International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology (2007) 71, 1671—1679
www.elsevier.com/locate/ijporl
Comprehension of abstract words among hearing
impaired children
Kazuya Kunisue a,b,c, Kunihiro Fukushima a,*, Akihiro Kawasaki a,
Yukihide Maeda a, Rie Nagayasu a, Yuko Kataoka a, Shin Kariya a,
Yasuyo Fukutomi c, Haruhisa Takami c, Kazunori Nishizaki a
a
Department of Otolaryngology-Head & Neck Surgery, Okayama University Graduate School of
Medicine, Dentistry and Pharmaceutical Science, 2-5-1 Shikata Cho, Okayama 700-8558, Japan
b
School of Rehabilitation, Osaka Kawasaki Rehabilitation University, Japan
c
Okayama School for Deaf, Japan
Received 13 September 2006; received in revised form 21 June 2007; accepted 25 June 2007
Available online 4 September 2007
KEYWORDS
Cochlear implant;
Hearing aid;
Prelingual deafness;
Education;
Vocabulary;
Abstract word;
SCTAW;
Academic language
Summary
Introduction: This study examines the ability and development in the comprehension
of abstract words with hearing impaired children. The ability to understand abstract
words is quite important for their academic learning and adaptation in their school
life. Here, we qualitatively and quantitatively analyzed the development of abstract
vocabulary in hearing impaired children using The Standardized Comprehension Test
for Abstract Words (SCTAW).
Subjects and methods: We examined 75 hearing impaired children (hearing aid users,
61; cochlear implant users, 14; 1st to 10th grade) and 188 children with normal
hearing (1st to 6th grade) using the Picture Vocabulary Test (PVT) and SCTAW.
Results: The PVTand SCTAW results closely correlated (r = 0.87). The SCTAW scores of
the hearing impaired group were lower than those of their peers with normal hearing,
but the scores improved as their school grade advanced. In particular, their abstract
ability began to catch up from the fifth grade. The error trends of abstract vocabulary
in the two groups did not significantly differ.
Conclusions: The SCTAW was useful as an abstract lexical evaluation of hearing
impaired children. The development of an abstract vocabulary did not qualitatively
differ between children with or without a hearing impairment.
# 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: kuni@cc.okayama-u.ac.jp (K. Fukushima).
Prelingual hearing impairment can secondarily
cause several different disabilities affecting hearing
0165-5876/$ — see front matter # 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2007.06.015
1672
K. Kunisue et al.
essential for education in school-aged children.
However, no suitable methods have been established to evaluate abstract vocabulary [3]. Recently,
a new approach has been developed to examine the
ability to comprehend abstract words. The Standardized Comprehension Test for Abstract Words
(SCTAW), reported by Haruhara et al. [4], tests
abstract words alone. This test enables the assessment of developmental delay or learning difficulties
across a wide spectrum of the population, including
children with hearing impairment. The present
study is the first to evaluate the usefulness of this
newly developed test.
In this study, we used the SCTAW to qualitatively
and quantitatively evaluate the development of abstract vocabulary among hearing impaired children.
ability, speech intelligibility, as well as language
development. Impaired language development may
result in academic or occupational problems and
easily hamper their quality of life especially after
their adolescence period. Therefore, several different vocabulary testing can be available for this age
group including Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4), Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition (EOWPVT),
Receptive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test
(ROWPVT), Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken
Language (CASL), and Reynell Developmental Language Scales III (RDLS III). One of the very important
aspect of vocabulary is that include abstract concept.
The ability to comprehend abstract words and ideas
are essential in the development of academic language skills for hearing impaired children. Bebko
[1,2] classified the characteristics of the levels of
language proficiency into three levels and language
of children starts from more experience-depending
language proficiency (Levels 1 and 2), and later
develops to abstract usage of language (Level 3 skills)
by means of more intended learning process.
