Gabrielle Collins 2015
Gabrielle Collins 2015
Gabrielle Collins 2015
Introduction
Pointless
tasks
are
viewed
as
such
because
the
process
neither
provides
something
to
gain
nor
aligns
with
an
individual’s
passions
or
interests.
“I
don’t
care”,
“This
is
boring”,
and
“When
am
I
actually
going
to
use
this?”
are
familiar
student
comments
to
teachers
who
try
to
inspire
students
to
do
their
school
work,
but
receive
the
same
responses
as
would
any
adult
enforcing
household
chores.
Education,
of
the
utmost
benefit
to
young
people
and
the
nation,
can
often
seem
to
students
as
an
obstruction
to
time
spent
doing
more
interesting
tasks.
Due
to
the
fact
that
such
disinterest
from
students
can
lead
to
off-‐task
and
disruptive
student
behaviors,
solutions
to
a
lack
of
motivation
for
academic
tasks
should
be
sought
from
research
on
student
motivation.
Cordova
(2006)
noted
that
existing
research
points
to
a
direct
relationship
between
grades
completed
in
school
and
a
lack
of
student
motivation
for
academics.
Modern
day
student
disinterest
in
learning
is
occasionally
attributed
to
perceived
bland
national
standards
and
ominous
high
stakes
testing.
Teachers
cognizant
of
this
issue
sometimes
attempt
to
motivate
students
academically
with
reward
systems
only
to
receive
variant
success.
Unfortunately,
due
to
the
current
day
assessments
and
objective
learning
measures,
extrinsic
incentives
are
proving
futile
for
encouraging
positive
attitudes
towards
education
(Bowman,
2007).
Teachers
who
wish
to
ensure
a
functioning
classroom
are
urged
to
encourage
students
to
locate
their
own
interests
and
how
concepts
can
be
learned
using
those
favored
or
stronger
skills
(Bowman,
2007).
Students’
interests
and
passions,
once
discovered,
provide
opportunities
that
teachers
can
use
to
encourage
student
enjoyment
while
Theoretical Framework
Autonomy,
competence,
and
relatedness
are
the
three
psychological
needs
presented
in
self-‐determination
theory
that
sustain
intrinsic
motivation
and
thus
predict
academic
performance
(Ryan
&
Deci,
2000).
Components
of
competence
or
self-‐efficacy,
as
identified
in
social
cognitive
theory
by
Bandura
(1977),
are
also
related
to
enhanced
student
intrinsic
motivation
toward
academic
performance.
I
based
my
theoretical
framework
on
both
theories
to
guide
how
I
attempted
to
increase
intrinsic
motivation
of
students
in
my
intervention.
I
also
reviewed
research
on
inspirational
language
to
propose
that
positive
messages
enhance
affective
feelings
and
performance
of
individuals,
in
the
hopes
that
intrinsic
motivation
would
flourish
through
avenues
described
in
my
theoretical
framework.
Literature Review
Autonomy
Recent
literature
as
analyzed
by
Niemiec
&
Ryan
(2009)
supports
the
theoretical
notion
that
intrinsic
motivation
occurs
when
students
are
allowed
autonomy
in
the
classroom.
Specifically,
teachers
who
supported
their
students’
autonomy
both
satisfy
and
stay
conscious
of
psychological
necessities
in
the
classroom,
most
commonly
by
providing
students
choice
(Cordova
&
Lepper,
1996;
Reeve
et
al.,
2004;
Urdan
&
Schoenfelder,
2006).
Students
benefit
from
this
autonomous
support
even
when
they
undergo
transitions
such
as
adolescence.
Soenens
and
Vansteenkiste
(2005)
administered
surveys
to
328
Belgian
adolescent
students
to
analyze
the
relationship
between
parent
autonomy
support,
teacher
autonomy
support,
and
student
self-‐determination
related
to
domains
such
as
academics.
Academic
self-‐determination
was
related
to
teacher
autonomy
support,
as
well
as
positively
associated
with
students’
GPAs.
