Wittgenstein's Certainty Is Uncertain: Brain Scans of Cured Hydrocephalics Challenge Cherished Assumptions
Wittgenstein's Certainty Is Uncertain: Brain Scans of Cured Hydrocephalics Challenge Cherished Assumptions
DOI 10.1007/s13752-015-0219-x
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
123
D. R. Forsdyke
attendant declaring that one was not present, encapsulates Shocking enough. But now for what really rocked the
the dilemma now facing neuroscientists worldwide. There neuroscientists. Half of Lorber’s 60 cases were of above-
are a few people on this planet—for most intents and normal intelligence (as determined by standard IQ tests).
purposes just like you and me—whose ‘‘bonnets’’ (their The central scans in the figure—virtually indistinguishable
skulls), when ‘‘opened’’ (x-rayed), reveal, at best, only 5 % from the severely impaired case on the right—are repre-
of the expected amount of brain tissue! sentative of this group. And doubtless a candid camera
It doesn’t take a neuroscientist to appreciate the would have caught Lorber’s jaw dropping when, among
dilemma. The logic is simple. A neuroscientist may find them, he found a student who was ‘‘socially completely
abundant neural tissue in a thousand skulls of normal normal’’ and had a first class honors degree in mathematics
individuals, thus certifying the close correlation between (Lewin 1980, p. 1232):
presence of tissue and normality. But the entire house of
Instead of the normal 4.5 cm thickness of brain tissue
cards tumbles to the ground when individual one thousand
between the ventricles and the cortical surface, there
and one is normal, yet has no tissue! Admittedly, in prac-
was just a thin layer of mantle measuring a millimeter
tice the distinction is not so stark. Individual one thousand
or so. The cranium is filled mainly with cerebrospinal
and one is found to have approximately 5 % of the brain
fluid. … I can’t say whether the mathematics student
tissue volume that is characteristic of each of the one
has a brain weighing 50 or 150 g, but it’s clear that it
thousand others. But we suspect that Ludwig would be no
is nowhere near the normal 1.5 kg.
less surprised by 5 %, than by 0 %.
Of course, when Lorber presented his findings at a con-
ference in 1980—under the title ‘‘Is Your Brain Really
Recovered Hydrocephalics Necessary?’’—he met with much scepticism. Journalist
Roger Lewin related in Science how experts had pointed to
In the 1970s a British pediatrician, John Lorber, reexam- difficulties in the interpretation of brain scans, and had even
ined, in adult life, the brains of people who had been declared that ‘‘Lorber’s style is less scientific than it might
treated as children for hydrocephaly, or ‘‘water on the be.’’ To the accusation that he was being ‘‘overdramatic,’’
brain’’ (Lewin 1980). In these children the circulation of Lorber replied, ‘‘Of course these results are dramatic, but
fluid in the ventricles of the brain is blocked. Yet fluid they’re not overdramatic. One would not make the claim if
continues to enter the brain, so the ventricles expand and one did not have the evidence’’ (Lewin 1980, p. 1232).
brain volume increases. In early life the skull bones allow But the ripples cast on the neurobiological pond soon
this expansion, and an enlarged head may be the first sign faded. Lorber disappeared from view. He died in 1996 at
that all is not well. By inserting a small tube (shunt), sur- the age of 82. Nevertheless, his results were considered
geons are able to drain off the excessive fluid, so relieving dramatic enough to warrant a TV documentary. Barry
the pressure. If successful, the ventricular expansion is Beyerstein of the Brain Behavior Laboratory at Simon
reversed and head size returns to normal. When the oper- Fraser University quipped (Beyerstein 1999, p. 17):
ation is carried out early, before the skull bones begin to
The telecast employs the ever-popular theme of a
fuse, a child can expect a normal life.