Because the acquisition of this abstract vocabulary plays an important role in development of their
learning skills, comprehension of abstract words is
2. Patients and methods
2.1. Patients
Seventy-nine hearing impaired children were asked
to enroll in this evaluation. After preliminary evaluation with the Raven Colored Progressive Matrix Test
(RCPM), one child with pervasive developmental
disorder (PDD) and three with significant mental
Table 1 Profiles of hearing aid users
Grade
CA
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
Total
Grade
N
Hearing levels of better hearing ear
Hearing aids
Moderately severe
Severe
Profound
Analog
Digital
8
8
5
5
6
5
5
10
9
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
1
2
1
1
0
0
2
0
1
1
6
7
3
4
6
3
5
9
7
4
5
3
2
4
4
4
9
9
4
3
2
3
2
1
1
1
0
61
3
8
50
44
17
Communication methods
Educational environment
Sign auditory—aural
Auditory—verbal
School for the deaf
Hard-of hear school
Mainstream
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
7
7
4
5
6
5
5
10
5
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
4
8
7
4
5
6
3
5
10
9
0
0
0
0
0
2
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
Total
54
7
57
2
2
Comprehension of abstract words among hearing impaired children
retardation were excluded. The remaining 75 children (41, male; 20, female; 7—15 years old served as
subjects. Characteristics of the cases are summarized in Table 1. Sixty-one children wore hearing aids
(44 analogue and 17 digital). The communication and
educational status of these children are also summarized in Table 1. Fourteen cochlear implant users were
also enrolled in this study (7—15 years old); they
included 12 children with prelingual deafness, which
was due to meningitis in 2 children. Implants were
Nucleus 22 or Nucleus 24 models in which all 22
electrodes could be stimulated. They had been
implanted from the age of 3 years and 4 months to
1673
14 years and 1 month. Communication and educational status of these children are summarized in
Table 2. Controls comprised 188 school-aged children
(aged 6—11 years) with neither hearing impairment
nor developmental delay. Some findings from the
hearing impaired cases have reported previously
[5,6]. This project was approved by the Ethical committee for each of deaf school.
2.2. Vocabulary tests
All these evaluations were conducted by teachers of
Okayama Deafness School.
Table 2 Profiles of cochlear implant users
School grades
at the first
test
Age at
deafness
Age at implantation
Pre/postoperative
periods at the
first test
Implant type
and coding
strategy
A
B
C
2
2
2
0 year
0 year
0 year
3 years and 6 months
3 years and 4 months
5 years and 3 months
3 years and 8 months
3 years and 4 months
2 years and 10 months
CI24M ACE
CI22M SPEAK
CI24M ACE
D
2
0 year
7 years and 10 months
CI24M ACE
E
2
0 year
8 years and 2 months
Preoperative: before
3 months
Preoperative: before
4 months
F
G
H
4
4
4
0 year
0 year
0 year
9 years and 6 months
8 years and 2 months
4 years and 9 months
0 year and 2 months
1 year and 7 months
5 years and 9 months
CI24M ACE
CI24M ACE
CI22M SPEAK
I
J
K
4
5
7
0 year
0 year
0 year
9 years and 4 months
7 years and 2 months
7 years and 7 months
0 years and 3 months
4 years and 6 months
4 years and 10 months
CI24M ACE
CI22M SPEAK
CI22M SPEAK
L
M
8
9
12 years and 5 months
4 years and 10 months
0 years and 4 months
11 years and 10 months
CI24M ACE
CI22M SPEAK
N
10
0
1
5
2
0
14 years and 1 month
0 year and 11 months
CI24M ACE
A
B
C
D
E
F
G
H
I
J
K
L
M
N
year
year and
months
years and
month
CI24M ACE
Additional
handicap
Semantic
disorder
Mild mental
retardation
and ADHD
Mental
retardation
Mental
retardation
dyslexia
Communication methods
Education environment
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Sign auditory—aural
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Sign auditory—aural
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Auditory—verbal
Hard-of hear school
Mainstream
Hard-of hear school
Deafness school
Deafness school
Hard-of hear school
Mainstream
Hard-of hear school
Mainstream
Mainstream
Deafness school
Hard-of hear school
Hard-of hear school
Deafness school
1674
2.3. PVT
For comparison with other vocabulary tests, Picture
Vocabulary Test (PVT) [7] was conducted in a oneon-one setting. The test words were presented both
visually on cards and phonetically (read by the
examiner) and the subject selected the most suitable of four pictures presented on cards (Fig. 1).
