Solomon Asch - Conformity Experiment

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

McLeod, S. A. (2008). Asch Experiment. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/asch-conformity.

html

Asch Experiment
Imagine yourself in the following situation: You sign up for a psychology experiment, and on a specified
date you and seven others whom you think are also participants arrive and are seated at a table in a small
room.

You don't know it at the time, but the others are actually associates of the experimenter, and their
behavior has been carefully scripted. You're the only real participant.

The experimenter arrives and tells you that the study in which you are about to participate concerns
people's visual judgments. She places two cards before you. The card on the left contains one vertical
line. The card on the right displays three lines of varying length.

The experimenter asks all of you, one at a time, to choose which of the three lines on the right card
matches the length of the line on the left card. The task is repeated several times with different cards.

On some occasions the other "participants" unanimously choose the wrong line. It is clear to you that they
are wrong, but they have all given the same answer.

What would you do? Would you go along with the majority opinion, or would you "stick to your guns"
and trust your own eyes?

If you were involved in this experiment how do you think you would behave? Would you conform to the
majority’s viewpoint?

Solomon Asch - Conformity Experiment


Asch believed that the main problem with Sherif's (1935) conformity experiment was that there was no
correct answer to the ambiguous autokinetic experiment. How could we be sure that a person conformed
when there was no correct answer?

Asch (1951) devised what is now regarded as a classic experiment in social psychology, whereby there
was an obvious answer to a line judgment task. If the participant gave an incorrect answer it would be
clear that this was due to group pressure.

Aim: Solomon Asch (1951) conducted an


experiment to investigate the extent to which
social pressure from a majority group could
affect a person to conform.

Procedure: Asch used a lab experiment to


study conformity, whereby 50 male students
from Swarthmore College in the USA
participated in a ‘vision test’. Using a line
judgment task, Asch put a naive participant in a room with seven confederates.
McLeod, S. A. (2008). Asch Experiment. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/asch-conformity.html

The confederates had agreed in advance what their


responses would be when presented with the line
task. The real participant did not know this and was led
to believe that the other seven participants were also real
participants like themselves.

Each person in the room had to state aloud which


comparison line (A, B or C) was most like the target
line. The answer was always obvious. The real
participant sat at the end of the row and gave his or her
answer last.

There were 18 trials in total and the confederates gave


the wrong answer on 12 trails (called the critical trials). Asch was interested to see if the real participant
would conform to the majority view. Asch's experiment also had a control condition where there were no
confederates, only a "real participant".

Results: Asch measured the number of times each participant conformed to the majority view. On
average, about one third (32%) of the participants who were placed in this situation went along and
conformed with the clearly incorrect majority on the critical trials.

Over the 12 critical trials about 75% of participants conformed at least once, and 25% of participant never
conformed. In the control group, with no pressure to conform to confederates, less than 1% of participants
gave the wrong answer.

Conclusion: Why did the participants conform so readily? When they were interviewed after the
experiment, most of them said that they did not really believe their conforming answers, but had gone
along with the group for fear of being ridiculed or thought "peculiar". A few of them said that they really
did believe the group's answers were correct.

Apparently, people conform for two main reasons: because they want to fit in with the group (normative
influence) and because they believe the group is better informed than they are (informational influence).

Evaluation: One limitation of the study is that is used a biased sample. All the participants were male
students who all belonged to the same age group. This means that the study lacks population validity and
that the results cannot be generalized to females or older groups of people.

Another problem is that the experiment used an artificial task to measure conformity - judging line
lengths. How often are we faced with making a judgment like the one Asch used, where the answer is
plain to see? This means that study has low ecological validity and the results cannot be generalized to
other real life situations of conformity. Asch replied that he wanted to investigate a situation where the
participants could be in no doubt what the correct answer was. In so doing he could explore the true limits
of social influence.

Some critics thought the high levels of conformity found by Asch were a reflection of American, 1950's
culture and tell us more about the historical and cultural climate of the USA in the 1950’s than then they
do about the phenomena of conformity. In the 1950’s America was very conservative, involved in an anti-
McLeod, S. A. (2008). Asch Experiment. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/asch-conformity.html

communist witch-hunt (which became known as McCarthyism) against anyone who was thought to hold
sympathetic left-wing views. Conformity to American values was expected. Support for this comes from
studies in the 1970s and 1980s that show lower conformity rates (e.g. Perrin & Spencer, 1980).

