İlk 10
İlk 10
İlk 10
Definition of Learning
5.1 Define the term learning.
The term learning is one of those concepts whose meaning is crystal clear until one has
to put it in actual words. “Learning is when you learn something.” “Learning is learning
how to do something.” A more useful definition is as follows: Learning is any relatively
permanent change in behavior brought about by experience or practice.
The “relatively permanent” part of the definition refers to the fact that when peo-
ple learn anything, some part of their brain is physically changed to record what they’ve
learned (Farmer et al., 2013; Loftus & Loftus, 1980). This is actually a process of memory, for
without the ability to remember what happens, people cannot learn anything. Although
there is no conclusive proof as yet, research suggests that once people learn something,
it may be present somewhere in memory in physical form (Barsalou, 1992; Smolen et al.,
2006). They may be unable to “get” to it, but it’s there. to Learning Objective 6.5.
As for the inclusion of experience or practice in the definition of learning, think
about the last time you did something that caused you a lot of pain. Did you do it again?
Probably not. You didn’t want to experience that pain again, so you changed your behav-
ior to avoid the painful consequence.* This is how children learn not to touch hot stoves.
In contrast, if a person does something resulting in a very pleasurable experience, that
person is more likely to do that same thing again. This is another change in behavior and
is explained by the law of effect, a topic we will discuss later in the chapter.
Not all change is accomplished through learning. Changes like an increase in height
or the size of the brain are another kind of change, controlled by a genetic blueprint. This
kind of change is called maturation and is due to biology, not experience. For example,
practice alone will not allow a child to walk. Children learn to walk because their nervous
systems, muscle strength, and sense of balance have reached the point where walking
is physically possible for them—all factors controlled by maturation. Once that matura-
tional readiness has been reached, then practice and experience play their important part.
salivary glands automatically start releasing saliva to help with chewing and digestion.
This is a normal reflex—an unlearned, involuntary response that is not under personal
control or choice—one of many that occur in both animals and humans. The food causes
a particular reaction, the salivation. A stimulus can be defined as any object, event, or
experience that causes a response, the reaction of an organism. In the case of Pavlov’s
dogs, the food is the stimulus and salivation is the response.
Pavlov soon discovered that his dogs began salivating when they weren’t sup-
posed to be salivating. Some dogs would start salivating when they saw the lab assis-
Dr. Ivan Pavlov and students working in his
tant bringing their food, others when they heard the clatter of the food bowl from the laboratory. Pavlov, a Russian physiologist,
kitchen, and still others when it was the time of day they were usually fed. Switching his was the first to study and write about the
focus, Pavlov spent the rest of his career studying what eventually he termed classical basic principles of classical conditioning.
conditioning, learning to elicit* an involuntary, reflex-like response to a stimulus other
than the original, natural stimulus that normally produces the response.
Elements of Classical Conditioning Pavlov eventually identified several key
elements that must be present and experienced in a particular way for conditioning to
take place.
UNCONDITIONED STIMuLus The original, naturally occurring stimulus is called the
unconditioned stimulus (UCS). The term unconditioned means “unlearned.” This is the
stimulus that ordinarily leads to the involuntary response. In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, reflex
the food is the unconditioned stimulus. an involuntary response, one that is
not under personal control or choice.
UNCONDITIONED REsPONsE The automatic and involuntary response to the uncondi-
tioned stimulus is called the unconditioned response (UCR) for much the same reason. It
classical conditioning
is unlearned and occurs because of genetic “wiring” in the nervous system. For example,
learning to make an involuntary
in Pavlov’s experiment, the salivation to the food is the UCR (unconditioned response).
response to a stimulus other than the
CONDITIONED STIMuLus Pavlov determined that almost any kind of stimulus could original, natural stimulus that nor-
become associated with the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) if it is paired with the UCS often mally produces the response.
enough. In his original study, the sight of the food dish itself became a stimulus for salivation
before the food was given to the dogs. Every time they got food (to which they automatically unconditioned stimulus (UCS)
salivated), they saw the dish. At this point, the dish was a neutral stimulus (NS) because it in classical conditioning, a naturally
had no effect on salivation. After being paired with the food so many times, the dish came occurring stimulus that leads to an
to produce a salivation response, although a somewhat weaker one, as did the food itself. involuntary and unlearned response.
When a previously neutral stimulus, through repeated pairing with the unconditioned stim-
unconditioned response (UCR)
ulus, begins to cause the same kind of involuntary response, learning has occurred. The
previously neutral stimulus can now be called a conditioned stimulus (CS). (Conditioned in classical conditioning, an invol-
untary and unlearned response to a
means “learned,” and, as mentioned earlier, unconditioned means “unlearned.”)
naturally occurring or unconditioned
CONDITIONED REsPONsE The response that is given to the CS (conditioned stimulus) is stimulus.
not usually quite as strong as the original unconditioned response (UCR), but it is essen-
tially the same response. However, because it comes as a learned response to the condi- neutral stimulus (NS)
tioned stimulus (CS), it is called the conditioned response (CR). in classical conditioning, a stimu-
lus that has no effect on the desired
Putting it all Together: Pavlov’s Canine Classic, or Tick Tock Tick Tock
response prior to conditioning.