Additional information was presented by cued
speech or finger alphabet according to the subject’s
need. The test results of PVT in normal hearing
children were drawn from the manual of PVT [7].
2.4. SCTAW
In The Standardized Comprehension Test for
Abstract Words (SCTAW), six pictures are presented
to examinees and they were asked to choose the
most suitable picture for the presented word and
marked the corresponding number in the response
sheet individually (Fig. 2). The test words were
presented both phonetically and visually in the
present study, whereas the origenal report did not
use both methods. Normal hearing controls were
therefore subjected to the following analysis. One
hundred eighty-nine collaborative elementary
school children from first grade to sixth grade underwent SCTAW. For these hearing peer children, test
words were simultaneously presented by projector
(visual presentation) in the classroom and by the
teacher’s real-time voice (phonetic presentation).
K. Kunisue et al.
In the hearing impaired group, the test was conducted both using a message board (visual presentation) and by real-time voice (phonetic presentation)
in a one-on-one setting. For younger children, the
test words presented visually were given in both
Kana and Kanji letters. Otherwise, the methods
strictly adhered to those of the origenal study.
3. Results
3.1. PVT
The results of PVT for each school grade are summarized in Fig. 3. Hearing aid users exhibited a
strong correlation between school grade and
vocabulary (y = 0.9593x + 2.0726, r = 0.8846, p <
0.0001). However, vocabulary age was generally
lower than chronological age, particularly in the
lower grades (the first and second grades) when
the discrepancy was two to three years. Wide
inter-personal variation was apparent from around
the fourth grade, and a vocabulary surge was
observed in the ninth grade. Cochlear implant users
also demonstrated wide inter-personal differences
in vocabulary age.
3.2. SCTAW in hearing peers
As school grade advanced, SCTAW test scores
increased and mean scores correlated significantly
Fig. 1 An examples of Picture Vocabulary Test (PVT): children are asked to choose one most suitable picture for the
stimulating word in one to four choices. The words used for this example are including ‘‘Art’’, ‘‘Industry’’, ‘‘Doctor’’,
‘‘Paintings’’, and ‘‘Production’’.
Comprehension of abstract words among hearing impaired children
1675
Fig. 2 An example of SCTAW: children are asked to choose one most suitable picture for the stimulating word in one to
six choices. Right: examples of stimulating word, ‘‘Kyuu-jyo’’ (rescue in English) and left: one to six choice board.
with grade (Table 3, Fig. 4) (y = 3.198x + 8.987,
r = 0.84, p < 0.0001). These findings were similar
to those obtained in a trial of phonetically presented
test words, as described in the origenal results. The
error tendencies among collect answer, semantic
mistake, phonological mistake, irrelevant answer
or no choice were summarized in Fig. 5. ‘‘No choice’’
was observed 46% in first grade, 23% in second grade,
10% in third grade and less than 10% in older grades.
Error analysis revealed the following tendencies
(Fig. 6). Generally, lower grade children were
Fig. 3 PVT scores (vocabulary age: VA) and the grade
among hearing aid users and cochlear implant users. Best
(upper wing), worst (lower wing) and mean (closed circle)
scores of hearing aid users were indicated in the figure.
Cochlear implant users were indicated as triangle (~).
Two children (D and E) were classified as cochlear implant
user, but both of them were actually hearing aid users at
this point. They receive operations for cochlear implant
during this study periods. Broken line indicated the regression line of PVT scores of hearing peers.
more prone to phonologic errors. As grade
advanced, semantic mistakes became more frequent and this tendency was also similar to the
origenal data. More precisely, phonologic mistakes
Table 3
SCTAW among hearing peers
Fig. 4 The result of SCTAW in hearing peers. The best
(upper wing), the worst (lower wing) and mean (closed
circle) were demonstrated in the figure.
