Attitude Toward Entrepreneurship: Structure, Prediction From Behavioral Beliefs, and Relation To Entrepreneurial Intention
Attitude Toward Entrepreneurship: Structure, Prediction From Behavioral Beliefs, and Relation To Entrepreneurial Intention
Attitude Toward Entrepreneurship: Structure, Prediction From Behavioral Beliefs, and Relation To Entrepreneurial Intention
DOI 10.1007/s13132-014-0227-2
Abstract The present research investigated the associations among the dimensions of
behavioral beliefs, attitude toward entrepreneurship, and entrepreneurial intention. Using
a structural equation modeling approach with a sample of 227 Greek tertiary education
students (114 males, 113 females), support is provided that entrepreneurial outcomes,
viewed as rewards, are distinguished into intrinsic (intangible/psychological/
nonpecuniary) and extrinsic (tangible/physical/pecuniary). Our results show further that
attitude toward entrepreneurship contains three separable components, one being instru-
mental or cognitive in nature, the other being experiential or affective, and the third
representing opportunity costs, that is, the personal and financial sacrifices one is willing
to incur for the sake of the entrepreneurial venture. Concerning intention for entrepre-
neurship, our findings support a two-factor structure that represents commitment to an
entrepreneurial career and nascent entrepreneurship. Evidence is also found for the
Fishbein and Ajzen’s expectancy-value model of attitudes: that attitude toward entrepre-
neurship is predicted by the expectation that entrepreneurship will be followed by a given
outcome, as well as the value that the individual assigns to that outcome. Moreover, our
results reveal that the intrinsic rewards of entrepreneurship are stronger predictors of
entrepreneurial attitude than extrinsic rewards and that the dimensions of attitude toward
entrepreneurship exert a differential impact on entrepreneurial intention, with affective
attitude appearing to be more strongly related to intention than instrumental attitude.
Introduction
Many studies have examined the relation between attitude toward entrepreneurship
and entrepreneurial intention within the framework of the TPB. According to the TPB,
attitudes toward a behavior are predicted by salient beliefs that the behavior will lead to
particular outcomes in multiplicative combination with outcome evaluations. However,
much fewer studies have examined the more affective aspects of behavioral beliefs and
attitude itself, in addition to the more instrumental or cognitive aspects of these
constructs usually examined (Dawson et al. 2009; French et al. 2005; Horvath et al.
2012; Kolvereid 1996a, 1996b; Kolvereid and Isaksen 2006; Kraft et al. 2005; Pardo
and Ruiz-Tagley 2011). In contrast to the cognitive factors, whose importance has been
highlighted in a significant body of literature on entrepreneurship (Liñán et al. 2013),
affect has long been neglected in models of behavioral decision making (Keer 2012).
The aims of this study were four-fold:
1. To assess the dimensionality of the three TPB constructs examined in this study
(attitude toward entrepreneurship, behavioral beliefs, and entrepreneurial
intention),
2. To provide empirical evidence for the assumption of a multiplicative combination
rule for the effects of belief strength and outcome evaluation on overall attitude,
3. To evaluate the relative importance of the various outcomes of entrepreneurship in
predicting attitude, and
4. To investigate whether the affective and cognitive dimensions of attitude toward
entrepreneurship exert a differential impact on entrepreneurial intention.
Deeply connected to intentional and volitional behavior are beliefs and attitudes (Elfving
2008). Attitude toward a behavior is a person’s overall evaluation of performing the
behavior in question (Ajzen 1991; Fishbein and Ajzen 1975; Mather and Romo 2007;
Schwarz and Bohner 2001). Fishbein and Ajzen (1975: 6) define an attitude as “learned
predisposition to respond in a consistently favorable or unfavorable manner with respect
to a given object.” A similar definition adopt Eagly and Chaiken (1993) who view
attitudes as “psychological tendencies expressed by evaluating a particular entity with
some degree of favor or disfavor.” For Souitaris et al. (2007: 570), “attitude towards self-
employment is the difference between perceptions of personal desirability in becoming
self-employed and organizationally employed.” According to Fini et al. (2012: 390)
“attitude toward behavior, refers to the degree to which a person has a favorable or
unfavorable appraisal of the behavior under scrutiny.”
Theoretical approaches and empirical evidence suggest that attitude should not be
deemed a one-dimensional construct, as overall evaluation often contains two separable
components, one being instrumental or cognitive in nature and the other being expe-
riential or affective. The instrumental component refers to a more cognitive consider-
ation of the extent to which the behavior achieves something valuable or advantageous
and corresponds to the thoughts, knowledge, and perceptions we have about the object
J Knowl Econ
which commonly take the form of beliefs. The experiential/affective component refers
to emotions, feelings (e.g., joy, satisfaction), and drives engendered by the prospect of
performing a behavior (Ajzen 1991; Fernandes and Proença 2013; French et al. 2005;
Kraft et al. 2005; Lawton et al. 2007; Rhodes and Courneya 2003; Trafimow and
Sheeran 1998; Yan 2014).
Based on the argumentation outlined above, the following hypothesis was
formulated and tested in the study:
Behavioral Beliefs
According to the TPB, attitude toward behavior is determined by the individual’s beliefs
about outcomes or attributes of performing the behavior (behavioral belief strength)
weighted by evaluations of those outcomes or attributes (outcome evaluation).
Entrepreneurial outcomes are viewed as rewards of one’s efforts. Entrepreneurship
research reveals that, although one of the key reasons for entering self-employment is
the potential economic benefits arising from the venture, yet entrepreneurship is not
driven solely by extrinsic, pecuniary rewards like income, but many nonmonetary,
intrinsic rewards play a significant role in motivating the entrepreneurial process (Chell
2001; Croson and Minniti 2012; Guerra and Patuelli 2013; Holland and Shepherd 2013).
Intrinsic (intangible/psychological/nonpecuniary) rewards vest in the activity itself
and include the independence, autonomy, freedom, and control gained by being one’s
own boss, an opportunity for creative expression, personal growth, recognition, chal-
lenge, excitement, satisfying a need for achievement, and self-fulfillment arising from
accomplishing personal goals and expectations. Extrinsic (tangible/physical/pecuniary)
rewards typically refer to obtaining monetary benefits and success (Aspaas 2004;
Carter et al. 2003; Chell 2001; Kuratko et al. 1997; Pardo and Ruiz-Tagley 2011;
Robichaud et al. 2001; Wiklund et al. 2003).
Based on these considerations, we put forward and tested the following hypothesis
regarding the structure of behavioral beliefs:
H2. Behavioral beliefs are best represented by a hierarchical model in which the
entrepreneurial outcomes are distinguished into intrinsic and extrinsic.
Entrepreneurial Intention
Bird (1992: 11) defined intention as “a state of mind directing a person’s attention,
experience and behavior towards a specific object or method of behaving.” According
to Boyd and Vozikis (1994: 64), an entrepreneurial intention is “the state of mind that
directs and guides the actions of the entrepreneur toward the development and
implementation of the business concept.” Along a similar line, Fini et al. (2012) suggest
to keep in mind that entrepreneurial intention reflects a state of mind directing a
person’s attention and action toward the enactment of entrepreneurial behavior.
J Knowl Econ
where ABis the attitude toward the behavior and bk is the strength of each belief
which is combined in a multiplicative fashion with the subjective evaluation ek
of the belief attribute, and the resulting products are summed over the K salient
beliefs.
This symbolic equation allows us to explain why persons holding different beliefs
may exhibit identical attitudes, and vice versa, as in the case of two people who may
hold an equally strong belief that entrepreneurship involves facing new challenges, but
one of them may view these challenges as very important to consider when he or she is
to decide his or her future career path while the other may consider them less important
(Moriano et al. 2012).
Evidence for the validity of the expectancy-value hypothesis would be
obtained if addition of the composite belief term in a regression analysis
accounts for additional variance in attitude, over and above the predictive
validity afforded by the belief strength scores and the evaluation scores
(Fishbein and Ajzen 2010).
J Knowl Econ
H3. Belief composite accounts for additional variance in attitude, over and above the
predictive validity afforded by belief strength.
