File 148404
File 148404
File 148404
VAs
MASTER THESIS
Tilburg, Netherlands
January 2019
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 2
Abstract
Prior research has identified the moderating impact of cultural dimensions on a user’s technology
acceptance. Contrastingly, this research attempts to address the question of how specific cultural
dimensions, collectivism and power distance correlate with the degree to which people accept Virtual
Assistants (VAs). This question is approached by developing three hypotheses based on constructs
derived from Hofstede’s cultural dimensions (Hofstede, 1980) and an extension of the Technology
Acceptance Model (Venkatesh & Davis, 1996; Venkatesh & Davis, 2003). To test these hypotheses, a
quantitative survey was administered to 159 Sri Lankan participants and 154 Dutch participants. The
main findings indicate that as predicted, collectivism and perceived usefulness of a VA are significantly
negatively correlated, however, this was only true of the Dutch sample. Furthermore, the hypothesis that
power distance has a negative relationship with behavioral intention to use a VA was not supported in
both samples. Finally, the hypothesis that the relationship between an individual's collectivist cultural
dimension traits and their intention to use a VA is moderated by normative beliefs was also not supported
in both samples. The research also identified several limitations with the survey instrument used and
propose adaptations that will contribute to improving the reliability of the subscales for future studies.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 3
Table of Contents
List of Tables
Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Constructs for Sri Lankan Respondents
Table 2: Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Constructs for Dutch Respondents
Table 3: Conditional and Interaction Effects Derived from the Multiple Regression for the Sri Lankan
Sample.
Table 4: Reduced Multiple Regression Model for the Sri Lankan Sample
Table 5: Conditional and Interaction Effects Derived from the Multiple Regression for the Dutch sample.
Table 6: Reduced Multiple Regression Model for the Dutch Sample
Table 7: Summary of Hypothesis Test Results
List of Figures
Figure 1: Amazon Alexa ("Echo Dot Smart speaker with Alexa", 2018)
Figure 2: Hofstede Insights culture comparison tool (Hofstede Insights, 2018)
Figure 3. Work Status of Sri Lankan and Dutch Participants.
Figure 4. Living Status of Sri Lankan and Dutch Participants.
Figure 5: The Best Fit Line between Collectivism and Perceived Usefulness of a VA within the Dutch
Sample.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 5
Virtual Assistants (VAs) are an accessible artificially intelligent innovation that mimic human
interaction through consistent training of algorithms and Natural Language Processing (Claessen,
Schmidt, & Heck, 2017). This technology is distributed to consumers as a pre-installed software on
speakers and mobile devices (Figure 1). VAs, like Amazon’s Alexa, comprise of a range of features,
including making appointments, responding to emails, having simple conversations with one or more
users, and managing home utilities. A VA’s ability to perform these functions and user engagement tasks
like a human assistant have resulted in a large increase in global consumer demand and controversy
around the product (Hoy, 2018; Koetsier, 2018; Smith, 2018).
Figure 1. Amazon Alexa ("Echo Dot Smart speaker with Alexa", 2018)
In order to narrow the scope of the research question, two countries, Sri Lanka and Netherlands,
are selected. This choice is based on the significant differences in cultural dimensions of the two countries
(Hofstede, 2001; Hofstede, Hofstede, & Minkov, 2010; Hofstede Insights, 2018) and the access to these
subject pools. Furthermore, of the six cultural dimensions introduced by Geert Hofstede shown in Figure
2, emphasis will be placed on the two dimensions that capture the greatest deviation between Sri Lanka
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 6
and Netherlands: Individualism and power distance (Hofstede, 2001; Hofstede, et al, 2010; Hofstede
Insights, 2018).
Here, individualism refers to the extent of how closely knit the social structure of a country is,
and power distance refers to the notion that society predominantly works on a strictly hierarchical
structure. To elaborate further, in a societal context, a person displaying collectivist cultural traits often
thinks of himself/herself as belonging to a group beyond their immediate family and is often loyal to this
group. Individualistic cultural traits represent the opposite end of this spectrum where individuals are
autonomous and focus only on themselves and their immediate family. Power distance represents the
extent to which society and its individuals value authority, a chain of command, and hierarchical structure
(Hofstede, 1980). In practice, this often refers to the extent to which you value and respect social or
organizational hierarchy. Thereby the research question is further specified as follows: How do cultural
dimensions (collectivism and power distance) correlate to the degree to which people accept VAs?
Most waves of consumer technology innovations such as social networks, e-learning, and mobile
internet banking have had extensive research conducted on user acceptance towards these technologies
through theoretical models such as the Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) (Alalwan, Dwivedi &
Rana, 2017; Rauniar, Rawski, Yang & Johnson, 2014; Yuan, Kanthawala & Peng, 2015). However,
research about the acceptance of AI technologies are limited to a few studies on recommender systems
(e.g., software responsible for recommending music on Spotify) (Oechslein, Fleischmann & Hess, 2014).
