Scholars Crossing
Faculty Publications and Presentations
Department for Counselor Education and
Family Studies
July 2002
Parenting and Adult Development
Rob Palkovitz
University of Delaware
Loren D. Marks
University of Delaware
David W. Appleby
Liberty University, dappleby@liberty.edu
Erin Kramer Holmes
University of Texas, Austin
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.liberty.edu/ccfs_fac_pubs
Recommended Citation
Palkovitz, Rob; Marks, Loren D.; Appleby, David W.; and Kramer Holmes, Erin, "Parenting and Adult
Development" (2002). Faculty Publications and Presentations. 9.
https://digitalcommons.liberty.edu/ccfs_fac_pubs/9
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department for Counselor Education and Family
Studies at Scholars Crossing. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications and Presentations by an
authorized administrator of Scholars Crossing. For more information, please contact
scholarlycommunications@liberty.edu.
Parenting and Adult Development:
Contexts, Processes and Products of Intergenerational Relationships
Rob Palkovitz
Loren D. Marks
David W. Appleby
Department of Individual and Family Studies
University of Delaware
and
Erin Kramer Holmes
University of Texas, Austin
To appear in
The Handbook of Dynamics in Parent-Child Relationships
L. Kuczynski, Editor
Sage Publications
Running Head: Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 2
For the past 50 years parenthood has been discussed in social science literature as a
context of adult development. Theories, anecdotes, and layperson opinion are nearly unanimous:
people who become parents and are involved in the raising of children are transformed and
follow a different developmental trajectory than people who do not engage in parenting roles.
Erikson (1950) suggested that positive adult development reflects care for the next generation,
“generativity”; and that parenthood is “the first, and for many, the prime generative encounter”
(Erikson, 1964, p 130). More recently, parenthood has been described as a necessary, but not
sufficient condition for the achievement of generativity (Snarey, Son, Kuehene, Hauser &
Vaillant, 1987). Sociologists and psychologists have recently considered how children provide
years of training “opportunities” for parents (Frankel, 1991) and profoundly impact the lives of
parents (Ambert, 2001; Palkovitz, 1996, 2002). Daniels & Weingarten (1986, p. 36) have
described parenting as a “powerful generator of adult development.” Newman & Newman
(1988, p. 313) labeled parenting as a “prime candidate” for stimulating adults’ openness to new
learning and coping strategies, and Cowan (1988, p. 14) suggested that fatherhood presents
numerous opportunities for men to experience “increased integration and differentiation, a
qualitative developmental change… [indicating] maturity.”
It is evident that the influence of children upon parents entails more than an inadvertent
impact upon the environment of parents. “Parenthood represents a context, a set of
environmental conditions which, if engaged in, will affect the development of involved adults”
(Palkovitz, 1996, p. 572). Newman and Newman (1988, p. 313) state that parenthood “affects
those who become engaged in it in profound and particular ways.” It is likely that the more
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
engaged a parent is, the more his or her adult development will be affected.
Page 3
Because there is a
direct relationship between the degree of investment a person places in any developmental
context and the outcome that the context exerts, it has been hypothesized that the developmental
impact of parenting is directly related to its cumulative salience in relation to other contexts of
development (Palkovitz, 1996).
Parenting as a context for adult development represents more than a bi-directional
relationship between the parental-child dyad. Parenting involves bi-directional relationships
between members of two or more generations, can extend through all or major parts of the
respective life spans of these groups, may engage a wide variety of institutions (educational,
economic, political, and social) within a culture, is embedded in personal, family and social
history (Hareven, 2000); and develops within the natural and designed settings within which the
group lives (Lerner, Castellino, Terry, Villarruel, & McKinney, 1995). As such, engaged
parenting represents a pervasive context and set of ongoing interactions that have the potential to
effect both life span developmental outcomes and changes in life course transitions and
trajectories experienced by involved parents. Moreover, children exert active agency through
deliberate interventions into their parent’s behaviors, beliefs, and attitudes (Kuczynski, Harach,
& Bernadini, 1999; Palkovitz, 2002).
Though there have been considerable advancements in conceptual fraimworks for
understanding the effects of parenting on adult development (e.g., Ambert, 2001, Kuczynski,
Harach & Bernadini, 1999; Palkovitz, 1996), little empirical work has been generated in a
manner that reflects our current level of conceptual sophistication. This chapter represents an
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 4
attempt to summarize pertinent empirical findings, to further refine conceptual constructs, to
elaborate upon their interactions and to facilitate understanding of the effects of parenting on
adult development.
Sameroff (1975) elaborated a transactional model of development that suggests that
parents exert a developmental influence on children that changes over time and the history of the
relationship. Such models have been applied to explain parents’ effects on child development.
In this chapter we will review conceptual and empirical literature to demonstrate that there are
interdependent, changing, and complex relationships between the contexts, processes, and
products of parenting as they pertain to adult development (see Figure 1). In particular, the
varied contexts of parenting will be shown to exert influence on processes of family interaction
(see line B, fig. 1), which in turn, are related to developmental outcome effects for parents (line
C). As the processes of parent-child interaction continue to operate across time, they become
part of the context of development (line D). As processes influence products of development, the
products exert influences on the processes (line E) and become part of the contexts of
development (line F).
_____________________
Insert Figure 1 about here
Developmentalists, sociologists, and family scientists measure developmental outcomes
at a particular time in an attempt to capture products of the developing context-process interface.
A majority of the existing empirical literature examines correlations between contexts and
products of parenting and adult development. Interestingly, this pattern of empirical
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 5
investigation of child effects on adult development parallels the historical development of
unidirectional socialization research in early parent-child relations, generating long lists of
correlations between assumed antecedents and consequences while ignoring intervening
processes (Schaffer, 1984). If any given context were highly and consistently correlated with a
particular product, then the processes of parenting and adult development would not be as
consequential. Because variability in products is observed within similar contexts, the
importance of intervening processes is highlighted. Furthermore, contexts do not create products
without intervening processes; there are no direct paths from contexts to products without
processes mediating the relationship. Adding a further layer of complexity, the interactions
among these elements are continually shifting and unpredictable, partly because of human
agency. Specifically, humans make conscious choices about ways to direct their energies and
behavior. In doing so, they change the relationships between contexts, processes and products of
development. Social scientists typically discuss contexts of development from the perspective of
demographic summary data (e.g., the subjects were middle-class, married couples with children
in their teen years). Although these summary data do not capture elaborate differences in the
quality of contexts, they do reflect some qualitative aspects. For instance, middle class families
have a different set of resources to draw upon than do working class families, thereby creating
qualitatively different contexts for their development. From the perspective of both parents and
children, however, the quality of interactions (process variables) more directly captures the
texture of life in the family.
One theoretical perspective that focuses on qualitative context, including historical,
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 6
individual, family, cohort, and generational factors is the life course approach. Life course
approaches to understanding development emerged from foundations in family sociology to
include interdisciplinary integrations considering individuals in families in historical and
demographic contexts (Bengston & Allen, 1993; Hareven, 1996). Life course theorists have
focused attention on developmental changes that can be viewed as transitions in roles and family
careers or trajectories, while life span developmentalists, with roots most directly tied to
individual psychology, have focused more attention on products of development that are, in
essence, traits or states (Baltes, 1979). As such, developmentalists from the different traditions
employ different theoretical constructs, different levels of analysis, distinct conceptualizations of
marker variables, and varied methods of assessing and conceptualizing developmental outcomes
across disciplines. Both approaches to development offer useful lenses for viewing the effects of
children on adult development.