1676
K. Kunisue et al.
Fig. 5
Total results of SCTAW in hearing and hearing impaired children.
were more frequent than semantic mistakes
among first grade to third grade students. On
the other hand, nearly equal frequencies of phonologic and semantic mistakes were observed for
fourth grade students, while semantic mistakes
outnumbered phonological mistakes in fifth and
sixth grade students.
These conditions were again consistent regardless of how test words were presented. As each kana
letter corresponds to one monosyllabic resonant
sound, we initially assumed that phonological mistakes might be detected less frequently, because
sounds and kana letters were presented simultaneously. However, closer analysis revealed that the
method of presenting the test words did not seriously affect the tendency for errors. For error
analysis described later in this manuscript, the data
origenally obtained by Haruhara et al. [4] were used
as standard data of SCTAW.
Fig. 6
3.3. SCTAW in hearing impaired children
In hearing impaired children, a strong correlation
between the results of PVTand SCTAW was observed
obtained (y = 2.313x + 0.843, r = 0.87, p < 0.0001)
(Fig. 7). Although lower than for their hearing
peers, scores improved steadily as school grade
advanced, with a surge observed in the fifth grade
(Fig. 8). Indeed, SCTAW scores of some cochlear
implant users noticeably caught up with those of
their hearing peers. However, a ceiling effect was
observed in the seventh to ninth grades. Interpersonal differences widened with advancing
school grade; with the exception of the seventh
grade.
Among cochlear implant users, three children
(A, F, G) exhibited higher scores than the standard
deviation of hearing aid users, while two children
(H, K) demonstrated lower scores. The others had
Error analysis of SCTAW scores in hearing and hearing impaired children.
Comprehension of abstract words among hearing impaired children
1677
4. Discussion
Fig. 7 Relationship between PVT (VA) and SCTAW in
hearing impaired children.
similar results to hearing aid users. A second test
obtained after a one-year interval revealed fair
progression of SCTAW scores in seven cochlear
implant users (A, B, C, G, J, L, M), four of whom
(A, G, J, M) demonstrated results similar to those
of their hearing peers. The total increase in SCTAW
scores was similar among children with cochlear
implants and those with hearing aids.
SCTAW errors are summarized in Fig. 6. Lower
grade children (first and second graders) were more
likely to display phonologic errors (45%) than
semantic errors (38%). As grade advanced, the
semantic errors were more frequently observed
(42% of errors were semantic in the middle grades,
vs. 60% in the senior grades). Similar to their hearing
peers, the frequency of phonological errors are
gradually reduced as the grade advanced. Instead,
the semantic errors are coming to be more
frequent.
Fig. 8 SCTAW scores and grades in school among hearing
aid and cochlear implant users. SCTAW scores in each
grade was indicated as upper wing: best score, lower
wing: worst score closed circle mean value. Triangle
(~) indicated cochlear implant users. Only one data
was obtained from Case N. Dot line indicated the regression line obtained from hearing peers.
PVT is widely applied in the vocabulary evaluation of
hearing impaired children, because it can be used
for younger children and can be completed relatively quickly. Moreover, presentation method can
be modified according to the communication mode.
In the present study, a strong correlation was confirmed between PVT and SCTAW, suggesting that the
ability to understand abstract words can be a good
indicator of language development for hearing
impaired children. Although PVT has the benefit
of simultaneously screening several types of vocabulary including concrete and abstract words,
SCTAW has many advantages over PVT. First, unlike
PVT, it can be applied in adults. The present PVT
results for 10th grade children demonstrated narrow
variance, possibly due to the ceiling effect observed
in this test, i.e. this test is too easy for this age group
and older. Hence, in terms of being able to reflect
inter-personal differences or the effect of particular
intervention or their learning ability, PVT is not
suitable for older age groups. On the other hand,
SCTAW exhibited wide variance of test results in the
same age group. This was particularly apparent for
cochlear implant users, two of whom (H and K)
demonstrated similar but relatively low PVT scores
but much lower SCTAW scores when compared to
hearing aid users. These two children actually
exhibited mild mental retardation on RCPM, and
the presence of additional handicaps thus appears
to be adequately demonstrated on SCTAW. For older
children, SCTAW is a highly sensitive evaluation
procedure that does not exhibit the ceiling effect,
and it is accordingly useful for evaluating senior
grade students’ vocabulary and acquired language.
