The Concept of Social Cohesion
The Concept of Social Cohesion
The Concept of Social Cohesion
Introduction
Cohesiveness has been a topic of long-term interest in sociology and psychology as well as in mental health and more recently in public health.
While the concept of social cohesion is intriguing, it has also been frustrating because its multiple definitions prevent its meaningful measurement and
application. Investigators have conceptualized social cohesion, and developed methods for studying it, based on the theoretical assumptions of their
own discipline. In sociology, social structure provides the framework for
studying the behavior of social groups and organizations.1 In social psychology, cohesiveness is considered an attribute along with other processes
operating within and between small groups.2 In psychology, cohesiveness
relates to the members of a group who share emotional and behavioral characteristics with one another and with the group as a whole.3 In mental health,
the small group is viewed as a dynamic system in which the differentiation
of roles during phases of group development is dependent upon a cohesive
group bond.4 And, in public health, cohesiveness is viewed as part of the
social and environmental context of individuals and societies that influence
health risks and protective factors.5 Disciplinary boundaries have protected
the definitions of social cohesion and made it difficult to investigate multidisciplinary, multilevel aspects of the concept.6
31
32
Table 2.1 Historical overview of conceptions of social cohesion in sociology and social
psychology
Theorist/investigator
Le Bon (1896)
Durkheim (1897)
Cooley (1909)
Freud (1921)
MacDougall (1921)
Moreno (1934)
Lewin (1943)
Deutsch (1949)
Homans (1950/1961)
Back (1951)
Schachter (1951)
33
French (1956)
Miligram (1965)
Janis (1972)
Granovetter (1973)
34
References for investigators are listed in the References section at end of book.
35
36
theory that is based on the proposition that human behavior is the function
of both the person and the environment. This means that ones behavior is
related to both ones personal characteristics and to the social situation in
which one finds oneself. Lewin found that experiential learning is best facilitated when there is a conflict between immediate concrete experience and
detached analysis within the individual. A cycle of action, reflection, generalization, and testing is characteristic of experiential learning.
The most fundamental construct of Lewins is that of the psychological
field or life space. All psychological events are a function of life space,
which consists of the independence of the person and the environment. He
saw the individual as an equilibrium-maintaining system. He viewed the
group as a dynamic whole the interdependence of its members in which
a change in any subpart changes the state of other subparts. The degree of
a groups interdependence depends on the groups size, organization, and
intimacy.
The early experiments of Ronald Lippitt and Robert White and others
such as J. R. P. French and Leon Festinger, were instrumental in initiating
experimental investigations of group life in social psychology and sociology.
Lewin, Lippitt, and Whites study of the effects of different types of leadership behavior demonstrated how crucial the position of leadership is in
determining the atmosphere of a group. Lippitt also studied behavioral contagion in groups, specifically the relationship between status and the ability
to influence others in the group. These studies provided the rationale for the
use of communication as a key instrument for characterizing group structure and for locating the occupants of various positions within this structure. Lippitt and White also studied the influence of process in organizations.
They believed that behavior is primarily influenced by authority, that is, the
control over reward and punishment and by persuasion, or by a combination
of these. The way in which these modes of influence are used by superiors
determines their style of leadership.
Morton Deutsch, a student of Lewins, is considered the founder of the
theory and intervention in conflict resolution. He found that a group may be
defined as a set of members who mutually perceive themselves to be cooperatively or promotively interdependent in varying respects and degrees. He
stated that it was clear that cohesiveness refers to the forces that bind the
parts of a group together and which resist disruptive influences. He believed
that the study of the conditions affecting social cohesiveness and of the
effects the variations in social cohesiveness have on group functioning was
at the basis for understanding group life. Deutsch found that group members
who were rewarded on a cooperative basis were more cohesive than members rewarded on a competitive basis. He proposed that members of cohesive
groups were (1) more ready to accept the actions of other group members as
37
substitutable for intended actions of their own, (2) more ready to be influenced by other group members, and (3) more likely to positively respond to
the actions of other group members. Deutsch also found that the motivation
of members to continue working with the group, feeling an obligation to the
group, and the evaluation of the groups performance were affected more by
the groups dynamics than by its goal attainment.
George Homans was the founder of social exchange theory. This perspective explains social change and stability as a process of negotiated exchanges
between parties. For example, when a person perceives the costs of a relationship as outweighing the perceived benefits, the theory predicted that the
person will choose to leave the relationship. When the costs and benefits
are equal in a relationship, then that relationship is considered as equitable.
Cohesiveness refers to the value of rewards in a group. The more valuable
the rewards, the greater the groups cohesiveness.
Leon Festinger, Stanley Schachter, and Kurt Back defined cohesiveness
as the total field of forces which act on members to remain in the group.