brave outsider struggling against a mulish establish-
Taking advantage of new brain-scanning technology,
ment to suggest that, once again, the so-called ‘ex-
Lorber anticipated that the adult brains of treated hydro-
perts’ aren’t as bright as they think they are. Along
cephalics would appear normal. For an example of a nor-
the way, the program encourages the misapprehen-
mal brain, see the scans at the left in Fig. 1 (de Oliveira
sion that there is a huge reserve of unnecessary brain
et al. 2012). Here brain tissue, encased within the outer
mass that can be casually dispensed with.
skull bones (white), is seen as the usual ‘‘gray matter’’
(gray) and ‘‘white matter’’ (pale gray), and there are small And in the ‘‘Ask the Experts’’ section of Scientific
black central areas (labeled ‘‘LV’’). These are the fluid- American, he attacked what he called ‘‘the 10 % myth,’’
containing ventricles. On the far right are the correspond- which implied that ‘‘90 % of the average brain lies
ing scans of a hydrocephalic whose surgery had failed and perpetually fallow’’ (Beyerstein 2004).
who remained severely impaired neurologically. Almost
the entire skull is filled with ventricular fluid (black) and
there remains only a small surrounding rim of actual brain Lorber Confirmed
tissue. Given the extent of tissue loss, it is astonishing that
he is alive. Yet 60 of the 600 cases Lorber studied dis- Nearly three decades after Lorber’s initial claim, no
played such extreme brain scans. Ventricular fluid occu- respectable neuroscientist had risen to his defense and
pied at least 95 % of cranial capacity! Lorber was still an easy target. In January 2007 the
123
Wittgenstein’s Certainty is Uncertain
Fig. 1 Brain scans. Normal adult appearance (left). Enlarged ventricles (middle and right). Reproduced under Creative Commons License from
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience (Forsdyke 2014)
anthropologist John Hawks—with dual interests in Sher- obviously incredible’’ and that Lorber had really been on a
lock Holmes and human skulls—set out to dissect Lorber’s well-intentioned publicity-binge ‘‘to show that hydro-
claim ‘‘part by part’’ (Hawks 2007). In A Study in Scarlet cephalus is a condition that can be successfully overcome’’
the great detective had opined that a brain should scale (Hawks 2007).
with the amount of information it contained (i.e., memory), A few months later, however, there appeared in The
and could easily become overloaded (Doyle 1887, Lancet the first of two independent confirmations sug-
Chap. 2): gesting that Lorber should not have been so lightly dis-
missed. Under the title, ‘‘Brain of a White-Collar Worker,’’
I consider that a man’s brain originally is like a little
French neurologists reported a ‘‘massive ventricular
empty attic, and you have to stock it with such fur-
enlargement’’ (like the central subject in the above figure)
niture as you choose. A fool takes in all the lumber of
in the brain scan of a civil servant who was married with
every sort that he comes across, so that the knowl-
two children and had come to them with relatively mild
edge which might be useful to him gets crowded out,
neurological symptoms that responded to treatment
or at best is jumbled up with a lot of other things, so
(Feuillet et al. 2007). And Brazilian neurosurgeons later
that he has a difficulty in laying his hands upon it.
reported a similar case (figured centrally in Fig. 1; de
Now the skillful workman is very careful indeed as to
Oliveira et al. 2012).
what he takes into his brain-attic. He will have
nothing but the tools which may help him in doing his
work, but of these he has a large assortment, and all
Female Brains Don’t Scale
in the most perfect order. It is a mistake to think that
that little room has elastic walls and can distend to
For me, these hydrocephalic cases provided welcome
any extent. Depend upon it, there comes a time when
support for the argument that the size of a human brain
for every addition of knowledge you forget some-
scales neither with its information content—specifically,
thing that you knew before. It is of the highest
with what the experts call ‘‘long-term memory’’—nor with
importance, therefore, not to have useless facts
intelligence. When gathering material for a biography of
elbowing out the useful ones.
the neurophysiologist George Romanes, who, with Charles
Thus, Hawks was highly sceptical of Lorber, concluding Darwin, was a founder of the science of evolutionary
that the story of the mathematics student was ‘‘quite psychology, I had been entertained by an article—‘‘Mental
123
D. R. Forsdyke
Differences between Men and Women.’’ Here ‘‘the missing Again, presumed information-content does not match head
five ounces’’ was of much concern: size.