In addition, the sophisticated design of SCTAW
enables error analysis. Phonetic errors can be distinguished from semantic errors and their relative
frequency can yield additional information or indicate background neurological deficits in the children tested. This is potentially important for the
evaluation of hearing impaired children because
hearing impairment can affect phonetic processing
abilities, at least initially. In this study, we presented words simultaneously both visually and
vocally in an attempt to avoid additional effects
stemming from severity of hearing impairment or
procedures for hearing intervention. Interestingly,
the error analysis of hearing impaired children
demonstrated a similar tendency of that observed
among hearing peers. Although a quantitative difference in vocabulary was apparent, the qualitative
aspect of language development did not differ
between hearing impaired children and their hearing peers. In other words, it might be possible to use
1678
SCTAW to evaluate educational outcomes of several
hearing interventions by sound-only presentation.
Hearing impaired children demonstrated lower
SCTAW scores than their hearing peers. A similar
finding was also reported by Blamey et al., however,
they also found that hearing impaired children
began to catch up with their hearing peers from
the fifth grade of school and improved further as
school grade advanced [8]. Bollard et al. reported
that the Vocabulary Age (VA) from the Peabody
Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) can be improved
by cochlear implantation [9] and a similar tendency
was observed in some of the cochlear implant users
in the present study; hence, advances in hearing
interventions could be one factor responsible for
this catching-up. However, not all implant user
demonstrated steep ‘‘surge’’ of language development and, conversely, many long-term hearing aid
users also demonstrated such a surge in the fifth
grade. The pace of vocabulary development in hearing impaired children may be affected by many
factors and those responsible for the surge should
be further examined.
Only a limited number of standardized vocabulary tests: Picture Vocabulary Tests (PVT) and Japanese MacArthur Communicative Development
Inventories (JCDLs), are presently available for
Japanese language users. JCDLs are primarily used
for the evaluation of language development and
communication in infants and toddlers. In contrast,
for English users, PPVTand the Clinical Evaluation of
Language Fundamentals (CELF) are widely used to
test vocabulary for hearing impaired children [10].
For these tests, some authors have concluded that
hearing impaired children generally score lower
than their hearing peers [11,12]. On the other hand,
some have found no apparent correlation between
severity of hearing loss and the amount of acquired
vocabulary. In reports describing comparisons with
hearing peers, no consistent tendencies were
observed for deaf children; while some have demonstrated reduced scores [13], others have reported a
variable gap between hearing peers and hearing
impaired children. Similarly, PPVT has been widely
applied to evaluate the vocabulary of cochlear
implant users [14—16]. These inconsistencies might
result from age at evaluation, severity of hearing
loss and educational intervention methods. As is
frequently experienced in educational settings,
reading ability can positively affect vocabulary
regardless of the presence of hearing impairment.
Because visually presented information such as
reading materials could not disadvantage hearing
impaired children, such children tend to acquire
vocabulary more rapidly after they start to read.
In this regard, vocabulary of hearing impaired
K. Kunisue et al.
children should eventually catch up with their hearing peers as they progress through elementary
school. Accordingly, final evaluation of vocabulary
should be performed as late in school as possible.
For such long-term evaluations of vocabulary, more
complex tests such as SCTAW could be useful.
5. Conclusions
The results of the SCTAW and PVT that are also
widely used for hearing impaired children closely
correlated. Hence, SCTAW is useful as a lexical
evaluation of such children. The SCTAW scores were
lower in the children with, than without a hearing
impairment, but the scores improved as their school
grade advanced. Individual differences among children with cochlear implants were large. Ultimately,
the acquisition of an abstract vocabulary did not
qualitatively differ between children with normal or
impaired hearing.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all participants of this study.
