Criminal Psychology 2017
Criminal Psychology 2017
Criminal Psychology 2017
Criminal
Psychology
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
This thoroughly updated and revised new edition provides an essential overview of a
full range of psychological contributions to the understanding of criminal behaviour,
as well as the processes of dealing with offenders, and supporting victims of crime.
From the cognitive, developmental and social processes that influence a diverse
range of crimes – including burglary, fraud, rape and murder – to the challenges faced
by the police and courts in investigating crime or securing reliable testimony, the text
is packed with pedagogical features that bring this fascinating subject to life. These
include boxes highlighting key topics or issues around research methods, further reading
and suggested essay titles.
Also including chapters on rehabilitation in prisons and the psychology of victims,
the text examines hot topics such as gang membership and terrorism, as well as
discussing how the field of psychology may better understand criminals and criminal
behaviour in the future. It builds to a comprehensive and accessible introduction, an
ideal text for students across psychology, criminology and socio-legal studies
and law.
Topics in Applied Psychology is a series of accessible, integrated textbooks ideal for courses
in applied psychology. Written by leading figures in the field, the books provide a
comprehensive academic and professional overview of the subject area, bringing the
topics to life through a range of features, including personal stories, case studies, ethical
debates and learner activities. Each book addresses a broad range of cutting-edge topics,
providing students with both theoretical foundations and real-life applications.
Clinical Psychology
Second Edition
Graham Davey
Educational Psychology
Second Edition
Tony Cline, Anthea Gulliford and Susan Birch
Health Psychology
Second Edition
Charles Abraham
Criminal Psychology
Second Edition
David Canter
Forthcoming Titles:
Counselling Psychology
Victoria Galbraith
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
David Canter
Second edition
Criminal
Psychology
Second edition published 2017
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2017 David Canter
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The right of David Canter to be identified as author of this work has been
asserted by him in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright,
Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or
reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical
or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying
and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system,
without permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks
or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification
and explanation without intent to infringe.
First edition published in 2008 by Hodder.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data
A catalog record for this book has been requested
References 252
Index 283
Preface to the series’
second edition
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Psychology is still one of the most popular subjects for study at undergraduate degree
level. As well as providing the student with a range of academic and applied skills
that are valued by a broad range of employers, a psychology degree also serves as the
basis for subsequent training and a career in professional psychology. A substantial
proportion of students entering a degree programme in psychology do so with a sub-
sequent career in applied psychology firmly in mind, and as a result, the number of
applied psychology courses available at the undergraduate level has significantly
increased over the recent years. In some cases, these courses supplement core academic
areas, and in others, they provide the student with a flavour of what they might
experience as a professional psychologist.
The original series of Topics in Applied Psychology consisted of six textbooks designed
to provide a comprehensive academic and professional insight into specific areas of
professional psychology. The texts covered the areas of clinical psychology, criminal
psychology, educational psychology, health psychology, sports and exercise psychology and work
and organizational psychology, and each text was written and edited by the foremost
professional and academic figures in each of these areas.
These texts were so successful that we are now able to provide you with a second
edition of this series. All texts have been updated with details of recent professional
developments, as well as relevant research, and we have responded to the requests of
teachers and reviewers to include new material and new approaches to this material.
Just as in the first series, each textbook is based on a similar academic formula that
combines a comprehensive review of cutting-edge research and professional knowledge
with accessible teaching and learning features. The books are also structured, so they
can be used as an integrated teaching support for a one-term or one-semester course
in each of their relevant area of applied psychology. Given the increasing importance
of applying psychological knowledge across a growing range of areas of practice, we
feel this series is timely and comprehensive. We hope you find each book in the series
readable, enlightening, accessible and instructive.
Graham Davey
University of Sussex, Brighton, UK
About the author
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
This second edition is a totally rewritten and updated version, somewhat different from
the first edition. That was a team effort. For the first edition, a number of colleagues
contributed chapters, many of whom were being supervised by me for research degrees.
They have all moved on to professional careers, so I have taken the opportunity in
this edition to develop my own vision of the many ways in which our understanding
of crime and criminals could be informed by psychology.
By authoring every chapter myself, I have set out to provide a coherent perspective
on the psychology of offending and ways of dealing with it. However, I have
deliberately avoided imposing a strong, or limited theoretical structure on the work
reviewed. Although where there are well-argued criticisms of that work, I have
summarised them.
I have also attempted to cover as wide a range of relevant topics as possible, with
web links and up-to-date overviews wherever possible. The book can, therefore, be
a jumping-off point to most of the areas of crime and criminality to which psychology
is contributing. Of course, a short book like this can never cover everything that is
relevant, not least because this is a rapidly developing area of research and professional
activity. In particular, where there have been notable developments since the first
edition, for example, in the detection of deception, I have attempted to cover the latest
advances as well as space allows.
Criminal Psychology opens with a section that provides a general map of the
approaches that are taken by psychologists to understand and explain offending. The
second section reviews the processes that underlie different subsets of crimes, as diverse
as burglary, rape and murder. After these reviews of some of the fundamentals of the
psychology of crime, the third section considers the challenges that the police and
courts face in obtaining effective evidence because of the vagaries of human memory
and the more overt problems of deception.
The insights that psychological research provide for those who have to deal with
crime and criminals are reviewed in the final section. This moves from psychological
contributions to policing through the courts and into the very important involvement
of psychologists in prison systems. The often-neglected consideration of the victims
of crime provides the penultimate chapter before the final review of where the study
of the psychology of crime and criminals may be heading.
David Canter
University of Huddersfield
July 2016
Acknowledgements
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
I am especially grateful to Kathryn Hughes for the great assistance she has given me
in overseeing the production of the chapters. The contribution of professional expertise
of Magdalene Ng for Chapter 11, Donna Youngs for Chapter 12, Michael Davis for
Chapter 14, Kevin Rogers for Chapter 15 and Jonathan Ogan for Chapter 16 is also
gratefully acknowledged.
1 Psychology and the
criminal process
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
1 Recognise the range of topics that are dealt with by psychologists in the area of
crime and criminality.
2 Understand the difficulties and challenges associated with research into crime
and criminals, including practical, legal and ethical demands on study in this area.
3 Describe the various approaches to research that are utilised when studying
criminal activity.
4 Acknowledge the many different disciplines within and outside of psychology
that contribute to our understanding of criminality.
5 Summarise the dominant theoretical perspectives that are drawn on in this area.
6 Discuss the differences in approaches to knowledge that distinguish psychology
from law enforcement and other disciplines.
7 Recognise how the present book is organised from considering principles and
theories in the early chapters to more practical applications in later chapters.
8 Acknowledge the topics that forensic psychologists deal with, which are given
greater emphasis in other publications.
The wide range of psychological studies of crimes and criminals, which have emerged
over recent years, is reviewed in this book. This introductory chapter lays the
foundations for subsequent chapters, by considering the process from crime to
investigation, trial and imprisonment, then on to attempts to manage offenders in prison
and help them out of criminality. This leads to exploration of the many different
institutions and agencies that have to deal with crime and opens up the debate on the
ways in which a psychological approach may differ from other ways of considering
crimes. This includes an examination of such processes as those by which people
become criminal; the variety of ways in which crimes are enacted; how an under-
standing of criminals’ thoughts and actions can help in understanding and reducing
criminality; factors influencing witness testimony; and managing and treating offenders
2 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
once they are convicted. The differences between criminological studies of delinquency
and psychologists’ studies of rare and extremely serious crimes are highlighted. The
study of all of these topics raises challenges in gaining access to appropriate data as well
as legal and ethical demands on the researchers.
written because of the rapid technological and social changes that are influencing crime
and criminality. Perhaps more emphasis should have been given to what changes in
psychology have implications for offending, but unfortunately, psychology changes far
more slowly than society. Perhaps a younger generation, especially the lively students
who read this book, will be able to get ahead of the curve and be ready for changes
in crime before they occur.
Yet, when considering criminals and their actions, it has to be borne in mind that a very
biased sample of people is being dealt with. For example, in most places, the offender
4 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
is appprehended in less than 10 per cent of burglaries. There is, thus, an open debate
on whether the burglars who get to court or are studied by researchers are typical of
all burglars or are, possibly, the less competent ones who do not escape arrest. By
contrast, around the developed world, as many as 95 per cent of murderers are caught.
This means that studies of those who kill others may be more representative of the
range and variety of homicides than is the case for most other crimes. It also means
that there is more information on this very rare crime than for other crimes that are
far more prevalent.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
important role here in trying to help criminals who have say, addiction problems, or
need to manage their aggression.
VARIETIES OF CRIME
It is unlikely that there will be one set of psychological explanations and procedures
that apply to all criminal acts. To understand the psychology of crime, it is important
to take on board the great variety of actions that can be regarded as criminal. In every
culture, crime covers a great range of very different sorts of activities, from arson to
fraud to serial killing of strangers.
Even within a subgroup of crimes, there are big variations. Burglary can include
stealing a purse through an open window or breaking into a fortified warehouse to
steal carefully selected works of art. Fraud can be signing a cheque from someone else’s
account or a complex abuse of pension funds. Murder can be a violent emotional
outburst or a studied and prepared killing for profit. It seems unlikely that the same
psychological issues are relevant for all these different forms of law-breaking. Indeed,
it would be expected that individuals who carry out different types of crime in
pursuance of the same overt aim (e.g., obtaining funds) may be more similar to each
other than people who carry out actions that are given the same legal definition but
do them in different ways.
6 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
To further complicate matters, crime is not one objectively defined, universal set
of actions. It is the interpretations of actions that make them criminal. These inter-
pretations come from the legal and cultural context within which actions occur. Even
the most obviously offensive action of killing another person may not be criminal, for
example, when perpetrated by a soldier in a war or in defence of one’s own life. More
complex and subtle issues surround crimes such as rape, where consent is a central
factor. Even the action of theft usually is only considered criminal where intention to
steal can be demonstrated. When it comes to fraud and other crimes typically associated
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
with bureaucracies and professional work, then what is illegal in one place may be
common practice in another.
The problem of the variety of activities that can be criminal is compounded by the
lack of any strong psychological basis to legal definitions of crimes. An arson attack
carried out to hide the evidence of a crime would be classed as the same sort of crime
as that which was an act of revenge. This is a problem of particular significance when
reviewing published accounts of psychological research on crime. When studies are
carried out of the characteristics of offenders, they typically use the legal definition of
the crime for which the offender was convicted, not some subtler exploration of what
sort of actions were involved.
Because of these complications, it will become apparent in later chapters that one
of the challenges that criminal psychologists face is to get a clear account and
classification of the differences between various illegal activities. It will not be enough
to just discuss burglary, rape or even serial killing. It will always be important to
determine whether there are specific subsets of these offences that may well relate
to different psychological processes.
Identifying subsets of crimes may be quite challenging because focusing on different
aspects of crimes may give rise to very different ways of dividing them up. For example,
the popular view that ‘motive’ is an important aspect of a crime turns out to be extremely
problematic. The term ‘motive’ could be the justification a criminal has for a crime such
as revenge for a wrong done to him, or the need for money to feed her family. But, it
could also be interpreted in a more deeply psychoanalytic way, as when it is declared
that a rapist was angry with women because of the way his mother had treated him
so he was unconsciously projecting his anger with his mother on other women. Or
motive could mean some benefit the crime had for the offender, such as obtaining
recognition from other members of his gang, enjoying the excitement of getting
away with the illegal act, or carrying out what the offender considers to be a moral
duty. All these different ‘motives’ overlap with each other and could all be applied to
the same crime. Therefore, in subsequent chapters, there will often be some emphasis
on being as clear as possible about what set of actions are being considered and how
they can be distinguished from other apparently similar crimes.
Criminal psychology, therefore, relates to many and various aspects of human
activity, and as a consequence, draws on many different disciplines, as varied as cog-
nitive science and jurisprudence. The related social science disciplines that deal with
aspects of criminality and the law are drawn on to understand the context and
significance of offence behaviour. In addition, there are related areas of study that
consider the forensic traces criminals leave, whether it be where crimes occur or aspects
of the chemistry or physics of a crime. These traces can inform the understanding of
the patterns of criminal behaviour.
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS 7
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
FIGURE 1.2 Showing many of the disciplines that contribute to the study of the
psychology of crime and criminals.
EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME
It is out of the differentiation of crimes and criminals that the various forms of psycho-
logical explanations of criminality emerge. An important point to get clear from
the start is that there is no evidence that mental illness is the cause of criminality. The
complexity and variety of criminal activity indicates that it is very unlikely that crime
is produced by mental illness or, more generally, mental disorder. The emphasis that
some psychologists, such as Blackburn (1994), give to mental problems in criminals is
partly a product of their focus being on clinical populations. This has also meant that
the aspect of the examination of criminals that emphasises a clinical psychological or
psychiatric perspective has tended to be biased towards rather extreme and unusual
subsets of criminals. In essence, the people studied by clinicians have been those who
have been referred for treatment. They, therefore, are likely to be people who are
obviously mentally disturbed or whose crimes have bizarre or extreme qualities to
them, such as serial rape, or serial arson or serial homicide.
The clinical focus has produced the rather distorted picture that much of the
psychological literature on offending is devoted to violent criminals and those whose
actions are extreme and rare. In contrast, the broader criminological literature tends
to explore the actions of delinquents and those who have carried out what is often
called ‘volume crime’, such as burglary, theft and car crime. It is, therefore, not
surprising that there is a difference in perspective between criminologists and clinical
psychologists about the causes of crime because they are really talking about different
populations.
8 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
ACTIVITY 1.1
www.westsidestory.com/site/level2/lyrics/krupke.html)
Crime is, by its very nature, very difficult to study. It is rare to be able to observe it
in action, and when it does occur, there will be many pressures to keep it secret. People
who commit offences will be reluctant to admit voluntarily that they have done so
and often will go to some effort to hide what they have done. Therefore, virtually all
study of crime and criminals depends on accounts that are given after the event, often
by people who are no longer in the situation in which the crime has occurred. It is,
therefore, rather surprising that so much has been learnt about offenders and offending.
Over the years, a large number of different sources of information have been utilised
to build up a picture of crimes and criminals. This varies from detailed interviews of
offenders themselves, or their victims, often in a clinic, prison or other institutional
setting. These may be formal questionnaire studies or more qualitative discussions with
offenders about their crimes and life experiences. These self-reports have the advantage
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS 9
of direct contact with the perpetrators themselves. However, they depend on the
person’s memory of their offences, which may be weak after long periods of time and
may be affected by the desire to hide all details.
Self-reports
There are also legal and ethical issues involved both in gaining access to offenders and
in dealing with their accounts. At the most extreme, if an offender in an interview
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
gave an account of a serious crime that he had not been convicted of, or was intending
to commit, the interviewer could be regarded as an accessory to that crime if they did
not subsequently report it. There may also be aspects of a crime and its investigation
that are crucial for understanding what happened, which are deliberately kept from
public view for security or other matters of sensitivity.
If victims are the source of the information, there will often also be legal constraints
over the information that can be recorded, and great care is required in the storage
and reporting of any accounts obtained. For example, in many countries, the identity
of a rape victim must not be made public. As a consequence, there are often many
barriers to gaining access to offenders or their victims.
Self-report data will also suffer from various biases because transient, difficult-to-
find, uncooperative offenders will be underrepresented. There will be much more
emphasis on people who are literally a captive audience for the research; those already
incarcerated. This leads to an overrepresentation in the published research of more
prolific and more serious offenders. Their imprisonment itself may also distort what
they are prepared to talk about or how they will present their actions.
Self-report data from offenders, and victim survey data on offences, have been used
to supplement official records. However, many studies of crime rely heavily on official
records, even though this means the ‘dark figure’ of crime (unrecorded and undetected)
remains unknown. The collection of official statistics is not initially for the purposes
of research, but for the benefit of agency personnel, and thus, has with it inherent
advantages and disadvantages. The advantage is that it is official, and therefore, has
some authenticity. Results derived from it also have more direct relevance to policy-
makers. The disadvantages are that the sorts of information relevant to psychologists,
such as details of the characteristics of offenders, may not be recorded. Also, the official
records draw heavily on legal definitions with all the problems associated with that
discussed above. It also has to be recognised that law enforcement officers who record
details may distort them in order to massage the official figures that are presented.
nature, developments and changes over time in these crimes. However, caution always
needs to be exercised when using these sources because there will often be crucial
information that the police and security services deliberately keep out of the public
domain. They keep it secret both to protect their own sources and security, but also
to ensure that anyone who admits to the crime can be questioned to see whether they
have information about the crime that was not in the public domain, thus supporting
their confession.
Official records
Around the world police authorities and ministries of justice publish records
of the crimes that have occurred. These are readily available and are the basis
of much quantitative sociology.
Advantages
• The main advantage of these records is that they are unobtrusive in
nature, and in their collection, they do not influence directly the behaviour
of the individual.
• Their official nature also gives them significance in policy-making, which
gives added value to any study of them.
Disadvantages
• Official data are a small fraction of the actual amount of crime recorded.
• They tend to be rather general with little detail or information on
individuals or crimes of the form that is of value to psychological research.
As well, they specify the exact dates on which the offences were
recorded and make it possible to determine the ordering of offences
FOCUS 1.1
Police data
For example, victim and witness statements, police interviews, notes and
photographs taken at the crime scene and any geographical information.
12 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
Advantages
• Police data are not derived under laboratory conditions. Therefore, findings
from these studies will be high in ecological validity.
• There may be a lot of information about the details of the crime that are
not available any other way.
• The information is collected in relation to the requirements of the law, and
so, may be much more thorough than that collected as part of some forms
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
of research.
Disadvantages
• Many crimes go unreported to the police.
• Police data are collected in order to collect evidence, or for official records,
rather than for psychological research. Therefore, variables that
psychologists may be interested in will not always be recorded in an easily
extractable condition, if at all.
• Information is collected in different ways by different police forces, and
therefore, comparison is often difficult.
• In general, the police are extremely chary about making any data available
to anyone outside the police force.
Victim surveys
Researchers often use victim surveys as another data source. For example,
the British Crime Survey (BCS) is the largest adult victim survey in the UK and
is carried out annually.
Advantages
• Anonymous surveys may offer a more accurate picture of the incidence
of people being victims of crime.
Disadvantages
• Victim surveys may be subject to sample bias. Some populations are
underresearched and difficult to study (e.g., rape victims who are male or
levels of rape within vulnerable populations, such as prostitutes or
homeless people).
FOCUS 1.1 Continued
• These surveys tend to have relatively little detailed information about the
crime.
• The traumatic nature of the event itself may distort the victims’ view of events.
• No information is available about the perpetrators.
Advantages
• As many victims do not report crimes to the police, the results from
medical centres may provide a more representative sample than those
based only on recorded crimes.
Disadvantages
• Such centres are often based in urban settings. Therefore, results derived
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
from these contexts may not reflect the nature of rape in more rural
areas.
• There are very significant problems of confidentiality, which may limit
access to researchers.
• Medical centres may have different methods of clinical evaluations.
Therefore, variations in results between different centres may reflect
different protocol.
Offender interviews
Offenders can be interviewed for research purposes, usually in a clinical or
prison setting.
Advantages
• Interviews with offenders may provide a greater insight into the reasons
why they carry out their crimes or how they may try to justify their
offending behaviour.
• The findings may be useful in terms of designing an appropriate
therapeutic programme for groups or individuals.
Disadvantages
• Offenders may distort events or lie in the hope that they may get a lesser
sentence on conviction or early release from prison.
• Only known offenders, or those who are not transient, will be available for
interview.
• Access to some offenders, for example terrorists, may be extremely
difficult.
• Unless other records are provided, only the offender’s side of the story
FOCUS 1.1 Continued
will be available, and the details of the crime may be very limited.
Court reports
The proceedings of courts are usually matters of public record following
the famous saying ‘Justice must not only be done but be seen to be done’.
Increasingly, the reports of courts are available online, www.Gov.public-
records.co.uk as well as a great deal of other information about legal
processes, especially in the USA, for example, LexisNexis Academic,
or www.public-records.com.
14 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
Advantages
• The details of the crime and of the offender are available.
• Information across many jurisdictions can be found.
Disadvantages
• Only the information considered appropriate and relevant to the court case
will be recorded.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Advantages
• This information is readily available.
• It covers crimes that occur all over the world and across history.
Disadvantages
• The information contained will relate to what is newsworthy and can be
very patchy.
• The accuracy of the information always has to be checked.
• The cases selected will tend to be the unusual and sensational ones.
AREAS OF CONTRIBUTION
The numerous explorations of the psychology of crime find their way into an
increasing range and quantity of contexts. It is, perhaps, easiest to understand the
contributions psychologists are making in terms of the different sorts of institutional
settings in which they work. In each of these settings, they will be bound by different
professional demands and codes of practice. This is leading to rather different profes-
sional disciplines emerging in these diverse areas even though they share a common
requirement to understand criminals and criminality.
The area of activity for which crime psychologists are most widely known, which is
popularly referred to as ‘offender profiling’, is actually still the most unusual role for them
to take. ‘Offender profiling’ was always more of a mass media creation than an actual
psychological contribution; being much more prevalent in fiction than in fact. The
accounts that are given usually owe a great deal more to the adventures of Sherlock Holmes
than to any real scientific activity. Nonetheless, there is much that psychology can
contribute to police and other investigations and it is out of this that the area of investigative
psychology has been created, as reviewed extensively in Canter and Youngs (2008).
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS 15
Although the first writings on investigative psychology date back less than 25 years
(Canter, 1989), the area has developed and expanded very rapidly. Of particular interest
has been the emergence of a psychology of the police, especially of investigators. They
often have to face very challenging tasks where decisions have to be made under
conditions of considerable uncertainty. There is still very little known about how they
actually face up to such tasks and the sorts of people who are most able to handle them
effectively.
Once a criminal comes before a court, there is a new range of psychological issues
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
that must be dealt with. Some of these are very much within the realm of clinical
psychology when the mental state and capabilities of the defendant are at issue. But,
others relate more directly to the sorts of cognitive processes that can help the court
to understand memory processes. Psychologists take the role of expert witnesses who
can comment on the value and validity of information presented before the court, but
in most jurisdictions, great care is taken to ensure that they do not offer opinions about
the innocence or guilt of the accused. Legal systems differ from one area to the next,
and so, the particular role of the psychologist and the sorts of topics on which they
can give evidence are different in different jurisdictions.
It is probably the case that most crime psychologists ply their trade with convicted
offenders. Assessing them, particularly the risk of them re-offending, as well as
providing support and treatment. Loosely, these may be called prison psychologists,
but there are many offenders being appraised and helped in many other institutions
besides prisons. The task of these psychologists is an especially difficult one because
they have to help people be able eventually to cope with the problems of their social
environment and community. Yet, the psychologist can have no influence over that
context. However, there are some signs that a basic understanding of criminal
behaviour, how it emerges and is sustained, can give rise to productive interventions
with offenders.
It is a moot point whether the experiences of those who suffer crimes, and ways
of understanding their experiences and helping them to cope with those experiences,
should sit firmly under the heading of criminal psychology. But, as it turns out, many
offenders are themselves victims. Furthermore, a great deal can be understood about
crime by a careful consideration of who the victims are. Victims are no more randomly
distributed through the population than are criminals.
Throughout the consideration of the psychology of crime, it has been clear that it does
not exist as an abstract, independent discipline, which operates in a vacuum. At all
points, it interacts with the demands and processes of other institutions and disciplines
and the perspective and constraints of the legal system and society at large. Therefore,
one of the greatest tasks that confronts psychologists working in this area is to bridge
the gap between psychology and the other related legal disciplines. There is a variety
of valid perspectives on crime, and the challenge is to bridge the gap between them.
Central to this is the need to deal with the tendency for the social sciences to look
for causes of criminality outside of the direct agency of the person committing the
16 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
crime. The criminal’s biology, personality or upbringing are blamed for the crime, not
the offender as such. In contrast, the law and public debate puts great emphasis on the
conscious intentions of the offender. Thus, it is clear that lawyers and those involved
in the legal profession formulate the questions they consider it important to answer
about criminality in quite a different way from social scientists.
One particularly important difference between lawyers and crime psychologists
is that lawyers are looking for definitive answers that relate to specific cases. This is
very different from the general trends that characterise the conclusions reached by
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
psychologists. There are also differences in pragmatic issues that relate to the ways the
different communities go about answering these questions. The courts and investigators
look for evidence that will point to innocence or guilt. By contrast, psychologists look
for statistical significance that emerges out of scientific studies. At the heart of these
differences are very diverse attitudes towards the nature of evidence, as well as differ-
ences in the central models of what is the nature of human beings.
CONCLUSIONS
The psychology of crime covers a very wide range of human activity from the most
mundane acts of minor theft, or the daily occurrence of deception, through to the
outrages of serial killing and terrorism. It consequently connects with very many other
disciplines across the spectrum of professions and sciences. The central challenges of
the psychology of crime are, on the one hand, the access to valid, reliable, robust and
representative data about crimes and criminals. On the other hand, the challenge is to
build effective bridges with the many other perspectives for which the psychology of
crime is relevant.
This rapidly growing area of study and practice seems set to continue expanding
into ever-wider areas of research and application. This will, in part, be shaped by
development in criminality. It will also be influenced by the areas of success that
psychologists have already had, leading to the opening of doors into as yet
unseen areas of law enforcement and offender rehabilitation.
• What are the main differences between studying crime and studying criminals?
• Review the various sources of information for studying criminals with their strengths
and weaknesses.
• Could you ever study crime under controlled laboratory conditions?
• What are the main ways in which psychology can contribute to the control and
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
management of crime?
• What do you think are the main differences between legal testimony and research
data, and what implications do you think this has for the understanding of criminality?
Books
Alison, L., & Rainbow, L. (Eds.). (2011). Professionalizing offender profiling: forensic and investigative
psychology in practice. London: Routledge.
Brewer, N., & Williams, K. D. (Eds.). (2005). Psychology and law: an empirical perspective. London:
Guilford Press.
Canter, D., & Youngs, D. (2008). Investigative psychology: offender profiling and the analysis of criminal
action. Chichester: Wiley.
Canter, D., & Žukauskiené, R. (Eds.). (2008). Psychology and law: bridging the gap. Farnham:
Ashgate.
Krauss, D. A., & Lieberman, J. D. (2016). Psychological expertise in court: psychology in the courtroom
(Vol. 2). London: Routledge.
Roesch, R., & McLachlan, K. (2007). Clinical forensic psychology and law. Farnham: Ashgate.
Towl, G. J. (Ed.). (2006). Psychological research in prisons. London: BPS Blackwell.
Wrightsman, L. S., Nietzel, M. T., & Fortune, W. H. (1998). Psychology and the legal system:
4th edition. London: Brooks/Cole.
Youngs, D. (Ed.). (2013). Behavioural analysis of crime: studies in David Canter’s investigative
psychology. Farnham: Ashgate.
18 PSYCHOLOGY AND THE CRIMINAL PROCESS
Journal articles
Canter, D., & Alison, L. J. (2003). Converting evidence into data: the use of law enforcement
archives as unobtrusive measurement. The Qualitative Report, 8(2), 151–76.
Farrington, D. P. (1992). Criminal career research in the United Kingdom. British Journal of
Criminology, 32(4), 521–36.
Haney, C. (1993). Psychology and legal change: the impact of a decade. Law and Human
Behaviour, 17, 371–98.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Kazemian, L., & Farrington, D. (2005). Comparing the validity of prospective, retrospective,
and official onset for different offending categories. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 21(2),
127–47.
Monahan, J., & Walker, L. (1988). Social science research in law: a new paradigm. American
Psychologist, 43, 465–72.
Otto, R. K., & Heilbrun, K. (2002). The practice of forensic psychology: a look toward the
future in light of the past. American Psychologists, 57, 5–18.
Websites
The following website gives a descriptive overview of investigative psychology:
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Investigative_psychology
The following website gives information about a career in forensic psychology:
http://study.com/articles/Criminal_Forensic_Psychologist_Career_Info_Duties_and_
Requirements.html
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
1 criminality
Explanation of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
2 Biological
explanations
of crime
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you will be able to:
SUMMARY
There is a multiplicity of explanations of why crimes occur that focus on aspects of the
individual. Some of these attempt to explain crime as being based in human
evolutionary origins and having roots in a person’s neurology and physiology, which
has roots in the person’s genetic makeup. Other theories, which focus on the
individual, deal more directly with the person, including their personality and cognitive
processes. These explanations do not necessarily assume a firm biological basis to criminal
actions, although many attempts are being made to relate the psychology to
neurobiology and genes. So, although popular discussions still set ‘nature’ against
‘nurture’ as two opposing explanations of criminality, current thinking sees them
interacting in what is often called a ‘biosocial’ explanation. However, the current chapter
focuses on the biological explanations, which have a long tradition in psychology.
There are many different attempts to explain criminality by reducing it to some sort
of biological basis. At their most fundamental, these explanations seek a genetic cause
for a person becoming an offender. This is the idea criminals are ‘born, not made’.
22 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
A somewhat less mechanical view is that there are aspects of human evolutionary
history that predispose people (especially men) to be violent. In a curious way, these
two views are rather in conflict because genetic explanations imply variations between
individuals, but evolutionary explanations imply that certain characteristics (such as
walking upright) will have become dominant for the species.
Other biological explanations look for hormonal aspects of the person, or com-
ponents of brain functioning, as the causes for criminal actions. As will become clear,
there are a variety of critiques of all these explanations.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
These sorts of explanations are called reductionist because they imply that complex
human interactions can be reduced to basic biological causes. They are often also
referred to as deterministic because they imply that the actions of people can be
determined from an identifiable number of specific causes, external to a person’s
own agency. Such explanations assume that the biological basis for human activity can
operate independently of the milieu in which that activity emerges. It is clear that
cannot be the case. Actions need a person and a context. Therefore, all current
explanations of crime accept a mixture of nature and nurture, in which they interact
with and modify each other, called socio-biological explanations. However, for clarity,
this chapter focuses on the individualistic explanations, and the next chapter deals with
more psychologically oriented ones.
NINETEENTH-CENTURY DETERMINISM
The determinist belief that human beings were essentially biological organisms was
greatly influenced by Charles Darwin’s The Descent of Man. It was argued that if humans
are just evolved animals, then they are no more than their bodies. Consequently,
variations in their characteristics ought to be reflected in variations in their bodies.
Therefore, the argument went, there ought to be features of the body that indicate
aspects of the person who inhabits that body.
The most well-known illustration of this in the nineteenth century was phrenology.
Initially put forward by Franz Gall, this purported to be a science that allowed a
phrenologist to determine a person’s characteristics from the shape of their skull,
and in particular, the protrusions on the skull. It was not a big step from this to claim
that whether a person was a criminal or not could be gleaned from bodily features.
One particularly popular version of this was the idea that a person’s propensity
towards criminality was revealed in the face, known as physiognomy. Johan Lavater,
among others, produced extensive catalogues of faces to show what different types of
criminals looked like.
The idea that a person’s appearance showed how evolved they were as human beings
was drawn on by Cesare Lombroso to claim that criminals were less-evolved human
beings. His proposal was that those who followed the laws of society were more
civilised, and thus, more highly developed. In 1895, he published his influential book
L’Homme Criminel Atlas, which contained sixty-four pages of pictures of what criminals
would typically look like. This book laid the foundations for what became known as
forensic anthropology. Lombroso drew attention to many aspects of people, which he
claimed indicated propensity to various forms of criminality. These included physical
abnormalities, such as asymmetrical skull, large ears, flattened or crooked nose, fat lips,
BIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 23
enormous jaws and high cheekbones. These ideas were widely believed until Goring’s
(1913) important study. He compared students and criminals showing there were no
differences at all in their physical or facial features. However, Lombroso’s ideas are so
powerful that they still linger on in some places even today.
As with most subsequent attempts to seek the causes of criminality in the offender’s
biology, one fundamental problem with all these early attempts is the assumption that
criminality is a characteristic of the person. They ignore the fact that some actions are
not regarded as criminal in all jurisdictions, as mentioned in Chapter 1. They also ignore
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
other influencing factors such as the opportunities for crime. Furthermore, when
classifying offenders as certain types of criminals, say as a robber or a rapist, they assume
that people only ever commit just one type of crime. All the evidence shows that most
criminals who carry out a number of crimes are versatile to some extent. Therefore,
the idea of claiming, for example, that a rapist will have a certain look and a robber
a different look runs into difficulties if a person commits both types of crimes.
However, these early ideas, rooted in evolutionary theory, were useful in drawing
attention to the significance of characteristics of the individual in understanding the
causes of crime. In particular, they emphasise what it is that a person is born with
that gives rise to their criminality. They were the first serious scientific theories to take
the stand that some individuals are more likely to become criminals than others. This
was assumed to be because of what they are born with, whether innate, being their
genetic inheritance or congenital. That is, factors present at birth, which include
influences of the uterine and perinatal environment. This challenged the popular
superstition that criminals are possessed by evil spirits. A view that still surfaces when
particularly nasty criminals are described as monsters or ‘evil’.
CONSTITUTIONAL THEORIES
The belief that criminals could be distinguished by their physical features did not fade
away completely after systematic studies discredited them. Much more carefully
constructed assessments of body type were carried out and related to criminality. The
most notable of these was Sheldon’s (1941) constitutional types. He described three
basic body types, which he believed were correlated with particular types of personality:
• thin and bony ectomorphs, who were introverted and restrained.
• large and heavy endomorphs, who were sociable and relaxed.
• broad and muscular mesomorphs, who were aggressive and adventurous.
He argued, after studying criminals, that they were more mesomorphic and less ecto-
morphic. This claim was supported by Glueck and Glueck (1956) who found that 57
per cent of their criminal sample were mesomorphic and 16 per cent ectomorphic,
compared with 19 per cent and 33 per cent, respectively, of controls. However,
whether these body types were a product of criminal lifestyles or the contexts from
which the samples were taken and not related to the biological causes of criminality
is open to question.
So, although the idea that it is possible to determine whether a person is a criminal
or not from what they look like has been shown to lack any support, there are aspects
of issues, such as disfigurement and ‘minor physical anomalies’ (MPAS), that have been
24 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
found to have weak correlations with delinquency. Masters and Greaves (1969)
showed that 60 per cent of 11,000 prisoners had facial deformities compared with 20
per cent in a non-criminal population. Agnew (1984) reported that schoolchildren rated
as unattractive self-reported higher delinquency. Thompson (1990) estimated that, in
any given year, there are 250,000 disfigured offenders in US prisons. Again the question
has to be asked whether the criminal lifestyle causes deformities or the deformities set
in motion a way of dealing with others that leads to criminality. There is also the
recurring problem of dealing with criminals as a homogenous mass, rather than many
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
GENETIC INHERITANCE
Family studies
The idea behind studies of families is that if criminality is inherited, criminal families
will tend to produce criminal children. The basis for this argument is the commonplace
finding that it is indeed the case that when one or more parent has been convicted of
crimes, then it is often the case that the children will also be convicted. Furthermore,
other members of the family also have a higher probability of being criminal than the
population at large. It has been recognised for centuries that there are criminal
families, rather than just criminal individuals. One widely cited set of findings that
illustrates this comes from the Cambridge Study of Delinquent Children. Osborn and
West (1979) showed that about 40 per cent of the sons of criminal fathers were
criminals themselves compared with 13 per cent for sons of noncriminal fathers.
Subsequently, West (1982) reported that boys with a criminal father were twice as
likely to become delinquent as those with noncriminal fathers. It should be noted,
however, that not all children of criminal families become criminal, and not all
criminals come from criminal families.
BIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 25
The prevalence of criminal children with criminal parents does not inevitably mean
that the relationship is due to criminal genes being passed on from generation to
generation, as was believed in the nineteenth century. Low income, large families and
social and psychological factors within the family, such as parents encouraging their
children to commit crimes, transmitting delinquent values and activity (Hollin, 1989)
are more likely causes. Although family studies point to the significance of domestic
circumstances in giving rise to criminality, more subtle investigations are essential to
determine to what extent genetics may be involved.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
FIGURE 2.1 The Kray Twins; Was it nature or nurture that led to these men controlling
London crime in the 1950s and 60s. How was it that only one of these
identical twins was diagnosed with schizophrenia?
26 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
It has to be emphasised though that the conclusions of these studies are not without
their challenges. Two problems in particular face anyone carrying out such studies.
One is that the distinction between MZ and DZ is not as simple as it may seem.
Gradations of identicalness exist. MZ twins develop from the splitting of a single egg
at the time of conception, and so, share a very high proportion of the same genes.
Yet, there is the possibility for random variations and different influences in the womb
that can make them slightly different. Consequently, they may not be totally identical.
The other challenge is that there are environmental factors that identical twins often
share, which are less common in fraternal twins. By contrast, DZ twins are like any
other siblings. They may share about 50 per cent of their genes. The influence of
genetic factors is, therefore, taken to be indicated by the difference between how similar
the MZ twins are when compared with how similar the DZ twins are.
Twin studies are, therefore, used in an attempt to determine the relative influence
of environment or genetics. Any findings for twin studies are inconclusive in the
absence of data on twins reared apart. Critics argue that similarities of appearance of
MZ twins means a closer relationship, more similar treatment by parents, and therefore,
similar interests, behaviour and social responses. As a consequence, it may be these
social processes that give rise to the concordance, rather than genetics.
Many researchers have pointed out that one further step towards unravelling the
confounding variables that confuse studies of the influence of genetic processes is to
compare MZ twin pairs who have been raised separately with those who have been
brought up in the same family. The argument here is that if identical twins reared apart
still show high concordance rates, then this must be explained by reference to genetic
factors. As Joseph (2003) points out in an extensive review of genetic factors in
psychology and psychiatry, there have not really been any major studies that make such
comparisons.
One reason for this lack of major studies is that there are very few twin pairs who
are separated at birth, and of those who are, relatively few will have developed criminal
records. Case studies of individual pairs of twins reared apart do sometimes show
dramatic similarities, but without knowing the frequency of such similarities, it is always
possible that they come to notice because they are so unusual. In one of the few studies
relevant to criminality, which did consider thirty-two MZ twins reared apart since
shortly after birth, Grove et al. (1990) measured a variety of ‘subclinical manifestations
of antisocial problems’. They found some concordance that indicated genetic influences
on both childhood conduct disorders and adult antisocial behaviours. However, the
former only accounted for about 16 per cent of the variations between individuals.
The adult figure was less than 8 per cent. So, although this does show some genetic
influence for aspects of the individuals that may be a precursor to criminality, they
suggest that environmental factors may be much more powerful.
BIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 27
The study of twins is usefully complemented by looking at people who have been
adopted, and therefore, brought up by people who are not their biological parents.
This helps to separate out the influence of genetics from the influence of family
environment. This may not be as straightforward, though, as may seem at first sight.
The whole process of adoption and indeed how people come to be adopted into
families that are criminal, or adopted from families that are criminal, raises many
questions about the social processes involved that may influence what the adopted
person does.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Crowe (1974) carried out one of the first studies of criminals who had been adopted.
A sample of adopted children whose biological mothers had a criminal record were
compared with those whose mothers had no criminal record. Almost 50 per cent of
adoptees who had criminal mothers had a criminal record by 18 years of age. Only
about 5 per cent of the adoptees whose mothers had no criminal record had been
convicted for a criminal offence. In a much larger study, Hutchings and Mednick
(1975) examining over 1,000 adoptees found that 21.4 per cent of sons became criminal
when the biological father was criminal, 11.5 per cent when the adoptive father
was criminal and 10.5 per cent when neither father was criminal. If both fathers
had criminal records, the percentage of adoptees with criminal records was higher at
36.2 per cent. Mednick, Gabrielli and Hutchings (1984) in their study with a sample
of 14,427 adoptees also showed that the percentage of criminal adoptees is higher when
both fathers are criminal (Table 2.1). All these studies imply a modest genetic
contribution to whether people become criminal and show that interaction between
genetic and environmental factors is crucial.
It is also to be expected that the weighting of these factors will vary for different
types of crimes. The ways the factors interact will also complicate any conclusions.
Rhee and Waldman’s (2002) important review is a good illustration of what it is
possible to conclude and the complexities of such studies. They examined fifty-one
twin and adoption studies that considered a wide range of antisocial activities. They
tried to discriminate between the influence of environment and genetics on individuals
and siblings as well as family processes. They also considered factors that moderate those
influences. These include methodological issues, such as how the DZ or MZ nature
of the twins was determined. They also considered the interesting moderation of the
age of the people being studied. The, perhaps, unexpected result of many studies is
that, as twins get older, they are more likely to be similar to each other. (Although as
mentioned above, Grove et al., 1990, found the opposite!) So, any studies of younger
delinquents may well get different results from those studying older people.
Overall, Rhee and Waldman’s (2002) review does lend support to some role for
genetic inheritance on whether a person becomes delinquent or not. Estimates of the
TABLE 2.1 Percentage of sons criminally convicted (Mednick, Gabrielli and Hutchings,
1984).
Convicted biological parents
Convicted adoptive parents Yes No
Yes 24.5 20.0
No 14.7 13.5
28 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
power of this influence are made in many different ways. But, broadly speaking, results
suggest that between a third and a half of the variations between people who become
criminal and those who do not may be derived from aspects of their genetic makeup.
So, although there are some indications of a genetic component to breaking the
law, and antisocial behaviour, especially in juveniles, the mechanisms by which
the genetics have their influence is far from clear. As Rutter and Giller (1983) put
it, ‘The question of just what is inherited remains unanswered; it is unlikely to be
criminality as such’ (p. 179). In other words, even though some, such as Beaver et al.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
(2007), have suggested that it is possible to identify genes that relate to conduct disorder,
this is still a long way from explaining what the processes are that give rise to criminal
actions. To get further with these explanations requires more detailed consideration
of the person, rather than just their genetics.
CHROMOSOMAL ABNORMALITIES
A fundamental problem with twin and adoption studies is that the genetic influence
has to be inferred from within a complex mix of environmental factors and interactions
between the people and their surroundings. A much more direct genetic link can be
claimed if some aspect of a person’s genetic makeup can be observed directly.
Examining a person’s chromosomes would seem to offer the possibility of observing
the genetic causes directly. The chromosome is a single, long molecule of DNA, which
contains the genetic material that influences many of the individuals’ biological
characteristics. Humans normally have forty-six chromosomes in pairs – forty-four
autosomes (that determine the shape and constitution of the body) and two sex
chromosomes (that determine the gender); XX for females and XY for males. A variety
of chromosomal abnormalities can exist. Of particular interest is condition 47,XYY,
in which men have an extra Y chromosome.
This extra Y chromosome in males has been linked to above average height,
borderline intelligence and violent criminal behaviour. Sandberg, Koepf, Ishihara and
Hauschka (1961) were the first to associate the XYY syndrome and violent crime.
Witkin et al. (1976) examined 4,591 men for the presence of the extra Y chromosome
and identified only twelve cases. He also found that these men were more likely to
be involved in crime (41.7 per cent), but curiously, not violent crime. Stochholm,
Jensen, Jull and Gravholt (2012) carried out a thorough study of XYY men and found
that the overall risk of being convicted for sexual abuse, burglary and arson was
significantly increased when compared with a control group. But they pointed out
that the increased risk of convictions may be explained by the poor socioeconomic
conditions related to the chromosome aberrations. However, once again, not all the
individuals with XYY syndrome are criminals, and the vast majority of criminals do
not have any chromosomal abnormality.
BIOCHEMICAL EXPLANATIONS
Distinct from genetic influences are those biological aspects of a person that may in-
fluence their criminality. Many aspects of a person’s biology may emerge during
BIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 29
pregnancy, the birth process or after birth and be influenced more by environmental
factors than genetics. The most obvious example is the abuse of drugs and alcohol.
These factors may have genetic roots or be modified by aspects of a person’s genes,
but for simplicity, it is helpful to consider them in isolation of complicating factors.
In general, it is also important to keep in mind that biological explanations tend to
relate to emotional reactions, and therefore, are more likely to be drawn on when
considering violent crime than purely property-related crime.
The most frequently cited biological explanations refer to hormones. These are
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
NEUROLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS
et al. (2000) found on average there was a small, but significantly lower amount of
prefrontal grey and white matter in the brains of those with antisocial personality disorder
(see Chapter 3) when compared with those who had not carried out violent acts. This
reduction in the implied effectiveness of the prefrontal cortex does accord with other
studies, showing that on average violent individuals do have some aspects of their brains
that tend to be different from others. That should come as no surprise. The question
though still remains as to why that is and what it implies other than that, indeed, people
with extremely aggressive personalities are somewhat different from others.
As Canter (2012) argued in some detail, trying to understand what is going on in
a person’s mind by measuring the surface electrical currents is a little like trying to
understand how a car engine works by seeing which parts heat up under different
driving conditions. More focused explorations looking at the details of brain activity
and the network of connections within the brain may offer more precise indications,
but there is still the challenge of being clear what sort of criminal is being studied and
relating brain function directly to human activity and experience.
Slightly more productive have been studies where there is known brain damage
due to complications around the time of birth, head injuries, tumours or infections.
For example, Bryant, Scott, Tori and Golden (1984) found that of the 110 people
they studied in prison, violent offenders tended to have serious neuropsychological
deficits. Those classified as brain damaged had a significantly higher rate of violent
criminal activity than did the non-brain-damaged group. Yet, here again, it is not
general criminality that is typically influenced, but aspects of social and emotional
interactions, notably aggression and antisocial behaviour.
There are many case studies that illustrate how brain damage can be associated with
dramatic personality and behavioural changes. Such brain damage, though, is always
associated with other forms of trauma that relate to life-changing circumstances, so,
even if there is a causal relationship, it may well be confounded by these other changes
in the person’s circumstances. It is also worth noting that there are reports that brain
damage has been associated with the sudden emergence of special skills as well as
extreme violence. Of course, there will always be correlates of brain activity with
human actions and experience, but the effect of any particular brain damage for any
particular person is likely to be difficult to predict.
As Beaver (2009) explains in some detail, there are a number of ways in which a
person’s biology, especially their genetic makeup, interacts with their experiences,
BIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 31
broadly captured in the term ‘environment’. One is that genes only have any influence
when they exist in a particular environment. Gene-environment interactions capture
the process by which genes only have effects when they are paired to certain environ-
ments, and vice versa (Beaver, Wright & DeLisi 2008). An example would be when
a biochemical imbalance that relates to aggressiveness occurs in a social context that
condones violence. Similarly, in such an environment, those prone to violence are
more likely to express that.
A rather different gene-environment relationship is when genetic factors contribute
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
to selecting, shaping and modifying the environment (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). An
example might be of a person of low intelligence with little support in school drifting
away from school into opportunist criminality. The genetic background helps to
predetermine the environment a person experiences.
CONCLUSIONS
An attempt to link criminality to genetic makeup, biology or hormones is the focus of the
present chapter. This must be an oversimplification on a number of counts. Crimes are
hugely diverse in their nature, so, explanations for one sort of crime, such as murder,
may be very different for another, such as burglary. Furthermore, most studies have
relatively weak findings, or are inconclusive, and reveal wide ranges of scores on any
measures. Thus, although there are, without doubt, aspects of the makeup of people
who commit crimes that, in very broad terms, distinguish them from the population at
large, these biologically based explanations of crime cannot be the whole story.
In general, most explanations of crime are what are known as ‘biosocial‘, bringing
together the nature of the individuals and the environments in which they grow up and
live. It would be difficult to find a hard and fast, fundamentalist, biological determinist
explanation of crime today. All experts will sit somewhere along a continuum from
emphasising biology to giving weight to environment. But, within that framework, there
is still considerable room for considering psychological explanations, the focus of the
next chapter.
Books
Beaver, K. M. (2009). Biosocial criminology: a primer. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and behavior: nature-nurture interplay explained. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Durrant, R., & Ward, T. (2015). Evolutionary criminology: towards a comprehensive explanation of
crime. London: Academic Press.
For a very readable and influential account of the evolutionary origins of human aggression,
read On Aggression by Konrad Lorenz (1966). London: Methuen.
A book that challenges the general use of biological explanations for psychological and social
phenomena is Canter, D. and Turner, D. (Eds.). (2014). Biologising the social sciences. London:
Routledge.
Journal articles
Rose, N. (2000). The biology of culpability: pathological identity and crime control in a bio-
logical culture. Theoretical Criminology, 4(1), 5–34.
Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., & DeLisi, M. (2008). Delinquent peer group formation: evidence
of a gene X environment interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(3), 227–44.
Websites
The following website gives a brief overview of the history of biological explanations of crime:
http://sociology.about.com/od/Deviance/a/Biological-Explanations-Of-Deviant-
Behavior.htm
The following website describes the physiological aspects of criminals’ brains:
www.livescience.com/13083-criminals-brain-neuroscience-ethics.html
This website gives an account of the neuroscience of psychopathy:
http://dujs.dartmouth.edu/2013/11/neurocriminology-the-disease-behind-the-crime/#.
Vznf0o-cHIU
3 Psychological
explanations
of crime
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
1 Know about the variety of mental disorders that are relevant to criminality.
2 Appreciate the role of mental disorder in some criminal behaviour.
3 Be aware of the various cognitive distortions characteristic of criminals.
4 Understand how theories of personality are applied to criminality.
5 Appreciate the contribution of offender’s personal narratives to understanding
criminality.
SUMMARY
psychology. There are some general trends of interest, but the variety of criminal
activity makes definitive correlations problematic.