These arguments were supported, albeit indirectly, by
Seeing that the average brain-weight of women is
some frank admissions of ignorance by neuroscientists at
about five ounces less than that of men, on merely
New York’s Columbia University. Nobelist Eric Kandel
anatomical grounds we should be prepared to expect
(2006, p. 423) declared that ‘‘in the study of memory
a marked inferiority of intellectual power in the for-
storage, we are now at the foothills of a great mountain
mer. … The disabilities under which women have
range. … To cross the threshold from where we are to
laboured with regard to education, social opinion, and
where we want to be, major conceptual shifts must take
so forth, have certainly not been sufficient to explain
place.’’ Fusi and Abbott (2007) called for ‘‘radical modi-
this. (Romanes 1887, pp. 654–656)
fication of the standard model of memory storage,’’ and
Apologizing for the ‘‘almost brutal frankness’’ of his Firestein (2012) echoed this in his book—Ignorance. How
remarks, there followed a litany of what seemed to be the It Drives Science. Furthermore, our brain’s storage capac-
self-evident intellectual deficiencies of women, including ity for visual detail is now seen as enormously greater than
in the ‘‘power of amassing knowledge.’’ But when previously estimated. This led researchers at MIT (Brady
Romanes compared men and women in a reading test: et al. 2008) to challenge the standard ‘‘neural models of
memory storage and retrieval.’’
The palm was usually carried off by the ladies.
Moreover, besides being able to read quicker, they
were better able to remember what they had just
Why Doesn’t Size Matter?
read—that is to give a better account of the paragraph
as a whole. One lady, for example, could read exactly
Given the doubts of these neuroscientists and the growing
four times as fast as her husband, and could then give
appreciation that brain size does not scale with information
a better account. (1887, p. 657)
quantity, it would seem timely to look anew at possible
Shortly thereafter a woman, Philippa Fawcett, beat the ways our brains might store their information. Broadly,
top man, known as the ‘‘Senior Wrangler,’’ in the Cam- three hypotheses exist:
bridge Mathematics Tripos. But alas, when it came to
1. Information relating to long-term memory is held
women’s ‘‘unnatural, and therefore impossible, rivalry with
within the brain in some chemical or physical form
men in the struggles of practical life,’’ Romanes, like most
consistent with current knowledge of brain chemistry
of his fellow Victorians, was not to be moved by such facts:
and physiology. This ‘‘standard model’’ has many
How long it may take the woman of the future to versions (e.g., Tsien 2013) that need not detain us.
recover the ground which has been lost … it is 2. Information relating to long-term memory is held
impossible to say; but we may predict with confi- within the brain in some extremely minute, subatomic,
dence that, even under the most favourable conditions form, as yet unknown to biochemists and physiolo-
as to culture, … it must take many centuries for gists. Those who have witnessed in recent decades the
heredity to produce the missing five ounces of the vast increase in the power of computers to store large
female brain. (1887, p. 666) quantities of information in progressively smaller
spaces should not be surprised if evidence for this
alternative eventually emerges.
3. Information relating to long-term memory is held
Savants and Microcephalics Don’t Scale
outside the brain. Since most nonneural tissues and
organs appear unsuited for this task, this extrapolates
Unaware of the hydrocephalic work, I used studies of rare
to long-term memory being outside the body—extra-
individuals with exceptional memory (savant syndrome),
corporeal! Amazingly, this startling alternative has
and with exceptionally small heads (microcephalics), to
been on the table for at least two decades. A George-
argue against the scaling of human head size with brain
town University professor of computing science has
information-content (Forsdyke 2009). With one prominent
sketched out how it might work (Berkovich 1993,
exception, those with savant syndrome tend to have nor-
2014). A 10th century Arabic philosopher-physician
mal-size heads. The presumed high information-content of
even had a version (Avicenna 1631).
their heads does not match their head size (Treffert 2010).