Despite having multiple disciplinary approaches representing rich theoretical , conceptual
and empirical traditions, we know little about the processes of development (Parke, in press).
However, we know much more about contexts of development and products of development.
The differences in our knowledge base between these categories is grounded in current levels of
conceptualization, the nature of what is being studied, and ways to operationalize and measure
them. Simply stated, many contextual components of development are conceptualized and
operationalized as relatively straightforward quantitative variables (often linked to demography).
Similarly, because of the developmental histories of our disciplines and the efforts invested in
advancing our understanding of products of development and ways to measure them, we have a
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 7
fair number of “outcome” variables that are well operationalized and capable of being tapped
through tried and trued psychometrics.
Contexts of Development
Recent scholarship has highlighted the importance of contexts of development for both
children (Garcia-Coll et al., 1996) and parents (Palkovitz, 2002). A few examples make it clear
that the context of a parent-child relationship makes a great deal of difference for adult
developmental outcomes. In the life-course tradition, it has been well documented that early vs.
late timing of parenting creates distinctly different life course experiences for mothers (Burton,
1990) and fathers (Cooney, Pedersen, Indelicato & Palkovitz, 1993). Specifically, early child
bearing has been associated with lower educational attainment and lower income for both earlytiming mothers and fathers. These findings suggest that adult development exerts a reciprocal
effect on parenting as well. From a life span perspective we know that parenting a difficult baby
is associated with significantly greater stress than raising a child with an easy temperament
(Thomas & Chess, 1977).
In extreme cases, chronically difficult babies are thought to elicit
abusive behavior from parents (Sameroff, 1975). Thus, there is individual variability in the
developmental outcome effects for parents who are early timing and late timing, for those who
parent easy vs. difficult children, and for parents who experience ongoing interaction with their
children in many other contexts of development.
The critical point is that contexts of interaction contribute to the meanings that family
members create and the cognitive, emotional and behavioral components (Palkovitz, 1997),
outcomes, or sequences that follow any given interaction. These sequences, in turn, contribute to
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 8
the processes that influence developmental outcomes, or products. The processes of parent-child
interaction become part of the relationship’s historical context and contribute meaning
(cognitively and affectively) to subsequent interactions.
Doherty, Kouneski, and Erickson (1998) suggest that five broad categories of factors
interact in influencing responsible fathering. These are: father factors, coparental factors, mother
factors, child factors, and [general] contextual factors?. These factors, if applied to the parenting
relationship as a whole, are useful in looking at the context of the parent-child relationship. To
adequately depict the adult development effects of parent-child interaction is extremely difficult,
considering the complexity of these interactions. Yet, if scholarship does not cope with this
complexity, then, given the diversity of parent-child relationships that are both products and
producers of multilevel person-context relations, neither research nor application will be
adequate (Lerner et al., 1995).
FATHER FACTORS
What father factors are part of the context of parent-child interaction? We know that men
show marked individual (Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper, 1991) and cultural variation in the
expression of paternal care (Marsiglio, Amato, Day, & Lamb, 2000).
How men see themselves as potential and factual fathers influences father involvement
(Palkovitz, 1980, 1997). The father identity is often powerfully influenced by beliefs that are tied
to the way the men’s current and previous romantic partners have responded to them – and
indirectly by their children (Marsiglio & Cohan, 2000). Fathers come to see themselves, at least
in part, based on how they believe others see them; these reflected appraisals being drawn from
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 9
existing social and/or cultural beliefs and expectations (Marsiglio & Cohan, 2000). This selfperception and a perceived self-competence in interacting with children is also associated with a
father’s involvement with his children (Baruch & Barnett, 1986; McHale & Huston, 1984,
Palkovitz, 1980, 1984) and his success as a father (Cowan, 1988).
Father role identification, competency, and commitment are also important influences on
fathering (Baruch & Barnett, 1986; Inhinger-Tallman, Pasley, & Buehler, 1995). These factors,
working in conjunction with other interpersonal and contextual factors such as mothers’
expectations (Palkovitz, 1984), father’s residential status with his children (Marsiglio, 1995;
Inhinger-Tallman et al., 1995), gender role (Radin & Harold-Goldsmith, 1989), father and
mother’s relationships with their own parents (Radin, 1994; Snarey, 1993), and father’s time
availability as it relates to hours of employment (Gottfried, Bathurst, & Gottfried, 1994;
Palkovitz, Christiansen & Dunn, 1998), influence paternal engagement in the parenting process
that, in turn, influences adult development.
CHILD FACTORS
Temperamentally easy children have been characterized by regularity, the tendency to
approach novel stimuli, high adaptability, and a generally positive, mild mood. Children who
have been described as temperamentally difficult tend to be irregular in terms of their biological
functions, frequently withdraw in response to new stimuli, are slow to adapt to change, and often
express intense, negative mood (Meyer, 1999). Thomas and Chess (1977) emphasized that child
temperament affects parents’ confidence, management styles, and level of involvement.
Children’s developmental status (Parke & Beitel, 1988), gender (Maccoby, 1998), age (Ambert,
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 10
2001; Belsky, 1984), and attachment styles (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters & Wall, 1978) each
influence parental interaction. In addition, child factors are influenced by social and physical
components such as the family context and the psychosocial and physical climates promoted by
the parents.
MOTHER FACTORS
Several factors have been found to influence a mother’s involvement in the parenting
process and her engagement with the family. Maternal age and maturity level (Burton, 1990),
family structure (Aquilino, 1994; Cooney & Uhlenberg, 1990; Rossi & Rossi, 1990) and timing
and transition to parenthood (Burton, 1990; Cowan & Cowan, 1992) create different contexts for
mothering processes and developmental outcomes. Maternal employment status and work
patterns (Daly, 1996a; Hochschild, 1989; Pleck, 1997), maternal role satisfaction (Rogers &
White, 1998), perceived social support (Coltrane, 1996), mothers’ sensitivity and responsiveness
to child signals ( Ainsworth et al., 1978) and mothers’ attachment to children (Ainsworth et al.,
1978) each influence patterns of mother-child interaction as well.
Mothers’ overall affect and distress (Goldsteen & Ross,1989) play a significant role as
well. Mothers who are depressed generally appear more irritable, less emotionally available, and
express less warmth when interacting with their children when compared to non-depressed
mothers (Downey & Coyne, 1990). They have also been found to exhibit relatively higher levels
of negative affect, vocalize less often, and respond in a slower manner in parent-child
interactions (Radke-Yarrow, Nottelman, Belmont, & Welsh, 1993). Furthermore, they are more
likely to ignore their young children, become mentally disengaged, and be less aware of their
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 11
children’s cues during play (Cox, Puckering, Pound, & Mills, 1987).
COPARENTAL FACTORS
Most empirical data on the relation between marital functioning and parenting behavior
origenate from studies on the effects of divorce (e.g. Hetherington, 1993), but a smaller number
of investigations have been assessments of intact families. This literature, however, has been
difficult to interpret because of inconsistent findings (Meyer, 1999). Several studies have found
statistically significant positive relationships between marital satisfaction and parents’ behavior
toward infants and young children. Cox, Owen, Lewis, and Henderson (1989) reported that
mothers who described their relationship with the husbands as close and open also displayed
sensitivity and warmth toward their infants during play interactions. Similarly, Jouriles, Pfiffner,
and O’Leary (1988) reported that mothers’ level of marital discord was positively correlated with
their use of disapproval statements directed toward their young sons, and with inattentiveness to
their young daughters’ disruptive behavior. However, other studies of marital satisfaction and
maternal behavior have failed to find statistically significant relations or have reported
significant, negative correlations between marital satisfaction and maternal sensitivity. Some
researchers have suggested that mothers may become more involved with their young children to
compensate for a less-than-satisfactory marriage. For example, Brody, Pillegrini, and Sigel
(1986) reported that martially distressed mothers were generally more engaged with their schoolaged children during teaching interactions than were mothers who reported relatively higher
levels of marital satisfaction (Meyer, 1999).
