We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 4
L2 Motivational Self System (L2MSS)
The importance of motivation in second language (L2) learning (L2 motivation) is
widely acknowledged to be as significant as other psychological aspects such as language aptitude (Gardner, 1985). The term ‘motivation’ has been commonly used in the educational research field. It has flourished within L2 studies, promoting numerous books and hundreds of articles dedicated to this subject since the 1960s (Dörnyei, 2005). ‘Motivation’ is defined in many different ways since different scholars propose different perspectives. In Gardner (1985)'s motivation theory, he introduced two types of motivation, one of which was integrative motivation. When learners have integrative motivation, they are interested in understanding and connecting with the people, culture, and lifestyle of the language-speaking community. In this term, ‘integrativeness’’ is introduced as a subcomponent. ‘Integrativeness’’ reflects the attraction of individuals to foreign languages, and their perspectives on L2 community (Gadner & MacIntyre, 1993a). Dörnyei (2005), however, emphasized the importance of rephrasing ‘integrativeness’ as the ideal L2 self, which did not contradict Gardner’s theory, since “there were many questions about its validity and relevance” (Dörnyei, 2005, pp. 95, 103). The reason is that English has become a lingua franca, meaning the global language. It is not only used to communicate with native speakers but also speakers from non-English speaking nations. Studies from Japan (McClelland, 2000; Yashima, 2000; Irie, 2003), Taiwan (Warden & Lin, 2000), and Indonesia (Lamb, 2004) reported that they avoided labeling 'ïntegrativeness' in their L2 motivation research conducted in their countries because it “failed to detect a motive that could be labeled as "integrative" in Gardner’s original sense.” (Dörnyei, 2005, pp. 96). Therefore, the theory needed extension to associate with different research contexts. In the study of Markus & Nurius (1986), the concept of ‘Possible Self Theory' was introduced in their psychological theory. In the context of L2 motivation, this theory suggests that individuals’ motivation to learn is influenced by how they imagine their future identities connected to that language. It analyzes the thoughts and feelings of individuals easier while they pursue a goal. Possible selves give shape and direction to our hopes and threats, in which the more vivid they are, the more motivationally effective they become. Although this theory is not directly focused on L2 motivation, it has broader implications for understanding motivation applied in second language learning. On the other hand, Higgin (1987)’s motivational theory mentioned the ideal self and ought self (Calvo, 2015). Whereas the ideal self refers to the qualities and characteristics individuals desire to possess, the ought self represents the attributes that ones believe they should own based on their sense of duty, obligations, or responsibilities (Dörnyei, 2005). With the need to address the limitations of existing motivation theories and to offer a more comprehensive perspective between motivation and self-concept, Dörnyei (2005) proposed L2 Motivational Self System. This theory is put together by the act of reinterpretation of ‘integrativeness’ ' (Gardner, 1979, 1985) and the integration of the Possible Self (Markus & Nurius, 1986) and The Ideal Self (Higgin, 1987). L2 Motivational Self System (L2MSS) (Dörnyei, 2005,2009) assumes that the gap between the learners’ actual self and their ideal self will motivate them to take action to idealize their aiming self. Based on the synthesis of Ushioda’s (2001) and Noels’ (2003) concepts, there are three proposed essential components of L2MSS: ideal L2 self, ought- to L2 self and L2 learning experience. Ideal L2 self, “referring to the L2-specific facet of one’s ideal self.” (Dörnyei, 2005, pp. 105). It represents the ideal image a learner would like to become in the future (Dörnyei, 2005). Ought-to L2 self, “referring to the attributes that one believes one ought to possess (i.e., various duties, obligations, or responsibilities, which control this attribute) to avoid possible negative outcomes.” (Dörnyei, 2005, pp. 105). It represents the standards and expectations that one should meet. L2 learning experience, concerning situation-specific motives as regards the immediate learning environment and experience. (Dörnyei, 2005, pp. 106). Situation-specific motives can be the curriculum, the second language teacher, the peer group, and the teaching materials, which can significantly impact one's motivated behavior (Papi, 2010). Discussion Several studies have been conducted on the relationship between L2MSS and intended learning effort since they both aim to offer practical insights to language learners, ultimately leading to more effective language learning experiences and outcomes. Among the three components of L2MSS, the ideal L2 self and L2 learning experience have consistently emerged as a strong indicators of learners’ intended learning effort. In contrast, the impact of the ought-to L2 self appears to be comparatively less influential to the intended learning effort. Since the ought-to L2 self is often associated with external factors such as family or societal expectations, learners may not develop a deep emotional connection to the language. Such misalignment between the ought-to L2 self and learners' values and interests can result in less effective goal pursuit. Very little research was conducted to investigate the relationship between L2MSS and writing achievement. Such limitation may result from the challenges when collecting and analyzing writing achievement data. Academic writing achievement data is quantitative, while motivational studies are more qualitative. Unlike quantitative approaches that depend on numerical measurements, qualitative observation aims to capture qualitative data, which refers to non-numerical information such as thoughts, emotions, perceptions, and social interactions. Therefore, researchers find it hard to measure learners’ L2MSS with academic writing achievement. In terms of the relationship between each element of L2MSS and writing achievement, the potential for a positive prediction of writing achievement lies within the concept of an ideal L2 self (Tahmouresi et al., 2021). Regarding the other two elements, there is not much research to evaluate their relationship with writing achievement. On the other hand, much research have evaluated the relationship between the ought-to L2 self and writing outcomes and provided that it had limited effects, primarily influencing writing learning behaviors (Csizér & Kormos, 2009). In a study by Jang and Lee (2019), they found that people who felt they should improve in a second language (ought-to L2 self) tended to use more revision strategies when writing. However, this did not necessarily lead to better writing results. L2 intended learning effort appears to have little impact on the writing achievement when a well-designed action plan (Bybee &Terry, 2006; Oyserman, 2008), Dörnyei, 2008),, which outlines detailed steps required for success, is provided. Additionally, when learners have clear goals, their ideal L2 self positively impacts their writing proficiency (Masgoret and Gardner,2003; Ushioda, 2009). Moreover, it is worth considering the impact of L2MSS on learners’ academic achievement is influenced by some factors such as teachers’ behaviors, the learning environment they create, and the goals they set. These conditions appear to shape the extent to which L2MSS contributes to learners’ academic achievement.