Invited Article Caloric Restriction For Longevity: I. Paradigm, Protocols and Physiological Findings in Animal Research
Invited Article Caloric Restriction For Longevity: I. Paradigm, Protocols and Physiological Findings in Animal Research
Invited Article Caloric Restriction For Longevity: I. Paradigm, Protocols and Physiological Findings in Animal Research
Invited Article
Caloric Restriction for Longevity: I.
Paradigm, Protocols and
Physiological Findings in
Animal Research
Kelly M. Vitousek*,y , Jennifer A. Gray and Kathleen
M. Grubbs
University of Hawaii, USA
The initial article in this series reviews basic findings in the field of
caloric restriction for longevity (CRL). To eating disorder specialists, the data are disconcerting. The chronic dieting and
subnormal weight we endeavour to prevent and treat in humans
appear highly beneficial when imposed on animals. In the
laboratory, organisms from nematodes to monkeys thrive when
forced to undereat, as long as they receive sufficient micronutrients. The most remarkable results are obtained through the most
extreme measures: mice, for example, do best if limited to a third of
expected caloric intake, beginning soon after weaning and
continuing throughout adulthood. Deprivation can be achieved
through an anorexic protocol of steady underconsumption or a
bulimic pattern in which periods of fasting alternate with bouts
of binge eating. The benefits of such regimens include delayed
senescence, postponement and/or attenuation of age-related disease and dramatic increases in average and maximum lifespan.
Although some biological functions are impaired (including
growth, reproduction and perhaps resistance to certain stressors),
the cost/benefit ratio clearly favours CRL when calculated on the
basis of physical outcomes in late age. Advocacy of comparable
regimens for people, however, is ill-considered. Enthusiasm for
CRL can be sustained only by detaching deprivation from the
context of daily life, ignoring psychological effects, and dismissing
data on human semi-starvation and eating disorders. The experiences of participants in Biosphere 2 and individuals with anorexia
nervosa suggest that the price of CRL is unacceptably high when a
wider range of outcome variables is examined. Copyright # 2004
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
Keywords: dietary restriction; ageing; eating disorders; semi-starvation; Biosphere 2
* Correspondence to: Kelly M. Vitousek, Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii, 2430 Campus Road, Honolulu,
Hawaii, USA. Tel: 808 956 6269. Fax: 808 956 4700.
E-mail: vitousek@hawaii.edu
y
The former name of the first author is Kelly M. Bemis.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/erv.594
K. M. Vitousek et al.
280
INTRODUCTION
For more than 60 years, compelling data on the costs
and the benefits of caloric restriction (CR) have accumulated in two separate literatures, generated by
specialists in the eating disorder (ED) field and the
study of caloric restriction for longevity (CRL).1 To
date, there have been few cross-references between
them and remarkably little recognition, by either
side, of the challenges or the opportunities that each
affords the other. Posing different questions, using
different methods, studying different populations,
the ED and CRL fields have reached sharply disparate conclusions about phenomena of clear common
interest. Continued mutual ignorance between these
specialty areas is untenable on scientific, clinical and
ethical grounds. As outlined in the introduction to
the present series (Vitousek, this issue), this article
is the first of three written to summarize the CRL literature for readers already knowledgeable about
human semi-starvation and disordered eating.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
281
through centuries of vain human search for the
elixir of youth. It is plausible to imagine only two
circumstances under which more than the odd, isolated (and probably non-replicating) organism
would satisfy all criteria: it becomes a laboratory
animal that is caged, sheltered, and served up an
enriched low-calorie diet by a CRL researcher
or it happens to be a human being in a stable, safe
society who harbours a powerful and persistent
motive to eat much less than he or she desires
and the knowledge and resources to construct his
or her frugal menus wisely. In the latter category,
two groups of candidates come to mind: the subset
of individuals with anorexia nervosa (AN) who
carefully manage both the quantity and the quality
of their restrictive diets, and a tiny new cohort of
recruits who undertake CR for the specific purpose
of longevity (Manke & Vitousek, 2002). In recent
years, a handful of peopleincluding some of
the researchers who study CRLhave been
attempting to self-impose the regimen, inspired
by the glowing health of thousands of caged-andsheltered animals who lived out their extra-long
lives on half-portions of super-chow in the service
of science.
Elsewhere, we outline the case for why the CRL
field should be keenly interested in AN and why
the ED area should be equally eager to profit from
the few individuals who have begun to adopt an
essentially AN regimen for apparently non-AN
reasons (Vitousek, this issue; Vitousek, Gray, &
Talesfore, European Eating Disorders Review, in press).
At the same time, both fields of study would also be
enhanced through collaboration in the design and
interpretation of relevant animal research. ED specialists should begin by familiarizing themselves with
existing evidence on the CRL paradigm.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
282
proportion of fat, protein and carbohydrate yield at
best minor effects; the key factor is overall
reduction in energy intake (Masoro, 1988, 1995;
Merry, 1995). Some research suggests that animals
respond more favourably when caloric cutbacks
are phased in gradually over weeks or months
(Walford, 1983).
In much of the rodent research, animals are
restricted to 5060% of the calories they would eat
if an unlimited supply were available. The benefits
of CR are inversely proportional to the amount consumed. Samples of female mice held to intakes
representing 35%, 45% and 75% of AL levels showed
maximum lifespans that exceeded the peak survival
age of AL controls by 54%, 47% and 19% respectively
(Weindruch, Walford, Fligiel, & Guthrie, 1986).
Obviously, it is possible to undercut too far. For
mice, the lower limit is somewhere around 30% of
expected intake, with those supplied with less than
that amount dying younger than free-feeding animals. On the mild end of the CR spectrum, modest
but discernible benefits are evident for rodents
allowed 8085% of AL consumption.
Unsurprisingly, animals on CR generally end up
much lighter and leaner than controls. Within
cohorts on equivalent levels of CR, however, bodyweight is positively correlated with longevity, so
that individuals who manage to remain (relatively)
hefty on severe CR outlast their thinner and less
metabolically thrifty peers (Bertrand, Lynd,
Masoro, & Yu, 1980; Masoro, Yu, & Lynd, 1980;
Weindruch et al., 1986). When intermittent schedules are used to achieve CR, most rodents weigh
in 3040% lighter than AL controls because they fail
to eat enough on free-feeding days to compensate
for fasting days. At least one strain of mouse, however, learns to gorge sufficiently when fed every
other day to sustain weight at near-normal levels
(Anson et al., 2003). These animals also attain
extended lifespans, suggesting that long periods
of caloric deprivation can trigger the mechanism(s)
of CRL even when overall intake and weight are not
significantly reduced.