A generally productive approach is to explore more directly the thought processes
and styles of personal interaction that are typical of offenders. There is considerable
research to show they do have characteristic cognitive distortions. More recently, this
has been developed to consider the personal narratives of offenders and how this relates
to their crimes.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
were persecuting him, and that at the time of the offence he had lost control
and was unable to resist his delusions. McNaughton was found Not Guilty by
Reason of Insanity (NGRI) and was among the first patients transferred to
Broadmoor where he remained until his death 22 years later. However, the
original decision gave rise to the McNaughton rules (West & Walk, 1977).
PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 35
Studies of mental disorder in criminals are almost entirely based on assessments of those
in prison. Prison is the only place where there is a literally captive population that
can be subjected to tests and interviews which will indicate their mental state. This
consequently raises the question as to whether incarceration caused or aggravated
their psychological condition (an issue that will be returned to in Chapter 15). This
point is supported by Ly and Foster (2005), who found 21 per cent of mentally dis-
ordered offenders were admitted to hospital from prison. A further possibility is that
their imprisonment was due to their mental disorder. It could also be the case that their
criminality has some roots in their mental problems. Sorting out these different
processes is not easy.
There is little doubt that there are high rates of mental disorder among those in
prison. Estimates for the UK range from 10 per cent to 80 per cent depending on
the prison and exactly how mental disorder is defined (Singleton, Meltzer, Gatward,
1998). This study was based on those in prisons, but there are a subset of people who
pass through the courts and are imprisoned in what are known in the UK as ‘special
hospitals’, or ‘high-security hospitals’. There are three of these in England, known as
Ashworth, Broadmoor and Rampton. They house people who are regarded as having
severe mental illness and are a danger to the public. The euphemism of their sentence
is that they are held ‘at her majesty’s pleasure’. This means that they can be imprisoned
in a ‘special hospital’ until they are deemed safe to allow out. Consequently, many
notorious killers are held in these places until they die.
Although a very small proportion of convicted criminals are sent to the secure
hospitals, there is still a high level of mental disorder among prisoners in the UK. The
government acknowledged some years ago ‘we continue to imprison too many people
with mental health problems’ (Home Office, 2006b, p. 26). This is not a problem unique
to Britain. Fazel and Danesh (2002) found similarly high rates of mental disorder among
prisoners in their meta-analysis of sixty-two surveys from twelve countries.
admitted from court or prison (National Health Service, 2006). Nonetheless, mentally
disordered people may be responsible for proportionately more crimes than their
numbers warrant. This is illustrated by the fact that people with schizophrenia and
psychopathy have been found to have higher rates of convictions for violence and
general criminality (Hodgins et al., 1996; Hare, 1999, 2003; Fazel, Grann & Psych
2006) than those without such disorders.
However, as Silverstein et al. (2015) make clear in an extensive review of the
relationship between schizophrenia and violence, the relationship is a complex one.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The increased risk of violence in those diagnosed with schizophrenia is due to a number
of factors, some of which are due to the psychosis and some of which are related to
having other mental disorders at the same time, or even associated demographic factors.
The most important risk factor related to psychosis is the presence of untreated
psychotic symptoms, especially persecutory ideation and the Capgras syndrome, in
which a person believes that a person (or even a pet) close to them has been replaced
with an identical-looking imposter.
In addition, substance abuse, antisocial personality disorder (discussed later) and
mood symptoms also increase the risk of violence. However, anxiety and negative
symptoms both appear to reduce violence risk. Other non-diagnostic clinical risk factors
include hostility, poor social skills, a prior history of violence and a history of being
physically or sexually abused.
Therefore, there are some crimes for which mental illness plays a role. Some people
with a psychotic disorder, such as schizophrenia, offend in response to their actual symp-
toms (i.e., delusions and/or hallucinations) of their illness. This process, called ‘psychotic
drive’ by Smith (2000), usually results in serious offences being committed (Taylor, 1985).
For some, this may be due to the persecutory nature of their delusions (Taylor et al.,
1996) and/or the tendency to perceive the world as threatening (Arseneault et al., 2000a).
The ‘symptom approach’ to explaining why some schizophrenics offend has been
applied to other crimes, including sexual offending (Smith & Taylor, 1999) as well as
property crimes, breaching probation and ‘mischief ’ (Hodelet, 2001). Other research
focusing specifically on sexual offending has highlighted the ways in which psychosis
may reduce inhibitions (Phillips, Heads, Taylor & Hill, 1999). The illness undermines
a person’s normal inhibitions and leaves them unable to look beyond their immediate
desires to the nature and consequences of their impulsive actions (Craissati & Hodes,
1992).
It is worth remembering that one of the origins of legal considerations of psycho-
logical characteristics of violent offenders was the case of McNaughten in 1843 (see
Focus box 3.1). It was the recognition that insane people could not be held responsible
for their actions that led to the development of laws that took account of mental illness.
PERSONALITY DISORDER
A rather curious framework for thinking about criminality that has its roots in
psychiatry and is, therefore, often considered a form of mental illness is that a person’s
personality can be disordered. The various forms of such disorders are given labels
as if they were medically recognised illnesses in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders (DSM) of the American Psychiatric Association, being revised from
PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 37
Personality disorders
People who have no obvious mental illness, but have difficulty managing
their emotions and relating to other people in ways that are acceptable
within their culture are described as having something wrong with their
‘personality’. They persistently and pervasively are found to have dealings
with others that are generally regarded as problematic. There are many types
of personality disorders, but the one most obviously associated with
criminality is psychopathy. But, people with other recognised personality
disorders are also often involved in various forms of offending.
Learning disability
These are people who are especially vulnerable or may not understand
the implications of their activities because of an impairment of intelligence
38 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
that came about before the person’s brain was fully mature. This continues
throughout their life and reduces most aspects of the person’s
functioning.
Intoxication
Abuse
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Dependence
Paraphilias (disorders of sexual preference)
Fetishism
Exhibitionism
Voyeurism
Paedophilia
Sadomasochism
time to time. It is now in its fifth incarnation (DSM 5). The important point is that
these disorders do not imply any of the mental states associated with actual mental
illness, such as schizophrenia or depression, mentioned earlier.
DSM defines personality disorder as:
Psychopathy
The most widely considered personality disorder of particular relevance to criminality
is psychopathy. This was given emphasis by Cleckley (1941), but its study in recent
years has been dominated by the work of Hare (2003).
The significance of psychopathy in contributing to offending is not surprising. It
can be viewed as the result of the basic features of this disorder, for example, callous-
ness, impulsivity, egocentricity, grandiosity, irresponsibility, lack of empathy, guilt or
remorse (Hare, 2003). Compared to other offenders, psychopaths begin offending at
an earlier age, commit a wider variety of crimes, pose serious management problems
while incarcerated and violate parole and/or re-offend sooner once released (Hare,
2003).
The relationship between psychopathy and violence is important. Psychopathic vio-
lence appears more instrumental, dispassionate and predatory. Further, psychopathic
violence appears motivated by factors such as greed, vengeance, anger, retribution or
personal gain (Hare, 1999). Several of these aspects are present when psychopaths
sexually offend. For example, along with opportunistic impulsivity, anger/hatred and
PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 39
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
FIGURE 3.1 The image of mental illness still has the stigma that goes back to early
mental asylums. How relevant is this to understanding the relationships
between mental disorders and criminality?
sexual deviancy (i.e., sadism) are important motivating factors among psychopathic
rapists (Brown & Forth, 1997; Greenall & West, 2007). Also, sexual homicides com-
mitted by psychopaths have been found to be more gratuitous and sadistic, with victims
tending to be strangers (Porter et al., 2003). Psychopathic offending, however, does
not result from a deluded mind, but as Hare states, ‘from a cold, calculating rationality
combined with a chilling inability to treat others as thinking, feeling human beings’
(1993, p. 5).
with most people falling in the middle range; comparatively few people fall at the
extremes.
Eysenck suggests that: ‘In general terms, we would expect persons with strong anti-
social inclinations to have high P, high E, and high N scores’ (1977, p. 58). He believed
that certain personality types were inclined towards crime, and these individuals could
be described as neurotic extraverts, sensation seekers, characterised by high levels of
anxiety and depression, and aggressive, cold and impersonal behaviour (Eysenck &
Gudjonsson, 1989). Offenders have been found to score highly on all three dimensions,
although this finding is not consistent.
What is particularly interesting about Eysenck’s theory is that he argues that certain
biological propensities, notably limited response to conditioning, and social aspects of
upbringing combine to generate personalities that are vulnerable to becoming criminal.
But, although this is an elegant and tidy theory backed up by neat experiments,
subsequent studies have produced a variety of inconsistent results (e.g., Levine &
Jackson 2004). Presumably, this is because of the great mix of types of criminal studied
and the different socio-economic contexts in which they exist.
Caspi et al. (1994) and Agnew et al. (2002) showed that there was empirical evidence
for the relationship between experiencing high negative emotionality and low
constraint and being engaged in crime. The plausible assumption here is that negative
emotionality may lead to crime because such people see the world as a threatening
and hostile place. The low constraint means that they will act on their impulses.
More focused studies of criminals have drawn attention to particular characteristics that
distinguish them from non-criminals. There are a great many such studies, so they are
mentioned here very briefly.
Thinking styles
A number of researchers have proposed that it is the thinking patterns of offenders
that are the foundation of offending. Walters (1995) developed a measure of these:
Psychological Inventory of Criminal Thinking Scales (PICTS). It was derived from
the fascinating writing of Yochelson and Samenow (1976). In their clinical work with
offenders, they concluded that those who commit crime often seek out excitement
and engage in reckless thrill-seeking. This is fostered by their belief that they are unique.
But, they are also disingenuous and manipulative, yet have difficulty in coping with
life’s challenges. Of course, these ideas overlap with many others. They are not that
dissimilar from the idea of psychopathy, or some of the other proposals relating to
personality disorder. The difference is in the emphasis placed on cognitive processes.
It is difficult, if not impossible, to change a personality, but how a person thinks can
be modified.
The sorts of cognitive distortions that have been identified are:
• Hostile attribution bias (Dodge, 1986): They see aggression and violence where there
is none.
• Lack of empathy (Sarason, 1968): The inability to see a situation from the perspective
of others or to respond to their feelings.
• Lack of self-control: Impulsivity and inability to delay reward. This is much more
likely to be present in younger offenders, or substance abusers. It does not
characterise a person who spends months carefully planning a bank raid or a
murder.
• Poor cognitive skills: Difficulties in problem-solving, lacking the ability to make
appropriate judgements in difficult social situations and to generate a range of
solutions to social problems or ambiguous social experiences (Freedman et al.,
1978; Higgins & Thies, 1981).
• Neutralisation and attributional processes: Excuses or rationalisations such as
– denial of responsibility (one’s actions are a consequence of external factors,
such as poverty, broken home or drunkenness),
– denial of injury (little harm is entailed), denial of victim (victim deserves it),
condemnation of the condemners (critical of criminal justice system),
– appeal to higher loyalties (e.g., peers) (Sykes & Matza, 1957).
42 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
Self-control
Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) proposed a general theory of criminality built around
the argument that offenders have low self-control and limited ability to delay
gratification. This also relates to the idea that criminals are often impulsive (Joliffe &
Farrington, 2009). Although when it comes to violent crime, an interesting distinction
has been made by Megargee (1966) between ‘over-controlled’ and ‘under-controlled’
offenders. The former explode after an apparent life of quiet conformity, while the
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
latter have a ‘short fuse’ and can become aggressive very readily.
As ever, these theories relate to some criminals, and not others. There is little doubt
that some teenage delinquents are impulsive and find it difficult to delay gratification.
But, a bank robber who spends months planning the perfect heist, or the murderer
who thinks carefully about how to kill and get away with it, can hardly be said to
have low self-control.
Machiavelianism
Prince Niccolò Machiavelli published his book The Prince in 1532. It gave guidance
on how to be an effective ruler and even discussed the methods of murder used
by Italian dukes. But, although this was regarded as the basis of political science, it
has many guidelines about how to manipulate others. In 1967, Christie and Geis
(reviewed in 2013) suggested that those who acted in ways similar to those proposed
by Machiavelli were exhibiting a particular personality trait, which they called
Machiavellianism, being a person who takes advantage of ‘the weaknesses, failings, and
foibles of others for his own purposes’ (p. 167). They developed a scale to measure
how Machiavellian a person was, which has been shown to give high readings in many
criminals (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). The concept does overlap with aspects of
personality disorder, such as narcissism and psychopathy, but nonetheless, has distinct
qualities.
CHARACTERISTICS OF OFFENDERS
Age
There are two recurrent findings in all studies of criminality and delinquency, which
stand out above all others. One is that the great majority of crimes are committed by
males. The second is that the great majority of these are in their mid-teens. As Smith
(2006) reports in a study that reflects many earlier finding: ‘Offending peaked around
the age of 14 among both boys and girls.’ He goes on to point out that after the mid-
teens, there is a ‘fairly steep and steady decline in the proportion of young people
involved in broad delinquency among both boys and girls. Among those who
continued to be involved, the amount of offending also declined.’ (p. 1).
Further, Smith (2002) elegantly summarises the implications of the many studies of
the typical age of criminals:
The emergence of crime and its unfolding across the life course raises many
questions about what the underlying processes may be that give rise to crime. There
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
must be some aspect of maturation that is having an influence, but it is also the case
that societies are so structured that what is possible for people to do, the contacts
they have and the expectations others have of them also vary considerably as people
emerge from childhood into adolescence and on into young adulthood. Therefore,
many theories of criminality focus on the life experiences of offenders and the
opportunities offered to them to learn criminality. Many of the social processes
discussed in Chapter 4, such as labelling and differential association, relate most
strongly to particular stages in an offender’s life.
When criminals are studied as a group, especially juvenile offenders, three broad
aspects of their characteristics emerge that they are more likely to have than non-
offenders. As Andrews and Bonta (2010) summarise it, these are impulsivity, risk-taking
and aggressiveness. These are a combination of belief and attitudes embedded in the
way the person interacts with others.
ACTIVITY 3.1
Find one of the many autobiographies written by gangsters or other people who
have committed crimes. Review the sorts of explanations and justifications they
give for their crimes. Do these owe more to some sort of mental disorder, the
types of cognitive processes discussed in this chapter or the social processes
discussed in Chapter 4?
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Low intelligence
Many of the claims for deficiencies in cognitive abilities, whether it be inability to
see the consequence of one’s actions, the lack of empathy or the lower moral develop-
ment, could all be seen as part of some general intellectual weakness on the part of
offenders. This is supported by a number of studies that have revealed that the average
criminal is below average intelligence. However, the findings are, as so often in studies
of criminals, not as strong as might be expected. It has been found, for example, that
youngsters with a higher than average level of intelligence are less likely to get caught.
Certainly, academic failure is associated with increased risks of delinquency (Hirschi,
1969). Adult offenders are more likely to have a history of low educational attainment
than the population at large.
PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 45
PERSONAL NARRATIVES
All the aspects of the criminals’ psychology, discussed above, deal with components
that are treated independently of each other. They also have the curious quality of
somehow abstracting the person from the criminal. It is not some feature of how
the offender makes sense of the world and the decisions they make to commit
crimes that is at the core of these explanations, but some property of the criminal’s
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
CONCLUSIONS
Aspects of the psychology of individual offenders are dealt with in this chapter. This
includes some forms of mental disorder, personality characteristics and distortions in
cognition. Although research suggests only a minority of mentally disordered people
offend, many offenders have a mental disorder. However, how this disorder interacts
with their crimes varies. In some cases, though, the disorder may have developed in
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Books
One of the first systematic studies of criminals was by Havelock Ellis (better known for his
work on human sexuality). His 1901 book The Criminal is still worth reading and his now
available in the Elibron Classics series from Adamant Media Corporation.
A useful book on mental illness aspects of criminality is Herschel Prins’ (2010) Offenders, Deviants
or Patients. London: Taylor and Francis.
PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 47
Journal articles
Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: a clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal Justice
and Behaviour, 23, 25–54.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Hart, S. D. (1998). The role of psychopathy in assessing risk for violence: conceptual and
methodological issues. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3(1), 121–37.
Hodgins, S., Mednick, S. A., Brennan, P. A., Schulsinger, F., & Engberg, M. (1996). Mental
disorder and crime: evidence from a Danish birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry,
53(6), 489–96.
Sahota, K., & Chesterman, P. (1998). Sexual offending in the context of mental illness. Journal
of Forensic Psychiatry, 9, 267–80.
Taylor, P. J. (1985). Motives for offending among violent and psychotic men. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 147(5), 491–8.
Websites
This website gives information regarding the link between mental illness and general offending:
www.apa.org/news/press/releases/2014/04/mental-illness-crime.aspx
The following website gives details of the links between violence and mental health:
www.health.harvard.edu/newsletter_article/mental-illness-and-violence
The following website gives a critical account of crime and personality:
www.studentpulse.com/articles/377/crime-and-personality-personality-theory-and-
criminality-examined
This webpage gives a full reference list with links to articles relating to the PCL-R, the PCL:SV,
the PCL:YV and other Hare scales:
www.hare.org/references/#h
4 Social explanations
of crime
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
Although social influences on criminality have always been recognised, it was Edwin
H. Sutherland’s (1947) proposal that criminal behaviour is learnt in interaction with
others, which laid the formal groundwork for current thinking. Although often
presented as a sociological or criminological theory, his arguments can be seen as
essentially social psychological. He proposed that learning to be a criminal includes
the techniques of committing crime as well as motives, rationalisations and attitudes.
In particular, the theory emphasises that it is the learning of favourable attitudes towards
violation of the law, rather than unfavourable attitudes that supports criminality.
Sutherland also proposed that the earlier, longer lasting, greater frequency of the
SOCIAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 49
exposure to attitudes and actions favourable to violation of the law, the more likely a
person would become criminal. This influence is increased if the interaction is with
others who are important or with whom the individual has a closer relationship.
An interesting and important aspect of Sutherland’s theory is that criminals are not
seen as having different needs or values from non-criminals. It is how they interpret
and act on those needs and values that distinguishes them. This contrasts with the
biological and many of the psychological explanations reviewed in earlier chapters.
Those explanations suggest there is something radically different about criminals.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The view that both behaviour and the attitudes, and conceptualisations that support
those actions, are learned, as well as the appropriateness of particular actions in given
situations (Smith & Paternoster, 1987; Coleman & Norris, 2000), requires a more
detailed consideration of how that learning occurs. A considerable contribution to
understanding criminal learning processes was made by Akers (1985). He drew on
behavioural psychologists’ studies, especially what is known as ‘operant conditioning’.
This is most closely associated with the work of Skinner (1953), much of which was
based on many different experiments training pigeons to peck for food.
Operant conditioning stresses the influences of environmental consequences on
behaviour. Thousands of studies show that behaviour is acquired through positive
reinforcement (reward) or the withdrawal of the reward (negative reinforcement). But,
its development through experiments with animals in a purely ‘behaviourist’ tradition
gave rise to many challenges to its applicability for humans. This eventually led to the
search for a more cognitive approach to understanding learning. This allowed for
people making sense of the patterns of reinforcement they were receiving, and being
50 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
able to learn through observing the actions of other people. Although initially
formulated by Rotter (1954), social learning theory is more generally associated with
the American psychologist Bandura (1976) who showed that much behaviour is initially
acquired by watching others, who he called ‘models’.
In agreement with Sutherland, according to Bandura, the more significant and
respected the models, the greater their impact on what people do. Observational
learning takes place primarily in the family, in the prevalent subculture and through
cultural symbols, such as television and books, all of which form part of the social
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Whether individuals will refrain from or commit a crime at any given time
(and whether they will continue or desist from doing so in the future) depends
on the past, present and anticipated future rewards and punishments for their
actions. (p. 66)
In a nutshell, if a person gets away with a crime and benefits from it through getting
approval from friends, a pleasant feeling, money or even food, then that reward will
increase the chance of repeating the action, unless some punishment or fear of
punishment intervenes.
Akers (1999) also draws attention to imitation. This is the carrying out of actions
after seeing others doing the same thing, especially if they benefit from their behaviour.
This relies on the salience of the models that are to be imitated, which, of course,
depends on the person’s assessment of those models. However, Akers’ studies emphasise
that this imitation is of particular importance in the initial acquisition and carrying out
of the criminal actions. The continuation of criminality is likely to rely on other
processes beyond the initial imitation.
Social learning theory, though, is not without its critics. For example, it has been
suggested that adolescents become delinquent and then seek out like-minded associates
(Sampson & Laub, 1990) ‘birds of a feather flocking together’. However, although
this is true in some cases, there is far more evidence that delinquency follows associa-
tion with delinquent peers, rather than preceding it (e.g., Menard & Elliott, 1994).
FAMILY INFLUENCES
From the earliest studies of criminality, it was recognised that crime commonly runs
in families. Initially, this was taken by people, such as Lombroso, to be evidence for
the inheritance of criminal propensity (discussed in Chapter 2). However, more careful
examination of what happens in families and how they may or may not influence
offending behaviour provides more support for a social learning interpretation of family
processes.
A number of studies have drawn attention to how the relationships between the
parents (especially the mother) and their children influence delinquency (Jensen, 1972;
McCord, 1991). Most obviously, this influence is present if the parents have criminal
convictions. In the Cambridge study, 38 per cent of boys who became delinquent had
a parent with a criminal record, compared with 15 per cent of non-delinquents
SOCIAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 51
(Farrington & West, 1990; Rowe & Farrington, 1997). The fact that not all delinquents
had criminal parents does indicate, though, that parenting is not the only cause of
delinquency.
PARENTING STYLES
Some particular aspects of families have been found to correlate with delinquency. For
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
example, West (1982) showed that delinquents were more likely to come from families
with four or more children. Of course, this could be, as Farrington (1991) suggests,
because such families are more likely to live in poor and overcrowded homes, which
may result in family disorganisation and material deprivation. However, a social
learning hypothesis would be that there is a greater possibility of exposure to delinquent
siblings in large families. Especially if limited contact with parents is such that children
rely more on their siblings as models of appropriate social behaviour.
This shows the complexity of disentangling the sources of reinforcement and social
learning. Child-rearing practices can support or reduce the impact of other influences.
When parenting is lax, neglectful, erratic, inconsistent, overly harsh or punitive, it is
predictive of adolescent delinquency. For instance, Jackson and Foshee (1998) found
a relationship between how well parents responded to the child when they were
needed (which they termed ‘responsive parenting’) and a parent’s inclination to have
rules and expectations (termed ‘demanding parenting’) and the development of antis-
ocial behaviour. More responsive and demanding parents were associated with lower
levels of delinquency in their children.
The idea that the relationships between parents and children can take many forms
has been systematised by considering what Hoffman (1984) called ‘child-rearing
styles’. The three styles are indicated in Table 4.1. Delinquent families are more likely
to use power assertion style. Parents of non-delinquents use more direct engagement
with their children, explaining the implications of their actions and threatening the
withdrawal of love. In general, physical punishment, inconsistent punishment, poor
parental monitoring, poor discipline and lack of family cohesion are found to be linked
to violent delinquency.
In relation to the consideration of parenting styles, the debate over the influence
of growing up in divorced or single-parent families on the likelihood of becoming
involved in crime should be mentioned. Over half a century ago, Bowlby (1944),
following Freudian ideas, emphasised the power of a breakdown in maternal attach-
ment as a cause of delinquency. Subsequent studies revealed that children from
single-parent families are more likely to engage in antisocial behaviour (Bank, Forgatch,
Patterson & Fetrow 1993). Overall, there seems to be a modest relationship to the
absence of a biological parent on delinquency. But, this may not be due to parent-
ing styles, but to a combination of problems associated with single-parent families,
such as the traumas of divorce, distress, poverty and mental health problems of the
parents. This possibility is supported by Rutter’s (1971) study showing that the loss of
a parent through death does not have the same association with delinquency as a
‘broken home’.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The changes in society, in which single-parent households are much more common,
create the likelihood that the relationship between single-parent families and
delinquency is much weaker than found in these earlier studies. Indeed, the existence
of ‘blended families’ and many different domestic lifestyles in Western societies make
it increasingly difficult to say what a ‘broken home’ is.
GANGS
Youngsters from unsupportive families, especially in their mid-teens, who drift towards
criminality, are likely to commit delinquent acts in groups and with co-offenders who
live close to each other (Farrington & West, 1990). This sort of group offending is
more common for nonviolent than for violent crimes. Most adult offenders offend
alone. Nonetheless, peer pressure alone cannot explain all juvenile crime. This was
shown by Agnew (1990) when he asked 1,400 adolescents what had led them to engage
in crime. Their answers indicated that peer influence was only one factor, the others
being a rational choice to obtain money or excitement. Violent crime can also result
in part from anger or provocation.
When groups of youngsters regularly associate with each other, they may be
regarded as a ‘gang’. The label itself is pejorative and is open to many meanings, with
no clear definition. This has not limited the amount of research exploring how gangs
operate and the nature of their influence, such as the seminal work of Short and
Strodtbeck (1965).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
In general, gangs are particularly prevalent in the United States and that is where
most of the research has been conducted (Miller, 2001). These studies show that the
nature of gangs varies enormously. Not all are involved in crime either. Yet, their
provision of a social learning context for offending is well established (Gordon et al.,
2004)
When reviewing all the explanations for criminality in this and previous chapters, the
question arises as to why more people do not commit crimes. Of course, many crimes
go unreported and many people do not get caught for their illegal activity (especially,
for property crimes), but still it is necessary to explain what stops people offending.
Hirschi (1969) argues that youths refrain from committing deviant acts because of
a bond to conventionality and conformity. They accept social control. Whenever the
social controls, or the bonds to society, are weakened, deviance is likely to occur (Reiss,
1951). After all, a major purpose of the law is to reduce or prevent illegal actions. The
evidence for the consequence of weak controls is revealed whenever there are riots,
looting and public disorder. Once again, the impact of social learning is also indicated
here, as looters follow the examples of others.
The idea that there are social controls requires that the individual accepts those
controls, requiring self-control and a conscience in which the norms of society are
internalised. Challenging these ideas as too subjective, Hirschi (1969) argued these aspects
of self-control implied an attachment to others. His theory, therefore, emphasises
bonding, rather than simply control. The main forms of bonding are given in Table 4.2.
The weakness of a social bond can only partially account for deviant behaviour.
Because everyone is exposed to some form of temptation, some account is needed of
how the bonds with society develop or break down. Are special motivations needed
to encourage people to deviate from acceptable social norms? Social control theorists
think not. They claim that criminal acts are natural consequences of human tendencies
to seek pleasure and avoid pain. Their commission is opportunistic, becoming more
likely when people lack self-control, as discussed in Chapter 3. They are seen as giving
in to temptations as happens with other socially disapproved acts (e.g., drug abuse,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
One direct explanation of why most people do not break the law is the view that the
certainty, severity and speed of punishment are what deter people. This, after all, is
the basis of Western criminal law. There is both a specific deterrence when individuals
are caught and punished and a general deterrence when it is widely known that
punishment for crime is very likely (Blackburn, 1993; Cusson, 2001). This general
deterrence is enhanced when it is taken up by informal social networks. That is
particularly important because the swiftness, severity and likelihood of formal
punishments are not always forthcoming. For example, in most jurisdictions, only about
one in ten burglaries lead to a conviction. Under such odds, potential criminals may
not see the law as a deterrent.
Consequently, the impact of legal sanctions is only going to be effective if there is
some consideration by an offender of the potential risks of committing a crime. Many
crimes are impulsive and involve very little consideration of the consequences.
Furthermore, if legal sanctions are perceived as being haphazard, their credibility is
diminished, decreasing the likelihood of them acting as a general deterrence. Also,
a person’s willingness to risk punishment will depend on their circumstances, which
will influence how they perceive the ‘costs’ and ‘benefits’ of breaking the law. Once
again, their social context through differential association will shape how they interpret
the possibilities and consequences of legal sanctions.
LABELLING THEORY
a person influences how they regard themselves. This idea has its roots in what is
known as symbolic interactionism (Mead, 1934). Cooley as early as 1902 captured this
perspective in the notion of the ‘looking glass self ’, in which a person’s self-identity
is a reflection of others’ conceptions of that person. It may be others in the peer group
or the legal process that provides this mirror.
A deviant person is, thus, someone to whom the label ‘deviant’ has successfully been
applied. In particular, the label may be assigned by those individuals who make the
social rules from a position of power (i.e., police officers, judges, psychiatrists, etc.)
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
(Coleman & Norris, 2000). Within this perspective, deviance is not seen as a quality
of the act committed, but as the application of rules and sanctions. The approach
particularly focuses on official reactions to delinquent acts (Sampson, 2001). But, how
these acts are interpreted may be influenced by the characteristics of offenders (i.e.,
age, race, gender or class), rather than by characteristics of the offence (Cusson, 2001).
While the original deviant activity may be carried out for various reasons, further
acts are sustained by adaptation to the stigmatising reactions to the primary deviance.
The initial act of norm violation (primary deviance) is largely incidental because it is
only a problem when labelled as such. Therefore, the delinquent career is sustained
by official labelling and sanctioning processes (Blackburn, 1993; Sampson, 2001)
through a self-fulfilling prophecy (Cusson, 2001). In effect, this is the view that a young
person will say, ‘If you are going to call me a delinquent, I will act like one’.
A significant influence of labelling theory is the attempt to keep youngsters out of
the court process. The idea is that appearance in a juvenile court will increase the self-
fulfilling prophecy and increase the probability of future criminality. However,
although there are clearly deleterious effects of juvenile court appearances, such as
reduced access to further education and employment, this is not the whole story.
Studies such as those by Smith and Paternoster (1990) show that the relationship
between court appearance and future delinquency results from the fact that youngsters
with a higher risk of recidivism in the first place have an increased likelihood of being
referred for court processing. They are, therefore, more likely to receive the delinquent
label. The question is, therefore, whether the label creates a new social identity or just
confirms a deviant identity already there (Cusson, 2001)?
So, although there is no strong evidence to indicate labelling causes continued
criminality, nonetheless, it does point to the unintended consequences of legal controls.
In particular, when certain subgroups in society are more likely to be labelled criminal,
this can create an atmosphere and set of stereotypical attitudes to those subgroups that
can reinforce other processes. Differentiation between groups may be influenced by
social labels, furthering the influence of members of those groups on their member’s
self-identity.
The great majority of offenders are male. This raises the fundamental question of why
so few women engage in deviant activity. It might have been thought that consideration
of gender differences in criminality should have been in the previous chapters on
individualistic explanations of crime. Yet, there are so many possible social correlates
of gender that these considerations sit better in the present chapter.
56 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
All of the social psychological theories explored here have relevance to gender
differences. For example, child-rearing practices are likely to be different for boys than
for girls (e.g., Lytton & Romney, 1991). Boys are more likely to differentially associate
with deviant others, especially in the context of gangs. There are also possibly
differences in attitudes to rule breaking that distinguish boys from girls.
However, it is also possible that the theories considered in this and earlier chapters
are biased to explanations of male offending. Most studies of crime have been
conducted with male samples (Smith & Paternoster, 1987; Goodkind, Ng & Sarri,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
2006) because of the disproportionate number of men who are criminal. Thus, it could
be argued that theories of deviance are only appropriate for understanding young men.
Without more detailed study of female offenders, the possibility of different
explanations for their offending remains an open question. The theories that have been
put forward for female involvement in deviant behaviour have generally drawn on
qualitatively different sets of explanatory factors. These have typically emphasised
aspects of personal maladjustments and psychological disturbance, often brought on
by experiences in the family. For instance, Smith and Paternoster (1987) argue that
female delinquency is due to the inability to adjust to a poor home situation. Any such
explanations are complicated by changes in the role of women in society. These are
doubtless – also reflected in changes in their criminality.
CRIMINAL DEVELOPMENT
A person’s social interactions and the opportunities for contacts outside the family
change as the person grows and develops. Therefore, it is not surprising that the
occurrence and nature of criminality change over the life course. Studies of these
changes that explore the activities of offenders who commit crimes throughout their
lives are often referred to as examinations of ‘criminal careers’, (Blumstein, Cohen &
Farrington, 1988; Farrington, 1992; Svensson, 2002). The term ‘career’ is not really
appropriate though because what is studied is usually just change, or lack of it, in
criminality over time. It does not usually imply a training phase, graduation and
eventual supervisory and on to management roles, with ultimate retirement, as is usually
understood by a ‘career’. It is simply a shorthand term to cover the sequence of crimes
committed by one individual over their life.
Two aspects of the length and intensity of a criminal ‘career’ have emerged from
a variety of studies (Stattin & Magnusson, 1991; Piquero & Buka, 2002; Svensson,
2002; Piquero, Brame & Lynam, 2004; Kazemian & Farrington, 2005).
1 life-course persistent antisocial behaviour
2 adolescent-limited antisocial behaviour
Life-course persistent antisocial individuals exhibit a changing manifestation of anti-
social behaviour from their early years. Moffitt (1993) argues that this type of behaviour
has roots in both earlier neurological deficits and in exposure to poor parenting and
parents offering models of antisocial behaviour (Moffitt, 1993; McCabe, Hough, Wood
& Yeh, 2001). These two risk factors aggravate each other’s impacts. Neurological
deficits can lead to difficult temperaments, which, in turn, leave the child especially
vulnerable to poor parenting, and poor parenting can then lead to poorer neurological
SOCIAL EXPLANATIONS OF CRIME 57
When the word ‘gang’ is mentioned, most will associate it with a male-
dominated group; yet, there are increasing numbers of females who are
becoming a part of the gang culture – some will form ‘girl only’ gangs, while
others will join the ‘traditional’ male gangs.
Female gangs stem from the same reasons that the male gangs develop –
economic deprivation, peer pressure, excitement, money, drugs, friendship,
self-affirmation, fear, threats and intimidation. The gang ‘family’ promises to
provide for their members with all the things they want or need or believe
that they lack in their lives, as well as providing an escape from what they
may be experiencing in their own family homes (Moore, 1991).
Female gang members have been documented as participating in all of the
criminal acts and violence associated with their male counterparts, but they
commit fewer violent crimes and are more inclined to committing property
crimes and status offences (Moore & Hagedorn, 2001). Female gang
members are no longer just ‘appendages’ of male gangs, and have been
developing their own gang culture and ‘taking care of their own’.
Many aspects of female gang members’ lives and the functioning of
FOCUS 4.1
development (McCabe, Hough, Wood & Yeh, 2001). From this point of view, the
causal factors for life-course persistent offenders are most likely to be located early in
their childhood and support the continuity of their antisocial behaviour throughout
their lives (Moffitt, 1993).
On the other hand, those individuals with adolescent-limited antisocial behaviour,
as the name suggests, begin their offending and antisocial behaviour in adolescence
and desist once they reach young adulthood. This group of offenders makes up a large
58 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
percentage of all young criminals. Moffitt (1993) argues that youngsters who engage
in criminal activity are immature. They engage in antisocial behaviour in order to gain
access to adult privileges. They model themselves on peers who have seemingly gained
autonomy from their parents through such activities. This type of adolescent-limited
antisocial behaviour will cease to occur during early adulthood as they gain adult auto-
nomy (McCabe, Hough, Wood & Yeh 2001).
Moffitt predicts that life-persistent offenders will partake in a variety of types of
crimes especially those that are more violent and aggressive and victim-oriented.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
SOCIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS
As made clear in the opening chapter, the focus of the present book is on psychological
processes. But, in the context of social explanations of crime, it is important to mention
briefly the wide range of sociological theories of what causes and sustains crime. A very
short list of the main theories is presented in Table 4.3. The library shelves (and Wiki-
pedia) are filled with discussions of these issues, so they are listed here merely as pointers
to be followed up. Akers and Sellers (2008) present a good overview of these theories.
The central argument of these theories is that it is processes within the society that
generate criminal activity, rather than characteristics of individuals. Those who commit
crime just happen to be in a particular social context that makes them more likely to
offend. So, for example, strain theories explore the mismatch (or strain) between the
aspirations that society encourages and the difficulties of achieving these, crime
resulting from attempts to cope with the strain.
The different sources of strain are explored in these theories. Some of them overlap
with the considerations in conflict theories, which emphasise the difference between
those who control society, making its laws and enforcing them, and those who suffer
the effects of this control. The inherent conflict in this situation fosters attempts to
resolve that conflict through offending.
to understanding crimes against women and how those crimes are dealt with.
These sociological theories are the obverse of the biological explanations presented
in Chapter 2. Similar to those explanations, they have a determinist focus seeking to
base the causes of crime in processes over which the individual has little control. They,
therefore, suffer from the same weaknesses of having difficulty explaining why all those
who experience social conflict, economic deprivation or are male do not commit
crimes. There can be little doubt that these sociological theories have some validity,
but the extent of their applicability is open to serious debate.
CONCLUSIONS
It is apparent from the explanations reviewed in this chapter that there are many social
psychological processes that contribute to a person becoming a criminal. But, these
explanations have to be put alongside the biological and psychological explanations in
previous chapters. When all of these explanations are reviewed together, it becomes
clear that no single explanation is adequate in accounting for all crime, especially as
there are many types of crimes and many types of offenders. Current theorising,
therefore, attempts to develop multi-causal models that combine biological,
psychological and social factors as explanations of criminal behaviour, sometimes
called psychosocial models (e.g., Jones, 2008).
• Why do you think most crimes are committed by males in their mid-teens?
• Identify two social factors that might account for criminal behaviour.
60 EXPLANATION OF CRIMINALITY
Books
Akers, R. L., & Sellers. C. (2004). Criminological theories: introduction, evaluation, and application,
4th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing.
Akers, R. L. (2011). Social learning and social structure: a general theory of crime and deviance.
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Akers, R. L. (1999). Criminological theories. Chicago, IL: Fitzroy Dearborn.
Bartol, C. R. (1999). Criminal behaviour: a psychosocial approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Blackburn, R. (1993). The psychology of criminal conduct. Chichester: Wiley.
Brandt, D. (2006). Delinquency, development, and social policy. New Haven, CT: University Press.
Cassel, E., & Bernstein, D. (2007). Criminal behaviour. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Feldman, P. (1993). The psychology of crime. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Harrower, J. (1998). Applying psychology to crime. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Hollin, C. (1989). Psychology and crime: an introduction to criminological psychology. London:
Routledge.
Journal articles
Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: a routine activity
approach. American Sociological Review, 588–608.
Paternoster, R., & Iovanni, L. (1989). The labeling perspective and delinquency: an elaboration
of the theory and an assessment of the evidence. Justice Quarterly, 6(3), 359–94.
Kruttschnitt, C. (2013). Gender and crime. Annual Review of Sociology, 39, 291–308.
Websites
The following website gives a very basic overview of the main sociological theories of crime:
http://sociology.about.com/od/Deviance/a/Sociological-Explanations-Of-Deviant-
Behavior.htm
This website gives an overview of gender and crime:
http://law.jrank.org/pages/1250/Gender-Crime-Differences-between-male-female-
offending-patterns.html
This website considers gangs and offending, as well as intervention and prevention:
www.merton.gov.uk/health-social-care/publichealth/jsna/cyp-maternal-health/
vulnerable-cyp/youth-offending-gangs-violence-jsna.htm
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
2
differences
Psychological
between crimes
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
5 Acquisitive crime
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
SUMMARY
There are two broad offence categories that have somewhat different psychological
features. One covers all those crimes that are focused on the acquisition of property.
These include burglary, theft and various forms of fraud. They are dealt with in
the present chapter. The second broad category includes all those crimes directed at
people, which includes violent and sexual offences, as well as homicide. There is much
more extensive study of the psychological issues in crimes against the person, so,
although property crime is the most common – often referred to by law enforcement
agencies as ‘volume crime’, there are more chapters in this book dealing with violent
crimes.
Crimes that focus on getting someone else’s property do take many different forms.
Breaking into a house and stealing something is, perhaps, the most ‘traditional’ of these,
but many types of frauds also need to be considered. These are growing rapidly as it
becomes possible to fraudulently steal in virtual reality, rather than on the street. The
psychological variations in these crimes relate to the characteristics of the offenders,
their skills and attitudes as considered in previous chapters.
64 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
for financial gain? Various forms of fraud and crimes, such as tax evasion and kid-
napping, are generally considered property crimes as well. There is also a range of
property crimes that do not involve direct financial benefits, where there is a transfer
or destruction of property. These vary from petty vandalism to major arson. But,
because property is the target, they are considered in the present chapter. The dis-
tinguishing psychological feature is that the target of the crime is not a person, as such,
but something physical.
Burglary
Burglary is usually very disturbing for victims and often has significance far beyond
the value of the property stolen. It is experienced as an intrusion and a violation. This
is relevant to understanding the psychology of the burglar. There is undoubtedly an
interpersonal element to burglary. A home is usually considered to be a sanctuary and
stronghold. Intrusion of this highly personal space is felt by some victims as a violation
second only to rape.
In an early study of the reasons for burglary, Chappell (1965) claimed that the main
reason is monetary gain, but he also highlighted the role fun and excitement play,
notably for younger offenders. Scarr (1973), drawing on a variety of sources, added
money and excitement, social needs – such as peer approval – and idiosyncratic reasons,
such as challenging the authorities. Respetto (1974) supported these claims that the
need for money was the prime motive in committing an offence with excitement,
revenge, curiosity and group solidarity being found commonly in younger criminals.
The revenge may be against the home owner, but is more often a general anger or
frustration with what the owner represents.
However, research would suggest that burglars, as with fraudsters, do not always
offend out of any desperate financial need or abject poverty (Rengert & Wasilchick,
1985). So, although many offenders cite financial gain as a reason for committing a
crime, it would appear that there are other important precipitating factors in the
decision to offend. Additionally, it would seem that in choosing a suitable target,
offenders are seeking more than rich pickings. This section will first explore the factors
that trigger an offender to commit a burglary, and second, the characteristics of targets
that are attractive to a burglar.
A variety of studies (reviewed in Canter & Youngs, 2008) have shown that burglars
tend to select targets within 2 kilometres of their homes, but this does vary in relation
to the overall population density of an area. Furthermore, evidence would suggest that
ACQUISITIVE CRIME 65
Target selection
Most of the understanding of burglary target selection comes from interviews
with offenders. They can be surprisingly willing to explain their actions. Such
research informs crime reduction initiatives (Mawby, 2001). Typically, they
want to avoid confrontation with property owners and avoid detection.
Some cues burglars use to select targets:
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
ease of access and escape routes are an important aspect in the target selection of an
area (see the video on David Canter’s YouTube channel of a burglar’s mental map).
Bevis and Nutter (1977) demonstrated that target selection was related to the degree
of access afforded by street design. Additionally, Luedtke (1970) found that houses near
a major highway were more likely to be selected as targets than houses far away from
major routes.
Differentiating burglars
Burglary is part of a general criminality, and thus, has a wide variety of offenders. The
variation between offenders has been examined by analysing burglary crime scene data.
This has supported two crucial ways in which burglars differ. One is the level of skill
or ‘professionalism’. The second is the interpersonal aspects of the crime (Merry, 1995;
Merry and Harsent, 2000). Canter (1989) emphasised the implicit interpersonal
element of burglary, where the perpetrator and victim become ‘intimate strangers’ as
a product of the intrusion into the resident’s personal space. Each of these two facets
generates a combination of low or high skill level and implicit or explicit interpersonal
interaction, creating four possible kinds of burglaries.
The Merry and Harsent (2000) model can be seen as a development of earlier
proposals, notably Walsh’s (1980) distinction between ‘challenge burglaries’, which are
66 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
are older offenders who are more likely to work alone, who have a more focused target
selection and resultant higher yield. High levels of professionalism are typical of
offenders who often work in conjunction with another offender and who are selective
and plan well in advance.
It is important to be aware that the majority of studies draw on convicted and often
incarcerated offenders. In most jurisdictions, only about one in ten burglaries are
detected; therefore, it is likely that there are significant differences between those
offenders who are convicted and those who evade detection. Consequently, there may
well be other types of offenders, especially highly professional ones, who are not
covered by the existing classifications.
Arson
As Fritzon, Lewis and Doley (2011) point out, there are several indications that there
is something distinctive about arsonists. For example, the majority of young fire-setters
are part of severely disruptive family environments with many disruptive changes (early
parental separation, illegitimacy, death of a close relative or brought up in a children’s
home). In Hurley and Monahans’ (1969) study, a comparison of arsonists with other
inmates revealed that the arsonists had eight times as many property convictions as a
comparison group, spent less overall time in institutions, but significantly longer in
institutions prior to age 14. Similarly, Rice and Harris (1991) note that the arsonists
that they studied within institutions were more socially isolated (i.e., hobbies, marital
status, living arrangements), less likely to be physically aggressive, less intelligent,
younger, less physically attractive and had a more extensive psychiatric history than
individuals convicted for other offences.
These studies suggest that there is some underlying psychological pattern that is
peculiar to arson. However, instead of the popular psychoanalytic examination of
‘motivation’ that relies on the assumption that arsonists experience sexual arousal when
setting fires, a relative lack of social skills appears to be a more appropriate analysis.
For example, Lange and Kirsch (1989) noted that of a sample of 243 fire-setters only
2.5 per cent were sexually aroused by fire stimuli, while Quinsey, Chaplin and Upfold
(1989) found no differences between normal subjects and fire-setters’ sexual arousal
to fire-related stimuli. Instead, Rice and Harris (1991) established in their sample that
lack of social skills in general and low assertiveness in particular are important correlates
of fire-setting. Jackson, Hope and Glass (1987) formulated a displaced aggression
hypothesis, where feelings of hostility were redirected from people to objects. Further,
this group appeared to have less stable or well-defined constructs of the seriousness of
property as opposed to person offences, as they showed no significant bias in rating
crimes against the person as more serious.
68 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
However, despite the seeming homogeneity of the arson group, subsequent studies
have suggested variations in the individual’s background. Furthermore, it has been estab-
lished that these relate to the style in which the offence was carried out. Canter and
Fritzon (1998) used a sophisticated statistical analysis to generate four themes that
differentiated between arsonists. These were derived from consideration of whether the
target of the arson was ostensibly a person or an object, and in addition, whether
the actions were fundamentally the expression of the person’s feelings in which the act
of setting fire is the crucial component, or the arson is to achieve some further objective
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
as a means to an end.
Robertson established the age ranges for the groups as early 20s to early 30s, mid
20s to mid 50s and under 25s. Furthermore, none of the opportunity group had prior
convictions, while the responsibility groups were linked more to lack of capability, all
of them having had less than 4 years of experience.
Again, as in Fritzon’s (2000) work, Robertson (2000) has highlighted the fact that
while there exist, at the broadest level, general explanations for organisational theft, these
do not account fully for the picture of individual variation within the same offence.
Fritzon and Robertson’s studies illustrate that there is no simple causal mechanism that
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
leads to the same behaviour. Instead, the variation in personal circumstances that lead
to individual offence events evolve through a sequence of actions that have influenced
the individual in ways that extend beyond the immediate offence behaviour. These may
accord more closely with general stylistic features of the individual’s personal history.
Fraud
Dodd (2000) states that while fraud is a relatively neglected area of research, it underpins
many other areas of criminality and is often a more general feature of the individual’s
life. Thus, much of the understanding of this offence may inform the broad features
of individual variation in more frequently studied offences. As with burglary, differ-
ences between intentional, sophisticated and organised forms of crime compared with
opportunistic, seemingly random and poorly organised actions also distinguish
fraudulent activity.
Dodd highlights the possible differences between opportunistic and sophisticated
offence styles. The first offence style does not involve the initial intention to defraud
and may begin, for example, by initially exaggerating an insurance claim. If successful,
the individual may have thought that was easy and determine to defraud again. This
pattern of behaviour is exemplified by those cases where the offender tries to secure
small amounts, especially when the individual is frequently in financial difficulty.
The sophisticated pattern involves the individual knowing their way around
organisational systems and creating their own opportunities, rather than abusing those
already in existence. Successful fraudsters, as Dodd states, have a good knowledge of
the system that they are defrauding. Therefore, the configuration of other influences
on the individual’s life, the contextual features of the offence landscape and the personal
predisposition and knowledge of the potential offender all combine to influence the
particular features of the way in which the offending behaviour is carried out.
Theory is necessary to make sense of why the rate and magnitude of various property
crimes – violent, fraudulent, stealthy, destructive or entrepreneurial – vary across time
and space. Beginning in the 1970s, theories of property crime focused largely on
explaining variation in property crime between situations, rather than across people or groups.
Two distinct but interrelated theoretical perspectives, namely, rational choice theory
and routine activities theory, have dominated the situational study of property crime.
Another situation-oriented perspective, phenomenology, has also been used to explain
property crime. Although the focus is on psychological explanantions, the general nature
70 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
of many sociological and social psychological theories considered in the previous chapter
(such as strain/anomie, bonds/disorganisation, learning/culture) means that they find
the causes of all property crimes in persons’ or groups’ criminality, or propensity to
engage in crime. In the end, it is likely that the interaction between criminality and
the characteristics of situations determines the rate and magnitude of property crime.
Rational choice
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The rational choice theory argues that crime becomes more likely as it becomes less costly
or more beneficial. For instance, the longer it takes to commit a crime (a cost) or the
less money obtained from it (a benefit), the less likely the crime is to occur. Undoubtedly,
the leading proponents of the rational choice theory within criminology are Clarke and
Cornish (1985), but Becker (1968) has provided a more directly economic interpretation.
He examined mathematically the interplay of the costs and benefits of crime and
punishment. Clarke and Cornish (1985) are less arithmetical in their considerations, but
do emphasise the practical implications for explaining acquisitive crime.