The rational agent-based models used in the research on entrepreneurship and self-
employment transitions assume that better prospects of entrepreneurial earnings as
compared to wages are a major stimulus toward self-employment (Guerra and
Patuelli 2013). Recently, however, the assumption that earnings act as a proxy for
utility has been relaxed, and a number of researchers claim that it is important to
include both economic and psychological motivations in the rational decision process
of potential entrepreneurs (Croson and Minniti 2012). Hamilton (2000) concluded that
nonpecuniary benefits of entrepreneurship are substantial for the self-employed in the
USA and that most entrepreneurs enter and persist in business despite lower initial
earnings and lower earning growth with respect to wage employment, implying a
median earning differential of 35 %. Benz and Frey (2008), using three major data
sets from Germany, Great Britain, and Switzerland, showed that the self-employed
derive higher satisfaction from work than those employed in organizations, irrespective
of income gained or hours worked. Similarly, Croson and Minniti (2012) demonstrated
that newly self-employed individuals are willing to accept lower earning outcomes in
exchange for psychic benefits from self-employment.
Based on this logic and evidence, we hypothesized that
Method
Sample
Two hundred twenty-seven Greek tertiary education students (114 males, 113 females)
from the University and the Technological Institute of Central Greece participated in
this cross-sectional study. The mean age and standard deviation of the students were 21
±1.8 years. Their grade point average based on a 0–10 grading scale was 6.9±0.8.
Out of the total number of participants, 106 were studying at the University of
Central Greece (52 at the Department of Economic Regional Development and 54 at
the Department of Informatics with Applications in Biomedicine). Out of the 111
remaining students, 43 were studying at the Faculty of Health and Caring Professions
and 74 were studying at the Faculty of Technological Applications of the Technological
Institute of Central Greece. Four participants did not report their department of studies.
Measures
Behavioral beliefs were assessed using the scale developed by Kolvereid (1996a,
1996b), and Kolvereid and Isaksen (2006) for tapping five outcomes of entrepreneur-
ship: economic opportunity, autonomy, authority, self-realization, and challenge. Two
items were used as indicators of each dimension. The items tapping the outcomes of
entrepreneurship are listed in Table 1.
Note that the items of the economic opportunity subscale tap monetary extrinsic
rewards of entrepreneurship, whereas all other items assess intrinsic rewards. In
specific, the items of the autonomy and the authority subscales assess the control one
feels in position of exerting over life; the items of the self-realization subscale tap the
opportunity for creative expression and the accomplishment of personal goals; and the
items of the challenge subscale assess personal challenge of an interesting and moti-
vating career.
1. Being an entrepreneur implies more advantages than Liñán and Chen 2009
disadvantages to me.
2. A career as entrepreneur is (totally) attractive for me.
3. If I had the opportunity and resources, I would love to
start a firm (business).
4. Being an entrepreneur would entail great satisfactions for me.
5. I would rather own my own business than earn a higher Kolvereid and Isaksen 2006;
salary employed by someone else. Gundry and Welch 2001
6. I would rather own my own business than pursue another
promising career.
7. I am willing to make significant personal sacrifices in order
to stay in business.
8. I would work somewhere else only long enough to make
another attempt to establish my business.
9. Being an entrepreneur evokes mainly positive thoughts. Leroy et al. 2010
10. Entrepreneurship would present more up than downsides.
J Knowl Econ
evaluative in nature. In addition, these items should reflect the two qualitative compo-
nents of evaluation, the instrumental/cognitive and the experiential/affective.
In this scale, items 1, 9, and 10 represent the instrumental component of attitude,
while items 2, 3, and 4 represent its affective component. The remaining items of the
scale (5, 6, 7, and 8) assess the sacrifices one is willing to incur in order to become self-
employed. The personal and financial sacrifices one is willing to incur for the sake of
the entrepreneurial venture are referred to as opportunity costs (Gundry and Welch
2001). Davidsson (1995), in his economic-psychological model of determinants of
entrepreneurial intentions, included payoff among the variables of the domain attitudes.
For Davidsson (1995), payoff is a composite of beliefs concerning the workload, risk,
and financial gain to be expected by a business founder and concerns the type of
expected outcomes that would typically be included in an explanatory model based on
microeconomic utility theory. Gassar (2006, p. 611) defined opportunity costs as “the
foregone benefit of the next available alternative as a consequence of making a choice”
and provided evidence that an entrepreneur’s opportunity costs are a significant
determinant of the intended scale of venturing activity.
Respondents were asked to indicate their level of agreement with the statements of
the scale from 1 (total disagreement) to 7 (total agreement). A high score on an item,
factor, or the full scale indicated a positive attitude toward entrepreneurship.
Entrepreneurial intention was assessed using the items listed in Table 3, along with
their source. Respondents were asked to indicate their level of agreement with the
statements of the scale from 1 (total disagreement) to 7 (total agreement). A high score
on an item, factor, or the full scale indicated a stronger intention toward
entrepreneurship.
Statistical Analyses
The statistical analyses were performed with SPSS and AMOS software for structural
equation modeling (Arbuckle 2012) using the full information maximum likelihood
procedure to deal with missing values and correlation matrices.
All a priori factor models tested were based on the restrictive assumption that the
error terms of the items measuring each construct were uncorrelated. Error correlations
between items within the same factor were incorporated only if they could be justified
theoretically and interpreted substantively (ten Klooster 2008; Joreskog and Sorbom
1988), as in the case when similar response formats are used for survey items (Evans
et al. 2007), whereas error correlations between items across factors were added only
for items with parallel wordings (Magson et al. 2013; Marsh and Hau 1996; Marsh
et al. 1997; Xu et al. 2013).
Competing factorial or structural models were compared by performing a Chi-square
difference test, when the models compared had a nesting relation (O’Connell and
McCoach 2008), and by using Akaike’s information criterion (AIC), when the models
compared were non-nested, with smaller values of AIC indicating improvement in fit
(Hagenaars and McCutcheon 2002; Kaplan 2000). Some researchers (e.g., Chen et al.
2005; Cheung and Rensvold 2002; Vandenberg and Lance 2000) have noticed that, in
evaluating nested models, the comparative fit index (CFI) change is independent of
both model complexity and sample size and suggested that the null hypothesis of no
difference should be rejected, when the reduction in CFI is 0.01 or more.
Fit indices used to determine goodness of fit included the ratio χ2/df with values less
than 5 indicating an adequate fit and less than three a good fit (Kline 2010; Marsh and
Hovecar 1985), the CFI (Bentler 1990), and the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI; Tucker and
Lewis 1973) with values above 0.90 representing an adequate fit and above 0.95 a good
fit (Hu and Bentler 1999), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA;
Browne and Cudeck 1993) with values of 0.05 or smaller representing close fit, below
0.08, reasonable fit; below 0.10, minimally acceptable fit; and above 0.10, unacceptable
fit (Browne and Cudeck 1993). RMSEA is considered one of the most useful indicators
of model fit (Byrne 2001).
Results
Dimensions of Attitude
French et al. (2005) suggest, before attempting to predict intention using measures of
instrumental and affective attitudes, to establish first that factor analytic techniques can
distinguish between items that are supposed to reflect each of the components, as, if
affective and instrumental components of attitude were genuinely distinct, one would
expect them to form different factors. Following the theoretical approaches to attitude
and the empirical evidence on its components, various competing models were tested
using a nested confirmatory factor modeling approach: a three-factor model, a
two-factor model, and a one-factor model.
We first estimated a three-factor model, M3, in which items 1, 9, and 10 were
allowed to load on only a factor labeled “instrumental attitude,” items 2, 3, and 4 were
allowed to load on only a factor labeled “affective attitude,” and items 5, 6, 7, and 8
were allowed to load on only a factor labeled “opportunity costs.” In model M3, the
correlations between the three factors were freely estimated. Next, we fitted to
J Knowl Econ
Table 4 Fit statistics for competing models of the factor structure of attitude
These results verify hypothesis H1 and are consistent with the view that attitude
should not be considered, nor measured, as a unidimensional construct.
