Cyberbullying: Social Competence, Motivation and Peer Relationships
Cyberbullying: Social Competence, Motivation and Peer Relationships
XXIV, 2016 | Media Education Research Journal | ISSN: 1134-3478; e-ISSN: 1988-3293
www.comunicarjournal.com
Dr. Eva M. Romera is Senior Lecturer in the Department of Psychology at University of Cordoba (Spain)
(eva.romera@uco.es) (http://orcid.org/ 0000-0002-9414-8019)
Juan-Jesús Cano is PhD Candidate in the Department of Psychology at University of Cordoba (Spain)
(m82caflj@uco.es) (http://orcid.org/ 0000-0003-4048-4815)
Dr. Cristina-M. García-Fernández is Lecturer in the Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology
at University of Seville (Spain) (cgarcia19@us.es) (http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7088-7028)
Dr. Rosario Ortega-Ruiz is Full Professor of Psychology in the Department of Psychology at the University of
Cordoba (Spain) (ortegaruiz@uco.es) (http://orcid.org/0000-0003-2110-6931)
ABSTRACT
The recognition of some overlap between face to face harassment (bullying) and via digital harassment (cyberbullying) could indi-
cate that variables of social cognition, whose influence has been identified in bullying, also are present in cyberbullying. The aim
of this research was to determine the social adjustment of roles involved in cyberbullying and to analyze the differences in the
perception of social competence, social goals and peer support, between victims, aggressors and bully-victims of cyberbullying. A
number of 505 teenagers (47.3% girls) between 12 and 16 years old (M=13.95, SD=1.42) participated in the study. Validated
instruments for Spanish teenagers were used and psychometric properties for the adaptation of the scale of social competence
were analyzed. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis showed optimal scores of reliability and validity. The cyber-bullying
victims showed greater involvement in cyberbullying. Comparisons between roles with nonparametric tests showed that cyber-
bullies had the highest levels of peer support and popularity social goals. Cybervictims were highlighted by a high perception of
social competence. Cyberbully-victims were described by their high popularity goals and low peer acceptance. These results sup-
port the conclusion that the way in which the peer group manages its emotional and social life may be explaining the situation of
cyberbullying among teenagers.
RESUMEN
El reconocimiento de cierto solapamiento entre el acoso cara a cara (bullying) y el ciberacoso (cyberbullying) puede indicar que
variables de cognición social, cuya influencia ha sido reconocida en el bullying, también estén presentes en el acoso cibernético.
El objetivo de la investigación fue estudiar el ajuste social de los implicados en cyberbullying y analizar las diferencias en la per-
cepción de la competencia social, la motivación y el apoyo de los iguales, entre víctimas, agresores y agresores victimizados del
cyberbullying. Un total de 505 adolescentes (47,3% chicas) con edades comprendidas entre los 12 y 16 años (M=13.95;
DT=1.42) participaron en el estudio. Se utilizaron instrumentos para adolescentes validados en español y se analizaron las pro-
piedades psicométricas para la adaptación de la escala de competencia social. Análisis factoriales exploratorios y confirmatorios
mostraron índices óptimos de fiabilidad y validez. Se observó una mayor implicación de los ciberagresores victimizados. Las com-
paraciones entre roles a través de pruebas no paramétricas mostraron en los ciberagresores un mayor apoyo social que el resto
de perfiles y altos niveles en metas de popularidad. Las cibervíctimas destacaron por su alta percepción de competencia social.
Los ciberagresores victimizados mostraron altos niveles de metas de popularidad y baja aceptación social. Los resultados obteni-
dos permiten concluir que la forma en que el grupo de iguales gestiona su vida emocional y social puede estar explicando la situa-
ción de cyberbullying entre los adolescentes.
Received: 2015-12-24 | Reviewed: 2016-01-20 | Accepted: 2016-02-22 | Preprint: 2016-05-15 | Published: 2016-07-01
DOI http://dx.doi.org/10.3916/C48-2016-07 | Pages: 71-79
72
As teenagers spend more and more time together, Salmivalli, 2005; Rodkin, Ryan, Jamison, & Wilson,
the peer context becomes increasingly important in 2013). Finally, it has been shown that trying to avoid
their social lives. The technological revolution, espe- negative judgments from others often leads to a lack of
cially communication via digital devices and social net- acceptance by peers (Ryan & Shim, 2006), with vic-
works, has given rise to a fluid and almost permanent tims of bullying exhibiting greater fear of negative eva-
exchange that is often far removed from the adult luations (Storch, Brassard, & Masia-Warner, 2003).