Routine activities
An important consequence of crime emerging out of the family, school and peer groups
is that it is integrated with all the other actions in which an offender may be engaged,
such as work and leisure. One implication of this is that criminal violations share many
of the same attributes of, and are interdependent with, other legitimate ‘routine
activities’ (Felson, 2006). They claim that people participate in legitimate routine
activities when satisfying their personal needs, through work, child rearing, shopping
or leisure pursuits. Each of these activities has a particular location associated with it,
whether it be their home, a pub they go to or a place of work. Consequently, if a
person decides to carry out a crime, where and when that occurs will be influenced
by the other routine activities in which they engage.
Cohen and Felson’s approach serves to link illegal and legal activities and builds
upon the concept of opportunity for crimes (Cohen, & Felson, 1979; Coleman, &
Norris, 2000). As such, crimes, more specifically predatory crime directed at people
and their property, involve the convergence in time and space of 1) motivated
offenders, 2) suitable targets and 3) the absence of capable guardians. Within this
framework, offending cannot be understood independently of the ecology of everyday
life (Sampson, 2001). Thus, the coming together in time and space of suitable targets
with no capable guardians present can lead to increases in crime rates, without any
increase or change in the personal condition that lead individuals to engage in crime
(Cohen & Felson, 1979; Blackburn, 1993).
However, although this account of where crimes may happen is appropriate for
opportunistic crimes, it is rather undermined if a criminal is more determined. For
people who commit many crimes, it is likely that their routines focus on the search
for criminal opportunities. For these people, their crimes are their routines, rather than
legal work or leisure activities. It also seems less plausible as a significant component
for non-predatory crimes, such as prostitution or drug dealing, or those crimes in which
the location of the activity is determined by the target of the crime, as may be the
case for a bank robbery.
ACQUISITIVE CRIME 71
for the culprit. The idea can even be used, in some circumstances, for linking crimes
to the same offender because of their proximity.
The calculation of the likely location of an offender’s home can be based on
statistical calculations using the relationship between known probabilities of offences
occurring at different distances from the home. Computer software has been developed
to assist these calculations and put the offences on a map together with the probabilities
of the location of the home (Canter & Hammond, 2006). One such software has been
called Dragnet (Canter, 2007), discussed in more detail in Chapter 12. Research using
this software has shown that in some cases, it can be remarkably accurate in identifying
home locations.
offending activity existed before their addiction (Mawby, 2001). It was the
proceeds of crime that provided the funds to buy drugs, which then became
an addiction. In some cases, even, drugs assisted carrying out the crime,
with depressants, such as cannabis and heroin, calming the nerves and
helping the offender to be more effective.
72 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
ACTIVITY 5.1
CONCLUSIONS
Most people who commit a number of crimes and are part of some form of criminal
community, being typical of those people, most readily labelled by the population at
large as ‘criminals’ who commit some form of burglary or theft at some time during
their criminal careers. This makes burglary the essence of criminality. However, the
direct instrumental gains of burglary are no more the sole purpose of carrying out the
act than are the consequence for the victim purely financial. Burglary is most
productively thought of as reflecting both styles of relating to other people and certain
forms of cognitive skills. From this perspective, then, burglars can be of great interest
to psychologists who wish to explore the processes that shape how people interact
with each other. Yet, there are very few studies of burglars that have been carried out
to examine these issues.
Books
Bennett, T., & Wright, R. (1984). Burglars on Burglary. Aldershot: Gower.
Canter, D. V., & Alison, L. (Eds.). (2000). Profiling property crimes. Farnham: Ashgate.
Canter, D., & Youngs, D. (2008). Geographical offender profiling: applications and oppor-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
tunities. In D. Canter and D. Youngs (Eds.), Applications of Geographical Profiling (pp. 3–24).
Farnham: Ashgate.
Kila, J., & Balcells, M. (2014). Cultural property crime: an overview and analysis of contemporary
perspectives and trends (Vol. 3). Brill. Online DOI: 10.1163/9789004280540
Maguire, M. (1982). Burglary in a dwelling. London: Heinemann.
Journal articles
Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: a routine activity
approach. American Sociological Review, 44, 588–608.
Farrington, D. P., & Lambert, S. (1994). Differences between burglars and violent offenders.
Psychology, Crime and Law, 1, 107–16.
Youngs, D. (2004). Personality correlates of offence style. Journal of Investigative Psychology and
Offender Profiling, 1(2) 99–120.
Websites
The following website gives an account of property crime in the USA:
www.fbi.gov/about-us/cjis/ucr/crime-in-the-u.s/2010/crime-in-the-u.s.-2010/property-
crime
The following website gives a detailed overview of property crime in the UK:
www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/crimeandjustice/bulletins/
focusonpropertycrime/2014to2015
6 Domestic violence
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
Violence within intimate relationships is remarkably common, with both men and
women as the perpetrators. The different forms it can take are discussed in this chapter,
together with a range of different explanations for its causes. The impact on children
in violent relationships is also considered. Stalking and related forms of harassment,
while legally a different crime, are also reviewed. The various approaches to the assess-
ment of the risks associated with intimate partner violence are examined, and the
interventions that are utilised in attempts to reduce future violence are discussed.
The terms intimate partner violence (IPV), domestic abuse, wife abuse, spouse abuse,
spouse battering and family violence are often used synonymously. Domestic violence
occurs in all types of intimate relationships, whether heterosexual or homosexual and
in all cultures and socioeconomic classes. It is perpetrated by both men and women;
however, the majority of cases involve male perpetrators and female victims. Although
Dutton and Nicholls (2005) argue that there is an underreporting of violence perpetrated
by women. This is possibly because men are reluctant to report being abused in this way.
DOMESTIC VIOLENCE 75
that domestic violence accounts for one quarter of all violent crimes and half of
all female homicides recorded by the police. According to the British Crime Survey
(2005), 28 per cent of women and 18 per cent of men experience domestic violence,
and it is more likely than any other criminal behaviour to involve repeat victimisation.
However, any estimates of the prevalence of domestic violence have to be treated
with caution. As Felson and Paré (2005) point out, there is considerable underreporting
of violence within the family, and this has not changed over the past half century. There
are many reasons for the underreporting. Victims may not think they will be believed;
some believe that it is too trivial to bother the police, underestimating the frequency
and severity of the abuse, or they think it is a private or family matter and not police
business, or that the police cannot do much. Others believe that it will result in more
violence because often they experience pressure and intimidation not to report the
attack. Feelings of fear, shame and embarrassment are often linked to the mistaken belief
that in some way they are to blame for the violence. There is also often the fear that
their children will be taken from them in any ensuing legal process. Frequently, the
violent partner will be full of remorse and apologise, giving rise to the victim believing
and hoping there will be no future violence. As a consequence, women are likely to
suffer many acts of violence before they inform the authorities.
The violence is not just physical, but includes sexual and psychological assaults, and
often, financial coercion as well, as detailed in Focus box 6.1. The victims of these
experiences are related to poorer health, difficulties in carrying out daily activities,
memory loss, emotional distress, suicidal thoughts and attempts (Ellsberg et al., 2008).
It is also important to be aware of the impact of domestic violence on any children
in the family. Perhaps, unsurprisingly, there is growing evidence that children who
come from homes in which violence is prevalent are more likely to be involved in
bullying, as victims or perpetrators, and to suffer a variety of psychological problems
(McLean & Balding, 2003).
In the past, and in many countries today, IPV was considered a private matter. Men
were expected to be dominant and to exert their power, through emotional, verbal,
financial and physical means. Indeed, until quite recently, in many jurisdictions, even
in Western democracies, rape within marriage was not recognised as a criminal
offence.
In the past 30 years, there have been changes within legal systems, especially in
developed democracies, especially in Western Europe and North America. These
changes have been driven by changes in attitudes towards the role of women in society
and the recognition of the prevalence of, and damage resulting from, domestic
violence (Gelles & Cornell, 1985). But this has not been the case everywhere.
President Putin of Russia has even recently made ‘moderate’ violence within the family
an administrative not a criminal matter.
76 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
Financial/economic violence
Controlling victim’s money and other economic resources (this involves
putting the victim on a strict ‘allowance’, withholding money at will and
forcing the victim to beg for the money until the abuser gives them some
money), preventing the victim from finishing education or obtaining
employment, stealing from or defrauding a partner of money or assets,
exploiting the intimate partner’s resources for personal gain, withholding
physical resources such as food, clothes, necessary medications or shelter.
Physical violence
Infliction of physical pain and/or injury for example, pushing, shoving,
slapping, hitting, pulling hair, biting, grabbing, choking, shaking, arm-twisting,
kicking, punching, hitting with objects, throwing things, burning, stabbing,
shooting, poisoning, indirect physical violence (destruction of objects/
property).
Sexual violence
FOCUS 6.1
common type of IPV and dominates general surveys, student samples and even
marriage counselling samples.
Mutual violent control (MVC) is where both partners are violent and controlling.
Recent research has focused on identifying different types of batterers and has found
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
that violent partners vary along a number of dimensions, including severity of violence,
alcohol use, anger, depression and so on, thus showing that they are heterogeneous.
Researchers developed a number of different typologies in an attempt to systematically
examine how and why different men use violence against their partners. This increases
the understanding of partner violence and facilitates effective identification, assessment
and intervention. The assumption behind all these different typologies is that a valid
typology of batterers could be used to match different types of abuse to different forms
of intervention.
Research models have differed in their emphasis on behavioural traits, form and
severity of violence or personality characteristics, motivation, causation, actions and
victim–offender interaction. All these inconsistencies have made interpreting results,
comparing findings and drawing conclusions problematic. As a consequence, there have
been numerous typologies developed in the domestic violence literature. Despite many
commonalities, no single profile has emerged that completely and reliably distinguishes
batterers from non-violent men. It must also be noted that the typology approach to
classification that seeks to identify strict categories has been criticised. Such typological
systems require that each individual belongs to only one ‘type’. This rigidity denies
the possibility of variation in an individual’s behaviour and of more complex multi-
dimensional aspects of the offending, as well as ignoring the potential for development
or change in someone’s actions.
Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994) in a review of fifteen batterer typologies
proposed that batterer subtypes can be classified along three descriptive dimensions:
1 severity and frequency of marital violence
2 generality of the violence (i.e., family-only or extra-familial violence)
3 the batterer’s psychopathology or personality disorders
These dimensions were used to identify three major subtypes of batterers: family-
only, dysphoric/borderline and generally violent/antisocial.
Family-only batterers are the most likely to feel remorse, admit having marital
problems and to seek help for such problems. They are the least violent group and
the least likely to engage in psychological and sexual abuse. The violence is generally
restricted to family members; they are the least likely to engage in violence outside
the home or to have related legal problems. Also, they evidence little psychopathology
and either no personality disorder or a passive-dependent personality disorder. Approxi-
mately 50 per cent of batterer research samples are in the family-only subgroup.
Dysphoric/borderline batterers engage in moderate to severe partner abuse, including
psychological and sexual abuse. Usually, the violence is confined to the family, but some-
times they may engage in extra-familial violence and criminal behaviour. They are the
most dysphoric, psychologically distressed and emotionally volatile. They experience
DOMESTIC VIOLENCE 79
police had been called out to their home at least nine times, and in 1989,
he had been charged with spousal assault and convicted. It was also revealed
that for several months after they separated, he hung around outside her
new home, he called her trying to persuade her that they needed to work
things out, brought her flowers and left them on her doorstep and showed up
at neighbourhood restaurants they used to go to in hope of seeing her there.
FOCUS 6.2
O.J. Simpson was eventually acquitted of the murder charges, but was
ordered, in the subsequent civil case in 1997, to pay $33.5 million to the
relatives of Nicole Brown and Ron Goldman. He has since then written a
book called If I Did It that somewhat ambiguously implies that he may well
have committed the murders.
delusional jealousy and cannot tolerate separation from their partner. They may evidence
borderline and schizoidal personality characteristics and may have problem with alcohol
and drug abuse. These men make up about 25 per cent of the research samples.
Generally violent/antisocial batterers feel little remorse and are most likely to blame
their victim. They engage in moderate to severe violence, including psychological and
sexual abuse, and they are the most violent subtypes. They engage in high levels of
partner and extra-familial violence and have the most extensive history of related
criminal behaviour. They are likely to have problems with alcohol and drug abuse
and be most likely to show the characteristics of antisocial personality disorder or psy-
chopathy. This type constitutes 25 per cent of the batterer research sample.
Other typologies include Elbow (1977), Gondolf (1988), Saunders (1992),
Hamberger, Lohr, Bonge & Tolin (1996), Cavanaugh and Gelles (2005) and Chiffriller,
Hennessy & Zappone (2006).
The term stalking is used to describe the willful, repeated and malicious following,
harassing or threatening of another person. The state of California was the first in the
USA to pass an anti-stalking law in 1990. In Britain, stalking was legally recognised
by the introduction of the Protection from Harassment Act in 1997.
Stalking typically consists of a broad range of behaviours. Stalkers most often perse-
cute their targets by unwanted communications, which can consist of frequent (often
nightly) telephone calls, letters, e-mails, graffiti, notes (e.g., left on the target’s car) or
80 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
packages (e.g., gifts, pictures). The offender may camp outside the house or workplace
of the victim. Somewhat more extreme forms include ordering goods and services in
the victim’s name and charging them to the victim’s account, placing false advertise-
ments or announcements, ordering funeral wreaths, spreading rumours about the victim,
destroying or moving their property. The perpetrator may threaten the victim with
violence and actually assault, rape and murder. In many cases, innocent parties and the
target’s circle of friends and associates become victims of the stalker’s behaviour.
In a study of stalking victims conducted by the National Institute of Justice and the
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Centres for Disease Control and Prevention, 8,000 women and 8,000 men were con-
tacted by telephone and asked whether they had ever experienced any number of acts
of stalking. The report indicated that 8.2 per cent of the women in the sample and
2.2 per cent of the men had been stalked sometime in their lives and that an estimated
1 million adult women and 0.4 million men are stalked annually in the USA (Tjaden
& Thoenness, 1997).
Stalking gained major media attention by the high-profile cases involving celebrities.
However, research shows that most cases of stalking take place between ordinary people
who had a prior intimate relationship or were acquaintances (Meloy, 1996). Emerson,
Ferris and Gardner (1998) found that of women victims, a total of 48 per cent reported
being stalked by a partner/spouse or ex-partner/spouse, 14 per cent by dates or former
dates, 19 per cent by acquaintances and 23 per cent by strangers. Most male victims,
about 70 per cent, were stalked by acquaintances or strangers.
Therefore, research shows that the largest victim group of stalking is female ex-intim-
ate partners (Meloy, 1998), establishing an association between stalking and domestic
violence. In general, a high correlation has been found between domestic violence and
stalking. However, very few studies have examined which factors predict the occurrence
of stalking in relationships characterised by domestic violence or that provide direct data
on the link between stalking and previous domestic violence. Those that have been
conducted do suggest that between 30 per cent and 65 per cent of stalking cases that
involve former intimates also involved a previous violent relationship (Tjaden &
Thoennes, 1997). Approximately 50–60 per cent of all stalking cases may be considered
‘domestic’ in the sense that cases involve former intimates. About half of this domestic
stalking group involves previous violent relationships (Douglas & Dutton, 2001). It is
estimated that between 29 per cent and 54 per cent of all female murder victims are
battered women, and in 90 per cent of these cases, stalking preceded the murder. This
has led many to conclude that stalking in intimate relationships is a form of domestic
violence.
One other finding to emerge from the research is that persons who stalk ex-intimate
partners tend to display more violence towards their victims than do persons who stalk
others. Meloy and Gothard (1995) determined that threats made by stalkers were more
common where the victim was a former intimate partner. Similarly, violent stalkers
were more likely to have a prior attachment to their victims (80 per cent) than were
non-violent stalkers (55 per cent) (Schwartz-Watts & Morgan, 1998). Generally,
findings suggest that men who stalked their former intimate partners after a break-up
were more likely than other men to have been abusive in the relationship. Violence
is common in the past relationship and is common during the stalking episode. Findings
suggest that the co-occurrence of stalking and domestic violence increases the risk of
serious violence and murder.
DOMESTIC VIOLENCE 81
TABLE 6.1 Common domestic violence stalking acts (Sonkin, 1997 in Meloy, 1998).
• Posting cards or other cryptic messages
• Breaking windows, breaking into or vandalising a partner’s home
• Taking a partner’s post
• Leaving things such as flowers on a doorstep or at work
• Watching a partner from a distance
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
TYPOLOGY OF STALKING
Mohandie, Meloy, McGowan & Williams (2006) when studying a random sample of
1,005 North American stalkers decided that the main difference between them was
determined by the nature of their prior relationship. Four types were identified –
intimate, acquaintance, public figure and private stranger. As others had found, the
highest risk of violence was from those who had a prior intimate sexual relationship
and a very low risk of violence from celebrity stalkers. Interestingly, they found that
the greater the evidence for psychosis in the stalker, the less the likelihood of violence.
Risk factors or risk markers refer to the characteristics associated with an increased like-
lihood that a problem behaviour, in this case violence, will occur. It should be noted
that the presence of one or more risk factors is not equivalent to a causal relationship.
It means that the odds of an associated event, in this case, domestic violence, are greater
when one or more risk markers are present. While it may be an indicator of which
82 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
groups of people are most vulnerable, a risk factor is not the same as the cause of the
violence because it might be correlated with something else that is associated with the
underlying cause.
Most of the work examining factors associated with domestic violence has focused
on identifying differences between men who have engaged in violence against their
partners and those who have not. Numerous risk factors for domestic violence
perpetration have been identified by this means.
Many studies have found that men who perpetrate violence against their wives are
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
more likely than non-violent comparison groups to report that they experienced
violence in the family of origin, either as a witness to spouse abuse or as the victim of
child abuse; but numerous men who grew up in violent homes do not abuse their
wives, and many men who do abuse their partners did not experience violence in their
families of origin. Fear of abandonment is an important aspect of abusive men’s
behaviour (Dutton, Saunders, Starzomski & Bartholomew 1994). A threat that a partner
might leave the relationship is dangerous. Leaving or attempting to leave was found
to provoke potentially lethal violence on the part of the husband (Aldridge & Browne,
2003) in many violent relationships.
While domestic violence occurs in all demographic groups, several demographic
characteristics have been related to the perpetration of partner abuse. For example,
youth has been found to be predictive of violence. Rates of domestic violence tend
to decrease somewhat as the age of the couples increases (Straus, Gelles & Steinmetz
1980). Lower socioeconomic status has also been identified as a predictor of violence
in the general population, as well as unemployment. But, it should be noted that
batterers come from all social backgrounds.
Prior arrest for violent crime is one frequently mentioned risk factor for domestic
violence re-assault. Generally, it was found that people with criminal records are more
likely to be violent. Also, substance (drugs and alcohol) abuse has consistently been linked
with domestic violence (Cattaneo & Goodman, 2003). It has been reported that women
who are pregnant are at increased risk of being assaulted by their abusive partners (Riggs,
Caulfield & Street 2000), as well as those who are unmarried and cohabiting. The
presence of children in the household is associated with the risk of domestic violence.
Concerning psychological characteristics, spouse abusers tend to be generally more
angry and hostile than non-violent men. Their personality exhibits characteristics of
emotional dependence, insecurity, low self-esteem, low empathy, low impulse control,
poor communication and social skills, antisocial personality, narcissism, anxiety,
depression, aggressive and hostile personality styles. In terms of psychopathology mood
disorders, depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and borderline personality
disorder have been identified as markers for domestic violence.
It should be noted that the differences in these studies between abusers and non-
abusers are relatively small, and there is no single factor that can be used to identify
men at risk. Violence may occur even in the absence of identified risk markers.
RISK ASSESSMENT
Risk assessment in cases of domestic violence can be defined as trying to identify those
victims who are most at risk of experiencing violence in the future. The assessment
DOMESTIC VIOLENCE 83
of risk for marital violence perpetration or victimisation is not a simple process. The
variety of risk markers and the general lack of information regarding risk markers for
specific incidents of spouse abuse make it quite difficult for professionals to confidently
evaluate the dangerousness of a particular person or situation.
Recently, attempts have been made to develop instruments to assess the risk of
domestic violence. These measures include items that evaluate a subgroup of the risk
markers described above, including characteristics of the perpetrators, victims and/or
abusive relationships. Accurate risk assessments are very important, as they provide a
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
structured way for responding officers to gather detailed and relevant information from
victims. This information when shared with other agencies can help provide better
services to victims and perpetrators because their specific needs are identified.
The Spousal Assault Risk Assessment (SARA; Kropp, Hart, Webster & Eaves 1995;
Kropp & Hart, 2000) is a twenty-item clinician administered rating form that was
designed to assess risk of re-offending in the criminal justice system. A complete
evaluation with the SARA requires psychological assessment of the perpetrator and
clinical judgment. Other risk assessment instruments include:
• The Danger Assessment (DA) is a measure designed to assist battered women in
the assessment of their own risk of femicide (Campbell, 1986).
• The Domestic Violence Screening Instrument (DVSI) (Williams & Houghton,
2004) is designed not only to assess risk of re-assault, but also to assess treatment
needs. Unlike the SARA, the DVSI is a structured questionnaire to be completed
by the perpetrator.
• The Kingston Screening Instrument for Domestic Violence (K-SID) was devel-
oped from the extensive program of research of Richard Gelles (Gelles, 1998) as
a screening instrument. The K-SID consists of ten risk markers for re-assault.
• The DV-MOSAIC is a computer-assisted method of threat assessment developed
by Gavin de Becker and associates (De Becker et al., 1997, 2006). DV-MOSAIC
is not a predictive instrument. It is an overall method used to aid police officers
in their assessments and investigations of domestic violence situations.
• The Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS; Straus, 1979) is used to measure intrafamily
conflict.
• The Index of Spouse Abuse (ISA; Hudson and McIntosh, 1981), the Danger
Assessment Instrument (Campbell, 1995) and the Propensity for Abusiveness Scale
(PAS; Dutton, 1995) were designed to measure the severity of abuse.
Various police forces and related agencies have also developed their own procedures
for assessing the risk associated with reports of violence within the home. Unfortun-
ately, these procedures are not developed using any established psychometric principles
and are not published in scholarly outlets where they can be openly and independently
evaluated. They, thus, tend to operate as checklists of what needs to be considered
with little indication that their use actually reduces risk (see Focus box 6.3).
INTERVENTIONS
of Quintas attacking again was low. Less than a week after her phone call,
Quintas cut her throat in her flat. She was pregnant. Hayley told police
where they could find Quintas, but officers who could have responded
were already dealing with a report of a dog locked in a car. The Independent
Police Complaints Commission blamed ‘institutional failings’. Quintas was
jailed for a minimum of 18 years. (Source: The Observer, 31 December
2006)
Julia Pemberton, 47, from Hermitage, Berkshire, was shot four times by her
estranged husband in November 2003. Before the killing, she reported three
incidents of serious abuse to Thames Valley police, but insufficient action
was taken. Julia had suffered years of emotional abuse by her husband. Alan
Pemberton killed their 17-year-old son outside their home while Julia hid
inside and called 999. An operator heard her scream: ‘Oh my God, I’m going
to die’, before gunshots were heard. Thames Valley Police were criticised for
taking almost seven hours to enter the property. Pemberton shot himself.
(Source: The Observer, 31 December 2006)
FOCUS 6.3
Vicky Horgan, 27, from Oxfordshire was shot dead, along with her 25-year-
old sister, Emma Walton, by estranged husband Stuart Horgan at a family
barbecue in June 2004. She had contacted Thames Valley police’s domestic
violence team about him on several occasions. He committed suicide while
on remand in prison. (Source: The Observer, 11 March 2007)
stop future occurrences or relapses. In cases of partner violence, this means helping
people lead non-violent and safe lives. Since the problem of violence against women
in the home was recognised, thousands of projects, programmes, policies and practices
have been developed worldwide to respond to those who suffer abuse and intervene
with respect to its perpetrators.
Many of these approaches consist of providing some form of safe haven or shelter
for the victims. Some of these try and enable the victims to understand the processes
that give rise to violence by teaching safety planning. This involves preparing in
advance the possibility of leaving when the victim recognises that the violence is likely
to become even more severe or life threatening.
DOMESTIC VIOLENCE 85
There has also been a growing move to improve the responses of the justice system,
particularly of the police, by providing support for those who have been abused and
responding more effectively to the perpetrators of abuse. Domestic violence units
(DVUs) were first established in London in 1987; more than half the police forces in
England and Wales have a DVU. The units have staff who offer advice and help to
victims of domestic violence. They have links with other specialist organisations and
agencies in their local areas, such as local solicitors and Women’s Aid groups.
Protection orders, injunctions, restraining orders, sanction through fines, probation and
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
prison are some forms of legal intervention. But these often do not deal directly with
the central psychological issues involved in the perpetuation of domestic violence. They
may even make matters worse by treating a psychological problem as a legal one.
A more psychological approach is the development of treatment programmes for
people who are violent. So, although the primary goals of virtually all programmes/
groups are to ensure the partners’ safety, they do put emphasis also on altering attitudes
towards violence. There are attempts to increase perpetrators’ sense of personal
responsibility and learn non-violent alternatives to past behaviours. Other treatments
include couple therapy where both partners are helped to take responsibility for their
actions and their own behaviour. Also mental health programs, such as individual
psychotherapy or group therapy are being made available.
ACTIVITY 6.1
Below is an example of a domestic violence case. Based on what you have read so
far, assess the risk of future violence to Emma.
Emma has been with 32-year-old James for 12 years and they have three
children, aged nine, seven and three, respectively. James is unemployed and has
a long criminal history, including violence and property offences. He often drinks
alcohol to excess. There have already been a number of instances of domestic
violence between the couple. Emma, in the past, decided not to put up with his
abusive behaviour any longer and asked him to leave the house. James hung
around outside her house and slept at night in his car parked across the street.
He began calling her on the telephone, begging her to let him return and promising
not to drink alcohol anymore. On one occasion, he entered her home, struck her on
the back of the head with a hammer, hit her in the face and sexually assaulted her.
After living apart for a month, James is now back at home living with Emma. Find
one of the many autobiographies written by gangsters or other people who have
committed crimes. Review the sorts of explanations and justifications they give
for their crimes. Do these owe more to some sort of mental disorder, the types of
cognitive processes discussed in this chapter, or the social processes discussed
in Chapter 4?
86 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
CONCLUSIONS
Domestic violence is a widespread problem, the full extent of which is not fully known
due to underreporting by both victims and official agencies. Many theories of domestic
violence have been offered, but none of them has been adequate in thoroughly
explaining domestic violence. A number of risk factors for domestic violence
perpetration have been identified, and many programmes for abusers and their victims
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
exist, with mixed results. More recently, professionals have taken a more responsible
and informed attitude to domestic violence, but resources available to the health, social
and law enforcement services are still too limited to deal appropriately with the issue.
The underlying psychological mechanisms are still only poorly understood. It is likely
that social processes such as those highlighted by the feminist perspective as well as
individual’s psychological problems, probably having roots in the violent person’s own
upbringing, combine together to give rise to this pressing problem. This means a variety
of different strategies at the individual and public level will be needed to reduce the
incidence of domestic violence.
Books
Browne, K., & Herbert, M. (1997). Preventing family violence. Chichester: Wiley.
Dobash, E. R., Dobash, R. P., Cavanagh, K., & Lewis, R. (2000). Changing violent men. Ontario:
Sage Publications.
Newman, C., & Iwi, K. (2015). Engaging with perpetrators of domestic violence: practical techniques
for early intervention. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Westwood, J., Farrelly, N., Radford, L., Thiara, R., Hester, M., Bunston, W., & Morris, A. (2015).
Domestic violence and protecting children: new thinking and approaches. London: Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
DOMESTIC VIOLENCE 87
Journal articles
Aldridge, M. L., & Browne, K. D. (2003). Perpetrators of spousal homicide: a review. Trauma,
Violence and Abuse, 4, 265–76.
Cattaneo, L. B., & Goodman, L. A. (2003). Victim-reported risk factors for continued abusive
behaviour: Assessing the dangerousness of arrested batterers. Journal of Community Psychology,
31(4), 349–69.
Douglas, K. S., & Dutton, D. G. (2001). Assessing the link between stalking and domestic
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Websites
Both of the following websites give a detailed overview of the issues and effects of domestic
violence and offer practical advice:
www.refuge.org.uk
www.joyfulheartfoundation.org
This website gives a detailed overview of all types of domestic violence, including stalking and
different types of victims:
www.domesticviolencelondon.nhs.uk
7 Sexual offences
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
Rape is sexual activity without consent. It, therefore, is a difficult crime to prosecute
and psychologically challenging to understand because it is not only what is observed
that is at issue, as with other crimes, but what is understood or consented to by the
parties involved. The issue of consent, therefore, underlies all aspects of the
consideration of rape. It relates to widely held attitudes to rape, known as ‘rape myths’,
and the high proportion of allegations that never result in a conviction. For those clearer
cases in which the perpetrator does not seek consent or has no concern for the victim’s
reactions, it is fruitful to compare explanations based on the characteristics of the rapist
with sociological or cultural theories. Classifications of rape and rapists help to
demonstrate the variety of forms the crime can take and the need for a combination
of explanations for its occurrence.
Sexual abuse of children raises distinct questions and challenges not least because
the victims are so vulnerable and may not be believed. Recent changes in public
attitudes and investigative processes are revealing the significance of these crimes against
young people.
SEXUAL OFFENCES 89
anus or mouth of another person’ when that other person ‘does not consent to the
penetration’ or that the offender ‘does not reasonably believe’ that the other person
consents. The act ensures that the laws are equally applied to both male and female
victims, as well as clarifying issues surrounding consent. The Act also outlines the crime
of ‘assault by penetration’ treating the penetration by other objects, other than the
penis, just as seriously. In the UK, both rape and assault by penetration hold the
maximum sentence of life in prison.
Rape myths
Unlike any other criminal activity, sexual assault and rape are unwanted aspects of an
activity that under different circumstances may be welcomed. Sexuality is also a
remarkably prevalent part of all cultures, either as overt expression or through strong
strictures. It is, therefore, to be expected that all cultures encompass many different
views about and attitudes towards sexuality.
In relation to rape, these views and attitudes have been found to include many
erroneous assumptions, referred to as ‘rape myths’ (see Focus box 7.1). Evidence for
these rape myths have been found in many studies, going back many years, such as
Scully and Marolla (1985) and Burt (1980). A more recent summary emphasises that
these myths are believed by many people, not just rapists (Ryan, 2004).
Because of the misunderstanding of the nature of rape, the UK judicial system has
issued guidelines to judges about how to advise juries to reduce the influence of pre-
existing attitudes or myths about rape that may influence the jury’s decision-making.
However, there are many other common misunderstandings about the situations in
which rape occurs; these are reviewed very thoroughly by Massaro (2015).
False allegations
The recognition of rape myths also opens up the contentious issue of false allegations
of rape. For example, if it is not true that women ‘cry rape’ for revenge or because
they regret what they did, can it ever be the case that a rape allegation is false? The
legal evidence is that a small number of cases do occur where it is clear that the
allegation was false. The estimate of exactly what proportion of cases is false varies
enormously between studies.
For example, Kelly, Lovett and Regan (2005) determined that sixty-seven of
2,643 (3 per cent) rape allegations studied were probably false and another 22 per cent
were possibly false. By contrast, Kanin (1994) found that forty-five of 109 rape reports
(41 per cent) were determined to be false over a nine-year period in a small US
metropolitan area. The highest estimate of false rape allegations is 89 per cent (sixteen
90 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
• You can tell if she’s ‘really’ been raped by how she acts.
FOCUS 7.1
• Women cry rape when they regret having sex or want revenge.
• Only gay men get raped/only gay men rape men.
• Prostitutes cannot be raped.
• If the victim did not complain immediately, it was not rape.
From www.cps.gov.uk/legal/p_to_r/rape_and_sexual_offences/societal_myths/
Regan 2005). Also, if the alleged perpetrator is a partner, ex-partner or friend rather
than a stranger or family member, victims are more likely to withdraw their statements
(Feist, Ashe, Lawrence, McPhee & Wilson, 2007).
Kelly, Lovett and Regan (2005) summarise the reasons for attrition at four key stages
in the criminal justice system at which cases are likely to be withdrawn.
1 Decision to report
A victim of rape may not report a rape to the police for a number of reasons, including
fears about the investigation and trial process, the trauma of having to relive the rape
again in court, feelings of shame, embarrassment, guilt or fear of retribution.
2 Investigative stage
Victims may decide to withdraw a complaint of rape or refuse to provide evidence to
prosecute the accused after they are detained. This may be due to the anticipated
trauma associated with a trial (as in Stage 1). It may also be that the police cannot
identify or find the suspect, and therefore, the case is dropped.
3 Discontinuance by prosecutors
If an offender has been apprehended, the crown prosecution service (CPS) in the UK
or district attorneys in the US may decide not to take the case to trial if they believe
there is not enough evidence to secure a prosecution. This may be because of doubts
about the credibility of the victim or any witnesses, or the difficulty of determining
whether consent was withheld.
4 Trial
If the case goes to trial, the jury may decide the suspect is not guilty of the crime, and
therefore, will acquit. Alternatively, the offender may be convicted for a lesser crime
or have his conviction overturned on appeal.
Cases are more likely to get to court, and the culprit receive a custodial sentence,
if it can be shown that a rape is part of a linked series (Feist et al., 2007).
THEORIES OF RAPE
Evolutionary explanation
Evolutionary explanations put the emphasis on rape as an inherent aspect of male make-
up that gives benefits to the survival of those men who spread their DNA around,
whether or not the recipient is willing (Thornhill & Palmer, 2000). It is even claimed
that there is a natural instinct to rape if consensual intercourse is not available.
SEXUAL OFFENCES 93
While this has some simple-minded appeal, it does not really explain why the great
majority of men do not rape. It does not explain the rape of men by men or men by
women. It also does not explain the great variations between cultures and situations
in the prevalence of rape. Furthermore, if the genetic driver were due to the inability
of the rapist to have consensual intercourse, then it would not be expected that rapists
had established consensual sexual relationships, or any offspring. This is clearly not the
case. Some rapists have long-standing sexual partners and children.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Psychological explanations
Many of the explanations for criminal activity discussed in earlier chapters are directly
relevant to rape. These include impulsivity, aggressiveness and cognitive distortions.
More specifically, the sorts of cognitive distortions that blame the victims have been
found in many rapists who minimise the impact of rape on their victims, misread the
social cues from potential victims and generally lack inhibitions and controls over their
desire for sexual gratification. Furthermore, male rapists have been found to have a
limited and stereotypical view of what it means to be a man. Importantly, these men
are likely to have many sexual partners and regard women as challenging opponents
to be conquered.
One widely quoted, but somewhat controversial, explanation of rape was put
forward by Groth (1979) some time ago. He argues that there are three fundamental
processes that motivate rape: anger, power and sadism. The anger rapist wants to humiliate
and insult his victim. It is women in general or particular women who he is enraged by.
He uses sex to defile and debase his victim. Groth (1979) claims that these offenders attack
their victims by grabbing them, tearing their clothes and beating them to the ground.
The power rapist wishes to demonstrate his control and dominance of his victims.
Groth (1979) explains this as a compensation for feelings of inadequacy that is fostered
by fantasies of sexual conquest. Their mistaken belief is that once the victim is over-
powered, she will enjoy the sexual activity. Unlike the anger rapist, it is proposed that
the power rapist will coerce by verbal threats and the use of a weapon and will not
be further violent once the victim is under his control.
When aggression and the infliction of pain have an erotic quality for the rapist, his
sadistic inclinations give rise to maltreatment of the victim. Groth (1979) proposes that
this sadistic rape is driven by the actual sexual pleasure that comes from tormenting the
victim. Their struggles, distress and suffering are what heighten his enjoyment of the
assault. Therefore, these rapists are likely to prolong their attacks and may have bizarre
rituals and use foreign objects to penetrate the victim. The victims may be killed as part
of these rituals as the rapists seek an ultimate gratification by committing the murder.
Groth’s (1979) classification of rapes is interesting in combining accounts of differ-
ent psychological motivations with different patterns of behaviour. The motivations
emphasise desires other than sexual gratification. It is anger, power or the delight in
hurting a victim that gives rise to rape, according to Groth. The inherent contradic-
tion in this assertion has been challenged by many authorities (e.g., Tedeschi &
Felson, 1994). The essence of this challenge is to question why it is sexual activity that
fosters power, anger and sadism, rather than other more direct expressions of these
motivations. Instead, emphasis is given to inhibited sexual gratification that is supported
by social processes.
94 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
A second challenge to Groth’s typology is the lack of clear empirical evidence for
his trichotomy. The theory assumes there are some actions in rape that co-occur and
there are others that tend not to co-occur. For example, the hypothesis is that ripping
the victims’ clothing will tend to happen in the same rapes as beating the victim to
the ground. But, in contrast, ritual activity and penetration with a foreign object will
not occur when the clothing is ripped. These and all the other hypotheses about the
co-occurrence of actions in a rape, implied by Groth’s classifications, are open to direct
test by examining what actually happens across many rapes. Research that has carried
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
out such examinations is considered below when the actions in a rape are examined
directly. But, first, the social processes that may support sexual assaults are discussed.
Social processes
Feminists also follow Groth’s proposal that rape is not ultimately about sexual gratifi-
cation, but is a direct product of the general male desire to dominate, control and punish
women. This argument has been developed to claim that rape exists to ensure that there
remains an unequal balance of power between men and women, keeping women under
the general control of men. Brownmiller (1975) puts this clearly: ‘it is nothing more or
less than a conscious process of intimidation by which all men keep all women in a state
of fear’ (p. 5). These arguments are the same as those put forward to explain domestic
violence in the previous chapter. They propose that all men are potential rapists.
The feminist perspective has come into criticism, especially the assertion that all
men are potential rapists. Evidence does not support this notion – the majority of men
do not rape women. The feminist approach has also been criticised for not seeking to
explain other types of rape, such as those perpetrated by women.
Developmental models
Developmental models assert that rape occurs as a result of inadequate bonding within
childhood. As discussed in Chapter 4, these are strongly influenced by Bowlby’s (1952)
ACTIVITY 7.1
Discuss the following situation in a mixed group of men and women and consider
the variations in their views.
A young man and a young woman, who who have recently met at a nightclub,
have had enough to drink to make them feel too drunk to drive home, but sober
enough to go back to her house for a coffee. When they are alone in her house,
she allows the man to undress her, but when she tells him she is too drunk to have
sex, he says she will enjoy it once it gets started. She struggles a little, but not
enough to prevent him having sex with her. The next morning she reports him to
the police for rape. Was she right to do this? What further information should the
police collect to proceed to a court case? What reasons might there be for the man
not being convicted?
SEXUAL OFFENCES 95
ideas of maternal deprivation and attachment. This suggests that rape occurs because as
a child, offenders do not form a close, ‘healthy’ bond with their mothers at a crucial
point in their development. Therefore, as an adult, the offender is unable to form nor-
mal relationships with peers and potential partners, and thus, has to rape in order to
gain intimacy and to satisfy a desire for social contact (Marshall, 1989). This theory is
supported by evidence that some rapists do score highly on measures of intimacy and
loneliness (Seidman, Hudson & Robertson, 1994). However, there is no evidence to
suggest that lack of a parental bond will have a causal relationship with lack of intimacy
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
in adulthood, and it is likely that other mediating factors are involved. Not all people
who do not have a parental influence within childhood go on to be criminals, let alone
become rapists.
Sexual assaults can take many different forms. These differences carry various impli-
cations. They may act as indicators that help to link offences to a common offender.
They can provide indications of the characteristics of the offender (‘offender profil-
ing’). They may also act as reference points to help determine false allegations or as
an aspect of considering the trauma caused by the assault. They are also important indi-
cators of the meaning of the rape for the perpetrator. This is of use in thinking about
the forms of treatment or sentence appropriate to that offender.
The study of these behavioural differences has its roots in investigative psychology
(Canter & Youngs, 2009), which is considered in more detail in Chapter 12. When
the actions of a criminal are the starting point for any inferences, it is essential to
understanding how those actions may differ from one crime to the next. With regard
to sexual offences, what they have in common is, of course, the sexual activity.
Consequently, it is aspects of the non-sexual actions that are particularly revealing.
Canter and Heritage (1990) developed the first behaviourally based classification
of rape by analysing sixty-six stranger sex offences from the UK police records and
content analysing the activities that occurred in each of those offences. Subsequently,
Canter, Bennell, Alison and Reddy (2003) continued to examine the behavioural
structure of rape in this way and developed a similar model using victim statements
from 112 British rapes. They identified a four-themed model of rape behaviour, where
the offences could be differentiated into hostile, involvement, controlling and steal-
ing types of behaviour. Canter et al. (2003) also used statistical techniques to examine
the levels of violation within rapes; they found that these themes could vary in the
types of sexual, physical and personal violation used in the offence. Sexual violation
was used in a majority of the cases, more than physical violation, whereas personal
violation was rarer, and thus, could be used to more readily differentiate between offen-
ces. The approach they used was to examine the patterns of co-occurrence of actions
in rapes.
Hostility
The hostility theme in rape, in the Canter et al. (2003) study, consists of nine items,
which reflect the overtly aggressive interaction between offender and victim. Six
96 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
Control
Six variables have been interpreted as offence behaviours that demonstrate the offen-
der’s control of the offence. The offender controls the victim through binding her,
gagging her, threatening her not to report the crime and using a weapon. Other actions
such as using a blindfold and wearing a disguise reflect the offender’s attempt to conceal
his identity. There is a clear parallel between the actions categorised as control and
the victim as object theme in the model proposed by Canter (1994) summarised in
Focus box 7.2. Both of these themes are comprised of actions that relate to
demobilisation of the victim and suggest various forms of pre-planning and preparation
on the part of the offender.
Theft
Four aspects of rape have been interpreted as offence behaviours that directly relate
to criminal behaviours beyond the sexual component of the crime. The offender in
this case is clearly using the opportunities presented by the crime for some further
instrumental goal, not just for the immediate sexual gratification of the rape. These
behaviours include demanding goods from the victim, stealing personal goods, stealing
identifiable goods and stealing unidentifiable goods. These variables support the
instrumental categorisation proposed by Bartol (1986).
Involvement
A further six actions are interpreted as offence behaviour that has distinct involve-
ment components. There is clearly a theme of attempted involvement with the victim
with behaviours such as offender sexual comment, compliments the victim, identifies
the victim, kisses the victim and implies knowing the victim. Again, there is a clear
parallel between the actions categorised as involvement here and the victim as person
theme in the model by Canter (1994), summarised in Focus box 7.2, and the intimacy
region of Canter and Heritage (1990).
These themes seem to reflect the various psychological processes identified within the
rape literature. All seem to have roots in theoretical models, from an arena of different
perspectives and reflect the type of interaction that the offender will have within the
rape situation.
SEXUAL OFFENCES 97
Vehicle
Within this theme, the offender uses the victim as a medium for their own
benefit. For example, an offender may act violently towards a victim in pursuit
of sexual gratification or monetary gain. Typical behaviours that could be seen
within this theme would include robbery and sexual assault. Behaviours
defined within this theme are also thought of as being more excessively
violent than those seen within the other two themes.
Object
Here, the offender treats the victim as a depersonalised object to be
manipulated and controlled. Typical behaviours seen within this style of
offending would, therefore, include such items as gagging, binding,
threatening and being verbally derogatory towards the victim.
Person
FOCUS 7.2
Here, the offender will treat the victim as a human being, rather than an
object and may show behaviours that indicate some kind of pseudo-intimacy.
The offender will, therefore, be interested in his or her victim’s life and may
request that the victim participate verbally or physically in any sexual contact
as if they were having a relationship.
Academic utility
1 The statistical methods used to establish styles of offending from the analysis of
behaviours empirically validate the theory or perspective linked to themes within
which they occur.
2 Although these classification systems are built on the analysis of offence behaviours,
protagonists of this approach do not assert that the behaviours occur in a social
vacuum. Indeed, Canter and Heritage’s early model emphasises that rape is a form
of social interaction and that the themes identified are ways in which the offender
interacts with his victim.
98 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
3 These models provide frameworks that can be tested for replicability or used as a
basis for further hypothesis testing.
Practical utility
1 The behavioural models describe the actions occurring at the crime scene, rather
than the rapists’ motivations. Therefore, when new cases are presented to criminal
investigations, police officers and analysts have an objective framework within
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
It is only in the last few decades in Western developed nations that sexual contact
between adults and children has been clearly defined as criminal. The legal definition
has included ‘statutory rape’, which is sexual contact with a child under the age of
consent, and laws forbidding marriage with a child under a designated age. Yet, there
are still many countries and many sub-cultures in which sexual activity with children
is not regarded as criminal. This has become clear as the waves of sexual abuse have
been revealed in country after country. Often, these activities have been implicitly
condoned in organisations, such as parts of the Catholic Church and institutions that
house children, supposedly for their own protection, such as orphanages.
Sexual abuse of children often combines physical and psychological abuse.
Therefore, it is not always possible to determine the impact of illicit sexual activity on
its own. However, all these forms of abuse can have a lasting impact on the victims.
As discussed in earlier chapters, the sources of later criminality of many different sorts
can be found in childhood experiences. Consequently, the long-term impact of abuse
SEXUAL OFFENCES 99
of children can be found not just in the later experiences and activities of the victims,
but in how those victims may often, but certainly not always, perpetuate the cycle of
abuse by assaulting others, causing them to become abusers later.
Although, as in other violent crimes, those who abuse children tend to be men, but
a distinct subset are women, often young women (Giguere & Bumby, 2007). The men
who carry out the abuse often have unusual sexual arousal patterns (Finkelhor, 2008),
but the abuse can also arise in relation to drinking problems, and as mentioned, earlier
experience of abuse themselves. In general, Finkelhor (2008) summarises findings that
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
indicate the following circumstances increase the risk of a child being abused:
• The child lives without one biological parent.
• When the mother is not available, for example, due to employment or illness.
• When the parent’s relationship is unhappy and full of conflict.
• When the child experiences severe discipline.
• When the child has a step-father.
It is important to emphasise that these circumstances do not imply that the abuse
will always take place within the family. It is more that the conditions make the child
vulnerable. Further, the studies summarised by Finkelhor go back over more than a
quarter of a century. Family dynamics are likely to have changed a lot since then. More
up-to-date studies may reveal different patterns.
CONCLUSIONS
Rapists vary considerably in terms of the nature of their relationship to their victim
and the underlying psychological processes that give rise to rape. Overall, the general
view of most theorists is that sexual gratification is only one aspect of the reasons why
rape is carried out. Some people argue that it is almost irrelevant and that power and
control of the victim are much more significant. However, the behaviourally based
models that show the sexual act to be such a prevalent and focal part of the assault do
give a rather different picture.
Books
Amir, M. (1971). Patterns in forcible rape. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Beech, A. R., Craig, L. A., & Browne, K. D. (Eds.). (2009). Assessment and treatment of sex
offenders: a handbook. Chichester: Wiley.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will: men, women and rape. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Canter, D. V. (1994). Criminal shadows. London: Harper Collins.
Groth, A. N. (1979). Men who rape: the psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum Press.
Laws, D. R., & O’Donohue, W. (Eds.). (2016). Treatment of sex offenders: strengths and weaknesses
in assessment and intervention. New York: Springer.
Wilcox, D. T., Garrett, T., & Harkins, L. (Eds.). (2014). Sex offender treatment: a case study approach
to issues and interventions. Chichester: Wiley.
Journal articles
Babchishin, K. M., Hanson, R. K., & VanZuylen, H. (2015). Online child pornography offen-
ders are different: a meta-analysis of the characteristics of online and offline sex offenders
against children. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(1), 45–66.
Canter, D., Bennell, C., Alison, L., & Reddy, S. (2003). Differentiating sex offences: a
behaviourally based thematic classification of stranger rapes. Behavioral Sciences & the Law,
21, 157–74.
Hodge, S., & Canter, D. (1998). Victims and perpetrators of male sexual assault. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 13, 222–39.
Scully, D., & Marolla, J. (1985). Riding the Bull at Gilley’s: convicted rapists describe the
rewards of rape. Social Problems, 32(3), 251–63.
Websites
This website offers legal guidance for rape and sexual offences:
www.cps.gov.uk/legal/p_to_r/rape_and_sexual_offences/
The following website gives an overview of types of rape and sexual assault and practical
guidance for victims:
http://rapecrisis.org.uk/
8 Homicide and
serial killing
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you will be able to:
This chapter provides an overview of homicide and serial killing and relevant research
within a forensic and criminological framework. Various offender and offence
classifications are discussed and evaluated. The advantages and disadvantages of such
classifications in an investigative setting are discussed. This chapter also highlights the
role cultural context may have on the manner in which people behave and interact
with one another, which can play a role in single and multiple homicides. It is
anticipated that this chapter will encourage the reader to seek out and critically evaluate
additional literature in this important area, which greatly affects not only victims and
their family and friends, but also the general public and individual feelings of safety
and security in society.
A great majority of crime fiction features a person who kills again and again. Usually,
the victims have had no direct contact with the murderer (Bartol, 1991; Egger, 1998).
But, this is a total distortion of how murder happens. The great majority of people
are killed by someone they know. Nearly all murderers only murder once. To
understand murder, then, it is essential to distinguish the major forms it can take and
the influence of context.