Pavlov did a classic experiment in which he paired the ticking sound of a metronome (a
simple device that produces a rhythmic ticking sound) with the presentation of food to
conditioned stimulus (CS)
see if the dogs would eventually salivate at the sound of the metronome (Pavlov, 1927).
in classical conditioning, a previously
Since the metronome’s ticking did not normally produce salivation, it was a n eutral
neutral stimulus that becomes able
stimulus (NS) before any conditioning took place. The repeated pairing of an NS and to produce a conditioned response,
the UCS (unconditioned stimulus) is usually called acquisition, because the organism is after pairing with an unconditioned
in the process of acquiring learning. Figure 5.1 explains how each element of the condi- stimulus.
tioning relationship worked in Pavlov’s experiment.
conditioned response (CR)
in classical conditioning, a learned
*elicit: to draw forth. response to a conditioned stimulus.
220 Chapter 5
Before Conditioning
Neutral Stimulus No Salivation
(NS) Metronome
During Conditioning
Neutral Stimulus Unconditioned Stimulus Unconditioned Response
(NS) Metronome (UCS) Food (UCR) Salivation
After Conditioning
Conditioned Stimulus Conditioned Response
(CS) Metronome (CR) Salivation
(Thomas, 1994). Although classical conditioning happens quite easily, Pavlov and his
other researchers formulated a few basic principles about the process (although we will
see that there are a few exceptions to some of these principles):
1. The CS must come before the UCS. If Pavlov sounded the metronome just after he
gave the dogs the food, they did not become conditioned (Rescorla, 1988).
2. The CS and UCS must come very close together in time—ideally, no more than
5 seconds apart. When Pavlov tried to stretch the time between the potential CS
and the UCS to several minutes, no association or link between the two was made.
Too much could happen in the longer interval of time to interfere with condition-
ing (Pavlov, 1926; Ward et al., 2012; Wasserman & Miller, 1997). Studies have
found that the interstimulus interval (ISI, or the time between the CS and
UCS) can vary depending on the nature of the conditioning task and even the
Strength of GSR
seconds) have been found to be ideal for conditioning (Polewan et al., 2006).
3. The neutral stimulus must be paired with the UCS several times, often many 15
times, before conditioning can take place (Pavlov, 1926).
4. The CS is usually some stimulus that is distinctive* or stands out from other
competing stimuli. The metronome, for example, was a sound that was not 10
0 1 2 3
normally present in the laboratory and, therefore, distinct (Pavlov, 1927; Stimuli
Rescorla, 1988).
Why does the removal of an unconditioned stimulus lead to extinction of the condi-
tioned response? One theory is that the presentation of the CS alone leads to new learning.
During extinction, the CS–UCS association that was learned is weakened, as the CS no
longer predicts the UCS. In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, through extinction they learned to
not salivate to the metronome’s ticking, as it no longer predicted that food was on its way.
Look back at Figure 5.1. Once conditioning is acquired, the conditioned stimulus (CS)
and conditioned response (CR) will always come before the original unconditioned stimu-
lus (UCS). The UCS, which comes after the CS and CR link, now serves as a strengthener,
or reinforcer, of the CS–CR association. Remove that reinforcer, and the CR it strengthens
will weaken and disappear—at least for a while.
The term extinction is a little unfortunate in that it seems to mean that the original
conditioned response is totally gone, dead, never coming back, just like the dinosaurs.
Remember the definition of learning is any relatively permanent change in behavior. The
fact is that once people learn something, it’s almost impossible to “unlearn” it. People
can learn new things that replace it or lose their way to it in memory, but it’s still there. In
the case of classical conditioning, this is easily demonstrated.
After extinguishing the conditioned salivation response in his dogs, Pavlov waited
a few weeks, putting the conditioned stimulus (i.e., the metronome) away. There were no
more training sessions, and the dogs were not exposed to the metronome’s ticking in that
time at all. But when Pavlov took the metronome back out and set it ticking, the dogs all
began to salivate, although it was a fairly weak response and didn’t last very long. This
brief recovery of the conditioned response proves that the CR is “still in there” somewhere
(remember, learning is relatively permanent). It is just suppressed or inhibited by the lack of
an association with the unconditioned stimulus of food (which is no longer reinforcing or
strengthening the CR). As time passes, this inhibition weakens, especially if the original
conditioned stimulus has not been present for a while. In spontaneous recovery the condi-
tioned response can briefly reappear when the original CS returns, although the response is
usually weak and short lived. See Figure 5.3 for a graph showing both extinction and spon-
taneous recovery.
Why Does Classical Conditioning Work? Pavlov believed that the conditioned
stimulus, through its association close in time with the unconditioned stimulus, came
to activate the same place in the animal’s brain that was originally activated by the
unconditioned stimulus. He called this process stimulus substitution. But if a mere asso-
ciation in time is all that is needed, why would conditioning fail to happen when the CS
is presented immediately after the UCS?