Based on the theoretical approaches to the rewards of entrepreneurship and the a priori
five-factor structure of the scales, alternative factor models were tested and compared
using confirmatory factor analyses. Each analysis included all 20 items from both
scales, while factorial invariance, that is, the same factor structure, was postulated
across the two scales. The models compared were as follows: (a) a model with five
first-order correlated factors: economic opportunity, autonomy, authority, self-realiza-
tion, and challenge; (b) a model with five first-order factors and two second-order
correlated factors: “self-fulfillment” consisting of the self-realization and challenge first-
order factors, and “control over career” consisting of the autonomy and authority first-
order factors; and (c) a model in which the self-fulfillment and control over career
second-order factors loaded on a third-order factor labeled “intrinsic rewards.” We
recall that a higher-order factor underlies correlations between lower order factors.
Due to the identical wording of the indicators (belief strength and outcome evalu-
ation items), the residuals between identically worded items across the two scales were
allowed to correlate in order to obtain more accurate estimates and avoid inflating the
correlation between belief strength and outcome evaluation corresponding factors.
The fit indices of the above described competing models are presented in Table 5. As
suggested by the AIC and the other data in Table 5, the third-order factor model fitted
better than the other two models. Moreover, the best fitted model had a close fit to the
data. In this model, all loadings and correlations were substantial and significant at
p<0.001. In addition, the squared multiple correlations of the indicators ranged from
0.54 to 0.82, indicating that a large portion of variance was accounted for in each
indicator by its underlying factor.
The Cronbach alphas of economic opportunity, autonomy, authority, self-realization,
and challenge dimensions were found equal to 0.79, 0.83, 0.81, 0.80, and 0.88,
respectively, for the belief strength scale, and equal to 0.75, 0.79, 0.76, 0.77, and
0.89, respectively, for the outcome evaluation scale.
These results verify hypothesis H2, showing that behavioral beliefs are best repre-
sented by a third-order factor model in which the entrepreneurial outcomes are distin-
guished into intrinsic and extrinsic.
Table 5 Fit statistics for competing models of the factor structure of the belief strength and the outcome
evaluation scales
Dimensions of Intention
Since no hypothesis was made about the dimensionality of the scale assessing entre-
preneurial intention, first, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of the items of the scale
was performed on the first random split-half sample to identify a viable factor structure.
The EFA was performed using principal component analysis with an oblique rotation,
which assumes factors to be correlated with each another (Jennrich and Sampson
1966). The compliance of the data with the factor analysis was ascertained with
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) and Barlett sphericity test. KMO was found equal to
0.86, and Barlett’s sphericity test was found to be meaningful [χ2(45)=743.317,
p<0.001], indicating that the scale is in compliance with the factor analysis. The
analysis yielded two eigenvalues greater than one (equal to 5.215 and 1.940)
suggesting, according to Kaiser’s (1960) criterion, that two factors should be extracted.
These two factors explained the 71.6 % of the total variance. The factor structure of the
scale was very clear. In specific, all items had primary loadings over 0.57, with only
two cross-loadings being slightly above the recommended cutoff level of 0.30
(Tabachnick and Fidell 1996). Under this two-factor solution, items 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and
8 were grouped in one factor which reflects “commitment to an entrepreneurial career”
and is consistent with the understanding of Krueger (1993) of entrepreneurial intention
as the commitment to start a new business, while the remaining four items loaded onto
the second factor which can be labeled “nascent entrepreneurship.”
Some researchers have challenged the view that entrepreneurial activity is a
static process with two outcomes—to engage or to refrain from it—and put
forth a dynamic approach that discriminates between prebirth, birth, and post-
natal stages of entrepreneurship, which may have different antecedents, with the
prebirth stage often being referred to as latent or nascent entrepreneurship
(Grilo and Thurik 2005, 2008; Masuda 2006; Reynolds 1997; van der Zwan
et al. 2010; van Gelderen et al. 2005). Latent entrepreneurship (hidden or
potentially existing but not yet realized) is defined as the preference for
entrepreneurship over paid employment, and it is visible in people who are
willing and able to become entrepreneurs but have not yet decided to start a
business, while nascent (emerging) entrepreneurship refers to individuals who
have made a decision and are taking some action toward creating a new
business (Blanchflower et al. 2001; Hayek 2012; Verheul et al. 2012).
The two-factor model for intention was subsequently verified through confir-
matory factor analysis conducted on the second random split-half sample. The
results of the analysis showed that the model had an acceptable fit to the data
[χ2/df=3.127, TLI=0.958, CFI=0.970, RMSEA=0.097]. These fit indices were
achieved by allowing correlated errors between the following pairs of items: (a)
item 6 (“I read books on how to set up a firm”) and item 7 (“I spend time
learning about starting a firm”), with both items referring to reading or learning;
(b) item 9 (“I attend seminars and conferences that focus on a start your own
business planning”) and item 10 (“I participate in seminars that focus on writing
a business plan”), with both items referring to attending or participating in a
seminar. Note that all these items are from the nascent entrepreneurship factor.
Incorporating these correlated errors in the model was considered acceptable on
the basis of the similarities in the content of the items involved.
J Knowl Econ
Correlational Analysis
After the factor analyses were performed, a score was calculated for each construct by
adding together all the items comprising the corresponding subscale to obtain a total
score for each. The correlations among the constructs are presented in Table 6. These
correlations fall within the range of those reported in relevant studies and reviews in the
literature.
For example, in a study by Kolvereid (1996b), the correlations of economic
opportunity, autonomy, authority, and self-realization with intention were found equal
to 0.11, 0.32, 0.31, and 0.20, respectively. In another study by Kolvereid and Isaksen
(2006), the corresponding correlations were found equal to 0.21, 0.07, 0.13, and 0.11.
In the same study, the correlations of economic opportunity, autonomy, authority, and
self-realization with attitude, as measured by the subscale used in our study to assess
opportunity costs, were found equal to 0.26, 0.28, 0.20, and 0.23, respectively. The data
in Table 6 show, however, that the relations of behavioral belief composite with attitude
and intention are somewhat stronger than those found by previous studies. We attribute
this difference in the magnitude of correlations to the fact that the belief construct used
in these studies was outcome evaluation while the belief construct in Table 6 is belief
composite.
Consistently with the findings of previous studies, the data in Table 6 show also that
attitude is related with intention at levels ranging from r=0.47 to r=0.75. In a meta-
analysis of 23 studies, Hausenblas et al. (1997) found a weighted average correlation of
0.52 between attitude and intention. In another meta-analysis of Armitage and Conner
(2001), conducted with 185 independent empirical tests of the theories of reasoned
action and planned behavior reported in 161 journal articles and book chapters, the
average correlation between attitude and behavioral intentions was found equal to 0.49.
In the most recent meta-analysis of 98 relevant studies (123 samples, N=114.007),
Construct 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Belief composite
1. Economic opportunity 1.00
2. Autonomy 0.53 1.00
3. Authority 0.54 0.71 1.00
4. Self-realization 0.46 0.61 0.48 1.00
5. Challenge 0.41 0.57 0.44 0.67 1.00
Attitude
6. Instrumental attitude 0.42 0.44 0.42 0.38 0.53 1.00
7. Affective attitude 0.41 0.48 0.42 0.37 0.48 0.79 1.00
8. Opportunity costs 0.34 0.41 0.39 0.32 0.39 0.64 0.70 1.00
Entrepreneurial intention
9. Commitment to entrepreneurship 0.37 0.43 0.41 0.35 0.40 0.65 0.75 0.68 1.00
10. Nascent entrepreneurship 0.23 0.27 0.34 0.24 0.23 0.50 0.47 0.52 0.51 1.00
All correlations are statistically significant at the 0.001 level (two-tailed tests)
J Knowl Econ
Toward testing whether belief strength and the corresponding belief strength/outcome
evaluation cross-product each contributes significantly, positively, and independently to
the prediction of attitude, we first recoded each outcome evaluation item into a bipolar
scale, and then, we multiplied each behavioral belief item score with its corresponding
outcome evaluation item score to obtain a composite score for each item of the scale in
Table 1. However, instead of using aggregate measures in a regression analysis
10
obtained by summing the belief strength scores ( ∑ bk ) and the product scores for each
k¼1 10
of the ten belief strength and outcome evaluation combinations ( ∑ bk ek ), we adopted a
k¼1
structural equation modeling approach where belief strength and belief composite were
treated as latent constructs to control for random measurement error and get unbiased
estimates of effects (Green et al. 1999).