Moreover,
teachers
who
supported
autonomy
were
predictors
of
autonomously
driven
self-‐determination
when
students
are
job
searching
after
high
school
(Soenens
&
Vansteenkiste,
2005).
This
suggests
that
teachers’
Vicarious
Experience.
Inspirational
quotes
can
be
influential
due
to
the
success
of
the
author
and
aligns
with
vicarious
experiences
in
SCT.
To
expose
students
to
more
vicarious
experiences
with
a
variety
of
successful
role
models
would
boost
student
self-‐
efficacy
or
competence.
Students
can
be
motivated
to
work
hard
and
take
pride
in
their
work
when
they
observe
others
achieve
and
perform
academic
tasks
(Phan
&
Bing,
2014).
In
the
elementary
school
setting,
students
are
unsure
about
their
academic
performance
as
they
have
little
experience.
Elementary
students
are
impressionable
and
are
likely
to
learn
from
modeling,
which
they
may
receive
from
specific
references
to
educational
strategies
in
inspirational
quotes
(Pajares,
2007).
Social
Persuasion.
Inspirational
quotes
also
send
positive
messages
that
individuals
utilize
to
achieve
success
which
can
be
considered
social
persuasions
of
SCT.
The
presence
of
inspirational
quotes
on
education
could
provide
students
with
positive
messages
about
struggle
and
perseverance
in
education
which
would
also
enhance
student
competence.
Froiland
et
al.
(2012)
recognized
that
praise
can
communicate
positive
messages
about
competence
to
students,
and
perhaps
quotes
in
the
classroom
can
also
send
empowering
messages.
Relatedness
Davidson,
Gest,
and
Welsh
(2010)
performed
a
relatedness
longitudinal
study
in
a
rural
community
of
the
United
States
on
383
students
in
either
third,
fourth,
and
fifth
grade
until
all
students
reached
seventh
grade.
Teacher
and
peer
relatedness
were
reported
by
teachers
and
peers
respectively
during
students’
time
in
elementary
and
middle
school,
to
investigate
the
impact
of
relatedness
on
adjusting
to
school
transitions.
Students
who
reported
high
relatedness
to
both
teachers
and
peers
post-‐transition
to
middle
school
were
strongly
correlated
with
reports
of
prosocial
behavior
in
elementary
school
(Davidson,
Gest,
&
Welsh,
2010).
In
contrast,
reports
of
less-‐social
student
behavior
in
elementary
school
were
associated
with
lower
relatedness
to
teachers
and
peers
in
middle
school.
Aggression
in
elementary
school
was
strongly
correlated
with
the
lower
relatedness
as
opposed
to
higher
relatedness
category
in
middle
school.
Similarly
important
was
the
fact
that
students
with
high
relatedness
to
teachers
and
peers
had
higher
self-‐worth
and
were
at
less
risk
for
academic
maladjustment
to
middle
school
than
those
with
lower
relatedness
(Davidson,
Gest,
&
Welsh,
2010).
This
follows
the
literature
that
supported
intrinsic
motivation
as
a
better
predictor
of
high
academic
performance
than
extrinsic
motivation.
Students
who
felt
a
lack
of
relatedness
to
their
teachers
tend
to
perform
academic
tasks
only
if
they
were
extrinsically
motivated
(Niemiec
&
Ryan,
2009).
This
is
because
students
internally
desired
belonging,
acceptance,
and
a
claim
to
be
valued
in
their
educational
experiences
(Bowman,
2007).
The
relevance
of
classroom
activities
also
helps
students
feel
related
to
their
tasks.
Cordova
and
Lepper
(1996)
conducted
a
study
with
72
fourth
and
fifth
graders
exposed
to
the
same
Methods
Intervention (for action research)
To
increase
autonomy,
competence,
and
intrinsic
motivation
in
the
classroom,
I
gave
students
the
chance
to
choose
what
quotes
on
education
to
display
in
the
classroom
that
had
positive
messages
towards
learning.