Furthermore, multiple cross-cultural studies applying Hofstede’s cultural dimensions have been
conducted to study the moderating relationship between acceptance of new technology (e.g., e-learning)
and culture (Tarhini, Hone & Liu, 2017; Dwivedi, Shareef, Simintiras, Lal, & Weerakkody, 2016).
However, there is a scarcity of quantitative research on possible correlations between cultural dimensions
and how users accept VAs. Therefore, this study will aim to contribute to scientific research by analyzing
the nature of the relationships that lie between cultural dimensions and technology acceptance of VA. In
addition, the paper will also aim to provide insight on the societal implications of varying levels of
technology acceptance and how AI tools such as VAs can be more effectively marketed and
communicated to potential global consumers.
Theoretical Framework
This theoretical framework consists of three key themes. First, it addresses technology acceptance
theories and discusses prominent theoretical models used to quantify consumer acceptance of new
technological innovations and their relevance to the study. Next, the framework addresses the impact of
cultural dimensions on technology acceptance. Finally, it presents the hypotheses for this research.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 8
2.1.1 TAM
Presented in 1989 by Davis, et al, TAM is a well-established technology acceptance model
(Alalwan, Dwivedi & Rana, 2017; Cai, Wohn, Mittal & Suresh Babu, 2018; Kessler & Martin, 2017,
Srite & Karahanna, 2006). Originally, the model was used to measure perceived usefulness and perceived
ease of use of information systems which are systems that analyze business information to help
companies make informed decisions. Here, perceived usefulness refers to the degree to which a user
believes that an information system improves his/her job performance, while perceived ease of use
measures how user-friendly a system is.
The finalized version of the original TAM theorized that perceived usefulness and perceived ease
of use of an information system influenced a user’s behavioral intention to use this system, and finally,
influenced the actual use of the system (Davis and Venkatesh, 1996). Furthermore, the model also places
significance on the role of external variables in influencing perceived usefulness and perceived ease of
use. Although this model does not provide explicit examples of what these external variables can be,
subsequent adaptations and extensions to TAM have identified a few possible variables.
For instance, adaptations of TAM have identified variables such as normative beliefs (also
referred to as subjective norms) to influence attitudes towards technology, making the model more
applicable to changing dynamics within social structures. Normative beliefs refer to the degree to which
an individual believes that people important to him/her would encourage the use of a technology (e.g.,
Believing that your sibling would think that you should use a VA). Srite & Karahanna postulated that the
addition of normative beliefs directly impacts a user’s behavioral intention to use a technology (2006).
The integration of normative beliefs as a variable to the model brings in an important social component to
TAM that reflects societal influence. Other identified variables include the introduction of hedonic
motivations to the TAM model, for instance when measuring technology acceptance towards live
streaming. Here, hedonic motivation refers to the element of enjoyment or entertainment present within
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 9
consumer technologies (Cai, et al., 2018). Therefore, even though TAM was originally intended to
measure user acceptance of information systems used within the workplace, case specific adaptations of
the model have extended their applicability outside of the workplace. Some examples of its applications
in consumer technologies are mobile banking (Alalwan, et al, 2017) and internet of things, a network of
devices and appliances that are interconnected (Kessler & Martin, 2017). This has not only increased the
model's robustness but has also strengthened its applicability in today’s growing consumer technology
market.
TAM is not without critique. The original model has shown limitations in its explanatory power
and has been criticized for not being able to holistically explain technology acceptance as it only
measures perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use (Lai, 2017; Straub & Burton-Jones, 2007).
However, this has been rectified by the extensions that have been developed specific to the type of
technology that is being measured, as explained in the latter part of section 2.1.1 (Alalwan et al., 2017;
Cai, et al., 2018; Kessler & Martin, 2017). This was taken into consideration when selecting a suitable
version of TAM for this study by using a model that captured normative beliefs.
Furthermore, even though the model has been used to quantify user technology acceptance
towards the internet of things, a technology that improves the functionality of VAs by connecting to
household appliances, TAM has still not been applied specifically to VAs. Additionally, the model has
not been used to explore user attitudes of any technology in Sri Lanka and The Netherlands specifically,
making it difficult to predict how constructs of the model will perform when applied to this technology
and cultures. Despite these limitations, this research aims to successfully contribute to these identified
gaps in research by using technology acceptance constructs derived from TAM.
Even though this model is accepted as a successor to the original TAM, it can be argued that
limiting the model to these identified external variables further restricts the areas in which the model can
be applied to. As such, simply extending the original TAM based on the technology and context measured
is sufficient and effective, as supported by recent studies (Alalwan et al., 2017; Cai et al., 2018; Kessler &
Martin, 2017). This further strengthens the reasoning behind the choice of using TAM1 based constructs
for this study.
conducted in Lebanon which examined the moderating role of cultural dimensions on e-learning tools
(Tarhini et al., 2017).