102 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
When thinking about homicide (i.e., the unlawful killing of another person), it is
crucial that we recognise the differences in the number of murders across countries
and the different rate per head of population this implies as listed in Focus box 8.1.
But these figures hide huge ranges between cities in some countries. For example,
in Mexico, Yucatan has a murder rate of 1.9 per 100,000, 42 cases in 2103, Tujana
had 34.1, 478 cases. These contrast with Ciudad Juarez that had a rate of 147.7, being
1,974 cases. Clearly, there are important social, cultural and political aspects of the
occurrence of murder. As Leyton (1995) argues, it is not the presence of guns, knives
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
or fists that generate homicidal behaviour, but rather ‘the will to use that technology
that is culturally coded, the decision that is half-consciously culturally applauded [that]
shapes the number of homicidal assaults in a nation’ (p. 161). In some cultures, conflict
is dealt with through compromise; in other cultures, it may be dealt with through
violence or displays of power and influence. Although these socio-cultural aspects are
significant, psychological issues are the focus of the current chapter.
frequently occur between current or former romantic partners when the relationship
is failing or has failed (Brookman, 2005). The Home Office Homicide Index from
1995–2000 reports roughly 30 per cent of homicides in England and Wales and 26 per
cent of homicides in Scotland were of this type. Female-on-male homicides
predominantly occur in a domestic setting with a partner or child as the victim. Such
offences comprise about 11 per cent of homicides per year in the UK. Female-on-female
homicides are the least common type of homicide, occurring in less than 3 per cent of
homicides per year in the UK from 1997–2001 (Home Office website given on p. 115).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Spree killing
Another form of murder as typified by Brevik or the horrific number of school
shooting in the USA is when a person kills a number of people in one mass attack,
typically with an automatic firearm. These are appropriately called ‘spree’ killings
because the murderer carries out his (I know of no female spree killers, except those
extremely rare cases where they accompany men) attack in one event against a number
of people usually in one or two places in a short period of time (Delisi & Sherer, 2006).
These killers almost invariably die in the course of their spree, either in a shootout
with law enforcement or by killing themselves. Even if that does not happen, they
expect to die. This has been likened to the Biblical figure of Samson (Canter, 2006)
who killed himself by bringing the temple down on himself and his captors, the
Philistines. Spree killing may, therefore, be usefully thought of as a form of suicide.
Indeed, where the killer deliberately puts himself in a situation where he knows he
will be killed by police, it may be referred to as ‘suicide by cop’.
It is interesting to relate spree killing to the trend over recent years of insurgent,
terrorist groups using suicide bombers as a form of attack. These killers have much in
common with spree killers. Their activities have even been characterised as a Samson
syndrome (Canter, 2006) that also resonates with Second World War Japanese Kamikaze
pilots.
Serial murder
There is no agreement among experts about the number of murders that must occur
before an offender is considered a serial murderer. However, typically, the term serial
murder is applied to killings that occur when three or more victims are killed by the
same person (sometimes pairs of people) over a period of time that can range from
days to years. Usually, to distinguish these from spree killings, between each murder,
there is a ‘cooling off ’ period in which the offender does not kill (Rappaport, 1988;
Gresswell & Hollin, 1994; Jenkins, 1998). Although many serial killings involve evi-
dence of sexual activity before, during and/or after death (Meloy, 2000), not all serial
murderers are sexual murderers.
Massacres
One other example of multiple murder is ‘mass murder’. This can happen in times of
war and in ethnically based conflicts as in Rwanda or the Balkans in recent years, or
Syria today. Often, these may be better thought of as co-ordinated serial killings,
104 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
Voluntary manslaughter:
This is unplanned, but the offender wilfully killed another individual, for
example, during an argument, where the original intent was not to kill (Bartol,
1991, p. 206). The UK Homicide Act (1957, p. 2–4) indicates this can be when
a person is provoked to kill, suffered mental incapacity or being part of a
suicide pact.
FOCUS 8.2
Involuntary manslaughter:
Where there is no intent to kill, but the law finds the individual responsible.
Such as a car accident or work-related accidents and some forms of suicide
pact.
perhaps having more in common with serial killing than is often realised? But, a
particularly interesting example from a psychological point of view is the sort of mass
murder that may be classed as a mass suicide. The Jonestown Massacre in 1978 in which
900 people died in cult commune in North-western Guyana is one example of such
an event. A more contentious example was the siege in 1993 near Waco in Texas.
That ended in seventy-six people being killed in a conflagration initiated during an
FBI assault on the religious community of Branch Davidians, who were storing many
weapons.
All these massacres share the aspect that one inward-looking group determines that
some other group, or society at large, are their enemy. The group-support process
strengthens these attitudes. When the belief system is fuelled by religious beliefs, and
when weapons, especially firearms, are available, a dangerous cocktail is created that
can lead to these mass murders. It is not a far step to see similarities in Nazi atrocities.
The variety of forms of single and multiple murders opens up the likelihood that there
are many different processes that can give rise to homicides. But, as with all other forms
of offence, variations within any given context are helpful in providing insights into
the processes and causes underlying human beings killing each other.
HOMICIDE AND SERIAL KILLING 105
Functions of violence
One of the most cited distinctions in criminality that has been related to killings was
first identified by Fesbach (1964) in relation to aggression in general. He proposed a
distinction between expressive violence, which occurs in response to anger-inducing
conditions, such as insults, physical attack or personal failures, and instrumental
aggression, which arises from the desire for material goods (money, car) or status
(power, prestige). This has been developed to cover killing as well (Salfati, 2000). So,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
for example, when drug gangs kill off the competition, or as in the early work of
Bolitho (1926) called Murder for Profit, people murder tenants to get their money; these
are clear cases of instrumental violence. In contrast, a sudden outburst by one partner
furious with another may be regarded as expressive homicide. In some cases, this latter
may be regarded as manslaughter, or second-degree murder (see Focus box 8.2).
Perhaps, the most obvious form of instrumental murder is what is known as ‘contract
killing’. In these cases, a person hires another person to kill a third person. But, while
it is often assumed that the contracted killer is some anonymous person who is met
clandestinely and paid for his services, many examples exist where the killer is actually
an associate of the person who contracts him (Crumplin, 2009). This means that the
processes understood in other murders between partners or acquaintances may still be
relevant for contract killings.
The issue of rather inappropriately called ‘honour killing’ also raises questions about
what underlies these murders. These are the extremely distressing cases of a family
member, often a daughter, being killed by other members of the family. The reasons
for this are given as being that she was insulting the honour of her family. Typically,
this is because of her relationship with someone the family did not approve of, usually
because he came from a different sect, clan or religion.
Of course, as with all classifications of human activity, simple typologies rarely exist
so purely in practice. For example, a person who is angry because a relative has taken
their inheritance may decide to kill that relative. Is that an expressive or instrumental
murder? There is also the killing of a potential witness, for example, killing a rape
victim. Is that instrumental?
A rather different approach to the interpretation of different forms of violence in
single and multiple homicides is to consider the role of the victim to the offender.
Canter (1994) proposed that there were three distinct roles. This was described in the
previous chapter in Focus box 7.2, but is worth repeating here because of its relevance
to murder.
1 The victim is an object for whom the offender has no sympathy, yet desires control
over. He murders and mutilates without feeling, as these victims are objects to do
with as they desire.
2 The victim is a vehicle upon whom the offender expresses anger and frustration.
These offences are described as taking on a more controlled form than when the
victim is an object. The victim will bear the brunt of the offender’s emotions to
indirectly suffer their pain.
3 The victim is a person with whom the offender tries to establish some kind of rapport.
When Salfati (2000) studied 247 British single offender-single victim homicides,
she showed that the role of the victim, as discussed by Canter (1994), related closely
106 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
In popular crime fiction, the main distinction between types of murder are in terms
of motive. Often, these are summarised as revenge, greed, anger or jealousy. Others
may be offered as well. But, as Wolfgang (1958, p. 185) emphasises, the only way to
identify motive would to be in the suspects head at the time of the offence. Indeed,
in many cases, even that may not help because the killer may not be clear themself as
to why they killed. The identification of motives is open to the danger of subjectivity
(Gibson, 1975, p. 21). There is no objective way to determine the offender’s motives
at the time of the offence. This challenging issue has led some researchers to focus on
more concrete aspects of the offence, particularly victim–offender interaction and
behaviour (Arrigo & Purcell, 2001).
Furthermore, an offence may have several motives. An offender may not be entirely
clear as to why they committed an offence. A motive recorded by police may not
recognise other important behaviours that occurred. For example, a crime recorded
as having a financial motive may have also featured torture, overkill and additional
features, which may contribute significantly to the overall nature of the offence (Keppel
& Walter, 1999 p. 418).
However, the legal process does often make some assumptions about the reasons
for a killing. These assumptions have implications for offender sentencing and
management. Usually, an expressive killing is seen as less serious than one that is
predetermined for greed. The French legal system is even open to a claim that a killing
was a crime passionnel (crime of passion), and therefore, deserving of a lesser sentence,
although it is not a formal term enshrined in law.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
FIGURE 8.3
David Berkowitz.
108 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
HOMICIDE AND SERIAL KILLING 109
A person who is known to be violent, especially if he is quick to lose his temper, may
not surprise people if one of his outbursts results in him killing someone. What is more
difficult to explain is the docile person who one day bursts into murderous violence.
Megargee (1966) proposed a distinction that could help explain this. He identified two
personality types, undercontrolled and overcontrolled, that reflected a person’s ability to control
aggression. Undercontrolled offenders have little control over their aggressive urges, and
consequently, react quickly and hastily. Overcontrolled offenders repeatedly suppress their
anger; the anger builds up to an eventual explosion of violence in response to an event.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
This implicit steam engine analogy of a boiler that constantly emits steam or that is so
constrained that it eventually blows up is very attractive. It has its roots in Freudian ideas
of an id supervised by a superego. But, it is really a labelling of actions that are either
consistently aggressive or suddenly explosive. Without very careful discussions with the
individuals concerned, it is difficult to substantiate.
Although the killing of a number of people by one person over a period of time is
extremely rare, it, nonetheless, is the bread and butter of crime fiction. It seems to
have a fascination for the general population. So, although the prevalence of serial killers
seems to be declining with a general decline in crime in Western democracies, the
rest of this chapter considers the phenomena because of widespread interest in it.
finding that also points to the way offence behaviours are affected by the social
environment in which they occur.
Offender characteristics
Despite public beliefs, serial murderers come from a variety of different ethnic back-
grounds (Egger, 2002; Hickey, 2002; Fox & Levin, 2003), though in Hickey’s study
(2002) of over 200 serial murderers, most were found to be roughly 30-years-old,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Caucasian and male. They had killed between eight and fourteen people over 4 to 6
years. The offenders generally start killing in adulthood, between the ages of 24
and 40 (Jenkins, 1988; Bartol, 1991), though most kill their first victim before the age
of 30 (Meloy, 2000).
A history of violence is often absent from their police records, though offences such
as fraud and theft are not uncommon, and serial offending exists within the offenders’
broader criminal lifestyle (Delisi & Scherer, 2006; p. 273). In a study comparing single
and serial homicide offenders, a greater percentage of serial murderers had pre-
convictions (79 per cent) than single homicide offenders (51 per cent). Canter, Missen
and Hodge (1996) found that 75 per cent of the 217 American serial murderers they
studied had pre-convictions and nearly half had been arrested as juveniles.
These criminal histories of serial killers do suggest that they are part of extremes of
criminality, and therefore, it would be expected that the origins of offending behaviour,
especially violent behaviour, found in less serious aggression would also be present in
serial killers. In McKenzie’s (1995) study of twenty serial murderers, this was found
to be the case. Eighty per cent of the people they studied had experienced family
violence in childhood, 93 per cent had experienced chaotic parenting, and 75 per cent
had at least one alcoholic parent. This, of course, leaves a substantial proportion who
has no obvious family background that would predict serial violence. Explaining how
such people become serial killers is much more difficult.
Mental health
It is often assumed that serial killers are mentally ill, mad, insane or psychotic.
Greswell’s (1991) study of English and Welsh multiple murderers found evidence of
psychiatric history in 45 per cent of cases, and only 24 per cent of offenders had a
history of mental illness. In cases of serial sexual murders, they often had a diagnosis
of sexual sadism and antisocial personality disorder (Gerberth & Turco, 1997). So, once
again, there are important variations between these killers. Some undoubtedly fit the
stereotype of the deranged psychotic, so-often caricatured in films, but many do not fit
this image at all and live as married men with families and survive in social interactions
without drawing attention to themselves.
Although it is thought that serial murderers are inevitably psychopaths, that is not
always the case. There, certainly, are examples of serial murderers who display the
characteristics of superficial charm, intelligence, lack of remorse, impulsivity and asso-
ciated psychopathic traits. However, there are those diagnosed as psychopathic who
are not serial murderers. One danger of the use of this term, or any other psychiatric
diagnosis, is that it is used as the sole explanation for the behaviours, rather than being
understood as an important feature in a series of unfolding events (Mitchell, 1997).
HOMICIDE AND SERIAL KILLING 111
ACTIVITY 8.1
The Internet is full of accounts of serial killers, their crimes and lives. Review those
you can find to determine whether accounts from totally different sources agree
with each other about the details. Then, determine what the typical background
characteristics are of the killers described. Also, review their victims. Do the victims
have anything in common?
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Victim characteristics
Most serial murder victims are women, particularly prostitutes, and those living on
the edges of society (Jenkins, 1988; Ressler, Douglas, Burgess & Burgess, 1992; Egger,
1998, p. 79). These individuals are more vulnerable and their absence is less likely to
be noticed. In cases of sexual homicide, the victims may be both strangers or acquaint-
ances and are often of the same race as the offender (Dietz, Hazelwood & Warren
1990; Meloy, 2000). This points to the way in which serial killers prey on vulnerable
people. They take advantage of the weaknesses of their victims. As a consequence,
where there are more of those victims available and law enforcement agencies less able
to investigate murders, there are likely to be more serial killers active. This is illustrated
by the much higher rate of serial killers in Russia and South Africa than in many other
countries.
Serial murder typologies
Classifications of serial murders contribute to an understanding of what gives rise
to them (Megargee, 1982; Arrigo & Purcell, 2001). Some of these classification
schemes have also been developed to aid investigations (Egger, 1984). Many typologies
classify offenders on the basis of a mixture of features, including inferred motives,
crime scene evidence and offender background characteristics. This is problematic
for the development of systematic tests of these typologies because they mix object-
ively based definitions, such as the gender of victims, with subjective interpretations,
such as motivations. Such classification schemes are also of limited practical use,
especially to investigators, because the only objective data are those drawn from crime
scenes (Canter & Wentink, 2004).
The organised/disorganised binary classification is the most frequently cited serial
killer typology. It was constructed by a number of special agents of the Federal Bureau
of Investigation (Ressler et al., 1986). This dichotomy rests on the assumption that the
offender’s behaviour and personality characteristics can be determined from crime
scene information. It is claimed that the organised offender leads a planned and orderly
life, which is reflected in the way they commit their crimes. They are described as
having average to high intelligence, likely to be employed and confident. The
organised crime scene shows evidence of planning, use of restraints and use of a weapon
brought by the killer, and subsequently, removed from the crime scene. The
disorganised offender, it is proposed, has low intelligence, lacks social confidence and
is less likely to be employed. Their crime scenes reflect features such as little to no
112 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
planning, a sense of disorder and careless leaving of blood, semen and/or the murder
weapon at the scene.
But, while there is an attractive simplicity to this dichotomy, it has many weak-
nesses, both in terms of the data on which it was based (Turco, 1990; Rossmo, 1997)
and lack of systematic analysis of that data. When careful data are collected and
statistically analysed, no evidence can be found for the distinction. In their study of
100 serial murders examining the co-occurrence of characteristics purported for each
type of offender, Canter, Alison, Alison and Wentink (2004) revealed that the
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
behavioural data did not indicate a dichotomy that distinguished between offenders.
Rather, the research showed that a subset of organised features are typical of most serial
murders, thereby facilitating offenders to successfully complete the crime and remain
undetected.
Disorganised features were not found to be a distinct type. Rather, offenders differed
in the forms of their disorganisation that Canter, Giles & Nicol (2004) labelled
execution, mutilation, sexual control and plunder. This study is one of many examples
of the ways in which the offender and offence typologies can be empirically examined
and evaluated as a first step towards developing more precise investigative tools and
ways to understand a constellation of offending behaviour.
TABLE 8.1 The relationship between Holme’s types of serial killer and the
organised/disorganised dichotomy (From Canter & Youngs, 2009, p. 343)
Feature Types of serial killers
Visionary Mission Comfort Lust Thrill Power
Disorganised X
Spontaneous X
Random X X
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Non-specific X X
Affiliated X
Act-focused X X X
Concentrated X X X X
Strangers X X X X X
Organised X X X X X
Planned X X X X
Specific X X X X
Process-focused X X X X
Non-random X X X
Dispersed X X
CONCLUSIONS
People kill each other for many different reasons. This chapter has only dealt with what
are called ‘extra-judicial’ killings. Those are homicides that are against the law of the
land. But, across the world, many countries have legal killings for reasons that would be
abhorrent in other countries. For example, some countries under strict Moslem law will
execute people found guilty of blasphemy, adultery or homosexuality. The US state of
Texas recognises twelve different situations in which it is legitimate to kill someone, for
example, a criminal running away from a crime. Situations that would not be regarded as
legitimate in other countries.
So, it is worth noting that the very first interpersonal crime in the Old Testament is
the murder of Able by his brother Cain. Therefore, this most fundamental and serious
type of human crimes has to be regarded as part of all societies and centuries.
Consequently, in this brief chapter, only the range of activities that constitute murder
can be touched on and only an outline to understanding this crime can be given. Of
importance is to distinguish between murder in fiction and in fact. Just about every
fictional account of crime, in novels, on television and in films, is built around murder.
Even Shakespeare’s tragedies are full of murders. This creates an environment in
which people are aware of fictional killing that can distort the understanding of
actual murder.
114 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
As a way forward from the fiction, this chapter has discussed various offender and
offence classifications for homicide and serial killing. Issues concerning offender
classifications have been highlighted. This chapter has also noted that situational and
cultural factors play a role in the emergence of homicide events.
Books
Bartol, C. R. (1991). Criminal behaviour: a psychosocial approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Brookman, F. (2005). Understanding homicide. Portland, OR: Sage.
Canter, D. (1994). Criminal shadows. London: HarperCollins.
Fitz-Gibbon, K., & Walklate, S. (Eds.). (2016). Homicide, gender and responsibility: an international
perspective. London: Routledge.
Hickey, E. (2002). Serial murderers and their victims, 3rd ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Lester, D. (1995). Serial killers. Philadelphia, PA: The Charles Press.
Ressler, R. R., Burgess, A. W., & Douglas, J. E. (1988). Sexual homicide. Lexington, MA:
Lexington Books.
Journal articles
Canter, D. V, Alison, L. J., Alison, E. & Wentink, N. (2004). The organized/disorganized
typology of serial murder: myth or model? Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 10(3),
293–320.
Delisi, M., & Sherer, A. M. (2006). Multiple homicide offenders: offence characteristics, social
correlates, and criminal careers. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 33(3), 367–91.
HOMICIDE AND SERIAL KILLING 115
Gresswell, D. M., & Hollin, C. R. (1994). Multiple murder: a review. British Journal of
Criminology, 34, 1–14.
Keppel, R. D., & Walter, R. (1999). Profiling killers: a revised classification model for
understanding sexual murder. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology, 43(4), 417–37.
Meloy, J. R. (2000). The nature and dynamics of sexual homicide: an integrative review.
Aggression and Violent Behaviour, 5(1), 1–22.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Websites
A Google Scholar search for ‘serial killers’ generates over 30,000 hits showing the fascination
with this very rare type of homicide. Much of the information in this material is a regurgitation
of popular misconceptions about serial killers. But, if you want descriptions of the most notorious
of these very unusual killers, you can find them at:
http://list25.com/25-most-evil-serial-killers-of-the-20th-century/2/
An overview of homicide in the UK is given by the very thorough Home Office Homicide
Index at:
http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20160105160709/http://www.ons.gov.uk/
ons/dcp171776_352260.pdf
9 Gangs, organised
crime and terrorism
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
1 Understand how crime is much more than just a lone activity or simply the result
of rational or pathological processes.
2 Appreciate different levels of social analysis, from individuals to co-offending
groups and teams, to organisations and networks.
3 Understand the difference between criminal gangs and criminal teams.
4 Appreciate the structure of various gang activities.
5 Describe the various explanations for terrorism.
SUMMARY
The term ‘gang’ is clearer in fiction than in systematic research. (But see Focus box
9.1 for one approach to determining if a gang exists). Also, just because a group of
GANGS, ORGANISED CRIME AND TERRORISM 117
youngsters hangout together, certainly does not mean they are delinquent or criminals.
Researchers, therefore, tend to just ask people ‘Do you belong to a gang?’ Apparently,
answers tend to accord with who police officers consider to be gang members (Curry,
2000).
Until recently, most research on gangs was carried out in the United Sates where
over 26,000 gangs of youths have been identified involving more than three-quarters
of a million people (Egley & O’Donnell, 2008). In the UK, the police in Glasgow
estimate there are as many as 170 gangs there, with 3,500 members aged between 11
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Social organisation
There are many levels of social organisation involved in crimes, where a number of
individuals are working together when committing crimes. When it is a small number
of people working together, an important distinction is between transient groups and
established teams. The term group implies a rather informal or casual set of people who
do not necessarily have clear assignment of roles. In contrast, a team implies regular
roles and shared ideas of offending activities (Cannon-Bowers, Salas & Converse, 1993).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
When people have distinct roles within a team, they are more likely to share
attitudes towards others and the identity of the team. If these roles are clearly defined
and complementary, they will promote cohesion and a distinct sense of being part of
something. This will further enhance mechanisms of control and processes of social
influence, especially conformity to the team’s norms and leadership.
One interesting example of how criminal teams are organised can be taken from
armed robbery. Most robbery teams consist of at least three members; a leader who
organises the ‘heist’ and may take the money, a ‘heavy’ who ensures the robbers are
not disturbed and a getaway driver. Their criminal records often reflect crimes
consistent with their roles. For example, the ‘heavy’ is likely to have a history of
violence and the getaway driver a background in stealing cars. Even when there are
more members, the number of roles is fairly consistent (Einstadter, 1969; McCluskey
& Wardle, 2000). This reflects considerable role differentiation and division of labour,
creating behavioural norms for each member and giving the group structure.
Roles arise from the demands of the task, and so, different crimes will involve more
or less role differentiation, while individual expertise and interactions determine role
allocation, and the experience of offending together gives role clarity (the transition
from group to team). Situational demands also affect role expression in terms of
representing different challenges to criminals’ goals and eliciting particular behavioural
strategies.
This difference in structure was demonstrated by Canter (2004), who showed that
groups of football hooligans were organised much more loosely than drug dealers. He
used social network analysis, as discussed below, to examine twelve drug-dealer
networks, eleven property crime networks and six hooligan networks. The results
revealed a wide range of group structures, ranging from very loose networks without
even central key figures through to highly structured networks that contained many
refinements of network structure. Interestingly, he demonstrated that as the size of the
group increased, so did the degree of formal structure to its organisation. This lead
Canter (2004) to propose three types of criminal groups:
• Ad hoc groups that were only loosely organised.
• Oligarchies that had a strong clique in overall command.
• Organised criminals, who had a clearly hierarchical structure.
Canter’s (2004) study supports many other studies that show that even in more
complex crimes, well-organised teams may be less common because many co-offenders
operate as transient groups (Reiss, 1988). Often, groups of individuals act in relation
to dynamics within the group. For example, one influential member may instigate
events, while others act as facilitators and supporters, conforming and imitating in order
that their group membership is not withdrawn. Such leading by example has been
GANGS, ORGANISED CRIME AND TERRORISM 119
documented in delinquent groups and also adult gang rapes and robberies (Warr, 1996;
Porter & Alison, 2001; Porter & Alison, 2006). Thus, co-offending behaviour is the
product of mutual influence and interaction between group members, which is, in
part, determined by their goals and their degree of organisation, as well as situational
demands including task complexity and victim reactions.
ORGANISED CRIME
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
When criminals work together over a period of time in order to profit from illegal
activities, their activities are usually given the label of organised crime. Organised crimes
are distinct from traditional crimes (such as larceny and assault) because they involve
planning, multiple participants and require corrupt measures, such as threats, to main-
tain and protect their ongoing criminal activities.
Some of the groups of criminals working together will be part of a network of
people that is indeed structured like a commercial enterprise. They will have an overall
manager and subordinates, people to look after the finances, those who fetch and carry
and so on. However, many criminal networks are much looser than that, with
associations that vary over time and in relation to different types of crime.
Mars (2000) draws on social anthropology, especially Mary Douglas’ (1978) cultural
theory, to distinguish four categories of criminal organisation, which he says also applies
to terrorist organisations. These categories are generated by two dimensions, one of
the strength of the group, the other the degree of classification within the group. The
‘pure’ types generated by this matrix are:
• Autonomy – Criminal individualists, in which constraints on group members’
activities are weak and assignment of individuals to distinct roles is limited. This
is similar to Canter’s (2004) ad hoc groups.
• Atomised – Isolated criminal, in which ‘hangers on’, who tend to be petty
criminals, are taken advantage of by more organised criminals.
• Criminal ideologues – Bounded but egalitarian, in which there is little internal
ranking of individuals, but they are held together by a commitment to an
ideology.
• Criminal hierarchies – in which the group has clear boundaries, but different levels
within, similar to Canter’s (2004) organised criminals.
This fourfold framework, generated by two dimensions, is illustrated in Figure 9.1.
TERRORISM
There are dozens of definitions of terrorism, but the seven crucial characteristics
are given in Focus box 9.3. The central idea behind these characteristics is that the
acts of violence are a political strategy carried out by groups that are not themselves
an established state. From this perspective, the strategy is very much aimed at carrying
out actions that have symbolic, propaganda significance. That is what distinguishes
them from organised criminals. But, it does not exclude organised criminal actions that
terrorist groups may carry out, for example, kidnap for ransom (Phillips, 2009).
120 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
Isolates Hierarchies
Degree of classification
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Individualists Ideologues
ACTIVITY 9.1
3 Communicates a message.
4 Act and victim symbolically significant.
5 Carried out by sub-state groups.
6 Victim is different from the audience.
7 Deliberate targeting of civilians.
It is also important to point out, as Horgan (2005) does, that gaining access to people
involved in terrorism may be dangerous, and is often a lengthy drawn-out process.
It is, therefore, understandable that most researchers are reluctant to follow this path
and rely instead on secondary and tertiary sources. This is one of the reasons why public
understanding of terrorists is often so misinformed, making it prey to the political
distortions that Danis and Stohl (2009) explore.
Statements made by terrorists, notably those by suicide bombers recorded for
broadcast after their death, suffer from similar difficulties. Merari (1990) suggests that
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
the preparation of such a statement is part of the process by which the bomber is tied
into the intended act. By committing themself in writing or on video to the action,
it is much more difficult to back out at a later stage without a tremendous loss of face.
It is also difficult to generalise from what is known about the psychology of one
set of terrorists to others. The changing world scene and evolving social processes also
mean that there is unlikely to be one psychology relevant to all groups. Yet, despite
these many difficulties in obtaining detailed information, a slowly growing series of
studies can be put alongside what is known about other acts of violence and of suicide,
to sketch a picture of how people find their way into these activities and what may
lead them out.
EXPLANATIONS OF TERRORISM
over 50 years of study of how people tend to compare their own experiences with
those of others that they know about and assess their personal deprivation relative to
those other experiences. Smith and Leach (2004) show that when a person’s identity
is closely associated with membership of a particular group, then the belief that the
group as a whole experiences certain deprivations can have a significant influence on
that person’s levels of dissatisfaction beyond their own personal comparisons.
Brainwashing
The graphic metaphor for clearing a person of previously held beliefs, washing their
brains, in order to insert some alien set of perspectives, has become a further explanation
of how people could turn from reasonably well-adjusted citizens to violent terrorists.
This perspective puts people, such as Osama Bin Laden and the Hamas leader Sheikh
Yassin, in roles that have only ever been demonstrated clearly in George Du Maurier’s
fictional story of the control of the opera singer Trilby by the manipulative Svengali.
Many other studies show that in real life, it is difficult to demonstrate the possibility
of such quasi-occult powers (cf. Heap & Kirsch, 2006, for a review of these issues).
This view of the influence of terrorist leaders also implies a very strong hierarchy,
very similar indeed to what would be expected in a rigid military structure. In general,
however, as with all illegal groups, as discussed above, especially those spread over a
wide geography, it is not possible to maintain the top-down discipline that is de rigueur
for a standing army. Instead, what Atran (2004) calls a ‘hydra-headed network’ is much
more likely to be the norm.
124 PSYCHOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CRIMES
that do interpret the Quran as endorsing violence against non-believers. But, the
dominant Islamic tradition is, nonetheless, extremely tolerant.
Even when considering suicide bombers, there is nothing new or particularly Islamic
about them. There is the ancient Jewish exemplar in the biblical account of Samson
bringing the temple down upon the Philistines as a way of escaping from his own
degradation and death at their hands. In modern time, as Gunaratna (2000) documents,
suicide bombing is certainly not limited to Moslem terrorists. The Kurdistan Worker’s
Party (PKK) that has carried out many suicide bombings in Turkey is anti-religious.
What Gunaratna (2003) calls ‘ethno-nationalism’, rather than religious doctrine, is what
drove the ‘Tamil Tigers’ to carry out suicide bombing in Sri Lanka and India.
Terrorism as a process
In his earlier writing, Horgan (2005) has developed the important argument that
terrorism is not an act, but a process of which a person is a part. Moghaddam’s (2005)
‘staircase’ model of terrorism makes a similar point of a person entering into and
becoming part of an ever more involving commitment to violence for political or
ideological ends. The framework for this is a mixture of first, recognition that the group
with which the terrorist identifies is distinct from and threatened by some other external
group, and second, that there are culturally remembered or experienced causes of
grievance that are nursed by the group to which the terrorist belongs.
The grievances become a way of specifying the special, distinct identity of the group
and what fuels its antagonism to those who the group sees as their enemies. An import-
ant point here is that it is not the deprivation or other degrading experiences themselves
that are seen as central causes of acts of violence, but the interpretation of these.
The grievance, however, only has to give rise to a few people acting violently before
the explosive mix of emotional turmoil and direct experience draws others into the
destructive cycle. Ferguson, Burgess and Hoyywood (2008) show how for many people
it is the psychological consequences of being part of violence that itself can set up a
continuing process of terrorist aggression. For these individuals, the world becomes
framed in terms of its potential for bloodshed.
This process of identification with a particular, violent group can take many and
often unexpected forms and is currently, certainly, not limited to jihadi factions of Islam.
Borgeson and Valeri (2009) describe a Christian group that is given little coverage in
most reviews of terrorism, but which has been responsible, directly or indirectly, for
a number of atrocities, most notably the Oklahoma City bombing by Timothy
McVeigh.
The essentially criminal nature of their actions can readily overlap with what is more
commonly thought of as organised crime. As Vishnevetsky (2009) shows in his fascin-
ating account of youth gangs in Chechnya, organised crime may be the route into
GANGS, ORGANISED CRIME AND TERRORISM 125
terrorism for some individuals. This accords with the Gupta et al.’s (2009) argument
that distinguishing between terrorists and organised criminals is indeed difficult.
The spread of interactions between terrorism and organised crime is also paralleled
in the ways in which terrorist acts drift into state structures, being covertly or overtly
supported by national authorities. In this regard, the actions of Chechyian leader Shamil
Basayev, who masterminded the Budennovsk, Dubrovka and Beslan suicidal, hostage-
taking raids as acts against Russian control of Chechnya, would place those actions
outside of Richardson’s (2006) (Focus box 9.3) definition of terrorism. However, most
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
of the processes that can be recognised in more obvious terrorist groups were present
in these violent attacks.
When a group of people are interacting together, they form a network of contacts.
This is open to study and analysis in order to reveal the structure of those contacts. This
can help to identify who the leaders of the network are and have a central role in com-
munications. It can also reveal whether there are cliques of individuals who are in close
contact with each other, forming a team that may, in effect, manage the network.
People on the fringe of the network who have little contact with more than one person
can also be clearly exposed by this analysis, helping to identify individuals who may
have less commitment to the organisation.
O. B. LADEN
MOTASSADEQ’S WIFE
AL-MIDHAR’S FAMILY
BAHAJI’S WIFE M. ATEF
K. S. MOHAMMED
S
S.BAHAJI
BAHAJI
KAL-MIDHAR
M. MOTASSADEQ
R. B. AL-SHIBH N. AL-HAZMI
A. SENGUEN
A. MZOUDI M. BELFAS F. BANIHAMMAD
Z. JARRAH
LINK TO AL-QAEDA
Z. ESSABAR M. AL-SHEHHI M. AL-SHEHRI
M. ATTA
AL-OMARI’S FAMILY
S. AL-HAZMI
M. H. ZAMMAR
M. MOQED A. A. AL-OMARI
M. O. SLAHI
S. SUQAMI
K. AL-MASRI A. AL-NAMI
H. AL-GHAMDI
NO INVOLVEMENT
S. AL-GHAMDI
SECONDARY PLOT FACILITATOR H. HANJOUR WD. AL-SHEHRI
MAJOR PLOT FACILITATOR A. AL-GHAMDI
PLOT PERPETRATOR A. AL-HAZNAWI W. AL-SHEHRI
FIGURE 9.3 Spring 2000 during the development of the 11 September terrorist network. Key members of the network are represented as
nodes. The ties connecting them signify different kinds of relationship between members, with thicker ties representing
stronger psychological and emotional bonds.1
GANGS, ORGANISED CRIME AND TERRORISM 127
SNA
At its simplest, SNA involves no more than identifying the presence or absence of
a link between nodes such as whether two people were arrested for the same
crime. This results in a matrix of zeros and ones, which is converted into a diagram
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
depicting the overall social structure of who was arrested with whom. Figure 9.3
demonstrates this, based on knowledge of personal relationships, but uses a third
level of association to indicate ‘stronger’ bonds between certain network members,
and is part of a sequence of network diagrams.
Networks are typically ever-changing fluid structures, as relationships between
people change. Network visualisations, combined with quantitative network
measures, help identify central or peripheral members, overall cohesion,
subgroups, roles and channels of communication and resource flow.
To learn more about SNA, there is freely available software such as UCINET
(Borgatti, Everett & Freeman, 2002) available online. Also, see recommendations
for further reading.
CONCLUSIONS
This chapter has served to highlight the importance of a social psychological perspec-
tive for comprehending the antecedents and functioning of criminal behaviour. The
surface has barely been scratched in terms of the theory and research presented
here; however, the reader is encouraged to delve deeper into these introductory topics
and also to reflect upon how any piece of social psychological research might apply to
the criminal world and how it may ‘fit’ with the existing theory. Currently, a good deal
of extrapolation is involved from studies of ‘legitimate’ social interactions, but with
increasing recognition of this relevance, the social psychology of crime will be more
directly explored.
Books
Canter, D. (2000). Destructive organisational psychology. In D. Canter & L. Alison (Eds.), The
social psychology of crime: groups, teams and networks. Farnham: Ashgate.
Canter, D., & Alison, L. (2000). The social psychology of crime: groups, teams & networks. Farnham:
Ashgate.
Canter, D. (Ed.). (2009). The faces of terrorism: multidisciplinary perspectives. Chichester: Wiley.
Harding, S. (Ed.). (2016). Global perspectives on youth gang behavior, violence, and weapons use.
Hershey, PA: IGI Global.
Horgan, J. (2014). The psychology of terrorism. London: Routledge.
Miller, J. (2001). One of the guys: girls, gangs, and gender. New York: Oxford University Press.
Wasserman, S., & Faust, K. (1997). Social network analysis: structural analysis in the social sciences.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Journal articles
Einstadter, W. (1969). The social organisation of armed robbery. Social Problems, 17(1), 64–83.
Hanneman, R., & Riddle, M. (2005). Introduction to social network methods, Riverside, CA:
University of California. Published online; available at http://faculty.ucr.edu/~hanneman/
nettext/.
Horgan, J., & Braddock, K. (2010). Rehabilitating the terrorists? Challenges in assessing the
effectiveness of de-radicalization programs. Terrorism and Political Violence, 22(2), 267–91.
Weerman, F. (2003). Co-offending as social exchange: explaining characteristics of co-offending.
British Journal of Criminology, 43(2), 398–416.
Note
1 Figure 9.3 is one of a sequence of SNA diagrams used to model the dynamic development
of this network over time. An in-depth account of this development is given by Mullins,
(2009), ‘Terrorist Networks and Small Group Psychology’ in Canter, D. (Ed.) Faces of
Terrorism: Cross-Disciplinary Explorations. Wiley.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
3 criminals
Interacting with
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
10 Testimony and
interviewing
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
The initial information about a crime comes from a report by a person; this then
becomes important for legal proceedings as testimony. A growing number of studies
have revealed some weaknesses in important aspects of testimony, including when a
person gives an account of what has been seen: eyewitness testimony, and how that
testimony may be distorted inadvertently. Chapter 11 considers the major topic of how
to determine whether the testimony has been distorted deliberately, through deception
or lying.
Interviewing is usually the starting point for obtaining testimony. The process
consists of helping people to remember, but also is an interpersonal transaction of some
sensitivity, which varies depending on whether a victim, witness or suspect is being
interviewed. To facilitate these matters, various procedures have been developed
including the ‘cognitive interview’ and in the UK the PEACE protocol.
TESTIMONY
remember happened. Typically, that is derived from an account they have given during
an earlier stage in the investigation. In some places, testimony is derived from an earlier
interview or by video to the court, or by a written account of what the person remem-
bers. Testimony can, therefore, be seen as a form of memory retrieval. Consequently,
any consideration of testimony draws on the psychology of remembering.
There are two distinct aspects of testimony. One is remembering what happened
and giving an account of that. The other is identifying some person or aspect of the
event witnessed from options being made available. The most common is selecting
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
a person from a line-up who is remembered as being present at a crime. It could also
be a weapon used or a make of a car, but these are rarely studied.
Memory has been a subject of psychological research since the earliest days of
modern psychology (Ebbinghaus, 1895), but the research developed significantly nearly
a century ago when Bartlett (1932) declared that memory is ‘far more decisively an
affair of reconstruction than one of mere reproduction’. In other words, the popular
notion that memory is some sort of recording process that fades with time, like printed
photographs when exposed to sunlight, is misleading. People build up their account
of what they remember in a variety of ways. This reconstruction is, therefore, open
to various forms of distortion, rather than just lacking detail over time.
earlier. But, there are drawbacks, usually expressed as a lack of ‘ecological validity’.
This means that the conditions do not reflect various important aspects of what
actually happens in the ‘real world’, outside of a laboratory experiment.
• Students are typically used as participants, which are not representative in age,
background or other aspects of the rest of the population.
• Laboratory experiments generate various ‘expectancy effects’ (see Research
methods box 10.2). The participants assume what it is that the experimenter is
looking for and inadvertently provide the sorts of answers
they think are expected.
• The nature of the crime to be witnessed is severely limited; it cannot, for
example, be too traumatic or extreme, for ethical and practical reasons.
• The witness’s role in the event is distant and external. There would be no
reason, for instance, for the witness to lie as may be the case in a real crime.
• The interview procedure is not constrained by the sorts of administrative and
legal demands that happen when a witness is interviewed by police officers.
• When a participant is required to act the part of an offender, their ‘crimes’
cannot be high stake or of great significance to them if they are caught out.
(This is even more relevant for study of deception in the next chapter.)
more complex than laboratory studies, although have the advantage of being more
realistic, more ‘ecologically valid’, and therefore, more reliably applicable outside
the laboratory.
These can be ‘field experiments’ in which the study is quite well controlled, but
takes place in a real-life situation. For example, an individual may purchase an item
from a shopkeeper in a shop and the shopkeeper is later asked to identify the
shopper from a photographic line-up.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
There are two crucial aspects of what eyewitnesses report. One is how much detailed
information they provide. The second is how accurate it is. Because the memory is
being reconstructed, it is open to influence from many different aspects of the event
as well as the processes used to remember. Influences subsequent to the event can also
lead to distortion in what people think they remember.
Many years of research on memory have identified four stages that influence how
well something is remembered:
• Acpects of the event – Factors relating to the event.
• Aspects of the person – Characteristics of the witness.
• Retention – How well the information is retained.
• Retrieval – How the information is retrieved.
• Obstruction – The fewer obstructions to the witnesses view, the better they will
remember the event.
• Known or seen before – If a witness has seen an offender before, they will be
more likely to remember that person when they see them again in different
situations.
• Any reason to remember – If something is salient or novel, then it is more likely to
be ‘memorable’ for the witness.
• Time lapsed – The greater the amount of time between an event and attempts to
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
recall it, the worse their memory for that event is likely to be.
• Errors or material discrepancies – If parts of a witness’s testimony are inaccurate, then
other aspects of their testimony are also likely to be inaccurate.
Other major event factors influencing eyewitness performance are stress or fear, and
violence. The more stressful or intense something is for a witness, the greater the impact
that it is likely to have on them and the more likely they are to remember it. For
example, violent events tend to be remembered better than non-violent events.
However, what has come to be known as ‘weapon focus’ has been identified as
occurring in situations where a weapon is used and the anxiety associated with that
may cause the witness to narrow their focus to looking at the weapon. Consequently,
other information relating to the event may not be remembered as well.
Retrieval factors
• Actions versus descriptions – Generally, witnesses are good at describing what hap-
pened during an event, but encounter more problems in describing the physical
characteristics of the perpetrator.
• Confidence and accuracy – Research has shown that the confidence of an eyewitness
in their testimony and recall is not necessarily related to the accuracy of what they
report. However, this relationship is complex (Kebbell & Wagstaff, 1999) and tends
to be malleable – it is easily influenced by, for example, comments or feedback
from interviewers.
Other factors influencing the retrieval of information include those relating to the
questioning of the witness, such as method of questioning and the influence of the
person asking the questions, see Research methods box 10.2.
Even subtle aspects of the procedure used to obtain identification can influence
the selection the witness makes. A number of general aspects of the commonly used
line-up procedure have been recognised as causing unwanted influences. These
include:
• The format of the line-up from which the witness is asked to make a selection and
the people that compose the line-up.
• The instructions given to the witness prior to the line-up.
• The influence of line-up administrators.
• Exposure to suspects or people featuring in the line-up previously.
To reduce unwanted effects on eyewitness identifications and reduce the risks of
mistaken identifications, the following proposals have been made:
• Individuals in actual or photographic line-ups should be presented sequentially.
• The individual conducting the line-up should not know the identity of the actual
suspect.
• Witnesses should be warned that the suspect may or may not be in the line-up.
• Additional line-up members should be selected based on the eyewitness’s
description of the perpetrator.
• No feedback should be given during or after a line-up.
TESTIMONY AND INTERVIEWING 137
Indirect influence
In a study carried out by Canter and his colleagues in preparation for the appeal
by Mr Al Megrahi who was convicted of the Lockerbie bombing (full report
available online at https://tinyurl.com/pkrncbm), respondents were given twelve
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
pictures of possible suspects and asked to guess which one was the actual
convicted bomber.
In the study using the photographs actually used in the investigation leading
to Al Megrahi’s conviction, for one condition, the interviewer was told who the
actual bomber was believed to be, but told not to tell anyone. In the other
condition, a different interviewer was not given any information.
The results (reported in Canter, Hammond & Youngs 2013) show very clearly
the indirect influence of the interviewer who knew the ‘expected’ answer.
TABLE 10.1 Frequencies of people selecting a target from a set of twelve under
different interviewer conditions.
Condition Someone Target Totals
other selected
than from
suspect line-up
selected
from
line-up
Interviewer did not know identity of suspect 20 0 20
in line-up
Interviewer knew identity of suspect in line-up 21 15 36
Total 41 15 56
FIGURE 10.1 A police ‘identification parade’. What weaknesses can you see in using this
line-up to help a witness recognise the offender?
ACTIVITY 10.1
Table Sit
Legs Seat
Couch Desk
Recliner Sofa
Wood Cushion
Swivel Stool
Sitting Rocking
Bench
(Roediger & McDermott, 1995).
How many did you remember? Check the words you wrote down against the list
of words here. Are there any words in your list that were not in the original list?
TESTIMONY AND INTERVIEWING 139
There almost certainly will be, especially the word ‘chair’. This and other words
you thought you remembered are those with similar meanings to the underlying
idea behind the list. This shows that your memory relates to what the words have
in common; what they mean. It is not just a passive process.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
INTERVIEWING
Given what has been said about memory and its weaknesses, a great deal of research
has been conducted over recent years to improve the way in which memories are
obtained in interviews. For although talking to a person may seem an everyday activity,
when this becomes an important investigative tool, it is open to study, development
and training. The study of investigative interviewing has, therefore, become an
important area of research, which is having a significant influence on police procedures
in many countries.
Victims and witnesses may be fearful of the process and even anxious that they will
be intimidated, as well as the problems of remembering what has occurred as discussed
above. Various procedures have, therefore, been developed to improve the effective-
ness of the interview. In most jurisdictions, these procedures operate within legally
defined boundaries. If the interview is not carried out as the law requires, then evidence
gained using it may not be allowed in court. In the UK, police officers are trained
in a procedure developed as part of the Police and Criminal Evidence Act (PACE)
1984 codes of practice. The main framework for this is known as PEACE as listed
in Table 10.2.
Broadly speaking, how questions are formulated can have a substantial effect on the
kinds of information produced during an interview.
Open questions such as ‘What happened?’ produce fuller accounts.
Closed questions such as ‘Were you scared?’ are likely to produce simple ‘yes’ and
‘no’ answers, which will not give much detail.
Leading questions, such as the notorious ‘When did you stop beating your wife?’,
will produce accounts that suffer greatly from misinformation effects, and would not
be acceptable in most courts.
a large and significant increase in correct details and a small increase in errors. In
addition we found that there were no differences in the rate at which details are
confabulated. The CI appeared to benefit older adult witnesses even more than
younger adults.
The origins of the CI in laboratory studies and the experimental literature studying
it have given the procedure considerable support. Nonetheless, like many other
psychological procedures that emerge from university laboratories, there are difficulties
in practice of using the procedure by actual police investigators (Kebbell, Milne &
Wagstaff, 1999).
Perhaps, not surprisingly, in practice, it is often difficult for police interviewers to
follow all the stages of the CI. In particular, it is rare for them to evaluate their
interviews because of pressures to complete paperwork and move on to the next case.
False memory
False memories occur when a person remembers something that has not
actually occurred. The most famous example of false memories can be found
in the Loftus and Palmer (1974) study where participants were asked to
watch a video of an automobile accident, then asked questions about the
footage. Participants reported aspects of the accident they could not possibly
have seen, with confidence. This occurred through a process known as
‘misinformation’ where participants are led to believe they have a memory
by the way in which a question is phrased.
Another aspect of false memories is that of ‘recovered memories’. These
particular forms of false memory came to the fore with a number of well-
FOCUS 10.2
Interviewing children
The use of children in criminal proceedings is uncommon and usually reserved for
serious offences, such as sexual assault. Children rarely appear in an actual court in
the UK, and it is far more likely that they will be interviewed in a separate room,
and the interview video-recorded. This provides a less intimidating atmosphere and
is, therefore, more likely to be successful.
It may be necessary to use an interview supporter, interpreter or intermediary in
an interview to ensure that a child can understand what is being asked of them. It may
also be useful to use props, such as dolls, in the interview.
The UK Home Office suggests that, when interviewing children, four main phases
should be followed:
• Establishing rapport with the child.
• Ask the child to provide a free narrative account of what happened.
• Asking specific questions based on the free narrative provided.
• Closure (e.g., post-interview counselling).
142 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
INTERVIEWING SUSPECTS
Many police officers view suspect interrogations as being the single-most important
stage in the investigative process (Baldwin, 1993). Almost all suspects are interviewed
at some stage in the judicial process, regardless of the offence type, and it is often at
this point that a suspect’s fate is sealed. Yet, there has been continued debate as to
what the central purposes of conducting a suspect interview are: is the aim to induce
a confession, or to get to the truth at the heart of the investigation? Research has found
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
that the majority of police officers view securing a confession as the principle purpose
of an interview. Baldwin (1993) shows that many officers approach interviews anti-
cipating a confession. Confessions are considered beneficial because securing a
confession reduces the need for further enquiries, although as discussed below, the
possibility of false confessions cannot be ignored. Therefore, in any civilised society,
the police will be required to demonstrate that there is evidence, from what is said,
or other sources to support the confession.
CONFESSING
So, what might make a suspect confess? Gudjonsson (1992) proposes the following
reasons:
• The suspect perceives the evidence against them to be very strong, and sees denial
as futile.
• The suspect is sorry for their crime and wants to explain what happened.
• The subject is reacting to pressure, for example, from the police or the stress of
confinement.
Strong evidence carefully and clearly presented to a suspect is, by far, the biggest
influence in obtaining a confession.
FALSE CONFESSIONS
A substantial body of research has emerged that indicates there are many situations in
which people may falsely confess to a crime (Lassiter & Meissner, 2010). These relate,
on the one hand, to the vulnerability of individuals, through for example having a
learning disability, or having a particularly suggestible personality, or even antisocial
personality traits (Gudjohnnson, 2003). On the other hand, the coercive nature of the
interview can cause the suspect to confess in order to avoid the uncomfortable
situation.