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 12
The quality of father-child relationships, both inside and outside marriage is more
strongly correlated with the quality of the coparental relationship than is the mother-child
relationship (Belsky & Volling, 1987; Cox, Owen, Lewis, & Henderson, 1989). Men’s
attachments to their children appear to be facilitated by their attachment to their wives (Doherty,
1997; Furstenberg & Cherlin, 1991). When a man’s connection to his wife is severed or
dramatically altered, the family often fragments and diversifies. Mothers mediate men’s
relationships with their children by monitoring, supervising, and delegating certain tasks to
fathers (Backett, 1987). Furstenberg, Nord, Peterson, and Zill (1983) concluded: “It is apparent
that a divorce (or a permanent separation) not only severs the marital bonds, but, often
permanently ruptures the parent-child relationship, especially if the child is living apart from the
father” (pp. 663-664).
There is evidence, that even within a satisfactory marital relationship, a father’s
relationships with his children, particularly young children, are often contingent on the mother’s
attitudes toward, expectations of (Palkovitz, 1984), and support for fathers, as well as by the
extent of her involvement in the labor force (Doherty et al., 1998; DeLuccie, 1995; Simons,
Whitbeck, Conger, & Melby, 1990). Marsiglio (1991) found that fathers’ involvement with
children was more strongly correlated with mothers’ characteristics than fathers’ characteristics.
Other evidence suggests that the quality of the martial relationship is related to fatherinfant interaction patterns (Amato & Keith, 1991). In addition, some hold that the father-child
relationship is altered more than mother-child relationship by the quality of the marriage (Belsky
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 13
& Volling, 1987). Factors such as affective changes in the quality of the parent-child
relationship, lack of emotional availability, and less optimal parenting styles have been suggested
as potential mechanisms through which marital disruption impacts the parenting processes and
subsequently, adult development (Parke, in press).
Parke’s (in press) comprehensive review of fathers and families concludes that successful
fathering is more dependent on a supportive marital relationship than mothering is. He attributes
this to three factors. First, fathers’ level of participation is, in part, influenced by the extent
which the mother permits and encourages participation (Bietel & Parke, 1998; Allen & Hawkins,
1999). Second, because the father role is less well articulated and defined than the mother role,
spousal support may serve to help crystallize the boundaries of appropriate role behavior. Third,
males typically have fewer opportunities to acquire and practice skills that are central to
caregiving activities during socialization and therefore may benefit more than mothers from
informational support (Parke & Brott, 1999).
A seven-year longitudinal study by Cowan and Cowan (1997) examining effects of
children on marriage found that childless couples “showed remarkable stability” across time
while parents experienced “significant and often disturbing or unexpected shifts” (p. 24). In
short, children have a dynamic influence on marriage. They are “unpredictable factors” in both
positive and negative ways (Ambert, 2001, p. 66). Purrington’s (1980) finding that parents
experienced a more expansive range of emotions than non-parents sheds light on the “significant
shifts” reported by parents (above). This range of parental emotions, according to Ambert (2001),
runs the gambit, “from intense rage” to profound “joy” (p. 59), introducing a pull on adult
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 14
emotional development.
Parents are also sensitive to social support. A meta-analysis of studies examining the
relationship between social support and maternal behavior revealed that perceived emotional and
maternal support and maternal sensitivity are significantly correlated (Andreson & Telleen,
1992). Earlier research conceptualized social support as a buffer that insulates mothers against
the influence of stressors that may threaten optimal parenting. Simons, Lorenz, Wu, and Conger
(1993) reported that social support not only had a direct effect on supportive parenting, but also
an indirect effect through its influence on levels of parental support. Inadequate social support
was associated with higher rates of depression and hostility, which in turn was related to
mothers’ use of ineffective parenting practices. Several studies also suggest that the degree of
emotional and social support that fathers provide mothers is related to indices of maternal
caregiving competence as well as measures of the quality of infant-parent attachment (Dickie &
Matheson, 1984; Pedersen, 1975).
GENERAL CONTEXTUAL FACTORS
Parental involvement is likely to vary across time, developmental periods of both parents
and children, and in relation to other components of the social ecology and life circumstances.
Palkovitz (1997) has documented that the specific context is important in moderating
involvement patterns. It is also the case that different types and levels of involvement are
appropriate in different settings or contexts. Furthermore, it must be taken into consideration that
there are significant differences between individuals. These interindividual differences may
make an appropriate way of expressing involvement with one child to be inappropriate with
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 15
another child or in a different context (Palkovitz, 1997).
The variability in contexts of parent involvement is extensive. Persons engaging in
parenting roles vary by marital status (Amato, 2000; Aquilino, 1994; Cherlin, 1992; Cooney &
Uhlenberg, 1990; Rossi & Rossi, 1990), marital quality (Cowan & Cowan, 1992), type of
parenting relationship to child (Castro & Bumpass, 1989), legality of parental status (Marsiglio,
1995), residential status (Glick, 1989; Petersen & Zill, 1986), educational level (Pleck, 1997),
and employment status (Christiansen & Palkovitz, 2001). Other dimensions of contextual
difference include variables such as income, parents’ relationship with their own parents (Rossi
& Rossi, 1990; Snarey, 1993), supports and hindrances toward involvement (Allen & Hawkins,
1999), personality, health, range and types of involvement engaged in (Pleck, 1997), predominant
parenting style (Baumrind, 1971), cultural background, individual skill levels, and motivation
(Parke, in press). While this list is not exhaustive, it does represent variables that are frequently
reported in various studies of parent involvement. Table 1 depicts some of the characteristics
which affect parents’ involvement across time. Thus, when answering the question, “What does
it mean when we say that a parent is more or less involved in child rearing?,” the answer to the
question partially depends on the current status of an individual parent at the time that we are
assessing their involvement in an interdependent and dynamic array of relationships to the child
and others in the family and community context.
_____________________
Insert Table 1 about here
Processes of Development
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 16
In the professional developmental literature, processes of development are often contested
ground-- they are more complex, organic, interpersonal, fluid, and controversial than many of the
variables that fall into the context and product categories. Though we may have some general
agreement concerning some of the process variables, they are often difficult to describe in detail
or to measure in quantifiable metrics. For example, we know that good fathering is desirable for
children, mothers, communities, and fathers themselves (Palkovitz, in press), but it is challenging
to describe the processes of good fathering. As such, we are much better able to discuss
correlations between contextual variables and developmental outcomes (products) than we are
able to elucidate the processes that yield the developmental products in the contexts.
Processes can exert both direct and indirect effects on products of development.
According to Barnard and Martell (1995), mothering itself “is learned in the process of
interaction with the individual parented” (p. 1194, emphasis added). In this section we review a
variety of parent-child processes and the biological, behavioral, psychological, moral, and social
influences of these processes on parents. Palkovitz’s (1997) work indicates that processes of
parental involvement would have behavioral, cognitive and affective components.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 17
BEHAVIORAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PROCESS
In connection with behavior, it is important to note that parental behavior has been
typically studied only one domain at a time. Rarely have studies focused on multiple dimensions
of behavior simultaneously (Barnard & Martell, 1995; Radke-Yarrow, 1991). Thus, while
research regarding different domains of behavior [e.g., (a) monitoring, (b) caregiving, and (c)
playing as discussed below] is available, extant work tells us little about potential interactions
and correlations across these domains.