The only other factors known to influence the magnitude of the CRL effect are the interrelated variables
of time of initiation and total duration. In rodents,
CR is almost equally effective if started immediately
after weaning or not until the end of the juvenile
growth phase (Yu, Masoro, & McMahan, 1985).
Although some advantages still accrue when restriction commences in middle age, postponement of CR
onset yields diminishing returns through the period
of adulthood. Some recent studies have found that
even older animals profit from underfeeding
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
283
damage from toxins and increase neuronal resistance to stroke and degenerative diseases such as
Alzheimers and Parkinsons (for reviews, see
Casadesus, Shukitt-Hale, & Joseph, 2002; Mattson,
Chan, & Duan, 2002; Mattson, Duan, & Guo, 2003;
Patel & Finch, 2002). Some of the rodent research
suggests that benefits observed on the molecular,
cellular and physiological levels are paralleled by
superior performance on learning tasks relative to
ageing AL controls. As discussed in the companion article, however, the results are not uniformly reassuring; moreover, there are as yet no
data on cognitive function from any of the primate
projects currently underway (Vitousek, Manke,
Gray, & Vitousek, European Eating Disorders
Review, in press).
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
284
but not directly attributable to their CR. For example, in one study, all seven deaths examined (which
represented 5.1% crude mortality over an 11-year
follow-up period) occurred in patients who were
both anorexic and bulimic (Herzog et al., 2000).
Six of the seven individuals had abused alcohol;
one died from acute alcohol intoxication and three
committed suicide. Suicides account for between
17 and 42% of the ED-related deaths in AN samples
(Emborg, 1999; Lowe et al., 2001; Nielsen et al.,
1998).2 Much of the mortality attributable to physical causes results from some combination of rapid
weight loss, intakes far below 50% AL and/or purging none of which is consistent with the principles of sound CRL. The more relevant reference
group is the subset of AN patients who sustain
their low weights through steady abstemiousness,
selecting diets that feature the same healthy food
choices (e.g. fruits, vegetables, bran, tofu, yogurt,
fish) favoured by CRL practitioners. ED specialists
will recognize that pattern as a variant of chronic
AN that is associated with minimal risk for medical
crises or premature death in the few anorexic individuals who can sustain it.
On the basis of the remarkable data on the benefits of long-term, correctly implemented CR,
should ED professionals reconsider their
position on the merits of restriction? Maybe we
should stop trying to convince anorexic patients
that they have got it terribly wrong and concede
that in fact they have got it more right than the
general public and most healthcare professionals
yet appreciate (or could ever manage to achieve,
once they did). Perhaps, rather than dispensing
well-intentioned advice about the value of normal
eating and weight, we should be soliciting our
patients expertise about how to subvert both.
Or, less facetiously and in complete accord with
the animal data on CRL perhaps we should
shift our treatment goal from eliminating dietary
restriction to refining dietary restriction.
2
CR cannot be absolved from making some contribution to
the AN suicide statistics, in view of the association between
food deprivation and depression in normal individuals
(Keys, Brozek, Henschel, Mickelsen, & Taylor, 1950); in
addition, many AN patients link their suicidal ideation to
despair over the constricted life required by CR and selfloathing for their inability to sustain it. It is probable,
however, that pre-existing psychopathology (quite likely
exacerbated by CR) explains most of the variance in deaths
by suicide. It is more awkward to contend that the negative
effects of bulimia should not count as direct costs of CR. Binge
eating is a lawful response to caloric insufficiency, and
purging a predictable reaction to its occurrence in people
committed to caloric control.
3
If our suggestion that AN patients might be encouraged to
replace haphazard CR with proper CRL seems far-fetched,
the following post to the CR Society listserve is instructive:
[CR practitioners should] reach out to specific groups who
might derive serious benefit from [CR] membership . . . [including individuals with] anorexia or bulemia
[sic]. [We] can take these people in, not telling them that
they are ill, but rather informing them that thinness is not
only OK, but desirable . . . They can be taught that both goals
[i.e. thinness and health] can be met together, and that they
will stay young and beautiful many extra years by [ensuring
adequate nutrition on a CRL regimen]. Instead of going to
therapy to deal with their mental aberration, they will attend
[CR support groups] to encourage their desire in a healthy
direction (Adzoe, 2002). Our own recommendation for the
fine-tuning of anorexic restriction is rhetorical; this correspondent seems disturbingly sincere.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
285
not interest researchers unless they impact the outcome variables that do: retardation of the ageing
process, resistance to disease and extension of the
lifespan. Most observers of human CR would be
disposed to classify phenomena such as cold intolerance, orthostatic hypotension, elevated stress hormones, decreased sex hormones, hunger, food
preoccupation, irritability and social withdrawal as
costs of caloric deprivation. None of these variables,
however, seems to interfere with the targeted outcomes of CRLindeed, one or two may actually
facilitate them. Accordingly, none of these consequences is consistently recognized as a liability in
the context of CRL research. Where others may view
the thin, cold, hungry, asexual, subfertile, moody
and occasionally dizzy organism with peculiar lab
values as unwell, CRL investigators point to their
criterion data: if such an animal can remain active,
disease-free and alive longer than its normally nourished peers, it is manifestly thriving.
With reference to research focused on the mechanisms of CRL, this indifference to extraneous costs is
not as callous as it first appears. Some investigators
take the position that the raw just eat less protocols
used with animals are not intended for direct translation to the human case. Radical restriction is simply a tool for elucidating mechanisms that might
be targeted in future through mimeticsdrugs
that exploit the underlying processes without
requiring self-starvation (e.g. Ingram, 2003; Lane,
Ingram, & Roth, 2002; Lane et al., 2002; Roth et al.,
2001; Spindler, Dhahbi, Mote, Kim, & Tsuchiya,
2003). Yet even toward this objective, it may be crucial to examine the full range of CR effects, since
desired and undesired elements in this well-coordinated multilevel network (Yu & Chung, 2001, p. 40)
could prove to be interdependent. Moreover, many
researchers do advocate the adoption of CR itself by
humanssometimes tentatively and provisionally
(e.g. Bucci, 1992; Roberts et al., 2001; Weindruch,
1996) and sometimes with an unsettling messianic
fervour (e.g. Mattson et al., 2002; Pinel, 2000;
Walford, 1983, 2000). When animal studies are construed as pilot projects for an intended application to
people, neglect of potential ill-effects is indefensible
on ethical grounds as well as scientific ones.