An interview procedure for getting suspects to confess, which is particularly open
to unwanted false confessions, was developed in the USA by Inbau, Reid, Buckley
and Jayne (2001). This is a nine-step framework for carrying out an interview. It
includes:
‘Intimidation’, for example, emphasising the seriousness of the offence; maximising
a suspect’s anxiety about their predicament and the officer’s position, which may
include leaning forward in an aggressive manner.
TESTIMONY AND INTERVIEWING 143
‘Manipulation’, for example, minimising the seriousness of the offence, the suspect’s
role within it and suggestions of scenarios for the offence.
‘Appeal’, appealing to the suspect to make a confession, telling them it is in their
best interests to do so.
It also includes the possibility of deceiving the suspect by claiming, for example,
that evidence has been found, which is untrue. The interviewer is further encouraged
to decide whether the suspect is lying using nonverbal cues.
Vrij (2008a) argues that as well as trickery and deceit (introducing false evidence)
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
being unlawful in many countries, such psychological manipulation may result in false
confessions in addition to the processes outlined by Gudjonsson and Pearse (2011)
discussed above. He also shows that these deceptive techniques can actually reduce
the possibility of detecting deception on the part of the interviewee.
Decisions by English courts have consistently outlawed the manipulative forms
of interview. For example, in one case, the evidence was not accepted by the court
because an officer had lied about the suspect’s fingerprints being found. The coercion
implicit in the interview may also lead to the suspect’s comments being declared as
not acceptable in court. Interestingly, in India, which had a history of police abuse
of the interview process to obtain a confession, any confession given in the presence of
a police officer, even in court, is not acceptable.
Many researchers have supported the view given, for example, by Baldwin (1993)
that it may be ‘more realistic to see interviews as mechanisms directed towards the
“construction of proof ” ’ (p. 327). In other words, suspect interviews are perhaps best
thought of as a search for evidence.
TORTURE
Most Western democracies outlaw torture in their human rights legislation, but the
definition of torture and the ways in which it is used in extra-judicial interrogations
have raised concerns among psychologists. Indeed, the American Psychological
Association passed a resolution against the use of torture (Suedfeld, 2007) and banned
any psychologists for assisting in its use. Therefore, when a US congress report made
clear that psychologists were helping to develop torture procedures for use in
Guantanamo Bay, there was outrage among psychologists (Kaslow, 2014).
As scientists, besides the obvious ethical and legal abhorrence for the practice, there is
also the direct empirical question of whether it is of any value. Clearly, this is a difficult
topic to study. However, a review by Sullivan (2014) concluded that it was ineffective
in reducing killings by insurgents. Also, an interesting examination of interviews of
Islamist terrorists by Alison et al. (2014) demonstrated clearly that using an adaptive
interrogation style that treats the interviewees with respect, reduces their attempts to
counteract the interview process.
CONCLUSIONS
This very brief overview of the psychology of interviewing has outlined the central
significance of memory in the interviewing processes. This has led to the recognition
144 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
that reliable testimony is more likely to be produced if procedures that enhance memory
are utilised. However, these do need to take account of the risks of distorting memory
by intrusive processes. The challenges of achieving effective testimony are especially
important in relation to eyewitness identification, in which the pressures on the witness
to give the answer the investigators want may be very strong. This area is consequently
noteworthy for the impact that psychological research can have in the improvement of
criminal justice processes.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Books
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. London:
Cambridge University Press.
Canter, D., & Alison, L. (1999). (Eds.). Interviewing and deception. Farnham: Ashgate.
Gudjonsson, G. (1992). The psychology of interrogations, confessions and testimony. Chichester: Wiley.
Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Police & Criminal Evidence Act. (1984). London: Home Office. www.homeoffice.gov.uk/
documents/PACE-cover
Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: pitfalls and opportunities, 2nd ed. Chichester: Wiley.
146 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
Journal articles
Baldwin, J. (1993). Police interview techniques: establishing truth or proof? British Journal of
Criminology, 33(3), 325–52.
Gieselman, R. E., Fisher, R. P., MacKinnon, D. P., & Holland, H. L. (1985). Eyewitness
memory enhancement in the police interview: cognitive retrieval mnemonics versus
hypnosis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70(2), 401–12.
Gudjonsson, G., & Pearse, J. (2011). Suspect interviews and false confessions. Current Directions
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Websites
The following website gives an overview of the cognitive interview and shows a video of one
being conducted:
www.simplypsychology.org/cognitive-interview.html
The following website gives a comprehensive outline of investigative interviewing techniques:
www.app.college.police.uk/app-content/investigations/investigative-interviewing/
The following website outlines factors relating to eyewitness testimony:
www.apa.org/monitor/apr06/eyewitness.aspx
11 Deception
and fraud
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
Detecting deception and identifying the processes of fraud provide significant chal-
lenges for psychologists. How deception occurs and what can lead to its detection
are reviewed in this chapter. The bases on which people judge plausibility are also
considered. The technology used for lie detection is discussed together with its
strengths and weaknesses.
Most psychological research is based on the assumption that the people involved in
the studies (usually referred to as ‘subjects’) are telling the truth. When they are asked
to complete questionnaires or answer questions about themselves or their activities, they
are not expected to lie. However, in the area of criminal psychology, that cannot be
assumed. Many people who are part of the criminal process might be assumed not
to be telling the truth. This covers not only suspects, but some witnesses and even
victims. A major area of research in criminal psychology is, therefore, concerned with
how and why people lie and how that deception can be detected.
148 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
than would have been obtained from flipping a coin. Bond and DePaulo (2006)
reviewed the results of 186 studies, including 22,282 observers. On average, observers
achieved a 54 per cent accuracy rate, correctly classifying 47 per cent of the lies as
deceptive and 61 per cent of truths as truthful. The better result for indentifying
truthfulness could be explained by a tendency to assume people tell the truth, a ‘truth
bias’. In contrast, studies conducted on prisoners found that they contain a lie bias
(Hartwig, Granhag, Strömwall & Andersson, 2004).
The poor ability to detect deception is related to the fact that people have incorrect
beliefs about indicators of lying (The Global Deception Research Team, 2006).
Common misconceptions
Zimmerman, Compo and Carlucci (2013) and Vrij (2008) demonstrated that many
people use invalid cues as indicators of deception. These include gaze aversion, which
is not a reliable predictor of deception. Vrij, Granhag and Porter (2010) describe the
‘Othello error’. This includes inappropriate nonverbal cues, such as nervousness, to
indicate that a person is lying. Such nervousness could be a product of being fearful
of being wrongly accused of committing a crime or of not being believed. Curiously,
law enforcement agents in the USA are often trained in the Criminal Interrogation and
Confessions manual by Inbau, Reid, Buckley and Jayne (2001), discussed in the
previous chapter. This proposes cues to deception that are unsupported by empirical
evidence such as shifting posture and placing a hand over the mouth (Vrij, Granhag
& Mann (2010).
Assumed preparation
Greuel (1992) reports that observers assessing veracity in repeated interrogations
gave emphasis to the ‘consistency heuristic’. Strömwall and Granhag (2003) describe
this as the assumption that inconsistency in a story indicates deceit and consist-
ency truthfulness. Although there is little firm evidence for this, Ekman (1992)
claims that liars tend to prepare their stories beforehand. He argues that liars who
do not prepare are not able to ‘keep their stories straight’. They are, therefore, more
likely to reveal contradictions in their stories. But, the conclusion that liars who have
prepared well will give the impression of being ‘overprepared’ is more difficult to
substantiate.
DECEPTION AND FRAUD 149
Some studies have examined the ability of professional lie catchers, such as police
officers, rather than undergraduates, to detect deception. Granhag and Vrij (2005)
in a review of ten studies found these professionals performed similarly to the under-
graduates with an overall accuracy rate of 55 per cent (55 per cent lie accuracy rate
and 55 per cent truth accuracy rate). However, Ekman, O’Sullivan and Frank (1999)
found that some professionals are better, such as members of the Secret Service
(64 per cent accuracy rate), Central Intelligence Agency (73 per cent accuracy rates)
and sheriffs (67 per cent accuracy rates). Intriguingly, Vrij and Semin (1996) and
Strömwall, Granhag and Hartwig (2004) found that prisoners used more accurate
indicators of deception than did college students and professional lie catchers.
O’Sullivan and Ekman (2004) found that individuals of differing occupations are
generally better at recognising the kinds of deceit they were most acquainted and familiar
with. They found that experts in law were considerably higher in accuracy of lie identi-
fication in task involving crime deception when compared with a task involving emotive
deception.
Many studies highlight individual differences in the ability to detect lies, with accur-
acy rates ranging from 20 per cent to 80 per cent (Vrij & Graham, 1997; O’Sullivan &
Ekman, 2004) across individuals. Some personality characteristics have also been found
to relate to lie detection. In particular, Nahari, Glicksohn & Nachson (2010) demonstrate
that people with lower levels of ‘absorption’ are more successful in detecting lies. This
aspect of personality is the extent to which a person has the ability to become involved
in sensory and imaginative experiences. Nahari, Glicksohn & Nachson (2010) suggest
that high absorption individuals are more likely to use irrelevant criteria, such as how
they experienced the reading of a text, rather than the narrative’s plausibility itself.
Liar characteristics
Porter, Campbell, Stapleton and Birt (2002) found that participants were most accurate
at detecting lies when the sender was not attractive and when sender and receiver were
the opposite sex. Bond and DePaulo (2008), however, argue sender detectability is
important. Some individuals just appear more credible than others.
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES
As discussed in the previous chapter, most studies of interviewing and lie detection
use experimental procedures that for ethical and practical reasons have low ecological
150 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
validity. As Frank, Feeley, Paolantonio and Servoss (2004) point out, very few studies
incorporate high-stake situations. Low-stake scenarios, in which the liar has little to
lose, reduce the determination to succeed in lying (Zimmerman, Compo & Carlucci,
2013). Miller and Stiff (1993) suggested that the higher the stakes, the more likely are
lies to be detected. Those who have studied actual high-stake situations, such as true
and false calls to the emergency services (Harpster, Adam & Jarvis, 2009) and the
television shows COPS and Jeremy Kyle, do claim that response latencies distinguish
truths from lies (Reynolds & Rendle-Short, 2010).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
JUDGING PLAUSIBILITY
Often the decision that is being made is whether a person’s account is plausible or
not, which can be regarded as the other side of the coin from detecting deception.
Attractiveness bias
Paunonen (2006), in his paper ‘You are honest, therefore I like you and find you
attractive’, explains that people draw on a person’s physical attributes to form a view
of their character. This has been shown in a jury’s decisions where attractive defendants
received judgments with less certainty of guilt than unattractive ones (Efran, 1974).
Also, after conviction, attractive defendants were given shorter sentences (Leventhal
& Krate, 1977). However, Sigall and Ostrove (1975) report that attractive defendants
received more lenient sentences in a mock jury study only when the offense was not
attractiveness-related, such as burglary compared to swindling.
Johnson, Podratz, Dipboye and Gibbons (2010) reviewing a number of studies
report that generally ‘what is beautiful is good’. Thus, the general implication from
the majority of research in social psychology and in criminology shows that attractive-
ness and appearance do influence judgments of veracity. The key reason seems to be
that attractiveness is a heuristic device, a mental shortcut (Tversky & Kahneman, 1985),
for coming to a conclusion in the absence of other information.
Emotional bias
A strong illustration of how people respond to expressed emotion in forming judgments
of plausibility is given by Doss (2002), who describes how a mother who murdered
her sons pleaded with great emotion for their ‘return’ and fooled many people into
believing she was innocent. She even mobilised people to help her find the ‘missing’
sons and catch a non-existent perpetrator who had supposedly kidnapped them.
As Shafir, Simonson and Tversky (1993) show, intense emotional aspects of a
situation can overpower any logical assessment. It has even been suggested by Epstein
(1994) that relying on emotional displays is faster especially when dealing with a difficult
situation that is ambiguous. It is, therefore, not surprising that the amount of emotion
expressed in a criminal context can influence judgments of plausibility. This was shown
by Antonio and Arone (2006) when jury members reported that a witness’s emotional
expression influenced their assessment of their credibility.
DECEPTION AND FRAUD 151
Semmler and Brewer (2002) and Vrij (2008) also showed some individuals
sometimes incorrectly relied on the display of emotions to decide how truthful an
account is. Further evidence of this effect is given by Peace and Sinclair (2012)
who found that emotional rape statements compromised the accuracy of judgements.
Also, Vrij and Mann (2001) found in their study using television press conferen-
ces that people were less accurate in judging an appealer’s veracity when they thought
the emotions of the appealer were real.
It is not just the way expressed emotion influences judgements that is import-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
ant, the level of sympathetic response can create a ‘felt emotion bias’. The emotion
the respondent feels can influence processing of source information. Another way
of looking at this is that in highly emotional situations, such as television appeals,
observers may falsely think what they feel is relevant for making veracity assessments,
especially when only limited information is available about the appealer. With jury
members, they may search for corroboration of what they are feeling and misinter-
pret this, although unreliable or invalid, to be consistent with their emotion (Salerno
& Bottoms, 2009). Granhag (2006) calls this experience a meta-emotion, where
observers are biased to pay more attention to cues exhibited by the sender that supports
their level of felt emotions, rather than more rational judgments.
Central to considerations about deception and its detection have been explorations
of what happens psychologically when people lie (see DePaulo et al., 2003, for an
overview).
A four-factor model outlines the differences between telling the truth and lying
(Zuckerman, DePaulo & Rosenthal, 1981):
• Arousal: Lying causes physiological arousal, either because of discomfort in breaking
social norms or due to fear of getting caught.
• Behaviour control: Liars attempt to keep their movements and other nonverbal
activity under control, so that they make a convincing impression.
• Emotion: Emotions change when lying, either because the liar is secretly pleased
at duping others or because of guilt.
• Thinking: There is pressure to think a lot harder when lying to ensure coherence
in the invented account.
However, not all people experience these processes when telling a lie. Individual
differences and situational differences, such as personality, motivation to succeed,
consequences of getting caught and planning what to say in advance, play a role in
the deceptive processes (Vrij, Granhag & Mann 2010). Further, truth-tellers may
experience these processes if they are highly motivated to be believed, such as during
a police interrogation. Indeed, Levine (2010) has argued that many people will not
give any indication at all that they are lying. This makes it difficult to distinguish truth-
tellers from deceivers.
152 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
They could be a brief rolling of the eyes or furrowing of the forehead, twitching of
the cheek or a repeated touching of the nose. These, Ekman claims, reveal emotions
such as fear, sadness, disgust, contempt, surprise and happiness. Those emotions, in
turn, are taken to indicate deliberate or unconscious feelings that relate to not telling
the truth, such as a very brief smile associated with pleasure in getting away with
the lie.
Although Ekman has contributed to many popular accounts of his work, including
a television series Lie to Me, and advises many agencies on how to use his ideas, they
are not without criticism. For example, Bond and Uysal (2007) claim others have been
unable to replicate Ekman’s findings. Russell has challenged the generality of micro-
expressions of emotion (e.g., Kayyal, Widen & Russell, 2015) in a series of studies
that question the generality of facial expression of emotion. He argues that how we
reveal emotions depends considerably on the context, which undermines the
exploration of general cues to lying.
Exploring content
Statement validity analysis (SVA) developed by Kohnken (2004) is a validity assessment
tool mainly applied to written statements, especially in child sexual abuse cases,
although it has since been expanded to evaluations of any statement in a criminal
context. SVA was built on detailed consideration of statement content using criteria-
based content analysis (CBCA). Akehurst, Kohnken and Hofer (2001) reported that
CBCA successfully discriminated between truthful accounts and untruthful ones, with
a hit rate of 70 per cent. CBCA is based on the hypothesis that a statement of an
experience originating from a true memory differs in content and quality from the
one that is fabricated. Furthermore, only a person who has had genuine experience
of the event will automatically include specific contents into the statement, adding to
its quality (Vrij, Kneller & Mann, 2010).
The central principle on which SVA is based was proposed by Undeutsch (1989),
claiming that untruthful accounts contain fewer details and are less vivid than statements
derived from true memory. This principle was developed further by Steller and
Kohnken (1989) into a set of criteria for truthfulness that can be summarised as:
• There are no contradictions or logical inconsistencies. Logical consistency is not to be
confused with plausibility. A statement may sound implausible, but still be logically
consistent.
• Contextual embedding, such that the event is described in relation to particular
locations, time schedules, personal relationships and other persons before and after
the incident.
DECEPTION AND FRAUD 153
Lying in pairs
Often, lies are perpetrated by a pair or group of individuals. The traditional police
approach has been to interview individuals separately to determine whether their stories
154 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
are consistent with each other. But, there is a growing amount of research (Vernham
& Vrij, 2015) that indicates interviewing people together can be very revealing. For
instance, Granhag, Strömwall and Jonsson (2003) found that liars in pairs were more
consistent between themselves than were pairs who told the truth. If, however,
substantial discrepancies are found in the description of the core of an event, then this
would at least require an explanation.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Narrative plausibility
A crucial aspect of deciding whether someone is telling the truth is whether the storyline
it is part of is convincing. Many laboratory studies of deception ignore this aspect. Yet,
in daily life, that is often how plausibility is assessed. One interesting aspect of stories
is that they have a natural form to them that can be drawn on to evaluate their likely
validity. In essence, this is a beginning, middle and end, usually in that order.
To study the influence of story sequence on judgments of plausibility, Canter,
Grieve, Nicol and Benneworth’s (2003) used Stein, Glenn and Freedle’s (1979) model
of the normal flow of a narrative ‘abstract, setting, initiating event, attempt, conse-
quence and reaction’. They created scenarios that in one condition used this structure.
In the second setting, they reversed the order, taking care that the narrative plausibility
of the story in terms of syntax and grammar remained intact. The ‘disordered’
narratives were judged less plausible than those that followed a conventional sequence.
In addition to these ‘internal’ aspects of the account, Canter, Grieve, Nicol
and Benneworth (2003) also introduced a ‘criminal anchor’ element. This draws on
stereotypical assumptions, such that if there is a hint that a person is a criminal, then
that colours all judgements about the narrative, much as attractiveness influences views
about other irrelevant matters discussed earlier. These ‘external’ factors that draw on
assumptions about what is likely to be the case were found to be a powerful influence
on assumptions about plausibility. For example, simply stating in the account that the
person had just come out of prison in contrast to just coming home from work leads
to quite different assessments about the credibility of the story. Of course, these were
highly controlled experiments in which a lot of other information was absent. But, it
is noteworthy that in many jurisdictions, previous convictions of a defendant are kept
hidden from the jury because of how it can distort their evaluating the evidence
objectively.
DECEPTION AND FRAUD 155
FRAUDSTERS’ NARRATIVES
Fraud consists of influencing a person, usually to part with money, by deceptive means.
It, therefore, requires determined deception for its success. Yet, studies of deception
because of their laboratory origins, which do not give a full account of the context of
the deception, usually ignore the real world of fraud. However, considering how
fraudsters operate does provide further understanding of the nature of deception and
how it can be detected.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
As Onyebadi and Park (2012) have shown, there are common characteristics to
what are known as 419, or ‘advance fee fraud’ messages. These messages typically offer
the recipient a large sum of money. Once the recipient takes the bait, they are asked
for a fee in advance to obtain the money, but, of course, the money is never received.
These forms of fraud are deeply embedded in a plausible narrative. A person in the
news, for example, is said to have died, leaving a lot of money to be got out of
the country, or a banker has stumbled on money in a closed account and so on. There
are usually many specific details, some of which can be checked on the Internet and
have some credibility. It is the plausible narrative that draws people in.
ACTIVITY 11.1
Find examples of 419 (advanced fee) fraud (e.g., at www.hoax-slayer.com/
nigerian-scam-list.shtml) and discuss why so many people are convinced by them.
The general findings from many studies are that behavioural cues, such as averting
gaze and broken speech, are not useful in detecting deception. DePaulo et al. (2003)
examined 158 behavioural cues and found there were no consistent cues that dia-
gnostically distinguished liars from truth-tellers, and no cues that are reliably associated
with lying. Most differences found were very small and difficult to discriminate in real-
life situations.
By contrast, listening carefully to what a person says can be of some value, especially
if there are external corroborating facts to draw on. Content analysis of text is of some
utility, but that is difficult to standardise and readily open to abuse. Computer software
that makes the analysis of text more objective and standardised has some potential, but
it does depend on having appropriate inbuilt dictionaries that probably need to be
developed specially for each context.
The work that Vrij and his colleagues have been developing with its emphasis on
the cognitive challenges of lying seems to have great potential, although making it
acceptable within a legal context has some way to go. Some aspects of it could be
regarded as coercion. Similarly, interviewing people jointly offers some very interesting
possibilities, but there would be real challenges to having the results of such interviews
accepted in court.
156 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
The ability to detect deception is a skill that is necessary within various occupations,
from the military to crime investigators, customs agents and insurance agents. Money
is invested to develop reliable lie detectors for use against terrorism and in the judicial
system. These technologies all assume that telling lies produces a physiological response.
In general, they detect the person’s emotional reaction to the situation and questions
being asked. But, whether that reaction is an indication of lying is an extremely
contentious issue.
FIGURE 11.1 A ‘polygraph’ in use. The various physiological measurements are all read
into a computer, which represents the changes in these and allows various
forms of analysis.
DECEPTION AND FRAUD 157
POLYGRAPH
From the Greek, ‘poly’ (= many) and ‘graph’ (= to write). It gets its name from a
general-purpose piece of equipment that is used in many areas of physiology that
records a number of different measures of physiological response at the same time. An
examinee is attached to a blood-pressure cuff to measure heart rate and blood pressure,
pneumographs to measure respiration and galvanometers to measure the skin resistance
produced by palm sweat. As the examinee answers questions put carefully by the
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
GKT is limited by the difficulty in certain cases of designing questions to which only
the suspect would know the answers. In cases where details are highly publicised (e.g.,
the O.J. Simpson case) or if memory is affected (e.g., the suspect is under the influence
of drinks or drugs, or there was a long period between the time of crime and the
polygraph test), there would be no guilty knowledge to detect. In general, GKT has
been found to help distinguish whether a person is innocent or not, but is much less
effective in determining if a person is guilty (Synnott, Dietzel & Ioannou 2015).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Voice stress analysis (VSA) assumes that liars will be more aroused than truth-tellers
due to feelings of guilt and fear, and as a result, display stress in their voice. VSA
measures and displays inaudible fluctuations in the human voice known as ‘micro-
tremor’ patterns on a computer screen, which indicate when a speaker delivers words
under stress. As with the polygraph, it is not a lie detector, but rather a stress detector;
an indirect measure of lie telling.
The appeal of VSA is its ease of use in the field. In crime investigations, the
computer voice stress analyser (CVSA) can be easily brought to the crime scene to
interview suspects, and the results are instant. CVSA is also being used by insurance
companies to identify false insurance claim calls.
The National Research Council (2003) has examined the reliability of VSA and
concluded that ‘The practical performance of voice stress analysis for detecting
deception has not been impressive’ (p. 168) and that the relevant research offers ‘little
or no scientific basis for the use of . . . voice measurement instruments as an alternative
to the polygraph . . .’ (p. 168).
Despite its lack of scientific basis for use, it continues to be used in the field. VSA
has been found to be effective in eliciting confessions from suspects and also in eliciting
false confessions as in the case of Michael Crowe who was accused of the murder of
his sister in 1998.
CONCLUSIONS
Lie detection is not as easy as many people think. In general, people are very poor at
detecting deceit. This may be because ‘white lies’ are a natural part of human
interactions, so we must all be able to lie convincingly at some time or other. However,
detecting deceit is a crucial part of the criminal justice system, and so, there is a great
deal of interest in developing procedures to help discover lies. The physiologically based
‘lie detectors’ have rather less utility than is often believed and may be of more value in
encouraging confessions than detecting lies. No system does a great deal better than
chance across all situations, but the cognitive and emotional load that sustained lying
requires is a reasonable basis for exploring the psychology of deceit, which may, in
limited circumstances with some people, provide a productive basis for understanding
how lying occurs, and so, how it may be detected.
160 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
• What are the main challenges to producing ecologically valid studies of lie detection?
• What are lie detectors? Do they work?
• Are any groups of people better at detecting liars than others? Why might that be?
• What are the main psychological processes involved in sustaining a lie?
• Describe the various factors that can enhance or impair ability to detect deception.
• Why might attractiveness be relevant to plausibility?
Books
Canter, D., & Alison, L. (Eds.). (1999). Interviewing and deception. Farnham: Ashgate.
Ekman, P. (2001). Telling lies: clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics and marriage. New York:
Norton.
Ford, C. V. (1996). Lies! lies! lies!: the psychology of deceit. London: American Psychiatric Press.
Goldstraw-White, J. (2011). White-collar crime: accounts of offending behaviour. New York:
Springer.
Granhag, P. A., & Strömwall, L. A. (Eds.). (2004). Deception detection in forensic contexts
(pp. 229–50). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Granhag, P. A., Vrij, A., & Verschuere, B. (2015). Detecting deception: current challenges and cognitive
approaches. Chichester: Wiley.
Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: pitfalls and opportunities, 2nd ed. Chichester: Wiley.
Journal articles
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K. & Cooper, H.
(2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74–118.
Krinsky, K. L. (2015). A review of eyewitness identification in the United States: problems and
policies. Crime Psychology Review, 1(1), 84–97.
National Research Council. (2003). The polygraph and lie detection. Committee to review the
scientific evidence on the polygraph. Washington, DC: The National Academic Press.
DECEPTION AND FRAUD 161
Rosenfeld, J. P. (2005). ‘Brain Fingerprinting:’ a critical analysis. Scientific Review of Mental Health
Practice, 4, 20–37.
Synnott, J., Dietzel, D., & Ioannou, M. (2015). A review of the polygraph: history, methodology
and current status. Crime Psychology Review, 1(1), 59–83.
Vernham, Z., & Vrij, A. (2015). A review of the collective interviewing approach to detecting
deception in pairs. Crime Psychology Review, 1(1), 43–58.
Vrij, A. (2004). Why professionals fail to catch liars and how they can improve. Legal and
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Websites
The following website highlights some of the difficulties in detecting deception as well as some
successes:
www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/detecting.aspx
The following website critically evaluates some of the methods for detecting deception:
www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/uncategorized/detecting-deception.html
12 Psychology and
investigations
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
Increasingly, in recent years, psychologists and ideas from psychology have contributed
to the investigation of a wide range of crimes. The contributions fall within three
broad areas. The first of these is the examination of the styles and patterns of criminal
action within offenders’ behaviour and the unravelling of how these relate to psycho-
logical and social characteristics; sometimes referred to as ‘offender profiling’. Although
sometimes assumed only to be relevant to serious, serial or sexual crimes, in fact, these
‘investigative inferences’ can be derived for all forms of criminality from burglary or
fraud or arson through to serial killing, kidnapping and terrorism.
The second contribution psychologists can make is to the analysis of the investi-
gation process itself to improve the effectiveness and appropriateness of investigative
decision-making.
Third, psychologists contribute to investigations through the assessment and
improvement of the material and information that is the basis of the investigation or
PSYCHOLOGY AND INVESTIGATIONS 163
The contributions that psychologists can make to police investigations are most widely
known and understood in terms of ‘offender profiles’. Offender profiling, as typically
practised, is the process by which individuals, drawing on their clinical or other
professional experience, make judgments about the personality traits or psychodynamics
of the perpetrators of crimes. From the perspective of scientific psychology, such a
process is flawed in its reliance on personal judgment, rather than actuarial (based on
empirical analysis) assessment. These flaws in personal judgements have been shown
in a wide range of professional applications through extensive studies first reviewed
Source www.ia-ip.org
164 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
by Meehl in 1954 (republished in 1996). The theories about criminals and their
behaviour upon which much ‘offender profiling’ has been based are also open to
question. The lack of scientific rigour evident in the profiling process has, for two
decades, driven proponents of investigative psychology research to map out a more
scientific approach to underpin and systematise this almost mystical ‘profiling’ process
(cf. Alison & Rainbow, 2011).
At the heart of this are what have become known as the ‘profiling equations’ (after
Canter, 1995). These are hypothetical equations that capture the scientific perspective
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
for inferring associations between the actions that occur during the offence – including
when and where they happen and to whom – and the characteristics of the offender,
including the offender’s criminal history, background, base location and relationships
to others. They are also known as the Actions → Characteristics or A → C equa-
tions, where A are the actions in the crime and C are the characteristics of typical
offenders for such crimes, and → is the theory or argument and the evidence for
inferring one from the other.
Investigative psychologists conduct a wide range of empirical studies of different
types of offences and the offenders who committed them, with the purpose of
establishing solutions to these equations, in the hope of providing objective bases for
the inferences that detectives make in an investigation about the perpetrator’s likely
characteristics. A number of ideas about the processes that may underlie A → C links
can be drawn from general social and psychological theory. These include personality
and interpersonal behaviour theories, as well as frameworks drawing on interpersonal
narratives and on socio-economic factors. Any or all of these theories could provide
a valid basis for investigative inferences if the differences in individuals that they propose
are associated with real variations in criminal behaviour.
for themselves cast their victims in less than human roles’, he proposed that differences
in the way offenders carried out their crimes would relate to the role assigned to the
victim or the ‘mode of interpersonal transaction’ adopted by the offender. He proposed
three types of roles to which a victim may be assigned:
1 Victim as an object
The offender treats the victim as an object, that is, as something to be used and
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
controlled through restraint and threat, often involving alternative gains in the form
of other crimes such as theft. Offenders have a complete lack of empathy. Victims
are chosen opportunistically, so tend to be vulnerable individuals.
2 Victim as a vehicle
The offender sees the victim as a vehicle for the offender’s own emotional state,
for example, anger and frustration. There is some awareness of the victim as human,
but this recognition facilitates the offender using the victim to express their feelings
and desires. The victim is typically subjected to extreme violence and abuse. The
victim may have symbolic importance, for example, women of particular
appearance.
3 Victim as a person
The offender sees the victim as a person. These offenders nurture the confused
belief that through the assault, they achieve some sort of personal intimacy with
the victim, so offending actions will include attempts to create a degree of rapport
or relationship. Offenders think they are heroes – in Criminal Shadows, one rapist
is quoted as saying to his victim ‘be more careful, next time someone nasty may
attack you’.
Adapted from Canter, 1994
Studies have shown that this general model does help to understand the specific
empirical differences in offending behaviour found within rape (Canter 1994) and
paedophilia (Canter, Hughes & Kirby 1998), as well as stranger homicide (Salfati &
Canter 1999).
More recently, the role offenders enact in a crime has been studied directly (Youngs
& Canter, 2012, 2013) by asking them to describe whether, for example, they thought
they were a ‘hero’ or a ‘victim’ or ‘just doing a job’. This is proving very fruitful in
opening up an understanding of what maintains criminal actions (Presser, 2010).
IS PROFILING POSSIBLE?
Youngs (2008) points out that it was in the 1970s and 1980s that the FBI first drew
attention to what investigators had long known: deductions about the likely perpe-
trator can be drawn from a consideration, in detail, of the crime itself (Douglas,
Ressler, Burgess & Hartman, 1986). The FBI special agents who drew particular
attention to this process gave prominence to the label ‘Offender Profiling’. Coming
from a scientifically grounded psychological perspective, David Canter argued that the
166 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
profiling process was a rather more profound one, requiring formal specification. He
proposed, as a first step in unravelling this process, that the relationship between actions
and characteristics was one that, in mathematical terms, could be represented as:
where the As represent the actions in the crimes, the Cs the characteristics of the per-
petrator and the Fs are the functions that relate the two, that is, the degree, level or
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The second thing that became clear in specifying the potential relationships between
the offending and the offender in these formal terms was that there were many reasons
why ‘offender profiling’ just was not going to be possible (see Canter & Youngs, 2008).
One of the reasons for this is the problem of ‘contingency destabilisation’ (Youngs,
2008). This effect is most readily understood through consideration of the example
she provides (Table 12.2).
Crime B differs from the previous one (A) on just one action of when the offender
chose to carry out the offence. Yet, this change in one behavioural detail puts the entire
offence in a different perspective, casting a whole new light on many of the other
components of the offence. For example, the smashing of the window is a rather
different behaviour in the middle of the night than in the middle of the day. Similarly,
the offender’s violent reaction to encountering the occupant is less readily construed
as a panic-based reaction, given that he must have expected someone to be in at that
PSYCHOLOGY AND INVESTIGATIONS 167
At 2 a.m. one Saturday morning, a suburban house in Manchester is burgled. The offender
disabled the alarm, then entered by smashing a downstairs window. He stole cash and
jewellery, without making any mess, but leaving fingerprints. The larger electronic goods in
the house were not stolen. Just as he was about to leave, the offender encountered the
occupant and reacted violently punching her in the face several times before running off.
Crime Description B
Adapted from Youngs (2008)
time. So, a change in one action component within a crime description can change
the meaning of exactly the same actions, indicating an entirely different style of offend-
ing and suggesting a very different type of offender.
A further complication is that, although some degree of consistency from one offence
to the next is useful for developing associations between actions and characteristics of
offenders, it also has to be recognised that there are many reasons why offenders may
change their pattern of activity from one crime to another (Sorochinski & Salfati, 2010).
Canter and Youngs (2009) list six processes that may give rise to differences for the
same offender from one crime to another:
• Responsiveness – The way the criminal reacts to the situation as they find it. A
burglar is less likely to climb a drainpipe on discovering the front door is open.
• Maturation – Physical, physiological and psychological ageing may bring maturity.
A hot-blooded robber may turn into a calm fraudster.
• Cognitive development – An increase in expertise and understanding of what is
possible, such as realising that certain people are more open to a charm offensive
than a violent one. This also includes greater familiarity with potential targets.
• Learning – The consequence of earlier criminal experience can produce learnt
changes. A serial rapist may threaten their victim to be quiet after a previous victim
screamed. Also, access to firearms, say, after a spell in prison may be regarded as
both learning and cognitive development.
• Career development – Criminals may move up through an organised network
becoming less of the ‘heavy’ forcing others and more of the ‘brains’ behind a crime.
• Cultural changes – Increased security makes some crimes more difficult. Airplane
hijacking is much more difficult with the changes at airports. This has given rise
to kidnapping and other forms of extortion. The development of the Internet,
discussed in Chapter 17, has also opened up other opportunities for criminality.
168 INTERACTING WITH CRIMINALS
Despite these complexities in relating actions to characteristics, there are now a num-
ber of studies that show these links between offending style and offender characteristics
do exist and can be established (e.g., Canter & Fritzon, 1998; Lobato, 2000; Youngs,
2004; Santtila, Junkkila & Sandnabba, 2005; Santtila, Laukkanen, Zappala & Bosco,
2008). The results from one such study are shown in Table 12.3. The identification
of such relationships points to the general feasibility of the inferential process. The chal-
lenge to develop theoretical and conceptual approaches to the complexities that will
allow full solutions to the original Canter profiling equations were mapped out at the
birth of investigative psychology.
are made about which lines of enquiry to pursue and what actions to take during that
process. The main challenge to investigators is to make the right decisions under highly
pressurised conditions. Where the crime under investigation is a serious one or an ongoing
series of offences, there will be intense media attention, political and organisational stresses
that make objective judgement very difficult. Alongside these pressures, the investigators
must handle a vast amount of information, much of which may be of unknown reliability
or simply irrelevant, that needs to be amassed, organised and interpreted.
These challenges generate uncertainty both from within the decision-making team
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
and from external contexts (Alison, Power, van den Heuvel & Waring, 2015). The
challenge for those in charge of investigations is, therefore, to manage the many
cognitive challenges this uncertainty creates. One way of working with these issues is
to conceptualise an investigation as a series of decisions that detectives must make. This
can be represented as in Figure 12.2 (Canter & Youngs, 2009). In this diagram, the
lines represent investigative actions by the police, while the nodes are the results of
that action, that is, new pieces of information or facts. In the period immediately after
a crime occurs, investigators will often have very little information. Rapidly, however,
the investigation will uncover further pieces of information that, in turn, will produce
more information, suggesting further directions for investigative action. Typically, after
a fairly short period of time, the options will narrow down as the police actions (the
lines) show more and more of the information to be irrelevant to the case, and
detectives establish facts that close off all but one of the lines of enquiry.
Understanding the investigation process in these terms draws attention to two
possible points at which detectives may be particularly prone to making erroneous
decisions, and therefore, at which psychological contributions could be most useful.
The first potential problem point occurs early on in the process. As listed in Figure
12.2, in investigative decisions about the actions to be undertaken or the leads to follow
up must rely on the information that comes in; information that has come to light as
Police
CRIME
actions
OCCURS
Information
CRIME
OCCURS
Investigation
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
CONVICTION
FIGURE 12.3 A simple model of how a police investigation develops towards a conclusion.
a result of previous actions. In other words, the range of options about what actions
to take at each point is contingent upon the previous decisions that have been made.
This opens up the potential for investigations to proceed along entirely wrong lines
if the early information is wrong or even just incomplete. It is in the early stages then
that investigative psychological findings about the features of the crime to focus on or
the geographical area to explore or the suspect pool to consider could then be most
instructive.
The second problem point is the result of the way in which information builds
rapidly in the early stages of an investigation. Each lead can give rise to numerous others
that, in turn, generate multiple follow-on leads. As Canter and Youngs (2009) argue,
this will give rise to exponential increases in the cognitive load on detectives, reaching
some maximum weight, typically after a few days. The weight of this cognitive load
on those involved in the investigation can distort the decisions that are made at this
point of the investigation.
Detectives labouring under huge quantities of information and time pressures may
rely on heuristics or ‘rules of thumb’, rather than more systematic approaches to make
decisions. Yet, as seminal psychological research has shown, decisions made in this way
are often inappropriate ones (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). As the investigation pro-
gresses, they will eventually be able to start to narrow down their lines of enquiry,
reducing the general demands upon them and the probability of distorted decisions.
The general diamond shape in Figure 12.3 shows the possible build up of conditions
under which various biases in investigators’ thought processes are likely to occur, with
consequent inadequacies in the decisions made and the subsequent actions.
PSYCHOLOGY AND INVESTIGATIONS 171
• Link crimes
• Prioritise suspects
• Build catalogues of offenders’ geo-behavioural profiles
• Generate potential TICs (further offences ‘taken into consideration’)
• Explore co-offending networks
• Indicate locations for intelligence gathering
• Map crimes and perform hotspot analysis
Adapted from Canter and Youngs (2008a)
However, administrative and bureaucratic complications mean that systems such as
iOPS are currtently rarely is use.
FIGURE 12.4 An illustration of the output from geographical profiling software (Dragnet).
The names of the victims are the locations of the bodies of the murdered
victims of Jack the Ripper. In this case, the prediction is that Jack, the
Ripper had a base on Middlesex Street between where Kelly was murdered
and where a Graffitto was left. (More details in Canter (2007) Mapping
Murder, p. 124.)
There have also been developments in building mathematical models that do not
draw upon the aggregate probability functions of decays, by working out the character-
istics of a series of crimes committed by an individual. This approach that draws on the
characteristics of individual crime series is known as ‘ideographic’ (Canter, Hammond,
Youngs & Juszczak 2012).
Various approaches to geographical profiling have been successfully applied to a
wide range of crimes. These include burglary (Block & Bernasco, 2009), serial killers
(Canter, Coffey, Huntley & Missen 2000), vehicle theft (Tonkin, Woodhams, Bond
& Loe, 2010). In places as different as India (Sarangi & Youngs, 2006) and New
Zealand (Hammond, 2013).
PSYCHOLOGICAL AUTOPSY
One other interesting contribution to investigations as well as the courts, which is most
prevalent in the USA, is what is known as a ‘psychological autopsy’. This is an
assessment of an individual’s state of mind after that person has died. It developed as
part of suicide intervention programmes (Farberow & Shneidman, 1961). It became
a crucial tool in insurance examinations of suspicious suicides, and questioned murders,
most notably in Canter’s (2005) questioning of whether Paula Gilfoyle had been
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
CONCLUSIONS
There are three broad forms of contribution psychologists can make to investigations.
These are a) investigative inferences, b) the investigation process and c) the assess-
ment and improvement of investigative information. Investigative psychology (IP) is the
academic discipline that has emerged in response to the need for a more scientific basis
to the ‘offender profiling’ advice given to investigators. The challenge at the heart of IP
is to resolve the Actions–Characteristics (A → C) equations, which relate the set of an
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
offender’s actions in a crime to the set of his or her characteristics, such that these
characteristics can be reliably inferred in the investigation context when the offender
is unknown.
Psychological principles are being applied to the investigation process itself to
facilitate decision-making and to evaluate the information upon which those decisions
are based. There is considerable psychological research that can be drawn upon to
improve both the usefulness and detail and the accuracy and validity of investigative
information. In particular, a number of specific psychological approaches to validity
assessment have been developed.
Books
Canter, D. V. (2007). Mapping murder: The secrets of geographical profiling, 2nd ed. London: Virgin.
Canter, D. V. (1995). Criminal shadows. London: Harper Collins (2006 edition, New York:
Dorset Press).
PSYCHOLOGY AND INVESTIGATIONS 175
Canter, D., & Youngs, D. (2008) Investigative psychology: offender profiling and the analysis of criminal
action. Chichester: Wiley.
Keppel, R. (2004). (Ed.). Offender profiling: readings in crime assessment and profiling. London:
Thomson/Custom Publishing.
Youngs, D. (Ed.). (2013). Behavioural analysis of crime: studies in David Canter’s investigative
psychology. Farnham: Ashgate.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Journal articles
Canter, D. V. (2011). Resolving the offender ‘profiling equations’ and the emergence of an
investigative psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(1), 5–10.
Canter, D., & Fritzon, K. (1998). Differentiating arsonists: a model of firesetting actions and
characteristics. Legal and Criminal Psychology, 3, 73–96.
Salfati, G., & Canter, D. (1999). Differentiating stranger murders: profiling offender character-
istics from behavioural styles. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 17, 391–406.
Websites
This website gives an overview of the topics covered by investigative psychology as well as other
useful resources:
www.davidcanter.com
The following website gives an account of various approaches to profiling:
www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/criminal.aspx
This website critically analyses the usefulness of offender profiles:
www.crimeandjustice.org.uk/publications/cjm/article/usefulness-criminal-profiling
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
4 Areas of
application
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
13 Psychology and
the police
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
1 Understand the issues involved in the recruitment and selection of police officers.
2 Understand what advancements there are in UK police training.
3 Discuss the role of stress in policing.
Although this book is about criminal psychology, there are many aspects of psychology
considered in the previous part of this book that overlap with more general aspects of
police work. In addition, there are contributions that psychologists make to policing
that draw on other areas of applied psychology. These include the recruitment and
selection of police officers, counselling in relation to critical incidents and more general
family matters. They also include direct operational assistance, especially in hostage
negotiations. Furthermore, contributions to training and management are increasingly
being made by psychologists, often drawing on the material discussed in earlier
chapters.
It is important to be aware that formal, organised police forces are a relatively new
development in modern society. Although an organisation dedicated to the main-
tenance of order in society can be traced back at least to the Roman Emperor Augustus,
it was only in 1829 that the first official police force was set up as the Metropolitan
Police in London. Previously, and still in many countries today, there was little distinc-
tion between the military and the police. As a consequence, there was (and is in some
places still) a general disquiet by the general public about what the role of the police
was and whether they would help or inhibit civil liberties. It is, therefore, instructive
to note the initial orders issued by Sir Robert Peel, when he established the police
force, regarding the conduct of police constables:
180 AREAS OF APPLICATION
He will be civil and obliging to all people of every rank and class. He must be
particularly cautious not to interfere idly nor unnecessarily in order to make a
display of his authority; when required to act, he will do so with decision and
boldness; on all occasions, he may expect to receive the fullest support in the
proper exercise of his authority.
(Klockars, 1985, p. 47)
In general, the police have three primary objectives, each of which carries impli-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The term police psychology is most readily recognised in the USA, with its own
organisations such as the International Association of Chiefs of Police, Police Psycho-
logical Services Section, Society for Police and Criminal Psychology and American
Psychological Association, Division 18, Police and Public Safety Section. Drawing on
these organisations, Aumiller and Corey (2007) identified over fifty distinct pro-
ficiencies that police psychologists need to demonstrate (see Focus box 13.1), although,
of course, none cover the full range of areas and many only deal with one.
Aumiller and Corey (2007) put these areas of activity under four domains of practice:
1 Assessment-related activities
2 Intervention services
3 Operational support
4 Organisational/management consultation
From the earliest days of psychological testing, various law enforcement agencies have,
from time to time, taken advantage of the ease and objectivity of psychological tests.
As early as 1916, police selection in California utilised the then novel intelligence
testing procedure (Dietz & Reese, 1986). But subsequently, although, in general, police
forces have been rather conservative in their approaches to selection, they have
increasingly made use of psychometric procedures.
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 181
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The use of various personality tests of police officers have found, very generally, that
when compared to the rest of the population, police officers are more conservative,
extraverted and tough-minded (Cook, 1977; Colman & Gorman, 1982). In a British
study, Burbeck and Furnham (1984), using the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire
(EPQ), found that successful candidates were more extraverted and less neurotic than
unsuccessful candidates. However, these findings do not necessarily predict later success.
Most assessments are aimed at determining whether applicants have any problems
of emotional adjustment, antisocial tendencies or mental disturbance at the time of
their application. In other words, the process is one of ‘screening out’ people who
may become an embarrassment or risk to the police force, rather than selecting those
who are predicted to be outstanding police officers. One inventory commonly used
is the Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI) (Inwald & Gebbia, 1993), which was
specifically devised for use in jobs relating to security and policing. The Minnesota
Multi-Phasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), which has a long history of use in many
areas of psychology (Hathaway & McKinley, 1940), is also popular with law
enforcement agencies, as it provides indications of many different psychological
disorders and other aspects of mental health.
Detrick and Chibnull (2002) examined whether police officer performance
could be predicted using the IPI. They examined whether performance on the job
after working for a year in the field as evaluated by their supervisors could be
predicted from pre-employment IPI scores. In their study of the performance and
IPI scores of 108 applicants, they found that three of the IPI scales – family conflicts,
guardedness and driving violations – significantly predicted supervisors’ ratings of
officers’ performance on the job.
182 AREAS OF APPLICATION
However, as Dunnette and Motowidlo (1976) pointed out, there are many aspects
of actually being a police officer that influence the person’s actions and attitudes.
As a consequence, there is little evidence that conventional measures of personality
on their own predict how a person will perform throughout a career in the police.
The idea that police culture and training influences and alters personality is referred
to as the socialisation model (Brown & Willis, 1985). Indeed, the evidence is that police
officers tend to show more symptoms relating to their body, more anxiety and more
alcohol vulnerability the longer they’ve been in the police (Hyatt & Hargrave, 1988).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Thus, predictors of these potential problems are less likely to be effective at the time
of selecting police officers. Curiously, Neal (1986) found that recruits who keep quiet
about their attitudes and feelings when being recruited are more likely to become
successful police officers.
Another problem with the use of psychometric testing is that the criteria to be used
have not been generally agreed (Ainsworth, 2002). Systematic research on the key
attributes required of a police officer is inconclusive. Identifying suitable recruits is an
extremely important issue because employing the wrong individual will cost a police
force financially due to the high costs involved in training recruits and could also
damage the reputation of the police force.
In the quarter of a century since these characteristics were listed, it may be expected
that other characteristics are becoming dominant. Certainly, at the senior levels in the
UK, intellectual and academic ability are coming to the fore. Awareness of human
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 183
Intervention domain
Employee assistance
Individual therapy
Group, couple, family therapy or counselling group
Critical incident early intervention
Dealing with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
Critical incident therapy or counselling
Counselling to cope with unique or chronic job stressors (e.g., deep
undercover, homicide, child abuse, sex crimes, etc.)
FOCUS 13.1
Disability recovery
Mental attitude preparation
Wellness programmes
Life coaching
Intervention-related education and training
Intervention-related research
184 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Threat assessments
Operations-related education and training
Operations-related process improvement
Consulting domain
FOCUS 13.1 Continued
diversity and respect for that is also being asked for. Rather than ‘traditional values’,
police in many countries are being required to accept the sorts of equalities that
Sir Robert Peel emphasised when he first established a police force.
So far, the focus has been predominantly on studies from America where police
psychologists have a much wider range of formal roles (see Focus box 13.1). Across
the forty-three police forces in England and Wales, there has been increasing
standardisation in the recruitment and selection of police officers (National Policing
Plan, 2005–2008). This has introduced more consistency in how police officers are
recruited. There is now a national recruitment application form that is used by all
forces. Each force has an operational assessment centre where recruits undergo a
competency-based structured interview comprising four questions, a numeric and
verbal logical reasoning test, two written exercises and four interactive exercises. Seven
core competencies for undertaking the role of police constable are assessed (refer to
Focus box 13.2 for details). These required competencies were put together by a Home
Office Working group.
The seven core competencies can be categorised into three types: job related, general
skills and personality characteristics. The competencies that are considered to be related
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 185
takes them into account. Treats people with dignity and respect at all
times, no matter what their background, status, circumstances or
appearance.
• Team working – Works effectively as a team member and helps build
relationships within the team.
Taken from: www.policecouldyou.co.uk/documents/pc-guide-brochure2835.pdf?view=Binary
to the nature of the work of a police constable are community focus and respect and
awareness of race and diversity issues. The key skills required are communication,
problem solving and ability to work in a team. The personality characteristics required
are resilience and responsibility.