Each of these two latent constructs was measured by three-item parcels as indicators:
economic opportunity, control over career, and self-fulfillment. Little et al. (2002) argue
that using item parcels as indicators produces more reliable latent variables than individ-
ual items. The economic opportunity score was produced by averaging the two items of
the corresponding subscale, while the four items of the autonomy and the authority
subscales and the four items of the self-realization and the challenge subscales were
averaged to produce a control over career score and a self-fulfillment score, respectively.
Overall attitude was also treated as latent construct formed by parceling the scale for the
direct measurement of attitude into three parcels composed of the average of the items of
the affective, the instrumental, and the opportunity costs subscales, which emerged as
distinct factors in the CFA of the scale assessing directly attitude.
The model used to test the expectancy-value approach to attitude toward entrepre-
neurship is illustrated in Fig. 1. In this model, direct paths were hypothesized from the
latent constructs of belief strength and belief composite to the latent construct of
attitude. Due to the identical wording of the item parcel indicators of belief strength
and belief composite, we allowed residuals to correlate between the matching indicators
across the two constructs. The theoretical model proposed and tested fitted the data well
(χ2/df=2.100, TLI=0.969, CFI=0.982, RMSEA=0.070). The structural coefficients for
the model are presented in Fig. 1 with standardized values. The path from belief
composite to attitude was significant at the p=0.05 level (critical ratio=2.354,
p=0.02), whereas the path from belief strength approached statistical significance
(critical ratio=1.853, p=0.06). These findings support the expectancy-value model of
attitudes of Fishbein and Ajzen (2010) and verify hypothesis H3.
1
Self-fulfillment 0.83
1 Beliefs
Control over career 0.85
strength
Instrumental 1
0 .72 attitude
1
Economic opportunity
0.30 0.87
0.12
0.87 Affective 1
0.48 Attitude 0.91 attitude
0 .54 0.40
0.76
1
Economic opportunity
Opportunity 1
costs
0.68
1
Control over career 0.81 Beliefs
composite
0.75
1
Self-fulfillment
Fig. 1 Path model predicting the effects of belief strength and belief composite on attitude. Note: latent
constructs are represented as ovals and multiple indicators of the latent constructs as rectangles. Single-
arrowed paths represent hypothesized directional relationships. Curved double arrow-headed lines represent
correlations between constructs or between construct residuals. The numbers by the single arrow-headed lines
are standardized regression weights. The dotted curved double arrow-headed line represents nonsignificant
correlation. Solid lines represent significant coefficients at the p=0.05 level
(economic opportunity, control over career, and self-fulfillment) were the independent
variables. The scores of the autonomy and authority first-order factors and the scores of
the self-realization and challenge first-order factors were used as observed indicators of
the control over career and self-fulfillment second-order constructs, respectively, while
economic opportunity was measured by the two indicators of the corresponding subscale.
Affective attitude and instrumental attitude were each measured by the three items of the
corresponding subscale, whereas the construct of opportunity costs was formed by
parceling the items of the corresponding subscale into two parcels.
All three models fitted the data very well (affective attitude χ2/df=1.804, TLI=
0.964, CFI=0.974, RMSEA=0.060; instrumental attitude χ2/df=2.101, TLI=0.949,
CFI=0.963, RMSEA=0.070; opportunity costs χ2/df=1.741, TLI=0.967, CFI=0.977,
RMSEA=0.058). The results obtained showed that the most important predictor of
affective attitude was control over career (Beta=0.353, critical ratio=2.871, p=0.004),
followed by self-fulfillment (Beta=0.286, critical ratio=2.252, p=0.024), whereas the
most important predictor of instrumental attitude was self-fulfillment (Beta=0.498,
critical ratio=4.913, p<0.001), followed by economic opportunity (Beta=0.212, crit-
ical ratio=2.314, p=0.021). Control over career was the only significant predictor for
opportunity costs (Beta=0.517, critical ratio=5.834, p<0.001).
These results verify hypothesis H4, showing that the intrinsic nonmonetary rewards
of entrepreneurship are more important in predicting attitude than the extrinsic mone-
tary rewards.
The conceptual framework for exploring the relations among behavioral beliefs,
attitude, and intention is presented in Fig. 2. In this model, behavioral beliefs, attitude,
and intention were treated as latent constructs. Consistent with the TPB and the results
J Knowl Econ
obtained when testing the Fishbein and Ajzen’s expectancy-value model of attitudes,
behavioral beliefs were represented by the latent construct of belief composite mea-
sured by three-item parcels as indicators: economic opportunity, control over career,
and self-fulfillment. To investigate whether the attitude dimensions have a differential
impact on intention, attitude was represented by three latent constructs: affective
attitude, instrumental attitude, and opportunity costs, measured as in the previous
section. Entrepreneurial intention and nascent entrepreneurship were each measured
also by two indicators formed by aggregating the items of the corresponding subscale
into two parcels.
In the model illustrated in Fig. 2, direct paths were allowed from belief composite,
which was treated as the independent variable, to affective attitude and instrumental
attitude. Noting that opportunity costs, as measured in our work, represent one’s
preference for entrepreneurship, affective attitude and instrumental attitude were, in
turn, allowed to impact opportunity costs, as suggested by Ajzen’s (1991) TPB. By
further viewing preference for entrepreneurship as reflecting what Ajzen and Fishbein
(1980) referred to as “choice intentions,” a direct path was postulated from opportunity
costs to commitment to entrepreneurship, since the effect of choice intentions is
predominant over behavioral intentions (Kolvereid 1996a; Verheul et al. 2012).
We also hypothesized that commitment to entrepreneurship directly affects nascent
entrepreneurship. This direct path is in accordance with the TPB that intention is
assumed to be the immediate antecedent of behavior and the view that the most
proximal measure of entrepreneurial behavior is that of nascent gestation behaviors,
such as writing a business plan (Martin 2012). Finally, we allowed residuals to correlate
1 1 1
#1 #9 #10
1 1 1
0.74 0.91 0.80 P1 P1 P1
1
Self-fulfillment
Instrumental 0.88
0.75 0.95
0.55 attitude
0.96 0.27 .88
1 Commitment to .65 Nascent
0.44 Economic opportunity Beliefs Opportunity
0 .49 entrepreneurship entrepreneurship
composite costs
0 .94 0.63
0.56 Affective 0.87 0.96 0.57
1 attitude
Control over career
P2 P2 P2
0.89 0.81 0.89
1 1 1
#2 #3 #4
1 1 1
Fig. 2 Conceptual path model of expected relationships among belief composite, attitude dimensions,
intention, and nascent entrepreneurship. Note: latent constructs are represented as ovals and measured
variables and multiple indicators of the latent constructs as rectangles. Single-arrowed paths represent
hypothesized directional relationships. The curved double arrow-headed line represents correlation between
construct residuals. The dotted single arrow-headed line represents significant coefficient at the p=0.10 level.
Solid lines represent significant coefficients at the p<0.001 level. The numbers by the single arrow-headed
lines are standardized regression weights. The numbers within the rectangles representing the indicators of
instrumental and affective attitude correspond to the item numbers of the attitude scale in Table 2. The
indicators of opportunity costs, intention, and nascent entrepreneurship are item parcels. For opportunity costs,
parcel P1 consists of items 5 and 6, and parcel P2 consists of items 7 and 8 of the attitude scale in Table 2. For
commitment to entrepreneurship, parcel P1 consists of items 1, 2, and 3, and parcel P2 consists of items 4, 5,
and 8 of the intention scale in Table 3. For nascent entrepreneurship, parcel P1 consists of items 6 and 7, and
parcel P2 consists of items 9 and 10 of the intention scale in Table 3
J Knowl Econ
between control over career and self-fulfillment, as these two constructs share a higher-
order factor of intrinsic rewards.
