomography |
oculomotor association. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 1101–1121 fornix transection and sulcus principalis ablation upon spatial working memory
(1995). learning by monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 31, 207–220 (1989). Access to this interactive links box is free online.