There are a number of roles in the police that are particularly stressful and put special
demands on the capabilities of those taking on those roles. They include:
• In the Special Weapons and Tactics (SWAT) teams. These are highly trained units
with weapons that allow rapid, confrontational entry into a building. There is no
direct equivalent in the UK, but there are officers who are specially selected and
trained to use firearms.
• Being involved in undercover operations is often a long-term commitment that
requires considerable skills. It also can create a unique variety of stressors, such as
the constant vigilance required to maintain an alternate identity, fears of exposure,
separations from family and friends and even pressures to commit criminal
acts (Aumiller & Corey, 2007). There is also the special ability to be able to avoid
initiating crime, and thus, becoming an agent provocateur, which would endan-
ger any subsequent court proceedings. There is a long history of undercover
policemen becoming corrupt, so the psychological issues of this role are consider-
able.
• Taking part in hostage negotiation is another highly skilled task that requires special
types of individuals and training, discussed in more detail below.
• Emergency consultations concerning the seriously mentally ill. Police psychologists
provide direct and indirect assessment of individuals identified by police officers
as potentially suicidal, imminently dangerous to self or others, unable to provide
for basic personal needs as a result of a mental disease or disorder, incapable of
understanding risks to health and safety as the result of a mental disorder, using
drugs or alcohol in a manner that might endanger the safety of the individual or
others or who are extremely combative in association with a mental disease or
disorder. The police psychologist consults with police officers on de-escalation,
crisis intervention and referral to a medical treatment facility for voluntary or
involuntary emergency psychiatric evaluation.
• Working with youngsters involved in crime also has its own requirements.
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 187
POLICE TRAINING
Police training is an area where psychologists in Britain have not been utilised as
extensively as in America (Hollin, 1989). However, the most notable input of British
psychologists to police training occurred in the early 1980s in London. In 1982, the
Metropolitan Police introduced a new training programme for police recruits, human
awareness training (HAT), which was later re-titled police skills training (PST). The
training programme focused on developing recruits in three main areas: interpersonal
skills, self-awareness and community relations. An independent evaluation project was
carried out over a 5-year period by two psychologists Ray Bull and Peter Horncastle
(for full details, see Horncastle & Bull, 1986; Bull & Horncastle, 1989, 1994). In
subsequent years, this sort of training has found its way throughout the police in various
diffused ways.
The findings of those early training programmes did show a reduction in social
anxiety of the recruits across the test periods, with levels of self-esteem showing little
change on completion of the training programme. Officers who had received HAT
training received fewer complaints against them during their first 3 years of service
when compared with a matched control group. In terms of improvements in the skills
that the HAT programme set out, to enhance the interpersonal skills component was
considered to be the best aspect of the training, self-awareness reasonable and
community relations poor. On completion of the evaluation, Bull and Horncastle
recommended a number of modifications that they felt needed to be made to the
training programme in light of their findings. These recommendations included
enhancing officers’ sympathy towards victims and improvement of the self-evaluation
and awareness components of the training programme.
In the USA, a broader range of training activity is sometimes carried out by police
psychologists. These centre on preventing and/or ameliorating problematic behaviours
in law enforcement officers. It can consist of providing individual or group education
188 AREAS OF APPLICATION
and training about the causes and consequences of such behaviours, as well as
techniques for changing behaviour. Such training typically focuses on critical issues in
law enforcement, such as understanding and controlling:
1 Police use of deadly force
2 Deviant or corrupt police behaviour
3 Police prejudice and discrimination
4 Violence-prone police officer
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
The concept of stress was developed by Selye (1956). Selye considered stress in terms
of the physiological responses of the body to the demands made upon it. No single
definition of stress exists. Ainsworth and Pease (1987: 43) ask ‘Is stress something
that is imposed on you (a stimulus) or is it a way in which you react to the world
(a response)?’ Many researchers approach stress from both angles considering stress in
terms of environmental stressors and how individuals respond to stress. Stress responses
can be divided into three categories, physiological, psychological and behavioural
(Schuler, 1980). Physiological symptoms consist of bodily changes such as increased
heart rate and headaches. Psychological responses include apathy, forgetfulness and
irritability. Behavioural changes include loss of appetite, disturbed sleeping patterns
and increased smoking or alcohol habits.
Policing is considered to be one of the most stressful occupations (Brown &
Campbell, 1994). Occupational stress is something that should be taken seriously
because stress can impact upon the health and welfare of officers and effect job
performance as a result of absenteeism or inability to focus on the job. There is an
extensive literature on stress in policing, most of which stems from America (see Toch,
2002).
HOSTAGE NEGOTIATION
Getty and Elam (1988) found that hostage negotiators had above-average ability to
communicate with others, were self-confident and good at divergent thinking and
helpful and sympathetic in their dealing with others.
Negotiators find it useful to identify different types of hostage situations (Blau, 1994):
1 Where a criminal takes hostages to ensure escape or for ransom.
2 Criminals unexpectedly trapped while robbing seize hostages to get out.
3 As part of a prison riot.
4 Mentally ill person with disturbed thinking.
5 Person under severe stress, possibly intoxicated, for example, a parent taking a child
hostage in a custody dispute.
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 189
underlying variations that Blau (1994) identified. Yokota draws on an action system
framework (Shye, 1985) to identify four processes underlying hostage events that
related to differences between the people taking and their reasons for doing so:
‘Integrative’ hostage situations had the most disturbed styles of destructive and inter-
personal activity. No substantive demands were made by these hostage-takers,
and variables such as ‘drug use’, ‘sexual activity’, ‘demand for alcohol’ or ‘demand
for victim’s comfort’ were actions in this mode. These hostage situations are often
public places. These were perpetrators with emotional or mental problems.
‘Adaptive’ situations are the ones in which the perpetrators react to an unprepared
environment and take hostages in order to obtain tangible rewards. Most typically,
the perpetrators held hostages as a consequence of their failure to carry out a crime
successfully.
‘Expressive’ mode in which a partner or relative is typically involved. These
hostage-takers are likely to hold as a hostage an individual with whom they have
a close but troubled relationship, often a wife or a girlfriend. Hostage taking arises
from the frustration of the perpetrator, and they are likely to attempt to redress
the problems of the relationship following a long period of difficulty.
In Conservative situations, perpetrators tend to have objectives that are related
to their own systems of beliefs and values. They plan in advance, anticipating
the impact their actions will have. They make various demands and goals for the
outcome. These are often related to their own beliefs or values and their political
backgrounds. They have little personal interest in their victims and view them
merely as means of achieving their goals.
(From Yokota, 2013, p. 70–71)
The way the hostage negotiator will deal with the situation would normally vary
considerably depending on what sort of event it was. A planned terrorist incident
would, for example, require a quite different approach from interacting with a
mentally disturbed individual, or one who has taken a family member hostage. A psy-
chologist may often be on-hand to help with this as well as supporting the police
officers in dealing with their own stresses.
One of the key psychological research studies on stress is the work carried out by
Holmes and Rahe (1967). They claimed that certain events in life can cause stress and
increase susceptibility to illness. Holmes and Rahe’s (1967) Social Readjustment
Rating Scale (SRRS) lists forty-three life events that are considered to be stressful.
190 AREAS OF APPLICATION
These events are ranked on a scale of stressfulness. The death of a spouse is rated as
the highest stressful life event and the lowest is a minor violation of the law. However,
there is likely to be variation in how individuals deal with stressful life events and the
scale does not take this into account. The scale also neglects other factors that may
play a role in stress and susceptibility to illness, such as lifestyle and diet. Sewell (1983)
has developed a critical life events scale specifically for law enforcement officers. The
scale consists of 144 items. At the top of the scale is the death of a partner in the line
of duty and at the lower end of the scale the completion of a routine report.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
STRESS MANAGEMENT
Stress can be managed at an individual and organisational level. In the area of police
stress, the focus has tended to be on the management of stress at the organisational
level (Bull, Bustin, Evans & Gahagan 1983). In two studies that asked officers about
ways of managing job stress, five organisational factors were identified: better training
to cope with demanding situations, support from senior ranks, better familiarity with
police procedures, improved police-community relations and fewer bureaucratic
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 191
obstacles (Gudjonsson & Adlam, 1982; Gudjonsson, 1983, cited in Hollin, 1989: 144).
Interestingly, no reference was made to the offer of counselling for officers who had
experienced or witnessed a traumatic event. This may be because of the police’s
negative view and rejection of psychological services (Miller, 1995). This relates to
the occupational culture of the police, whereby counselling services are likely to be
perceived as an inability to cope and frowned upon by colleagues. It is important
for the police service to develop an understanding of which aspects of the job cause
stress and also how individuals deal with stress to determine how best they can manage
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
One further area of recent social psychological research that is informing the work
of the police is the study of crowd behaviour (Drury & Scott, 2011). The result of
this work is finding its way into policy and training of those police officers who deal
with protests and other events demanding maintenance of public order (Hoggett &
Stott, 2012). These studies of crowd behaviour draw on a form of participant
observation (being part of the crowd) and interviews of those participating in public
events. They challenge earlier views that crowds were primitive challenges to civilised
society. Instead, they offer a nuanced view of individuals within crowds forming
an identity of themselves in relation to any other groups, notably law enforcement
that may challenge them. The implications of this are that public order policing
needs to be aware of the individuals within a crowd and not treat any large group of
192 AREAS OF APPLICATION
people as a homogenous mass. When training in the social psychological issues are acted
on notable reductions in public disorder are possible (Stott, Hoggett & Pearson, 2012).
POLICE CULTURE
create a working climate and foster patterns of behaviour that are supportive of but
can inhibit or exacerbate individuals adverse reactions to stressor exposure’ (p. 259).
Police forces consist of predominantly male officers, it is, therefore, of no surprise that
the police culture has a ‘strong masculine ethos’ (Brown, 2000). Females and ethnic
minorities are still underrepresented in the police force (Ainsworth, 2002). The police
develop informal support systems with their colleagues, but they do not discuss and
share emotional issues (Stephens, Long & Miller, 1997). This links back to the com-
ments outlined above regarding the acceptance and acknowledgement of the need for
counselling and critical incident debriefing to address the emotional pressures police
officers may experience.
CONCLUSIONS
The role of a police officer is diverse and multifaceted, which poses a challenge for
selection. Attempts to deal with this have been made using psychometric testing in
police recruitment and selection in the USA. However, research on the key attributes
required for suitable police recruits is inconclusive. The use of assessment centres is
now a standard procedure in the recruitment and selection process for police officers in
the UK. Assessment involves a structured interview, role-play exercises, numerical and
verbal testing.
The training of police officers is generally conducted ‘in-house’, within the police
organisation. In the UK, training courses are rarely taught by psychologists. The most
notable contribution of British psychologists to police training is an evaluation of the HAT
programme for new recruits carried out by Bull and Horncastle (1986). However, there
have been recent advancements in UK police training, with the consultation and input
from academics and practitioners in the development and implementation of training
programmes. This has included the utilisation of simulation exercises, a form of
immersive learning that offers a unique and effective way of training officers in
specialised roles and investigations.
Policing is one of the most stressful occupations. Work stressors vary according to
the rank of officer and the policing tasks they are involved in. Stress needs to be
managed at both an individual and organisational level. Psychological counselling and
CISD are two ways in which stress in policing can be addressed. Police culture impacts
upon the stress experienced by officers. The tough, masculine ethos and informal
support system mean officers are likely to keep their emotional feelings bottled up and
hold a negative view of psychological services.
PSYCHOLOGY AND THE POLICE 193
Over recent years, the police service in the UK has become much more open to input
from psychologists, especially with regard to the training of officers and the development
of training programmes, which marks a positive step forward. To further contribute
psychological principles, theories and research into the recruitment, selection and training
of police officers, the police service and psychologists must work together and foster
closer links to further understanding of each other’s line of work. The combination of
practical knowledge and experience of police procedures and scientific endeavour will
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
produce more effective developments within the field of psychology and policing.
• Discuss the advantages and disadvantages of the current methods used to recruit
and select police officers.
• How can psychology be used to advance the training of police officers?
• Outline the key stressors related to police work and the ways in which stress can be
managed.
Books
Ainsworth, P. B. (2002). Psychology and policing. Cullompton: Willan Publishing.
Blagdon, N. (2012). Policing and psychology. London: Sage.
Brown, J. M., & Campbell, E. A. (1994). Stress and policing: sources and strategies. New York:
Wiley.
Bull, R., Bustin, R., Evans P., & Gahagan, D. (1983). Psychology for police officers. Chichester:
Wiley.
Oxburgh, G., Myklebust, T., Grant, T., Milne, R. R. (2015). Communication in investigative
and legal contexts: integrated approaches from forensic psychology, linguistics and law enforcement.
Chichester: Wiley.
Toch, H. (2002). Stress in policing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
194 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Journal articles
The Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology publishes papers that deal with many of the issues
in the present chapter.
Bartol, C. R. (1996). Police psychology: then, now and beyond. Criminal Justice and Behaviour,
23, 70–89.
Bull, R., & Horncastle, P. (1994). Evaluation of police recruit training involving psychology.
Psychology, Crime and Law, 1, 143–9.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Ross, L., & Alison, L. (1999). Critical incident stress debriefing and its effects on coping
strategies and anger in a sample of Australian police officers involved in shooting incidents.
Work and Stress, 13(2), 144–61.
Websites
The following website outlines the qualities of potential officers by the College of Policing:
http://recruit.college.police.uk/Pages/home.aspx
The following website gives an overview of the symptoms and effects of stress causes and
impacts on police officers:
www.nij.gov/topics/law-enforcement/officer-safety/stress-fatigue/Pages/welcome.aspx
The following website gives an overview of how psychology can be integrated into every day
policing:
https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-27/edition-9/ferguson-policing-and-social-
psychology
14 Psychology
in court
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
This chapter provides an overview of forensic psychology in its most literal form –
direct application to the courts. Psychology contributes to and explores the judicial
process before the trial, during it, and after. Typically, this is by providing expert
testimony in a clinical, experimental, actuarial or advisory role. A variety of procedures,
including intensive interviews and psychometric procedures, are drawn upon. Social
psychologists have studied the legal process through mock juries and surveys. These
studies include how juries are influenced and the impact of external influences on
aspects of the legal process.
Haward (1981; see also Gudjonsson & Haward, 1998) identified four roles that a forensic
psychologist may fulfil depending on the nature of the legal case. These are clinical,
experimental, actuarial and advisory. In some instances, the psychologist may fulfil more
than one of these roles. In all but the advisory role, the psychologist acts as an expert
196 AREAS OF APPLICATION
witness. This means that, unlike other witnesses who are restricted to stating facts, experts
are allowed to provide opinion evidence on issues that are considered outside the know-
ledge of the jury or judiciary and within their specialist competence (Kapardis, 1997;
Ogloff & Polvi, 1998; Cox, 1999; Schuller & Ogloff, 2001; Hess, 2006). Within most
Western legal systems expert witnesses, like all witnesses, are subjected to examination-
in-chief by the party that called them, cross-examination and re-examination.
Psychologists usually submit reports in advance of giving evidence in court. If the
report is not deemed useful to the side that requested the report, it may not be
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
presented to the court. In this situation, the psychologist will not be asked to be a
witness in the actual court. If both the defence and prosecution accept the report, then
the psychologist may not have to defend it in person either. Consequently, writing a
clear report that addresses the issue the court is interested in is crucial.
Being an expert witness does, therefore, require some understanding of the legal
process. In the present chapter, it is assumed that the process is what is known as the
‘adversarial system’, which is the main process in most English language, Anglo-Saxon-
origin countries, notably the UK, the USA, Australia and Canada. This is the system
where a defence and prosecution are clearly identified, having distinct roles in the court.
In this process, most serious cases are tried in front of a jury of ordinary people. In
many other countries, for example, across mainland Europe and countries such as India
and South Africa, juries are far less usual. In those countries, the final decisions are
made by a judge or magistrate, so it is known as a ‘magisterial system’. In the absence
of a jury and with the expert not explicitly called by the defence or the prosecution,
the expert has a somewhat different, and often more significant role. In the adversarial
system, the judge usually decides the sentence the convicted person will receive.
Although in some US cases, the jury may decide this.
Act 1991, innocent individuals go free and a range of disposals are now available for
those who have acted as charged (Ormerod, 2005).
In unfitness to plead deliberations, the question essentially is, ‘What is the defendant’s
state of mind at the present time, or at the time of the trial?’ An individual who was
seriously mentally disordered at the time of an offence may be fit to plead by the time
of their trial. Conversely, a person may be mentally stable during an offence, but may
later become disordered and be determined Unfit to Plead.
This is assessed through an intensive interview. Sometimes, the sorts of assessment
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
that is far less of a problem (if at all) when working in a therapeutic role (Melton,
Petrila, Poythress & Slobogin 2007). Accordingly, specialised psychometric tests may
be employed and corroborative evidence from other sources is important.
Although the clinical role of the forensic psychologist has some obvious overlap
with the work of forensic psychiatrists (Grisso, 1993), the training of psychologists,
grounded as it is in an understanding of human behaviour in general, does mean
that they can go beyond issues of mental disorder, to discuss personality, social and
interpersonal functioning and cognitive abilities. Psychologists also tend to use stand-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
ardised tests, whereas psychiatrists tend to focus on information gleaned from a clinical
interview (Grisso, 1993; Gudjonsson & Haward, 1998).
SYNDROME EVIDENCE
The legal process in many countries is greatly influenced by the medical framework,
which provides diagnoses and possibilities for treatment. As a consequence, especially
in the USA, a variety of ‘psychological syndromes’ have gained favour for use in court.
They are drawn on to explain, and often exonerate, defendants.
can or cannot remember. Claims of amnesia are made in many homicide cases.
Posttraumatic stress disorder
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is the development of characteristic symptoms
following exposure to extreme traumatic stressors involving direct personal experience of an
event that involves actual or threatened death or serious injury or witnessing or learning
about an event that involves death, injury or a threat to the physical integrity of another
person. The precipitating event is usually experienced with intense fear, terror and
helplessness. A successful PTSD defence usually results in a verdict of diminished
responsibility, rather than not guilty by reason of insanity.
Rape trauma syndrome
It is proposed that the trauma of being raped may itself lead to delays in reporting,
appearance of self-doubt in initial reports of an incident and other counterintuitive reactions.
This is used in court to explain to a jury why the reactions of a victim were not what they
might have expected (Kennedy, 2009).
Parental alienation syndrome
A contentious syndrome proposed particularly in child custody cases is the focused effort to
instil negative views of the other parent (Kennedy, 2009). This amounts to unjustified
disparagement of a loving parent. It is used in court, almost exclusively in divorce disputes to
undermine any viewpoints put forward by a child about a parent.
Personality disorders
As discussed in Chapter 3, personality disorders cover a range of conditions, the most
common one presented in court being psychopathy. Defence psychiatrists argue that this is
a medical condition that means the person does not have certain actions under control. This
is used to claim that violence and homicide committed by the defendant should be treated
as manslaughter or that the defendant has diminished responsibility (see Focus box 14.1).
credit, the police had apparently gone to great lengths to avoid bias in the procedure,
by seeking advice from a linguist. However, despite the fact that all voices in the parade
were apparently similar, only the defendant’s voice was taken from a police interview.
A psychologist subsequently conducted an experiment in which participants were asked
to identify which voices were taken from a police interview. The defendant’s voice
was identified at a rate beyond chance. This evidence was deemed admissible at trial.
The case resulted in a hung jury.
The second type of experimental role involves the psychologist testifying about the
state of knowledge regarding a particular psychological topic or process that is relevant
to the case (Pfeifer & Brigham, 1993; Kapardis, 1997; Schuller & Ogloff, 2001). For
example, Loftus (1991) described testifying in numerous cases about the vagaries of
eyewitness memory (discussed in Chapter 10). In this way, the psychologist takes
a particularly instructive position while giving evidence.
200 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Civil law
Psychologists are also involved as experts in civil disputes within the UK. For example,
in regard to the law of torts (i.e., ‘wrongs’), compensation is the goal. Psychologists have
been involved in cases in which a particular trauma, emotional distress or cognitive
impairment is claimed to have been sustained. Conditions such as PTSD and acute stress
disorder are common afflictions to be assessed. Once again, the possibility of deception
and malingering must be considered (Melton, Petrila, Poythress & Slobogin 2007).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Psychologists are also involved in areas of children’s and family law. This includes areas
such as child abuse and neglect, and child custody in cases of divorce.
Statistical evidence
The third role is the actuarial role. This involves applying statistical probabilities to
behaviours and events. This may be obtained from a literature search or by collecting
new observational data (Gudjonsson, 1996; Gudjonsson & Haward, 1998). Haward
(1981) described several examples of the actuarial role, such as ‘the probability of earning
a living with a given IQ, the probability of finding two identical cars passing a road
within a given period of time, (and) the probability of finding two persons with a given
number of personal characteristics in the same town’ (p. 55). Unsurprisingly, this role
is often adopted by statisticians or other professionals as well (see Activity box 14.1).
It is, perhaps, the least common of the four roles (Gudjonsson & Haward, 1998).
Advising lawyers
The final role is the advisory role. This involves advising lawyers about the psychological
evidence of other experts, pointing out weaknesses, and thus, assisting in the pre-
paration of cross-examination strategies (Haward, 1981). By its very nature, this role
is a destructive one, but can be seen as a form of peer review. Nonetheless, it is
important that the advisor draws attention to clearly unsound material without
becoming an advocate (Gudjonsson & Haward, 1998).
An interesting example of this is the study by Canter and Chester (1997). They
challenged the claims of Reverend Morton that he had a procedure he called CUSUM
(cumulative sum control chart) that could be used to determine whether text or
utterances were likely to contain the authorship of more than one person. This
procedure was being used in court to argue that confessions contained the utterances
of more than one person and could, therefore, be regarded as having been falsified.
Through carefully studying the way the CUSUM procedure operated, Canter was able
to show that its results were virtually random and did not distinguish authorship at all.
This was accepted by the courts and CUSUM evidence was no longer accepted.
Petrila, Poythress & Slobogin 2007). Over the past three decades, the latter has become
a prominent focus in the forensic mental health literature. Monahan (1981) reviewed
the extant literature and found that mental health professionals were accurate in
only one out of three violence predictions. This can be contrasted with current risk
assessment approaches that perform well above chance (Douglas, Cox & Webster,
1999; Davis & Ogloff, 2008).
The modern literature suggests that risk opinions made on the basis of unstructured,
intuitive clinical judgment are inadequate. Greater accuracy can be attained by using
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Fit to be executed
An area of court psychology that is unique to the USA is the study of how decisions
are made when the sentence of the defendant can be execution (reviewed by Lynch,
2009). In most such ‘capital cases’, the jury is called on to decide whether the convicted
person (almost always male) should be sentenced to death. The curiosity of the US
legal system is that jurors must be ‘death qualified’. This means that they do not hold
opinions about the death penalty, which are so strong that they prejudice their ability
to follow the law when deciding on the sentence. Therefore, this essentially
psychological issue about a juror’s opinion has to be determined by the court before
the person is allowed to be a member of the jury.
The research on how decisions are reached by juries in capital cases gives strong
support to the view that their decisions are, in fact, not fair within the legal
requirements and those of the American constitution. In general, the selection of jurors
202 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Three medical witnesses testified that Byrne was a ‘sexual psychopath’ who
had suffered from violent perverted sexual desires from an early age and
which he found difficult or impossible to control, such that in some cases, he
had indulged them. These impulses or urges were stronger than the normal
impulse or urge of sex and the killing was done under such impulses or
urges. Indeed, when not under the influence of such desires, Byrne was
FOCUS 14.1
considered normal. All three doctors considered the killing was done under
the influence of Byrne’s perverted sexual desires. Although not amounting to
insanity, as defined by the McNaughton rules, his sexual psychopathy could
properly be described as partial insanity. Byrne’s diminished responsibility
plea was accepted, and he was convicted of manslaughter ([1960] 3 All
England Reports 1).
tends to be biased to those in favour of the death penalty (Luginbuhl & Middendorf,
1988). They also appear unable to follow instructions closely and are thrown back on
‘their own resources with few limits on their discretion’ (Lynch, 2009, p. 175). As a
consequence of this research, a number of US states have put a moratorium (no pun
intended) on executions until they can be sure the system has improved. This is a clear
case of psychological research influencing the judicial policy.
One further psychological aspect of this archaic punishment is that the 8th
Amendment of the US Constitution prohibits ‘cruel and unusual punishment’. This
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
has been interpreted as meaning that if a person is so mentally disturbed that they
cannot understand that they will suffer the death penalty then they cannot, therefore,
be executed. This, therefore, requires an assessment, usually by a psychiatrist, but
sometimes by a forensic psychologist, of whether the convicted person is mentally well
enough to be executed. These assessments draw on the various clinical psychology
issues discussed in earlier chapters.
A court case is an interesting set of interactions between many different people. The
different attorneys, the judge and when the jury is present, their interaction with each
other and with the various lawyers is interesting. These interactions are difficult to
study because recordings of all the details of what happens in court are complex and
hard to come by. Most importantly, in all jurisdictions in which juries occur, no direct
access to the jury is allowed. Their deliberations are secret, and usually, they are not
allowed to tell anyone else about them. However, sometimes, in the US, researchers
are allowed to interview people who have been on juries.
The most frequent way of studying jury decision making is to set up ‘mock juries’
(DeMatteo & Anumba, 2009). A common way of doing this is just to give
undergraduates descriptions of the information that would be available in a trial and
then ask them to reach a decision about the guilt or innocence of the defendant alone
or in a discussion. This obviously lacks the reality of an actual trial, but can reveal some
useful indicators. A more expensive and demanding process consists of inviting people
who are typical of those who could form a jury to attend a session that is run like an
actual court case. Various aspects of the proceedings are varied in the usual experimental
way, and the decision of the mock jury is then monitored. Those involved are also
likely to be questioned about their experiences and how they reached their decisions.
But, even here the simulation is likely to be shorter and less complex than an actual
case (Schuller & Yarmey, 2001). There are, therefore, still many questions raised about
the possibility of generalising results from studies of juries to actual cases.
From these studies, various models have been proposed of jury decision-making
(Groscup & Tallon, 2009). Some of these emphasise the cognitive challenge that jurors’
have to face and how they may deal with those challenges by putting more weight
on whether they trust the person providing the evidence than on the evidence itself
204 AREAS OF APPLICATION
(Chaiken, 1980). Surprisingly, little research has been carried out on the social pro-
cesses of jury discussions, presumably because such data are so difficult to collect and
analyse. More emphasis has been given to the ethnic background of jurors and how
that might influence their decisions (Sommer, 2006). Attempts have also been made
to create models of how juries come to a verdict. Two contrasting models are briefly
outlined below.
Mathematical approaches
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
One is a mathematical model in which jurors are assumed to give weight to each piece
of evidence in terms of whether it indicates guilt. To come to a verdict, they then
average the weighted values (Pennington & Hastie, 1981). This rather elegant approach
allows a variety of mathematical approaches to be applied. But, they all assume the
juror is a) relatively logical, b) deals with each piece of evidence separately from all
the others, c) is not influenced by the order of the evidence, d) there are no social
psychological matters at work in the discussion about the verdict.
The knowledge of how juries decide could be drawn on to influence how evidence
is presented in court in order to achieve the desired outcome. Quite rightly, there is
some debate about the ethics of doing this and whether it is an inappropriate way of
biasing the jury. The book by John Gresham and later film The Runaway Jury is a witty
and enthralling exploration of the extremes of what might be possible if psychological
techniques were to be applied unethically.
The possibility for these forms of influence is particularly present in US legal
procedures that allow great flexibility in selecting members of the jury. The idea behind
this is that jurors are expected to be neutral and objective. Consequently, any hint
that a juror may be biased one way or the other can be the basis for having that person
excluded from the jury. However, as Lieberman and Olson (2009) make clear in their
review of studies of jury selection, the process has far less influence on the verdict
reached than may be expected. It is the evidence and how it is presented that has the
dominant influence.
PSYCHOLOGY IN COURT 205
ACTIVITY 14.1
intercourse with a 14-year-old boy 4 hours after meeting him in a local park. Neither
man has any prior criminal history for sexual or non-sexual offences. Neither has a
history of substance abuse. Neither man has ever received treatment for a mental
illness. Clinical psychologists indicate that neither man suffers from a psychotic
illness. With this information, you have been asked to provide an opinion about the
risk that each man may pose for committing further sexual offences. From the
limited information described here, who would you consider to pose the greatest
risk and what information would you focus on to make this judgment? One solution
is provided at the end of this chapter (in Activity box 14.2).
One bias which turns out to be very difficult to combat is any information that
jurors may have about the case prior to the trial. This can also include evidence that
becomes available during the trial, which the jury is told to ignore because it is
inadmissible. These matters are more significant in the USA than in other jurisdictions
because freedom of information traditions allow a great deal to be said about a court
case before it comes to trial. In the UK, the sub judice laws make it illegal to comment
on any features of a case once a person has been charged, so ‘trial by media’ is less
common. To reduce the bias of information external to what is revealed in court, jurors
are often instructed to disregard anything they may have heard prior to being in court.
But intriguingly, as Lieberman, Arndt and Vess (2009) show, it is extremely difficult
for jurors to do this.
CONCLUSIONS
In its most literal form, the topic of this chapter is forensic psychology which is
concerned with ‘the collection, examination, and presentation of evidence for judicial
purposes’ (Haward, 1981, p. 21). The field has grown considerably since the first
experimental psychologist gave expert evidence in court in 1897. Present day forensic
psychology comprises four broad roles: clinical, experimental, actuarial and advisory.
Forensic psychologists provide opinions on a range of topics within both criminal and
civil law and have managed to be admitted in areas that were previously the domain of
the psychiatrist. Even the field of investigative psychology, developed to assist police
investigations, has seen some limited application in the courts, although this has largely
been seen to encroach upon the ultimate issue. Nonetheless, advances made within the
field of forensic psychology over the past century have clearly been enormous.
206 AREAS OF APPLICATION
ACTIVITY 14.2
instrument. He is above the age of 25, has been married, has not been charged
with non-sexual violence in his index offence, has no previous charges or
convictions and has not committed a sexual offence against someone outside of
his family, a stranger or a male. A score of zero places this offender in the ‘low’
category of risk. In the studies on which this measure was developed, 13 per
cent of the offenders who had a score of zero went on to sexually reoffend over
15 years. This is half the base rate in the development sample (which was 26 per
cent).
In contrast, the second offender has five risk factors. He is under the age of 25,
has never been in a cohabiting relationship and has committed a sexual offence
against a victim who is outside of his family, a stranger and a male. A score of five
places this man in the ‘moderate-high’ category of risk. In the development studies,
40 per cent of the offenders who had a score of five went on to sexually reoffend
over 15 years. This is considerably higher than the base rate of 26. Thus, based on
the static risk factors, the second offender would appear to pose a higher risk of
sexual recidivism than the first offender.
Those conducting risk assessments generally do not rely exclusively on the
results of the Static-99. Indeed, the manual for the instrument indicates that it
‘does not address all relevant risk factors for sexual offenders’ (Harris, Phenix,
Thornton & Hanson 2003, p. 3). Nonetheless, it does provide a useful anchor point
for conducting a risk assessment.
• Discuss the strengths and weaknesses of the various models used to understand
jury decision-making.
• What are the main roles for psychologists in court?
• In what sorts of civil court cases might psychology be relevant?
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Books
Costanzo, M., & Krauss, D. (2015). Forensic and legal psychology: psychological science applied to
law, 2nd ed. New York: Worth Publishers.
Goldstein, A. M. (Ed.). (2007). Forensic psychology: emerging topics and expanding roles. Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & Haward, L. R. C. (Eds.). (1998). Forensic psychology: a guide to practice.
London: Routledge.
Huss, M. T. (2013). Forensic psychology, 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Krauss, D., & Lieberman, J. (Eds.). (2009). Psychology in the courtroom: volume ii, psychological
expertise in court. Farnham: Ashgate.
Lieberman, J., & Krauss, D. (Eds.). (2009). Psychology in the courtroom: volume i, jury psychology.
Farnham: Ashgate.
Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (2007). Psychological evaluations
for the courts: a handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers, 3rd ed. New York:
Guilford Press.
Weiner, I. B., & Hess, A. K. (Eds.). (2006). The handbook of forensic psychology, 3rd ed. Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Journal articles
Brigham, J. (1999). What is forensic psychology anyway? Law and Human Behavior, 23, 273–98.
Cattell, J. M. (1895). Measurements of the accuracy of recollection. Science, 2, 761–6.
Heilbrun, K., & Brooks, S. (2010). Forensic psychology and forensic science: a proposed agenda
for the next decade. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 16(3), 219.
Risinger, D. M., & Loop, J. L. (2002). Three card monte, monty hall, modus operandi and
‘offender profiling’: some lessons of modern cognitive science for the law of evidence.
Cardozo Law Review, 24, 193–285.
Websites
The following website gives an overview of the roles forensic psychologists play in the court
room:
www.nwforensicpsychology.com/wordpress/articles-media/paperspublications
208 AREAS OF APPLICATION
The following website is an informative account of the roles and difficulties of forensic
psychology in Australia:
www.psychology.org.au/publications/inpsych/2010/august/allan/
This website gives an account of fitness to plead assessment:
www.psychologydirect.co.uk/what-is-a-fitness-to-plead-assessment/
Note
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
1 For simplicity laws in Great Britain are referred to here as the UK, but actually Scotland
has a somewhat different legal system to England and Wales and Northern Ireland law has
some differences too.
15 Psychology
in prison
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
Denying people their freedom is a common form of punishment. But, it carries with
it many unwanted and, often, unexpected consequences. Dealing with these conse-
quences is especially important if one objective of this punishment is to reduce the
likelihood that people will reoffend. This has become a major task for the increasing
number of psychologists who have been employed in prisons, especially in the UK and
Western Europe, over the last 60 years. Their task is to help mitigate the unwanted
effects of imprisonment. It is also to use the opportunity to work with prisoners in a
way that will reduce those aspects of their ways of seeing themselves and dealing with
others that underlie their offending. Various approaches to reducing reoffending are
considered with an explanation of the psychological issues involved.
The term ‘prison’ in this chapter covers all those places in which people are placed in
which the punishment is to deny them their freedom for a period of time. It includes
locations called jails (or goals), borstals, young offender institutions, and even in the
210 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
US, the euphemistically labelled ‘corrections’. In the UK, there are also ‘special
hospitals’ that are for people deemed mentally ill, but who have committed crimes for
which they are imprisoned. It is crucial to emphasise that in Western democracies, the
punishment that a prison provides is not intended to be any form of discomfort or
torture. It is only to curtail liberty. In both US law and European law, as well as in
many other countries, ‘cruel and inhuman’ punishment is illegal. Many prisoners who
have experienced extremely harrowing conditions in prison have successfully won cases
against the authorities because the circumstances of their incarceration were illegal.
In general, the imprisonment has a number of purposes. The most obvious one is
to protect the public by stopping offenders having the opportunity to commit any
crimes outside of prison. Another clear objective is to punish people for their misdeeds.
It is also intended to be a deterrent to stop others committing crimes or the prisoner
committing other crimes in the future. In enlightened countries, it is also regarded as
being a basis for rehabilitating offenders, so that whatever gave rise to them committing
crimes would be dealt with, so that they did not commit crimes in the future. It has
been known for some time that, broadly speaking, imprisonment fails in all these
purposes (Sommer, 1976).
Recidivism rates
Recidivism is the term used to describe a person committing a crime after having
been punished for something similar. The rate at which people do commit subsequent
PSYCHOLOGY IN PRISON 211
crimes, the ‘recidivism rate’, varies depending on the time period that is used to
calculate when the subsequent crime happens. But, however it is calculated, it is
certainly one important measure of the effect of locking a person up. Yet, the rates
with which people reoffend are stubbornly consistent as Havelock Ellis (1901) in his
informative book The Criminal pointed out. He noted that about two-thirds of the
offenders committed further crimes after being in prison. However, despite many
attempts at apparent prison reform studies, a century after Havelock Ellis, very similar
broad recidivism rates of over 60 per cent are still reported (e.g., Redondo, Sanchez-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
However, there are some interventions that are much more effective reducing
recidivism rates to less than 40 per cent (Redondo, Sanchez-Meca & Garrido 2002).
So, even though this is far from perfect, it does show that psychological approaches
can have some benefit. A range of the approaches being used are, therefore, considered
below.
PEOPLE IN PRISON
The main reason why imprisonment can be so ineffective was captured in a slightly
archaic form of words over a century ago by Havelock Ellis (1901):
If, as now scarcely admits of questions, every truly criminal act proceeds from a
person who is, temporarily or permanently, in a more or less abnormal condition,
the notion of ‘punishment’ loses much of its foundation. We cannot punish a
monstrosity for acting according to its monstrous state.
(p. 295)
Our discussions in early chapters of the psychological and social basis of criminality
raise doubts about the general abnormality or ‘monstrous’ nature of all criminals, but
the idea that when committing a crime offenders are driven by various processes that
neutralise the reflection on possible punishment is certainly relevant. Treatments in
prison, therefore, are most likely to be effective if they address the fundamental processes
that give rise to criminality, rather than just providing some general form of punishment.
The challenge, though, is that many aspects of prison life may itself give rise to
processes that encourage general criminality. Therefore, psychological support in prison
has to mitigate against these deleterious effects as well as helping offenders overcome
whatever it was that brought them into prison in the first place.
212 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Being in prison is emotionally taxing, although generally, it has been found that the
most depressing effect of confinement is that the majority of prisoners who have served
time in prison return to the outside world with little or no permanent ‘clinically
diagnosable’ psychological disorders as a result (Haney, 1997). Nonetheless, there are
many other effects that may not emerge so clearly as clinical diagnoses.
These effects were first articulated in Goffman’s (1961) book Asylums. He identified
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
what he called ‘total institutions’, places in which every aspect of the residents’ daily
life are controlled by the institution, where, when and how to eat and sleep, who is
allowed to meet with whom and when. All those aspects of daily life that most people
take for granted as under their own control are actually managed in detail by others.
Prisons are the extreme example of total institutions.
Imprisonment apparently does not ordinarily cause people to be mentally ill,
although as discussed in Chapter 3, there is a considerable amount of mental illness
among prisoners. Although for at least some, incarceration can generate damaging,
long-term psychological change (Bonta & Gendreau, 1990). Most agree that the more
psychologically demanding the type of the imprisonment, the greater the number of
people who will suffer and the deeper the damage that they will experience (Bonta &
Gendreau, 1990). As Goffman (1961) was the first to emphasise, the norms and organ-
isation of a total institution do have psychologically debilitating effects. This is outlined
in Focus box 15.1.
of others. Some learn to project a ‘hard man’ image that conveys the
ability for violence, so that they are not exploited. This leads to:
About 200 people a year die in UK prisons and over 4,000 a year in the USA (Rohloff
& Ginder, 2015). Around half of these deaths are caused by suicide. Much of this is
a consequence of the experience of prison life and the isolation from those outside
who may have provided social support previously. Attempts to prevent, or at least
reduce, these problems together with the other difficulties outlined in Focus box 15.1
may be carried out by psychologists on an individual basis or by helping to implement
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
PSYCHOLOGICAL INTERVENTIONS
The most common form of psychological intervention in prison, usually carried out
in one-to-one sessions, is Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT). An illustration of
this process is given in Focus box 15.2. CBT is based on the theory that most unwanted
thinking patterns and emotional and behavioural reactions are learnt over a long period
of time. The aim is to identify the thinking that is causing those unwanted feelings
and behaviours and learn to replace them with more positive thoughts.
CBT helps prisoners to make sense of the problems they may experience by
breaking them into smaller parts. This makes it easier to see how they are connected
and to formulate manageable ways of dealing with each of them. These parts are:
• A situation (a problem, event or difficult situation)
From this can follow:
• Thoughts
• Emotions
• Physical feelings
And, this will determine:
• Actions
An example of CBT with a prisoner
SITUATION: You’re expecting a visit from your partner. She has promised to
arrive by 2pm. At 2.15pm, you’re escorted to the visits room and she’s not
there.
HELPFUL UNHELPFUL
THOUGHTS: Perhaps she’s stuck in traffic She’s found someone else.
or missed the bus. She’ll be She’s dumped me and can’t face
here soon. me to tell me.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
EMOTIONAL Happy, positive, in high spirits. Angry, upset, jealous, low, sad,
FEELINGS: rejected.
PHYSICAL: None – feel comfortable. Stomach cramps, low energy,
feel sick.
ACTION: Wait quietly, get a coffee and Go back to the wing, telephone
chat with prison staff. her tonight, accuse her of being
unfaithful and tell her exactly
what I think of her.
The same situation has led to two very different results, depending on
how you thought about the situation. How you think has affected how you
felt and what you did. In the example in the right hand column, you’ve
jumped to a conclusion without very much evidence for it – and this matters,
because it’s led to:
Situation
Thoughts
Actions Feelings
FOCUS 15.2
This ‘vicious circle’ can make you feel worse. It can even create new
situations that make you feel worse. You can start to believe quite unrealistic
(and unpleasant) things about yourself. This happens because, when we are
distressed, we are more likely to jump to conclusions and to interpret things
in extreme and unhelpful ways.
216 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Each one of these can affect the others. How a person thinks about a problem can
affect how they feel physically and emotionally. It can also alter what the person does
about it.
Overall, CBT programmes do seem to be effective in reducing recidivism (as
reviewed, for example, by Lipton, Pearson, Cleland & Yee 2002). Although the effects
are significant statistically and socially, they are not enormous at the extreme reducing
the typical recidivism rate of around 60 per cent to about 40 per cent (Lipton, Pearson,
Cleland & Yee 2002). Although typically, with CBT as the starting point, well-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
designed interventions matched to the needs and abilities of the offender and delivered
systematically are only likely to reduce the base rate of offender reconviction by around
10 per cent (Hollin, 1999). This shows that psychological intervention can never be
the whole answer to criminality; cultural, social and economic process are also of great
importance as emphasised in Chapter 4.
Another form of therapy that combines individual sessions with group sessions is
Dialectical Behaviour Therapy (DBT). It has many similarities to CBT, but was
especially developed for working with people with personality disorder (described in
Chapter 3) who may have unusually high emotional reaction to situations and not have
learnt how to relate to others effectively. As the person who developed this approach
puts it:
Both between and during sessions, the therapist actively teaches and reinforces
adaptive behaviors, especially as they occur within the therapeutic relationship
. . . The emphasis is on teaching patients how to manage emotional trauma rather
than reducing or taking them out of crises. . . . contact with the individual
therapist between sessions is part of DBT procedures.
(Linehan, 1993)
In other words, it focuses on helping the individual to develop the skills to recog-
nise the situations in which heightened unproductive emotions are likely to happen
and to learn ways of dealing with those effectively.
Many of the psychological interventions in prison deal directly with the problems of
the offending behaviour, rather than general issues of the psychological difficulties the
person is experiencing. These all build on the cognitive behavioural approach, but are
shaped to deal directly with the criminal actions. A variety of such programmes has been
developed in the UK prison system. Some of the main ones in use are outlined below.
Each programme of intervention used in the prison service is designed around the
following criteria:
• A clear model of change – There must be an explicit model to explain how the
programme is intended to bring about relevant change in offenders. The
programme’s theory manual must explain who the programme is for and which
areas of risk it will reduce. It must specify how it will do this and what is achieved
at each stage of the programme. It must describe why this combination of targets
PSYCHOLOGY IN PRISON 217
and methods is likely to work with the offenders selected. Evidence from existing
research must be given to support the approach. The methods and exercises in
the programme manual have to fit with the theory manual.
• Selection of offenders – There must be clear specification of the types of offender for
whom the programme is intended, and the methods used to select them. The
programme has to identify the characteristics of the offenders selected. These
should include the nature of the offences that the programme is tackling, risk,
motivation, learning style, gender and race. The measures used to assess these must
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Staff should be positively committed to the programme. The content and the
methods of teaching should match the way participants learn best and motivate
them to want to change.
• Continuity of programmes and services – There must be clear links between the
programme and the overall management of the offender, both during a prison
sentence and in the context of community supervision. Relevant information is
shared. Key agencies concerned with protection of the public are kept informed
to aid work with victims and monitor offenders.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
This programme is run by both the prison and probation service and is designed to
reinforce learning from general offending programmes (such as ETS) through skills
rehearsal and relapse prevention.
The controlling anger and learning to manage it (CALM) programme is not designed
to target offenders who have a poor institutional history of anger/aggression alone.
PSYCHOLOGY IN PRISON 219
In order for a sex offender to stop offending, they need to be motivated to do so and
they need to possess the insight, skills and strategies to avoid risk and control
temptation. The prison service sex offender treatment programme (SOTP) is based
on established cognitive behavioural principles of the type known to be most effective
in reducing risk of reoffending. The cognitive behavioural approach to treatment
teaches clients to understand and control thinking, feelings and behaviour to achieve
this goal.
A range of programmes is available for sexual offenders, providing a menu that is
offered according to the level of risk and need of the offender. These are:
• Core programme: The treatment goals of this programme include helping
offenders develop an understanding of how and why sexual offences were
committed to increase awareness of victim harm issues and to develop meaningful
life goals as part of a relapsed prevention plan. The programme is targeted at male
medium- and high-risk sex offenders, and challenges thinking patterns, develops
victim empathy and relapse prevention skills.
• Extended programme: A supplementary programme for high-risk offenders
covering five treatment need areas: dysfunctional thinking styles, emotion
management, offence-related sexual fantasy, intimacy skills and inadequate relapse
prevention plans. It is targeted at male high- and very high-risk sex offenders.
• Adapted programme: Treatment goals are similar to the core programme,
but treatment methods are adapted to suit learning-disabled sex offenders across
all risk levels. The programme is designed to increase sexual knowledge, modify
offence-justifying thinking, develop ability to recognise feelings in themselves and
others, to gain an understanding of victim harm and to develop relapse prevention
skills.
• Rolling programme: This programme covers the same topics as the core
programme, but with less emphasis on obtaining an adequate offence account, and
more emphasis on relationship skills and attachment deficits. It is targeted at male
low- and medium-risk sex offenders, but sex offenders who have completed
primary treatment programmes and who are serving long sentences can attend as
a ‘top-up’ programme.
• Booster programme: This programme is designed to provide an opportunity
for offenders to refresh their learning in treatment and to prepare for additional
relapse prevention and release work.
222 AREAS OF APPLICATION
• Offending behaviour
• Related psychological needs, for example, to manage depression, anger or
anxiety
They work with individuals, and with organisations, within the judicial and
penal systems, such as:
• Offenders
• Victims
• Witnesses
• Probation services
• Judges and juries
• Prisoners and prison staff
quality
• Overseeing the training of prison/probation service staff
• Overseeing the provision of support during serious incidents
• Advising prison governors on incidents such as riots, demonstrations and
hostage-taking
• Attending team and area meetings
PSYCHOLOGY IN PRISON 223
The healthy relationships programme (HRP) intervention is designed for male offen-
ders who demonstrate a risk of being violent to an intimate female partner. High-risk
offenders receive a high intensity programme, while lower-risk offenders receive a
moderate intensity programme. Both programmes target attitudes supporting or con-
doning domestic violence, poor emotional control, skills deficits and motivational
issues.
CHROMIS
Chromis is a complex and intensive programme that aims to reduce violence in high-
risk offenders whose level or combination of psychopathic traits disrupts their ability
to accept treatment and change. Chromis has been specifically designed to meet the
needs of highly psychopathic individuals and provides participants with the skills to
reduce and manage their risk.
CONCLUSIONS
• Taking into consideration the ‘What Works’ literature, critically evaluate its usefulness
in the punishment or rehabilitation debate.
• Consider ways in which you might intervene with a prisoner displaying anger
management problems towards other prisoners.
• Why is skills-based training used in offending behaviour programmes?
Books
Caulfield, L., & Wilkinson, D. (2014). Forensic psychology (undergraduate revision guide). London:
Pearson.
Hollin, C. R. (Ed). (2001). Handbook of offender assessment and treatment. Chichester: Wiley.
Liebling, A., & Maruna, S. (2005). The effects of imprisonment. London: Routledge.
McGuire, J. (Ed.). (1995). What works: reducing reoffending – guidelines from research and practice.
Chichester: Wiley.
Needs, A., & Towl, G. J. (2003). Applying psychology to forensic practice. Oxford: Blackwell.
Sommer, R. (1976). The end of imprisonment. London: Oxford University Press.
Towl, G. J., & Crighton, D. A. (1996). The handbook of psychology for forensic practitioners. London:
Routledge.
Journal articles
Andrews, D. A., Zinger, I., Hoge, R. D., Bonta, J., Gendreau, P. & Cullen, F. T. (1990). Does
correctional treatment work? A clinically relevant and psychologically informed meta-
analysis. Criminology, 28, 369–404.
PSYCHOLOGY IN PRISON 225
Izzo, R. L., & Ross, R. R. (1990). Meta-analysis of rehabilitation programmes for juvenile
delinquents. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 17, 134–42.
Ogloff, J. R. P., & Davis, M. R. (2004). Advances in offender assessment and rehabilitation:
contributions of the risk-need-responsivity approach. Psychology, Crime and Law, 10(3),
229–42.