On the other hand, while most microcephalics are intel- Only with hypotheses two and three, which seem so
lectually impaired, there are a few cases where intelligence improbable, do we really confront the dilemma posed by
is normal; hence, long-term memory is likely to be normal. the brain scans of hydrocephalics. Yet some neuroscientists
123
Wittgenstein’s Certainty is Uncertain
123
D. R. Forsdyke
schizophrenics, and the bizarre condition described by the deem plasticity and redundancy explanations as more
American Psychiatric Association (manual of psychiatric plausible.
conditions; DSM-5) as ‘‘disassociative identity disorder’’
(otherwise ‘‘multiple personality syndrome’’)? And, of
course, when speaking of extracorporeal memory we enter
References
the domain of ‘‘mind’’ or ‘‘spirit,’’ with corresponding
metaphysical implications. In the words of Canadian Al Shargi H, Berkovich S (2009) Biological information processing
philosopher Ian Hacking (1995), we begin to ‘‘secularize as cloud computing. Applications of Digital Information and
the soul.’’ Indeed, ‘‘the scientific search for the soul’’ was Web Technologies, ICADIWT ‘09. Second International Con-
ference, pp 417–422. doi:10.1109/ICADIWT.2009.5273833
how Francis Crick (1995) described his own brain studies.
Avicenna (1631) Conimbricenses, In De Memoria et Reminiscentia.
Perhaps we should return to 1867 and harken to an Cited by William Hamilton. In: Mansel HL, Veitch J (eds)
exchange between two of Charles Darwin’s contempo- Lectures on metaphysics and logic, vol 2. Blackwood, Edin-
raries, Robert Chambers and Alfred Russel Wallace: ‘‘My burgh, p 209
Bateson P, Gluckman P (2011) Plasticity, robustness, development
idea is that the term ‘supernatural’ is a gross mistake. We
and evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
have only to enlarge our conceptions of the natural, and all Berkovich SY (1993) On the information processing capabilities of
will be alright’’ (Wallace 1905, pp. 285–286). We chuckle the brain: shifting the paradigm. Nanobiology 2:99–107
on learning how spiritualists duped such characters. Yet the Berkovich SY (2014) Formation of artificial and natural intelligence
in big data environment. In: Pino RE (ed) Network science and
possibility now emerges of at least some grains of truth
cybersecurity, Advances in Information Security, vol 55.
amidst the dross that we poor creatures, imprisoned within Springer, New York, p 189–203
the second decade of the 21st century, can understand no Beyerstein BL (1999) Whence cometh the myth that we only use ten
better than those imprisoned in the latter decades of the percent of our brains? In: Della Sala S (ed) Mind myths:
exploring everyday mysteries of the mind and brain. Wiley, New
19th could fathom ‘‘the missing five ounces’’ (Romanes
York, pp 3–24
1887; Forsdyke 2014, 2015). Amongst the early 20th Beyerstein BL (2004) Do we really use only 10 percent of our brains?
century beliefs of Wittgenstein (1969, p. 37e) was ‘‘that it Sci Am 290(6):116
isn’t possible to get to the moon; but there might be people Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA, Oliva A (2008) Visual long-term
memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proc
who believe that that is possible … they are wrong and we
Nat Acad Sci USA 105:14325–14329
know it.’’ It seems that yet another of his certainties must Clark A (2008) Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action and
now topple. cognitive extension. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Crick F (1995) The astonishing hypothesis. The scientific search for
the soul. Touchstone Books, London
de Oliveira MF, Pinto FCG, Nishikuni K et al (2012) Revisiting
Conclusion hydrocephalus as a model to study brain resilience. Front Hum
Neurosci 5:181. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00181
Three independent studies agree that there are, among us, Doyle AC (1887) A study in scarlet. Ward Lock, London. http://www.