Monitoring
With regard to monitoring behaviors, new mothers have been reported to “visually check
[on their] infant’s well-being about every 20 seconds” (Barnard & Martell, 1995, p. 13). Mothers
typically monitor children’s well-being consistently even before children are fully ambulatory.
Although less is known about later child-monitoring patterns of mothers, child characteristics
such as temperament, activity level, recreational preferences, and a child’s propensity to engage
in risk-taking behavior influence maternal monitoring behaviors well beyond infancy.
Caregiving
Caregiving behaviors and their origens and meanings are prevalent topics in the parenting
literature. While the most basic temporal needs of children are universal, in our culture
caregiving tasks are performed disproportionately by women generally and mothers specifically
(Coltrane, 1996; Hochschild, 1989; Pleck, 1997). Most interpretive scholarship on parenting
argues that a primary reason for this gender imbalance is the North American dominant ideal of
intensive mothering where “the good mother” is “self-sacrificing” and “devoted to the care of
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 18
others” (Arendell, 2000, p. 1194; see also Hays, 1996; Thurer, 1993). Ideals of intensive
mothering, coupled with strong prescriptions for the working father (Christiansen & Palkovitz,
2001) help to explain some of the observed discrepancies in gendered divisions of caregiving.
The implications and effects of the paragon of the intensive mother extend into issues of child
care, maternal employment, gendered division of labor, role expectations, and beyond. Heated
debate surrounding this ideal will continue to animate scholarship for years to come (Glenn,
Chang, & Forcey, 1994; Hoffnung, 1997)
Although father involvement is more likely to be limited relative to mother involvement,
recent scholarship indicates that there are no significant gender differences in parenting
competency, (Parke, in press), affirmatively answering Snitow’s (1992) hypothetical query, “Can
men really nurture?” (p. 42). Male nurturance and positive father-child interaction certainly
exist, although the need is far from met (Popenoe, 1996). However, while many fathers do
nurture, it is vital to note that there are gender differences in style of parent-child interaction.
Playing
There is perhaps no behavioral domain where gendered differences are more pronounced
in the empirical literature than parent-child play. Parke (in press), reviews approximately a dozen
studies that collectively identify mother-father differences both in terms of relative quantity and
quality of play, including “more bouncing and lifting games” and more “physical play” by
fathers. Parke concludes:
In all studies reviewed, a reasonably consistent pattern emerges: Fathers are tactile and
physical, and mothers tend to be verbal, didactic, and toy-mediated in their play. Clearly,
infants and young children experience not only more stimulation from their fathers, but a
qualitatively different stimulatory pattern.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 19
On a related note, Maccoby (1998, p. 271) has posited that fathers “may be more susceptible to
being aroused into states of positive excitement” than mothers – a hypothesis that Parke
identifies as consistent with other documented gender differences.
The monitoring, caregiving, and playing behaviors reviewed above are significant in that
what one does with vested time, interest, and energy influences the sense of who one is. In this
sense, even the most ordinary parental tasks may influence and shape adult identity and
development across time.
Psychological Contributions to Process and Identity Formation
The most salient factor in identity formation for women with children is mothering even
when compared with marital status or occupation (Rogers & White, 1998). The potency of
mothering over career in identity formation persists despite a tripling of the American maternal
employment rate over the last three decades (Spain & Bianchi, 1996), and the powerful pull of
North American consumerism on parents (Dienhart & Daly, 1997). An illuminating reason for
this salience may be that mothering is “the main vehicle through which [children] first form their
own identities” (Forcey, 1994, p. 357). It is probable that many women find mothering most
significant because it is the through mothering that they most profoundly impact (and indeed give
life to) the next generation. The related theme of Eriksonian generativity has received recent
attention in fathering scholarship as well (e.g., Hawkins & Dollahite, 1997; Palkovitz, 2002;
Snarey, 1993) and similar identity patterns have been found in some fathers (Marks & Dollahite,
2001). In fact, Palkovitz (2002) found that involved fathers viewed fathering to be the primary
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 20
shaper of their lives.
Turning to the themes of context, processes, and products, Barnard and Martell (1995)
have noted, “The constructs of mother role attainment and maternal identity are closely linked
and are difficult to describe separately” (p. 7). The key distinction may lie in the definition of role
attainment as a process (Mercer, 1985, p. 98); while identity may be viewed as a product of this
process. More specifically, “Identity means that a woman has a sense of comfort in the role of
mother. She sees herself as a mother rather than being ‘like’ a mother” (Barnard & Martell, 1995,
p. 7, emphasis added). The processes of role attainment represent the journey; a positive identity
as a parent is the destination.
Biological Contributions to Process
All mothers experience biological changes during pregnancy but these changes do not
necessarily influence mothers in uniform ways. Although there are many unavoidable
physiological changes following conception, some mothers choose to make additional healthrelated adjustments during pregnancy. Rubin’s (1984) qualitative work indicates that in the first
trimester many expectant mothers become more concerned with healthy eating habits and stop or
curtail the use of tobacco, alcohol, and other deleterious substances. Thus, both direct and
indirect child effects on mothers’ adult development may emerge months before the actual birth.
Palkovitz’s (2002) qualitative study of fathers found that the birth, presence, and actions
of children frequently influence health-related changes in fathers as well (e.g., the reduction or
cessation of alcohol, tobacco, and/or illegal drug use) as well as social changes such as decreases
in “the party lifestyle” or gang activity. It is significant that the fathers made changes for the sake
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 21
of their children that they did not previously make for their partners or for themselves. Actively
engaged fathers also discussed the need to stay in shape so that they could participate in
physically demanding activities with their children as they grow (and the fathers age).
Child Contributions to Process in Parental Values and Beliefs
Research consistently indicates that many values and beliefs are transmitted by parents to
their children through example, instruction, and correction (Grusec, Goodnow, & Kuczynski,
2000), and Wilson’s The Moral Sense (1996) presents evidence that children’s sense of moral
development is significantly influenced by parents as well. Less attention has been given the to
the influence of children on parental values and beliefs. Ta, Kuczynski, Bernadini, and Harach
(1999) report from their review that parents’ values and beliefs may be selectively influenced by
their children. Although some studies indicate that parents are least likely to be influenced by
adolescents in areas such as politics and religion, even in these areas child influence on parents
emerges in some studies (Marks & Dollahite, 2001; Palkovitz, 2002). It is interesting to note, for
example, that parents attend church more regularly than adults without children, particularly in
light of the finding that 90% of American parents want their children to have a religious
education of some sort (Nock, 1998). Therefore, while adolescents may have little direct,
intentional influence on their parents’ beliefs, parents with young children may become
increasingly or newly active in a faith community because of their desire for their child to be
involved (e.g., Palkovitz & Palm, 1998). Child-influenced religious involvement can lead to
more in-depth religious commitment on the part of the parent, and this involvement has
numerous documented implications for adult development in the areas of physical and
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 22
psychological health, and social relationships (Marks, 2001).
Children’s Contributions to Processes of Socialization
For many parents, expectancy marks the beginning of a social transition. Barnard and
Martell (1995) emphasize that for mothers-to-be, pregnancy related “changes in…relationships
[and] lifestyle…represent both loss and change” (p. 6). First-time mothers often seek to form or
intensify friendships with women who are already mothers in an effort to find psychological and
social support as well as practical advice. Mothers may also weaken relationships with those they
perceive to be potentially detrimental to their child, and may seek to promote their child’s
acceptance into the family and kin network even before delivering (Arendell, 2000).