The theoretical model proposed and tested fitted the data well (χ2/df=2.735, TLI=
0.922, CFI=0.939, RMSEA=0.088). Moreover, none of the direct paths not included in
the model had a modification index greater than 5. However, in this model, the direct
path from instrumental attitude to opportunity costs was not significant although
approached significance (critical ratio = 1.687, p = 0.09). The total effect of
instrumental attitude on both commitment and nascent entrepreneurship was roughly
half as great as that of affective attitude (total effect of affective attitude on
commitment=0.55, total effect of instrumental attitude on commitment=0.28, total
effect of affective attitude on nascent entrepreneurship = 0.39, total effect of
instrumental attitude on nascent entrepreneurship = 0.20). Overall, the model
explained the 76.5 % of the variance in commitment to an entrepreneurial career and
the 42.7 % of the variance in nascent entrepreneurship.
These results verify hypothesis H5 and are consistent with the research findings from
the literature that affective attitude tends to be more strongly connected to intention than
instrumental attitude (French et al. 2005; Kraft et al. 2005; Trafimow et al. 2004) and
that attitudes explain over 50 % of the variance in intentions and intentions account for
over 28 % of the variance in behavior (Kim and Hunter 1993; Sheeran 2002).
More recently, Zampetakis et al. (2009), Iakovleva et al. (2011), and Liñán et al.
(2013) conducted studies using structural equation modeling to test the TPB in the
prediction of entrepreneurial intention. In the study of Zampetakis et al. (2009), the
direct effect of attitude to intention was found equal to 0.92; in the study of Iakovleva
et al. (2011), this effect was equal to 0.57, while Liñán et al. (2013) obtained a direct
effect equal to 0.60. We note that these studies incorporated all three antecedents of
intention, namely, attitude, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control. In our
study, the direct effect of opportunity costs to intention was found equal to 0.88
(standardized value).
Discussion
This study focused on three of the TPB constructs within the context of entrepreneur-
ship, namely, entrepreneurial behavioral beliefs, attitude toward entrepreneurship, and
entrepreneurial intention, by specifically examining the dimensionality of these con-
structs and evaluating structural relations among their components using structural
equation modeling via AMOS.
Consistent with prior research which suggests that entrepreneurial outcomes can be
both extrinsic, such as income, personal wealth, value creation, innovation, growth, and
profit, and intrinsic, such as satisfaction, independence, excitement, and challenge
(Petkova 2009), our findings revealed that entrepreneurial outcomes, as assessed by
the self-report measure of our study, are not a unitary construct but consist of five
distinct components: economic opportunity, autonomy, authority, self-realization, and
challenge. However, our study goes one step further in that it provided strong support
for a hierarchical structure of entrepreneurial outcomes.
In common with previous research examining the dimensions of attitude toward
entrepreneurship, the data in our study supported a two-factor model consisting of both
J Knowl Econ
instrumental and affective attitudes than a model consisting of only a single attitude
factor. Our findings add to the literature regarding the dimensionality of attitude toward
entrepreneurship, which provides adequate evidence for the distinction of attitude into
two subtypes: instrumental and affective (Ajzen 1991; Fernandes and Proença 2013;
French et al. 2005; Kraft et al. 2005; Lawton et al. 2007; Rhodes and Courneya 2003;
Trafimow and Sheeran 1998; Yan 2014). It should, however, be noted that, although
instrumental and affective attitude emerged as distinct factors in our factor analytic
investigation of the data, these factors are both evaluative in nature, and as such, it is
expected to be interrelated.
Having established the multidimensional nature of attitude, it is reasonable to
explore how each of the components of attitude relates to behavioral beliefs and to
entrepreneurial intention.
Consistent with previous studies, our findings suggest that affective attitude is a
stronger predictor of intention than instrumental attitude. Attitude is one of the central
constructs in the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior. As implied by its
name, the TRA suggests that cognitive attitudes should be more strongly related to
intentions, as intentions are presumed to be based on an expectancy-value model of
attitudes, which takes into account both the subjective probability and the value of
various potential outcomes produced by a given behavior (Trafimow et al. 2004). On
the other hand, research from an evolutionary perspective (e.g., Damasio 1994;
Johnston 1999) suggests that affect provides the motivational power for behaviors
and should, therefore, be more important than cognition for predicting most intentions.
Intention is the personal, deliberate, goal-oriented, proactive aspect of behavior and
is closely related to the concept of volition and intrinsic motivation (Fernandes and
Proença 2013), which refers to the individual’s desire to engage in a behavior because it
is personally rewarding, essentially, performing an activity for its own sake and not in
order to attain some extrinsic goal (Bénabou and Tirole 2003; Kivetz 2005). As positive
affect enhances intrinsic motivation (Isen and Reeve 2005), affectively based attitudes
tend to be more persistent and resistant to change despite situational conditions or
rational arguments, while instrumental attitudes are based on calculative considerations
of the advantageous or disadvantageous outcomes of a certain behavior, and as such,
they are more vulnerable to external incentives and can be more easily invalidated and
changed (Fernandes and Proença 2013; Heath and Nairn 2005; LaBarge 2007). This
argument offers an explanation for the primacy of affective attitude over instrumental
attitude in the prediction of intention,
Another explanation can be derived from the work of those researchers who
suggested that the relative importance of affective attitude and instrumental attitude
in the prediction of intention depends either on the individual’s personality or on the
nature of the behavior. Thus, Trafimow et al. (2004) noted that some people are more
under affective control, across behaviors, whereas other people are more under cogni-
tive control, and concluded that the predictive validity of affect and cognition in
determining entrepreneurial intention should depend upon whether participants in a
study are affectively or cognitively controlled. However, other researchers (e.g., Yan
2014) argue that the impact of the instrumental and affective components of attitude on
the formation of the behavioral intention is moderated by the nature of the behavior and
that affective attitude should be a more powerful predictor of intention for behaviors
which are under the volitional control of the individual. The favorable feelings or
J Knowl Econ
approach is flawed (for a review, see Francis et al. 2004b). French and Hankins (2003)
recommend either the use of alternative analytic strategies, such as experimental
designs and ANOVA analyses, or the use of individually salient beliefs rather than
“modal” beliefs. This view has been disputed by Francis et al. (2004b) who see it as
leading to an uncomfortable pair of options.
We argue that multiplying the strength of each belief by the corresponding subjec-
tive evaluation, and summing these cross-products over all salient beliefs to obtain on
overall index of belief composite can obscure important distinctions between subtypes
of behavioral beliefs. On these grounds, in our study, we adopted a structural equation
modeling approach where belief strength and belief composite were treated as latent
constructs measured by use of multiple indicators to control for random measurement
error and get unbiased estimates of effects, but also to analyze data at the item
or item-parcel level.
intention. Finally, our study adds to the existing evidence for the expectancy-value
model of attitude justifying the suggestion that both types of measures (direct and
indirect) should be used in TPB questionnaires, as by doing so, it will be possible to
explain more variance in the model (Francis et al. 2004b).
The study’s findings have implications for theory and practice. At a theoretical level,
our work highlights the emotional aspect of entrepreneurship process showing that
emotions play an important role in determining and regulating entrepreneurs’ cogni-
tions, behaviors, and venture efforts (Cardon et al. 2009; Foo et al. 2009). Thus, our
study suggests that in order to effectively increase entrepreneurial intention, it is
important to target both the cognitive and affective bases of attitudes toward entrepre-
neurship (Zampetakis et al. 2009).