Perrin and Spencer (1980) suggested that the Asch effect was a "child of its time". They carried out an
exact replication of the original Asch experiment using engineering, mathematics and chemistry students
as subjects. They found that on only one out of 396 trials did an observer join the erroneous majority.
They argue that a cultural change has taken place in the value placed on conformity and obedience and in
the position of students. In America in the 1950s students were unobtrusive members of society whereas
now they occupy a free questioning role.

However one problem in comparing this study with Asch is that very different types of participants are
used. Perrin & Spencer used science and engineering students who might be expected to be more
independent by training when it came to making perceptual judgments.

Finally, there are ethical issues: participants were not protected from psychological stress which may
occur if they disagreed with the majority. Evidence that participants in Asch-type situations are highly
emotional was obtained by Back et al. (1963) who found that participants in the Asch situation had
greatly increased levels of autonomic arousal. This finding also suggests that they were in a conflict
situation, finding it hard to decide whether to report what they saw or to conform to the opinion of others.

Asch also deceived the student volunteers claiming they were taking part in a 'vision' test; the real purpose
was to see how the 'naive' participant would react to the behavior of the confederates. However, deception
was necessary to produce valid results.

Factors Affecting Conformity


In further trials, Asch (1952, 1956) changed the procedure (i.e. independent variables) in order to
investigate which situational factors influenced the level of conformity (dependent variable). His results
and conclusions are given below:

Group Size
Asch (1956) found that group size influenced whether subjects conformed. The bigger the majority group
(no of confederates) the more people conformed, but only up to a certain point. With one other person
(i.e. confederate) in the group conformity was 3%, with two others it increased to 13% and with three or
more it was 32% (or 1/3).

Optimum conformity effects (32%) were found with a majority of 3. Increasing the size of the majority
beyond 3 did not increase the levels of conformity found. Brown and Byrne (1997) suggest that people
might suspect collusion if the majority rises beyond 3 or 4.

According to Hogg & Vaughan (1995) the most robust finding is that conformity reaches its full extent
with 3-5 person majority, with additional members having little effect.
McLeod, S. A. (2008). Asch Experiment. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/asch-conformity.html

Lack of Group Unanimity / Presence of an Ally


As conformity drops off with 5 members or more, it may be that it’s the unanimity of the group (the
confederates all agree with each other) which is more important than the size of the group.

In another variation of the original experiment Asch broke up the unanimity (total agreement) of the
group by introduced a dissenting confederate. Asch (1956) found that even the presence of just one
confederate that goes against the majority choice can reduce conformity as much as 80%. For example, in
the original experiment 32% of participants conformed on the critical trials, whereas when one
confederate gave the correct answer on all the critical trials this conformity dropped to 5%.

This was supported in a study by Allen & Levine (1968). In their version of the experiment they
introduced a dissenting (disagreeing) confederate wearing thick-rimmed glasses – thus suggesting he was
slightly visually impaired. Even with this seemingly incompetent dissenter conformity dropped from 97%
to 64%. Clearly the presence of an ally decreases conformity.

The absence of group unanimity lowers overall conformity as participant feel less need for social
approval of the group (re: normative conformity).

Difficulty of Task
When the (comparison) lines (e.g. A, B, C) were made more similar in length it was harder to judge the
correct answer and conformity increased. When we are uncertain, it seems we look to others for
confirmation. The more difficult the task the greater the conformity.

Answer in Private
When participants were allowed to answer in private (so the rest of the group does not know their
response) conformity decreases. This is because there is less group pressure and normative influence is
not as powerful, as there is no fear of rejection from the group.

References
Allen, V. L., & Levine, J. M. (1968). Social support, dissent and conformity. Sociometry, 138-149.

Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgment. In H. Guetzkow
(ed.) Groups, leadership and men. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press.

Asch, S. E. (1952). Group forces in the modification and distortion of judgments.

Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous
majority. Psychological monographs: General and applied, 70(9), 1-70.

Back, K. W., Bogdonoff, M. D., Shaw, D. M., & Klein, R. F. (1963). An interpretation of experimental conformity
through physiological measures. Behavioral Science, 8(1), 34.

Longman, W., Vaughan, G., & Hogg, M. (1995). Introduction to social psychology.

Perrin, S., & Spencer, C. (1980). The Asch effect: a child of its time? Bulletin of the British Psychological
Society, 32, 405-406.

Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1953). Groups in harmony and tension. New York: Harper & Row.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy