Devries 2015
Devries 2015
Abstract
When children are not ready to write, assessment of fine motor coordination may be indicated. The purpose of this
study was to evaluate which fine motor test, the Nine-Hole Peg Test (9-HPT) or the newly developed Timed Test of
In-Hand Manipulation (Timed-TIHM), correlates best with handwriting readiness as measured by the Writing Read-
iness Inventory Tool In Context-Task Performance (WRITIC-TP). From the 119 participating children, 43 were poor
performers. Convergent validity of the 9-HPT and Timed-TIHM with WRITIC-TP was determined, and test-retest re-
liability of the Timed-TIHM was examined in 59 children. The results showed that correlations of the 9-HPT and
Timed-TIHM with the WRITIC-TP were similar (rs = 0.40). The 9-HPT and the complex rotation subtask of the
Timed-TIHM had a low correlation with the WRITIC-TP in poor performers (rs = 0.30 and 0.32 respectively).
Test-retest reliability of the Timed-TIHM was significant (Intraclass Correlation Coefficient = 0.71). Neither of these
two fine motor tests is appeared superior. They both relate to different aspects of fine motor performance. One of
the limitations of the methodology was unequal numbers of children in subgroups. It is recommended that further
research is indicated to evaluate the relation between development of fine motor coordination and handwriting profi-
ciency, on the Timed-TIHM in different age groups. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Keywords
handwriting; fine motor coordination; Nine-Hole Peg Test, Timed Test of In-Hand Manipulation (Timed-TIHM); Writing Readiness Inventory
Tool in Context-Task Performance (WRITIC-TP)
*Correspondence
Liesbeth de Vries, Center for Rehabilitation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, CD 35, Dilgtweg 5 9751 NJ
Haren, The Netherlands.
†
Email: L.de.vries@umcg.nl
Published online 23 February 2015 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/oti.1385
Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 61
Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting de Vries et al.
and self-esteem (Bart et al., 2007). Early identification perceptual–motor skill of preliminary writing, “visual
of children who are not ready to learn the mastery of motor integration” and “fine motor coordination”
handwriting can provide information for tailored ad- are important performance components (Volman
vice and timely interventions in order to prevent the et al., 2006). Fine motor coordination (motor plan-
negative consequences of handwriting difficulties. ning and execution) has proven to be a key compo-
From the literature, we know that in the develop- nent in the early learning stages of handwriting
ment of handwriting, several processes are involved. (Berninger, 2009).
These processes are represented in a conceptual To assess handwriting readiness in the pre-
model, comprising factors related to handwriting writing phase, a new occupation-based instrument
readiness (Figure 1) (Berninger et al., 1992; Abbott has been developed: the Writing Readiness Inven-
and Berninger, 1993; Volman et al., 2006; van tory Tool in Context (WRITIC; van Hartingsveldt
Hartingsveldt et al., 2014a). Handwriting readiness is et al., 2014a, 2014c). The WRITIC has items in
the stage before handwriting (Marr et al., 2001; three domains and six subdomains: child (“interest”
Schneck and Amundson, 2010) and is defined as a de- and “sustained attention”), environment (“physical”
velopmental stage at which a child has the capacity to and “social”) and paper-and-pencil tasks (“task per-
profit satisfactorily from the instruction given in the formance” and “intensity of performance”). In a se-
teaching of handwriting (Marr et al., 2001). The con- ries of studies, the reliability and validity of the
ceptual model, based on the model of Berninger, is WRITIC have been established (van Hartingsveldt
used to identify the perceptual–motor and cognitive et al., 2014a, 2014c).
factors relating to handwriting readiness. This model In the WRITIC, handwriting readiness is measured
shows that learning “text writing” is based on differ- as having a proper seating posture (Pollock et al.,
ent processes: the perceptual–motor process “hand- 2009; Schneck and Amundson, 2010), a mature pencil
writing” and the cognitive language processes of grasp (Schwellnus et al., 2012, 2013) and performance
“spelling” and “composition” (Abbott and Berninger, of age-appropriate tasks such as colouring, writing pat-
1993). In the phase in which children learn the terns, writing own name and copying letters and
Figure 1. Conceptual model of handwriting readiness and its relation to handwriting, text writing and performance components, based on
the model of Berninger (Abbott and Berninger, 1993; Berninger et al., 1992; van Hartingsveldt et al., 2014a, 2014b; Volman et al., 2006)
62 Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
de Vries et al. Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting
numbers. The WRITIC discriminates between children 63.7% of the variance in scores on a handwriting test
who are ready for instruction in handwriting and (Cornhill and Case-Smith, 1996). Feder et al. (2005)
children who are not (van Hartingsveldt et al., 2014c). determined that IHM significantly correlated with
slow handwriting speed (r = 0.43; p < 0.01).
Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 63
Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting de Vries et al.
64 Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
de Vries et al. Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting
and the times an external surface was used to compen- for the subgroups with poor and good performance
sate were recorded as supplemental qualitative on paper-and-pencil tasks.
information. Correlation is interpreted according to Andresen
The Timed-TIHM was modified from the TIHM-R (2000): strong correlation rs > 0.60, moderate correla-
with approval of the test developers (Pont et al., tion rs = 0.30–0.60 and weak correlation rs < 0.30. A
2008) to allow for separate scores for the three ele- high correlation was expected between the Timed-
ments of IHM (finger-to-palm translation, palm-to- TIHM and the 9-HPT, because both are timed tests
finger translation and rotation), to consider compo- evaluating fine motor coordination. A moderate correla-
nents in the literature on hand skills (Exner, 2010) tion was expected between the Timed-TIHM and 9-HPT
and to use the timed scores of these three elements as with the WRITIC-TP. Test–re-test reliability was calcu-
an outcome measure: the Timed-TIHM. lated using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC).
Agreement of the ICC was interpreted using the classifi-
cation of Portney and Watkins (2008): 0.01–0.50 = poor,
Writing readiness inventory tool in context
0.50–0.75 = moderate and 0.75–1.0 = good. To process
The WRITIC is an occupation-based measurement the data, SPSS 19.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA)
to evaluate handwriting readiness in 5- and 6-year-old was used.
children. The WRITIC contains items in three do-
mains: child, environment and paper-and-pencil tasks
Results
(Figure 2). The WRITIC is administered in the class-
room, where the influence of the context can be taken Convergent validity study
into account. First, the child’s interests in paper-and- In total, 119 children were included, of which 60
pencil tasks are evaluated. After that, the child com- (50.4%) were boys. One child was excluded because
pletes a drawing booklet with five paper-and-pencil the parents did not give their written consent. In the
tasks (including tracing, colouring, making pre-writing group of poor writers (n = 43), there were 36 boys
patterns, name writing and copying letters and (83.7%), and in the group of good writers (n = 76),
numbers) while an assessor observes and scores there were 24 boys (31.6%). Because teachers selected
performance. more children who in their opinion had good perfor-
The subdomain “task performance”, used in this mance on paper-and-pencil tasks than children who
study, consists of seven items scored on a 3-point scale in their opinion had poor performance on paper-and-
and six items scored on a 7-point scale (range 0–50). pencil tasks, as they did not meet the selection criteria,
The other subdomains are criterion referenced and the groups of good writers and poor writers were not
provide valuable information for advice and interven- equally divided.
tion. The WRITIC-TP has high internal consistency af- The mean age of the total group was 70.4 months
ter factor analysis, discriminates between children with (70.1 months for the poor writers and 70.6 months
good and poor performance of paper-and-pencil tasks for the good writers). The majority of the children were
and has excellent test–re-test and inter-rater reliability right-handed (84% of the total group, 79.1% of the
(van Hartingsveldt et al., 2014a, 2014c). The poor writers and 86.8% of the good writers). Fifteen
WRITIC-TP, administered in kindergarten, is found percent of the children in total were left-handed
to be the main predictor for handwriting quality (20.9% of the poor writers and 11.8% of the good
(van Hartingsveldt et al., 2014b) evaluated in Grade 1 writers), and only one had a variable hand use (in the
by the Systematic Screening for Handwriting Difficulties group of good performers). Neither age nor handed-
(Smits-Engelsman et al., 2005). ness was significantly different between the two sub-
groups, although gender distribution was significantly
different between the two groups (p < 0.000).
Data analysis
The good writers differed significantly (p < 0.001)
Raw scores were used for the Timed-TIHM, 9-HPT from the poor writers on the WRITIC-TP, on the
and WRITIC-TP. Because the WRITIC scores at an or- Timed-TIHM and on the 9-HPT (Table 1) with the
dinal level, convergent validity was calculated using poor writers performing more poorly on each of the
Spearman’s rho correlation for the total group and measures.
Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 65
Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting de Vries et al.
Figure 2. Overview of the domains and subdomains of the Writing Readiness Inventory Tool in Context with the number of items, sort of
scale and range in each
For the total group, the correlations of the WRITIC- (Table 2), but did not show a significant correlation
TP with the Timed-TIHM and the 9-HPT were all sta- with either of the translation tasks of the Timed-TIHM.
tistically significant except the finger-to-palm transla-
tion task (p = 0.065). The correlations of the total
scores of the Timed-TIHM and 9-HPT with the scores Test–re-test study
of the WRITIC-TP were similar (rs = 0.40 and The study population consisted of 59 children with
rs = 0.40 respectively). 57.6% boys (n = 34); one child was excluded because
The correlation of the total scores of the Timed-TIHM of an incomplete dataset. The mean age was 66 months
with the scores of the 9-HPT was rs = 0.40 (p < 0.001). (range 59–81, standard deviation [SD] 4.6), the major-
For the poor writers, the WRITIC-TP had a signifi- ity was right-handed (88.1%), 8.5% was left-handed
cant moderate correlation with the complex rotation and two children had variable hand use (3.4%).
task of the Timed-TIHM and the 9-HPT, rs = 0.32 The ICC of the total scores of the Timed-TIHM was
(p = 0.042) and rs = 0.30 (p = 0.05), respectively moderate, r = 0.71, p = 0.001. The ICCs for the different
66 Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
de Vries et al. Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting
Table 1. Descriptive data of the outcome scores (mean, ranges) for the total group and for the subgroups of poor and good performers
SD = standard deviation; Timed-TIHM = Timed Test for In-hand Manipulation; 9-HPT = Nine-hole Peg Test; WRITIC-TP = Writing Readiness
Inventory Tool in Context – Task Performance.
a
Poor and good performers as rated by the teacher.
b
A high score corresponds to poor in-hand manipulation.
A high score corresponds to poor fine motor coordination.
c
d
A high score corresponds to good writing readiness.
Table 2. Correlations of the Timed-TIHM and 9-HPT with the WRITIC-TP for the total group and for the subgroups of poor and good
performers
WRITIC-TP total group (n = 119), WRITIC-TP good performersa (n = 76), WRITIC-TP poor performersa (n = 43),
Spearman r (p-value) Spearman r (p-value) Spearman r (p-value)
Timed-TIHM
Timed-TIHM = Timed Test for In-hand Manipulation; 9-HPT = Nine-Hole Peg Test; WRITIC-TP = Writing Readiness Inventory Tool in Context
– Task Performance.
a
Poor and good performers as rated by the teacher.
tasks were r = 0.53 (p < 0.001) for the finger-to-palm However, the Timed-TIHM did not show better corre-
translation task; r = 0.63 (p < 0.001) for the palm-to- lation with the WRITIC-TP in the total group or the
finger translation task; and r = 0.60 (p < 0.001) for the group of poor writers than the 9-HPT, in contrast to
complex rotation task. what we expected. Thus, our hypothesis that the
Timed-TIHM is more closely related to writing readi-
ness in children who are not ready for handwriting is
Discussion not supported. The complex rotation task of the
Our hypothesis that both fine motor coordination tests Timed-TIHM and the 9-HPT showed similar correla-
would have a moderate correlation with the perfor- tions with the WRITIC-TP in the total group and in
mance of paper-and-pencil tasks was supported. the poor-performance group.
Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 67
Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting de Vries et al.
In contrast with our hypothesis, the correlation be- than the poor performers. This is in line with the devel-
tween the Timed-TIHM and the 9-HPT was moderate. opment of IHM as described by Exner (2010). Manip-
This could be explained by the assumption that these ulation tasks with stabilization (the translation from
tests measure different aspects of fine motor coordina- finger to palm and from palm to finger in the Timed-
tion. The Timed-TIHM evaluates complex patterns, in- TIHM) are more difficult than those without stabiliza-
cluding IHM skills, and the 9-HPT evaluates simple tion (the complex rotation in the Timed-TIHM).