gutenberg.org/files/244/244-h/222-h.htm#link2HCH0002.
people leading normal lives with approximately 5 % of the
Accessed 7 July 2015
quantity of brain tissue found in others. Even those without Doyle AC (1890) The sign of four. Spencer Blackett, London
expertise in the interpretation of brain scans can appreciate Feuillet L, Dufour H, Pelletier J (2007) Brain of a white-collar
the Brazilian team’s photo (Fig. 1). The French team’s worker. Lancet 370:362
Firestein S (2012) Ignorance. How it drives science. Oxford
photo (Feuillet et al. 2007) is no less spectacular, as is
University Press, New York
recognized by Bateson and Gluckman (2011) who give it Forsdyke DR (2009) Samuel Butler and human long-term memory. Is
as an example of brain plasticity, and by Majorek (2012) the cupboard bare? J Theor Biol 258:156–164
who also considers the recovery of hemispherectomized Forsdyke DR (2011) Memory: what is arranged and where? In:
Evolutionary bioinformatics. Springer, New York, pp 377–389
subjects (Fig. 2). Drawing on sources both literary (Doyle
Forsdyke DR (2014) Long-term memory: scaling of information to
1887, 1890) and historical (Romanes 1887; Wallace 1905), brain size. Front Hum Neurosci 8:397
the present article has explored some philosophical, neu- Forsdyke DR (2015) ‘‘A vehicle of symbols and nothing more.’’
roscientific, and clinical implications of Lorber’s now George Romanes, theory of mind, information, and Samuel
Butler. Hist Psychiatry 26(3):270–287
validated observations (Lewin 1980). Developing the the-
Fusi S, Abbott LF (2007) Limits on the memory storage capacity of
sis, made here and in earlier articles, that human brain size bounded synapses. Nat Neurosci 10:485–492
does not scale with its information content, it has been Hacking I (1995) Rewriting the soul. Multiple personality and the
argued that the scope of possible explanations should not sciences of memory. Princeton University Press, Princeton
Hawks J (2007) Ninety percent of your brain is (not) useless. John
exclude extracorporeal information storage. This thesis is
Hawks Weblog. http://johnhawks.net/weblog/reviews/brain/
defended against those who might exclude it because development/ten_percent_brain_myth_2007.html. Accessed 29
Lorber’s work is not credible (Hawks 2007), or who might April 2015
123
Wittgenstein’s Certainty is Uncertain
Kandel ER (2006) In search of memory. Norton, New York Treffert DA (2010) Islands of genius. Jessica Kingsley, London
Lewin R (1980) Is your brain really necessary? Science Tsien RY (2013) Very long-term memories may be stored in the
210:1232–1234 pattern of holes in the perineuronal net. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
Majorek MB (2012) Does the brain cause conscious experience? 110:12456–12461
J Conscious Stud 19(3–4):121–144 Wallace AR (1905) My life. A record of events and opinions, vol 2.
Noë A (2009) Out of our heads: why you are not your brain, and other Chapman and Hall, London, pp 285–286
lessons from the biology of consciousness. Farrar, Straus and Wittgenstein L (1922) Tractatus logico-philosophicus. Routledge and
Giroux, New York Kegan Paul, London
Pribram KH (1991) Brain and perception. Lawrence Erlbaum, Wittgenstein L (1969) On certainty. (Anscombe GEM, von Wright
Hillsdale, pp xxii–xxiv, 277–278 GH, eds; Paul D, Anscombe GEM, trans) Basil Blackwell,
Romanes GJ (1887) Mental differences between men and women. Oxford
Ninet Century 123:654–672
Rudolph TG (2008) The speed of instantly. Nature 454:831–832
Talbot M (1991) The holographic universe. Harper Collins, New
York
123