world. It has been widely recognized that feelings of Social acceptance, a third indicator of social
group belonging, reciprocity, social competence or adjustment, refers to the degree to which students are
peer acceptance are linked to psychological, social and accepted or rejected by their peers. It involves enga-
emotional well-being during adolescence (Buhrmester, ging in positive interactions, spending time with others
1990; Parker & Asher, 1993). and having someone that provides support and well-
The work of Vaughn and colleagues has shown being. There is general agreement in the research lite-
that competent social behavior, social motivation and rature that the lack of acceptance by peers can lead to
peer acceptance constitute a multifaceted and hierar- victimization (Kendrick, Jutengren, & Stattin, 2012).
chically organized construct that explains social adjust- Although victims and bully-victims who suffer bullying
ment in peer groups (Bost, Vaughn, Washington, Cie- report less social support from peers (Cerezo, Sánchez,
linski, & Bradbard, 1998; Vaughn & al., 2009). Social Ruiz, & Arense, 2015; Holt & Espelage, 2007), it has
adjustment is defined as the degree to which an indi- also been shown that many boys and girls who are not
vidual engages in socially competent behaviors that accepted by their peers use aggression as a behavioral
provide a good fit between their behavior and their strategy in social interaction (Crick, Grotpeter, & Big-
immediate social context (Crick & Dodge, 1994). bee, 2002). However, social support has been re-
Perceived social competence is the cognitive esti- cognized in bullies, because certain peer groups or
mation of one’s skills, abilities and behaviors that ena- contexts constituted on the basis of immoral norms
ble positive development outcomes (Zhang & al., accept aggression as a way to gain acceptance within
2014). As regards bullying, it has been shown that vic- the group (Berger & Caravita, 2016).
tims have a deficit in social skills (Fox & Boulton,
2005). In contrast, bullies have been characterized as 1.1. Social adjustment in cyberbullying
having a low level of emotional skill in managing their The technological advances in recent decades
relationships effectively, but have also been recogni- have changed social interactions from face-to-face to
zed to be popular and skilled in manipulating social virtual exchanges. While this increased connectivity
situations to their own advantage (Gini, Pozzoli, & provides some social benefits for the virtual relations-
Hauser, 2011). Bully-victims, on the other hand, are hips of adolescents, such relationships are not without
those that exhibit the worst social and emotional skills risks, including cyberbullying (Fernández-Montalvo,
(Habashy-Hussein, 2013). Peñalva, & Irazabal, 2015).
Social motivation refers to the cognitive represen- Research on cyberbullying has described this phe-
tation of what people want to attain, and marks the nomenon as an indirect form of traditional bullying
direction, effort and persistence required to achieve which shares the defining characteristics of intimida-
the desired behavior (Austin & Vancouver, 1996). tion: an intentional, aggressive act carried out against a
Ryan and Shim (2006, 2008) have identified three victim by one or more perpetrators repeatedly and
types of goals: development goals, social demonstra- over time, causing an imbalance of power (Olweus,
tion or popularity goals and avoidance goals. The pur- 1999). However, when this phenomenon occurs via
suit of development goals in adolescents has been the Internet or other digital communication devices, it
associated with learning new ways of relating, perso- exhibits specific characteristics, such as anonymity,
nal growth and enhanced social outcomes, which publicity, which extends or may extend the damage
contribute to social efficacy and greater acceptance caused to a wider audience, and the difficulty of dis-
from peers (Mouratidis & Sideridis, 2009; Ryan & connecting from the cyber environment, which can
Shim, 2006, 2008). However, adolescents may also increase the vulnerability of the victims (Juvonen &
be driven by the pursuit of goals whose aim is to achie- Gross, 2008; Olweus, 2012; Smith, 2015).
ve popularity, social success and higher status within The fact that cyberbullying shares the defining
the group. Several studies have highlighted that boys characteristics of bullying has led many researchers to
and girls who seek social recognition are more likely to study the similarities and differences between the phe-
nomena. Early research gave greater attention to the involvement by bullies, victims, bully-victims and those
who like them the way they are BUT other kids have 2.3. Procedure
Comunicar, 48, XXIV, 2016
classmates who wish they were different»), with two After selecting the schools, pupils were informed
response options each («Really true for me» or «Sort of of the research aims and asked to participate in the
true for me»). Respondents are asked to choose which study. Authorization was obtained from the schools
profile best describes them and once they have chosen and the families. Emphasis was placed on the volun-
the profile they are asked to select one of the two tary nature of their participation and the confidentiality
options. The internal consistency of the scale with the of their responses.