Our study has also implications for educators and policy makers. Programs to
enhance and develop traits that are associated with entrepreneurial success and provide
skills that entrepreneurs will need latter, with the aim of encouraging young people to
start their own businesses, have been spreading over the last decades at a considerable
pace and courses are being implemented in universities, secondary schools, and even
primary ones (do Paço et al. 2011; Liñán 2004). The European Commission notes that
the “primary purpose of entrepreneurship education [at higher education level] is to
develop entrepreneurial capacities and mindsets” (European Commission 2008: 11)
and recommends integrating entrepreneurship more fully into university curricula. The
results from this study suggest that entrepreneurship education should not be confused
with general business and economic studies and focus only on the learning of entre-
preneurial competencies, such as business planning; entrepreneurship education should
always include affective learning goals and develop personal attributes and skills that
form the basis of an entrepreneurial mindset and behavior (creativity, sense of initiative,
autonomy, etc.). (European Commission 2008; Müller 2011).
On a policy level, the results obtained here suggest that a government should create
and support an environment that fosters entrepreneurship through less restrictive
legislation and regulations, lower bureaucratic barriers for engaging in self-employ-
ment, and sufficient access to credit for enterprises, because this would provide
individuals with added procedural utility resulting from the individual’s sense of control
over career (Benz and Frey 2008).
However, this study is not without its limitations which need to be brought to the
reader’s attention. First, the study relies on student participants who were drawn from
two higher education institutions, located in two neighboring cities of Central Greece.
However, although the sample size (N=227) was relatively small, it was greater than
the suggested for SEM minimum sample size of 200. In addition, the number of cases
per estimated parameter, equal to 6.14 for the model with the greater number of
parameters in Fig. 2, falls within the typical range from 5 to 20 (Brown 2006;
Jackson 2003; Weston and Gore 2006). Therefore, caution must be applied in
interpreting our results and generalizing the findings to other cohorts of students.
Future research may explore the hypotheses put forth in this study with a larger and
more representative sample of students from various disciplines and different Greek
higher education institutions, or even with respondents other than students. Replication
of this study with such a sample would add weight to the reported results.
Another limitation of the study is its cross-sectional design, which limits the
assessment of causality. Attitude theory researchers have long argued that cognition
J Knowl Econ
and affect interact in a reciprocal process in attitude formation and that instrumental
attitude and affective attitude are both a cause and an effect of each other (Fernandes
and Proença 2013). In order to establish a causal relationship between these two
components of attitude, longitudinal data are needed.
Future research may also attempt to include beliefs of the outcomes of being organiza-
tionally employed along with the beliefs for entrepreneurial outcomes, as by disentangling
the reasons for preferring self-employment from those of organizational employment; a
better understanding of the former can be achieved (Guerra and Patuelli 2013).
Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the two anonymous reviewers for their constructive and
supportive comments that helped improve the quality of the paper.
References
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: a theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.),
Action-control: from cognition to behaviour (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg: Springer.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
50(2), 179–211.
Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned
behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 665–683.
Ajzen, I. (2006). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: conceptual and methodological considerations. Available
at: http://www.people.umass.edu/aizen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf. Accessed 4 October 2013.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice Hall.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2012). Amos 21 user’s guide. Spring House: Amos Development Corporation.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analytic review.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 471–499.
Aspaas, H. R. (2004). Minority women’s microenterprises in rural area of the United States of America:
African American, Hispanic American and Native American case studies. GeoJournal, 61(3), 281–289.
Autio, E., Keeley, R. H., Klofsten, M., Parker, G. G. C., & Hay, M. (2001). Entrepreneurial intent among
students in Scandinavia and in the USA. Enterprise and Innovation Management Studies, 2(2), 145–160.
Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, J., & Yi, Y. (1989). An investigation into the role of intentions as mediators of the
attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Economic Psychology, 10(1), 35–62.
Bénabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2003). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Review of Economic Studies,
70(3), 489–520.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246.
Benz, M., & Frey, B. S. (2008). Being independent is a great thing: subjective evaluations of self-employment
and hierarchy. Economica, 75(298), 362–383.
Bird, B. (1988). Implementing entrepreneurial ideas: the case for intention. Academy of Management Review,
13(3), 442–453.
Bird, B. (1992). The operation of intentions in time: the emergence of the new venture. Entrepreneurship:
Theory and Practice, 17(1), 11–20.
Blanchflower, D. G., Oswald, A., & Stutzer, A. (2001). Latent entrepreneurship across nations. European
Economic Review, 45(4–6), 680–691.
Boyd, N., & Vozikis, G. (1994). The influence of self-efficacy on the development of entrepreneurial
intentions and actions. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 18(4), 63–77.
Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford Press.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long
(Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newsbury Park: Sage.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: basic concepts, applications and program-
ming. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cardon, M. S., Wincent, J., Singh, J., & Drnovsek, M. (2009). The nature and experience of entrepreneurial
passion. Academy of Management Review, 34(3), 511–532.
J Knowl Econ
Carter, N. M., Gartner, W. B., Shaver, K. G., & Gatewood, E. J. (2003). The career reasons of nascent
entrepreneurs. Journal of Business Venturing, 18(1), 13–39.
Chell, E. (2001). Entrepreneurship: globalization, innovation and development. Stamford: Thomson Learning.
Chen, F. F., Sousa, K. H., & West, S. G. (2005). Testing measurement invariance of second-order factor
models. Structural Equation Modeling, 12(3), 471–492.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement
invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233–255.
Croson, D. C., & Minniti, M. (2012). Slipping the surly bonds: the value of autonomy in self-employment.
Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(2), 355–365.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.
Davidsson, P. (1995). Determinants of entrepreneurial intentions. Paper presented at the RENT IX
Conference, 23–24 November, 1995, Piacenza Italy.
Dawson, C., Henley, A., & Latreille, P. (2009). Why do individuals choose self-employment? IZA Discussion
Paper, No. 3974. Available at: http://ftp.iza.org/dp3974.pdf. Accessed 29 November 2013.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination
of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268.
do Paço, A. M. F., Ferreira, J. M., Raposo, M., Rodrigues, R. G., & Dinis, A. (2011). Behaviors and
entrepreneurial intention: empirical findings about secondary students. Journal of International
Entrepreneurship, 9(1), 20–38.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brance Javanovich.
Elfving, J. (2008). Contextualizing entrepreneurial intentions: a multiple case study on. entrepreneurial
cognition and perception. Turku: Åbo Akademi Förlag.
Engle, R. L., Dimitriadi, N., Gavidia, J. V., Schlaegel, C., Delanoe, S., Alvarado, I., He, X., Buame, S., &
Wollf, B. (2010). A twelve-country evaluation of Ajzen’s model of planned behavior”. International
Journal of Entrepreneurial Behavior and Research, 16(1), 33–57.
European Commission (2008). European Commission Expert Group: entrepreneurship in higher education,
especially within non-business studies. Available at: http://ec.europa.eu/enterprise/ newsroom/cf/
itemshortdetail.cfm?item_id=3366. Accessed 30 November 2009.
Evans, B., Glendon, A. I., & Creed, P. A. (2007). Development and initial validation of an aviation safety
climate scale. Journal of Safety Research, 38(6), 675–682.
Fayolle, A. (2006). Effect and counter effect of entrepreneurship education and social context on student’s
intention. Estudios de Economica Aplicada, 24(2), 509–523.
Fayolle, A. (2007). Handbook of research in entrepreneurship education: a general perspective. Cheltenham:
Edward Elgar.
Fernandes, T., & Proença, J. (2013). Reassessing relationships in consumer markets: emotion, cognition, and
consumer relationship intention. Journal of Relationship Marketing, 12(1), 41–58.
Fini, R., Grimaldi, R., Marzocchi, G. L., & Sobrero, M. (2012). The determinants of corporate entrepreneurial
intention within small and newly established firms. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 36(2), 387–414.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and
research. Reading: Addison-Wesley.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: the reasoned action approach. New
York: Taylor and Francis.
Foo, M.-D., Uy, M. A., & Baron, R. A. (2009). How do feelings influence effort? An empirical study of
entrepreneurs’ affect and venture effort. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4), 1086–1094.
Francis, J. J., Eccles, M. P., Johnston, M., Walker, A., Grimshaw, J., Foy, R., Kaner, E. F. S., Smith, L., &
Bonetti, D. (2004a). Constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour: a manual for
health services researchers. Newcastle: Centre for Health Services Research, University of Newcastle.