However, research has not explored the possible linear correlation cultural dimensions may have
with technology acceptance of VAs. It is possible that the results detailed in Muk & Chung (2015) as well
as Chou et al. (2015) are due to the direct influence of cultural dimensions. Interestingly, Sri Lanka and
Netherlands, show largely contrasting scores on individualism (Sri Lanka: 35 vs. Netherlands: 80) and
power distance (Sri Lanka: 80 vs. Netherlands: 38) (Hofstede, 2001; Hofstede, et al., 2010; Hofstede
Insights, 2018). This may have an impact on how technology is accepted in the two countries.
A VA’s ability to perform tasks like a human and eliminate the need for a human assistant or to
ask for help from another individual counteracts with inherent high power distance attitudes like
delegating tasks to another individual or group. This argument is supported in Yeniyurt and Townsend
(2003) where power distance was found to have a negative relationship with the acceptance of new
products in general. On this basis it could be argued that: H2: Power distance has a negative
relationship with the behavioral intention to use a VA. This hypothesis is based on the notion that
individuals who value hierarchy would rather transfer a task over to another individual than do it
themselves via an assistive technology (i.e., Recruiting a travel agency to book flight tickets on your
behalf).
Finally, attention is drawn to the moderating effect of the TAM construct, normative beliefs. A
few studies have been conducted on the relationship between collectivism and normative beliefs. Ra &
Cho (2018) found that normative beliefs are a significant moderator to smoking intentions among risk
taking women in Korea, predominantly described as a collectivist nation (Kim & Park, 2010). This begs
the question if the moderating effect of normative beliefs can be generalized to collectivism with regard to
the technology acceptance of VAs. Furthermore, as discussed before, recent TAM based models have
identified normative beliefs as a direct influencer to behavioral intention to use a technology when
moderated by collectivism (Srite and Karahanna, 2006; Tarhini, et al., 2015; Tarhini et al, 2017).
However, it is unclear if the same effect will be observed if the roles were reversed and the relationship
between collectivism and behavioral intention to use a VA was moderated by normative beliefs instead.
This effect would most likely impact those who are more collectivist than those who are individualistic
and do not place too much importance on what society thinks of them. Based on that reasoning, the
following hypothesis is proposed: H3: The relationship between an individual's collectivist cultural
dimension traits and their intention to use a VA is moderated by normative beliefs. Therefore, it can
be predicted that there is a significantly positive relationship between a person’s degree of collectivism
and behavioral intention to use a VA when moderated by normative beliefs.
Methodology
This chapter will lay out the methodology used to test the hypotheses presented in the preceding
section. It begins with laying out the process in which the survey instrument was developed and
administered to the sample; this also includes the characteristics of the sample. This chapter concludes
with information on the preparation of the data, as well as arguments for the selected approach to test the
hypotheses presented in the previous chapter. The measurement instrument can be found in Appendix B.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 13
Furthermore, Srite and Karahanna’s subscales measuring cultural dimensions were originally
derived from Hofstede (1980) and Dorfman and Howell (1988); the subscales measuring perceived ease
of use, perceived usefulness, and behavioral intention to use were originally derived from Davis (1989),
and lastly, the subscale measuring normative beliefs was developed by the authors themselves (Srite &
Karahanna, 2006). The adaptations to the original scale for the purpose of the current research ensured
that the survey addressed respondents who were students and/or employees, and also referred to their
perceptions towards VAs. This was done in order to make the scale relevant to VAs and the Sri Lankan
and Dutch demographic groups surveyed.
3. 2 Survey Distribution
The survey was created and distributed via Qualtrics, an online survey builder. As a control for
bias caused due to differing access to technology between respondents of the two countries, the sample
was restricted to those from a similar age group, educational background, and access to technology.
Furthermore, the survey was administered to respondents who do not presently own a VA, this decision
was made in order to capture the perceptions toward a VA instead of capturing retrospective user
experience.
All steps were taken to ensure full anonymity by configuring Qualtrics to refrain from collecting
IP addresses and any identifiable personal data. The survey was distributed using purposive and snowball
sampling methods, where the survey was only distributed to respondents who fit the profile described
above and were encouraged to share the survey link with friends fitting the same profile. In practice, this
was done using two primary methods: 1) distribution of survey links to respondents via Facebook and
WhatsApp with a request to share among friends; 2) in-person distribution of the surveys with a similar
request, where the participant completes the survey on the spot. Due to difficulties experienced when
distributing the surveys to Dutch respondents, two additional distribution methods were adopted for this
demographic, they are: 3) collaborating with two lecturers at Tilburg University to present a link to the
survey before four lecture sessions for consenting respondents to complete on the spot, and 4) collecting
email addresses from consenting respondents gathered at a class and sending the survey link via email for
them to complete and requesting them to share the link with their friends.