Robert Rescorla (1988) found that the CS has to provide some kind of information
about the coming of the UCS in order to achieve conditioning. In other words, the CS must
predict that the UCS is coming. In one study, Rescorla exposed one group of rats to a tone,
and just after the tone’s onset and while the tone was still able to be heard, an electric shock
was administered for some of the tone presentations. Soon the rats became agitated* and
Stage 1
Metronome (NS) UCR (salivation) Conditioning Metronome (CS1) CR (salivation)
UCS (food)
Stage 2
Metronome (CS1) CR (salivation) High-Order Snapping (CS2) CR (salivation)
Snapping (NS) Conditioning
reacted in fear by shivering and squealing at the onset of the tone, a kind of conditioned
emotional response. But with a second group of rats, Rescorla again sounded a tone but
administered the electric shock only after the tone stopped, not while the tone was being
heard. That group of rats responded with fear to the stopping of the tone (Rescorla, 1968).
The tone for the second group of rats provided a different kind of information
than the tone in the first instance. For the first group, the tone means the shock is com-
ing, whereas for the second group, the tone means there is no shock while the tone is
on. It was the particular expectancy created by pairing the tone or absence of tone with
the shock that determined the particular response of the rats. Because this explanation
involves the mental activity of consciously expecting something to occur, it is an example
of an explanation for classical conditioning called the cognitive perspective.
The next time you watch television, watch the commercials closely. Advertisers often
use certain objects or certain types of people in their ads to generate a specific emotional
response in viewers, hoping that the emotional response will become associated with their
product. Sexy models, cute little babies, and adorable puppies are some of the examples of
stimuli the advertising world uses to tug at our heartstrings, so to speak. But advertisers
also use vicarious classical conditioning, often showing people reacting emotionally in the
ad (either positively or negatively) to a product. They hope that the viewer will become con-
ditioned to experience that same emotion when seeing the same product on store shelves.
The good news is that the same learning principles that can contribute to pho-
bias and anxiety disorders can also be used to treat them, as we’ll see in the video Using
Classical Conditioning to Treat Disorders.
CC
butterfly is poisonous to birds, but the moth isn’t. The moth’s mimicry causes birds to
avoid eating it, even though it is quite edible. Researchers have found that some asso-
ciations between certain stimuli and responses are far easier to form than others and
that this is true in both animals and people. This is called biological preparedness.
While mammals are biologically prepared to associate taste with illness, birds are bio-
logically prepared to associate visual characteristics with illness (Shapiro et al., 1980).
As for phobias, fear is a natural emotional response that has ties to survival—we need
to remember what the fear-inducing stimuli are so we can safely avoid them in the future.
Nausea and fear are both examples of involuntary reactions that help organisms survive
to reproduce and pass on their genetic material, so the innate tendency to make quick and
Conditioned taste aversions in nature. This strong associations between stimuli and these reactions has evolutionary importance.
moth is not poisonous to birds, but the
Biological preparedness for fear of objects that are dangerous makes sense for
monarch butterfly whose coloring the moth
survival, but when objects are not typically dangerous, it turns out to be very difficult
imitates is quite poisonous. Birds find their
food by vision and will not eat anything that to condition a fear of those objects. In one study, monkeys easily learned to be afraid
resembles the monarch. of a toy snake or crocodile by watching videos of other monkeys reacting fearfully to
these stimuli (a good example of vicarious classical conditioning). But the monkeys
never learned to fear flowers or a toy rabbit by the same means (Cook & Mineka,
1989). Snakes and crocodiles are predators; flowers and rabbits are not.
Drug Dependency The “high” of drug use, whether it comes from an opiate deriva-
tive, a stimulant, or a depressant such as alcohol, often takes place in certain surroundings,
with certain other people, and perhaps even using certain objects, such as the tiny spoons
used by cocaine addicts. These people, settings, and objects can become conditioned stim-
uli that are associated with the drug high and can produce a conditioned "high" response.
The presence of these cues can make it even harder to resist using the drug because the
body and mind have become classically conditioned to associate drug use with the cues.
Thinking Critically
biological preparedness
Do you think that humans are as controlled by their biology as other animals? Why or why not?
referring to the tendency of animals
to learn certain associations, such as
taste and nausea, with only one or few
pairings due to the survival value of
the learning.
Definition of Learning
(any relatively permanent change in behavior brought about by experience or practice)
Classical Conditioning
(learning to make an involuntary response to a stimulus other than the original, natural stimulus that normally produces it)
unconditioned response (UCR): involuntary response
several key elements to the unconditioned stimulus
must be present conditioned stimulus (CS): previously neutral stimulus that begins
and experienced to cause the same kind of involuntary response when paired Learning 227
repeatedly with the UCS
conditioned response (CR): response that is given to the CS
Classical Conditioning
(learning to make an involuntary response to a stimulus other than the original, natural stimulus that normally produces it)
conditioned emotional responses: emotional responses that have some associations are
become classically conditioned to occur in response to learned stimuli; relatively quick and easy
based on work of John B. Watson; helps explain development of phobias to learn due to survival
value for organism
conditioned taste aversion is one situation where classical conditioning
(biological preparedness)
can occur quickly without repeated pairings
other
features
vicarious conditioning can occur by simply watching someone else respond to a stimulus