patterns of fine motor coordination (picking up, Therefore, translation from finger to palm is easier
placing and releasing pegs). This needs further than translation from palm to finger, which is in agree-
investigation. ment with the correlations. Until the age of 7 years,
Overall, the correlations of the Timed-TIHM and IHM skills develop progressively into more complex
the 9-HPT with the WRITIC-TP were moderate. This skills (Exner, 2010), and there is still a large variety in
is comparable with studies that show the correlation the skills that children master. The three different tasks
of fine motor coordination with the quality of hand- of the Timed-TIHM show a large variety in scores. This
writing (Feder et al., 2005; Volman et al., 2006). This might represent a wide range of scores in the perfor-
can be explained by the fact that fine motor coordina- mance of these IHM skills in these children. Large var-
tion is one of several factors that are involved in hand- iation in performance is an indication that these skills
writing (Figure 1). are not yet fully automatized, children are still
For the poor writers, the highest correlations were searching for the most efficient strategy and these
found between the 9-HPT and the WRITIC-TP and be- IHM skills are still in the developmental phase (Pehoski
tween the complex rotation subtask of the Timed- et al., 1997a, 1997b). Using the Timed-TIHM could
TIHM and the WRITIC-TP. This could mean that the thus possibly show how far children are in their devel-
9-HPT and this complex rotation subtask of the opment of IHM, marking their progress from master-
Timed-TIHM are most appropriate for evaluating fine ing complex rotation (without stabilization) to
motor coordination in this group and for discriminat- mastering translation from finger to palm and, finally,
ing between good and poor fine motor skills in this translation from palm to finger (with stabilization).
age group. The 9-HPT has the advantages that (1) nor- This is an advantage of the Timed-TIHM over the 9-
mative values are available for children between 5 and HPT.
10 years of age (Smith et al., 2000); (2) it is an interna- For the current study, we adapted the TIHM-R and
tionally well-known test; and (3) it is quicker and easier developed the Timed-TIHM. Changes were made in
to apply. order to improve sensitivity of scores and test–re-test
The Timed-TIHM consists of three different tasks reliability. The Timed-TIHM is now easier and quicker
that show different correlations with the WRITIC-TP. to assess, because only time scores are used as com-
These three different tasks of the Timed-TIHM require pared with the TIHM-R in which time scores and qual-
different finger–thumb movements and are of different ity scores are combined. The stability of the test scores
complexity. The complex rotation task shows the best of the Timed-TIHM is acceptable, which is shown in
correlation with the WRITIC-TP, and the palm-to- moderate test–re-test reliability for the total score as
finger translation task shows the poorest correlation well as for the three subtasks. This was not expected be-
and is more variable than the other tasks in the poor cause children are likely to use different performance
performers and also in the total group. The reason for strategies during the test and the re-test.
this could be that this task requires movements that In this study the “finger succession task” or “sequen-
are too complex and not (yet) well developed in this tial finger movements” task was not included.
age group. This is especially the case in the poor Berninger et al. (1992, cited in Berninger, 2009) deter-
writers, and therefore, children are using different and mined that sequential finger movements have a closer
varying strategies (Pehoski et al., 1997a, 1997b). On relationship to handwriting than other fine motor tasks
the other hand, the palm-to-finger translation task only do (r = 0.32). In this task, the child has to touch the
correlates significantly with the WRITIC-TP in the thumb with each finger in sequential order, starting at
good-performance group; the variance in scores is the little finger and moving to the index finger, as
smaller, which could mean that this group is more ma- quickly as possible. In Berninger’s interdisciplinary,
ture and uses a more consistent and efficient strategy programmatic line of research on writing over the past
68 Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
de Vries et al. Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting
25 years, the sequential finger movements task was a different skills that seem related to handwriting, so we
frequently used fine motor test (Berninger, 2009). Be- suggest the use of both tests.
cause the finger succession task falls outside the scope
of the definitions of fine motor coordination by Exner
Conflict of interest
(2010) and the ICF-CY (WHO, 2007), we did not in-
clude this in our research on fine motor tests. Possibly, The authors declare no conflict of interest.
this test has a significant correlation with the 9-HPT
and/or the Timed-TIHM. To investigate this, further Acknowledgements
research is needed.
We would like to thank the children who participated
The correlations that were found are specific for chil-
in the study, as well as their parents and kindergarten
dren aged from 5 to 6 years. Findings and conclusions
teachers. Thanks to Nicole Arink; Mariette Stroo and
might be different in other age groups, such as in chil-
Edith Thijssen (students in the School of Occupational
dren aged over 8 years who have already mastered the
Therapy, Hogeschool van Amsterdam); and Janine
skill of handwriting and are more stable in their fine
Grootendorst, Priscilla Hardus and Inge Hanssen
motor performance. Future studies are recommended
(students in the master’s programme in Paediatric Physi-
on the Timed-TIHM in different age groups to evaluate
cal Therapy, Avans Hogeschool) for their data collection.
the relation between development of fine motor coordi-
nation and handwriting proficiency.