There are, of course, inevitable costs to caloric
deprivation. The reason for their inevitability is
tucked away in the verb of a sentence used earlier
to introduce the benefits of CR: when an organism
is not getting enough calories to eat, it automatically
reallocates resources, shifting priorities for energy
utilization. By definition, the underfed organism
has less fuel available for distribution than its
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
286
normally-eating fellows; therefore, if some functions
are geared up, others must be shifted down. As it
turns out, the processes that Nature privileges
during times of food shortage are just the sorts of
variables that CRL investigators select for close
examination, while the functions that Nature opts
to shortchange or to disrupt are precisely those that
fail to capture their interest. Most of the benefits of
the CRL paradigm are manifested on the molecular,
cellular and physiological levels; many of its costs
show up in the behavioural, soft cognitive, affective and social domains. Because CRL specialists
are generally animal physiologists by training and
inclination, it is unsurprising that they are more
struck by the wonders CR works in the systems they
favour than by the damage it does elsewhere.
The scanty but ominous data on behavioural outcomes will be the focus of the second paper in this
series (Vitousek, Manke, Gray, & Vitousek, European
Eating Disorders Review, in press). The remainder of
the present paper outlines the subset of physiological adaptations that carry direct costs for the foodrestricted animalat least under some conditions
and with reference to the achievement of particular
goals. A notable feature of the biological economies
of CR is that their impact is context-dependent. With
a few exceptions, the physical price paid for restriction must be calculated on a sliding scale according
to the organisms circumstances and objectives.
Reproductive Function
However long the underfed organism survives, it is
less likely to enjoy an active sex life along the way or
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
Cold Intolerance
Organisms on CR become frigid in a literal sense as
well. To avoid squandering energy on heat, basal
body temperature is turned down. Underfed animals, like individuals with AN, are likely to feel
287
chilled in environments that the fully fed find comfortable, and will be less able to withstand cold
stress. In one of the original CRL experiments in
the 1930s, two failures in the laboratory heating system killed 35 of the 73 rats on CR; all subjects in the
AL condition survived the cold snaps (McCay,
Maynard, Sperling, & Barnes, 1939). In a small footnote to contemporary CRL research, it has been
reported that water-maze testing often proves fatal
to aged CR mice, presumably as a function of lower
body temperature and reduced thermal insulation
as well as decreased buoyancy (Means, Higgins, &
Fernandez, 1993). Although few humans on CR
would be confronted with comparable sink-or-swim
situations, both anorexic patients and CRL practitioners describe cold intolerance as an unpleasant
side-effect of restriction.
On the positive side, rodents on CR cope better
than AL animals when lightly baked in radiant
ovens, and show a slower age-related decline in
the synthesis of heat shock proteins (Aly et al.,
1994a, 1994b; Heydari, Wu, Takahashi, Strong, &
Richardson, 1993). In fact, in a mirror-image mishap
decades after McCays CR subjects perished in their
unheated laboratory in New York, restricted rats
had the edge when the air conditioning failed at a
facility in Texas: 84% of AL rodents succumbed to
the heatwave, while 75% of those on CR pulled
through (Heydari et al., 1993).
Stress Resistance
Little is known about whether CR facilitates or compromises response to most other forms of stress. The
paucity of research in this area is surprisingnot
only to us as ED specialists, but to a prominent
CRL researcher as well. Weindruch (1996) has commented that: Oddly enough, stress resistance has
been little studied in rodents on low-calorie diets,
and so they have little to teach about this issue
(p. 52). The matter of resilience to stress is clearly relevant to proposed extensions of the CRL paradigm
outside laboratory settings. Except for specimens
with the bad luck to be assigned to a water-maze or
heat shock study, the only stresses encountered by
the average CR rodent are tedium, confinement, the
occasional blood draw and (presumably) hunger.
The world that humans inhabit is more taxing and
intrusive, periodically subjecting them to competing
task demands, sensory overload, sleep deprivation,
interpersonal pressures and exposure to a wide
range of pathogens. It would seem worthwhile to
evaluate whether facsimiles of these conditions have
differential impact on the health and behaviour of CR
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
288
and AL animals. The net effects of life lived alone in a
germ-free laboratory may differ from those that
come with immersion in a bustling, sometimes hostile milieu (which is also dense with food cues).
On the physiological level, the evidence suggests
that CR regimens are chronically stressful in their
own right, producing elevated mean corticosterone
levels (Armario, Montero, & Jolin, 1987; Han, Evans,
Shu, Lee, & Nelson, 2001; Leakey et al., 1994; Patel &
Finch, 2002). One complication of assessing stress
hormones in CR rodents is that the daily peak shifts
from the onset of darkness to the time just before
feeding and then plummets during meal consumption (Stewart, Meaney, Ailken, Jensen, & Kalant,
1988). Some investigators speculate that this jolt of
preprandial anxiety may actually contribute to the
life-prolonging effects of CR (Masoro & Austad,
1996), noting that mice subjected to regular electric
shocks or periods of cold exposure enjoy longer lives
too (although the verb seems incongruous under the
circumstances). Other investigators emphasize the
paradox inherent in the data on stress hormones
during CR (Mattson et al., 2002; Patel & Finch,
2002). Chronic elevation of glucocorticoids from
other causes is associated with a broad range of ill
effects, while CR-induced changes are either directly
beneficial or counteracted by other protective factors
activated by restriction.
Bone Density
One specific discrepancy that may surprise ED
experts is the omission of decreased bone density
from the list of CR liabilities. Osteopenia and osteoporosis are among the most predictable and disabling physical effects of AN (Grinspoon et al.,
1999; Katzman & Zipursky, 1994; Lennkh et al.,
1999; Pomeroy & Mitchell, 2002; Rigotti, Neer,
Skates, Herzog, & Nussbaum, 1991; Serpell & Treasure, 1997; Siemers, Chakmakjian, & Gench, 1996). In
CRL research, however, underfeeding has minimal
effect on rats bone mineral content (Sanderson
et al., 1997). Equivocal results have been reported
to date from the primate studies (Ramsey et al.,
2000; Roberts et al., 2001). Slight reductions in bone
mineral density and peak bone mass have appeared
in both male and female monkeys on CR, but may
simply reflect their lower weight status.