Francis, J. J., Eccles, M. P., Johnston, M., Walker, A., Grimshaw, J., Foy, R., Kaner, E. F. S., Smith, L., &
Bonetti, D. (2004b). Measurement issues in the theory of planned behaviour: a supplement to the manual
for constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour. Newcastle: Centre for Health
Services Research, University of Newcastle.
French, D. P., & Hankins, M. (2003). The expectancy-value muddle in the theory of planned behaviour—and
some proposed solutions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8(1), 37–55.
French, D. P., Sutton, S. R., Hennings, S. J., Mitchell, J., Wareham, N. J., Griffin, S., Hardeman, W., &
Kinmonth, A. L. (2005). The importance of affective beliefs and attitudes in the theory of planned behavior:
predicting intentions to increase physical activity. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35(9), 1824–1848.
Gassar, C. (2006). Opportunity costs and intended venture growth. Journal of Business Venturing, 21(5), 610–632.
Green, D. P., Salovey, P., & Truax, K. M. (1999). Static, dynamic, and causative bipolarity of affect. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 856–867.
J Knowl Econ
Grilo, I., & Thurik, A. R. (2005). Entrepreneurial engagement levels in the European Union. International
Journal of Entrepreneurship Education, 2, 143–168.
Grilo, I., & Thurik, A. R. (2008). Determinants of entrepreneurial engagement levels in Europe and the US.
Industrial and Corporate Change, 17(6), 1113–1145.
Guerra, G., & Patuelli, R. (2013). The role of job satisfaction in transitions into self-employment. Quaderni
DSE Working Paper N° 849. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com /sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2151872.
Accessed 11 July 2014.
Gundry, L. K., & Welch, H. P. (2001). The ambitious entrepreneur: high growth strategies of women-owned
enterprises. Journal of Business Venturing, 16(5), 453–470.
Hagenaars, J. A., & McCutcheon, A. L. (2002). Applied latent class analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Hales, D., Evenson, K. R., Wen, F., & Wilcox, S. (2010). Postpartum physical activity: measuring theory of
planned behavior constructs. American Journal of Health Behavior, 34(4), 387–401.
Hamilton, B. H. (2000). Does entrepreneurship pay? An empirical analysis of the returns to self‐employment.
Journal of Political Economy, 108(3), 604–631.
Hausenblas, H. A., Carron, A. V., & Mack, D. E. (1997). Application of the theories of reasoned action and
planned behavior to exercise behavior: a meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19(1),
36–51.
Hayek, M. (2012). Control beliefs and positive psychological capital. Journal of Management Research,
13(1), 3–13.
Heath, R. G., & Nairn, A. C. (2005). Measuring emotive advertising—implications of low attention process-
ing on recall. Journal of Advertising Research, 45(2), 269–281.
Hindle, K., Klyver, K., & Jennings, D. F. (2009). An ‘informed’ intent model: incorporating human capital,
social capital and gender variables into the theoretical model of entrepreneurial intentions. In A. Carsrud
& M. Brännback (Eds.), Understanding the entrepreneurial mind: opening the black box (pp. 35–50).
Heidelberg: Springer.
Holland, D. V., & Shepherd, D. A. (2013). Deciding to persist: adversity, values, and entrepreneurs’ decision
policies. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 37(2), 331–358.
Horvath, C., Lewis, I., & Watson, B. (2012). The beliefs which motivate young male and female drivers to
speed: a comparison of low and high intenders. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 45, 334–341.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.
Iakovleva, T., Kolvereid, L., & Stephan, U. (2011). Entrepreneurial intentions in developing and developed
countries. Education and Training, 53(5), 353–370.
Isen, A., & Reeve, J. (2005). The influence of positive affect on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: facilitating
enjoyment of play, responsible work behavior, and self-control. Motivation and Emotion, 29(4), 295–323.
Jackson, D. L. (2003). Revisiting sample size and the number of parameter estimates: some support for the n:q
hypothesis. Structural Equation Modeling: A multidisciplinary Journal, 10(1), 128–141.
Jennrich, R. I., & Sampson, P. F. (1966). Rotation for simple loadings. Psychometrika, 31(3), 313–323.
Johnston, V. S. (1999). Why we feel: the science of emotions. Reading: Helix Books.
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1988). LISREL 7: a guide to the program and applications. Chicago: SPSS, Inc.
Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computer to factor analysis. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 20(1), 141–151.
Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling: foundations and extensions. London: Sage.
Kato, S. (2013). Entrepreneurship as a process of self-fulfillment: well-being, affect, and behavioral strategies.
PhD dissertation, Syracuse University.
Keer, M. (2012). Persuasion through facts and feelings: integrating affect and cognition into behavioral
decision models and health messages. PhD thesis, Universiteit van Amsterdam.
Kennedy, J., Drennan, J., Renfrow, P., & Watson, B. (2003, September). Situational factors and entrepre-
neurial intentions. Paper presented at the Small Enterprise Association of Australia and New Zealand 16th
Annual Conference, 28 September-1 October, 2003, Ballarat, Australia.
Kim, M. S., & Hunter, J. E. (1993). Relationships among attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior: a
meta-analysis of past research, part 2. Communication Research, 20(3), 331–364.
Kivetz, R. (2005). Promotion reactance: the role of effort-reward congruity. Journal of Consumer Research,
31(4), 725–736.
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling (3rd ed.). New York: The
Guildford Press.
Kolvereid, L. (1996a). Organizational employment versus self-employment: reasons for career choice inten-
tions. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 20(3), 23–31.
J Knowl Econ
Kolvereid, L. (1996b). Prediction of employment status choice intentions. Entrepreneurship: Theory and
Practice, 21(1), 47–57.
Kolvereid, L., & Isaksen, E. (2006). New business start-up and subsequent entry into self-employment.
Journal of Business Venturing, 21(6), 866–885.
Kraft, P., Rise, J., Sutton, S., & Roysamb, E. (2005). Perceived difficulty in the theory of planned
behaviour: perceived behavioural control or affective attitude? British Journal of Social Psychology,
44(3), 479–496.
Krueger, N. F. (1993). The impact of prior entrepreneurial exposure on perceptions and new venture feasibility
and desirability. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 18(1), 5–21.
Krueger, N. F., & Brazeal, D. (1994). Entrepreneurial potential and potential entrepreneurs. Entrepreneurship:
Theory and Practice, 18(3), 91–104.
Krueger, N. F., Reilly, M. D., & Carsrud, A. L. (2000). Competing models of entrepreneurial intentions.
Journal of Business Venturing, 15(5–6), 411–432.
Kuratko, D. F., Hornsby, J. S., & Naffziger, D. W. (1997). An examination of owners’ goals in sustaining
entrepreneurship. Journal of Small Business Management, 35(1), 24–33.
LaBarge, M. C. (2007). Integral affect and attitude strength in health communications. PhD dissertation,
University of Oregon.
Lange, T. (2012). Job satisfaction and self-employment: autonomy or personality? Small Business Economics,
38(2), 165–177.
Lawton, R., Conner, M., & Parker, D. (2007). Beyond cognition: predicting health risk behaviors from
instrumental and affective beliefs. Health Psychology, 26(3), 259–267.
Leroy, H., Maes, J., Sels, L., & Kickul, J. R. (2010, August). Gender differences in entrepreneurial intentions:
a TPB multi-group analysis at factor and indicator level. Paper presented at the 2010 DIANA
International Conference on Women’s Entrepreneurship Research, 3–4 August, 2010, Banff, Canada.
Liñán, F. (2004). Intention-based models of entrepreneurship education. Piccolla Impresa/Small Business,
3(1), 11–35.
Liñán, F., & Chen, Y.-W. (2009). Development and cross-cultural application of a specific instrument to
measure entrepreneurial intentions. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 33(3), 593–617.
Liñán, F., Nabi, G., & Krueger, N. (2013). British and Spanish entrepreneurial intentions: a comparative study.
Revista de Economía Mundial, 33, 73–103.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: exploring
the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 151–173.