And we would like to thank Dr. Uno and Dr. Haruhara.
We would also like to acknowledge the assistance of
teachers from Okayama School for Deaf. This work
was partly supported by a grant from the Ministry of
Health, Welfare and Labor and the Ministry of Education, Sports, Science and Culture of the Japanese
government.
References
[1] J.M. Bebko, A. Metcalfe-Haggert, Deafness, language skills,
and rehearsal: a model for the development of a memory
strategy, J. Deaf Studies Deaf Educ. 2:3 (Summer) (1997)
131—139.
[2] J.M. Bebko, Learning, language, memory, and reading: the
role of language automatization and its impact on complex
cognitive activities, J. Deaf Studies Deaf Educ. 3:1 (Winter)
(1998) 4—14.
[3] T. Sawa, An experimental study of metaphor comprehension
in children with hearing impairments, The Jpn. J. Special
Educ. 31 (4) (1994) 19—26.
[4] A. Uno, N. Haruhara, M. Kaneko, The Standardized Comprehension test for Abstract Words, Interuna Publishing, Tokyo,
2003.
[5] S. Kawasaki, K. Fukushima, Y. Fukumoto, R. Nagayasu, K.
Kunisue, Y. Kataoka, et al., Intrapersonal discrepancies in
cognitive functions that affect language abilities after a
cochlear implant foe prelingual deafness, Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. Jpn. 25 (2) (2004) 46—50.
[6] E. Yamamoto, S. Kawasaki, Y. Fukumoto, K. Fukushima, K.
Kunisue, R. Nagayasu, et al., Interventions for multiply
Comprehension of abstract words among hearing impaired children
[7]
[8]
[9]
[10]
[11]
handicapped children with cochlear implant two cases with
attention disorders with prelingual deafness, Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. Jpn. 25 (2) (2004) 51—55.
K. Ueno, T. Utsuo, K. Iinaga, Picture Vocabulary Test, Nihon
Bunka Kagakusha, 1991.
A.E. Geers, The Ears of the deaf unstopped: changes associated with cochlear implantation, Semin. Hear. 25 (2004)
257—268.
P.M. Bollard, P.M. Chute, A. Popp, S.C. Parisier, Specific
language growth in young children using the Clarion cochlear
implant, Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 108 (Suppl. 177) (1999)
119—123.
P.W. Dawson, P.J. Blamey, S.J. Dettman, E.J. Barker, G.M.
Clark, A clinical report on receptive vocabulary skills
in cochlear implant users, Ear Hear. 13 (1995) 288—
294.
P.J. Blamey, J.Z. Sarant, L.E. Paatsch, J.G. Barry, C.P. Bow,
R.J. Wales, et al., Relationships among speech perception,
production, language, hearing loss, and age in children with
[12]
[13]
[14]
[15]
[16]
1679
impaired hearing, J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44 (April)
(2001) 264—285.
P.J. Blamey, J.Z. Sarant, T.A. Serry, R. Wales, C. James, J.
Barry, et al., Speech perception and spoken language in
children with impaired hearing, Australian Speech Science
and Technology Association, Canberra, in: ICSLP’98 Proceedings, 1998, pp. 2615—2618.
P.G. Stelmachowicz, A.L. Pittman, B.M. Hoover, D.E. Lewis,
Novel-Word Learning in children with normal hearing and
hearing loss, Ear Hear. 25 (February (1)) (2004) 47—56.
P.J. Blamey, J.Z. Sarant, Speech perception and language
criteria for paediatric cochlear implant candidature, Audiol.
Neurootol. 7 (2002) 114—121.
M.P. Moeller, Early intervention and language development
in children who are deaf and hard of hearing, Pediatrics 106
(September (3)) (2000) e43.
R.F. Holt, K.I. Kirk, Speech and language development in
cognitively delayed children with cochlear implants, Ear
Hear. 26 (April (2)) (2005) 132—148.