A possible limitation of this study is that the group REFERENCES
of 43 children who were poor writers was smaller than
Abbott RD, Berninger VW (1993). Structural equation
the expected 60 children because teachers selected modeling of relationships among developmental skills
more children with good performance on paper-and- and writing skills in primary-grade and intermediate-
pencil tasks. For evaluating the psychomotor properties grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology 85(3):
of measurements, the group must contain at least 50 478–508. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.478
participants (Terwee et al., 2007), which was not the Bart O, Hajami D, Bar-Haim Y (2007). Predicting school
case in our study. There were more boys in the group adjustment from motor abilities in kindergarten. Infant
with poor performance on paper-and-pencil tasks. This and Child Development 16(6): 597–615. doi: 10.1002/
depicts the actual situation, as is also shown in several icd.514
studies: namely that the handwriting skill of girls ex- Berninger VW (2009). Highlights of programmatic, inter-
disciplinary research on writing. Learning Disabilities
ceeds that of boys (Berninger et al., 2008). However,
Research & Practice 24(2): 69–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-
the total group had an equal balance between boys
5826.2009.00281.x
and girls.
Berninger VW, Nielsen KH, Abbott RD, Wijsman E,
Raskind W (2008). Gender differences in severity of
writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psy-
Conclusion
chology 46(2): 151–172. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.007
The correlations of the 9-HPT and Timed-TIHM with Berninger VW, Yates C, Cartwright A, Rutberg J, Remy E,
the WRITIC-TP for the total group were similar. The Abbott R (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in
Timed-TIHM and the 9-HPT seem to measure differ- beginning writing. Reading and Writing 4(3): 257–280.
ent constructs of fine motor coordination. The doi: 10.1007/bf01027151
Timed-TIHM evaluates complex fine motor patterns, Cornhill H, Case-Smith J (1996). Factors that relate to
including IHM skills, and the 9-HPT evaluates simple good and poor handwriting. American Journal of Occu-
pational Therapy 50(9): 732–739. doi: 10.5014/
patterns of fine motor coordination. Both have their
ajot.50.9.732
advantages in the evaluation of fine motor coordina-
Exner CE (2010). Evaluation and interventions to develop
tion in children who are not ready for handwriting.
hand skills. In: Case-Smith J, O’Brien JC (eds). Occupa-
The 9-HPT is easier, quicker and more internationally tional Therapy for Children (6th ed., pp. 275–324).
known, and it has normative values for children aged Maryland Heights, Missouri: Mosby Elsevier.
from 5 to 10 years, whereas the Timed-TIHM provides Feder KP, Majnemer A, Bourbonnais D, Platt R, Blayney M,
information about the development of the IHM of the Synnes A (2005). Handwriting performance in preterm
child. However, both tests provide information about children compared with term peers at age 6 to 7 years.
Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 69
Fine Motor Coordination and Handwriting de Vries et al.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 47(3): (SOS): een hulpmiddel voor leerkrachten bij het
163–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01110.x signaleren van motorische schrijfproblemen van
Marr D, Windsor M-M, Cermak S (2001). Handwriting leerlingen in het Basis en Speciaal onderwijs. [System-
readiness: locatives and visuomotor skills in the kinder- atic screening of handwriting problems (SOS): an in-
garten year. Early Childhood Research & Practice 3(1): strument for teachers for screening of handwriting
1–6. problems of children in primary school and special ed-
Pehoski C, Henderson A, Tickle-Degnen L (1997a). In- ucation]. Kinderfysiotherapie 17: 16–21.
hand manipulation in young children: rotation of an Streiner DL, Norman G R (2008). Health Measurements
object in the fingers. American Journal of Occupational Scales: A Practical Guide to Their Development and
Therapy 51(7): 544–552. doi: 10.5014/ajot.51.7.544 Use (4th ed.). Oxford: University Press.