On
Monday
of
the
first
week
of
my
intervention,
I
asked
students
to
complete
an
anonymous
nine
item
affective
Likert-‐type
survey
(see
Appendix
B).
This
survey
served
as
the
pre-‐questionnaire,
with
three
statements
that
each
pertain
to
the
students’
feelings
of
autonomy,
competence,
and
intrinsic
motivation
in
the
classroom.
Students
then
had
to
rate,
on
a
scale
from
1-‐5
with
1
listed
as
“not
true
at
all”
to
5
listed
as
“very
true”,
how
correct
they
found
the
statements
to
be.
For
instance
a
statement
focused
on
autonomy
was
“My
teacher
listens
to
how
I
would
like
to
do
things”
whereas
a
competence
statement
was
“I
am
able
to
achieve
my
goals
in
class.”
After
collecting
the
questionnaires,
I
introduced
the
students
to
quotes
they
could
select
from
a
pool
that
encouraged
efforts
towards
academic
work,
recounted
vicarious
educational
experiences
and
came
from
a
variety
of
authors
with
different
ethnicities,
genders,
and
socioeconomic
statuses.
Due
to
Bandura’s
(1977)
description
of
vicarious
experiences,
Ryan
&
Deci’s
(2001)
definition
of
relatedness,
and
the
significance
of
role
model
similarity
in
the
Lockwood
&
Kunda’s
(1997)
study
it
was
important
to
provide
the
students
with
quotes
from
people
with
whom
they
identified
and
had
something
in
common.
Students
also
received
the
option
to
bring
in
inspirational
quotes
they
found
on
academic
work
as
long
as
the
quotes
either
(1)
represented
an
educational
experience
of
Table
1:
Research
questions
and
data
sources
Pre-‐Post
Affective
Quote
Response
Daily
Observation
Questionnaire
Journals
Journal
Will
student
choice
of
ü
ü
ü
what
quotes
are
displayed
in
the
classroom
increase
student
perception
of
autonomy
in
the
classroom?
Will
student
choice
of
ü
ü
ü
what
quotes
are
displayed
in
the
classroom
increase
student
perception
of
competence
in
the
classroom?
Will
student
choice
of
ü
ü
ü
what
quotes
are
displayed
in
the
classroom
increase
students’
intrinsic
motivation
towards
learning?
Data analysis
After
gathering
all
completed
pre
and
post
questionnaires,
I
scored
each
by
numerical
value
using
the
Likert-‐scale
method.
I
totaled
student
scores,
as
well
as
item
scores,
and
divided
student
scores
for
a
class
average
pretest
value
for
autonomy,
competence,
and
intrinsic
motivation
questions
respectively
on
the
survey.
I
performed
a
paired
two-‐tailed
t-‐test
of
the
class
averages
for
the
pre
and
post
questionnaires
for
each
affective
measure,
as
well
as
the
questionnaire
overall,
in
Excel
(2010).
Those
t-‐test
p-‐values
indicated
whether
a
significant
difference
occurred
in
the
average
class
rating
of
autonomy,
competence,
or
intrinsic
motivation
due
to
my
intervention
with
quotes.
I
collected
student
quote
response
journals
and
analyzed
all
for
any
specific
examples
of
statements
indicating
low
or
high
autonomy,
competence,
and
intrinsic
motivation
in
the
classroom.
Student
response
journals
provided
narratives
that
supported
the
existence
or
lack
of
significant
differences
in
any
of
the
class’
average
affective
statuses.
As
suggested
by
Creswell
(2012),
I
coded
the
journal
responses
to
look
for
examples
of
45
Student
Totals
(Likert
scale)
40
35
30
25
20
15
10
5
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
Students
Pre-‐IntervenBon
Post-‐IntervenBon
Figure
1.
Pre-‐
and
post-‐Likert
scale
questionnaire
totals
for
students
with
no
significant
difference
between
class
means
(p
>
0.05).