Magson, N. R., Bodkin-Andrews, G. H., Craven, R. G., Nelson, G. F., & Yeung, A. S. (2013). Questioning
new directions in understanding student motivation: an investigation into the domain specificity of
motivational goals. The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 30(2), 171–190.
Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (1996). Assessing goodness of fit: is parsimony always desirable? Journal of
Experimental Education, 64(4), 364–390.
Marsh, H. W., & Hovecar, D. (1985). Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of self-concept:
first-and higher order factor models and their invariance across groups. Psychological Bulletin, 97(3),
562–582.
Marsh, H. W., Roche, L. A., Pajares, F., & Miller, D. (1997). Item-specific efficacy judgments in mathematical
problem solving: the downside of standing too close to trees in a forest. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 22(3), 363–377.
Martin, B. C. (2012). Entrepreneurship as a means of improving the social and economic conditions of
persons with disabilities. Unpublished PhD dissertation, McMaster University.
Masuda, T. (2006). The determinants of latent entrepreneurship in Japan. Small Business Economics, 26(3),
227–240.
Mather, R. D., & Romo, A. (2007). Automaticity and cognitive control in social behavior. Southlake:
Fountainhead.
McGee, J. E., Peterson, M., Mueller, S. L., & Sequeira, J. M. (2009). Entrepreneurial self-efficacy: refining the
measure. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 33(4), 965–988.
Moriano, J. A., Gorgievski, M., Laguna, M., Stephan, U., & Zarafshani, K. (2012). A cross-cultural approach
to understanding entrepreneurial intention. Journal of Career Development, 39(2), 162–185.
Müller, S. (2011). Increasing entrepreneurial intention: effective entrepreneurship course characteristics.
International Journal of Entrepreneurship and Small Business, 13(1), 55–74.
O’Connell, A. A., & McCoach, D. B. (2008). Multilevel modeling of educational data. Charlotte: IAP.
Pardo, C., & Ruiz-Tagley, J. (2011). The dynamic role of specific experience in the selection of self-
employment versus wage-employment. Working Paper, Chile: Mimeo, Centro de Microdatos,
Universidad de Chile.
J Knowl Econ
Pelling, E. L., & White, K. M. (2009). The theory of planned behavior applied to young people’s use of social
networking websites. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 12(6), 755–759.
Petkova, A. P. (2009). A theory of entrepreneurial learning from performance errors. International
Entrepreneurship Management Journal, 5(4), 345–367.
Pruett, Μ., Shinnar, R., Toney, Β., Llopis, F., & Fox, J. (2009). Explaining entrepreneurial intentions of
university students: a cross-cultural study. International Journal of Entrepreneurial Behaviour and
Research, 15(6), 571–594.
Reynolds, P. D. (1997). Who starts new firms? Preliminary explorations of firms-in-generation. Small Business
Economics, 9(5), 449–462.
Rhodes, R. E., & Courneya, K. S. (2003). Relationships between personality, an extended theory of planned
behaviour and exercise behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8(1), 19–36.
Robichaud, Y., McGraw, E., & Roger, A. (2001). Toward the development of a measurement instrument for
entrepreneurial motivation. Journal of Developmental Entrepreneurship, 6(2), 189–202.
Schlaegel, C., & Koenig, M. (2014). Determinants of entrepreneurial intent: a meta-analytic test and
integration of competing models. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 38(2), 291–332.
Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (2001). The construction of attitudes. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Intrapersonal processes: Blackwell handbook of social psychology (pp. 436–457). Oxford:
Blackwell.
Schwarz, E. J., Wdowiak, M. A., Almer-Jarz, D. A., & Breitenecker, R. J. (2009). The effects of attitudes and
perceived environment conditions on students’ entrepreneurial intent: an Austrian perspective. Education +
Training, 51(4), 272–291.
Shapero, A., & Sokol, L. (1982). The social dimensions of entrepreneurship. In C. A. Kent, D. L. Sexton, & K.
H. Vesper (Eds.), Encyclopedia of entrepreneurship (pp. 72–90). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention–behavior relations: a conceptual and empirical review. In W. Stroebe & M.
Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology Vol. 12 (pp. 1–36). Chichester: Wiley.
Souitaris, V., Zerbinati, S., & Al-Laham, A. (2007). Do entrepreneurship programmes raise entrepreneurial
intention of science and engineering students? The effect of learning, inspiration and resources. Journal of
Business Venturing, 22(4), 566–591.
Stam, E., Bosma, N., Van Witteloostuijn, A., De Jong, J., Bogaert, S., Edwards, N., & Jaspers, F. (2012).
Ambitious entrepreneurship. A review of the academic literature and new directions for public policy.
Report for the advisory council for science and technology policy (AWT) and the Flemish council for
science and innovation (VRWI). The Hague: Advisory Council for Science and Technology Policy
(AWT).
Sutton, S. (1998). Predicting and explaining intentions and behavior: how well are we doing? Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 28(15), 1317–1338.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York: HarperCollins.
Tegtmeier, S. (2012). Empirical implications for promoting students’ entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of
Enterprising Culture, 20(2), 151–169.
ten Klooster, P. M. (2008). Interpreting patient reported outcomes in rheumatology. PhD. dissertation,
University of Twente.
Thompson, E. K. (2009). Individual entrepreneurial intent: construct clarification and development of an
internationally reliable metric. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 33(3), 669–694.
Trafimow, D., & Sheeran, P. (1998). Some tests on the distinction between cognitive and affective beliefs.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34(4), 378–397.
Trafimow, D., Sheeran, P., Lombardo, B., Finlay, K. A., Brown, J., & Armitage, C. J. (2004). Affective and
cognitive control of persons and behaviours. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43(2), 207–224.
Truong, Y. (2009). An evaluation of the theory of planned behavior in consumer acceptance of online video
and television services. The Electronic Journal Information Systems Evaluation, 12(2), 177–186.
Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis.
Psychometrika, 38(1), 1–10.
van der Heijden, H. (2003). Factors influencing the usage of websites: the case of a generic portal in The
Netherlands. Information and Management, 40(6), 541–549.
van der Zwan, P., Thurik, A. R., & Grilo, I. (2010). The entrepreneurial ladder and its determinants. Applied
Economics, 42(17), 2183–2191.
van Gelderen, M., Thurik, A. R., & Bosma, N. (2005). Success and risk factors in the pre-start up phase. Small
Business Economics, 24(4), 365–380.
van Gelderen, M. W., Brand, M., van Praag, M., Bodewes, W., Poutsma, E., & van Gils, A. (2008). Explaining
entrepreneurial intentions by means of the theory of planned behaviour. Career Development
International, 13(6), 538–559.
J Knowl Econ
Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature:
suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research
Methods, 3(1), 4–69.
Verheul, I., Thurik, A. R., Grilo, I., & van der Zwan, P. (2012). Explaining preferences and actual involvement
in self-employment: gender and the entrepreneurial personality. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(2),
325–341.
Weston, R., & Gore, P. A. (2006). A brief guide to structural equation modeling. The Counseling Psychologist,
34(5), 719–751.
Wiklund, J., Davidsson, P., & Delmar, F. (2003). What do they think and feel about growth? An expectancy–
value approach to small business managers’ attitudes towards growth. Entrepreneurship: Theory and
Practice, 27(3), 247–270.
Xu, M. K., Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., Ho, I. T. F., Morin, A. J. S., & Abduljabbar, A. S. (2013). The internal/
external frame of reference of academic self-concept: extension to a foreign language and the role of
language of instructio. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 489–503.
Yan, Z. (2014). Predicting teachers’ intentions to implement school-based assessment using the theory of
planned behaviour. Educational Research and Evaluation: An International Journal on Theory and
Practice, 20(2), 83–97.
Zampetakis, L. A., Kafetsios, K., Bouranta, N., Dewett, T., & Moustakis, V. S. (2009). On the relationship
between emotional intelligence and entrepreneurial attitudes and intentions. International Journal of
Entrepreneurial Behaviour and Research, 15(6), 595–618.