Pehoski C, Henderson A, Tickle-Degnen L (1997b). In- Terwee CB, Bot SD, de Boer MR, van der Windt DA,
hand manipulation in young children: translation Knol, DL, Dekker J, Bouter LM, de Vet HC (2007).
movements. American Journal of Occupational Ther- Quality criteria were proposed for measurement prop-
apy 51(9): 719–728. doi: 10.5014/ajot.51.9.719 erties of health status questionnaires. Journal of Clinical
Pollock N, Lockhart J, Blowes B, Semple K, Webster M, Epidemiology, 60(1), 34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.20
Farhat L, Jacobson J, Bradley J, Brunetti S (2009). 06.03.012
McMaster Handwriting Assessment Protocol. Hamilton, van Hartingsveldt MJ, Aarts PBM, de Groot IJM, Nijhuis-
Ontario: McMaster University. Van Der Sanden MWG (2011). Standardized tests of
Pont K, Wallen M, Bundy A (2009). Conceptualising a handwriting readiness: a systematic review of the lite-
modified system for classification of in-hand manipula- rature. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology
tion. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal 56(1): 53(6): 506–515. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03895.x
2–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2008.00774.x van Hartingsveldt MJ, Cup EHC, de Groot IJM, Nijhuis-
Pont K, Wallen M, Bundy A, Case-Smith J (2008). Reli- Van der Sanden MWG (2014a). The Writing Readi-
ability and validity of the Test of In-hand Manipulation ness Inventory Tool in Context (WRITIC), reliability
in children ages 5 to 6 years. American Journal of Occu- and convergent validity. Australian Occupational
pational Therapy 62(4): 384–392. doi: 10.5014/ Therapy Journal 61(2): 102–109. doi: 10.1111/1440-
ajot.62.4.384 1630.12082
Rosenblum S (2008). Development, reliability, and validity van Hartingsveldt MJ, Cup EHC, Hendriks JCM, de Vries
of the Handwriting Proficiency Screening Question- L, Groot IJMd, Nijhuis-Van Der Sanden MWG
naire (HPSQ). American Journal of Occupational Ther- (2014b). Predictive validity of a kindergarten assess-
apy 62(3): 298–307. doi: 10.5014/ajot.62.3.298 ment on handwriting readiness. Research in Develop-
Schneck CM, Amundson SJ (2010). Prewriting and hand- mental Disabilities, accepted.
writing skills. In: Case-Smith J, O’Brien J. C. (eds). Oc- van Hartingsveldt MJ, de Vries L, Cup EHC, de Groot
cupational Therapy for Children (6th ed, pp. 555–582). IJM, Nijhuis-Van Der Sanden MWG (2014c). The de-
Maryland Heights, Missouri: Mosby Elsevier. velopment of the Writing Readiness Inventory Tool in
Schwellnus H, Carnahan H, Kushki A, Polatajko H, Context (WRITIC). Physical & Occupational Therapy
Missiuna C, Chau T (2012). Effect of pencil grasp on in Pediatrics. doi: 10.3109/01942638.2014.899285
the speed and legibility of handwriting in children. Volman MJ, van Schendel BM, Jongmans MJ (2006).
American Journal of Occupational Therapy 66(6): Handwriting difficulties in primary school children: a
718–726. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2012.004515 search for underlying mechanisms. American Journal
Schwellnus H, Carnahan H, Kushki A, Polatajko H, of Occupational Therapy 60(4): 451–460. doi: 10.5014/
Missiuna C, Chau T (2013). Writing forces associated ajot.60.4.451
with four pencil grasp patterns in grade 4 children. WHO. (2007). International classification of function, dis-
American Journal of Occupational Therapy 67(2): ability and health (ICF): children and youth version.
218–227. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2013.005538 Retrieved 05-04-2014, from World Health Organization
Smith YA, Hong E, Presson C (2000). Normative and val- http://apps.who.int/classifications/icfbrowser/
idation studies of the Nine-hole Peg Test with children. Ziviani J, Wallen M (2006). The development of
Perceptual Motor Skills 90(3 Pt 1): 823–843. doi: graphomotor skills. In: Henderson A, Pehoski C (eds).
10.2466/pms.2000.90.3.823 Hand Function in the Child, Foundations for Remedia-
Smits-Engelsman BC, Stevens B, Vrenken I, van Hagen A tion (2nd ed., pp. 217–236). St Louis, Missouri: Mosby
(2005). Systematische Opsporing Schrijfproblemen Elsevier.
70 Occup. Ther. Int. 22 (2015) 61–70 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Copyright of Occupational Therapy International is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.