The distribution of surveys to the Sri Lankan respondents was delegated to associates who
distributed the survey according to methods 1 and 2. The survey distribution to Dutch participants was
conducted solely by the primary researcher by approaching students at Tilburg University, Talent Square
- a student housing complex, posting on Facebook groups, WhatsApp messaging, and collaborating with
lecturers as detailed above.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 15
Participation in the survey was completely voluntary and the respondents were notified of their
right to withdraw from the study at any time. Furthermore, no financial incentives or rewards were
provided to respondents for completing the survey.
Furthermore, as seen in Figure 3, the Sri Lankan sample included 30 employees who were recent
graduates, 80 students, and 49 students who were also employees, while the Dutch sample included 3
employees who were recent graduates, 93 students, and 58 students who were also employees.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 16
As seen in Figure 4, 129 Sri Lankan respondents lived with their parents, while 15 lived by
themselves, 13 lived with housemates, and 2 lived with their partners. In contrast, most of the Dutch
respondents lived with housemates (N = 74) while 36 lived by themselves, 30 lived with their parents, and
only 14 lived with their partners. In addition, on average, Sri Lankan respondents had a disposable
income of EUR 116 (SD = 186) while the Dutch respondents had a higher disposable income of EUR 372
(SD = 402).
Srite & Karahanna (2006), Tarhini, et al (2017), and Chou et al. (2015), a decision was made to remove
these missing responses in its entirety using the list-wise deletion technique. This method is also
supported by Bennett (2001) who states that list-wise deletion is acceptable in the instance that the
missing data accounts for less than 10% of the sample as is not considered large enough to cause a bias to
subsequent statistical analysis.
Next, all disposable income of Sri Lankan respondents was converted into Euros for
standardization. Then, mean scores were computed for the cultural dimension and technology acceptance
subscales thereby converting this data from ordinal to continuous data. Finally, the reliability of the scores
were measured using a Cronbach's alpha and descriptive statistics of the constructs measured through the
subscales were recorded.
Results
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Constructs for Sri Lankan Respondents
M SD Cronbach’s a
A notable discovery within Table 1 is that Sri Lankan respondents displayed a moderately low
power distance which is contrary to the assumption made in the theoretical framework which suggested
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 19
that a high power distance could be expected from this sample (Hofstede, 2001; Hofstede, et al., 2010;
Hofstede Insights, 2018).
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Constructs for Dutch Respondents
M SD Cronbach’s a
Table 2 is consistent with the expected outcomes of this sample with low levels of collectivism,
power distance and normative beliefs.
4.2.1 Hypothesis 1
To test H1: Collectivist cultural dimension traits have a negative relationship with perceived
usefulness of a VA, a one-tailed Pearson’s correlation analysis was performed taking into account the
large sample size and data type (Sri Lanka: N=159 and Dutch: N=154).
Sri Lanka: The average collectivism score was 4.17 (SD = 1.05), the average perceived
usefulness score given to a VA was 4.94 (SD = 1.24). The assumptions of normality for this sample were
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 20
met: The z-scores for collectivism were: skewness z-score= -1.04 and kurtosis z-score = 0.74, the z-scores
for perceived usefulness were: skewness z-score = -0.26 and kurtosis z-score = 1.22. The results of the
Pearson’s correlation analysis revealed a non-significant positive correlation between the two variables,
Pearson’s r (157) =.12, p = .062. Therefore, H1 is not supported within the Sri Lankan sample.
Netherlands: The average collectivism score for Dutch respondents was 3.69 (SD = 0.97),
showing more individualistic cultural dimension traits. The average perceived usefulness score given to a
VA by this sample was 4.06 (SD = 1.35). As the assumption of normality was not met within this sample
(kurtosis z-score for collectivism = -3.74), a bootstrapped Pearson’s correlation analysis was done. The
results suggest that a significant negative correlation between collectivism and perceived usefulness was
found, Pearson’s r (152) = -.16, 95% CI [-.32,-.01], p = .022. The 95% CI based on the percentile method
confirms the significance of the correlation. Therefore, H1 is supported within the Dutch sample.
Figure 5: The Best-Fit Line between Collectivism and Perceived Usefulness of a VA within the
Dutch Sample.
The best-fit line shown in Figure 5 suggests that as collectivism decreases (displaying higher
individualistic cultural dimension traits), the perceived usefulness of a VA increases. That is, people
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 21
displaying individualistic cultural dimension traits find VAs more useful than those displaying collectivist
cultural dimension traits, as predicted.
4.2.2. Hypothesis 2
A one-tailed Pearson’s correlation was performed to test H2: Power distance has a negative
relationship with behavioral intention to use a VA. This test was once again selected for the analysis after
taking the data type and the large size of the sample into consideration (Sri Lanka: N = 159 and Dutch: N
= 154).