The mismatch between the animal and human
findings on bone density could be attributable in
part to differences in diet composition or the pattern
of weight loss. In AN, however, adequate calcium
intake may reduce bone degeneration but does
not appear to prevent it (Bachrach, Guido,
Katzman, Litt, & Marcus, 1990; Biller et al., 1989;
Castro, Lazaro, Pons, Halperin, & Toro, 2000). Moreover, decreased density has been recorded in nonanorexic women who lose as little as 10 pounds
(Prior, cited in Roberts et al., 2001). No data are yet
available on the small cohort of (mostly male) individuals who have adopted high-quality low-calorie
regimens for the purpose of longevity. According
to an informal scan of self-reported status posted
to a CRL interest group, early signs of osteoporosis
have been detected in a few cases (Johannes, 2002).
Clearly, such anecdotal accounts provide no useful
information in the absence of baseline data, control
groups and verification of dietary intake; it is reassuring only that freelance CRL practitioners seem
aware of the potential risk and that at least some
are monitoring this aspect of their health on CR.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
Individual Differences
Another variable that may differ across species and
strains is the optimal degree of reduction below AL
intake (Weindruch et al., 1995); even when desirable
CR levels are established on the group level, it is
likely to remain hazardous to generalize across individuals. Although rodents are described as brighteyed, sleek and glossy-coated on drastic 5065%
cutbacks, several of the rhesus monkeys in the
Wisconsin CRL project began to deteriorate on the
modest 30% restriction level prescribed, showing
hair loss and a clinically ill appearance (Ramsey
et al., 2000, p. 1135). Apparently, these primate participants had entered the anorexic zone. A full complement of micronutrients notwithstanding, caloric
deprivation was impairing rather than enhancing
their physical condition. The protocol was modified
to permit intake adjustments on the basis of body
composition measured by dual-energy X-ray
absorptiometry (DEXA), in order to allow a margin
of safety for each animal.
Individual differences in response to CR do not
receive comparable attention in rodent studies, for
which subjects can be purchased and randomly
assigned in large batches. If the occasional rat or
mouse languishes on the CR level to which it has
been allocated, it is not whisked off for DEXA scan-
289
ning of its body fat content and issued extra rations
based on its personal needs. Even if a few animals
die from underfeeding, the consequences are minimal (except, of course, for the animals themselves).
Data points from stray casualties of CR barely deflect
group results, since, as noted earlier, many of the
survivors go on to enjoy remarkably long lives
(Walford, 2000, p. 77). Primates are more precious,
in part because each has a proportionally greater
impact on averaged outcomes. Accordingly, when
some monkeys appeared to suffer from CR, investigators intervened, changing the CR itself rather than
waiting to see just how adversely the ailing animals
would be affected by adherence to the original
protocol.
Such concern for individual outcomes anticipates
the vigilance that would be required in the monitoring of human CRL. At least for some organisms, the
margin between flourishing on CR and foundering
on CR appears distressingly narrow. Unfortunately,
fine-tuning of optimal-but-safe restriction through
regular DEXA scanning is not a realistic possibility
for people outside the context of research. Moreover,
it is troubling that some CRL experts have not hesitated to recommend levels of human CR equivalent
to the 30% reduction that made a number of monkeys look clinically ill. None of these advocates
has highlighted the risk that moderate, correctly
implemented CRL might jeopardize rather than
improve the health of normal humans who adopt it.
Marked variability is already evident in the small
group of humans who are attempting to implement
the CRL regimen. Just like individuals desperate to
control their weight, people determined to extend
their longevity seem to differ in the ability to tolerate
CR. Some describe their hunger as manageable or
even enjoyable, and claim to be energized by restriction. Others find hunger oppressive and sometimes
intolerable, and report feeling fatigued, impaired
and unwell. In the on-line community of CRL practitioners, these subgroups are termed cruisers and
strugglers. The former are strikingly reminiscent
of patients with restricting AN; the latter seem to
share characteristics with those more likely to
develop bulimic behaviour in response to dieting
(Vitousek, Gray, & Talesfore, European Eating Disorders Review, in press). Because the phenomenon of
human CRL is relatively new and virtually unstudied, it is too soon to know whether individuals will
shift category membership from cruising to struggling (or from restricting to bingeing) as a function
of duration, matching the temporal pattern associated with AN (e.g. Eddy et al., 2002). As ED specialists routinely observe, even those patients who react
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
290
positively to extreme restraint in the early going
tend to find it increasingly difficult to sustain over
time.
Less commonly, individual CRL practitioners
report serious side-effectsagain of a type familiar
to ED experts but apparently not anticipated by professional advocates of CRL. For example, one man
who had practised CR for several years began to
experience cardiac irregularities at a BMI of 18, on
an ostensibly CRL-consistent regimen of 1500 kcal/
day (Best, 1998). Prudently, he decided to retreat to a
more moderate CR level and a higher weight. Now
more than 10 years into CR, however, he reports that
he still has difficulty restraining his incessant compulsion to overeat and currently follows a less rigorous regimen in hopes of keeping [the] psychological
strain [of CR] to a tolerable comfort level (Best, 2002,
p. 2).
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
291
classes of costs off the books. Although varying the
set of environmental conditions will not necessarily
lower the overall rating of CR, expanding the criteria
used to measure its success certainly will. Critical
thinking about the adaptive purpose of the CR syndrome supports that conclusion (as does the available evidence from both animals and humans). The
system of defensive responses to food shortage
evolved to serve survival, and furthers that objective
with remarkable vigour and efficiency. The system
was not designed to facilitate sociability, sexuality,
parenting, play or psychological well-being; in fact,
as discussed in the companion article, it appears to
do a comparably vigorous and efficient job of disrupting them (Vitousek, Manke, Gray, & Vitousek,
European Eating Disorders Review, in press). As soon
as CRL researchers concede the relevance of outcomes unrelated to physical health and survival,
the recognized price of restriction will soar.