Mears, D. P., Cochran, J. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2015). Incarceration heterogeneity and its
implications for assessing the effectiveness of imprisonment on recidivism. Criminal Justice
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Websites
The following website gives an overview of the objectives and principles of prisons in the UK:
www.justice.gov.uk/about/hmps/
The following website gives some facts and figures about mental health in prison:
www.prisonreformtrust.org.uk/projectsresearch/mentalhealth
The following website gives an overview of the treatment programmes available in prison:
www.justice.gov.uk/offenders/before-after-release/obp
16 Concerning
victims
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
SUMMARY
People who suffer crimes, the victims, have often been ignored in many areas of
criminology and crime psychology that tend to focus on the criminals. Over the recent
years, this gap is being filled by studies of who becomes a victim, the consequences
of experiencing and fearing crime and how criminal justice systems may take more
account of helping victims, rather than just punishing the culprits. This chapter, there-
fore, focuses on fear of crime, the experience of crime, who becomes a victim and
how victimisation occurs.
FEAR OF CRIME
Fear of crime is an area that is increasingly studied (Vanderveen, 2006), with some
law enforcement agencies even aiming to reduce the fear of crime as well as the actual
crime levels. These studies show that there is neither a close relationship between fear
of crime and actual crime levels, or fear of crime and the actual experience of crime.
This somewhat paradoxical finding that being a direct victim of an offence has no rela-
tionship with levels of fearfulness (DuBow, McCabe & Kaplan 1979) has opened up
the consideration that what is feared is not the possibility of crime but of being a victim.
CONCERNING VICTIMS 227
The common-sense hypothesis that those who have been victimised before are likely
to have an increased estimation of their own risk of victimisation has no clear support
for this premise. Box, Hale and Andrews (1988) found previous victims had lower
levels of fear than those who had not suffered a crime. Skogan and Maxfield (1981)
found previous victims to be more fearful, but the levels of fear varied by crime as
well as age and sex. Although Liska, Sanchirico and Reed (1988) did find that those
who had been victims were more fearful than those who had not. Rountree (1998)
showed that the type of victimisation experienced influenced the nature of the fear.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Victims of violent crimes had increased levels of fear of both property and violent
crime, but property victims only had increased fear of property crime.
In general, then, fear of crime is often highest in those people who feel vulnerable, such
as the infirm elderly, but they are the least likely to have directly suffered a crime (Warr,
1987). Over the last decade, a growing number of people think crime is increasing, but,
in fact, crime has generally been decreasing. The murder rate in Oxford at the time of
William Shakespeare was many times higher than it is anywhere in the UK today.
EXPERIENCE OF CRIME
Although most crimes are legally defined in terms of physical injury or loss of pro-
perty, there are also a range of psychological consequences that legal processes tend
to undervalue, except in certain aspects of sentencing. Focus box 16.2 summarises the
results of some studies of the experience of being burgled. This demonstrates the trauma
that can be caused even when there is no direct contact between the criminal and the
victim. All forms of crime, then, from burglary to rape can have lasting psychological
effects on the victims far beyond any economic or physical consequences. These
consequences can include posttraumatic stress (Griffing et al., 2006).
Two stages are often identified in response to rape (Burgess & Holmstrom, 1974).
In the hours just after the assault, victims may experience shock, disbelief, anger and
general anxiety. This is likely to be accompanied by confusion and disorganisation in
their activities with considerable general fear. Later on, as victims begin to put their
life back together, they are likely to feel humiliation, embarrassment and a growing
desire for revenge.
One of the most debilitating psychological aspects of this is when victims blame
themselves in some way for what happened. They may think they gave the wrong
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
signals, through their actions, what they said or wore. In some cultures, the tendency
to blame the victims can be so strong that they accept their culpability quite
inappropriately.
Women, in particular, experience rape as life threatening, even if there were no
direct physical or verbal threats. This, of course, aggravates all the other anxieties
associated with unwanted sexual activity. There are big variations, though, in how
victims react to sexual assaults. Those with more sexual experience may be more able
to find the ability to pull through and deal with the trauma. Also, family and social
support is very significant in helping victims to cope.
Male victims
Men who experience rape can suffer particular traumas. These can be experienced
when their assailants are women as well as men. Men may feel that the attack chal-
lenges their identity as men. Many men also feel that they are usually in control in
social situations and sexual encounters and have some autonomy over their intimate
relationships. A rape can shatter all that and make them feel especially vulnerable and
even guilty in some ill-defined way for not being manly enough.
It is useful to recognise different situations in which male rape can occur, although
these situations have parallels when the victims are women.
• The victim may be overwhelmed by physical forces that he is unable to resist.
• A friendly, mildly sexual encounter may be taken further than the victim wishes.
• The victim may be trapped in a situation he cannot manage due to substance abuse
or unwanted drugs.
• Threats may be used to coerce the victim.
• Children will experience physical assault as humiliation, and thus, have lowered
self-esteem.
• Their relationships with other children will be problematic, expressed as aggression
towards others, hyperactivity, truancy, inability to form friendships and poor social
skills. Self-destructive behaviour, including excessive risk-taking, may also be
present.
• Their inability to be part of a social group or to relate to others can make the
educational process very challenging for them, with poorer cognitive and language
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
decreased leisure activities create a renewed dependence of the victim on their family
to meet these needs.
One particular aspect of the interview that a forensic psychologist will take account
of is how the victim deals with the interview process itself, sometimes called response
style. These can take a number of different characteristics:
• Malingering, especially the deliberate fabrication of symptoms or greatly exag-
gerating them.
• Minimisation, the denial of any symptoms or a reduction in the account of their
seriousness.
• Distraction, dealing with questions by going off at a tangent to talk about irrele-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Victim or survivor?
There is a problem with identifying the traumatic consequences of crime and devel-
oping accounts of victims’ experiences that can be taken to imply they have something
close to a medical problem. This can lead to them being assigned a clinical label that
can then ignore their unique experiences and ways of dealing with those experiences.
They may find themselves being treated as a medical ‘case’, rather than a person, with
expectations of how they will act being influenced by general ideas about those sorts
of ‘cases’, rather than the special way the particular person is acting.
Many victims are, therefore, encouraged not to think of themselves in those terms,
but as people who have survived something awful. This also gives them confidence
to take back control of their lives and not allow the offender still to exert an influence
over what they do by continuing to give rise to their fears and anxieties. One way of
helping victims feel empowered is by confronting the person who attacked them or
stole from them. This can include testifying in court or being part of a restorative justice
process described below.
Secondary victimisation
Many crimes have an influence beyond their immediate victims, family, friends and
neighbours. Even people who happen to be passing-by and witness what happened
can be disturbed by a criminal event. When it is a major event, such as the increasing
number of terrorist attacks in major cities, then the consequent trauma spreads around
the world. In New York, it was reported that there was a 25 per cent increase in
alcohol consumption after the terrorist attack on the Twin Towers on 9/11, a sign of
the increase in fear and anxiety.
232 AREAS OF APPLICATION
lived than the effects of violent crime, there is a clear cost above and beyond
the loss of possessions or damage to property (Maguire & Corbett, 1987). In
the 2006 British Crime Survey (BCS) burglary victims reported the following:
• 86 per cent were emotionally affected
• 56 per cent said anger was experienced
• 46 per cent reported annoyance
• 28 per cent to feelings of fear
• 10 per cent experienced depression
• 11 per cent suffered anxiety or panic attacks
FOCUS 16.2
Bennett and Wright (1984) report that many feel that they became
paranoid and distrustful even of neighbours and friends. They have an
overwhelming sense that the crime was committed by somebody known to
them. Not an unfounded paranoia, with the 2006 BCS reporting that 52 per
cent of offenders are known to the victim in some capacity.
CHARACTERISTICS OF VICTIMS
Not everyone has the same chance of becoming a victim (Nicholas, Kershaw & Walker
2008). Personal, behavioural and aspects of locations all have an influence. In general,
there are two contributory factors to whether a person becomes a victim or not.
• Their personal characteristics and vulnerabilities. A disorganised lifestyle, not
looking after property or being less able to look after oneself may be taken
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
advantage of by criminals.
• The location in which they are. Inevitably, living in or often visiting a high crime
area increases the likelihood of experiencing crime.
In the UK, the following groups are more than twice as likely to be burgled as the
average household:
• Young households
• Lone parents
• The unemployed
There are also big variations in the prevalence of crimes in different regions, especi-
ally property crimes. There are more crimes in cities and fewer in the countryside,
although the types of crimes vary, so comparisons have to be treated with caution.
There is not much cattle rustling in New York or London, and not a lot of fraudulent
bankers on the Yorkshire moors or Indiana prairies.
Violent crimes tend to have the same relative frequency across the UK. This contra-
dicts the general assumption that violence has a higher rate of incidence in the inner
city. There are many more people living in inner cities and many more vulnerable
people in inner cities than in small towns, so the actual number of violent crimes is
much higher.
The relative incidence of crimes can be very different when there are particular
local issues; gangs in Chicago or Detroit, vendettas between organised crime groups
and subsets of the population for whom violence is a way of life, as discussed in
Chapter 4. These may capture the attention of the mass media and raise public concern,
but the simple fact is that, in Western countries, the number of crimes reported overall
has been dropping steadily for the last 20 years or so.
The variations in experience of crime do reflect cultural aspects of who the victims
are. In the USA, African-Americans are almost twice as likely to experience a violent
crime as whites. In the UK and the USA, people in their teens and young adults are
twice as likely to experience violent crime compared with the rest of the population.
In general, as people get older, they are less likely to experience violent crime. A teen-
ager in the USA is ten times more likely to experience some sort of assault that a person
over 65 years of age.
Summarising many studies (e.g., reviewed by Dijk, Kesteren & Smit 2008), the
following features increase the chance of becoming a victim:
• Attractiveness target object – When a product is so valued that the offender can sell
it on to others, for example, an expensive car or the latest model mobile phone.
This list is ever-changing, especially as new desirable objects come onto the market.
It may be assumed it could also apply to victims of sexual assaults, but there is no
234 AREAS OF APPLICATION
clear evidence that women who are generally regarded as attractive have a higher
probability of being victims of such crimes. Although younger women are more
likely to suffer rape than older women, this is just as likely to be a consequence
of lifestyle, being out and about, mixing with a variety of people, than any special
attractiveness to rapists.
• Proximity – Whether the offender can access the target either geographically or
by person-to-person interaction. Some victims are selected because they are near
to where the offender operates, and thus, does not have to travel far.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
• Deviant place – Locations where crime can flourish, such as where large numbers
of people meet at the same time. For example, there tends to be more crime
around football matches. If these places are not patrolled by the police, they can
become known as ‘crime hot spots’, where people will be at a higher risk of
victimisation.
• Vulnerability – A lack of protection of property or the reduced ability of a person
to resist an attack increases the risk of being a victim. The elderly, very young or
infirm, or those with learning disabilities may all be more at risk if they are in the
wrong place at the wrong time.
All of the factors that can increase the possibility of a person becoming a victim are
relevant within a community of criminals, especially within a prison, as well as for
law-abiding citizens outside. Therefore, one of the challenges of imprisonment is to
create an environment in which vulnerable individuals do not become victims with
all the traumatic and personally destructive consequences that can entail as discussed
in the previous chapter.
One particularly vulnerable group of people are those women and men who earn
money as street-level sex workers. The great majority are under pressure from pimps
or due to drug addiction to earn money. Their circumstances do make them more
vulnerable to assault and homicide. Their vulnerability illustrates the circumstances that
can combine to increase the risk of suffering crime (Canter, Ioannou & Youngs, 2009):
• Sex work is illegal in most places, therefore, it has to be carried out away from
the relative safety of public settings. This is especially hazardous if it is outdoors
without any recourse to others for help.
• When a crime is committed against a sex worker, it is difficult for investigators
to gain information from the victim or her associates because of the way revelations
about their activities will open them up to prosecution. Furthermore, their clients
are also committing an illegal act, as well as one they would not want others to
know about, so are extremely reluctant to volunteer information to help the police.
It is also difficult for detectives to identify them.
• The general public are less likely to be concerned over such victims, and so, may
be unwilling to come forward with information that may help the police.
• Many, probably most, street sex workers have alcohol and/or drug dependency.
This makes them desperate to obtain money to maintain their addiction. This will
encourage them to take risks relating to where they go and with whom. If under
CONCERNING VICTIMS 235
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
the influence of drugs or alcohol, they are probably less able to defend themselves
or remember the details to report to the police.
• Criminals are aware of these vulnerabilities, and so, may prey on street sex workers.
They are the favoured victims of serial killers.
REPEAT VICTIMISATION
When considering the conditions that make people more at risk of being a victim of
a crime, it is apparent that many of those conditions are still present once a crime has
happened. As a consequence, it is to be expected that some people will be unfortunate
enough to experience crimes a number of times over a relatively short period, say, a
year. Yet, although this may seem obvious, it was only in the last decade or so that
this susceptibility to repeat crime has been recognised by law enforcement with a
resulting direct policy for tackling it, as reviewed, for example, by Outlaw, Ruback
and Britt (2002) in relation to the USA, and Perreault, Sauve and Burns (2010) for
Canada.
Studies show (Pease, 1998) that more than one in ten people who suffer a crime,
such as burglary, are likely to experience a similar crime within 12 months, if direct
efforts are not taken to reduce the risks. In the days and weeks following the crime,
the chances of a further similar crime are the greatest. Efforts to reduce future risk of
crime need to take account of the following factors that make people particularly prone
to repeat victimisation:
• Living in a crime-prone area, especially an area where many criminals live or where
they visit.
• Chaotic lifestyles, occupations or leisure activities that put people at risk of crime.
• Lack of concern to control the crime as sometimes happens with theft from shops
or petty vandalism, or in student accommodation.
236 AREAS OF APPLICATION
• Crimes that are part of destructive relationships, most notably domestic violence,
continue as long as the relationship does, or some outside agent intervenes to stop
the violence.
Reducing repeat victimisation, therefore, consists of dealing with the context that
supports the crime, either from the locality or from the weakness of the target of crime,
often referred to as ‘target hardening’ (Farrington, 1992). This may be something as
simple as making sure buildings are securely locked, students do not leave their laptops
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
by open windows or more complex such as introducing careful stock control. Where
the reasons for the repeat victimisation are within the people who suffer, helping them
to understand what they can do to reduce the risk is crucial.
RESTORATIVE JUSTICE
One approach to helping victims is known restorative justice (Braithwaite, 2002) because
it emphasises the repairing of the harm that has been caused by the crime. It focuses
on the fact that crime involves a relationship between the offender and the victim,
even if this is an implicit relationship as in a theft when the victim is absent. It is this
that needs to be dealt with, whether it is burglary or rape. Bringing the offender and
victim together can also have beneficial effects on the criminals.
Many pre-industrial societies had processes that can now be recognised as restorative
justice. In these societies, the offender and victim are brought together to resolve their
differences and for the offender to make amends. The victim is given a significant role
in the judicial process and their suffering is at centre stage. This differs considerably
from most advanced legal systems in which the state punishes the criminal and the
victim is just one more witness to provide evidence for the state.
At the heart of restorative justice processes is the offender acknowledging his
wrongdoing and apologising for it. Studies have shown that even this humane
acceptance that they should not have done what they did is very beneficial to the
victim. It helps them to accept the validity of their own suffering and to understand
more fully the reasons why the crime occurred, which also helps them come to terms
with it.
Victims offer the following reasons for requesting a restorative justice procedure:
• To find out from the offenders why they committed the crime in that way with
that particular victim.
• To make the offender understand and accept the effect the crime has had on the
victim and to accept and apologise for that effect.
• To have the opportunity to forgive the offender, and thus, bring the experience
to a resolution.
The actual process can be set up many different ways, such as small group meetings,
more formal conferences and mediation through a third party. They can include
support from legal or psychotherapeutic professionals or even ex-offenders. In general,
they follow the process of:
• Meeting: Bringing together all those on whom the crime has impacted to talk about
the crime and its consequences.
CONCERNING VICTIMS 237
• Recompense: Explore how the offender will help to repair the harm they have
caused. This will include clear indications of remorse, apology and acceptance of
the impact of the offence.
• Reintegration: Set in motion, the restoration of victims and offenders to be full,
positive members of society.
• Inclusion: Ensure that all those for whom the crime has particular relevance
participate in the actions agreed.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Many experts point out that restorative justice is not a soft option for criminals.
Many of them refuse to accept responsibility for their crimes, drawing on the
justifications discussed in Chapter 3. They also do not want to face their victims or
set in motion anything that would redress the damage they have done. There are many
examples where offenders have preferred to go to prison, rather than participate
in restorative justice. This is the central weakness of what is otherwise a good idea.
It requires the offender’s full and open participation.
CONCLUSIONS
The experience of being a victim is too often a limited aspect of the study of crime.
Yet, as has been explored in this chapter, the fear of becoming a victim is an important
influence on human activity. It does not have a strong relationship to actual experience of
crime, but to the general feeling of vulnerability. When people do experience crime, even
when the target of the crime is property, they often have strongly emotional responses.
This trauma can be even more severe if violent crime is experienced, notably rape.
Helping victims is, therefore, an important area for psychological intervention. Judicial
processes that take note of the victim’s experiences, known as ‘restorative justice’, is
therefore an area that will benefit considerably from future psychological research.
• Explore which recent laws deal directly with the psychological experience of being a
victim.
• Give an outline of restorative justice.
• What are the psychological consequences of becoming a victim of crime?
• Describe the ways in which a forensic psychologist can assess victims.
238 AREAS OF APPLICATION
Books
Karmen, A. (2001). Crime victims: an introduction to victimology, 4th ed. London: Wadsworth.
Kirchengast, T. (2016). Victims and the criminal trial (Palgrave studies in victims and victimology).
Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Shapland, J., Robinson, G., & Sorsby, A. (2011). Restorative justice in practice: evaluating what works
for victims and offenders. London: Routledge.
Vanfraechem, I., Fernández, D. B., & Aertsen, I. (Eds.). (2015). Victims and restorative justice.
London: Routledge.
Walklate, S. (2001). Gender, crime and criminal justice. Cullompton: Willan Publishing.
Walklate, S. L. (2014). Victimology (Routledge revivals): the victim and the criminal justice process.
London: Routledge.
Journal articles
Averdijk, M. (2011). Reciprocal effects of victimization and routine activities. Journal of
Quantitative Criminology, 27(2), 125–49.
Beaton, A., Cook, M., Kavanagh, M., & Herrington, C. (2000). The psychological impact
of burglary. Psychology, Crime & Law, 6(1), 33–43.
Davis, R. C., Taylor, B., & Lurigio, A. J. (1996). Adjusting to criminal victimization: the
correlates of postcrime distress. Violence and Victims, 11(1), 21–38.
Fohring, S. (2015). An integrated model of victimization as an explanation of non-involvement
with the criminal justice system. International Review of Victimology, 21(1), 45–70.
Kury, H., & Ferdinand, T. (1998). The victim’s experience and fear of crime. International Review
of Victimology, 5(2), 93–140.
Veneziano, C., Veneziano, L., & LeGrand, S. (2000). The relationship between adolescent sex
offender behaviors and victim characteristics with prior victimization. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 15(4), 363–74.
Websites
This website is a good source of victim and recorded crime statistics in the UK, along with
details of governmental initiatives:
www.homeoffice.gov.uk
The following website is a useful international website that holds a number of news items on
the developments of victimology, with links to other relevant sources of information:
www.victimology.nl/
This is an American website of the National Criminal Victim Center, useful for crime and victim
statistics:
www.ncvc.org
CONCERNING VICTIMS 239
This is a website of the Rape Abuse and Incest National Network (RAINN) that provides the
reader with information on serious sexual crimes in America:
www.rainn.org/
The following is a link to the FBI’s website – a useful resource for their national crime figures
and research findings on a variety of topics:
www.fbi.orgs
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
17 The future of
psychology
and crime
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:
1 Discuss the changes in the world that are relevant for considering the
developing nature of crime and criminality.
2 Appreciate how offenders and their actions evolve in relation to their social and
personal context.
3 Consider the possible significance of psychology for helping to prevent crime.
4 Appreciate the challenges to psychological research that are inherent in the
evolving nature of crime and criminality.
5 Understand that there are many questions about the psychology of crime that
still remain to be answered.
SUMMARY
The many processes that give rise to changes in the nature of criminality of
psychological relevance are discussed. They include the many aspects of the Internet
and related technologies, as well as the associated globalisation, including easier travel
between countries. Related changes in society also need to be considered. These
include, in many countries, a more professional and highly trained police force, which
is consequently more able to utilise psychological input. The changing family structure
and role of women, as well as the emerging ideological battles between different
religious and ethical frameworks, all create a fertile ground in which new forms of
criminality will grow. Psychologists seem destined to chase these trends, rather than
be ahead of them, but a revised evolutionary perspective that recognises that crimes
will emerge to fill the environmental niches that are available could change this. Such
a perspective will require closer integration between psychological approaches to crime,
sociological, political and technological ones.
THE FUTURE OF PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 241
Internet-related changes
Criminal actions operate on the opportunities available, whether this is other people
or property that those people own, and the resources that facilitate crime. Therefore,
as the opportunities change, so do the crimes. If psychologists are to play any role in
understanding, predicting or managing crimes, then the unfolding opportunities and
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
their implications for human behaviour need to be understood. The variety and range
of emerging opportunities for crime are mindboggling. They include those listed in
Table 17.1.
It is not surprising therefore that the majority of property crimes are now committed
over the Internet. The annual Crime Survey of England and Wales between April 2015
and March 2016 revealed that one in ten adults were victims of online fraud during
that period. This makes it much more likely that people will lose money online than
having their house burgled, being robbed or mugged on the street. As indicated in
Table 17.1, these crimes are often committed by criminals who are living overseas out
of reach of the country’s law enforcement agencies. Some fraudulent activity online,
such as advance fee fraud, discussed in Chapter 11, may even be difficult to prosecute
because the victim gives up their money freely in the belief that they will get a much
larger sum in return.
The Internet can also operate as a breeding ground for people who in the past might
not have drifted into offending. Youngsters may start off ‘stealing’ objects in online
games, but that opens them up to being influenced by criminals to steal actual property
online (National Crime Agency, 2016. www.nationalcrimeagency.gov.uk). Because
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
they are not in direct contact with victims, this reduces their awareness of the damage
they are causing. This is especially the case if they can steal from and defraud large
anonymous institutions.
Furthermore, the opportunities for more, and more secret, contact between
criminals has been greatly increased by the Internet and mobile phones. People do not
even need to send messages overtly to each other. A seemingly innocent message can
be put on a Facebook page or a title to an Instagram picture that hundreds or thousands
of people can read without law enforcement knowing who the intended recipient is.
SOCIAL CHANGES
These economic and technological changes are also being joined by big social changes,
which drive and are driven by some processes that draw on the Internet, but also have
their own momentum. As indicated in Table 17.2, they carry implications for the
nature of criminals and their criminality as much as for the sorts of offences they
commit. Many of these changes have direct implications for how individuals see the
world and their fundamental ethical stands.
There are four important points about the nature of criminals and criminality, which
derive from these technological and social changes.
• Those who may have become criminals in the past may now and in the future
turn to different sorts of crime. Bank robbers turn to penetration of Internet
accounts, rather than wearing mask and wielding guns in a bank. Stalkers will harass
their victims over e-mail, through Facebook, YouTube and other social media.
abuse are also an example of criminal activity that can be stimulated by the
opportunities the World Wide Web offers.
• The possibility of contact with likeminded people across the world gives
psychological support to people who may have been reluctant to recognise their
own predilections. The most obvious example of this is the explosion of child
pornography over the Internet. Paedophiles view their actions as more acceptable
when they discover how many other people share them. The emergence of lone
terrorists who draw on information from the Web, which encourages their
actions, is also a product of the open availability of propaganda on the Web.
But, it is an open question as to whether the terrorists and hackers of today are
drawn from the same psychological mix as their analogous counterparts from 50 or a
100 years ago.
One way of conceptualising the future of crimes is to recognise that crime has always
emerged in relation to the context and opportunities for breaking the norms of society.
This is a much more solidly evolutionary approach to crime than the rather bizarre
search for atavistic origins of offending within humanity’s distant animal heritage. It
sees behaviour as emerging in response to the niches that are made available by the
way people relate to each other. Such behaviour may well be passed down from one
generation to the next in many different ways, but it will continually evolve in relation
to the changing circumstances.
This evolution is partly a product of changes within the direct experiences of
individuals, either within the family or in the culture and ideology to which that person
is exposed. It is also partly a product of changes in the opportunities for crime and the
way those opportunities are accessed by those who, for whatever reason, have the
potential for criminality. Thus, when ships had no ready contact with the shore and
spent many months at sea with valuable cargo, piracy was a constant threat. It still is
a problem in those parts of the world where a rapid response from law enforcement
is unlikely. Highwaymen could similarly rob stagecoaches because of their isolation
on some journeys. Today, there are some forms of crime that only occur on very long
train journeys, such as thieves in India, who politely offer fellow travellers biscuits that
are laced with sedatives, so that they can steal from them when they are knocked out
by the drug. Or the ‘door-pushers’ that Canter (2005) describes in Las Vegas who take
advantage of the vast number of hotel doors in that city to look for the very small
number that are not locked properly by visitors, so that they can steal from what the
hotel guest has left in the room.
244 AREAS OF APPLICATION
The many forms of opportunity for crime that the Internet provides have spawned
a new generation of criminals for whom this is their hunting ground. But, the increase
in security also requires those who would steal or rob to find new ways of getting
away with their crimes. They may decide to be more violent, attacking people to steal
their cars, when in the past, they would just have stolen the car and jump started it.
It is noticeable how many fewer aeroplane hijackings there have been since airport
security has been strengthened, but the number of kidnappings has increased.
The question for psychologists in all this is whether these changes are bringing new
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
sorts of people into criminality or whether criminals have always been the same, but
find their outlets in different ways. The answer is probably a bit of both. It is certainly
the case that the sort of people who would have sold snake oil as a panacea in Victorian
times will now have websites selling equally useless products. It is even the case that
people who stalked an estranged loved one in the past by sitting outside her house or
sending her endless letters will these days fill her answer-phone with messages and
bombard her with e-mails, and leave messages on her Facebook page or Snapchat
address. Therefore, many of the psychological processes discussed in this volume will
still be relevant for many of the crimes of the future. It may even be suggested that
the young dropout that Charles Dickens portrays in Oliver Twist as stealing from
gentlemen on the street, under the management of Fagin, a criminal mastermind, may
now be set to work at computer terminals by a latter-day Fagin to send fraudulent
offers of business contracts by e-mail.
The changing culture and opportunities are probably also drawing some people
into criminality, who in earlier generations would not have been the sorts of people
who would have committed crimes. The fundamentalist young men and women who
become suicide bombers may have some counterparts in earlier generations, but as
Canter (2006) suggests, the psychological processes that are now at play may have some
rather different emphases. Others may regard the possibility of defrauding large
• Digital piracy
• Identity theft
• Cyber bullying, harassment, stalking
• Sexual deviance
• Child pornography, predation
• Cyber terrorism, information warfare
Internet-aided outrage
On the night of Friday, 22 July 2016, a teenager shot and killed nine people
and wounded fifteen at a shopping centre in Munich. As mentioned in
Chapter 8, this type of spree killing often ends in the death of the killer, as it
is a form of suicide. News reports indicated that 18-year-old Ali Sonboly had
searched the Web for information about previous spree attacks of this kind.
He also hacked a young woman’s Facebook account to issue an invitation to
a local McDonald’s, claiming he would buy them a meal, so that he could
attack those who took up the offer.
Like so many spree killers before him, he had been depressed enough to
seek treatment and was generally regarded as a lonely individual who did not
have much contact with others. He is reported to have bought the firearm
FOCUS 17.2
with which he killed and injured so many people over a secret area of the
Internet, known as the Dark Web.
Videos of his killing spree were instantly available around the world as well
as details of the police process that led to his identification. A number of
other violent attacks against strangers in Germany happened in the following
days. Were these encouraged by the huge media coverage?
246 AREAS OF APPLICATION
anonymous organisations, that they have never had actual contact with in person, such
as insurance companies, as not really a crime at all. In past times, those same criminals
would have had such direct contact with people in the same companies that they would
never have thought of cheating them.
It is, therefore, an open question as to the extent that there will emerge new forms
of crime or new forms of criminal.
One further development over recent years, which many will find a positive aspect of
developments in computing, is the way it can enhance psychological contributions: the
Dragnet software described in Chapter 12, for instance, that helps to locate where an
offender may be based. Other software is also emerging that deals more directly with
the type of clinical assessments discussed in Chapter 14, notably the prediction of
whether a person is likely to be more violent in the future. For example, Cunningham,
Sorensen and Reidy (2005) used aspects of a prisoner’s behavioural history to predict
the risk of a prisoner becoming violent. Berk et al. (2009) report on the effectiveness
of using a computer program based on statistics about previous incidents to forecast who
is likely to murder within a population of probationers and parolees.
The development of computer-generated support for policing has also found its way
into general approaches to crime prevention under the heading of ‘Predictive Policing’
THE FUTURE OF PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 247
(Perry et al., 2013). This approach tends to be based upon examinations of where and
when crimes have occurred. These statistics are then turned into guidelines for mobil-
isation of police resources. However, an understanding of what is generating the
statistics, in other words, the criminal psychology processes, is often lacking.
Furthermore, unthinking computer algorithms can have unwanted effects. As
Amore and De Goede (2005) point out, a self-fulfilling cycle can be set in motion
that brings more attention on alienated, marginal groups. This, in turn, can make some
members of those groups feel victimised, encouraging them to criminal activity. The
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
US practice of what is known as ‘racial profiling’, where it is assumed that the statistical
evidence that points to some subgroups in society being more prone to crime, and
therefore, more appropriate for police to stop and search, is criticised in detail by Banks
(2003). These critiques do demonstrate that statistical algorithms, which are not
founded in psychological understanding, may potentially do more harm than good.
CRIME PREVENTION
One issue that has not really been touched on directly in the present book is the
contribution that psychology can make to the reduction and prevention of crime. Of
course, one of the most powerful ways in which psychologists help is by reducing the
number of people who become criminal. Of those that do, psychological interventions
can reduce the number who return to crime after being caught. There is still an open
question about how much psychologists really can offer to either of these. Yet, as dis-
cussed in earlier chapters, some progress is being made in the managing of offenders
in prisons and mental hospitals. Perhaps, even more important is the contribution that
educational and child psychologists can make to children who have difficulties in their
early years.
Many offenders try to avoid capture and do find the risk of apprehension a serious
constraint on their criminality. So, the contribution that investigative psychology is
making to the development of decision-support systems for the police, such as geo-
graphical profiling (Canter, 2005) and help to crime analysts, as discussed by Clarke
and Eck (2003), are important ways in which crime reduction is being assisted by
behavioural science. There are also a growing number of studies of what actually works
when policing processes attempt to reduce crime (Sherman, Farringdon, Welsh &
Mackenzie 2002).
The understanding of how criminals make decisions and the situations they choose
to commit crimes is also elucidated by the sorts of considerations in earlier chapters.
These perspectives are finding their way into more general criminological considera-
tions about crime prevention. Indeed, this is one area in which the borders between
criminal psychology and traditional criminology are very vague. The cognitive pro-
cesses of offenders may lead to a distortion in their understanding of which targets are
safe to attack or worthwhile attacking. This may be reflected in criminological
processes such as ‘repeat victimisation’ as discussed in the previous chapter by Farrell
(2005), for example. Yet, there have been no detailed attempts to link these two
perspectives together.
In general, psychological and social psychological perspectives on offenders have been
remarkably absent from the consideration of crime prevention. This is in part due to
248 AREAS OF APPLICATION
the difference, noted in the opening chapter, between the focus of criminologists and
of psychologists. Those who see themselves as studying crime tend to deal with volume
crime and often characterise their work as being as much about delinquency and
deviance as about criminality. They, therefore, tend to be considering young offenders
involved in relatively minor crimes. Crime prevention, thus, focuses on deterrence and
target hardening. But, as we have seen in the chapters on rape and homicide, these more
serious crimes tend to be perpetrated by older offenders, and there is more of a possibility
of their having some obvious mental disorder (although as discussed in Chapter 3, that
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
relationship is not as clear cut as might be expected). It is these older, more serious
offenders with whom criminal psychologists often work. Bridging these two realms is
an important future development for a more thorough approach to crime prevention.
CHANGING ROLES
Throughout this book, a great variety of roles for crime psychologists has been
described as well as a range of contexts in which they operate. These exist in many
different sorts of institutions, working with many different agencies. There seems to
be no reduction in the contributions of psychology to the understanding and manage-
ment of crime. It, therefore, seems very likely that the contributions of psychologists
will continue to expand, relating to new areas of crime control and new ways of dealing
with offenders, often in unpredictable ways.
Some of these developments will doubtlessly relate to the new forms of crime that
are emerging as well as the recognition of emerging forms of criminality. One signi-
ficant example, explored in Chapter 6, is the identification of harassment, or in popular
parlance ‘stalking’ as a crime. By creating laws that deal with this behaviour specifically,
the way is open for the search for treatment programmes or other ways of managing
this behaviour, which in the past would just have been regarded as a nuisance.
The realms of psychological expertise is also broadening, and whenever the expertise
becomes established enough for wider recognition, there are pressures to draw it into
court cases. One unusual example described by Canter (2008) is the use of environ-
mental psychology principles to support the case for Human Rights Abuse in prison
solitary confinement. Other examples of new, emerging expert evidence are reviewed
by Kennedy and Sakis (2008) dealing, for example, with the liability that organisations
may have for creating environments that facilitate criminality. The specification of new
syndromes, such as ‘battered wife syndrome’, also creates opportunities for psychological
evidence in court, although not without its difficulties as Kennedy (2008) makes clear.
In all of this, the professional psychologist offers an ethical framework and opinion
rooted wherever possible in empirical research. As in so many other areas of human
progress, the scientific tradition eventually survives over other more subjective, biased
approaches.
DEVELOPMENTS IN POLICING
of the value of sound empirical research as a basis for the management of crime. This
has been called evidence-based policing (Sherman, 2013). In general, the approach
emphasises randomised controlled trials of specific aspects of police work, often in
relation to crime reduction, for example, exploring the effects of certain sorts of police
patrols.
This sort of applied research is also increasingly possible as police forces around the
world utilise information technology to collect larger amounts of progressively more
accurate data about crimes and criminals. Although, as Sherman (2003) pointed out,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
such processes require educated users within the police, if they are not to be misled
by weak research. Thus, as is always the case, an understanding of what is behind the
results and the strengths and limitations of the methodologies used is essential.
It is research and a scientific basis that distinguishes psychology from personal opinion
and ideology. However, as has emerged many times throughout this volume, there
are many challenges to carrying out crime psychology research. Modern psychology
grew out of laboratory and clinical studies, mainly with institutional subject groups.
But, crime has to be studied on the streets. Examining how people may cheat in
laboratory experiments, or if they will generate aggressive behaviour having watched
a film of violence, are so far removed from the rough and tumble of the day to day
world outside of the controlled circumstances of a laboratory that such pure studies
can only hint at genuine processes. People in institutions are also subject to so many
significant pressures that their reactions have to be carefully evaluated before any
generalisations can be made.
Once psychologists get out of their institutions and laboratories, they discover
that they do not have all the answers to all the world’s problems. Crime, like many
other areas of human activity, requires a fundamentally multi-disciplinary approach.
A burglary or violent assault in a public house, a fraud carried out over the Internet
or a terrorist attack can never be fully understood only from the examination of the
individual involved. Their social, economic, political and cultural context all play a
part, and thus, need to be integrated into our understanding. The changing nature
of crime and criminality, as well as the broadening roles of psychologists, there-
fore, provides very severe challenges to how psychological studies of criminals are
carried out. However, the improving education of police officers in many countries,
and the developments in the information they collect, is increasing the possibilities for
collaboration across a range of criminal psychology topics.
CONCLUSIONS
The consideration of a wide range of human activity in the present volume has stretched
the boundaries of what is often regarded as ‘psychological’. The methodologies for the
studies described are also often a long way from the ethereal purity to which many
academic psychologists aspire. In the present volume, for example, there has been
virtually no mention of ‘control groups’, or ‘dependent’ variables; hardly a single analysis
250 AREAS OF APPLICATION
of variance has been mentioned. Some may see this as indicating the weakness of the
field because it lacks what they regard as scientific rigour. But, the rapid expansion of
criminal psychology indicates otherwise. It suggests that by harnessing and developing
research tools and approaches that are relevant to the questions to be answered, rather
than shaping the questions to fit the tools available, that a fruitful discipline is emerging.
A discipline that very many people find to be extremely valuable. It is still too early to
determine how this discipline will unfold, but the considerable interest in it from the
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
current generation of students does show that it certainly has a vibrant future.
Crime psychology is still very much part of the broader science and profession of
psychology. It is very likely to continue to influence its parent discipline. It can be seen to
be leading to a rather different sort of psychology from what is often considered
mainstream. The study of crime requires psychologists to consider very carefully not only
the more obvious ethical and professional implications of what they do, but also the
consequences of their growing involvement with the many and various agencies that deal
with crime and criminals. There may be a general message here for the way in which the
practice of psychology changes when it engages with real-world issues.
Psychologists are also faced with the question of whether those who offend today
are likely to be the same sorts of people as they would have been in the past, but are
now acting in different ways, or whether new sorts of people are becoming criminal.
This raises the fundamental question of whether the types of explanations and
categorisations of crime and criminals, which have been such a dominant part of the
present volume, will still be relevant in the future. Will some rather different approaches
to the psychology of crime be necessary in coming years?
You will certainly need to consider this book as a whole in answering these
questions, not just the details in the present chapter.
• Which do you think is most likely to change in the future, the types of crimes that are
committed or the sorts of people who commit crimes?
• How might psychologists contribute to crime prevention?
• What roles do you see for crime psychologists?
• Is there any difference between criminal psychology and criminology?
• Can crime be studied in a laboratory?
THE FUTURE OF PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 251
• Drones
• Apps that use a person’s location
• Live streaming
• Ready access to virtual reality
• Street views of places
• Augmented reality gaming
• Bio-engineered body parts
• Rapid DNA identification
• Genetic markers of physique or character
Books
Canter, D. (1994). Criminal shadows. London: HarperCollins.
Canter, D., & Zukauskiene, R. (Eds.). (2008). Psychology and law: bridging the gap. Farnham:
Ashgate.
Tilley, N. (Ed.). (2005). Handbook of crime prevention and community safety. Cullompton: Willan
Publishing.
Journal articles
Association of British Insurers. (2000). Future crime trends in the UK. London: Association of
British Insurers, http://projects.bre.co.uk/frsdiv/crimetrends/Crime_Trends_Report.pdf
Witte, A. D. (1993). Some thoughts on the future of research in crime and delinquency. Journal
of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 30, 513–24.
Websites
The following website gives an overview of the ways in which psychology can be applied to
investigating criminal behaviour:
www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/criminal.aspx
The following website gives a definition and outline of investigative psychology as well as
outlining the practical applications:
www.ia-ip.org/index.php?page=origins
References
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Aamodt, M. G. (2013). Serial killer statistics. Retrieved 15 August 2013 from http://maamodt.
asp.radford.edu/Serial%20Killer%20Information%20Center/Project%20Description.htm
Achieving best evidence in criminal proceedings: guidance for vulnerable or intimidated witnesses, including
children. (2006). Home Office.
Agnew, R. (1984). Appearance and delinquency. Criminology, 22, 421–40.
Agnew, R. (1990). The origins of delinquent events: an examination of offender accounts.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 27, 267–94.
Agnew, R., Brezina, T., Wright, J. P., & Cullen, F. T. (2002). Strain, personality traits, and
delinquency: extending general strain theory. Criminology, 40(1), 43–72.
Ainsworth, P. B. (1998). Psychology, law and eyewitness testimony. Chichester: Wiley.
Ainsworth, P. B. (2002). Psychology and policing. Cullompton: Willan Publishing.
Ainsworth, P. B., & Pease, K. (1987). Psychology in action: police work. London: The British
Psychological Society and Methuen.
Akehurst, L., Kohnken, G., & Hofer, E. (2001). Content credibility of accounts derived from
live and video presentations. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 6, 65–83.
Akers, R. L. (1985). Deviant behavior: a social learning approach, 3rd ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Reprinted 1992. Fairfax, VA.: Techbooks.
Akers, R. L. (1999). Criminological theories. Chicago, IL: Fitzroy Dearborn.
Akers, R. L., & Sellers, C. S. (2008). Criminological theories: introduction, evaluation, and application,
5th ed. New York: Oxford University Press.
Akers, R. L., & Silverman, A. (2004). Toward a social learning model of violence and
terrorism. In H. H. Brownstein, S. L. Jackson & M. A. Zahn (Eds.), Violence: from theory
to research (pp. 19–35). Cincinnati, OH: Lexis-Nexis-Anderson.
Aldridge, M. L., & Browne, K. D. (2003). Perpetrators of spousal homicide: a review. Trauma,
Violence and Abuse, 4, 265–76.
Alison, L., Alison, E., Noone, G., Elntib, S., Waring, S., & Christiansen, P. (2014). The efficacy
of rapport-based techniques for minimizing counter-interrogation tactics among a field
sample of terrorists. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 20(4), 421–30.
Alison, L., Power, N., van den Heuvel, C., & Waring, S. (2015). A taxonomy of endogenous
and exogenous uncertainty in high-risk, high-impact contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology,
100(4), 1309–18.
Alison, L., & Rainbow, L. (Eds.) (2011). Professionalizing offender profiling: forensic and investigative
psychology in practice. London: Routledge.
American Psychiatric Association. (Eds.) (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.
REFERENCES 253
Amore, L., & De Goede, M. (2005). Governance, risk and dataveillance in the war on terror.
Crime, Law and Social Change, 43(2), 149–73.
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2010). The psychology of criminal conduct. London: Routledge.
Antonio, M. E., & Arone, N. E. (2006). Damned if they do, damned if they don’t: Jurors’
reactions to defendant testimony or silence during a capital trial. Judicature, 89, 60–7.
Arbuthnot, J., & Gordon, D. A. (1986). Behavioural and cognitive effects of a moral reasoning
development intervention for high risk behavior-disordered adolescents. Journal of Consulting
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Baruth, C. (1988). Routine mental health checkups and activities for law enforcement personnel
in dealing with hostage and terrorist incidents by psychologist trainer/consultant. In J. Reese
& J. Horn (Eds.), Police psychology: operational assistance. Washington, DC: US Department
of Justice.
Beaton, A., Cook, M., Kavanagh, M., & Herrington, C. (2000). The psychological impact of
burglary. Psychology, Crime & Law, 6(1), 33–43.
Beaver, K. M. (2009). Biosocial criminology: a primer. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Beaver, K. M., & Wright, J. P. (2007). The stability of low self-control from kindergarten
through first grade. Journal of Crime and Justice, 30, 63–86.
Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., & DeLisi, M. (2008). Delinquent peer group formation: evidence
of a gene X environment interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(3), 227–44.
Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: an economic approach. Journal of Political
Economy, 76(2), 169–217.
Beeghley, L. (2003). Homicide: a sociological explanation. Latham, MD: Rowan & Littlefield
Publishers.
Bennett, T., & Wright, R. (1984). Burglars on burglary: prevention and the offender. Aldershot: Gower.
Berk, R., Sherman, L., Barnes, G., Kurtz, E., & Ahlman, L. (2009). Forecasting murder within
a population of probationers and parolees: a high stakes application of statistical learning.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society), 172(1), 191–211.
Bevis, C., & Nutter, J. B. (1977). Changing street layouts to reduce residential burglary.
In G. Rengert & J. Wasilchick. (1985). Suburban burglary: a time and a place for everything
(p. 85). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Bijleveld, C., & Smit, P. (2006). Homicide in the Netherlands: on the structuring of homicide
typologies. Homicide Studies, 10(3), 195–219.
Bishop, D. V., Jacobs, P. A., Lachlan, K., Wellesley, D., Barnicoat, A., Boyd, P. A., Fryer, A.,
Middlemiss, P., Smithson, S., Metcalfe, K., Shears, D., Leggett, V., Nation, K., & Scerif,
G. (2011). Autism, language and communication in children with sex chromosome
trisomies. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 96(10), 954–9.
Blackburn, R. (1993). (Reprinted 1994). The psychology of criminal conduct: theory, research and
practice. Chichester: Wiley.
Blau, T. H. (1994). Psychological services for law enforcement. New York: Wiley.
Block, R., & Bernasco, W. (2009). Finding a serial burglar’s home using distance decay and
origin destination patterns: a test of empirical bayes journey-to-crime estimation in the
Hague. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 6(3), 187–211.
Blumstein, A., Cohen, J., & Farrington, D. (1988). Criminal career research: its value for
criminology. Criminology, 26, 1–36.
Bodansky, Y. (2001). Bin Laden: the man who declared war on America. New York: Random House.
Bogaard, G., Meijer, E. H., Vrij, A., Broers, N. J., & Merckelbach, H. (2014). Contextual bias
in verbal credibility assessment: criteria-based content analysis, reality monitoring and
scientific content analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(1), 79–90.
Bohl, N. (2013). Professionally administered critical incident debriefing for police officers.
In M. Kurke & E. Scrivner (Eds.), Police psychology into the 21st century (p. 169). New York:
Psychology Press.
REFERENCES 255
Bonta, J., & Gendreau, P. (1990). Reexamining the cruel and unusual punishment of prison
life. Law and Human Behavior, 14(4), 347–72.
Borgatti, S. P., Everett, M. G., & Freeman, L. C. (2002). Ucinet for windows: software for social
network analysis. Harvard, MA: Analytic Technologies.
Borgeson, K., & Valeri, R. (2009). Terrorism in America. Jones & Bartlett Learning.
Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty four juvenile thieves. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 1–57.
Bowlby, J. (1952). Maternal care and mental health. Geneva: World Health Organisation.
Bowman, P. C., & Auerbach, S. M. (1982). Impulsive youthful offenders; multimodal cognitive
behavioural treatment programme. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 9, 432–54.
Box, S., Hale, C., & Andrews, G. (1988). Explaining fear of crime. British Journal of Criminology,
28, 340–56.
Braithwaite, J. (2002). Restorative justice and responsive regulation. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Brookman, F. (2005). Understanding homicide. Portland, OR: Sage.
Brown, A. L., Testa, M., & Messman-Moore, T. L. (2009). Psychological consequences of sexual
victimization resulting from force, incapacitation, or verbal coercion. Violence Against
Women, 15(8), 898–919.
Brown, J. M. (2000). Occupational culture as a factor in the stress experiences of police officers.
In F. Leishman, B. Loveday & S. Savage (Eds.), Core issues in policing, 2nd ed. Harlow:
Pearson Education.
Brown, J. M., & Campbell, E. A. (1994). Stress and policing: sources and strategies. Chichester:
Wiley.
Brown, L., & Willis, A. (1985). Authoritarianism in British police recruits: importation,
socialization or myth? Journal of Occupational Psychology, 58(2), 97–108.
Brown, S. L., & Forth, A. E. (1997). Psychopathy and sexual assault: static risk factors, emotional
precursors, and rapist subtypes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(5), 848.
Browne, K., & Herbert, M. (1997). Preventing family violence. Chichester: Wiley.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will: men, women and rape. New York: Bantam Books.
Brugman, D., & Aleva, A. E. (2004). Developmental delay or regression in moral reasoning
by juvenile delinquents? Journal of Moral Education, 33(3), 321–38.
Bryant, E. T., Scott, M. L., Tori, C. D., & Golden, C. J. (1984). Neuropsychological deficits,
learning disability, and violent behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(2),
323.
Bull, R., Bustin, B., Evans, P., & Gahagan, D. (1983). Psychology for police officers. Chichester:
Wiley.
256 REFERENCES
Bull, R., & Horncastle, P. (1986). Metropolitan police recruit training: an independent evaluation.
Police Foundation.
Bull, R., & Horncastle, P. (1989). An evaluation of human awareness training: coming to terms with
policing. London: Tavistock.
Bull, R., & Horncastle, P. (1994). Evaluation of police recruit training involving psychology.
Psychology, Crime and Law, 1(2), 143–9.
Burbeck, E., & Furnham, A. (1984). Personality and police selection: trait differences in
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
successful and non-successful applicants to the metropolitan police. Personality and Individual
Differences, 5(3), 257–63.
Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. (1974). Rape trauma syndrome. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 131(9), 981–6.
Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for rape. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 38(2), 217–30.
Campbell, J. C. (1986). Nursing assessment of risk of homicide for battered women. Advances
in Nursing Science, 8(4), 36–51.
Campbell, J. C. (1995). Prediction of homicide of and by battered women. In J. C. Campbell
(Ed.), Assessing the risk of dangerousness: potential for further violence of sexual offenders, batterers,
and child abusers (pp. 96–113). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cannon-Bowers, J., Salas, E., & Converse, S. (1993). Shared mental models in expert team
decision making. In N. Castellan (Ed.), Individual and group decision making: current issues
(pp. 221–46). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Canter, D. V. (1989). Offender profiles. The Psychologist, 2, 12–16.
Canter, D. (1994). Criminal shadows. London: HarperCollins.
Canter, D. (2000). Destructive organisational psychology. In D. Canter & L. Alison (Eds.),
The social psychology of crime: groups, teams and networks. Farnham: Ashgate.
Canter, D. (2004a). A partial order scalogram analysis of criminal network structures.
Behaviormetrika, 31(2), 131–52.