study sample was Ω=.75. The instruments were administered to the classes
Social motivation was measured using the Spanish as a whole in their respective classrooms without the
adaptation of the Social Achievement Goals Scale (He- presence of teachers in a single, 30-minute session.
rrera-López, Romera, Gómez-Ortiz, & Ortega-Ruiz,
2016) designed and validated by Ryan and Shim (2006). 2.4. Data analysis
This scale measures three types of social goals: deve- To determine the psychometric properties of the
lopment goals (ΩMD=.78) (e.g., «In general, I strive Perceived Social Competence Scale in adolescents,
to develop my interpersonal skills»); popularity goals confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed
(ΩMA=.89) (e.g., «I want to be friends with ‘popular’ using the robust maximum likelihood method. The
people»); and avoidance goals (ΩME=.77) (e.g., «I following fit indices were used: the Satorra-Bentler
would be successful if I could avoid being socially chi-square (2S-B), the comparative fit index (CFI)
awkward»). The scale comprises a total of 12 items (>.95), the non-normed fit index (NNFI) (>.95), the
that are measured on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1= goodness-of-fit index (GFI) (>.95), the root mean
Not at all true and 5=Very true). The internal con- square error of approximation (RMSEA) (<.08) and
sistency with the study sample was adequate the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR)
(ΩMD=.82, ΩMA=.85, ΩME=.75). (< .08) (Byrne, 2006; Hu & Bentler, 1999). EQS 6.2
Self-perceived social competence was measured software was used to perform the analyses. To calcu-
using the Perceived Social Competence Scale II (An- late involvement in the cyberbullying roles, the crite-
derson-Butcher, Amorose, Riley, Gibson, & Ruch, rion of Del-Rey & al. (2015) was taken into account.
2014). This scale assesses the perception of social self- To study the mean differences in involvement in
competence by means of five items (e.g., «I show con- the cyberbullying roles, nonparametric tests were used
cern for others» or «I give support to others»). Res- (Kruskal-Wallis and Mann-Whitney U tests for pairwi-
ponses are measured on a 5-point Likert scale (1=Not se comparisons) after verifying the lack of normality by
at all true and 5=Really true). To date, no studies the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. The data were coded
have used this scale with Spanish teenagers. The and analysed using SPSS statistical software version 20.
results of the validation of the Spanish adaptation of Given the ordinal characteristics of the variables, inter-
the scale are presented in the results section. The nal consistency was analyzed based on the results of
internal consistency with the study sample was ade- McDonald’s omega (Elosua Oliden & Zumbo, 2008),
quate (Ω=.91). which was calculated using the Factor 9.3 program.
The European Intervention Project Cyberbullying
Questionnaire (Del-Rey & al., 2015) was used to mea- 3. Analysis and results
sure two dimensions of cyberbullying: cybervictimiza- The descriptive analyses of the sample indicate a
tion (α=.97) (e.g., «Someone said nasty things to me 29.7% incidence of cyberbullying. Of the total respon-
or called me names using texts or online messages» or dents, 9.9% were victims, 5.5% were bullies and
«Someone posted embarrassing videos or pictures of around 14.3% were bully-victims. No statistically signi-
me online») and cyberaggression (α=.93) (e.g., «I cre- ficant differences regarding involvement in each of the
ated a fake account, pretending to be some else» or «I roles were observed for the gender variable.
excluded or ignored someone in a social networking The results of the CFA for the Perceived Social
site or Internet chat room»). The questionnaire con- Competence Scale in adolescents were optimal (figure
sists of 22 Likert items with five response options: 1): χ2S-B=13.96; p=.01; NNFI=.971; CFI=.985;
0=No; 1=Yes, once or twice; 2=Yes, once or twice RMSEA=.059; SRMR=.27. The values of the cova-
a month; 3=Yes, about once a week; and 4=Yes, riances between items ranged from .46 to .71. The
more than once a week. The internal consistency for value of Mardia’s multivariate coefficient was 30.63.
the study sample was adequate for cybervictimization The univariate statistics for each item are presented in
(Ω=.95) and cyberaggression (Ω=.97). table 1.