The nature and strength of forces acting on a member to remain in the
group may vary from member to member. There may be many different
forces acting upon an individual as well as those they initiate. However,
Back found that in more cohesive groups, members made more efforts to
reach agreement and were more influenced by discussion than in less cohesive groups, no matter what the basis of attractiveness was for joining the
group. People in groups composed of members attracted to the group by
a liking for other group members were more chatty, but where cohesiveness was based on the prestige of the group, members were more cautious
and less relational with one another, and where cohesiveness was based
on the group as a means to a goal, members were more impersonal and
task-oriented.
A number of experimental investigations bear on the factors determining
group cohesiveness. Back found that he could produce high cohesiveness by
stressing to members how much they would like each other, how important
it was for the group to do well on the task since the task was a test of ability,
or how prestigious the group was. Schachter produced clubs with high cohesiveness by grouping students who expressed moderate or high interest in
their activities; he created clubs with low cohesiveness by grouping students
who expressed little or no interest in their activities.
Festingers theory of social comparison had significant implications for
group formation and group structure. He found that the drive for selfevaluation can lead people to associate with one another and to join groups.
His theory suggests that the selective tendencies to associate with others of
similar opinion and ability guarantee relative homogeneity of opinions and
abilities within groups. The theory of social comparison was extended by
38
39
reference and the individual person. A person who has neither the ability
nor expertise to make decisions, especially in a crisis, will leave decisionmaking to the group and its hierarchy. The group is the persons behavioral model. Miligram set up an experiment to test how much pain a person
would inflict on another person simply because he/she was told to do so by
an experimenter. He found that people would go to almost any length to
obey a command by an authoritatives figure. His work pointed out that people will carry out orders which have destructive effects and are incompatible with fundamental standards of morality when they have few resources
to resist authority. Miligram repeated his experiments throughout the world
with similar results.
Albert Lott and Bernice Lott were interested in the relationship between
group cohesiveness and individual learning. They predicted that children
would learn better if they studied with children they liked than if they studied with children they liked less. They presumed that the degree of member liking was an indicator of group cohesiveness. They found that high
IQ children who were in high cohesive groups performed better on learning tests than high IQ children who were in low cohesive groups. For low
IQ children, however, cohesiveness, or the degree of interpersonal attraction
among group members, made no difference, although there was a tendency
for low IQ children to do better in high cohesive groups. The investigators
believed that children who worked with other children they liked would be
more likely to have a greater drive to learn than children who were neutral or had negative attitudes toward their fellow group members. However,
cohesiveness made little difference in learning among high IQ children.
Social psychologists Muzafer Sherif and Carolyn Sherif studied the origin
of conflict in social groups in a classic study called the Robbers Cave experiment, a Boy Scout Camp surrounded by Robbers Cave State Park in Oklahoma. During the study, M. Sherif posed as an observer in the role of camp
janitor. Twenty-two 11-year-old boys who did not know each other were
assigned to two groups of 11 each. They chose names for their groups and
developed internal social hierarchies. Contact between the two groups in the
form of sports competitions elicited hostility between the groups. To lessen
friction and promote cooperation Sherif devised tasks, or superordinate
goals, that required the two groups to work together. Hostilities subsided
and the groups bonded to the extent that all the boys insisted that they ride
the same bus home. The experiment provides an example of how superordinate goals can transcend intergroup conflict and promote social cohesion.
Irving Janis is known for the formulation of groupthink. Groupthink
occurs when a group makes faulty decisions because group pressures lead
to a deterioration of mental efficiency, reality testing, and moral judgment.
Groups affected by groupthink ignore alternatives and tend to take irrational
40
41
42
43
44
Instrument/method
Objective of instrument
Gross Cohesiveness
Scale
Group Atmosphere
Scale
Mackenzie (1981)
Group Climate
Questionnaire
Piper et al. (1983) Group-MemberLeader Cohesion
Scale
Carron et al. (1985) Group Environment
Questionnaire
Evans & Jarvis
(1986)
Budman et al.
(1987)
References for investigators are listed in the Reference section at the end of the book.
the theoretical framework for understanding the social processes that create and sustain social cohesion. Furthermore, Scott Budge16 pointed out
the need to abandon current assumptions about cohesiveness that define
it as a static, positive, totality, in favor of a paradigm that views cohesion as a dynamic process through which cohesiveness develops. Similarly,
Kaplan17 and his colleagues have suggested that small groups are dynamic
equilibrium-seeking social systems that evolve gradually, through sequences
of developmental phases or stages. The fact that a group develops over time
also suggests that its adaptive capacities will allow it to become cohesive.18
This should indicate that an assessment of a groups degree or level of cohesiveness must be both situationally and developmentally sensitive.
45
46
given to the Divine Light members. In addition, they were given a series of
items reflecting their religious beliefs. Not surprisingly, a strong adherence
to group norms was found. The cohesion items (beliefs) were found to be
strong predictors of psychological well-being.
According to Galanter, these two studies lend support to the hypothesis
that there is an innate relationship within the individual between distress
and alienation on the one hand, and between psychological well-being and
affiliation on the other hand. Large groups play a major role in defining
the identity and social roles of their individual members. Members social
affiliations lie in large part with individuals who have joined the group.
Finally, large groups rely on their members and eschew the surrounding
culture.