Biosphere 2
The first example comes from the Biosphere 2 project
(Allen, 1991; Alling & Nelson, 1993; Walford, Harris,
& Gunion, 1992; Walford et al., 1995). This quirky little quasi-experiment is often cited as the most
encouraging evidence to date that the CRL effect
would hold for humans; more mysteriously, it is also
construed as proof that the regimen is reasonable
and feasible for people to adopt. To the extent that
any conclusions can be drawn from the Biosphere
story, we would extract a different take-home message (Vitousek, Manke, Stumpf, & Gray, in preparation). Rather than demonstrating the feasibility of
human CRL, the Biosphere data foreshadow its futility. If we were invested in promoting the regimen,
we would be less inclined to put this poster project
for CRL on display than to roll it up and hide it in
the back of a closet.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
292
The condensed version of the Biosphere story is
that between 1991 and 1993, eight scientists and
technicians spent 2 years on closely monitored CR
while enclosed in a glass-domed complex in the
Arizona desert. Unlike the conscientious objectors
recruited for the Minnesota Study (Keys et al.,
1950), the participants had not signed up to semistarve for science. They had enlisted as crew members for a highly publicized attempt at selfsustained living within a sealed environment. The
CR subplot came about through misadventure,
when crop yields proved insufficient to meet the
dietary requirements of both human and animal
residents. In lawful response to deprivation, the pigs
stopped making piglets, the chickens laid few eggs
and the goats produced less milk. Shortchanged by
all their intended food sources, the four men and
four women of Biosphere 2 rapidly lost weight.
Intakes averaged 18002200 kcal/day during the
first 6 months, easing to 20002400 kcal/day over
the remaining 18 months of enclosure (Silverstone
& Nelson, 1996); maximum weight loss approached
20% for males and 13% for females.
One of the crew members was Roy Walford,
already a prominent CRL researcher and long-term
CRL practitioner (Walford, 1983, 2000; Weindruch &
Walford, 1988). While an ordinary nutritionist
would have freaked out and insisted that food
be brought in (Walford, in Kahn, 1996, p. 48),
Walfords background prepared him to recognize
the opportunity within the ordeal. Here were eight
human subjects caught up in a CR protocol much
like those he routinely imposed on rodents back
home in his UCLA lab: confinement to a small space
on short rations of high quality and equal portion
size in the context of a shared, sheltered and virtually germ-free environment. In contrast to other
human samples, this committed little cohort was
unlikely to drop out of the study and unable to cheat
on their diet. Like people stranded on life rafts or ice
floes (e.g. Callahan, 1986; Greely, 1886), the enclosed
Biospherians could gratify their hunger only
through fantasy. Following the classic traditions of
the semi-starved, they did so with relishindulging in animated discussions of food, imagining
themselves in supermarkets and restaurants, compiling cookbooks, and making lists of all the delicacies they looked forward to eating in the future.
Some took to chewing on peanut shells, fennel stalks
and banana skins, and some to hoarding their
rations. There were also a few deplorable incidents
of banana-stealing, discouraged through the installation of locks on the community storeroom and
freezer (Alling & Nelson, 1993). For the duration of
the 2-year mission, however, the only real behavioural choices were to undereat or to abandon the
premise of self-sufficiency. With Walfords assurance that their health would be enhanced rather than
jeopardized through prolonged CR, all eight Biospherians stuck with semi-starvation.
As Walford predicted, many physiological indices
showed dramatic shifts in a desirable direction, paralleling short-term changes seen in lab animals on
CR. Blood pressure, cholesterol, fasting blood
glucose, insulin and white cell counts were all lowered from 18% to 42% (Verdery & Walford, 1998;
Walford, 2000; Walford et al., 1992, 1995); body temperature also decreased, but could not be measured
with precision because the available thermometers
were not calibrated for values below 96 F (Walford,
Mock, McCallum, & Laseter, 1999). The duration of
internment was too brief (by some 25100 years) to
ascertain whether crew members would age more
slowly or reach a lifespan of 130, but the immediate
results seemed to presage such possibilities.
Unfortunately, the moment team members were
released from the portion-controlled confines of
their complex, they got right down to the business
of undoing CRL. Although most had found the regimen arduous during their stint under glass,4 at least
some had planned to continue moderate restriction
after the project ended, in hopes of preserving their
positive lab values (Alling & Nelson, 1993; Silverstone, 1993). Yet despite 2 years of direct experience
with successful CR (and, presumably, 2 years of
earnest advocacy from their expert-in-residence),
the Biospherians food preoccupation predicted
their subsequent behaviour more accurately than
did their intent. Once back in the world within
which all other humans must implement the CR
regimen, seven of the eight promptly relapsed into
AL eatingand eight of the eight rapidly gained
4
Consistent with the positive spin placed on every aspect of
life on CR, Walford repeatedly maintains that the Biospherians were not overly hungry and did not feel undue
hunger while on their restricted regimen (Walford &
Walford, 1994, pp. 20, 24, 29). All the accounts we have
located from Walfords fellow team members contradict his
characterization of their experience. For example, Silverstone
(1993) indicated that hunger was an almost constant
companion, Nelson (in Erickson, 1993) that hunger was a
nearly constant, nagging presence, Alling and Nelson (1993)
that hunger [was] . . . always there to struggle against and
Leigh (Associated Press, 1996) that [it] made us all a little
cranky, always being hungry. The classic semi-starvation
phenomena that emerged in Biosphere 2including food
fantasizing, maximizing, hoarding, stealing, massing, substitution habits and mistrust of others over the distribution of
rationsare also inconsistent with the claim that participants
were not overly hungry.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
Anorexia Nervosa
The second example comes from our own specialty
area: the observation of patients with chronic, relatively stable AN. If rodents and primates allocated
to a CR condition are the most fortunate of laboratory animals, these individuals hold the winning
ticket in the human species. Through their own
initiativealbeit for different reasonsthey have
found their way to a dietary regimen that should
be associated with unprecedented health, vigour
and longevity.
In some senses, individuals with longstanding AN
make even better exemplars of human CRL than the
tiny sample of enclosed Biospherians. Their existence affirms that at least a few people can practise
radical restriction at liberty; their persistence means
that we can trace its effects over much longer periods
than the token 2-year stint in Biosphere 2. Of course,
only a fraction of AN patients will meet criteria for
293
correct CR over time, and their compliance cannot
be verified with precision. Experts stress, however,
that the critical element in the CRL paradigm is
simply prolonged caloric deficit in the absence of
malnutrition. Some AN patients clearly fulfill those
specifications. So what can this subgroup of individuals tell us about serious, sustained CR outside the
context of the laboratory?