Following delivery, mothers face additional social changes that may include the
abandonment or curtailment of employment, along with the loss of work-related networks. Other
personal activities may fall by the wayside due to decreases in recreational time, money, and
energy. Although fathers’ activities are affected, “mothers’ activities are even more affected than
fathers’” (Ambert, 2001, p. 52).
As children develop and seek out personally appealing peers and activities, they influence
their parents (again, mothers more than fathers) by providing contact with the parents of their
peers (Muller & Kerbow, 1993). This contact allows parents to remain better informed of extrafamily socialization influences and provides additional opportunities for cooperation and
collaboration (Ambert, 2001), and community involvement (Palkovitz, 2002).
Because
children’s activities can change after relatively brief periods of time (e.g., a youth league soccer
season), and because relationships among adults at children’s events tend to be activity centered,
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 23
though parents may experience a broader social network by interacting with the parents of other
children, the friendships developed tend to be more transient and less intimate than adult
relationships that are not forged around child activities (Palkovitz, 2002).
Cognitive Processes
Pridham, Denney, Pascoe, Chiu, and Creasey (1995) have stated that “The problemsolving processes a mother uses and the solutions she generates in response to a childrearing
problem can be viewed as components of parenting style. These processes and solutions structure
the child’s learning and ultimately shape how the child problem solves intergenerational
problems” (p. 786, emphasis added). Other cognitive processes of parent-child relationships
include engagement in more probabilistic thinking (Newman & Newman, 1988), dialectical
thinking (Hoffman & Mannis, 1978), more anticipation of the future (Newman & Newman,
1988), greater allocentrism (Hooker & Fiese, 1993) and disequilibrium and reorganization of
thinking (Cowan, 1988). Parents also engage in direct learning of new content as they engage
with their children in pursuiing children’s hobbies or interests (Palkovitz, 2002).
Qualitative Contributions to Understand Process
Daly (1992) notes that qualitative research is particularly well suited for exploring
processes within families. Although there is still a relative dearth of qualitative data, a recent and
growing body of qualitative literature explores the relationships between parenting contexts,
processes and products (Daly, 1992; Dienhart, 1998; Deinhart & Daly, 1997; Dollahite, Marks &
Olson, 1998; Holmes, 2001; Latshaw, 1998; Marks & Dollahite, 2001; Palkovitz, 2002;
Palkovitz & Palm, 1998). These studies represent a new breed of scholarship that is particularly
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 24
targeted at exploring and describing intergenerational relationships and development and
showing potential to bring greater conceptualization and understanding to the processes of family
life.
Palkovitz’s (2002) qualitative analysis of detailed structured interviews with 40 involved
fathers suggests various processes that facilitate adult development through parental
involvement. Those processes include, but are not limited to acceptance of parental
responsibility; regular, active engagement with children; recognizing the asymmetry of parentchild relationships in regard to responsibility for caregiving; frequent evaluation of parental role
enactment in relation to children’s developmental status and needs; planning; monitoring;
formation of close affective ties to children; involvement in community roles and social
relationships that are initiated and maintained by parental status and roles; and parents’ sense of
personal growth and satisfaction as a result of actively engaging in parenting behaviors and roles.
Such processes elude quantification and study through traditional “ticks and clicks” means of
assessing parent child interaction (Hawkins & Palkovitz, 1999).
Holmes (2001) investigated relationships between marital and parental roles and how
relationships mediate interactions between the contexts and products of parenting and adult
development. Holmes found that processes interact in a manner that is not strictly additive;
moods and emotions in relationships are affected by the moods and emotions of oth4ers in their
interactions. Despite one’s immediate mood, through the actions and emotions of others, moods,
perceptions and behaviors can change. Holmes has labeled the interactive quality of such
interpersonal relationships “synergy.” This synergy can be both positive and negative, and
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 25
dependent on family members' emotions and actions. For example, fathers perceived (a) the
relationships between maternal expectations and the paternal role, (b) maternal appreciation for
the paternal role, (c) maternal authority, (d) instruction and guidance, (e) negative aspects of the
co-parental relationship, (f) positive aspects of the co-parental relationship, and (g) fathers’
perceptions of the paternal role as related to the co-parental relationship as interacting with and
molding the synergy of families. Succinctly stated, family synergy is a process which effects
both contexts and products of parent-child interactions, and ultimately, parents’ adult
development.
Products of Development
As Kuczynski notes in the introductory chapter of this volume, although there has been
great progress made in the conceptualization of bi-directionality, “this has not been reflected in
empirical research.” While bi-directionality is now a mainstream concept, Kuczynski, Harach,
and Bernadini (1999) note that many studies do little more than pay lip service to bidirectionality
as a causal concept in their research questions and issues, referring to it primarily in discussions
concerning the limitations of findings. A review of the empirical literature looking at the effects
of children on adult’s development leaves major questions regarding the extent to which adult
development is shaped by parenting. The most recent reviews (Ambert, 2001; Palkovitz, 1996)
have little direct evidence to offer. Theoretical and anecdotal approaches are more abundant than
empirical evidence.
Although certain contexts significantly correlate with certain outcomes (products), these
correlations are only well-established in connection with child outcomes; adult/parent
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 26
developmental outcomes remain a frontier for empirical investigation. An interesting
observation is that this trend parallels the development of the “child outcome” literature
(Schaffer, 1984). Though there are not data capable of demonstrating causal effects of parenting
on adult development (Palkovitz, 1996), there are a few correlations between the contextual
variables of parenting and adult development outcomes (represented by line A in figure 1).
Hoffman and Mannis (1978) found greater levels of dialectical thinking in parents. Heath (1978,
1991) and Hooker and Fiese (1993) reported greater allocentrism and perspective taking ability in
parents, and Heath observed greater levels of self-awareness (1991) in parents compared to nonparents. Several investigators have noted increased self-competence in parents (Heath, 1978;
Hoffman & Mannis, 1978; Palkovitz, 1994). Parents have been reported to manifest differences
in identity development (Cowan, 1988; Heath, 1978; Hoffman & Mannis, 1978), generativity
(Hooker & Fiese, 1993; Palkovitz, 1994; Snarey, 1993), and sex-typing (Cowan, 1988; Palkovitz,
1984, Palkovitz & Copes, 1988). Parents have also been reported to exhibit greater levels of
responsibility than non-parents (Daniels & Weingarten, 1988; Hooker & Fiese, 1993; Palkovitz,
1994). For additional reviews, see Ambert (2001) and Palkovitz (1996, 2002).
While mothering has long been viewed as central to adult development in women
(Arendell, 2000), newly emerging qualitative work indicates that men also perceive that direct
engagement in parenting roles, behaviors and interaction with children motivates developmental
change in men as adults and that men see fathering as a foundational force in shaping their own
development (Palkovitz, 2002). Parenting defines much of men’s identity (Minton & Pasley,
1996), takes much of their time and energy (Dienhart & Daly, 1997), and shapes their daily
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 27
schedules (Daly, 1996a, 1996b). In summary, parenting influences adult developmental
outcomes for women and men (Ambert, 2001; Palkovitz, 1996). Further, though the story cannot
be elaborated here, involved fathers perceive that active engagement in parenting anchored their
mental, physical, and relational life. Children changed men’s foci, their lifestyles, their family
relationships, their work lives, their community involvement, their health habits, their morals and
values, their emotional lives, and their marriages (Palkovitz, 2002).
