Developmental Perspective
Developmental Perspective
Developmental Perspective
Barbara D. DeBaryshe
University of Hawai`i
Dale Fryxell
Author note: Address requests for reprints to the first author at: University of Hawai`i, Center on
the Family, 2515 Campus Road, 103 Miller Hall, Honolulu, HI 96822. E-mail:
debarysh@hawaii.edu.
Developmental perspective on anger 2
Abstract
A model of the development of anger in family and peer contexts is presented. Emotional
awareness and understanding of their own and other people’s anger depends on exposure to
models of anger expression and conflict resolution, parental emotional coaching strategies such as
discussing feelings and problem-solving about emotion-based actions, coercive vs. authoritative
parenting practices, and opportunities to engage in constructive conflict resolution with family
members. Emotional socialization in the home affects children’s physiological reactivity and
physiological self-regulation, social information processing, and behavioral strategies for anger-
provoking situations. When the child brings these characteristics to the peer arena, anger and
emotion management skills affect peer social status, aggressive vs. prosocial peer interactions and
the ability to form and profit from close peer relationships. Implications for school psychologists
are among the most common and serious concerns of parents and teachers (McGee, Silva &
Williams, 1983). Despite widespread concern with the negative consequences for both affected
individuals and society as a whole, there has been remarkably little attention paid to anger per se.
While a voluminous literature exists on the etiology and sequellae of children’s aggressive and
antisocial behavior, the theorized accompanying characteristic difficulty with anger frequency,
The purpose of this paper is to present a developmental model of anger. We will explore
how children come to recognize and label emotions in themselves and others, how they come to
learn appropriate or inappropriate methods for dealing with anger, and what links may exist
between manifestations of anger in family and peer settings. We will show how emotional
socialization within the family affects children’s physiological regulation of emotional states, social
information processing, and response strategies in emotion-laden situations. We will then explore
the links between these aspects of children’s emotional competence and the quality of their peer
interactions and relationships with peers. Finally, we will discuss the implications of this review
Family life is our first school for emotional learning; in this intimate cauldron we
learn how to feel about ourselves and how others will react to our feelings; how to
think about these feelings and what choices we have in reacting; how to read and
express hopes and fears. This emotional schooling operates not just through the
things that parents say and do directly to children, but also in the models they offer
for handling their own feelings and those that pass between husband and
Developmental perspective on anger 4
consequences for the child’s emotional life (Goleman, 1995, pp. 189-190).
Family members, particularly parents, play a crucial role in emotional socialization. Family
members may be the cause of different emotional experiences, depending on the content and
affective tenor of family interactions. Family members provide models of emotional expression
and affect-related coping and behavior strategies. Finally, family members provide direct
instruction, teaching children how to recognize and label emotions, and how to act on their
emotional states.
Early parental responsiveness to infant emotions. Infants and young children rely on
adults for help in regulating negative emotional states (Kopp, 1989). Neonatal distress is almost
entirely a function of physiological status and the young infant has only a small repertoire of
reflexive behaviors (such as sucking or shutting of the eyes) that have a self-soothing function.
Between 3-8 months, several important developments occur. The first is the emergence of
associative learning related to emotions. For example, the infant may calm at the sound of a
parent’s voice, as a conditioned response to the parent’s anticipated assistance. Infants also
attempt to control their environments through techniques such as voluntary crying or using
distinct calls to express different emotional states. Infants also engage in social referencing; in
ambiguous or novel situations, they look for the parent’s emotional reaction and use parental
affect as a guide for their own behavior. As infants express a wider variety of emotions, parents
show greater differentiation in their response to infant behaviors, e.g., mothers are more soothing
when the child is fearful than when the child is angry. Kopp proposes (a) that caretakers’
Developmental perspective on anger 5
distinctive behaviors help infants recognize variation in their own emotional states and (b) that by
providing assistance at times of distress, parents teach children that the child’s own behavior can
be instrumental in soliciting assistance. She further proposes that true self-regulation of emotional
distress does not occur until age two when children have the requisite physical agility and
cognitive insights. Across the preschool years, parents increase their expectations for self-
regulation and gradually cede responsibility for emotional control to the child.