Canter, D. (2004b). Geographical profiling of criminals. Medico-Legal Journal, 72(2), 53–66.
Canter, D. (2005). Suicide or murder? Implicit narratives in the Eddie Gilfoyle case. In L. Alison
(Ed.), Forensic psychologist’s casebook: psychological profiling and criminal investigation (pp. 315–33).
Cullompton: Willan Publishing.
Canter, D. (2006). The Samson syndrome: is there a kamikaze psychology? 21st Century Society,
1(2), 107–27.
Canter, D. (2007). Mapping murder. London: Virgin Books.
Canter, D. (2008). In the kingdom of the blind. In D. Canter & R. Zukauskiene (Eds.),
Psychology and law: bridging the gap (pp. 1–22). Farnham: Ashgate.
Canter, D. (2012). Challenging neuroscience and evolutionary explanations of social and
psychological processes. Contemporary Social Science, 7(2), 95–116.
Canter, D. V., & Alison, L. J. (2000). Profiling property crimes. Farnham: Ashgate.
Canter, D., & Alison, L. J. (2003). Converting evidence into data: the use of law enforcement
archives as unobtrusive measurement. The Qualitative Report, 8(2), 151–76.
REFERENCES 257
Canter, D. V., Alison, L. J., Alison, E., & Wentink, N. (2004). The organized/disorganized
typology of serial murder: myth or model? Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 10(3),
293–320.
Canter, D., Bennell, C., Alison, L., & Reddy, S. (2003). Differentiating sex offences: a
behaviourally based thematic classification of stranger rapes. Behavioral Sciences & the Law,
21, 157–74.
Canter, D., & Chester, J. (1997). Investigation into the claim of weighted CUSUM in
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Canter, D., & Youngs, D. (2008b). Interactive offender profiling system (IOPS). In S. Chainey
& L. Thompson (Eds.), Crime mapping case studies: practice and research (pp. 153–60).
Chichester: Wiley.
Canter, D., & Youngs, D. (2009). Investigative psychology: offender profiling and the analysis of criminal
action. Chichester: Wiley.
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Krueger, R. F., & Schmutte, P.
S. (1994). Are some people crime-prone? Replications of the personality-crime relationship
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Cooley, C. H. (1902). The looking-glass self. In Human nature and the struggle for self. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Cooper, C. L., Davidson, M. J., & Robinson, P. (1982). Stress in the police service. Journal of
Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 24(1), 30–6.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R).
In J. B. Gregory, M. Gerald & S. H. Donald (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality theory
and assessment vol. 2, personality measurement and testing (pp. 179–98). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Councell, R., & Olagundoye, J. (2003). The prison population: England and Wales 2002. London:
National Statistics Cm5996. Available online from www.archive2.official-documents.co.uk
Cox, K. (1999). Psychologists as expert witnesses. In D. V. Canter & L. J. Alison (Eds.), Offender
profiling series: vol. 2, profiling in policy and practice (pp. 189–206). Farnham: Ashgate.
Craissati, J., & Hodes, P. (1992). Mentally ill sex offenders: the experience of a regional secure
unit. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 161(6), 846–9.
Crowe, R. R. (1974). An adoption study of antisocial personality. Archives of General Psychiatry,
31(6), 785–91.
Crumplin, P. (2009). Personal communication concerning MSc dissertation from the University
of Liverpool.
Cunningham, M. D., Sorensen, J. R., & Reidy, T. J. (2005). An acturarial model for assessment
of prison violence risk among maximum security inmates. Assessment, 12(1), 40–9.
Curry, G. D. (2000). Self-reported gang involvement and officially recorded delinquency.
Criminology, 38, 1253–74.
Curtis, L. A. (1975). Violence, race, and culture. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Cusson, M. (2001). In N. J. Smelser, & P.D. Baltes (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of the Social
and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 2730–5). Oxford: Elsevier Science.
Danis, M., & Stohl, M. (2009). Media framing of terrorist incidents in the United States and
United Kingdom: implications for public opinion, civil liberties, and counter terrorism
policies. In International communication association conference.
Darwin, C. (1930). The descent of man. London: Watt and Co.
Davis, M. R. (2006). Linking behaviour to characteristics: evidence-based practice and offender
profiling. Paper presented at the 8th international investigative psychology conference. London,
England.
Davis, M. R., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2008). Key considerations and problems in assessing risk
for violence. In D. V. Canter & R. Zukauskiene (Eds.), Psychology, crime and law: new
horizons – international perspectives. Farnham: Ashgate.
De Becker, G. (1997). The gift of fear. Boston, MA: Little, Brown & Co.
De Becker, G., & Associates. (2006). Domestic Violence Method (DV MOSAIC). Available
online from www.mosaicsystem.com/dv.htm
Delisi, M., & Sherer, A. M. (2006). Multiple homicide offenders: offence characteristics, social
correlates, and criminal careers. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 33(3), 367–91.
DeMatteo, D., & Anumba, N. (2009). The validity of jury decision making research.
In L. Lieberman & D. Krauss (Eds.), Psychology in the courtroom (Volume 1 pp. 1–24).
Farnham: Ashgate.
260 REFERENCES
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H.
(2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74–118.
Detrick, P., & Chibnull, J. T. (2002). Prediction of police officer performance with the inwald
personality inventory. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 17, 9–17.
DeWall, C. N., Deckman, T., Gailliot, M. T., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). Sweetened blood cools
hot tempers: physiological self-control and aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 37(1), 73–80.
Dietz, P. E. (1986). Mass, serial, and sensational homicides. Bulletin of the New York Academy of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Dutton, D. G., Saunders, K., Starzomski, A., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Intimacy, anger and
insecure attachment as precursors of abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 24, 1367–86.
Ebbinghaus, H. (1895). Memory: a contribution to experimental psychology. Mineola, NY: Dover
Publications.
Efran, M. G. (1974). The effect of physical appearance on the judgment of guilt, interpersonal
attraction, and severity of recommended punishment in a simulated jury task. Journal of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Farrington, D. P. (1992). Was the Kirkholt burglary prevention project effective? Report to the
Home Office crime prevention unit. Cambridge: Institute of Criminology, Cambridge
University.
Farrington, D. P., Snyder, H. N., & Finnegan, T. A. (1988). Specialization in juvenile court
careers. Criminology, 26(3), 461–88.
Farrington, D. P., & West, D. J. (1990). The Cambridge study in delinquent development: a
long term follow up of 411 London males. In G. Kaiser & H.J. Kerner (Eds.), Criminal:
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Geiselman, R. E., Fisher, R. P., MacKinnon, D. P., & Holland, H. L. (1985). Eyewitness
memory enhancement in the police interview: cognitive retrieval mnemonics versus
hypnosis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70(2), 401–12.
Gelles, R. J. (1994). Research and advocacy: can one wear two hats? Family Process, 33, 93–6.
Gelles, R. J. (1998). Lethality and risk assessment for family violence cases. Paper presented at
the 4th international conference on children exposed to family violence. San Diego, CA.
Gelles, R. J., & Cornell, R. (1985). Intimate violence in families. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Gerberth, V. J., & Turco, R. N. (1997). Antisocial personality disorder, sexual sadism,
malignant narcissism, and serial murder. Journal of Forensic Science, 42(1), 49–60.
Getty, E., & Elam, J. (1988). Identifying characteristics of hostage negotiators and using
personality data to develop a selection model. In J. Reese & J. Horn (Eds.), Police
psychology: operational assistance. Washington, DC: US Department of Justice.
Gibson, E. (1975). Homicide in England and Wales, 1967–1971 (No. 31). London: HM
Stationery Office.
Giguere, R., & Bumby, K. (2007). Female sex offenders. Silver Spring, MD: Center for Effective
Public Policy.
Global Deception Research Team. (2006). A world of lies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
37(1), 60–74.
Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1956). Physique and delinquency. New York: Harper & Brothers.
Goffman, E. (1961). Asylums: essays on the social situation of mental patients and other inmates. New
York: Doubleday Anchor.
Goldstein, P. J. (1985). The drugs/violence nexus: A tripartite conceptual framework. Journal
of Drug Issues, 39, 143–74.
Gondolf, E. W. (1988). Who are those guys? Toward a behavioural typology of batterers.
Violence and Victims, 3, 187–203.
Goodkind, S., Ng, I., & Sarri, R. C. (2006). The impact of sexual abuse in the lives of young
women involved or at risk of involvement with the juvenile justice system. Violence Against
Women, 12(5), 456–77.
Gordon, R. A., Benjamin B. L., Eriko, K., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Farrington,
D. P. (2004). Antisocial behavior and youth gang membership: selection and socialization.
Criminology, 42(1), 55–88.
Goring, C. (1913). The English convict. Montclair, NJ: Patterson Smith.
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Granhag, P. A. (2006). Rethinking implicit lie detection. The Journal of Credibility Assessment
and Witness Psychology, 7(3), 180–90.
Granhag, P. A., Strömwall, L. A., & Jonsson A. C. (2003). Partners in crime: how liars in
collusion betray themselves. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(4), 848–68.
Granhag, P. A., & Vrij, A. (2005). Detecting deception. In N. Brewer & K. Williams (Eds.),
Psychology & law: an empirical perspective (pp. 43–92). New York: Guilford Press.
Greenall, P. V., & West, A. G. (2007). A study of stranger rapists from the English high security
hospitals. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 13(2), 151–67.
264 REFERENCES
Grisso, T. (1993). The differences between forensic psychiatry and forensic psychology. Bulletin
of the Anerican Academy of Psychiatry & the Law, 21, 133–45.
Groscup, J., & Tallon, J. (2009). Theoretical models of jury decision making. In J. Lieberman
& D. Krauss (Eds.), Psychology in the courtroom (pp. 41–66). Farnham: Ashgate.
Groth, A. N. (1979). Men who rape: the psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum.
Grove, W. M., Eckert, E. D., Heston, L., Bouchard, T. J., Segal, N., & Lykken, D. T. (1990).
Heritability of substance abuse and antisocial behavior: a study of monozygotic twins reared
apart. Biological Psychiatry, 27(12), 1293–304.
Grubin, D., Kelly, D., & Brunsdon, C. (2001). Linking serious sexual assault through behaviour.
London: Home Office.
Gruel, L. (1992). Police officers beliefs about cues associated with deception in rape cases.
In F. Lösel., D. Bender, & T. Bliesener (Eds.), Psychology and law: international perspectives
(pp. 234–39). New York: Walter de Gruyter.
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1985). Psychological evidence in court: results from the BPS survey. Bulletin
of the British Psychological Society, 38, 327–30.
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1992). The psychology of interrogations, confessions and testimony. Chichester:
Wiley.
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1996). Forensic psychology in England: one practitioner’s experience and
viewpoint. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 1, 131–42.
Gudjonnson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations, confessions: a handbook. Chichester:
Wiley.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & Adlam, K. R. C. (1983). Personality patterns of British police officers.
Personality and Individual Differences, 4(5), 507–12.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & Haward, L. R. C. (Eds.) (1998). Forensic psychology: a guide to practice.
London: Routledge.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & Pearse, J. (2011). Suspect interviews and false confessions. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 133–37.
Gumpert, C. H., & Lindblad, F. (2000). Expert testimony on child sexual abuse: a qualitative
study of the Swedish approach to statement analysis. Expert Evidence, 7(4), 279–314.
Gumpert, C. H., Lindblad, F., & Johansson, A. (1999). Child sexual abuse: expert testimony
in Swedish district courts. Child Maltreatment, 4(4), 343–52.
Gunaratna, R. (2000). Suicide terrorism: a global threat. Janes Intelligence Review, 12(4), 52–5.
Gupta, J., Raj, A., Decker, M. R., Reed, E., & Silverman, J. G. (2009). HIV vulnerabilities
of sex-trafficked Indian women and girls. International Journal of Gynaecology and Obstetrics,
107, 30–4.
REFERENCES 265
Hamberger, L. K., Lohr, J. M., Bonge, D., & Tolin, D. F. (1996). A large sample empirical
typology of male spouse abusers and its relationship to dimensions of abuse. Violence and
Victims, 11, 277–92.
Hammond, L. (2013). Geographical profiling in a novel context: prioritising the search for New
Zealand sex offenders. Psychology, Crime and Law, 20(4), 358–71.
Hammond, L., Wagstaff, G. F., & Cole, J. (2006). Facilitating eyewitness memory in adults
and children with context reinstatement and focused meditation. Journal of Investigative
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Hirschi, T., & Stark, R. (1969). Hellfire and delinquency. Social Problems, 17(2), 202–13.
Hodelet, N. (2001). Psychosis and offending in British Columbia: characteristics of a secure
hospital population. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health: CBMH, 11(3), 163.
Hodgins, S., Mednick, S. A., Brennan, P. A., Schulsinger, F., & Engberg, M. (1996). Mental
disorder and crime: evidence from a Danish birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry,
53(6), 489–96.
Hodgskiss, B. (2003). Lessons from serial murder in South Africa. Journal of Investigative
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Hutchings, B., & Mednick, S. A. (1975). Registered criminality in the adoptive and biological
parents of registered male criminal adoptees. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the American
psychopathological association (No. 63, p. 105).
Hyatt, D., & Hargrave, G. (1988). MMPI profiles of problem police officers. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 52(4), 722–31.
Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2001). Criminal interrogation and
confessions, 4th ed. Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Inwald, R., & Gebbia, M. (1993). Confirmation of the utility of the IPI with same-sex norms. Hilson
Research.
Ireland, J. L., & Ireland, C. A. (2011). Personality structure among prisoners: how valid is the
five-factor model, and can it offer support for Eysenck’s theory of criminality? Criminal
Behaviour and Mental Health, 21(1), 35–50.
Jackson, C., & Foshee, V. A. (1998). Violence-related behaviours of adolescents: relations with
responsive and demanding parenting. Journal of Adolescent Research, 13(3), 343–59.
Jackson, H. F., Hope, S., & Glass, C. (1987). Why are arsonists not violent offenders?
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 31(2), 143–51.
Jacobs, B. A. (1999). Dealing crack: The social world of streetcorner selling. Boston, MA: Northeastern
University Press.
Jacobs, B. A. (2000). Robbing drug dealers: Violence beyond the law. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Jacques, S., & Richard, W. (2008). The relevance of peace to studies of drug market violence.
Criminology, 46(1), 221–54.
Jenkins, P. (1988). Serial murder in England 1940–1985. Journal of Criminal Justice, 16(1), 1–15.
Jensen, G. F. (1972). Parents, peers, and delinquent action: a test of the differential association
perspective. American Journal of Sociology, 78, 63–72.
Johnson, M. P. (2006). Apples and oranges in child custody disputes: intimate terrorism vs.
situational couple violence. Journal of Child Custody, 2(4), 43–52.
Johnson, S. K., Podratz, K. E., Dipboye, R. L., & Gibbons, E. (2010). Physical attractiveness
biases in ratings of employment suitability: tracking down the ‘beauty is beastly’ effect. The
Journal of Social Psychology, 150(3), 301–18.
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2009). A systematic review of the relationship between
childhood impulsiveness and later violence. Personality, Personality Disorder and Violence,
41–61.
Jones, D. W. (2008). Understanding criminal behaviour: psychosocial approaches to criminality.
Cullompton: Willan Publishing
Jones, J. W., & Terris, W. (1983). Predicting employees’ theft in home improvement centers.
Psychological Reports, 52(1), 187–201.
Jordan, J. (2004). Beyond belief? Police, rape and women’s credibility. Criminal Justice, 4(1), 29–59.
Joseph, J. (2003). The gene illusion: genetic research in psychology and psychiatry under the microscope.
Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decisions under risk.
Econometrica, 47, 313–27.
Kanin, E. J. (1994). False rape allegations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23(1), 81–92.
268 REFERENCES
Kapardis, A. (1997). Psychology and law: a critical introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Kaslow, N. J. (2014). The psychology of torture. Available online from www.apa.org/news/press/
op-eds/psychology-torture.aspx
Kayyal, M., Widen, S., & Russell, J. A. (2015). Context is more powerful than we think:
contextual cues override facial cues even for valence. Emotion, 15(3), 287.
Kazemian, L., & Farrington, D. (2005). Comparing the validity of prospective, retrospective,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
and official onset for different offending categories. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 21(2),
127–47.
Kebbell, M. R., Milne, R., & Wagstaff, G. F. (1999). The cognitive interview: a survey of its
forensic effectiveness. Psychology, Crime and Law, 5(1–2), 101–15.
Kebbell, M. R., & Wagstaff, G. F. (1999). Face value? Evaluating the accuracy of eyewitness
information. Police Research Series: Paper 102. Published by the Home Office: www.
homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/prgpdfs/fprs102.pdf.
Kelly, L., Lovett, J., & Regan, L. (2005). A gap or chasm? Attrition in reported rape cases. Home
Office Research Study, 293. London: Home Office.
Kennedy, D. B., & Sakis, J. R. (2007). From crime to tort: criminal acts, civil liability and the
behavioral science. In D. Canter & R. Zukauskiene (Eds.), Psychology and law: bridging the
gap (pp. 119–42). Farnham: Ashgate.
Kennedy, M. A. (2009). Psychological syndrome evidence: psychology in the courtroom.
In D. A. Krauss & J. D. Lieberman (Eds.), Psychological expertise in court (pp. 103–24).
Farnham: Ashgate.
Keppel, R. D., & Walter, R. (1999). Profiling killers: a revised classification model for
understanding sexual murder. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology, 43(4), 417–37.
Klockars, C. B. (1985). The idea of police (Vol. 3). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: the cognitive-developmental perspective.
In Moral development and behavior: theory, research, and social issues. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Kohnken, G. (2004). Statement validity analysis and the detection of the truth. In P. A. Granhag,
& L. A. Stromwell (Eds.), The detection of deception in forensic contexts (pp. 41–63). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Krebs, V. E. (2002). Mapping networks of terrorist cells. Connections, 24(3), 43–52.
Kropp, P. R., & Hart, S. D. (2000). The spousal assault risk assessment (SARA) guide: reliability
and validity in adult male offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 24(1), 101.
Kropp, P. R., Hart, S. D., Webster, C. D., & Eaves, D. (1995). Manual for the spousal assault
risk assessment guide. Vancouver: The British Columbia Institute Against Family Violence.
Lange, E., & Kirsch, M. (1989). An arsonist with a sexual motivation. Psychiatrie, Neurologie,
und medizinische Psychologie, 41(6), 361–6.
Lassiter, G. D., & Meissner, C. A. (Eds.) (2010). Police interrogations and false confessions: current
research, practice, and policy recommendations. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
REFERENCES 269
Le Corff, Y., & Toupin, J. (2009). Comparing persistent juvenile delinquents and normative
peers with the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6),
1105–08.
Leonard, R., & Alison, L. (1999). Critical incident stress debriefing and its effects on coping
strategies and anger in a sample of Australian police officers involved in shooting incidents.
Work & Stress, 13(2), 144–61.
Lester, D. (1995). Serial killers: the insatiable passion. Philadelphia, PA: The Charles Press.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Leventhal, G., & Krate, R. (1977). Physical attractiveness and severity of sentencing. Psychological
Reports, 40(1), 315–8.
Levine, S. Z., & Jackson, C. J. (2004). Eysenck’s theory of crime revisited: factors or primary
scales? Legal and Criminological Psychology, 9, 135–52.
Levine, T. R. (2010). A few transparent liars explaining 54 per cent accuracy in deception
detection experiments. Annals of the International Communication Association, 34(1), 41–61.
Leyton, E. (1995). Compulsive killers: the story of modern multiple murder. New York: New York
University Press.
Lieberman, J., Arndt, J., & Vess, M. (2009). Inadmissible evidence and pretrial publicity: the
effects (and ineffectiveness) of admonitions to disregard. In J. Lieberman, & D. Krauss
(Eds.), Psychology in the courtroom (pp. 67–96). Farnham: Ashgate.
Lieberman, J., & Olson, J. (2009). The psychology of jury selection. In J. Lieberman, &
D. Krauss (Eds.), Psychology in the courtroom (pp. 97–128). Farnham: Ashgate.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York:
Guilford Press.
Lipton, D. S., Pearson, F. S., Cleland, C., & Yee, D. (2002). The effectiveness of cognitive-
behavioural treatment methods on offender rscidivism. In J. McGuire (Ed.), Offender
rehabilitation and treatment: effective programmes and policies to reduce re-offending (pp. 79–112).
Chichester: Wiley.
Liska, A. E., Sanchirico, A., & Reed, M. D. (1988). Fear of crime and constrained behavior
specifying and estimating a reciprocal effects model. Social Forces, 66, 827–37.
Lobato, A. (2000). Criminal weapon use in Brazil: a psychological analysis. In D. V. Canter &
L. J. Alison (Eds.), Profiling property crimes. Offender Profiling Series Vol IV. Farnham:
Ashgate.
Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Loftus, E. F. (1991). Witness for the defense. New York: St Martin’s Press.
Loftus, E. F. (2003). Make believe memories. American Psychologist, 58(11), 867–73.
Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: an example of the
interaction between language and memory. Journal of Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 13, 585–9.
Lombroso, C. (1895). L’homme criminel: atlas. Alcan.
Luedtke, G. (1970). Crime and the physical city: neighbourhood design techniques for crime prevention.
Springfield, VA: National Technical Information Service.
Luginbuhl, J., & Middendorf, K. (1988). Death penalty beliefs and jurors’ responses to
aggravating and mitigating circumstances in capital trials. Law and Human Behavior, 12,
263–81.
270 REFERENCES
Maas, C., Herrenkohl, T. I., & Sousa, C. (2008). Review of research on child maltreatment
and violence in youth. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 9, 56–67.
McCabe, K., Hough, R., Wood, P., & Yeh, M. (2001). Childhood and adolescent onset
conduct disorder: a test of the developmental taxonomy. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 29(4), 305–16.
McCluskey, K., & Wardle, S. (2000). The social structure of robbery. In D. Canter &
L. Alison (Eds.), The social psychology of crime: groups, teams & networks (pp. 247–85). Farnham:
Ashgate.
McCord, J. (1991). Family relationships, juvenile delinquency, and adult criminality. Criminology,
29, 397–417.
MacDonald, J. M. (1963). The threat to kill. American Journal of Psychiatry, 120, 125–30.
MacDonald, J. M., & Michaud, D. L. (1992). Criminal interrogation. Denver, CO: Apache
Press.
MacDonald, J. M., & Michaud, D. L. (1995). Rape: controversial issues: criminal profiles, date rape,
false reports and false memories. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
McGuire, J. (Ed.) (2003). Offender rehabilitation and treatment: effective programmes and policies to
reduce re-offending. Chichester: Wiley.
McKenzie, C. (1995). A study of serial murder. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology, 39, 3–10.
McLean, I., & Balding, V. (2003). Some characteristics of 7289 cases of rape and sexual assault
seen at St. Mary’s sexual assault referral centre since 1986. Health Psychology Update, 12,
56–61.
Maguire, M. (1982). Burglary in a dwelling. London: Heinemann.
Maguire, M., & Corbett, C. (1987). The effects of crime and the work of victims support schemes
(pp. 224–5). Farnham: Ashgate.
Mars, G. (2000). Culture and crime. The Social Psychology of Crime: Groups, Teams and Networks,
3, 21–50.
Marshall, W. L. (1989). Intimacy, loneliness and sexual offenders. Behavioral Research in Therapy,
27, 491–503.
Massaro, T. M. (2015). Experts, psychology, credibility, and rape: the rape trauma syndrome
issue and its implications for expert psychological testimony. Psychology, Credibility, and
Rape: The Rape Trauma Syndrome Issue and Its Implications for Expert Psychological Testimony
(March 23, 2015), 69.
Masters, F., & Greaves, D. (1969). The quasimodo complex. British Journal of Plastic Surgery,
20, 204–10.
REFERENCES 271
Monahan, J. (1981). Predicting violent behavior: an assessment of clinical techniques. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage.
Moore, J. W. (1991). Going Down to the Barrio. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.
Moore, J. W., & Hagedorn, J. (2001). Female Gangs a Focus on Research. Office of Juvenile Justice
and Delinquency Preventaion, March.
Moskowitz, D. S., Sutton, R., Zuroff, D. C., & Young, S. N. (2015). Fetal exposure to
androgens, as indicated by digit ratios (2D: 4D), increases men’s agreeableness with
women. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 97–101.
Mullins, S. (2009). Terrorist networks and small group psychology. In D. Canter (Ed.), Faces
of terrorism: cross-disciplinary explorations. Chichester: Wiley.
Nahari, G., Glicksohn, J., & Nachson, I. (2010). Credibility judgments of narratives: language,
plausibility, and absorption. The American Journal of Psychology, Fall, 123(3), 319–35.
National Health Service. (2006). In-patients formally detained in hospitals under the Mental Health
Act 1983 and other legislation, England: 1994–95 to 2005–05. London: The Information
Centre: Mental Health Statistics. Available online from www.ic.nhs.uk
National Research Council. (2003). The polygraph and lie detection: committee to review the scientific
evidence on the polygraph. Washington, DC: The National Academic Press.
Neal, B. (1986). The K scale (MMPI) and job performance. Psychological Services for Law
Enforcement, 83–90.
Netter, G. (1982). Killing one another. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.
Nicholas, S., Kershaw, C., & Walker, A. (Eds.) (2008). Crime in England and Wales, 2006/07.
Home office statistical bulletin. London: Home Office.
Ogloff, J. R. P., & Polvi, N. H. (1998). Legal evidence and expert testimony. In D. Turner
& M. Uhlemann (Eds.), A legal handbook for the helping professional, 2nd ed. pp. 379–401.
Victoria: The Sedgewick Society for Consumer and Public Education.
Onyebadi, U., & Park, J. (2012). ‘I’m Sister Maria. Please help me’: A lexical study of 4-1-9
international advance fee fraud email communications. International Communication Gazette,
74(2), 181–99.
Ormerod, D. (2005). Smith and Hogan: criminal law, 11th ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Osborn, S. G., & West, D. J. (1979). Conviction records of fathers and sons compared.
The British Journal of Criminology, 19(2), 120–33.
O’Sullivan, M., & Ekman, P. (2004). The wizards of deception detection. In P. A. Granhag
& L. A. Stromwell (Eds.), The detection of deception in forensic contexts (pp. 269–81).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Outlaw, M., Ruback, B., & Britt, C. (2002). Repeat and multiple victimizations: the role of individual
and contextual factors. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.
REFERENCES 273
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: narcissism,
Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556–63.
Paunonen, S. V. (2006). You are honest, therefore I like you and find you attractive. Journal
of Research in Personality, 40(3), 237–49.
Peace, K. A., & Sinclair, S. M. (2012). Cold-blooded lie catchers? An investigation of
psychopathy, emotional processing, and deception detection. Legal and Criminological
Psychology, 17(1), 177–91.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Pearson, M., & West, P. (2003). Drifting smoke rings: social network analysis and Markov
processes in a longitudinal study of friendship groups and risk-taking. Connections, 25(2),
59–76.
Pease, K. (1998). Repeat victimisation: taking stock. Crime detection and prevention series 90. London:
Home Office.
Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count
(LIWC): A computerized text analysis program. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1981). Juror decision-making models: the generalization gap.
Psychological Bulletin, 89(2), 246–87.
Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1992). Explaining the evidence: test of the story model for
jury decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 189–206.
Perreault, S., Sauve, J., & Burns, M. (2010). Multiple victimization in Canada, 2004. Canadian
centre for justice statistics profile series. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.
Perry, P., McInnis, B., Price, C. C., Smith, S. C., & Jollywood, J. S. (2013). Predictive policing:
the role of crime forecasting in law enforcement operations. Washington, DC: Rand Corporation.
Pfeifer, J. E., & Brigham, J. C. (1993). Ethical concerns of nonclinical forensic witnesses and
consultants. Ethics and Behavior, 3, 329–43.
Phillips, E. M. (2009). The business of kidnap for ransom. In D. Canter (Ed.), The faces of
terrorism: multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 189–207). Chichester: Wiley.
Phillips, E. M. (2013). Violence in captivity indicate which kidnappers will kill? The social
organization of violence toward hostages: does violence in captivity indicate which
kidnappers will kill? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6.
Phillips, S. L., Heads, T. C., Taylor, P. J., & Hill, G. M. (1999). Sexual offending and antisocial
sexual behavior among patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60(3),
170–5.
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child (Vol. 5). Psychology Press.
Pillman, F., Rohde, A., Ullrich, S., Draba, S., Sannemuller, U., & Marneros, A. (1999).
Violence, criminal behaviour and the EEG: significance of left hemispheric local
abnormalities. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 11, 454–7.
Piquero, A. R., Brame, R., & Lynam, D. (2004). Studying criminal career length through early
adulthood among serious offenders. Crime & Delinquency, 50(3), 412–35.
Piquero, A. R., & Buka, S. L. (2002). Linking juvenile and adult patterns of criminal activity
in the providence cohort of the national collaborative perinatal project. Journal of Criminal
Justice, 30, 259–72.
Porter, L., & Alison, L. (2001). A partially ordered scale of influence in violent group
behaviour: an example from gang rape. Small Group Research, 32(4), 475–97.
274 REFERENCES
Porter, L., & Alison, L. (2006). Leadership and hierarchies in criminal groups: scaling degrees
of leader behaviour in group robbery. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 11(2), 245–65.
Porter, S., Campbell, M. A., Stapleton, J., & Birt, A. R. (2002). The influence of judge, target,
and stimulus characteristics on the accuracy of detecting deceit. Canadian Journal of
Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 34(3), 172.
Porter, S., Doucette, N. L., Woodworth, M., Earle, J., & MacNeil, B. (2008). Halfe the world
knowes not how the other halfe lies: investigation of verbal and non-verbal signs of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial
behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128(3), 490.
Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1991). Firesetters admitted to a maximum security psychiatric
institution offenders and offenses. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6(4), 461–75.
Richards, P. (1999). Homicide statistics. Research paper no. 99/56. London: House of Commons
Library.
Richardson, L. (2006). What terrorists want: understanding the enemy, confronting the threat. New
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Salfati, C. G., & Canter, D. (1999). Differentiating stranger murders: profiling offender
characteristics from behavioural styles. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 17, 391–406.
Salfati, C. G., & Dupont, F. (2001). Canadian homicide: an investigation of crime scene actions.
Homicide Studies, 10(2), 118–39.
Salfati, C. G., & Harastis, E. (2001). Greek homicide: a behavioural examination of offender
crime-scene actions. Homicide Studies, 5(4), 335–62.
Sampson, R. J. (2001). Sociology of delinquency. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
encyclopaedia of the social and behavioural sciences (pp. 3380–84). Oxford: Elsevier Science.
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. (1990). Crime and deviance over the life course: the salience of
adult social bonds. American Sociological Review, 55, 609–27.
Samuels, J., Bienvenu, O. J., Cullen, B., Costa, P. T., Eaton, W. W., & Nestadt, G. (2004).
Personality dimensions and criminal arrest. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45(4), 275–80.
Sandberg, A. A., Koepf, G. F., Ishihara, T., & Hauschka, T. S. (1961). An XYY human male.
The Lancet, 278(7200), 488–9.
Santtila, P., Junkkila, J., & Sandnabba, N. K. (2005). Behavioural linking of stranger rapes. Journal
of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 2, 87–103.
Santtila, P., Laukkanen, M., Zappala, A., & Bosco, D. (2008). Distance travelled and offence
characteristics in homicide, rape, and robbery against business. Legal and Criminological
Psychology, 13, 345–56.
Sapir, A. (2005). The LSI course on scientific content analysis (SCAN). Phoenix, AZ: Laboratory
for Scientific Interrogation.
Sarangi, S., & Canter, D. (2009). The rhetorical foundation of militant jihad. In D. Canter (Ed.),
The faces of terrorism. Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 35–61). Chichester: Wiley.
Sarangi, S., & Youngs, D. (2006). Spatial patterns of Indian serial burglars with relevance to
geographical profiling. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 3, 105–15.
Sarason, I. G. (1968). Verbal learning, modeling, and juvenile delinquency. American Psychologist,
23(4), 254.
Saunders, D. G. (1992). A typology of men who batter: three types derived from cluster analysis.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62, 264–75.
Scarr, H. A. (1973). Patterns of Burglary, National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice,
Government Printing Office. Washington, DC.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: a theory of
genotype → environment effects. Child Development, 54(2), 424–35.
Schrenck-Notzing, A. (1897). Über suggestion und erinnerungsfälschung in Berchtold prozeß. Leipzig:
Johann Ambroisius Barth.
Schuler, R. S. (1980). Definition and conceptualization of stress in organizations. Organizational
Behavior and Human Performance, 25(2), 184–215.
Schuller, R. A., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2001). An introduction to psychology and law. In R. A.
Schuller & J. R. P. Ogloff (Eds.), Introduction to psychology and law: Canadian perspectives
(pp. 3–28). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Schuller, R. A., & Yarmey, M. (2001). The jury: deciding guilt or innocence. In R. A. Schuller
& J. R. P. Ogloff (Eds.), Introduction to psychology and law: Canadian perspectives (pp. 157–87).
Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
REFERENCES 277
Schwartz-Watts, D., & Morgan, D.W. (1998). Violent versus non-violent stalkers. Journal of
the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 541–5.
Scottish Executive. (2001). Homicide in Scotland. Statistical Bulletin Criminal Justice Series.
November.
Scully, D., & Marolla, J. (1985). ‘Riding the bull at gilley’s’: convicted rapists describe the
rewards of rape. Social Problems, 32(3), 251–63.
Seidman, B. T., Hudson, S. M., & Robertson, P. J. (1994). An examination of intimacy and
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Smith, A. D. (2000). Motivation and psychosis in schizophrenic men who sexually assault
women. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 11(1), 62–73.
Smith, A. D., & Taylor, P. J. (1999). Serious sex offending against women by men with schizo-
phrenia. Relationship of illness and psychotic symptoms to offending. The British Journal
of Psychiatry, 174(3), 233–7.
Smith, D. A., & Paternoster, R. (1987). The gender gap in theories of deviance: issues and
evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 24(2), 140–72.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Smith, D. A., & Paternoster, R. (1990). Formal processing and future delinquency: deviance
amplification as selection. Artifact, Law and Society Review, 24(5), 1109–31.
Smith, D. J. (2002) Crime and the life course. In M. Maguire, R. Morgan & R. Reiner (Eds.),
The Oxford handbook of criminology, 3rd ed (pp. 702–55). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Smith, D. J. (2006). Social inclusion and early desistance from crime. University of Edinburgh, Centre
for Law and Society.
Smith, H. J., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Group membership and everyday social comparison
experiences. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(3), 297–308.
Soibelman, M. (2004). Palestinian suicide bombers. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender
Profiling, 1(3), 175–90.
Sommer, R. (1976). The end of imprisonment. London: Oxford University Press.
Sommers, S. R. (2006). On racial diversity and group decision making: identifying multiple
effects of racial composition on jury deliberations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
90(4), 597–612.
Sorochinski, M., & Salfati, G. C. (2010). The consistency of inconsistency in serial homicide:
patterns of behavioural change across series. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender
Profiling, 7, 109–36.
Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (1991). Stability and change in criminal behaviour up to age 30.
The British Journal of Criminology, 31(4), 327–46.
Stein, N. L., Glenn, C. G., & Freedle, R. O. (1979). New directions in discourse processing.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Steller, M., & Kohnken, G. (1989). Criteria-based content analysis. In D. C. Rankin (Ed.),
Psychological methods in criminal investigations and evidence (pp. 212–45). New York: Springer
Verlag.
Stephens, C., Long, N., & Miller, I. (1997). The impact of trauma and social support on
posttraumatic stress disorder: a study of New Zealand police officers. Journal of Criminal
Justice, 25(4), 303–14.
Stewart, C. H. (1981). A retrospective survey of alleged sexual assault cases. Police Surgeon, 28,
32–5.
Stochholm, K. A., Jensen, A. S., Jull, S., & Gravholt, C. H. (2012). Criminality in men with
Klinefelter’s syndrome and XYY syndrome: a cohort study. British Medical Journal Open:
Genetics and Genomics. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2011–000650
Stott, C., Hoggett, J., & Pearson, G. (2012). ‘Keeping the peace’: social identity, procedural
justice and the policing of football crowds. British Journal of Criminology, 52(2), 381–99.
Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: the conflict tactics (CT)
Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75–88.
REFERENCES 279
Strauss, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1990). Physical violence in American families. New Brunswick,
NJ: Transaction.
Strauss, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind closed doors: violence in the American
family. Garden City, NY: Anchor/Doubleday.
Strömwall, L., & Granhag, P. A. (2003). How to detect deception? Arresting the beliefs of police
officers, prosecutors and judges. Psychology, Crime and Law, 9(1), 19–36.
Strömwall, L., Granhag, P. A., & Hartwig, M. (2004). 10 Practitioners’ beliefs about
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
deception. In P. A. Granhag & L. Stromwell (Eds.), The detection of deception in forensic contexts
(pp. 299–326). Chichester: Wiley.
Stuesser, L. (2005). Experts on eyewitness identification: I just don’t see it. International
Commentary on Evidence, 3(1), 1–15.
Suedfeld, P. (2007). Torture, interrogation, security, and psychology: absolutistic versus
complex thinking. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 7(1), 55–63.
Sullivan, C. M. (2014). The (in)effectiveness of torture for combating insurgency. Journal of
Peace Research, 51(3), 388–40.
Sutherland, E. H. (1947). Principles of criminology, 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.
Svensson, R. (2002). Strategic offences in the criminal career context. British Journal of
Criminology, 42, 395–411.
Swindler, A. (1986). Culture in action. American Sociological Review, 51, 273–86.
Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: a theory of delinquency.
American Sociological Review, 22(6), 664–70.
Synnott, J., Dietzel, D., & Ioannou, M. (2015). A review of the polygraph: history, methodology
and current status. Crime Psychology Review, 1(1), 59–83.
Taylor, P. J. (1985). Motives for offending among violent and psychotic men. The British Journal
of Psychiatry, 147(5), 491–8.
Taylor, P. J., Garety, P., Buchanan A, et al. (1996). Delusions and violence. In J. Monahan,
H. Steadman (Eds.), Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment
(pp. 161–81). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, and coercive actions. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Exploring personality through test construction:
development of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. The SAGE Handbook
of Personality Theory and Assessment, 2, 261–92.
Terry, W. C. (1981). Police stress: the empirical evidence. Journal of Police Science and Admin-
istration, 9(1), 61–75.
Thompson, K. M. (1990). Refacing inmates: a critical appraisal of plastic surgery programs in
prison. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 17(4), 448–66.
Thornberry, T., Krohn, M. D., Lizotte, A. J., Smith, C. A., & Tobin, R. (2003). Gangs and
delinquency in developmental perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Thornhill, R., & Palmer, C. (2000). A natural history of rape: biological bases of sexual coercion.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Thornton, D., & Reid, R. L. (1982). Moral reasoning and type of criminal offence. British Journal
of Social Psychology, 21(3), 231–8.
280 REFERENCES
Tjaden, P. G., & Thoennes, N. (1997). The prevalence and consequences of intimate partner
violence: findings from the national violence against women survey. In 49th Annual Meeting
of the American Society of Criminology. San Diego, CA.
Tjaden, P. G., & Thoennes, N. (1998). Stalking in America: findings from the national violence against
women survey. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.
Toch, H. (2002). Stress in policing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Tonkin, M., Woodhams, J., Bond, J. W. & Loe, T. (2010). A theoretical and practical test of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
geographical profiling with serial vehicle theft in a U.K. context. Behavioral Sciences and the
Law, 28, 442–60.
Turco, R. N. (1990). Psychological profiling. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology, 34, 147–54.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1985). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice.
In Environmental impact assessment, technology assessment, and risk analysis (pp. 107–29). Berlin
Heidelberg: Springer.
Undeutsch, U. (1989). The development of statement reality analysis. In Credibility assessment
(pp. 101–19). Netherlands: Springer.
Vanderveen, G. (2006). Interpreting fear, crime, risk, and unsafety. The Hague, The Netherlands:
Boom Juridische Uitgevers.
Vernham, Z., & Vrij, A. (2015). A review of the collective interviewing approach to detecting
deception in pairs. Crime Psychology Review, 1(1), 43–58.
Virkkunen, M. (1986). Reactive hypoglycemic tendency among habitually violent offenders.
Nutrition Review, 44(Suppl), 94–103.
Vishnevetsky, M. (2009). Case study – youth gangs and terrorism in Chechnya: recruitment,
activities and networks. In D. Canter (Ed.), The faces of terrorism: multidisciplinary perspectives
(pp. 151–68). Chichester: Wiley.
Vrij, A. (2008a). Detecting lies and deceit: pitfalls and opportunities, 2nd ed. Chichester: Wiley.
Vrij, A. (2008b). Nonverbal dominance versus verbal accuracy in lie detection: a plea to change
police practice. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 35(10), 1323–36.
Vrij, A., Edward, K., & Bull, R. (2001). People’s insight into their own behaviour and speech
content while lying. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 373–89.
Vrij, A., & Graham, S. (1997). Individual differences between liars and the ability to detect
lies. Expert Evidence, 5(4), 144–8.
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Mann, S. (2010). Good liars. The Journal of Psychiatry & Law, 28,
77–98.
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2011). Outsmarting the liars: toward a cognitive
lie detection approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(1), 28–32.
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Porter, S. (2010). Pitfalls and opportunities in nonverbal and verbal
lie detection. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11(3), 89–121.
Vrij, A., Kneller, W., & Mann, S. (2000). The effect of informing liars about criteria based content
analysis on their ability to deceive CBCA raters. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 5, 57–70.
Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2001). Telling and detecting lies in a high-stake situation: the case of a
convicted murderer. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 187–203.
REFERENCES 281
Vrij, A., & Semin, G. R. (1996). Lie experts’ beliefs about nonverbal indicators of deception.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20(1), 65–80.
Walker, I., & Smith, H. J. (Eds.) (2002). Relative deprivation: specification, development, and inte-
gration. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Walsh, D. (1980). Break-ins; burglary from private houses. London: Constable.
Walters, G. D. (1995). The psychological inventory of criminal thinking styles Part I: reliability
and preliminary validity. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 22(3), 307–25.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Warr, M. (1987). Fear of victimization and sensitivity to risk. Journal of Quantitative Criminology,
3, 29–46.
Warr, M. (1996). Organisation and instigation in delinquent groups. Criminology, 34(1), 11–37.
Warren, J., Hazelwood, R., & Dietz, P. (1996). The sexually sadistic serial killer. Journal of
Forensic Sciences, 41, 970–4.
Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., & Sechrest, L. (1966). (New edition 1999.)
Unobtrusive measures: non-reactive research in the social sciences. Chicago, IL: Rand
McNally.
Webster, C. D., Douglas, K. S., Eaves, D., & Hart, S. D. (1997). HCR-20: assessing risk for
violence (Version 2). Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada, Simon Fraser University. Mental
Health, Law, and Policy Institute.
Wekstein, L. (1979). Handbook of suicideology. New York: Bruner/Mazel.
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
277–95.
Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Tulero, S. M., & Brinacombe, C. A. E.
(1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: recommendations for line-ups and
photospreads. Law and Human Behaviour, 22, 603–47.
West, D. J. (1982). Delinquency: its roots, careers and prospects. London: Heinemann.
Williams, K. R., & Houghton, A. B. (2004). Assessing the risk of domestic violence reoffending:
a validation study. Law and Human Behavior, 28(4), 437.
Witkin, H. A., Mednick, S. A., Schulsinger, F., Bakkestrom, E., Christiansen, K., Goodenough,
D., Hirschhorn, K., Lundsteen, C., Owen, D., Philip, J., & Rubin, D. (1976). Criminality
in XYY and XXY men. Science, 193, 547–55.
Wolfgang, M. E. (1958). Patterns in criminal homicide. Philadelphia, PA: University of Philadelphia
Press.
Wolfgang, M. E., & Ferracuti, F. (1967). The subculture of violence. New York: Tavistock.
Wyrick, P. A., & Howell, J. C. (2004). Strategic risk-based response to youth gangs. Juvenile
Justice, 9(1), 20–9.
Yochelson, S., & Samenow, S. E. (1976). The criminal personality, volume I: a profile for change.
Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
Yokota, K. (2013). Differentiation of hostage barricade incidents: through the application of
the acton system model. In D. Youngs (Ed.), The behavioural analysis of crime: investigative
psychology studies in honour of David Canter (pp. 67–86). Farnham: Ashgate.
Young, H. S. (1984). Practicing RET with lower class offenders. Special issue: the work of
Howard Young. British Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2(2), 33–59.
282 REFERENCES
Youngs, D. (2004). Personality correlates of offence style. Journal of Investigative Psychology and
Offender Profiling, 1, 99–119.
Youngs, D. (2008). Contemporary challenges in investigative psychology: revisiting the Canter
offender profiling equations. In D. Canter & R. Zikausien (Eds.), Psychology and law: bridging
the gap. Aldershot: Ashgate.
Youngs, D., & Canter, D. (2012). Narrative roles in criminal action: an integrative framework
for differentiating offenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 17(2), 233–49.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Youngs, D., & Canter, D. (2013). Offenders’ crime narratives as revealed by the narrative roles
questionnaire. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 57(3),
289–311.
Youngs, R. (2006). Democracy and security in the Middle East (Internal report submitted for
publication). Madrid, Fundacin para las Relaciones Internacionales y el Dialogo Exterior.
Zhou, L., Burgoon, J. K., Nunamaker, J. F., & Twitchell, D. (2004). Automating linguistics-
based cues for detecting deception in text-based asynchronous computer-mediated
communications. Group Decision and Negotiation, 13(1), 81–106.
Zimmerman, L., Compo, N. S., & Carlucci, M. E. (2013). Lie detection during high-stakes
truths and lies. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 18(2), 314–23.
Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communi-
cation of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology vol. 14
(pp. 1–57). New York: Academic Press.
Index
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
Control Question Technique (CQT) 157–8 delinquency 7, 23–4, 27, 40, 42, 43, 44, 49,
Cooper, H. 155 50; families 50–1; female offenders 56;
Corey, D. 180 gangs 52–3; parenting styles 24, 51–2; self-
Cornish, D. B. 69 identity 55
courts 4, 13–14, 15, 16, 195, 196, 203; DePaulo, B. M. 148, 149, 155
experiment evidence 198–9; expert destructive organisational psychology 125
witnesses 15, 195–6, 248; fitness to plead deterministic explanations 22–3
196–7; insanity defence 34, 36, 197–8; deterrence theory 54
psychological syndromes 198, 199, 248 deviance 53, 55–6
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 15:57 19 May 2017
CQT see Control Question Technique Dialectical Behaviour Therapy (DBT) 216
crime 1–6, 7–9, 24, 243–4, 246, 249; differential association theory 48–9
see also criminality differential reinforcement 50
crime prevention 246, 247–8 diminished responsibility 199, 202
crime psychologists 3, 14, 15, 16, 247–8, disfigurement 23–4
250 dissociative amnesia 199
crime psychology 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 14, 15–16, distortions 132, 134, 136
249, 250 Dodd, M. J. 69
criminal actions 4, 5–6, 241 Doley, R. 67
criminal activity 3–4, 5–6, 49, 54 domestic violence 74–5, 76, 77–9, 81–5, 86;
criminal behaviour 6, 15, 48, 49 psychological intervention 78, 83–5; risk
criminal development 56–8, 167–8 assessment 82–3; risk factors 81–2, 83, 86;
criminal families 24–5; see also families stalking 79–80, 81
criminality 1–6, 15–16, 240, 242–3, 249; domestic violence units (DVUs) 85
biological explanations 2, 21–5, 26–31, Doucette, N. L. 153
33; evolutionary explanations 22–3, Dragnet 71, 171, 246
243–4, 246; gangs 52–3, 56, 57, 116–17; drugs 71
gender differences 55–6, 59; mental dysphoric/borderline batterers 78–9
disorders 7, 33, 34, 35–6, 37, 45–6;
personality disorders 36, 37, 38, 39, 46, Earle, J. 153
199; psychological explanations 5, 7, Eck, J. 247
33–4, 35–9, 40–6; social explanations Ekman, P. 148, 152
48–52, 53–5, 56–9, 94 Ellis, H. H. 211
criminal learning processes 48–50 Elntib, S. 143
criminal networks 116, 118, 119, 120 emotional bias 150–1
criminal process 3, 4 emotional/psychological violence 75, 76
criminal psychology see crime psychology employee theft 68–9
criminals see crime; criminality enhanced thinking skills (ETS) 218, 219–20
criminologists 7, 247–8 evidence-based policing 249
Critical Incident Stress Debriefing (CISD) evolutionary explanations 22–3, 92–3, 243–4
191, 192 expectancy effects 133, 135, 137
crowd behaviour 191–2 experiment evidence 198–9
Crowe, R. R. 27 expert testimony 15, 195–6, 248
Cullen, F. T. 41 expert witnesses 15, 195–6, 248
cybercrime 243, 245 eyewitness identification 136–7, 145
eyewitness testimony 131, 132, 133–6, 145
DBT see Dialectical Behaviour Therapy Eysenck, H. J. 40, 54
death penalty 201, 203 false allegations 89, 91, 95
deception 2, 9–10, 16, 147–9, 151, 152–4, false confessions 142–3
155–6, 159 false memories 138
deception detectors 156–7; polygraphs 156, families 24–5, 50–51; parenting styles 24,
157–9; voice stress analysis 159 51–2, 56–7
defendants 4, 15 family-only batterers 78
INDEX 285