Summary
47
Summary
Carron and Spink20 said, It could be argued that the terms cohesion and
group are tautological; if a group exists, it must be cohesive to some
degree. Thus it is probably no surprise that even in collectives where minimal group characteristics are present, manifestations of cohesion are evident (pp. 8687). There seems little doubt that group cohesion exists,
but disciplinary eyes see it differently and, in turn, researchers have different ways of measuring what they see. Therefore, there are only disciplinary pockets of agreement on the definition of cohesiveness. We seem
to define cohesiveness best by identifying consequences when it is absent
and are less clear about how cohesiveness is created, nourished, and
sustained.
As definitions of cohesiveness have evolved over time and become more
specific, the concept has become fragmented and specialized, which is
reflected in the diverse instruments used to measure it. Issues of the measurement of cohesiveness differ in small and in large groups. Because of
the complexities of assessing cohesiveness most attention has been given to
small group cohesion.
Despite repeated calls for consensus in the definition of cohesiveness in
the literature there appears little progress in this regard. There are some fresh
approaches to theorizing and studying cohesion using social network analysis. This approach is appealing because it stresses the patterns of social ties
and network connections that are conducive to different degrees of cohesiveness irrespective of group size.
48
Notes
1. P. E. Mudrack, 1989, Defining group cohesiveness: A legacy of confusion. Small Group
Research, 20(1): 3749; N. E. Friedkin, 2004, Social cohesion. Annual Review of Sociology,
30: 409425.
2. M. A. Hogg, 1992, The Social Psychology of Group Cohesiveness: From Attraction to Social
Identity, New York: New York University Press.
3. M. Deutsch, 1968, Field theory in social psychology, Chapter 6, in G. Lindzey & E. Aronson
(Eds.) The Handbook of Social Psychology, 2nd ed., Vol. 1, Menlo Park, CA: AddisonWesley.
4. H. Kellerman, (Ed.) 1981, Group Cohesion: Theoretical and Clinical Perspectives,
New York: Grune & Stratton.
5. A. V. Diez Roux, 2004, The study of group-level factors in epidemiology: Rethinking variables, study designs, and analytical approaches. Epidemiologic Reviews, 26: 104111.
6. R. E. Pahl, 1991, The search for social cohesion: From Durkheim to the European Commission. European Journal of Sociology, 32(2): 345360.
7. V. Nee & J. Sanders, 2001, Trust in ethnic ties: Social capital and immigrants, in K. S. Cook
(Ed.) Trust in Society, Vol. II in the Russell Sage Foundation Series on Trust, pp. 374392,
New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Also see R. Hardin, 2001, Conceptions and explanations
of trust, in K. S. Cook (Ed.) Trust in Society, Vol. II in the Russell Sage Foundation Series on
Trust, pp. 339, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
8. L. M. Libo, 1953, Measuring Group Cohesiveness, Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan
Research Center for Group Dynamics Series, Publication No. 3.
9. H. Kellerman, 1981, The deep structures of group cohesion, in H. Kellerman (Ed.) Group
Cohesion: Theoretical and Clinical Perspectives, pp. 321, New York: Grune & Stratton.
10. N. E. Friedkin, op. cit., p. 412.
11. N. Gross & W. E. Martin, 1952, On group cohesiveness. American Journal of Sociology, 57:
546564.
12. J. S. Bruner, 1950, Social Psychology and group processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 1:
119150.
13. A. A. Cota, K. L. Dion & C. R. Evans, 1993, A reexamination of the structure of the Gross
Cohesiveness Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53: 499506.
14. N. E. Friedkin, op. cit., p. 410.
15. N. E. Friedkin, op. cit., p. 422.
16. S. Budge, 1981, Group cohesiveness revisited. Group, 5: 1018.
17. S. R. Kaplan & A. M. Razin, 1981, Psychosocial dynamics of group cohesion, in
H. Kellerman (Ed.) Group Cohesion: Theoretical and Clinical Perspectives, pp. 227244,
New York: Grune & Stratton. Also, S. R. Kaplan, M. Roman, 1963, Phases of development
in an adult therapy group. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 13: 1026.
18. H. Kellerman, 1979, Group Psychotherapy and Personality: Intersecting Structures.
New York: Grune & Stratton.
19. M. Galanter, 1981, Cohesiveness in the Large Group: A psychobiological perspective, in
H. Kellerman (Ed.) Group Cohesion: Theoretical and Clinical Perspectives, pp. 413427,
New York: Grune & Stratton.
20. A. V. Carron & K. S. Spink, 1995, The group size-cohesion relationship in minimal groups.
Small Group Research, 26(1): 86105.
21. J. Moody & D. R. White, 2003, Structural cohesion and embeddedness: A hierarchical concept of social groups. American Sociological Review, 68(1): 103127.
22. M. E. J. Newman, D. J. Watts & S. H. Strogatz, 2002, Random graph models of social
networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(February 19, Suppl.):
25662572.
http://www.springer.com/978-1-4419-0363-1