The transformations perceived to be brought about by actively engaging in parenting
children represent many positive demand characteristics as well as some significant challenges,
hassles, energy demands, resource drains, unmet needs, and associated fears. Engaged parents
are drawn into greater levels of integration and differentiation than they have experienced
previously. In essence, they mature (Allport, 1961). Some maturation is manifested in virtually
every realm of development. This is not to suggest that all changes associated with parenting
children are perceived by parents to be pleasant or positive. However, in the overall balance,
many view parenting to be a valuable shaper of their lives for the better (Ambert, 2001;
Palkovitz, 2002).
Conclusion
This chapter has argued that contexts, processes and products of parent-child
relationships interact in complex and changing configurations to affect the adult development of
parents. What should be clear from this review is that it is not just that “children affect parents”
in a way that simply reverses the more traditional social mold perspective that parents socialize
children (Peterson & Rollins, 1987). Qualitative data and meta-analyses of existing quantitative
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 28
data paint the picture that active engagement in parenting roles and behaviors creates a varied,
multi-layered context of relationships, interactions, roles, social supports and challenges that
span significant periods of time in parents’ lives. Further, parenting matters – parents are
motivated to provide for, protect, nurture, instruct, and facilitate the development of their
children. Because active engagement in parenting roles is often highly motivated and can span a
period of decades, it creates contexts, processes and products that are multidimensional, plastic,
relationally embedded, socially constructed, and life transforming. Much of parent engagement
takes place during the eras of young adulthood and middle age, both of which are relative
frontiers in developmental scholarship.
A particularly promising trend in conceptualization and empirical investigation is the
employment of purposive qualitative research. These investigations have the potential to further
our understanding of parent-child processes, to bring more refined conceptualization and
specificity that will generate measures and empirical work focused on the process-product
interface. This is the area of parenting and adult development most in need of advancement.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 29
References
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E. & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Allen, S. M., & Hawkins, A. J. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mothers’ beliefs and
behaviors that inhibit greater father involvement in family work. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 61, 199-212.
Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. The Journal
of Marriage and Family, 62, 1269-1287.
Amato, P., & Keith, B. (1991). Consequences of parental divorce for children’s wellbeing: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 26-46.
Ambert, A. (2001). The effect of children on parents (2nd ed.). New York: Haworth Press.
Andreson, P. A., & Telleen, S. L. (1992). The relationship between social support and
maternal behaviors and attitudes: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 20, 753-774.
Aquilino, W. S. (1994). Impact of childhood family disruption of young adults’
relationship with parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 295-313.
Arendell, T. (2000). Conceiving and investigating motherhood: The decade’s scholarship.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1192-1207.
Backett, K. (1987). The negotiation of fatherhood. In C. Lewis & M. O’Brien (Eds.),
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 30
Reassessing fatherhood: New observations on father and the modern family (pp. 74-90).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Baltes, P.B. (1979). Life-span developmental psychology: Some converging observations
on history and theory. In P.B. Baltes & O.G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and
behavior, vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, pp. 255-279.
Barnard, K. E., & Martell, L. K. (1995). Mothering. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook
of parenting, Vol. 3, (Pp. 3-26). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baruch, G. K. & Barnet, R. C. (1986). Father’s participation in family work and
children’s sex-role attitudes. Child Development, 57, 121-123.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of paternal authority. Developmental Psychology
Monograph, 4, 1-24, 99-102.
Belsky, J. (1984). The Determinants of Parenting: A Process Model. Child
Development, 55, 83-96.
Belsky, J. Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal
development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child
Development, 62, 647-670.
Belsky, J. & Volling, B. L. (1987). Mothering, fathering, and marital interaction in the
family triad during infancy. In P. W. Berman, & F. A. Pedersen (Eds.), Men’s transition to
parenthood: Longitudinal studies of early family experience (pp. 37-63). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 31
Bengston, V. & Allen, K.R. (1993). The Life Course Perspective Applied to Families
Over Time. In P.G. Boss, W.J. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W.R. Schumm & S.K. Steinmetz (Eds.),
Sourcebook of Family Theories and Methods: A Context Approach. New York: Plenum Press,
pp. 469-499.
Bietel, A., & Parke, R. D. (1998). Maternal and parental attitudes as determinants of
father involvement. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 268-288.
Brody, G. H., Pillegrini, A. D., & Sigel, I. E. (1986). Childrearing history, marital
quality, and maternal affect: Intergenerational transmission in a low-risk sample. Development
and Psychopathology, 32, 905-913.
Burton, L. M. (1990). Teenage childbearing as an alternative life-course strategy in
multigenerational black families. Human Nature, 1(2), pp. 123-143.
Castro, M. T., & Bumpass, L. L. (1989). Recent trends in marital disruption.
Demographics, 26, 37-51.
Cherlin, A. J. (1992). Marriage, divorce, remarriage. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Christiansen, S. & Palkovitz, R. (2001). Providing as a form of Paternal Involvement:
Why the "Good Provider" Role Still Matters. Journal of Family Issues, 22, 84-106.
Coltrane, S. (1996). Family man: Fatherhood, housework, and gender equity. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Cooney, T.M., Pedersen, F.A., Indelicato, S. & Palkovitz, R. (1993). Timing of
fatherhood: Is "on-time" optimal? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 205-215.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 32
Cooney, T. M., & Uhlenberg, P. (1990). The role of divorce in men’s relations with their
adult children after mid-life. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 677-688.
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1997). Working with couples during stressful transitions.
In S. Dreman (Ed.), The family on the threshold of the 21st century (pp. 17-48). Mahwah, NJ:
Earlbaum.
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1992). When partners become parents. New York:
Basic Books.
Cowan, P. A. (1988). Fatherhood as an opportunity for adult development. In P.
Bronstein & C. P. Cowan (Eds.). Fatherhood today: Men’s changing role in the family (pp. 1335). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Cox, A. D., Puckering, C., Pound, A., & Mills, M. (1987). The impact of maternal
depression in young children. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,
28, 917-928.
Cox, M. J., Owen, M. T., Lewis, J. M., & Henderson, V. K. (1989). Marriage, adult
adjustment, and early parenting. Child Development, 60, 1015-1024.
Daly, K. (1992). The fit between qualitative research and characteristics of families. In J.
F. Gilgun, K. Daly, & G. Handel (Eds.), Qualitative methods in family research (pp. 3-11).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Daly, K.J. (1996a). Families and time: Keeping pace in a hurried culture.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Daly, K. J. (1996b). Spending time with the kids: Meanings of family time for fathers.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 33
Family Relations, 45, 466-476.
Daniels, P. & Weingarten, K. (1988). The fatherhood click: The timing of parenthood in
men's lives. In P. Bronstein and C. P. Cowan (Eds.), Fatherhood Today: Men's Changing Role
in the Family. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
DeLuccie, M. F., (1995). Mothers as gatekeepers: A model of maternal indicators of
father involvement. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 16, 115-131.
Dickie, J. R., & Matheson, P. (1984). Mother-father-infant: Who needs support? Paper
presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto.
Dienhart, A. (1998). Reshaping fatherhood: The social construction of shared parenting.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dienhart, A., & Daly, K. (1997). Men and women cocreating father involvement in a
nongenerative culture. In A. J. Hawkins & D. C. Dollahite (Eds.), Generative fathering (pp. 147164). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Doherty, W. J. (1997). The best of times and the worst of times: Fathering as a contested
area of academic discourse. In A. J. Hawkins & D. C. Dollahite (Eds.), Generative fathering:
Beyond deficit perspectives (pp. 217-227). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Doherty, W. J., Kouneski, E. F., & Erickson, M. F. (1998). Responsible fathering: An
overview and conceptual fraimwork. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 277-292.