Sri Lanka: An average power distance of 2.36 (SD = 0.91) was observed within the sample. The
average behavioral intention score was 4.30 (SD = 1.48). The assumptions of normality were met. The z-
scores for power distance were: skewness z-score= 0.26 and kurtosis z-score = 1.15, the z-scores for
behavioral intention to use a VA were: skewness z-score = -0.45 and kurtosis z-score = -0.85. Next, the
Pearson’s correlation test performed showed a positive correlation between the two variables. However,
this was not statistically significant, Pearson’s r (157) = .01, p = .458. Therefore, H2 is not supported
within the Sri Lankan sample.
Netherlands: The average power distance score was 2.53 (SD = 0.77). The average behavioral
intention to use a VA score was 3.45 (SD = 1.70). The assumptions of normality were met: The z-scores
for power distance were: skewness z-score = 1.35 and kurtosis z-score = -0.74, the z-scores for behavioral
intention to use a VA were: skewness z-score = 1.17 and kurtosis z-score = -0.32. Subsequently, the
Pearson’s correlation test showed a positive relationship between the two variables. This too was not
statistically significant, Pearson’s r (152) = .13, p = .056, therefore, H2 is also not supported for the Dutch
sample.
4.2.3 Hypothesis 3
In order to test H3: The relationship between an individual's collectivist cultural dimension traits
and their intention to use a VA is moderated by normative beliefs, a moderated multiple regression
analysis was performed using the tool PROCESS Model 1 (Hayes, 2012).
Sri Lanka: The average normative beliefs score observed for the sample was 3.91 (SD=1.24).
The model summary for the multiple regression analysis was as follows: F(3,155) = 11.92, p <.001, R2
= .19, suggesting that as a set, collectivism, normative beliefs, and the interaction between the two
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 22
account for 19% of the variance in behavioral intention to use a VA. Table 3 illustrates the conditional
and interaction effects individually.
Table 3
Conditional and Interaction Effects Derived from the Multiple Regression for the Sri Lankan Sample.
β SE t p
Collectivism .07 .10 .67 .501
Normative Beliefs .48 .09 5.53 <.001
Interaction .09 .07 1.20 .231
Interpreting Table 3 suggests that normative beliefs do not have a significant moderating effect on
the relationship between collectivism and behavioral intention to use a VA, β= .09, t(155)= 1.20, p
=.231. As such the results do not support H3. Furthermore, the addition of the interaction did not make a
significant change to the model as a whole, F(1,155) = 1.45, p =.231, with R2 change = .01, representing a
small sized effect. As previously mentioned, the moderating effect of normative beliefs on collectivism
and behavioral intention to use a VA is not statistically significant, thereby not supporting H3 for the Sri
Lankan sample.
As the interaction effect of the model was deemed non-significant a follow up analysis was
conducted by removing the interaction and conducting a multiple regression on the reduced model. The
diagnostics suggested that the residuals deviated from normal (normative beliefs skewness z-score = -
2.84, behavioral intention to use a VA skewness z-score=-2.21). As such, bootstrapping based on 1000
samples was done.
The model summary for the reduced model was as follows: F (2, 156) = 17. 11, p<.001, R2 = .18,
suggesting that this model accounts for 18% of the variance in behavioral intention to use a VA.
Table 4
Reduced Multiple Regression Model for the Sri Lankan Sample.
b β SE t p LLCI ULCI
Collectivism .10 .06 .11 .85 .435 -0.15 0.30
Normative Beliefs .50 .41 .12 5.66 .001 0.27 0.72
Note. LLCI= Lower level confidence interval, ULCI= Upper level confidence interval.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 23
As seen in Table 4, the bootstrapped coefficients showed consistent results. Collectivism did not
significantly predict behavioral intention to use a VA: b = .10, p = .396, 95% CI [-0.15, 0.30]. However,
normative beliefs were able to significantly predict behavioral intention to use a VA: b = .50, p = .001,
95% CI [0.27, 0.72]. Furthermore, the bootstrapped 95% confidence interval does not cross zero,
indicating that the model generalizes to the population.
Netherlands: The average normative beliefs score observed for the sample was 2.82 (SD = 1.15).
Similar to the Sri Lankan sample, a moderated multiple regression analysis was performed using the tool
PROCESS Model 1 (Hayes, 2013) to test if H3 is supported within the Dutch sample. The model
summary was as follows: F(3,150) = 9.62, p <.001, R2 = .16, suggesting that as a set, collectivism,
normative beliefs, and the interaction between the two account for about 16% of the variance in
behavioral intention to use a VA. Table 5 illustrates the conditional and interaction effects individually.
Table 5
Conditional and Interaction Effects Derived From the Multiple Regression for the Dutch Sample.
β SE t p
Collectivism -.23 .13 -1.74 .085
Normative Beliefs .60 .11 5.29 <.001
Interaction -.10 .11 -.80 .396
When interpreting Table 5, normative beliefs do not have a significant moderating effect on the
relationship between collectivism and behavioral intention to use a VA, β= -.10, t(150) = -.80, p =.396,
thereby not supporting H3. Furthermore, the addition of the interaction did not bring by a significant
change to the model as a whole, nor did it meaningfully contribute to its explanatory power,
F(1,150)= .62, p =.431, R2 change = .00.