among socially vulnerable boys and girls who are more relationships and take the initiative to meet others,
socially isolated and already immersed in a process of make friends and learn to get along with others, all of
face-to-face victimization and hence less able to which are related to lower peer rejection (Mouratidis
defend themselves, such social defenselessness could & Sideridis, 2009; Ryan & Shim, 2006, 2008). In this
be a prior risk factor for cyberaggression. The low paper, those who were not involved in bullying repor-
peer support perceived by cyberbully-victims may ted higher levels in the social development goal varia-
have the same explanation since, to a large degree, bles, while cyberbullies showed lower scores, thus
cyberbully-victims have similar functional characteris- confirming that cyberbullies are characterized by low
tics to those of cybervictims. The lack of peer support levels of positive social motivation or development.
and cybervictimization may intensify negative feelings, As regards the pursuit of popularity, cyberbully-
which in turn increases the risk of cyberbullying. This victims and cyberbullies were most driven by the need
is in line with previous studies, which have shown that to be socially recognized. These results are consistent
peer rejection may be a source of tension that contri- with those found for bullying, thus suggesting that the
butes to cyberbullying (Hinduja & Patchin, 2009; desire to attain social recognition leads many boys and
Wright & Li, 2013). girls to intimidate others. It should be noted, however,
Although there is little research on social develop- that cyberbullies do not harass others at random, but
ment goals in relation to cyberbullying roles, we know do so in order to strengthen their social position or
that when adolescents pursue social development marginalize opponents in a group (Navarro & al.,
goals, they find new ways of relating, enhance their 2015), which has important moral implications regar-
ding the impact of bullying and cyberbullying on the reach conclusions that more closely reflect the social
lescents and Preadolescents’ Roles on Bullying, and its Relation with Sage Publications.
Comunicar, 48, XXIV, 2016
Social Climate and Parenting Styles. Psicodidáctica, 20, 139-155. Holt, M.K., & Espelage, D.L. (2007). Perceived Social Support
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1387/RevPsicodidact.11097 among Bullies, Victims, and Bully-victims. Journal of Youth and
Crick, N.R., & Dodge, K.A. (1994). A Review and Reformulation of Adolescence, 36, 984-994.
Social Information-processing Mechanisms in Children’s Social Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cut off Criteria for fit Indexes in
Adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101. doi: http://dx.- Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New
doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74 Alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. http://dx.doi.-
Crick, N.R., Grotpeter, J.K., & Bigbee, M.A. (2002). Relationally org/10.7334/10.1080/10705519909540118
and Physically Aggressive Children’s Intent Attributions and Feelings Juvonen, J., & Gross, E.F. (2008). Extending the School Grounds?
of Distress for Relational and Instrumental Peer Provocations. Child Bullying Experiences in Cyberspace. Journal of School Health, 78,
Development, 73, 1134-1142. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467- 496-505. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-1561.2008.00335.x
8624.00462 Katzer, C., Fetchenhauer, D., & Belschak, F. (2009). Cyberbullying:
Del-Rey, R., Casas, J.A., Ortega-Ruizb, R., Schultze-Krumbholzd, Who are the Victims?: A Comparison of Victimization in Internet
A., Scheithauerd, H, Smithe, P... Plichtai, P. (2015). Structural Va- Chatrooms and Victimization in School. Journal of Media Psycho-
lidation and Cross-cultural Robustness of the European Cyber- logy: Theories, Methods, and Applications, 21, 25-36. doi: http://-
bullying Intervention Project Questionnaire. Computers in Human dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105.21.1.25
Behavior, 50, 141-147. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.- Kendrick, K., Jutengren, G., & Stattin, H. (2012). The Protective
03.065 Role of Supportive Friends against Bullying Perpetration and Vic-
Del-Rey, R., Elipe, P., & Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2012). Bullying and timization. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1.069-1.080. doi: http://dx.-
Cyberbullying: Overlapping and Predictive Value of the Co-occu- doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014
rrence. Psicothema, 24, 608-613. Kowalski, R.M., Giumetti, G.W., Schroeder, A.N., & Lattanner,
Ellison, N.B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The Benefits of M.R. (2014). Bullying in the Digital Age: A Critical Review and Meta-
Facebook ‘Friends’: Social Capital and College Students’ Use of analysis of Cyberbullying Research among Youth. Psychological