I
analyzed
the
average
student
scores
for
autonomy
and
competence,
as
well
as
intrinsic
motivation
statements
on
the
pre-‐
and
post-‐questionnaires
as
seen
in
Figure
2
below.
Interestingly
all
statements
related
to
those
three
affective
measures
averaged
between
a
3
and
4
on
the
Likert
scale.
Students
felt
that
it
was
anywhere
from
“somewhat
true”
to
“more
true
than
false”
that
they
were
autonomous,
competent,
and
intrinsically
motivated
in
the
classroom.
6
Mean
Student
RaBngs
(Likert
5
4
3
scale)
2
1
0
Autonomy
Competence
Intrinsic
MoBvaBon
AffecBve
Values
Pre-‐IntervenBon
Post-‐IntervenBon
Figure
2.
Mean
pre-‐
and
post-‐Likert
scale
questionnaire
values
for
autonomy,
competence,
and
intrinsic
motivation
with
no
signification
difference
(p
>
0.05).
Conclusion
Autonomy
and
competence
are
essential
needs
that
must
be
fulfilled
in
order
for
individuals
to
be
intrinsically
motivated
to
perform
a
task.
Student
choice
of
inspirational
quotes
on
education
in
the
classroom
did
not
make
students
feel
particularly
intrinsically
motivated
towards
learning,
and
neither
did
the
presence
of
the
inspirational
quotes
themselves.
Limitations
My
participants
were
25
fourth
graders,
ages
nine
and
ten
who,
based
on
previous
observation,
struggled
with
abstract
thinking
tasks
such
as
inference
or
paraphrasing.
It
is
possible
that
they
did
not
grasp
the
meaning
of
some
quotes
I
chose
for
the
pool,
and
therefore
neither
connected
with
nor
internalized
the
inspirational
messages
about
education.
They
were
also
a
small
sample
size
for
my
study,
which
affected
the
implications
I
can
state
moving
forward
from
this
research
project.
The
students
were
already
part
of
a
classroom
culture
with
an
extrinsic
motivation
system
through
the
reward
of
classroom
currency
for
participation
and
student
performance.
In
a
way,
students
may
have
felt
that
they
were
competent
or
had
choices
in
class
based
on
the
amount
of
money
they
earned
or
the
fact
that
they
could
spend
their
money
as
they
pleased.
Their
relationship
with
their
resident
teacher
for
the
year
or
their
families’
views
towards
education
may
have
fostered
the
students’
intrinsic
motivation
previously.
In
this
case,
the
students
may
have
had
high
existing
intrinsic
motivation
which
made
my
pre
and
post
questionnaire
data
source
ineffective.
Standardized
testing
occurred
during
the
time
in
which
the
quotes
were
displayed
and
before
the
students
took
the
post
questionnaire.
Despite
all
attempts
to
comfort
students
during
such
a
stressful
time,
the
students
may
have
internalized
negative
attitudes
during
testing
and
translated
those
feelings
to
their
overall
perspective
of
self.
Most
quotes
chosen
for
the
pool
were
authored
by
older,
accomplished
adults.
To
a
certain
extent,
the
students
might
not
have
internalized
the
quotes
in
such
a
way
as
they
would
have
were
the
author
their
age
or
also
currently
in
school.
For
example,
the
only
quote
I
provided
in
the
quote
pool
that
was
authored
by
someone
close
to
the
students’
age
was
Malala
Yousafzai.
The
students
may
have
found
it
difficult
to
relate
to
the
authors
of
the
quotes
and
benefit
from
others’
experiences,
yet
positive
experiences
of
other
peers
is
known
to
boost
the
confidence
of
students
towards
academic
tasks
(Phan
&
Bing,
2014).
References
Alivernini, F., & Lucidi, F. (2011). Relationship between social context, self-efficacy,
motivation, academic achievement, and intention to drop out of high school: A
longitudinal study. The Journal of Educational Research, 104(4), 241-252. doi:
10.1080/00220671003728062
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84(2), 191-215.