One ready conclusion is that CR does indeed
work for human beings, at least in the same limited
sense affirmed by the Biosphere data. CRL advocates were excited (if not surprised) when food
restriction was shown to lower the blood pressure,
body temperature, glucose levels and white cell
counts of the eight Biospherians. Precisely the same
results can be read off the medical charts of thousands of AN patients. (Moreover, such benefits are
discernible not only in the model cases who adopt
nutritionally sound CR, but the considerably larger
percentage of patients who practise unsanctioned
forms of restrictionalthough the animal research
predicts that only the former will enjoy the full array
of long-term benefits.) Two recent datasets also offer
tantalizing hints about the potential protective
power of prolonged CR. In one records-based study
of patients with possible, probable or definite AN
seen up to 63 years earlier, the total sample appeared
at heightened risk of death from psychiatric causes,
including suicide and alcoholism; however, allcause mortality was not elevated and there was a
decreased risk of death from cardiovascular disease
(Korndorfer et al., 2003; see discussions in Palmer,
2003, and Sullivan, 2003). Because of diagnostic
uncertainties and lack of information about diet
and duration, these findings are no more than suggestive. But through the noise of methodological
limitations, the signal that CRL researchers would
most like to discern emits a faint hum. Whatever
damage AN may reflect and/or inflict in other areas
of patients lives, it could be working wonders in
their circulatory systemsjust as imposed CR
improves the cardiovascular health of underfed
rodents and monkeys. Another retrospective study
of 7303 women previously hospitalized for AN
found a 53% lower incidence of breast cancer over
the follow-up interval (Michels & Ekbom, 2004)
ironically, almost precisely matching the risk reduction for mammary tumours in energy-restricted
mice (Dirx, Zeegers, Dagnelie, van den Bogaard, &
van den Brandt, 2003).
On the other hand, data from the ED field suggest
that CR virtually never works, in the sense that it is
rarely sustained over time and generally done quite
badlyeven by individuals who are fiercely
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
294
committed to keeping it running and doing it right.
Dieters regain, restrained eaters limit their intake in
theory more than practice, and a majority of restricting anorexics slide inexorably towards bulimia.
Quite commonly, AN patients find themselves
unable to continue hard-core restraint without ever
having made an affirmative decision to let it go.
After yearssometimes even decadesof grimly
successful CR, they can no longer summon the
strength required for the constant battle with their
own biology.
We can also learn more about the significance of
the silent terms in the CRL syllogism by analysing
how some AN patients manage to restrict as
valiantly and persistently as they do. At the start of
their disorder, the external circumstances of anorexics-in-the-making show little resemblance to those
of lab rats or Biospherians. By the time AN is well
established, however, most have recreated a
strikingly similar environment. In effect, anorexic
individuals construct their own virtual cages and
move in for the duration of their illness. Each finds
her way, individually but lawfully, to the same set
of conditions that researchers create for animals on
CR: isolation from others; protected, predictable
and constricted surroundings; minimal demands
or expectations; fixed and monotonous rations; elimination of activities and goals incompatible with the
maintenance of CR. It seems probable that those are
the only circumstances under which severe restriction can be practised or endured. For psychological
reasons, individuals with AN may be willing to pay
the astronomical costs of chronic deprivation that
less troubled people reject as unacceptable. Advocates of CRL are urging the general public to reconsider, in view of the objective benefits to be gained
from an anorexic lifestyle. From our perspective, it
is fortunate that their efforts will seldom succeed
(Vitousek & Gray, 2002).
Recently, experts have begun to acknowledge that
CRL may not gain widespread acceptance (e.g.
Mattson et al., 2003; Pinel et al., 2000; Roth et al.,
2001)but they have yet to come to terms with
why that is so. Many seem to view the human reluctance to semi-starve as a blend of ignorance, shortsightedness, weakness and hedonism. Whatever
the merits of these models in explaining the steep
rise in obesity rates, they do not provide an adequate
account for the rejection of radical CR and subnormal weight. To understand why Biospherians and
lab animals refeed the moment they are reprieved
from restrictionor why anorexics must retreat
from the world in order to pursue itwe need to
look to the CR syndrome itself. In addition to the
REFERENCES
Adzoe. (2002). Proactive and bullish CR. Retrieved July 7,
2003 from http://group.yahoo.com/group/crsociety/
message 21136.
Allen, J. (1991). Biosphere 2: The human experiment. New
York: Viking.
Alling, A., & Nelson, M. (1993). Life under glass: The inside
story of Biosphere 2. Oracle AZ: The Biosphere Press.
Allison, D. B., Miller, R. A., Austad, S. N., Bouchard, C.,
Leibel, R., Klebanov, S., et al. (2001). Genetic variability
in responses to caloric restriction in animals and in
regulation of metabolism and obesity in humans.
Journals of Gerontology (Series A), 56A, 5565.
Aly, K. B., Pipkin, J. L., Hinson, W. G., Feuers, R. J., Duffy,
P. H., & Hart, R. W. (1994a). Temporal and substratedependent patterns of stress protein expression in the
hypothalamus of caloric restricted rats. Mechanisms of
Ageing and Development, 76, 110.
Aly, K. B., Pipkin, J. L., Hinson, W. G., Feuers, R. J., Duffy,
P. H., Lyn-Cook, L., et al. (1994b). Chronic caloric
restriction induces stress proteins in the hypothalamus
of rats. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development, 76, 1123.
Anson, R. M., Guo, Z., de Cabo, R., Iyun, T., Rios, M.,
Hagepanos, A., et al. (2003). Intermittent fasting
dissociates beneficial effects of dietary restriction on
glucose metabolism and neuronal resistance to injury
from calorie intake. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, 100, 62166220.
Arking, R. (2004). Extending human longevity: A biological probability. In S. G. Post, & R. H. Binstock (Eds.),
The fountain of youth: Cultural, scientific, and ethical
perspectives on a biomedical goal (pp. 177200). New
York: Oxford Press.
Armario, A., Montero, J. L., & Jolin, T. (1987). Chronic
food restriction and the circadian rhythms of pituitaryadrenal hormones, growth hormone and thyroidstimulating hormone. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism, 31, 8187.
Armstrong-Esther, C. A., Lacey, J. H., Crisp, A. H., &
Bryant, T. N. (1978). An investigation of the immune
response of patients suffering from anorexia nervosa.
Postgraduate Medical Journal, 54, 395399.
Associated Press. (1996). Biosphere tested temperaments.
July 29.
Austad, S. N. (1997). Why we age: What science is discovering
about the bodys journey through life. New York: John Wiley.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
295
comparison of anorexia nervosa subtypes. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 191201.
Emborg, C. (1999). Mortality and causes of death in eating
disorders in Denmark 19701993: A case register
study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25,
243251.
Erickson, J. (1993). Biospherians return to world: 2-year
mission described as magnificent. Arizona Daily Star,
September 27, p. 1A. Retrieved September 16,
2003, from http://pqasb.pqarchiver.com/azstarnet/
47916874.