As the interaction effect was non-significant, the interaction term was removed and a multiple
regression analysis was done on the reduced model as a follow up analysis. The diagnostics suggested
that the residuals were not normally distributed (behavioral intention to use a VA kurtosis z score = -
3.15), as such bootstrapping based on 1000 samples was done prior to continuing with the analysis. The
reduced model summary was as follows: F (2, 151) = 14.15, p<.001, R2= .16, suggesting that the reduced
model accounts for 16% of the variance in behavioral intention to use a VA within the Dutch sample.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 24
Table 6
Reduced Multiple Regression Model for the Dutch sample.
b β SE t p LLCI ULCI
Collectivism -.22 -.12 .14 -1.70 .096 -.51 .10
Note. LLCI= Lower level confidence interval, ULCI= Upper level confidence interval.
Furthermore, as seen in Table 6, collectivism did not significantly predict behavioral intention to
use a VA; b = -.22, β= -.12, p = .096, 95% CI [-.51, .10]. However, normative beliefs was able to
significantly predict behavioral intention to use a VA; b =.60, β= .40, p <.001, 95% CI [.35, .80].
Furthermore, the bootstrapped 95% confidence interval does not cross zero, indicating that the model
generalizes to the population.
Table 7
Summary of Hypothesis Test Results
H1: Collectivist cultural dimension traits have a negative Not supported Supported
relationship with perceived usefulness of a VA
H2: Power distance has a negative relationship with Not supported Not Supported
behavioral intention to use a VA
H3: The relationship between an individual's collectivist Not supported Not supported
cultural dimension traits and their intention to use a VA
This research set out to answer the question of how cultural dimensions, specifically collectivism
and power distance correlate to the degree to which people accept a VA. To answer this question, three
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 25
hypotheses were developed and tested. The following section will discuss the conclusions derived from
the hypothesis tests summarized in section 4.3.
Karahanna (2006) who in turn omitted four items to correct this before finalizing the instrument for
distribution. This may represent an indication that the subscale is weak in general and requires
reformation in its entirety.
Furthermore, the low mean scores for power distance contradict the findings of extensive research
conducted by Hofstede (Hofstede, 2001; Hofstede, et al., 2010; Hofstede Insights, 2018) and local
research (Dissanayake, 2015) which suggest that the country has a very high power distance. This
discrepancy is most likely due to the sample tested. The sample only consisted of students or recent
graduates who are not in a relative position of power when considering professors or managers. They
often feel disengaged and desire to be listened to by their superiors and this plight is reflected in the
politically charged protests occurring across the country (Haviland, 2018). These underlying emotions
may have played a role on how they scored this subscale; as a reflection of their desired reality as
opposed to their reality. Perhaps basing the subscale on a different context as opposed to their reality
could derive a more reliable response or statistically significant result (e.g., Pilots should make most
decisions without consulting their co-pilots). Alternatively, the rejection of the hypothesis may have also
been due to confounding or control variables that have not been accounted for in the research, the nature
of the relationship itself, or the sample size. Further analysis of the data using an improved measurement
instrument is essential to arrive as a more definite conclusion.
H3 proposing normative beliefs as a moderator for the relationship between collectivism and
behavioral intention to use a VA was rejected through statistical analysis. Thus, suggesting that
collectivism may not have a direct impact on behavioral intention to use a VA when moderated by
normative beliefs. The follow-up analysis done on the reduced model also suggested that collectivism was
not a predictor of behavioral intention to use a VA, but normative beliefs was. This suggests that the
intention to use non-essential technologies such as VAs vastly depends on the recommendations of
trusted individuals, regardless of their level of collectivism. This is interesting as conventional wisdom
would argue that normative beliefs are endogenous to collectivism, however, this is not reflected in either
one of the tested models for this sample. Nonetheless, the change in the model specifications did not have
a significant impact on exploratory power which suggests that further analysis including other variables is
necessary to gain a better understanding of the model and its performance.
It could also be argued that this outcome may also have been influenced by an error in the
measurement instrument. As pointed out in Srite and Karahanna (2006), the subscale for normative
beliefs included both one’s inner circle (i.e., family, friends, and classmates) and outer circle (i.e.,
professors and managers). However, it is unclear if Sri Lankans are equally influenced by their outer
circle as they are by their inner circle. It could be postulated that students may have an average high
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 27
normative belief score for their inner circle, but a lower score for their outer circle, thereby rendering the
measurement inaccurate.