In the attachment paradigm, consistent, responsive caretaking of infants, especially during times
of stress, is thought to be crucial. Responsive caretaking sets the stage for the development of a
secure parent-infant attachment. Securely attached infants are thought to have an unconscious,
positive “inner working model” of the world as a safe and welcoming place and the self as
efficacious in eliciting assistance (Cassidy & Berlin, 1994; Kobak, Cole, Ferenz-Gillies & Fleming,
1993). Cassidy and Berlin (1994) suggest that attachment in toddlers and preschoolers is
associated with distinct patterns of emotional regulation. Securely attached children have been
taught that their negative emotions will be soothed; they more openly express their emotions, are
most easily consoled and may be the most skillful and proactive in emotional self-regulation.
Insecurely attached infants have been taught either that their parents will reject their expressions
of negative emotion, or that negative affect will be met with inconsistent parental reactions. In
the first case these infants will suppress affect and avoid parental contact in stressful situations.
In the latter case, infants become emotionally labile, difficult to soothe and show conflicted
approach-avoidance patterns towards the parent during times of stress. Overall, a secure
attachment is associated with more positive social competence. Children with secure attachments
show more positive ego development, prosocial skills and social relationships across the lifespan
Talking about emotions. Starting in toddlerhood, family members talk with children about
emotions. When young children exhibit negative emotions (anger or sad distress), mothers
attempt to teach their children how to interpret and act on these emotional states. Mothers’ label
their children’s emotions, provide explanations, and attempt to guide children’s behavior towards
acceptable resolutions (Saarni, Mume & Campos, 1997). Toddlers’ goals in the conversations are
primarily expressive, the aim is to let the mother know what they are feeling. Later, children
initiate talk about emotions when they themselves are in a neutral state; these more mature
conversations tend to be less emotionally self-centered (Dunn & Brown, 1994). Finally,
Compared to mothers, siblings are more interested in communicating their own states and
achieving self-interested goals (Brown & Dunn, 1992). Sibling discussions about emotions are
more frequent and more intense than discussions with mothers, and provide the preschooler with
more opportunities to focus on and talk about another person’s emotional state.
Talking about emotions appears to affect emotional competence. The frequency and
emotional intensity of mothers’ discussions about distress displayed by a third person is associated
with toddlers’ propensity to show comforting, altruistic behavior (Zahn-Waxler & Radke-Yarrow,
1979). The frequency of emotion-focused discussions at age three also predicts children’s
sensitivity to other people’s emotional signals at age six (Dunn, Brown & Beardsall, 1991; cited in
School age children and their parents most often discuss emotions in the context of
conversing about prohibited child behavior or peer issues. With older children, a nondirective,
reflective parental discussion style is associated with greater social competence. Nondirective
parents (a) encourage self-reflection by acknowledging the child’s feelings, (b) provide principles
about emotional causes and the consequences of the child’s actions, and (c) encourage the child to
Developmental perspective on anger 7
try to apply these principles to his or her own situation. When parent-child discourse follows this
style, children show better social perspective-taking skills, greater person-centered peer
communication, and more positive peer relationships (Burlson, Delia, Applegate, 1995).
Noncrictical discussions of children’s real-world problems in which parents focus on eliciting and
understanding the child’s perspective without imposing a solution are also associated with more
rapid growth in the maturity of moral reasoning (Walker & Taylor, 1991).
distressed emotional states. Empathy, the ability to mirror another person’s emotions is a
prerequisite for most prosocial or altruistic behavior (Eisenberg et al., 1992). Empathic responses
may be adaptive or maladaptive. Sympathy, the adaptive variation, involves the expression of
concern and taking action to relieve another person’s distress while maintaining a low level of
arousal oneself. The maladaptive empathic response is one of personal distress. When faced with
other people’s negative emotions, some children (and adults) experience anxiety and heightened
physiological arousal. Personal distress inhibits prosocial responding and may disinhibit
aggressive reactions. Sympathy, on the other hand, is associated with positive peer status and
social competence (Cummings & Davies, 1994; Eisenberg & McNally, 1993).
in response to child negative emotions appears to play a causal role in determining whether
children will show adaptive or maladaptive empathic responses. Mothers who are themselves
empathic, concerned and sympathetically responsive have the most empathic and self-regulating
children (Eisenberg et al., 1992; Eisenberg, Fabes & Murphy, 1996; Eisenberg & McNally, 1993).