Fichter, M. M., & Quadflieg, N. (1999). Six-year course
and outcome of anorexia nervosa. International Journal
of Eating Disorders, 26, 359385.
Fishbein, L. (Ed.). (1991). Biological effects of dietary restriction
(ILSI Monographs). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Forster, M. J., Morris, P., & Sohal, R. S. (2003). Genotype
and age influence the effect of caloric intake on
mortality in mice. FASEB J, 17, 690692.
Golla, J. A., Larson, L. A., Anderson, C. F., Lucas, A. R.,
Wilson, W. R., & Tomasi, T. B., Jr. (1981). An
immunological assessment of patients with anorexia
nervosa. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 34,
27562762.
Graves, J. L. (1993). The costs of reproduction and dietary
restriction: Parallels between insects and mammals.
Growth, Development & Aging, 57, 233249.
Greely, A. W. (1886). Three years of Arctic service: An
account of the Lady Franklin Bay Expedition of 18811884
and the attainment of the farthest North (2 vols). London:
Richard Bentley.
Grinspoon, S., Miller, K., Coyle, C., Krempin, J.,
Armstrong, C., Pitts, S. et al. (1999). Severity of
osteopenia in estrogen-deficient women with
anorexia nervosa and hypothalamic amenorrhea.
Journal of Clinical Endrocrinology and Metabolism, 84,
20492055.
Han, E.-S., Evans, T. R., Shu, J. H., Lee, S., & Nelson, J. F.
(2001). Food restriction enhances endogenous and
corticotropin-induced plasma elevations of free but
not total corticosterone throughout life in rats. Journal
of Gerontology: Biological Sciences, 56A, B391B397.
Hansen, B. C., & Bodkin, N. L. (1993). Primary prevention
of diabetes by prevention of obesity in monkeys.
Diabetes, 42, 18091814.
Harrison, D. E., & Archer, J. R. (1988). Biomarkers of
aging: Tissue markers, future research needs, strategies,
directions
and
priorities.
Experimental
Gerontology, 23, 309321.
Harrison, D. E., & Archer, J. R. (1991). Genotype
modulates the effects of caloric restriction on aging
processes in rodents. In D. K. Ingram, G. T. Baker III, &
N. W. Shock (Eds.), The potential for nutritional
modulation of aging processes (pp. 137155). Trumbull,
CT: Food & Nutrition Press.
Hart, R. W., Neumann, D. A., & Robertson, R. T. (1995).
Dietary restriction: Implications for the design and
interpretation of toxicity and carcinogenicity studies.
Washington, DC: ILSI Press.
Hart, R. W., & Turturro, A. (1998). Evolution and dietary
restriction. Experimental Gerontology, 33, 5360.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
296
Hass, B. S., Lewis, S. M., Duffy, P. H., Ershler, W., Feuers,
R. J., Good, R. A., et al. (1996). Dietary restriction in
humans: Report on the Little Rock Conference on the
value, feasibility, and parameters of a proposed study.
Mechanisms of Ageing and Development, 91, 7994.
Heilbronn, L. K., & Ravussin, E. (2003). Calorie restriction
and aging: Review of the literature and implications
for studies in humans. American Journal of Clinical
Nutrition, 78, 361369.
Herzog, D. B., Greenwood, D. N., Dorer, D. J., Flores,
A. T., Ekeblad, E. R., Richards, A., et al. (2000).
Mortality in eating disorders: A descriptive study.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 2026.
Heydari, A. R., Wu, B., Takahashi, R., Strong, R., &
Richardson, A. (1993). Expression of heat shock
protein 70 is altered by age and diet at the level of
transcription. Molecular and Cellular Biology, 13,
29092918.
Holliday, R. (1989). Food, reproduction and longevity: Is
the extended lifespan of calorie-restricted animals an
evolutionary adaptation? BioEssays, 10, 125127.
Holloszy, J. O. (1997). Mortality rate and longevity of
food-restricted exercising male rats: A reevaluation.
Journal of Applied Physiology, 82, 399403.
Holloszy, J. O., & Kohrt, W. M. (1995). Exercise. In
E. J. Masoro (Ed.), Handbook of physiology, Section 11:
Aging (pp. 633666). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Holloszy, J. O., & Schechtman, K. B. (1991). Interaction
between exercise and food restriction: Effects on
longevity of male rats. Journal of Applied Physiology,
70, 15291535.
Hursting, S. D., Lavigne, J. A., Berrigan, D., Perkins, S. N.,
& Barrett, J. C. (2003). Calorie restriction, aging, and
cancer prevention: Mechanisms of action and applicability to humans. Annual Review of Medicine, 54,
131152.
Ingram, D. K. (2003). Development of caloric restriction
mimetics as a prolongevity strategy (Abstract).
Biogerontology, 4(Suppl. 1), 4546.
Ingram, D. K., Cutler, R. G., Weindruch, R., Renquist, D.
M., Knapka, J. J., April, M., et al. (1990). Dietary
restriction and aging: The initiation of a primate study.
Journal of Gerontology: Biological Sciences, 45,
B148B163.
Johannes, L. (2002). Lean times: The surprising rise of
radical, calorie-cutting dietcould self deprivation be
the secret to a longer, albeit famished, life? The Wall
Street Journal Online. June 3. Retrieved August 5, 2003
from http://www.legacyalliance.net/Articles/News/
Diet-Longevity.htm
Kahn, C. (1996). Eat less, live longer. Longevity, 4748,
9095.
Katzman, D. K., & Zipursky, R. B. (1994). Adolescents
with anorexia nervosa: The impact of the disorder on
bones and brains. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences, 817, 127137.
Kemnitz, J. W., Weindruch, R., Roecker, E. B., Crawford,
K., Kaufman, P. L., & Ershler, W. B. (1993). Dietary
restriction of adult male rhesus monkeys: Design,
methodology, and preliminary findings from the first
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
297
Nelson, J. F., Gosden, R. G., & Felicio, L. S. (1985). Effect of
dietary restriction on estrous cyclicity and follicular
reserves in aging C57BL/6J mice. Biology of Reproduction, 32, 515522.
Nielsen, S., Moller-Madsen, S., Isager, T., Jorgensen, J.,
Pagsberg, K., & Theander, S. (1998). Standardized
mortality in eating disorders: A quantitative summary
of previously published and new evidence. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 44, 413434.
Palmer, R. L. (2003). Death in anorexia nervosa. The
Lancet, 361, 1490.
Patel, N. V., & Finch, C. E. (2002). The glucocorticoid
paradox of caloric restriction in slowing brain aging.