Online Social Network Sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Bulletin, 140, 1.073-1.137. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035618
Communication, 12, 1143-1168. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/- Kowalski, R.M., Morgan, C.A., & Limber, S.P. (2012). Traditional
j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x Bullying as a Potential Warning Sign of Cyberbullying. School Psy-
Elosua Oliden, P., & Zumbo, B.D. (2008). Coeficientes de fiabili- chology International, 33, 505-519. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.11-
dad para escalas de respuesta categórica ordenada. Psicothema, 20, 77/0143034312445244
896-901. Modecki, K.L., Minchin, J., Harbaugh, A.G., Guerra, N.G., &
Fernández-Montalvo, J., Peñalva, A., & Irazabal, I. (2015). Hábitos Runions, K.C. (2014). Bullying Prevalence across Contexts: A Meta-
de uso y conductas de riesgo en Internet en la preadolescencia. analysis Measuring Cyber and Traditional Bullying. Journal of
[Internet Use Habits and Risk Behaviours in Preadolescence]. Adolescent Health, 55, 602-611. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.-
Comunicar, 44, 113-120. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3916/C44- jadohealth.2014.06.007
2015-12 Mouratidis, A., & Sideridis, G. (2009). On Social Achievement
Garaigordobil, M. (2011). Prevalencia y consecuencias del cyber- Goals: Their Relations with Peer Acceptance, Classroom Belon-
bullying: una revisión. International Journal of Psychology and gingness, and Perceptions of Loneliness. The Journal of Experi-
Psychological Therapy, 11, 233-254. mental Education, 77, 285-307. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/-
García-Fernández, C.M., Romera, E.M., & Ortega, R. (2015). JEXE.77.3.285-308
Explicative Factors of Face-to-face Harassment and Cyberbullying Navarro, R., Yubero, S., & Larrañaga, E. (2015). Psychosocial Risk
in a Sample of Primary Students. Psicothema, 27, 347-353. doi: Factors for Involvement in Bullying Behaviors: Empirical Compa-
http://dx.doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2015.35 rison between Cyberbullying and Social Bullying Victims and
Fox, C. L., & Boulton, M. J. (2005). The Social Skills Problems of Bullies. School Mental Health, 7, 235-248. doi: http://dx.doi.org/-
Victims of Bullying: Self, Peer and Teacher Perceptions. British 10.1007/s12310-015-9157-9
Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 313-328. doi: http://dx.- Ojanen, T., Grönroos, M., & Salmivalli, C. (2005). Applying the
doi.org/10.1348/000709905X25517 Interpersonal Circumplex Model into Children’s Social Goals:
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hauser, M. (2011). Bullies have Enhanced Connections with Peer Reported behavior and Sociometric Status.
Moral Competence to Judge relative to Victims, but Lack Moral Developmental Psychology, 41, 699-710. doi: http://dx.doi.org/-
Compassion. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 603-608. 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.699
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.002 Olweus, D. (1999). Norway. In P.K. Smith, Y. Morita, J. Junger-
Habashy-Hussein, M. (2013). The Social and Emotional Skills of Tas, D. Olweus, R. Catalano, & P. Slee (Eds.), The Nature of
Bullies, Victims, and Bully-victims of Egyptian Primary School School Bullying: A Cross-national Perspective (pp. 28-48). Lon-
Children. International Journal of Psychology, 48, 910-921.http:- don: Routledge.
//dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.702908 Olweus, D. (2012). Cyberbullying: An Overrated Phenomenon?
Harter, S. (1985). The Self-perception Profile for Children: Re- European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 520-538. doi:
vision of the Perceived Competence Scale for Children. Denver, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.682358
CO: University of Denver. Ortega-Barón, J., Buelga, S., & Cava, M.J. (2016). Influencia del
Herrera-López, M., Romera, E.M., Ortega-Ruiz, R., & Gómez-Ortiz, clima escolar y familiar en adolescentes, víctimas de ciberacoso.
O. (2016). Influence of Social Motivation, Self-perception of Social [The Influence of School Climate and Family Climate among Ado-
Efficacy and Normative Adjustment in the Peer Setting. Psicothema, lescents Victims of Cyberbullying]. Comunicar, 46, 57-65. http://-
28, 32-39. http://dx.doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2015.135 dx.doi.org/10.3916/C46-2016-06
Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. (2009). Bullying Beyond the Schoolyard: Parker, J.G., & Asher, S.R. (1993). Friendship and Friendship
Preventing and Responding to Cyberbullying. Thousand Oaks: Quality in Middle Childhood: Links with Peer Group Acceptance
and Feelings of Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction. Develop- 15, 176-184. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1471-3802.12114