Children are also more likely to show adaptive, sympathetic behaviors when (a) family discussions
Developmental perspective on anger 8
focus on “gentle” negative emotions such as sadness and grief, (b) when mothers engage in other-
oriented induction by drawing links between their children’s own experiences and the distress
experiences of other people, and (c) when mothers express approval of children’s sympathetic acts
Parents who show moderate encouragement of their children’s own negative emotional
expressions may be providing an optimal experience (Eisenberg, Fabes & Murphy, 1996). If
parents are overly permissive of negative expressions, children may not develop self control.
However, when the expression of negative emotions is too strongly discouraged, children remain
arousal interferes with information processing, leads to withdrawal from the situation, and may
include encouraging the child to express his or her feelings, providing comfort, and helping the
child problem solve about ways to handle the situation (Eisenberg et al, 1992). Supportive
parental reactions are associated with lower child negative emotionality, better coping skills, and
better peer relations. Unsupportive parental reactions such as minimizing or punishing children’s
expressions of negative states are associated with poorer coping and social skills.
Gottman has recently identified four distinctive styles of parental reactions to children’s
negative emotions (Gottman & DeClaire, 1997). Disapproving parents are actively critical or
punitive in reaction to their children’s negative emotions. Dismissing parents downplay or ignore
such emotions and attempt to distract their children, believing that negative emotions are harmful
and reflect poorly upon the parent. Laissez faire parents accept the child’s emotions empathically,
but do not know how to counsel their child in handling the negative emotional state; these parents
feel that emotional experiences must simply be ridden out. Finally, emotional coaches show both
empathic acceptance of negative emotions and supportive, directive guidance. Emotional coaches
Developmental perspective on anger 9
approach a child’s negative state as a teachable moment; they want the child to learn adaptive,
positive response strategies. Emotional coaches (a) are sensitive in recognizing children’s
emotions, (b) are empathic listeners, (c) help the child label his or her feelings and (d) set limits
for acceptable and unacceptable behavior while exploring problem-solving strategies with the
child.
desirable child outcomes including better physical health, school performance, social skills, peer
relations and psychological well-being. By maternal report, children of emotional coaches show
more positive affect and less negative affect than children of other parental styles. Most
important, Gottman found evidence that emotional coaching was associated with better ability to
regulate stress at the level of autonomic physiology. Under conditions of stress the sympathetic
nervous system activates the “fight or flight syndrome.” The parasympathetic nervous system
exerts counter-acting control, or calming of physical arousal; specifically, the vagus nerve controls
parasympathetic response. Children of emotional coaches have high vagal tone. This enables
them to show quicker recovery to emotional distress and lower levels of overall arousability.
In summary, parents are involved from the very first days of their child’s life in shaping and
socializing their child’s emotional style. Emotional socialization by family members helps children
learn to recognize and label their own and other people’s emotions, influences both physiological
and behavioral capacities for emotional regulation, and provides children with models and
strategies for assisting other people in emotionally-charged situations. Although the refinement of
these skills is doubtless a life-long process, individual differences in the basic style of emotional
reactivity and emotion-related social behavior are clearly identifiable by the end of the preschool
period.
Children as witnesses of anger. Considerable evidence shows that marital conflict co-
occurs with child dysfunction. Conflict between parents, whether in intact or divorced families, is
a risk factor for externalizing and internalizing behavior problems, low school achievement and
low social competence (Cummings & Davies, 1994; Grych & Finchum, 1990). The more specific
question as to how marital conflict might affect children’s understanding of anger and anger
management strategies has received less attention. A related issue is whether children can learn
positive lessons as a result of witnessing parental conflict. For example, can children learn that
anger does not necessarily impair relationships, and that conflicts can be constructively
negotiated?
Since children are frequent observers of interparental conflict, Cummings and his
colleagues have investigated the effects of witnessing adult anger (Cummings, Ballard, El-Sheik &
lake, 1991; Cummings & Davies, 1994; Cummings, Ianotti, & Zahn-Waxler, 1985; Cummings,
Pellegrini, Notarius, & Cummings, 1989; Cummings, Simpson, & Wilson, 1993; Cummings,
Zahn-Waxler, & Radke-Yarrow, 1981). One of the most striking findings of this work is that
children as young as 10 months of age show behavioral reactions to adult anger such as concerned
facial expressions or crying (Cummings et al., 1981). Among two- to five-year-olds, distress is an
almost universal reaction to observed adult anger, and the repertoire of distress reactions now
includes behaviors such as freezing, seeking comfort from the parents, discussing the incident, and
attempting to mediate the adult’s quarrel (Cummings et al., 1985, 1989). Preschoolers also
showed delayed anger as a common reaction (Cummings et al., 1985). After the adult conflict
subsides, preschoolers are likely to direct (nonimitative) aggressive behavior toward peers. Such
peer victimization is more common when the offending child is male, has an aggressive reputation,
or has been exposed to repeated episodes of background anger. Prosocial child interventions in
Developmental perspective on anger 11
adult anger episodes are also seen; one-quarter of preschoolers attempt to intervene, offer
Children exposed to frequent parental conflict at home appear to be most sensitive to adult
anger. Low marital satisfaction between the child’s parents, and particularly, physical conflict
between spouses is associated with greater attention, concern and involvement with conflicts
between the mother and an adult confederate (Cummings et al., 1989, 1985). As with younger
children, early adolescents report stronger negative reactions, including anger, fear, sadness and
shame as adult conflict intensity increases (Grych & Finchum, 1993). Intensity increases
children’s judgments that the conflict could escalate and that the child may be blamed and/or
drawn in.
Not all adult conflicts have negative or lasting effects on children. When conflicts are
unresolved, child witnesses feel greater fear and anger and adolescents are more likely to
intervene (Cummings et al., 1991, 1993). When conflicts are fully resolved, and both participants
display congruent, friendly emotions, children’s emotional states quickly revert to baseline.
Moreover, children as young as age five can infer from friendly behavior and descriptions that
conflicts have been resolved, even if the resolution occurred in the child’s absence.
When parental conflict is about the child, children feel greater shame, guilt, and fear of
being drawn into the argument and are also more likely to intervene (Grych & Finchum, 1993).
Of particular concern is the finding that younger children are quite inaccurate in judging whether
or not they are the cause of an adult’s anger (Covell & Abramovitch, 1988; Covell & Miles,
1992). Younger children erroneously blame themselves when a parent is angry, especially if the
anger was caused by another family member (Covell & Abramovitch, 1988).
The implications for parents are straightforward. Children need not be shielded from
parental conflict per se, as long as the conflict style is not aggressive and the adults behave
Developmental perspective on anger 12
lovingly (or at least civilly) towards each other once the dispute is resolved. Parents of young
children should reassure the child that he or she is not responsible for anger caused by others.
Additionally, for children of any age, parents should be aware of the special significance and
child and sibling conflicts are ubiquitous events. For example, preschool children present their
mothers with 3-4 mildly aversive events per hour and mothers direct a command or criticism to
their two- to three-year-olds once every three minutes (Patterson, 1980). A second peak period
of parent-child conflict occurs during adolescence, when 15-20% of families report severe levels
of parent-child conflict (Montemayor, 1986, cited in Rubenstein & Feldman, 1993) and the quality
appears to be the peak time of parent-child conflict and mothers and children are the most likely
Most parent-child conflicts revolve around low-level, mundane concerns such as chores,
homework, personal appearance and getting along with others (Barber, 1994; Smetana, 1989).
Such frequent, low-stakes events potentially provide children opportunities to learn negotiation
and conflict resolution skills. Parent-child conflict may be an especially important arena in which
adolescents may gradually bid for and be granted increased autonomy (Smetana, 1989).
Parents and children tend to show reciprocity, or matching, of both affect and behavior in
the course of family conflicts (Forgatch, 1989; Kobak et al., 1993). Some families are able to
These families tend to listen attentively, generate a wide variety of possible solutions, and agree
upon reasonable, feasible strategies that each participant can support (Forgatch, 1989; Reuter &
Conger, 1995). It seems to be important for children to be granted floor time, to have their ideas
Developmental perspective on anger 13
heard, and to be given room to negotiate and advocate on their own behalf (Kavanaugh, Frey &
Larsen, undated; Smetana, 1989). Dysfunctional parent-child conflict strategies include anger,
criticism, avoidance of continued discussion, parental coalitions against the child, and unilateral
parental power (Forgatch, 1989; Kobak et al., 1993; Rubenstein & Feldman, 1993; Reuter &
other settings and with their overall adjustment. For example, adolescents who listen, reason and
compromise during parent-child conflicts show low internalizing and externalizing problems, high
academic achievement and high school-related effort (Rubenstein & Feldman, 1993). Adolescent
conflict avoidance, e.g., the child “clams up” during family arguments is associated with
internalizing problems, whereas child anger and verbal or physical belligerence during parent-child
conflict is associated with internalizing, externalizing and academic problems (Rubenstein &
Feldman, 1993). Negative emotion expressed by the parent-child dyad during problem-solving
leads to lower quality solutions, and poor solutions are characteristic of children who show verbal
and/or physical aggression at home and school (Forgatch, 1989). Finally, parents who mediate
sibling conflicts with fairness and low differential treatment set the stage for low rates of sibling
conflict both now and in the future (Brody, Stoneman, McCoy & Forehand, 1992).