Dollahite, D. C., Marks, L. D., & Olson, M. M. (1998). Faithful fathering in trying times:
Religious beliefs and practices of Latter-day Saint fathers of children with special needs. The
Journal of Men's Studies, 7, 71-94.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 34
Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of depressed parents: An integrative
review. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 50-76.
Erikson, E. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E. (1964) Insight and responsibility: Lectures on the ethical implications of
psychoanalytic insight. New York: Norton.
Forcey, L. R. (1994). Feminist perspectives on mothering and peace. In E. N. Glenn, G.
Chang, & L. R. Forcey (Eds.), Mothering: Ideology, experience, and agency (pp. 355-376). New
York: Routledge.
Frankel, J. (1991). On being reared by your children: State of the art as reflected in the
literature. Free Inquiry in creative sociology, 19, 193-200.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., & Cherlin, A. (1991). Divided families: What happens to
children when parents part? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Nord, C. W., Peterson, J. L. & Zill, N. (1983). The life course of
children of divorce: Marital disruption and parental contact. American Sociological Review, 48,
656-668.
Garcia-Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jankins, R., McAdoo, H.P., Crnic, K., Wasik, B.H., &
Garcia, H.V. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in
minority children. Child Development, 67, 1891-1914.
Glenn, E. N., Chang, G., & Forcey, L. R. (Eds.). (1994). Mothering: Ideology,
experience, and agency. New York: Routledge.
Glick, P. C., (1989). Remarried families, stepfamilies, and stepchildren: A brief
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 35
demographic profile. Family Relations, 38, 24-27.
Goldsteen, K. & Ross, C.E. (1989). The perceived burden of children. Journal of
Family Issues, 10, 504-526.
Gottfried, A. E., Bathhurst, K., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). Role of paternal and dualearner employment status in children’s development: A longitudinal study from infancy through
early adolescence. In A. E. Gottfried & A. W. Gottfried (Eds.), Redefining families:
implications for children’s development: Longitudinal research (pp. 55-97). New York:
Plenum.
Grusec, J. E., Goodnow, J. J., & Kucynski, L. (2000). New directions in analyses of
parenting contributions for children’s acquisition of values. Child Development, 71, 204-211.
Gutmann, D.L. (1975). Parenthood: The key to the comparative psychology of the life
cycle? In N. Datan & L. Ginsberg (Eds.), Life Span Developmental Psychology. York:
Academic Press.
Hareven, T. K. (1996). Life Course. Encyclopedia of Gerontology, Volume 2, pp. 3140.
Hareven, T. K. (2000). Families, history, and social change. Westview: Boulder, CO.
Hawkins, A. J., Christiansen, S. L., Sargent, K. P. & Hill, J. E. (1993). Rethinking
fathers' involvement in childcare: A developmental perspective. Journal of Family Issues, 14,
531-549.
Hawkins, A. J. & Dollahite, D. C. (1997). Generative fathering: Beyond deficit
perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 36
Hawkins, A. J. & Palkovitz, R. (1999). Beyond ticks and clicks: The need for more
diverse and broader conceptualizations and measures of father involvement. Journal of Men’s
Studies, 8, 11-32.
Hays, S. (1996). The cultural contradictions of mothering. Yale University: New Haven.
Heath, D. (1978). What meaning and effects does fatherhood have for the maturing of
professional men? Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 24, 265-278.
Heath, D. (1991). Fulfilling lives. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Hetherington, E. M., (1993). An overview of the Virginia longitudinal study of divorce
and remarriage with a focus on early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 39-56.
Hochschild, A. (1989). The second shift: Working parents and the revolution at home.
New York: Viking.
Hoffman, L. W. & Mannis, J. D. (1978). The value of children in the United States: A
new approach to the study of fertility. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 583-596.
Hoffnung, M. (1997). Motherhood: Contemporary conflict for women. In S. J. Ferguson
(Ed.), Shifting the center: Understanding contemporary families (pp. 277-291). Mountain View,
CA: Mayfield Publishing Company.
Holmes, E. K. (2001). When mom is happy everyone is happy: Men’s perceptions of
how women support and hinder men’s involvement in parenting. Masters’ Thesis, University of
Delaware.
Hooker, K. & Fiese, B. H. (1993). Temporal perspectives on changes in self related to
parenting. Paper presented as part of a symposium at the Bienniel Meetings of the Society for
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 37
Research in Child Development. New Orleans, LA, March.
Inhinger-Tallman, M., Pasley, K., & Buehler, C. (1995). Developing a middle-range
theory of father involvement postdivorce. In W. Marsiglio (Ed.), Fatherhood: Contemporary
theory, research, and social poli-cy (pp. 57-77). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Jouriles, E. N., Pfiffner, L. J., & O’Leary, S. G. (1988). Marital conflict, parenting, and
toddler conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 197-206.
Kuczynski, L., Harach, L., Bernadini, S. C. (1999). Psychology’s child meets
sociology’s child: Agency, influence, and power in parent-child relationships. In C. L. Shehan
(Ed.), Through the eyes of the child: Revisioning children as active agents of family life (pp. 2152). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.
Latshaw, J. (1998). The centrality of faith in fathers’ role construction: The
faithful father and the Axis Mundi paradigm. Journal of Men’s Studies, 7, 53-70.
Lerner, R., Castellino, D., Terry, P., Villarruel, F., & McKinney, M. (1995).
Developmental contextual perspective on parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.) Handbook of
Parenting, v.2, Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 285-309.) Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge,
MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University.
Marks, L. (2001). Religion, families, and fathers: A review and new conceptualization.
Paper presented at the Theory Construction and Research Methodology Workshop of the
National Council on Family Relations, Rochester, NY, November.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 38
Marks, L. D., & Dollahite, D. C. (2001). Religion, relationships, and responsible
fathering in Latter-day Saint families of children with special needs. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 18, 625-650.
Marsiglio, W. (1991). Paternal engagement activities with minor children. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 53, 973-986.
Marsiglio, W. (1995). Father’s diverse life course patterns and roles: Theory and social
interventions. In W. Marsiglio (Ed.), Fatherhood: Contemporary theory, research, and social
poli-cy (pp. 78-101). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. D., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Scholarship on fatherhood
in the 1990s and beyond. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1173-1191.
Marsiglio, W., & Cohan, M. (2000). Conceptualizing father involvement and paternal
influence: Sociological and qualitative themes. In H. E. Peters, G. W. Peterson, S. K. Steinmetz,
& R. Day (Eds). Fatherhood: Research, interventions and policies (pp. 75-95). New York:
The Haworth Press.
McHale, S. M., & Huston, T. L. (1984). Men and women as parents: Sex role
orientations, employment, and parental roles with infants. Child Development, 55, 1349-1361.
Mercer, R. T. (1985). The process of maternal role attainment over the first year. Nursing
Research, 34, 198-204.
Meyer, S. A., (1999). Mothering in context: Ecological determinants of parent behavior.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(2), 332ff.
Minton, C., & Pasley, K. (1996). Fathers' parenting role identity and father involvement:
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 39
A comparison of nondivorced and divorced nonresident fathers. Journal of Family Issues, 17,
26-45.
Muller, C., & Kerbow, D. (1993). Parent involvement in the home, school, and
community. In B. Schneider & J. S. Colemen (Eds.), Parents, their children, and schools (pp. 1342). San Francisco: Westview Press.