Bandura, A. (2004). Swimming against the mainstream: The early years from chilly tributary to
transformative mainstream. Behavior Research and Therapy, 42(1), 613-630.
doi:10.016/j.brat.2004.02.001.
Black, A. E. & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors’ autonomy support and students’
autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-determination theory
perspective. Science Education, 84, 740-756.
Bowman, R. F. Jr. (2007). How can students be motivated: A misplaced question? Heldref
Publications, 81(2), 81-86.
Bowman, R. F. Jr. (2011). Rethinking what motivates and inspires students. The Clearing House,
84, 264-269.
Brown, N. (2004). What makes a good education? The relevance of meta programs. Assessment
and Evaluation in Higher Education, 29(5), 515-533.
Cordova, D. I. & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning:
Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 88(4), 715-730.
Creswell, J. W. (2012). Analyzing and interpreting qualitative data. In Educational research:
planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research (4th ed., pp.
174-203). Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.
Davidson, A. J., Gest, S. D., & Welsh, J. A. (2010). Relatedness with teachers and peers during
early adolescence: An integrated variable-oriented and person-oriented approach. Journal
of School Psychology, 48, 483-510.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in
education: Reconsidered once again. Review of Educational Research, 71(1), 1-27. doi:
10.2307/3516064
Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and education:
The self-determination perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26(3-4), 325-346. doi:
10.1080/00461520.1991.9653137
Froiland, J., Oros, E., Smith, L., & Hirchert, T. (2012). Intrinsic motivation to learn: The nexus
between psychological health and academic success. Contemporary School Psychology:
Formerly "The California School Psychologist", 16(1), 91-100. doi:
10.1007/BF03340978
Gottfried, A. E. (1985). Academic intrinsic motivation in elementary and junior high school
students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(6), 631-645.
Lockwood, P. & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 91-103.
Niemiec, C. P. & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Autonomy, competence, and relatedness in the classroom:
Applying self-determination theory to educational practice. Theory and Research in
Education, 7(2), 133-144.
Appendix A
Quote Response Journal Prompt Date
1. Pick
a
quote
that
you
like
best
and
write
it
down.
2. What
does
it
mean
to
you
as
a
student?
Appendix B
How do YE feel about it, Matey? Pirate Opinions Survey
First things first, ye Captain demands ye be honest on all accounts. We’ll start ye off with a
pirate-specific question…please circle the answer that best says how ye feel…
I feel that our class hamster, Scully, has a good name for a pirate hamster!
1
2
3
4
5
Not
true
More
false
Somewhat
More
true
Very
at
all
than
true
true
than
false
true
Now
that
ye’ve
answered,
think
about
how
honest
ye
were.
Ye
answered
that
Scully
question
and
it
has
NOTHING
to
do
with
ye
grade
in
class!
The
same
is
true
of
the
questions
down
yonder…
so
be
mindful
that
ye
answer
with
the
same
honesty,
as
is
stated
in
pirate
code!
1. I
do
not
feel
that
my
teacher
provides
me
choices
and
options.
1
2
3
4
5
Not
true
More
false
Somewhat
More
true
Very
at
all
than
true
true
than
false
true
2. I
do
not
like
learning.
1
2
3
4
5
Not
true
More
false
Somewhat
More
true
Very
at
all
than
true
true
than
false
true
3. I
am
capable
of
learning
material
in
class.
1
2
3
4
5
Not
true
More
false
Somewhat
More
true
Very
at
all
than
true
true
than
false
true
4. My
teacher
listens
to
how
I
would
like
to
do
things.
1
2
3
4
5
Not
true
More
false
Somewhat
More
true
Very
at
all
than
true
true
than
false
true
5. I
am
able
to
achieve
my
goals
in
class.
1
2
3
4
5
Not
true
More
false
Somewhat
More
true
Very
at
all
than
true
true
than
false
true