Coercive family interactions. Problems with high levels of anger and low anger control
are almost by definition central to externalizing behavior problems such as conduct disorder and
antisocial behavior disorder. Patterson’s coercive family process model (Patterson, 1982;
Patterson, Reid & Dishion, 1992) is one of the most widely-accepted explanations of how family
interaction plays a key role in the etiology of childhood conduct problems. According to this
model, the more severe and chronic forms of antisocial behavior are rooted in patterns of family
interaction that begin in early childhood. When parents routinely acquiesce to their child’s
Developmental perspective on anger 14
negative behaviors, young children quickly learn to initiate negative behaviors as a way of getting
what they want (Gardner, 1989; Patterson et al., 1992). Over time, family members attempt to
control each other’s behavior primarily through aversive means. Children with conduct disorders
live in coercive families where interactions are characterized by (a) high rates of aversive
behaviors, (b) a tendency to reciprocate negative behaviors in kind, (c) rapid escalation to high
levels of intensity, and (d) longer bouts of negative interaction. The problem child is most often
the “winner” of these coercive cycles. However, parents in coercive families are inconsistent and
at unpredictable times will revert to more explosive, harsh discipline strategies, in an attempt to
control the problematic child. In addition to this pattern of ineffective discipline, coercive families
are also characterized by poor parental monitoring of children’s companions, activities and
whereabouts when children are not at home, poor family problem-solving patterns, lower
responsiveness to child prosocial behavior, and low warmth and involvement with the child
Coercive children face many social challenges outside the home as well. When coercive
children enter school, some are socialized by peers and teachers to adopt more typical, positive
modes of social influence. Children who do not become more prosocial are rejected by their
peers; teachers also tend to minimize their interactions with coercive children (Patterson et al.,
1992; Patterson, DeBaryshe & Ramsey, 1989). Over time, coercive children suffer decrements in
self-esteem and academic motivation. With few alternatives for socialization, they coalesce in
deviant peer groups in early adolescence. These peer groups become the prime training ground
for the adoption of delinquent behaviors and antisocial attitudes (Patterson et al., 1989, 1992).
The coercive family process model may be placed in the context of a larger literature on
global parenting styles. The authoritative parenting style involves an overall pattern of parental
warmth, involvement, clear rules, rationale-based discipline, and granting the child psychological
Developmental perspective on anger 15
autonomy. Children of authoritative parents tend to show positive social outcomes such as high
self-esteem, school achievement, and work orientation, as well as low substance use, internalizing
and externalizing behavior problems (Barber, Olsen, & Shagle, 1994; Maccoby & Martin, 1983;
The literature on both coercive family processes and antisocial behavior has by and large
ignored the specific topic of emotional socialization and the development of anger regulation per
se. However, it is quite reasonable to suggest that coercive families do a poor job of emotional
coaching and that antisocial children have significant problems with anger management. The links
between family emotional socialization (whether skillful or coercive) and peer competence are
As children reach the preschool and early elementary school years they begin to venture
out of the home and family environment and spend increasing amounts of time and energy
interacting with peers. Even though they are leaving the family environment, many of the things
that they initially learned there, including their experiences with anger and aggression, accompany
them into this larger social world. A foundation for social interaction patterns that children
originally developed through relationships with parents serves as the child's basis for interpreting
and navigating the larger social environment. The patterns that young children have developed to
process social information appear to play a significant role not only in how they will experience
Social information processing models provide a clue into the role that cognitive
processing plays in a child's decision to use aggressive or negative behaviors in response to angry
Developmental perspective on anger 16
feelings and hostile thoughts. Children who often demonstrate competent behavior in social
situations with peers are thought to be skilled at social information processing, whereas children
with social information processing deficits are more likely to experience chronic anger and act out
in aggressive or socially inappropriate ways with peers (Trachtenberg & Viken, 1994).
