Morpheme
Morpheme
113
infelicitous, intangible, indirect, insane, incorrect and
inglorious. All consist of at least a prefix morpheme and a root
morpheme (some of these words have a prefix, a root and a
suffix). Many speakers have the following pronunciations of
these words:
(6)
(a)
[ɪmpʰɒsɪbɫ]
impossible
(b)
[ɪmbælənst]
imbalanced
(c)
[iɱfəlɪsɪtəs]
infelicitous
(d)
[ɪntʰændʒɪbɫ]
intangible
(e)
[ɪndɪɹεkt]
indirect
(f)
[ɪnseɪn]
insane
(g)
[ɪŋkəɹεkt]
incorrect
(h)
[ɪŋglɔ:ɹiəs]
inglorious
It is part of the native speaker’s unconscious linguistic
114
knowledge of English that these words all have the same
prefix. That prefix is one of the morphemes of English, and,
like all morphemes in the language, has a syntax (it is a prefix),
a semantics (it has a specific meaning) and a phonology. But
what is the phonological form of that morpheme? We know
from the data that the suffix may be realized as [ɪm], [ɪɱ], [ɪn]
or [ɪŋ]. It is clear, then, that the first phoneme in the prefix is /
ɪ/ and the second phoneme is a nasal, but which nasal
phoneme? We claimed above that English has three nasal
phonemes: /m/, /n/ and /ŋ/. So the phonological form of this
prefix might be [ɪm], [ɪn] or [ɪŋ]. Let us consider [ɪŋ]. We
could say that the /ŋ/ phoneme is realized as [n] before /t/, /d/
and /s/, and as [m] before [p] and [b]. This seems to make
sense: we can say that, when the prefix is added to a root, the
place of articulation of the nasal becomes identical to that of
the first consonant in the root. Thus, it is alveolar when
followed by an alveolar consonant (such as /t/, /d/ and /s/),
labio-dental when followed by a labio-dental consonant (such
as /f/), and bilabial when followed by a bilabial consonant
(such as /p/ or /b/). This is the process of assimilation we
referred to in chapter 2, in which one segment becomes similar,
in some respect, to another when the two are adjacent. Here,
the assimilation is in place of articulation. Further evidence that
nasals in English undergo place of articulation assimilation is
not hard to come by. Consider the following data, which are
representative of the speech of many speakers of English:
(7) Nasal assimilation in English
(a)
[ʌŋkʰlɪə]
unclear
(b)
115
[ʌŋgɒdli]
ungodly
(c)
[ʌɱfεə]
unfair
(d)
[ʌɱvælju:d]
unvalued
(e)
[ʌntʰɹu:]
untrue
(f)
[ʌndʌn]
undone
(g)
[ʌmbεəɹəbəɫ]
unbearable
(h)
[ʌmbaɪəst]
unbiased
While the [ɪŋ] solution is plausible, it faces a difficulty: we
might equally say that the phonological form of the morpheme
is [ɪn], or [ɪm], and that, in either case, the nasal assimilates to
a following consonant. On the evidence presented thus far,
there is no non-arbitrary way of choosing between the three
alternatives: each is as plausible as the others. The following
data, however, allow us to make a non-arbitrary choice:
(8)
(a)
[ɪæktɪv]
inactive
116
(b)
[ɪnɐpɹətɪv]
inoperative
(c)
[ɪnεfəbɫ]
ineffable
(d)
[ɪnədvaɪzəbɫ]
inadvisable
(e)
[ɪnɔ:dɪbɫ]
inaudible
(f)
[ɪneɪliənəbɫ]
inalienable
In each case, there is no consonant at the beginning of the
root to which the nasal could assimilate: each root begins with
a vowel. From the fact that vowel-initial roots are realized with
the [ɪn] form, we can therefore conclude that the phonology of
the prefix takes the form [ɪn], and that the nasal does not
change its place of articulation if the root-initial segment is a
vowel or an alveolar consonant. Note that this is generally true
of alveolar nasals in English, as the following data, involving
the prefix seen in (7), suggest:
(9)
(a)
[ʌneɪdəd]
unaided
(b)
[ʌnətʰɹæktɪv]
unattractive
117
(c)
[ʌnivεntfəɫ]
uneventful
(d)
[ʌnɔ:θədɒks]
unorthodox
We might, of course, have said that the morpheme in
question has four different phonological forms: /ɪm/, /ɪɱ/, /ɪn/
and /ɪŋ/, and that words such as impossible, infelicitous,
indirect and incorrect are each stored mentally with the
appropriate prefix. There are two problems with this approach.
Firstly, there is no independent evidence that there is an /ɱ/
phoneme in English ([ɱ] never functions contrastively with
any other nasal). Secondly, even if there were no [ɪɱ] forms,
we would be committed to claiming, under the ‘several
phonological forms’ approach, that it is mere coincidence that
the /ɪm/ form is attached only to roots beginning with a bilabial
consonant, the /ɪŋ/ form only to roots beginning with a velar
consonant, and the /ɪn/ form only to roots beginning with
alveolar consonants and vowels. But that is surely an
implausible claim. So, for this sort of case at least, the idea that
we should postulate more than one phonological form for a
morpheme is deeply unattractive and implausible. Given the
data we have seen thus far, it appears much more plausible to
say that any given morpheme has a single phonological form.
And if that is the case, then it is the phonologist’s task to
hypothesize as to what that form might be. In doing so, she or
he will be guided by evidence and argumentation: the facts of
the matter, since they are mental in nature, and thus not
directly observable, will not be available for immediate
118
inspection via the five senses.
In adopting this ‘one phonological form per morpheme’
approach, we are allowing that, while any given morpheme has
only one phonological form, that phonological form may
‘correspond’ in some sense to a variety of different phonetic
forms. In the case we have just looked at, the prefix has the
phonological form [ɪn], but that in turn corresponds to four
different phonetic forms: [ɪm], [ɪɱ], [ɪn] and [ɪŋ]. Phonologists
refer to such phonetic forms as alternants: we say that there is
an alternation between the four forms. Which alternant of a
given morpheme occurs in a given word is entirely predictable;
there is a generalization which captures that predictability, and
we are able to express it in the form of a phonological rule, just
as we did with [ɾ] and [l] in Korean. In our English case, the
rule in question concerns nasals in general; it might be put
informally as:
(10) The rule of nasal assimilation in English
If the phonological form of a prefix ends in a nasal
then that nasal will assimilate to the place of
articulation of a following consonant.
We could have formulated this generalization as a formalized
rule, or as some kind of constraint on the phonological form of
morphemes in English. We will not go into the types of
formalism required to express such generalizations, or inquire
whether they are best expressed as rules or as constraints. The
most important point is that native speakers appear to be in
possession of generalizations of this sort, and that these appear
to constitute a part of their largely unconscious phonological
knowledge.
The data we have just considered also exemplify an
important phenomenon: that of phonemic overlapping. On
119
the basis of the data in (5), we postulated the following nasal
stop phonemes, with the realizations shown:
(11)
120