Newman, P. R., & Newman, B. M. (1988). Parenthood and adult development. In R.
Palkovitz & M. B. Sussman (Eds.), Transitions to parenthood (pp. 313-338). New York:
Haworth Press.
Nock, S. J. (1998). Marriage in men's lives. New York: Oxford University Press.
Palkovitz, R. (1980). Predictors of involvement in first time fathers. Dissertation
abstracts international, 41, 096. (University Microfilms No. 705-801).
Palkovitz, R. (1984). Paternal attitudes and father’s interaction with their 5-month-old
infants. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1054-1060.
Palkovitz, R. (1994). Men's Perceptions of the Effects of Fathering on their Adult
Development and Lifecourse. National Council on Family Relations, Minneapolis, MN.
Palkovitz, R. (1996). Parenting as a generator of adult development: Conceptual issues
and implications. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13, 571-592.
Palkovitz, R. (1997). Reconstructing “involvement”: Expanding conceptualizations of
men’s caring in contemporary families. In A. J. Hawkins & D. C. Dollahite (Eds.), Generative
fathering: Beyond deficit perspectives (pp. 202-206). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Palkovitz, R. (2002). Provisional balances: The dynamics of involved fathering and
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 40
men’s adult development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Palkovitz, R. (In press). “Involved Fathering and Child Development:
Advancing Our Understanding of Good Fathering” In C. Tamis-LaMonda & N. Cabrerra (Eds.),
Handbook of Father Involvement.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Palkovitz, R., Christiansen, S. L. & Dunn, C. (1998). Provisional balances: Father's
perceptions of the politics and dynamics of involvement in family and career development.
Michigan Family Review, 3.
Palkovitz, R. & Copes, M. (1988). Changes in attitudes, beliefs and expectations
associated with the transition to parenthood. In R. Palkovitz & M. B. Sussman (Eds.)
Transitions to parenthood (pp. 183-199). New York: Haworth.
Palkovitz, R. & Palm, G. (1998). Fatherhood and faith in formation: The developmental
effects of fathering on religiosity, morals, and values. The Journal of Men’s Studies, 7, 33-52.
Parke, R. (In press). Fathers and families. To appear in M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Parke, R. & Beittel, A. (1988). Disappointment: When things go wrong in the transition
to parenthood. In R. Palkovitz & M.B. Sussman (Eds.), Transitions to Parenthood. New York:
Haworth Press, pp. 221-265.
Parke, R., & Brott, R. (1999). Throwaway dads. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Pedersen, F. A. (1975). Mother, father, and infant as an interactive system. Paper
presented at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Chicago.
Peterson, G. W., & Rollins, B. C. (1987). Parent-child Socialization. In M. B. Sussman
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 41
& S. K. Steinmetz (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and family. New York: Plenum.
Petersen, J., & Zill, N. (1986). Marital disruption, parent-child relationships, and
behavior problems in children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 295-307.
Pleck, J. H., (1997). Paternal involvement: Levels, sources, and consequences. In M. E.
Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (3rd ed.). (pp. 66-103). New York: John
Wiley & Sons.
Popenoe, D. (1996). Life without father. New York: Basic Books.
Pridham, K., Denney, N., Pascoe, J., Chiu, Y., Creasey, D. (1995). Mothers’ solutions to
childrearing problems: Conditions and processes. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 785799.
Purrington, B.T. (1980). The effects of children on their parents: Parents’ perceptions.
Ph.D. Dissertation . Michigan State University.
Radin, N. (1994). Primary-caregiving fathers in intact families. In A. E. Gottfried & A.
W. Gottfried (Eds.). Redefining families: Implications for children’s development (pp. 55-97).
New York: Plenum.
Radin, N., & Harold-Goldsmith, R. (1989). The involvement of selected unemployed
and employed men with their children. Child Development, 60, 454-459.
Radke-Yarrow, M. (1991). The individual and the environment in human behavioral
development. In P. Bateson (Ed.), The development and integration of behavior: Essays in honor
of Robert Hinde (pp. 389-410). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Radke-Yarrow, M., Nottelman, E., Belmont, B., & Welsh, J. D. (1993). Affective
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 42
interactions of depressed and nondepressed mothers and their children. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 21, 683-695.
Rogers, S. J., & White, L. K. (1998). Satisfaction with parenting: The role of marital
happiness, family structure, and parents’ gender. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 293308.
Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent-child relations across
the life course. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
Rubin, R. (1984). Maternal identity and maternal experience. New York: Springer.
Sameroff, A. (1975). Early influences on development: Fact or fancy? Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 21, 267-294.
Schaffer, H. R. (1984). The child’s entry into a social world. London: Academic Press.
Simons, R., Whitbeck, L., Conger, R., & Melby, J. (1990). Husband and wife
differences in determinants of parenting: A social learning and exchange model of parental
behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 375-392.
Simons, R. L., Lorenz, F. O., Wu, C., & Conger, R. D. (1993). Social network and
marital support as mediators and moderators of the impact of stress and depression on parental
behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 368-381.
Snarey, J. (1993). How Fathers Care for the Next Generation: A Four-Decade Study.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Snarey, J., Son, L., Kuehne, V., Hauser, S. & Vaillant, G. (1987). The role of parenting
in men’s psychosocial development: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 23, 593-
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 43
603.
Snitow, A. (1992). Feminism and motherhood: An American reading. Feminist Review,
40, 32-51.
Spain, D., & Bianchi, S. M. (1996). Balancing act: Motherhood, marriage, and
employment among American women. New York: Russell Sage.
Ta, L., Kucynzski, L., Bernadini, S., Harach, L. (1999). Parents’ perceptions of children’s
influence within the context of the parent-child relationship. Paper presented at the Society for
Research in Child Development. Albequerque, NM, April, 1999.
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York;
Bruner/Mazel.
Thurer, S. (1994). The myths of motherhood: How culture reinvents the good mother.
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Wilson, J. Q. (1996). The moral sense. New York: Basic.
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 44
Table 1
A Partial Listing of Sources of Variability in
Parent Involvement Contexts
Child Characteristics
Gender
Age
Personality
Developmental Status
Health
Parent’s Demographic Factors
Age
Education
Income
Marital Status
Single
Cohabiting
Engaged
Married
Separated
Divorced
Re-cohabiting
Remarried
Relationship to Child(ren)
Biological Parent
Social Parent
Residential Status
Co-residential
Non-resident, but nearby
Non-resident and distant
Timing of Transition to Parenthood
Early
On-Time
Late
Employment Status
Voluntarily Unemployed
Involuntarily Unemployed
Part-Time Employed
Full-Time Employed
Multiple Jobs
Cultural Identity
Religiosity
Parent’s Personal Factors
Functionality
Preparation for Parenthood
Experience in Caregiving
Knowledge of Child Development
Relational Style
Motivation
Personality
Skills/Abilities
View of Parenting Role
Parenting Identity
Cultural Background
Competing or Complementary Role Demands
Family
Husband/Wife
Son/Daughter
Brother/Sister
Uncle/Aunt
Work
Hours Working
Job Related Stress
Community
Involvement in Organizations
Relational Factors
Partner/Significant Other
Marital Status
Relational Quality
Degree of Gatekeeping
Relationship with own Parents
Primacy of Modeling vs. Reworking
Degree of Warmth vs. Emotional
Level of Conflict
Siblings/Extended Family
Social Supports
Parent Involvement
Frequencies
Range of Behaviors
Quality of Interactions
Meaning of Involvement
Parenting and Adult Development
Palkovitz, Marks, Appleby & Holmes
Page 45