Aggressive children exhibit difficulties or biases at one or more of the sequential steps used in
social information processing (Dodge & Frame, 1982; Dodge & Somberg, 1987; Gouze, 1987).
The steps include (a) encoding social cues from the environment, (b) interpreting this social
information, (c) clarifying goals, (d) constructing possible responses, (e) making a response
decision, and (f) enacting that social behavior (Crick & Dodge, 1994).
Two distinct deficient patterns in social information processing have been found in
aggressive children (Dodge, 1980). The first pattern includes children who have negative biases
at the interpretation step. These children are likely to interpret ambiguous social information
negatively and attribute hostile or malicious intent to social cues much more often than would
normally be expected. As a result, they are more likely to see others, including peers, as
threatening and respond aggressively in response to a perceived threat. This reactive response
pattern has been found to result in a negative cycle that begins when the child attributes hostile
intent to another child and reacts in an aggressive manner (Crick & Dodge, 1996). As a response
to this aggressive behavior the child's peers respond negatively in return. This negative response
confirms for the child that his or her earlier biased interpretation was correct, reinforcing the belief
The second deficient social information processing pattern involves the response decision
step in the model. Dodge and Crick (1990) state that as part of this step, children evaluate
possible responses and assess the likely outcomes for each response. In addition, they determine
their confidence level regarding their ability to perform each of the possible responses. It is
Developmental perspective on anger 17
through this process that children select the response option that they believe will result in a
favorable outcome. Many children who have a history of anger and aggressive behaviors are
highly confident in their ability to perform aggressive acts and expect that aggressive acts will be
effective in meeting their social goals (Crick & Ladd, 1990). This positive assessment increases
the likelhood that aggressive strategies will actually be employed. If the aggression is indeed
effective, the child’s behaviors as well as his or her judgements will be reinforced.
Thus, aggressive children, have poor social problem solving skills (Lochman, Lampron, &
Rabiner, 1989). They often fail to attend to social cues (Dodge, Bates, & Petit, 1990), interpret
peer intentions incorrectly and often assume hostile intent (Dodge & Frame, 1982; Dodge,
Murphy, & Buchsbaum, 1984), are the targets of unprovoked aggression more often than their
peers (Dodge & Frame, 1982), and interpret their aggressive behaviors as leading to positive
Aggressive and conduct disordered children also appear to also show information
processing deficits in regards to positive emotions and behaviors. They have difficulty
interpreting other people’s prosocial initiations and their own emotional reactions to these
prosocial events (Casey & Schlosser, 1994). They show more surprise and hostility when they are
the recipients of a peer’s compliments. They are also less able to remember the details of the
peer’s positive act, less accurate in describing their own external emotional displays, and less able
Finally, problem children may be less upset by anger and aggressive confrontations for
physiological reasons. Studies of antisocial adolescents and adults have led some researchers to
conclude that they experience a chronic state of underarousal (see Patterson, et al., 1992 for a
needed physiological stimulation. This need for stimulation may appear as early as the preschool
Developmental perspective on anger 18
years. For example, four- and five-year-olds high on externalizing symptoms show less
physiological reactivity when exposed to angry situations involving others (Cummings & Davies,
1994). Problem boys in particular feel less aroused by these scenes and will actually elect to view
The risks associated with social information processing deficits are well known. These
chidren are often ostracized or rejected by their peers, primarily because of their aggressive
behaviors (Ladd, Price & Hart, 1990). Aggressive children who are socially rejected often
congregate into groups with other deviant children (Coie, 1990). Membership in such peer
groups is predictive of several other negative outcomes including dropping out of school and
delinquent behavior (Cairns, Cairs & Neckerman, 1989; Patterson et al., 1992).
Is There a Link Between Social Cognitive Processes Involving Family and Peers?
Much theoretical emphasis has been placed on cognitive structures that are thought to
provide a bridge between the quality of family and peer relations (Burks & Parke, 1996). Indeed,
there is evidence that family interaction patterns affect social information processing in nonfamily
situations. Weiss and colleagues (Weiss, Dodge, Bates & Pettit, 1992) found preschoolers
exposed to harsh parental discipline showed classic decrements and negative biases in social
information processing in kindergarten. In middle childhood and early adolescence, parents and
children tend to show corresponding social information processing styles. Coercive mother-child
dyads make mutual hostile attributional biases about the other dyad member (MacKinnon-Lewis,
Volling, Lamb, Dechman, Rabiner & Curtner, 1994). Finally, nonproblem children and their
parents hold similar goals for social situations, make similar attributions about other people’s
behaviors, and hold similar expectations for success in social situations (Burks & Parke, 1996).