Furthermore, given that the technology being tested is a non-essential tool (VA), it could be that
participants are unable to accurately decide if their inner or outer circle would want them to use this
technology. Realistically, it is likely that non-essential technologies such as VAs would not be an
encouraged purchase, especially in a lesser developed country like Sri Lanka (GDP per capita: USD
4350.00) (World Bank, 2019). This would depend on the disposable income of the respondents, which
have not been accounted for in this analysis.
model did not show a difference in exploratory power. However, the reduced model did confirm the
predictive power of normative beliefs on behavioral intention to use a VA within the sample. A similar
discussion to that of the Sri Lankan sample could be made in this instance. Perhaps young people place
greater trust on the opinions of those closer to them when purchasing non-essential technology tools such
as VAs. Again, the non-significance of the predictive power of collectivism is an interesting outcome, as
inherently, the value placed on the opinions of others is intertwined with one’s level of collectivism.
However, it is difficult to interpret why this may be without ensuring the measurement instrument is
operating optimally. As such, it is important to reiterate that the same drawbacks applied to the reliability
of the normative beliefs subscale for the Sri Lankan sample may also be applicable here. Therefore,
further research with a more robust measurement instrument is necessary to explore if these changes will
yield a significant moderating effect for the original model of this sample, or a significant predictive
power for collectivism in the reduced model.
5.2 Limitations
The objective of this study was to address the research question of how cultural dimensions such
as collectivism and power distance correlate to the degree to which people accept a VA. Based on the
conclusions made in sections 5.1.1 and 5.1.2, this question has been sufficiently addressed. However, this
research has numerous limitations. In addition to the drawback of the measurement instrument
extensively discussed in sections 5.1.1 and 5.1.2, a larger sample size could have strengthened the
reliability of the significance testing.
Furthermore, the survey was distributed at a time where respondents are burdened with exams
and filling out multiple surveys for ongoing research during the end of the semester. It may be that this
environment made respondents answer hastily to questions, thereby compromising quality and reliability
of the survey responses. It is advised that future researches provide incentive for respondents to accurately
fill in the survey distributed.
Additionally, another element to take into consideration is the video that the respondents based
their responses for the TAM constructs on. The two videos used were promotional content produced by
Amazon, as such it is created to attract customers. This may have impacted the responses on perceived
usefulness and perceived ease of use of VAs. It is beneficial for future studies using this methodological
approach to create their own video content that highlights both the features and drawbacks of VAs to
probe a more unbiased response from the participants.
Finally, in regards to generalizability, given the limitations of the measurement instrument it is
advisable to duplicate this research on an improved measurement instrument in order to more accurately
make these assumptions.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 29
In conclusion, the scientific and societal contributions of this research have provided interesting
insights into technology acceptance research and potential marketing methods for communicating VAs to
two demographics. Furthermore, the commentary made on the measurement instrument used to
operationalize the widely accepted cultural dimension theory and TAM act as a solid base upon which to
conduct further studies on the intersection between culture and technology acceptance. In addition, the
proposed scope for future research act as an indicator of the future direction of technology acceptance
research within AI. Thereby, this research will undoubtedly contribute to yielding interesting and
impactful insights into the future of VAs in a growing global consumer market.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 30
References
Abbasi, M. S., Tarhini, A., Elyas, T., & Shah, F. (2015). Impact of individualism and collectivism over
the individual’s technology acceptance behaviour: A multi-group analysis between Pakistan and
Turkey. Journal of Enterprise Information Management, 28(6), 747-768.
Amazon (2017, October 29) Amazon Echo Plus #AskAlexa [Video File]. Retrieved from
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9qkW75JsY3U&ab_channel=AmazonEchoIndia
Amazon (2015, June 23) Amazon Echo - Now Available [Video File]. Retrieved from
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=FQn6aFQwBQU&ab_channel=amazon
Bennett, D. A. (2001). How can I deal with missing data in my study? Australian & New Zealand Journal
of Public Health, 25(5), 464–469. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-842X.2001.tb00294.x
Besikci, E., Agnew, C. R., & Yildirim, A. (2016). It's my partner, deal with it: Rejection
sensitivity, normative beliefs, and commitment. Personal Relationships, 23(3), 384-395.
Davis, F. D., Bagozzi, R. P., & Warshaw, P. R. (1989). User acceptance of computer technology: a
comparison of two theoretical models. Management science, 35(8), 982-1003.
Deepananda, K. A., Amarasinghe, U. S., & Jayasinghe-Mudalige, U. K. (2016). Neither bust nor boom:
Institutional robustness in the beach seine fishery of southern Sri Lanka. Ocean & Coastal
Management, 128, 61-73.
Dissanayake, D. M. S. (2015). A comparative study of positions of collectivism in Japanese and
Sri Lankan societies.
Dorfman, P. W., & Howell, J. P. (1988). Dimensions of national culture and effective leadership patterns:
Hofstede revisited. Advances in international comparative management, 3(1), 127-150.