Neurobiology of Aging, 23, 707717.
Phelan, J. P., & Austad, S. N. (1989). Natural selection,
dietary restriction, and extended longevity. Growth,
Development, and Aging, 53, 46.
Pinel, J. P. J. (2000). Biopsychology (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn
& Bacon.
Pinel, J. P. J., Assanand, S., & Lehman, D. R. (2000).
Hunger, eating, and ill health. American Psychologist,
55, 11051116.
Poehlman, E. T., Turturro, A., Bodkin, N., Cefalu, W.,
Heymsfield, S., Holloszy, J., et al. (2001). Caloric
restriction mimetics: Physical activity and body
composition changes. Journals of Gerontology (Series
A), 56A, 4554.
Pomeroy, C., & Mitchell, J. E. (2002). Medical complications of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In C. G.
Fairburn, & K. D. Brownell (Eds.), Eating disorders and
obesity: A comprehensive handbook (pp. 278285). New
York: Guilford Press.
Ramsey, J. J., Colman, R. J., Binkley, N. C., Christensen,
J. D., Gresl, T. A., Kemnitz, J. W., et al. (2000). Dietary
restriction and aging in rhesus monkeys: The
University of Wisconsin study. Experimental Gerontology, 35, 11311149.
Reed, M. J., Penn, P. E., Li, Y., Birnbaum, R., Vernon, R. B.,
Johnson, T. S., et al. (1996). Enhanced cell proliferation
and biosynthesis mediate improved wound repair in
refed, caloric-restricted mice. Mechanisms of Ageing and
Development, 89, 2143.
Rigotti, N. A., Neer, R. M., Skates, S. J., Herzog, D. B.,
& Nussbaum, S. R. (1991). The clinical course
of osteoporosis in anorexia nervosa: A longitudinal
study of cortical bone mass. JAMA, 265,
11331138.
Rikke, B. A., Yerg, J. E. III, Battaglia, M. E., Nagy, T. R.,
Allison, D. B., & Johnson, T. E. (2003). Strain variation
in the response of body temperature to dietary
restriction. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development,
124, 663678.
Roberts, S. B., Pi-Sunyer, X., Kuller, L., Lane, M. A.,
Ellison, P., Prior, J. C., et al. (2001). Physiologic effects
of lowering caloric intake in nonhuman primates and
nonobese humans. Journals of Gerontology (Series A),
56A, 6675.
Roecker, E. B., Kemnitz, J. W., Ershler, W. B., &
Weindruch, R. (1996). Reduced immune responses in
rhesus monkeys subjected to dietary restriction. Journal
of Gerontology: Biological Sciences, 51A, B276B279.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
K. M. Vitousek et al.
298
Roth, G. S., Ingram, D. K., & Lane, M. A. (2001). Caloric
restriction in primates and relevance to humans.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 92, 305315.
Sanderson, J. P., Binkley, N., Roecker, E. B., Champ, J. E.,
Pugh, T. D., Aspnes, L., et al. (1997). Influence of fat
intake and caloric restriction on bone in aging male
rats. Journal of Gerontology: Biological Sciences, 52A,
B20B25.
Serpell, L., & Treasure, J. (1997). Osteoporosis: A serious
health risk in chronic anorexia nervosa. European
Eating Disorders Review, 5, 149157.
Shanley, D. P., & Kirkwood, T. B. L. (2000). Calorie
restriction and aging: A life-history analysis. Evolution,
54, 740750.
Siemers, B., Chakmakjian, Z., & Gench, B. (1996). Bone
density patterns in women with anorexia nervosa.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19, 179186.
Silverstone, S. (1993). Eating in: From the field to the kitchen
in Biosphere 2. Oracle, AZ: The Biosphere Press.
Silverstone, S., & Nelson, M. (1996). Food production and
nutrition in Biosphere 2: Results from the first mission
September 1991 to September 1993. Advances in Space
Research, 18, 4961.
Sohal, R. S., & Weindruch, R. (1996). Oxidative stress,
caloric restriction, and aging. Science, 273, 5963.
Spindler, S. R., Dhahbi, J. D., Mote, P. L., Kim, H. J., &
Tsuchiya, T. (2003). Rapid identification of candidate
CR mimetics using microarray (Abstract). Biogerontology, 4(Suppl. 1), 89.
Stewart, J., Meaney, M. J., Ailken, D., Jensen, L., & Kalant,
N. (1988). The effects of acute and life long food
restriction in basal and stress-induced serum corticosterone levels in young and aged rats. Endocrinology,
123, 19341941.
Sullivan, P. F. (2003). Editorial: Discrepant results
regarding long-term survival of patients with anorexia
nervosa? Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 78, 273274.
Sun, D., Muthukumar, A. R., Lawrence, R. A., &
Fernandes, G. (2001). Effects of calorie restriction on
polymicrobial peritonitis induced by cecum ligation
and puncture in young C57BL/6 mice. Clinical and
Diagnostic Laboratory Immunology, 8, 10031011.
Taubes, G. (2000). Famine of youth. Scientific American
Presents, June. Retrieved April 27, 2002 from http://
www.sciam.com
Thygesen, P., Hermann, K., & Willanger, R. (1970).
Concentration camp survivors in Denmark:
Persecution, disease, disability, compensation. Danish
Medical Bulletin, 17, 65108.
Verdery, R. B., & Walford, R. L. (1998). Changes in plasma
lipids and lipoproteins in humans during a 2-year
period of dietary restriction in Biosphere 2. Archives of
Internal Medicine, 158, 900906.
Vitousek, K. M., & Gray, J. (2002). Comment: Sun, moon,
and stars. American Psychologist, 57, 370371.
Vitousek, K. M., Gray, J. A., & Gonzalez, V. Hunger in
anorexia nervosa. Manuscript in preparation.
Vitousek, K. M., Gray, J. A., & Talesfore, C. Caloric
restriction for longevity in humans: A new rationale
for semi-starvation. In press.
Vitousek, K. M. Manke, F. P., Gray, J. A., & Vitousek,
M. N. Caloric restriction for longevity, II: The
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.
299
Yu, B. P., & Chung, H. Y. (2001). Stress resistance by
caloric restriction for longevity. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 92, 3947.
Yu, B. P., Masoro, E. J., & McMahan, C. A. (1985).
Nutritional influences on aging of Fischer 344 rats: I.
Physical, metabolic, and longevity characteristics.
Journal of Gerontology, 40, 657670.
Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd and Eating Disorders Association.