Thus, parents and children seem to show corresponding social information processing styles and
Developmental perspective on anger 19
the way that children have learned to think about social episodes in the family context shows a
Finally, there is evidence for correspondence between parents’ behavior strategies for
dealing with emotional situations and their children’s strategies for dealing with peer-evoked
anger. Children of parents who provide comfort and problem solving assistance when their child
is distressed have children who show appropriate verbal assertion, low anger intensity, and the
ability to remove themselves from provocative peer situations (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994). These
children also show higher levels of moral reasoning and are more likely to report that they would
offer help to a distressed peer (Miller, Eisenberg, Fabes & Shell, 1996).
cognitive skills, and personal perspectives. Peer relations are more egalitarian. For these reasons,
theorists have stressed that peer interaction provides children with different and valuable learning
experiences, primarily in the areas of role-taking and conflict management (Larsen, Hartup &
Koplas, 1996). Because peer play alternates between calm and rough and tumble, arousing
activities, one of the main outcomes of peer play is the chance to learn to regulate one’s own
It has also been suggested that close friendship and dating relations have a particularly
special role. These relationships are uniquely voluntary and require high levels of equity and
affective investment, since participants can exit the relationship if the expectations are unfulfilled
(Laursen et al., 1996). Conflicts between close peers are more risky than those with parents,
teachers, siblings or nonfriend peers. Fights with close peers more often end in compromise or
negotiation, are more positive in tone, more likely to address each person’s needs and more often
are felt to improve the relationship (Laursen et al, 1996). Close friendships and romantic
Developmental perspective on anger 20
relationships seem to provide children with the best opportunities to learn constructive and caring
A model of the anger development process that summarizes and integrates the information
reviwed in this paper is shown in Figure 1. Although this paper did not comment extensively on
anger in the school context, the developmental model portrayed has several implications for
school psychologists, counselors, and teachers. Based on the processes specified in the model, we
make the following six recommendations for improving the content and effectiveness of school-
-------------------------------------------------
--------------------------------------------------
1. It is important to recognize that the way children understand anger (and other emotions) and
the way they react to their own and to other people’s feelings is the result of a socialization
process that started in early infancy. Because these patterns are long-standing, they may not
2. To support the child, staff may need to also understand emotional socialization processes in
the child’s family. Recognize that families show both cultural and individual differences in
their expectations of acceptable emotional expression. If at all possible, work with the family
4. Educate parents about the widespread and lasting impact they have on their children’s
emotional style and emotional skills. Teach parents how to replace coercive discipline
Developmental perspective on anger 21
practices with authoritative methods, teach effective family problem-solving skills and stress
the importance of talking about the child’s emotions and modeling constructive conflict
resolution.
5. Learn from the research on effective parenting practices. Teachers, psychologists, and others
can engage in the same strategies used by emotionally competent parents, and thereby serve as
6. Capitalize on the unique nature of close peer relations. Raise children’s awareness about the
high levels of skill and motivation exhibited in these relationships and help children learn to
transfer these skills and insights to a wider set of relationships and settings.
Developmental perspective on anger 22
References
Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. E., & Shagle, S. C. (1994). Associations between parental
psychological and behavioral control and youth internalized and externalized behaviors. Child
Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., McCoy, J. K., & Forehand, R. (1992). Contemporaneous and
longitudinal associations of sibling conflict with family relationship assessment and family
Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1992). Talk to your mother or your sibling? Developmental
changes in early family conversations about feelings. Child Development, 63, 336-349.
Burleson, B. R., Delia, J. G., & Applegate, J. L. (1995). The socialization of person-
Burks, V. S., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Parent and child representations of social
relationships: Linkages between families and peers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 358-378.
Casey, R. J. & Schlosser, S. (1994). Emotional responses to peer praise in children with
Coie, J. (1990). Toward a theory of peer rejection. In S. R. Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.),
Covell, K. C., & Miles, B. (1992). Children’s beliefs about strategies to reduce parental
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information
Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1990). Children's perceptions of the outcomes of social
strategies: Do the ends justify being mean? Developmental Psychology, 26, 612 - 620.