Echo Dot Smart speaker with Alexa. (2018). Retrieved from https://www.amazon.com/Amazon-Echo-
Dot-Portable-Bluetooth-Speaker-with-Alexa-Black/dp/B01DFKC2SO
Haviland, C. (2018). Sri Lanka protest students evicted. Retrieved from
https://www.bbc.com/news/world-asia-16469642
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis.
Hennekam, S., & Tahssain-Gay, L. (2015). Changing attitudes towards diversity: The Netherlands and
Morocco compared. Management Decision, 53(9), 2135-2155.
Hofstede Insights. (2018). Retrieved from https://www.hofstede-insights.com/
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and
organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind.
Revised and expanded third edition. New York.
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 31
Koetsier, J. (2018). Aug 2, 2018, 07:44 pm Amazon Echo, Google Home Installed Base Hits 50
Million; Apple Has 6% Market Share, Report Says. Retrieved from
https://www.forbes.com/sites/johnkoetsier/2018/08/02/amazon-echo-google-home-
installed-base-hits-50-million-apple-has-6-market-share-report-says/#15e1bbea769c
Lai, P. C. (2017). The literature review of technology adoption models and theories for the novelty
technology. JISTEM-Journal of Information Systems and Technology Management, 14(1), 21-38.
Mohammed, Z. A., & Tejay, G. P. (2017). Examining privacy concerns and ecommerce adoption in
developing countries: The impact of culture in shaping individuals’ perceptions toward
technology. Computers & Security, 67, 254–265.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cose.2017.03.001http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db
=bsh&AN=94947442&site=ehost-live
Muk, A., & Chung, C. (2015). Applying the technology acceptance model in a two-country study of SMS
advertising. Journal of Business Research, 68(1), 1-6.
Oechslein, O., Fleischmann, M., & Hess, T. (2014, January). An application of UTAUT2 on social
recommender systems: Incorporating social information for performance expectancy. In System
Sciences (HICSS), 2014 47th Hawaii International Conference on (pp. 3297-3306). IEEE.
Ra, J. S., & Cho, Y. H. (2018). Role of social normative beliefs as a moderating factor in smoking
intention among adolescent girls in Korea. Nursing & health sciences, 20(4), 530-536.
Rauniar, R., Rawski, G., Yang, J., & Johnson, B. (2014). Technology acceptance model (TAM) and social
media usage: an empirical study on Facebook. Journal of Enterprise Information Management,
27(1), 6-30.
Rogers, E. M. (1995). Lessons for guidelines from the diffusion of innovations. Joint Commission Journal
on Quality and Patient Safety, 21(7), 324-328.
Smith, C. (2018). Alexa and Siri Can Hear This Hidden Command. You Can’t. Retrieved from
https://www.nytimes.com/2018/05/10/technology/alexa-siri-hidden-command-audio-attacks.html
Srite, M., & Karahanna, E. (2006). The role of espoused national cultural values in technology
acceptance. MIS quarterly, 679-704.
Straub, D., & Burton-Jones, A. (2007). Veni, vidi, vici: Breaking the TAM logjam. Journal of the
Association for Information Systems, 8(4), 5.
Tarhini, A., Hone, K., & Liu, X. (2015). A cross‐ cultural examination of the impact of social,
organisational and individual factors on educational technology acceptance between British and
Lebanese university students. British Journal of Educational Technology, 46(4), 739-755.
Tarhini, A., Hone, K., Liu, X., & Tarhini, T. (2017). Examining the moderating effect of individual-level
cultural values on users’ acceptance of E-learning in developing countries: a structural equation
IMPACT OF CULTURAL DIMENSIONS ON TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE OF VAs 32
Appendix A
Videos shown during the survey:
Appendix B
Scale used for the survey:
Collectivism:
● Being accepted as a member of a group is more important than having autonomy and
independence.
● Being accepted as a member of a group is more important than being independent.
● Group success is more important than individual success.
● Being loyal to a group is more important than individual gain.
● Individual rewards are not as important as group welfare.
● It is more important for a professors/manager to encourage loyalty and a sense of duty in
students/subordinates than it is to encourage individual initiatives.
Power distance:
● Professors/Managers should make most decisions without consulting students/subordinates.
● Professors/managers should not ask students/subordinates for advice because they might appear
less powerful.
● Decision-making power should stay with senior professors/senior management in the
institution/organization and not be delegated to lower level students/employees.
● Students/employees should not question their professor's/manager's decisions.
Perceived usefulness:
● Using a Virtual Assistant would enhance my productivity.
● I find Virtual Assistants would be useful in my daily activities.
● Using Virtual Assistants would enhance the effectiveness of my daily life.
● Using Virtual Assistants would improve my daily performance.
Normative beliefs:
● My relatives would think I should use a Virtual Assistant.
● My friends would think I should use a Virtual Assistant.
● My professors/managers would think I should use a Virtual Assistant.
● I believe that my classmates/colleagues would think I should use a Virtual Assistant.