Cummings, E. M., Ballard, M., El-Sheik, M., & Lake, M. (1991). Resolution and
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and marital conflict: The impact of
Cummings, E. M., Ianotti, R. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1985). Influence of conflict between
adults on the emotions and aggression of young children. Developmental Psychology, 21, 495-
507.
Cummings, J. S., Pellegrini, D. S., Notarius, C. I., & Cummings, E. M. (1989). Children’s
responses to angry adult behavior as a function of interparent hostility. Child Development, 60,
1035-1043.
978-985.
Developmental perspective on anger 24
responses to expressions of anger and affection by others in the family. Child Development, 52,
1274-1282.
Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence.
Dodge, K. A., & Crick, N. R. (1990). Social information processing bases of aggressive
behavior in children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 53, 1146 - 1158.
Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., & Brown, M. M. (1986). Social
competence in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51(2,
Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. R. (1987). Hostile attributional biases among aggressive
boys are exacerbated under conditions of threats to the self. Child Development, 58, 213 - 224.
Dunn, J. & Brown, J. (1994). Affect expression in the family: Children’s understanding of
emotions, and their interactions with others. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 120-137.
Relations to children’s temperament and anger behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 138-156.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Troyer, D., Speer, A. L., Karbon, M. & Switzer, G.
(1992). The relations of maternal practices and characteristics to children’s vicarious emotional
negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior. Child
effects of negative emotion. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 115-124.
Grych, J. H., & Finchum, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: A
Grych, J. H., & Finchum, F. D. (1993). Children’s appraisal of marital conflict: Initial
Kavanaugh, K., Frey, J. & Larsen, D. (undated). Growing opportunities for parents of
Kobak, R. R., Cole, H. E., Ferenz-Gillies, R., & Fleming, W. S. (1993). Attachment and
Ladd, G. W., Price, J. M., & Hart, C. H. (1990). Preschoolers' behavioral orientations and
pattern of peer contact: Predictive of peer status? In S. R. Asher & J. E. Coie (Eds.), Peer
Laursen, B., Hartup, W. W., & Koplas, A. L. (1996). Towards undestanding peer conflict.
Lochman, J.E., Lampron, L. B., & Rabiner, D. L. (1989). Format differences and salience
effects in the assessment of social problem-solving skills of aggressive and nonaggressive boys.
Maccoby, E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child
psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 1-101). New York:
Wiley.
MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Volling, B. L., Lamb, M. E., Dechman, K., Rabiner, D., & Curtner,
McGee, R., Silva, P., & Williams, S. (1983). Parents' and teachers' perceptions of
behavior problems in seen year old children. The Exceptional Child, 30, 151-161.
Miller, P. A., Eiseberg, N. Fabes, R. A., & Shell, R. (1996). Relations of moral reasoning
and vicarious emotion to young children’s prosocial behavior toward peers and adults.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B. & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR:
Castaglia.
communication and understanding. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.),
Handbook of child psychology (5th ed.). Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development
Smetana, J. G. (1989). Adolescents’ and parents’ reasoning about actual family conflict.
Steinberg, L. (1987). Impact of puberty on family relations: Effects of pubertal status and
Steinberg, L., Mounts, N. S., Lamborn, S. D., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Authoritative
parenting and adolescent adjustment across varied ecological niches. Journal of Research on
Adolescence, 1, 19-36.
Trachtenberg, S., & Viken, R. J. (1994). Aggressive boys in the classroom: Biased
Vuchinich, S. Angelelli, J., & Gatherum, A. (1996). Context and development in family
Vuchinich, S., Vuchinich, R., A., & Wood, B. (1993). The interparental relationship and
family problem solving with preadolescent males. Child Development, 64, 1389-1400.
Developmental perspective on anger 28
Walker, L. J. & Taylor, J. H. (1991). Family interactions and the development of moral
Waters, E., Vaughn, B. E., Posada, G., & Kondo-Ikemura, K. (1995). Caregiving,
cultural, and cognitive perspectives on secure-base behavior and working models: New growing
points in attachment research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60
Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S. (1992). Some consequences of early
harsh discipline: Child aggression and a maladaptive social information processing style. Child
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Childre rearing and children’s
Figure Caption